13
This article was downloaded by: [UQ Library] On: 24 November 2014, At: 01:14 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition: A Journal on Normal and Dysfunctional Development Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/nanc20 Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects Ilana Blatt-Eisengart & Margie E. Lachman Published online: 09 Aug 2010. To cite this article: Ilana Blatt-Eisengart & Margie E. Lachman (2004) Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects, Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition: A Journal on Normal and Dysfunctional Development, 11:1, 68-79 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/anec.11.1.68.29364 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms- and-conditions

Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects

This article was downloaded by: [UQ Library]On: 24 November 2014, At: 01:14Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Aging, Neuropsychology, andCognition: A Journal on Normal andDysfunctional DevelopmentPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/nanc20

Attributions for Memory Performancein Adulthood: Age Differences andMediation EffectsIlana Blatt-Eisengart & Margie E. LachmanPublished online: 09 Aug 2010.

To cite this article: Ilana Blatt-Eisengart & Margie E. Lachman (2004) Attributions for MemoryPerformance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects, Aging, Neuropsychology, andCognition: A Journal on Normal and Dysfunctional Development, 11:1, 68-79

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/anec.11.1.68.29364

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as tothe accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinionsand views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Contentshould not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sourcesof information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoeveror howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to orarising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Page 2: Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects

Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition2004, Vol. 11, No. 1, pp. 68–79

Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood:Age Differences and Mediation Effects

Ilana Blatt-Eisengart and Margie E. LachmanPsychology Department, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA, USA

ABSTRACT

We examined age differences in attributions to internal (controllable and uncontrollable), external (uncontrol-lable), and unstable factors for performance on a free recall memory task in 149 young, middle-aged, and olderadults. Attributions varied by age and by level of memory performance. Middle-aged and older adults ratedinternal, uncontrollable factors (ability and genes) as more influential for high performance than for lowperformance, and they were less likely than young adults to attribute low performance to these factors. Withinage groups, only the older adults rated memory ability as more influential than strategy use, even though theywere as likely as the other age groups to use a categorization strategy. Attributions to both internal controllable(strategy use) and uncontrollable (ability) factors as well as to health were associated with better memoryperformance. These attributions partially mediated the relationship between age and memory performance.Thus, attributions may provide some insight into sources of age differences in memory performance.

Age differences in memory performance are well

documented with older adults showing poorer

performance on many aspects of memory, includ-

ing episodic memory (Backman, Small, &

Wahlin, 2001). Much work has been focused on

identifying the sources of memory decline,

especially age related changes in speed and

attentional processes. There has been less empha-

sis on the attitudinal factors that could contribute

to memory decline, although there is evidence

that beliefs and attitudes about memory are re-

lated to performance on memory tasks (Lachman,

Steinberg, & Trotter, 1987; West & Thorn, 2001).

One set of beliefs, performance attributions, may

provide insight into the ways that the outcome of

a memory task is interpreted, which can in turn

affect future behavior in similar achievement

situations (Weiner et al., 1972). We view attribu-

tions as a stylistic tendency with trait-like qual-

ities (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978).

These attributional styles may vary across situa-

tions and with age.

Previous research has shown that older and

younger adults make different attributions for the

memory performance of others (Erber, Szuchman,

& Prager, 1997; Guo, Erber, & Szuchman, 1999),

as well as for their own memory performance

(Lineweaver & Hertzog, 1998). Such age differ-

ential patterns of attributions may contribute to

age differences in performance. For example,

those who attribute to internal or controllable

factors such as effort or strategy may be more

likely to use effortful or strategic resources avail-

able to them in order to optimize their memory

performance, whereas attribution to uncontrolla-

ble factors such as luck or task difficulty may

result in limited effort or strategy use (Weiner,

1985). In the present study, we examined age

Address correspondence to: Margie E. Lachman, Psychology Department, Brandeis University, MS 062, Waltham,Massachusetts 02454-9110, USA. E-mail: [email protected] for publication: August 27, 2003.

1382-5585/04/1101-068$16.00 # Taylor & Francis Ltd.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UQ

Lib

rary

] at

01:

14 2

4 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 3: Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects

differences in patterns of attributions, and the

relationship between attributions, strategy use,

and performance on a free recall memory task

for young, middle-aged, and older adults.

Much of the prior research in this area has

focused on the attributions that people make for

performance of others in hypothetical situations.

Lachman and McArthur (1986) found that sub-

jects were more likely to make ability attributions

for the poor memory performance of older adults

and the good performance of younger adults.

More recently, it has been found that people are

more likely to attribute the failures of older adults

to internal, stable, and uncontrollable causes

(such as lack of ability) and the failures of

younger adults to internal, unstable, or controlla-

ble causes (lack of sufficient effort or attention)

(Bieman-Copland & Ryan, 1998; Erber, Prager,

Williams, & Caiola, 1996; Erber et al., 1997;

Lachman, 1990; McCracken, Hayes, & Dell,

1997).

Although most past research has dealt with

attributions for the performance of a hypothetical

other, there have been some studies of attribu-

tions for one’s own performance. Lachman and

her colleagues (Lachman et al., 1987) found that

among older adults, attributions to internal factors

were related to better performance and more

positive self-assessment on a memory task.

Additionally, several researchers have focused

on the distinction between controllable and un-

controllable attributions. Hultsch, Hertzog, and

Dixon (1987) and Lachman and Jelallian (1984)

found that older adults believed that they had less

memory ability and less control over their mem-

ory than did younger adults. Several studies have

found that in comparison to younger adults, older

adults are more likely to make attributions to

uncontrollable factors such as ability than to

controllable factors such as effort or strategy use

(Baldi & Hertzog, 2000; Devolder & Pressley,

1992). Within age groups, older adults are more

likely to attribute performance to uncontrollable

than controllable factors (Devolder & Pressley,

1992). Hertzog, McGuire, and Lineweaver (1998)

examined the relationship between attributions,

memory control beliefs, and strategy use in a free

recall memory task. They divided participants’

responses to an open-ended question into two

groups – those who attributed primarily to skill

(internal, controllable attributions) and those who

attributed primarily to ability (internal, uncontrol-

lable) attributions. They found that older adults

were less likely to attribute to skill (internal,

controllable) and more likely to attribute to abil-

ity (internal, uncontrollable) factors than were

younger adults.

Researchers have found a relationship between

attributions to controllable factors and perfor-

mance (Baldi & Hertzog, 2000). Devolder and

Pressley (1992) found that participants who per-

formed better on a memory task made more

attributions to controllable causes (effort, strategy

use), and that those older adults who attributed to

controllable causes performed as well as younger

adults.

Past work also shows that it is important to

differentiate between attributions made for suc-

cess and failure (Abramson et al., 1978; Lachman

& McArthur, 1986; Weaver & Lachman, 1990).

For success, internal and stable factors can be

self-enhancing by acknowledging credit for the

outcome and the likelihood of repeated success.

For failure, attribution to external and unstable

factors may serve a self-protective function. The

findings suggest that more adaptive attributions

(e.g., to more external or unstable factors) are

made for the failures of younger adults than those

of older adults (Erber et al., 1996; Lachman &

McArthur, 1986). Attributions to controllable

factors are usually considered adaptive regardless

of performance outcome, because attribution to

controllable causes may be associated with taking

responsibility for successful outcomes or with

the opportunity to change future outcomes when

unsuccessful by using more effort or new strate-

gies (Weiner, 1985).

In this study, we defined success and failure

groups based on participants’ actual performance

on a memory task. We predicted that age differ-

ences in attributional patterns would differ for

those subjects who performed well and those who

performed poorly on the task, with older adults

making less adaptive attributions (more internal,

stable, and uncontrollable causes) for failure than

younger adults.

We also examined the relationship between at-

tributions, strategy use, and memory performance

ATTRIBUTIONS FOR MEMORY 69

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UQ

Lib

rary

] at

01:

14 2

4 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 4: Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects

as well as comparing the relationships of actual

strategy use and attribution to strategy use with

age and recall. Hertzog et al. (1998) found that

older adults felt less control over their memory

than the young, and those who felt less control

were less likely to show spontaneous strategy use

in completing the memory task. Hertzog et al.

(1998) also found that controlling for attributions

significantly reduced the effect of age on recall. In

the present study we tested this further by exam-

ining whether attributions for memory perfor-

mance would mediate the relationship between

age and performance on a memory task.

Past studies have either given participants a

relatively short list of attributions to consider or

have asked open-ended questions about attribu-

tions. In the present study, participants were asked

to make judgments about the influence of 13

different attributional variables. This expanded

list gave us an opportunity to examine age differ-

ences in a greater variety of performance attribu-

tions while using the same list of attributions for

all participants.

To review, we expected to find age differences

in attributions that vary by level of memory

performance. Based on findings from past studies

(e.g., Devolder & Pressley, 1992; Lachman &

McArthur, 1986), we expected that older adults’

attributions would be less adaptive than those of

younger adults. For poor performance, older

adults were expected to rate internal uncontrolla-

ble and stable factors as more influential than

did younger adults. Further, young adults were

expected to take credit for their good performance

by making more internal controllable and stable

attributions for high performance than older

adults. Within age groups, we expected older

adults to rate uncontrollable factors (e.g., ability)

as more influential for performance than control-

lable factors (e.g., strategy use) (Hertzog et al.,

1998). Middle-aged adults were expected to show

similar patterns of attribution to those made by

older adults as has been found in past work (Blank

& Levesque, 1993), even though their memory

performance is typically more similar to younger

adults (Miller & Lachman, 2000). We also ex-

pected attributions to internal and controllable

factors to be correlated with better performance

on the memory task. Additionally, we examined

whether age differences in memory would be

mediated by attributions.

METHOD

ParticipantsParticipants were 149 men and women between theages of 21 and 80 years. There were 50 young adultsranging in age from 21 to 40 years (M¼ 32.4,SD¼ 5.7), 50 middle-aged adults ranging in age from41 to 60 years (M¼ 48.5, SD¼ 5.4), and 49 older adultsranging in age from 61 to 80 years (M¼ 69.6, SD¼5.3). Sixty-five percent were female and 89% wereWhite (8% African-American, 2% Asian-American,1% unknown). Participants generally rated themselvesas healthy (young M¼ 4.0, SD¼ 0.9, middle-agedM¼ 4.1, SD¼ 0.8, older M¼ 3.9, SD¼ 1.2) based ontheir response to the question ‘‘Compared to othersyour age, how would you rate your overall health?’’(1¼ poor to 5¼ excellent). There were no significantage differences in health.

Participants were recruited from a random samplingof individuals living in the Greater Boston Metropol-itan Area (within a 20 mile radius of Waltham,Massachusetts) using lists that were stratified by ageand gender and generated by a sampling firm. Partic-ipants were initially contacted by mail and then bytelephone. Participants were excluded from the study ifthey had a history of stroke in the last 5 years, serioushead injury, or Parkinson’s disease, did not have full useof both of their hands, or were not native Englishspeakers. Participants received $25 for their time.

Measures

EducationEducation was measured on a 12-point scale with1¼ some grade school; 2¼ junior high/8th grade;3¼ some high school; 4¼Graduate EquivalencyDiploma (GED); 5¼ graduated from high school;6¼ 1–2 years of college, no degree; 7¼ 3 years ofcollege, no degree; 8¼ 2-year college degree, 9¼4-year college degree; 10¼ some graduate school;11¼master’s degree; 12¼ doctoral level degree (M¼8.48, SD¼ 2.14). There were no significant educationdifferences by age group (young M¼ 8.6, SD¼ 1.7,middle-aged M¼ 8.6, SD¼ 2.3, older M¼ 8.2,SD¼ 2.4).

Word List RecallTwo lists of 30 categorizable nouns developed byHertzog, Dixon, and Hultsch (1990) were used. Eachlist contained 6 words from five distinctive taxonomic

70 ILANA BLATT-EISENGART & MARGIE E. LACHMAN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UQ

Lib

rary

] at

01:

14 2

4 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 5: Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects

categories, which were developed using norms fromHoward (1980). The categories were: Metals, animals,trees, sports, flowers, relatives, fruit, birds, furniture,and weapons. The order of words was randomized, butwords from the same category did not appear adjacentto one another. Words were presented in two columnson a computer screen in the same random order for allparticipants. There were three consecutive trials foreach word list. Participants studied the word list for1 min at each trial and had unlimited time for recall ateach presentation of the list. The presentation orderof the two word lists was counterbalanced acrossparticipants.

Recall data were scored using the Scoring Optionsfor Recall Tests (SORT) Version 2.0 (Elie & Payne,1999). List recall was scored as the total number ofwords correctly recalled out of 30. The variable used inanalyses was the mean number of words recalled on thethird trial averaged across lists (M¼ 22.04, SD¼ 5.28).The SORT program was also used to calculate anAdjusted Ratio for Clustering (ARC) score (Roenker,Thompson, & Brown, 1971) indicating the extent towhich participants categorized the words at recall.Categorization scores range from 0 to 1 and higherscores reflect a greater degree of clustering. The ARCwas used as a measure of actual strategy use for therecall task (M¼ .76, SD¼ 0.23). There were nosignificant age differences in ARC.

Actual Performance LevelPerformance was recoded as a dichotomous variable(high vs. low) based on a median split of words recalledfor the third trial. Each age group’s median wasassessed and split separately: (young¼ 25.5, middle¼23.5, older¼ 19.5). Young adults had higher (p< .01)performance than middle-aged and older adults, andmiddle-aged adults were higher than older adults,F(2, 146)¼ 19.22, p< .001. Actual recall (youngM¼ 24.9, SD¼ 3.7, middle M¼ 22.2, SD¼ 5.4, olderM¼ 19.0, SD¼ 4.9) was significantly correlated(r¼ .631, p< .001) with perceived performance, asmeasured by response to the question ‘‘In general, howwould you rate the level of your performance?’’(1¼ very poor to 7¼ very good). This correlation wasalso significant for each individual age group (youngr¼ .42, p< .01; middle-aged r¼ .67, p< .001; olderr¼ .57, p< .001).

Performance AttributionsTask specific performance and attribution questionsadapted from Hertzog et al. (1998), Lachman et al.(1987), and Weaver and Lachman (1990) were used toassess performance attributions and perceived perfor-mance. The performance attribution questions askedparticipants to think about the factors that influenced

their performance on the word list recall test. Using a 5-point scale (1¼ not at all to 5¼ very much so),participants indicated to what extent they believed agiven factor influenced their performance (e.g.,‘‘________’’ influenced my memory performance).Items included ‘‘how tired I was’’, ‘‘my mood’’, ‘‘thedifficulty level of the task’’, ‘‘how hard I tried’’,‘‘luck’’, ‘‘my memory ability’’, ‘‘my age’’, ‘‘howrelevant or meaningful I found the task’’, ‘‘my health’’,‘‘my anxiety level’’, ‘‘the strategy I used’’, ‘‘medica-tion’’, and ‘‘my genes (inherited characteristics)’’.

Based on conceptual definitions from past research(Baldi & Hertzog, 2000; Devolder & Pressley, 1992;Hertzog, McGuire, Powell-Moman, & York, 2001;Weiner, 1985; Weiner et al., 1972), we organized theattribution variables into three subsets: internal con-trollable (effort, strategy use) and uncontrollable(ability, genes); external uncontrollable (task difficulty,task meaningfulness, luck); and unstable (tiredness,anxiety level, medications, health, mood). Age was notincluded in any subset because it was influenced by themanipulation used in the overall study as discussedbelow. Means and standard deviations for the attribu-tion variables organized by subset can be found inTable 1.

ProcedureThese data were collected as part of a larger studyexamining memory and control beliefs. All sessionswere run by one of two female experimenters. Fifty-eight percent of the participants were tested in theirhomes and 42% were tested in a university research lab.Age (3) by location (2) analyses of variance wereperformed for all variables, and no significant effects oflocation were found. Using MediaLab2000 software(Jarvis, 2000), most of the protocol was presented on alaptop computer (Dell Latitude CPx). Participants readinstructions on the computer screen and proceededthrough the session at their own pace. No computerexperience was necessary and the researcher wasavailable throughout the session. Participants first com-pleted the entire Word List Recall Test (two lists, threetrials each), and then the attribution questionnaire.

The purpose of the overall study was to examine theeffects of beliefs about memory on performance. Foranother aspect of the study, participants were randomlyassigned to three memory information groups (agedifferences, no age differences, and control) andmatched on education across age groups. Subjects weretold either that there were or were not age differences inperformance on the test that they were taking, or weregiven no information regarding age differences. [SeeAndreoletti & Lachman (2004, in press) for moredetails]. Although condition differences were not the fo-cus of this study, we performed Age (3)�Condition (3)

ATTRIBUTIONS FOR MEMORY 71

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UQ

Lib

rary

] at

01:

14 2

4 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 6: Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects

analyses of variance for all variables and found asignificant difference on only the attribution to agevariable, F(2, 146)¼ 7.101, p< .01, which was thenexcluded from this study. As expected, those in the agedifferences condition attributed performance more toage than did those in the control condition (agedifferences condition, M¼ 3.00, control conditionM¼ 2.49, no age differences condition M¼ 2.20). Asno main effects of condition or age by conditioninteractions were found for other attributions, numberof words recalled, or strategy use, subjects werecollapsed across condition for all analyses presentedin this paper.

RESULTS

We first used multivariate analyses of variance to

examine whether the attributions differed by age

and whether age differences were qualified by

level of actual memory performance for the three

subsets of items. We also compared the level of

attribution ratings for ability and strategy use

within age groups as has been done in previous

studies (Hertzog et al., 1998). The second set of

analyses includes correlations between attribu-

tions and memory performance and a regression

analysis investigating whether age differences in

memory are mediated by attributions. Although

education level did not vary by age group, this

sample included a wide range of education levels.

Therefore, education was entered as a covariate

in all analyses to examine whether it affected

the results. Because including education did not

change the results, all analyses are presented

without the covariate.

Age and Performance Attributions

To examine the relationship between age, level of

memory performance, and performance attribu-

tions, a (3) Age Group (young, middle-aged, and

older) by (2) Performance (high, low) MANOVA

was performed on each of the three subsets of

attribution variables (internal, external uncontrol-

lable, and unstable). Pillai’s trace criterion was

used for all multivariate tests.

For the first set of variables, internal attributions,

results showed significant multivariate main effects

of age F(8, 282)¼ 4.80, p< .001, �2¼ .12 and

performance F(4, 140)¼ 4.91, p¼ .001, �2¼ .12.

Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations for Attribution Variables.

Attribution variable Younger adults Middle-aged adults Older adults Total

M SD M SD M SD M SD

Internal attributionsUncontrollable

Memory ability� 4.44 0.73 4.16 0.93 3.92 1.15 4.17 0.97Genes 2.86 1.21 2.68 1.33 2.37 1.48 2.64 1.35

ControllableStrategy use��� 4.38 0.86 4.08 1.01 3.12 1.22 3.87 1.16Effort 3.56 1.20 3.28 1.32 3.02 1.20 3.29 1.25

ExternalUncontrollable attributions

Task difficulty� 3.04 1.28 2.68 1.29 2.37 1.20 2.70 1.28Meaningfulness of task 2.62 1.31 2.80 1.41 2.31 1.21 2.58 1.32Luck�� 1.58 0.76 1.26 0.49 1.24 0.52 1.36 0.62

Unstable attributionsAnxiety level 2.40 1.14 2.52 1.40 2.04 1.15 2.32 1.25Health�� 2.68 1.41 2.34 1.52 1.80 1.35 2.28 1.47Mood 2.34 1.26 2.26 1.26 1.82 1.05 2.14 1.21Tiredness 2.38 1.24 1.98 1.00 1.92 1.08 2.09 1.12Medication 1.28 0.81 1.26 0.66 1.37 0.86 1.30 0.78

Note. Variables showing significant age differences are marked �p� .05; ��p� .01; ���p� .001.

72 ILANA BLATT-EISENGART & MARGIE E. LACHMAN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UQ

Lib

rary

] at

01:

14 2

4 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 7: Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects

These effects were qualified by an age�performance interaction F(8, 282)¼ 2.48, p< .05,

�2¼ .07. Univariate tests showed main effects of

age for memory ability F(2, 143)¼ 3.58, p< .05,

�2¼ .05 and strategy use F(2, 143)¼ 20.77,

p< .001, �2¼ .23, and main effects of performance

for memory ability F(1, 143)¼ 8.28, p< .01, �2¼.06, strategy use F(1, 143)¼ 10.22, p< .01, �2¼.07, and genes F(1, 143)¼ 7.35, p< .01, �2¼ .05.

For internal uncontrollable factors, these results

were qualified by age� performance interactions:

memory ability F(2, 143)¼ 6.02, p< .01, �2¼ .08

and genes F(2, 143)¼ 4.00, p< .05, �2¼ .05.

Post hoc pairwise comparisons showed that

young adults attributed significantly (p< .05)

more highly to memory ability than did older

adults. Young and middle-aged adults attributed

significantly more highly to strategy use than did

older adults. Means for these effects are presented

in Table 1. Across age, high performers rated in-

ternal dimensions as more influential than did low

performers for memory ability (High M¼ 4.40,

SD¼ 0.81, Low M¼ 3.97, SD¼ 1.06), strategy

use (High M¼ 4.14, SD¼ 0.91, Low M¼ 3.62,

SD¼ 1.30), and genes (High M¼ 2.94, SD¼1.24, Low M¼ 2.37, SD¼ 1.40). The age by

performance interactions for memory ability and

genes showed significant age differences in attri-

bution only among those with low performance.

Within this group, young adults attributed more

highly to memory ability than did the middle-

aged, who attributed significantly more highly to

ability than did older adults. Young adults attri-

buted more highly to genes than did middle-aged

or older adults. Further, middle-aged and older

adults made higher attributions to internal un-

controllable factors (ability and genes) for high

performance than for low performance, but the

young did not make different attributions as a

function of performance level. These results are

presented in Figure 1.

For the second set of variables, external attri-

butions, results showed a significant main effect

of age F(6, 284)¼ 2.75, p< .05, �2¼ .06, and

univariate tests showed significant main effects

of age for task difficulty F(2, 143)¼ 3.44, p< .05,

�2¼ .05 and luck F(2, 143)¼ 4.79, p¼ .01,

�2¼ .06. Post hoc pairwise comparisons showed

that young adults attributed significantly ( p< .05)

more highly to task difficulty than did older

adults, as well as attributing significantly more

highly to luck than did middle-aged and older

adults. Means for these effects are presented in

Table 1.

For the third set of variables, unstable attribu-

tions, results showed significant main effects of

age F(10, 280)¼ 2.08, p< .05, �2¼ .07 and per-

formance F(5, 139)¼ 4.34, p¼ .01, �2¼ .14.

Health was the only univariate variable with

significant age differences F(2, 143)¼ 4.93,

p< .01, �2¼ .06. Significant performance differ-

ences were found for health F(1, 143)¼ 6.61,

p< .05, �2¼ .04 and anxiety level F(1, 143)¼4.84, p< .05, �2¼ .03. Post hoc pairwise compar-

isons showed that both young and middle-aged

adults attributed more highly to health than did

older adults. Means for these effects can be found

in Table 1. Across age, higher performers rated

Fig. 1. Means for age� performance interactions for attributions to memory ability and genes.

ATTRIBUTIONS FOR MEMORY 73

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UQ

Lib

rary

] at

01:

14 2

4 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 8: Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects

health as more influential than did those with

lower performance (High M¼ 2.59, SD¼ 1.57,

Low M¼ 2.00, SD¼ 1.31), while lower perfor-

mers rated anxiety level as more influential than

did those with high performance (High M¼ 2.09,

SD¼ 1.16 , Low M¼ 2.53, SD¼ 1.29).

Within Age Comparisons

To compare the attributions to internal control-

lable (strategy use) and uncontrollable factors

(ability) within age groups, a (3) age groups

(young, middle-aged, and older) by 2 (attribution)

repeated measures analysis of variance was per-

formed with attributions to memory ability and

attribution to strategy use as the within-subjects

variable. Tests of within subjects effect showed a

significant main effect of attribution F(1, 146)¼10.67, p< .01, �2¼ .07. This result was qualified

by a significant age by attribution interaction

F(2, 146)¼ 6.38, p< .01, �2¼ .08. Post hoc

comparisons within age groups showed that

young and middle-aged adults did not signifi-

cantly differ on attributions to strategy use and

to memory ability, whereas older adults were

significantly (p< .001) more likely to attribute to

memory ability (M¼ 3.92, SD¼ 1.15) than to

strategy use (M¼ 3.12, SD¼ 1.22). This result is

illustrated in Figure 2. An analysis adding per-

formance level as a between subjects factors

showed that the age by attribution effect did not

vary by level of performance.

Age, Recall, and Attributions

The correlations between the attribution variables,

age, and recall revealed that memory ability,

strategy use, and health attributions were related

to both age and recall (See Table 2). Those who

attributed more highly to these factors were

younger and had higher performance on the

memory task.

In order to examine whether attributions

mediated the relationship between age and recall,

we performed regression analyses as described by

Baron and Kenny (1986) to determine whether the

attribution variables reduced the effect of age on

performance. An initial regression showed that

age predicted recall F(1)¼ 47.08, p< .001, and a

second set of regressions showed that age also

predicted all three attributions F(1)¼ 9.51,

p< .01 (health), F(1)¼ 7.46, p< .01 (memory

ability), F(1)¼ 35.76, p< .001 (strategy use).

Thus, the preliminary assumptions for mediation

analysis were met. As shown in Table 3, at Step 1

we entered age as a predictor of recall. At Step 2,

we entered age and the three attributions, showing

that attributions and age significantly predicted

recall F(4)¼ 22.13, p< .001, and the effect of age

on performance was significantly reduced, as

indicated by the Sobel test statistic (z¼ � 3.89,

p< .001) for the average of the three attributions.

When each of the three attribution variables were

examined separately, each significantly reduced

the effect of age on recall as indicated by the

Sobel test statistic (health z¼ � 2.39, p¼ .01,

strategy use z¼ � 3.26, p¼ .001, memory ability

z¼ � 1.90, p¼ .057 (Kenny, Kashy, & Bolger,

1998).

Actual Strategy Use, Attributions,

and Performance

We looked separately at the relation between

actual strategy use (ARC), categorization, and the

Fig. 2. Means for age� attribution interaction for attributions to memory ability and strategy use.

74 ILANA BLATT-EISENGART & MARGIE E. LACHMAN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UQ

Lib

rary

] at

01:

14 2

4 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 9: Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects

attribution to strategy use, which showed the

highest correlations with age and memory per-

formance (see Table 2). Actual strategy use

(ARC), was also related to performance, but age

groups did not differ in their use of categorization.

Those who made attributions to strategy use were

significantly more likely to categorize the words

during recall. As reported above, attribution to

strategy use partially mediated the effects of age

on recall. Actual use of strategies, however, which

was not significantly related to age, was not a

mediator of the relationship between age and

recall.

DISCUSSION

Consistent with past research, our results demon-

strated that a relationship exists between age,

attributions, and performance on a memory task.

As expected, age differences in performance

attributions were found and in some cases they

varied by level of performance. Young adults

were expected to make more internal control-

lable attributions for high performance than for low

performance. Contrary to predictions, young adults

did not show different attributional patterns as a

function of performance, whereas middle-aged and

Table 3. Regression Analysis Showing the Effects of Age on Recall are Mediated by Internal Attributions.

Step and predictor variable B SE B � R2 �R2

Step 1 .24 .24���Age of participant � .16 .02 �.49���

Step 2 .38 .14���Age of participant � .11 .02 �.33���

AttributionsStrategy use .77 .36 .17�Memory ability .99 .39 .18�Health .80 .25 .22��

�p� .05; ��p� .01; ���p� .001.

Table 2. Correlations of Attributions With Age, Recall, ARC, and Education.

Age Recall ARC score Education

Age – – – –Recall � 0.49�� – – –ARC score � 0.02 0.41�� – –Education � 0.04 0.28�� 0.27�� –Memory ability � 0.20� 0.33�� 0.15 0.05Genes � 0.08 0.28�� 0.13 0.25��Strategy use � 0.43�� 0.45�� 0.25�� 0.23��Effort � 0.13 0.15 0.12 0.11Task difficulty � 0.17� 0.06 0.00 � 0.02Meaningfulness of task � 0.10 0.02 � 0.08 0.02Luck � 0.19� 0.08 0.04 0.22��Anxiety level � 0.09 � 0.09 � 0.10 � 0.02Health � 0.25�� 0.37�� 0.15 0.13Mood � 0.18� 0.12 0.08 0.20�Tiredness � 0.15 0.01 � 0.06 0.14Medication 0.08 � 0.08 � 0.09 � 0.02

�p� .05; ��p� .01.

ATTRIBUTIONS FOR MEMORY 75

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UQ

Lib

rary

] at

01:

14 2

4 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 10: Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects

older adults made stronger attributions to internal

uncontrollable factors for high performance than

for low performance, an adaptive pattern. Con-

sistent with past work (Hertzog et al., 1998), when

comparing internal controllable and uncontrollable

attributions within age groups, middle-aged and

older adults were more likely to attribute to an

uncontrollable factor (ability) than to a controllable

factor (strategy use), irrespective of performance

level.

Older adults were expected to rate internal

uncontrollable and stable factors as more influ-

ential for poor performance than younger adults.

However, regardless of level of performance,

younger adults rated internal (ability, strategy

use), unstable (health), and external uncontrollable

(task difficulty, luck) factors as more influential

than older adults did. Interestingly, there were no

age differences for effort, an internal controllable

factor. Thus, although older adults were as likely as

the young to attribute performance to their effort,

they were less likely to attribute to use of strategies.

Post-performance attributions can be indica-

tive of strategies for management of success and

failure (e.g., Abramson et al., 1978; Weiner et al.,

1972). Those who blame themselves (i.e., make

internal attributions) for low performance or fail-

ure are likely to feel worse about their perfor-

mance than those who attribute failures to

external factors. Contrary to predictions, in this

study, middle-aged and older adults with low

performance were more likely than younger

adults to use adaptive attributions (i.e., lower

attributions to internal uncontrollable factors for

low performance compared to high performance).

Younger adults used internal attributions equally

for high and low performance, suggesting they do

not distinguish sources of success and failure.

This is consistent with recent findings by Wrosch

and Heckhausen (2002), which showed that

younger adults were more likely than older adults

to attribute failures to internal factors, without

negative emotional or motivational consequences.

Middle-aged and older adults who perform poorly

may use adaptive attributions (i.e., less internal)

as a protective strategy to avoid self-blame or

negative feelings.

Our findings regarding age differences in con-

trollability were not completely consistent with

our predictions. As predicted, attribution to strat-

egy use, an internal controllable factor, was

related to better performance on the memory

task, and attribution to external uncontrollable

factors was not related to performance. Contrary

to expectations, young adults attributed more

highly to some external uncontrollable factors

(luck and task difficulty) than middle-aged and

older adults regardless of performance.

The results supported our prediction of a rela-

tionship between internal attributions and perfor-

mance, but contrary to past findings (Devolder &

Pressley, 1992), both internal controllable and

uncontrollable factors were positively associated

with performance. Across age, internal attributions,

whether to controllable or uncontrollable factors,

were associated with better performance on the

memory task. Additionally, as predicted, these

attributions partially mediated the relationship

between age and performance. Although the effects

of age on performance were not fully mediated, the

results are consistent with past findings that age

differences in memory are not significant among

those who make internal controllable attributions

(Devolder & Pressley, 1992).

It is interesting to note that although attribution

to strategy use partially mediated the relation-

ship between age and performance, actual use of

strategies (as measured by the ARC score) was not

related to age. This is consistent with findings by

Hertzog et al. (1998), who found that there were

no age differences in actual strategy use but that

older adults were less likely to make attributions

to strategy (internal-skill). In the present study,

younger and older adults used categorization stra-

tegies equally, but after performance young and

middle-aged adults attributed to strategy use sig-

nificantly more than the older adults, suggesting

that older adults were less likely to see a connection

between use of strategies and performance. More-

over, older adults were more likely to attribute

performance to ability than to strategy use, whereas

young and middle-aged adults thought both factors

had similar levels of influence on performance.

This was contrary to our prediction that the attribu-

tions of middle-aged adults would be more similar

to those of older adults.

Middle-aged adults sometimes showed no dif-

ferences from other age groups, falling somewhere

76 ILANA BLATT-EISENGART & MARGIE E. LACHMAN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UQ

Lib

rary

] at

01:

14 2

4 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 11: Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects

in the middle (for memory ability and task diffi-

culty), sometimes were only significantly different

from older adults (for strategy use and health), and

sometimes were only significantly different from

younger adults (for genes, luck). When the age by

performance interaction for memory ability was

examined, there were significant differences

between all age groups among those with low

performance on the memory task. Middle-aged

adults and older adults both showed a similar

adaptive pattern by attributing more highly to

internal uncontrollable factors for high perfor-

mance than for low performance. This indicates

that by middle age, attributions may already serve

a self-enhancing function to protect against con-

cerns about memory loss (Lachman, 1991). In

fact, in this study middle-aged adults did have

significantly lower performance than the young,

although the findings from past research regarding

actual memory performance for middle-aged

adults are inconclusive (Lachman, 2004, in press).

Age differences in memory were partially

accounted for by internal attributions. However,

given the design, we cannot make clear inferences

about the directionality of these effects. As is

typically the case, the attributions were made

after the memory task was completed. Thus, it is

possible that performance outcomes influence attri-

butions, and that age differences in attributions are

mediated by performance. Conceptually, it is more

plausible that attributions, which are seen to be a

relatively stable explanatory style (Abramson et al.,

1978), should have an effect on performance,

through mechanisms such as effort expenditure

and strategy use (Lachman et al., 1987). Those

who attribute performance to internal or controlla-

ble factors should have higher performance

because they would be more likely to engage in

effortful and strategic behaviors (Bandura, 1997).

Indeed, we found that those who made more

internal attributions, especially to strategy use,

which is also controllable, were more likely to

use an adaptive organizational memory strategy.

Future research with multiple assessments of attri-

butions and performance and investigation of beha-

vioral mediators is needed to explore this.

Our study was limited by the removal of age

from the list of attributions analyzed, due to the

effect of the manipulation in the larger study.

Future work should explore the relationship of

attribution to age with recall and performance.

Further studies are also needed to explore the

relationship between performance attributions

and performance on other types of memory

tasks.

In summary, our results confirm previous find-

ings that there is a relationship between internal

attributions and memory performance. This was

true for both controllable (strategy use) and

uncontrollable (ability) factors. Among middle-

aged and older adults, lower performing partic-

ipants considered internal uncontrollable factors

to be less influential than higher performing par-

ticipants, a self-protective pattern. Attributional

style did not interact with performance in younger

adults, perhaps indicating that there is less need

for protective or self-enhancing attributions for

memory among young adults.

Attributions to internal factors partially

mediated the relationship between age and per-

formance. This research adds to the growing body

of work indicating that beliefs about memory are

related to performance. It is apparent that there

are age differences in beliefs about memory, and

that these age differences may be among the

factors contributing to age differences in memory

performance. It is possible that interventions

designed to influence older adults’ beliefs about

the importance of strategy use and other control-

lable factors would be useful in helping older

adults to preserve better memory functioning.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENT

This research was funded by grant R01 AG17920 toMargie E. Lachman from the National Institute onAging. We appreciate Carrie Andreoletti’s many con-tributions to this project. We would also like to thankShana Ratner for her help with data coding and entry.

REFERENCES

Abramson, L.Y., Seligman, M.E.P., & Teasdale, J.D.(1978). Learned helplessness: Critique and reformu-lation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.

Andreoletti, C., & Lachman, M.E. (2004, in press).Susceptibility and resilience to memory aging

ATTRIBUTIONS FOR MEMORY 77

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UQ

Lib

rary

] at

01:

14 2

4 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 12: Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects

stereotypes: Education matters more than age.Experimental Aging Research, 30.

Backman, L., Small, B.J., & Wahlin, A. (2001). Agingand memory: Cognitive and biological perspectives.In K.W. Schaie (Ed.), Handbook of the psychologyof aging (5th ed., pp. 349–377). San Diego, CA: USAcademic Press.

Baldi, R., & Hertzog, C. (2000). Age differences inattributions about memory task performance byoneself and others. Washington, DC: AmericanPsychological Association.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise ofcontrol. New York: Freeman.

Baron, R.M., & Kenny, D.A. (1986). The moderator/mediator variable distinction in social psychologicalresearch: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical con-siderations. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 51, 1173–1183.

Bieman-Copland, S., & Ryan, E.B. (1998). Age-biasedinterpretation of memory successes and failures inadulthood. Journal of Gerontology: PsychologicalSciences, 53B, 105–111.

Blank, T.O., & Levesque, M.J. (1993). Constructingsuccess and failure: Age differences in perceptionsand explanations of success and failure. Interna-tional Journal of Aging and Human Development,37, 105–118.

Devolder, P.A., & Pressley, M. (1992). Causal attribu-tions and strategy use in relation to memoryperformance differences in younger and olderadults. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6, 629–642.

Elie, C.J., & Payne, D.G. (1999). Scoring Options forRecall Tests (SORT), version 2.0: Updated toinclude false memory analyses, nominal groupcreation, and phenomenological measures. Behav-ioral Research Methods, Instruments, and Compu-ters, 31, 380–383.

Erber, J.T., Prager, I.G., Williams, M., & Caiola, M.A.(1996). Age and forgetfulness: Confidence in abilityand attribution for memory failures. Psychology andAging, 11, 310–315.

Erber, J.T., Szuchman, L.T., & Prager, I.G. (1997).Forgetful but forgiven: How age and life style affectperceptions of memory failure. Journal of Geron-tology: Psychological Sciences, 52B, P303–P307.

Guo, X., Erber, J.T., & Szuchman, L.T. (1999). Age andforgetfulness: Can stereotypes be modified? Educa-tional Gerontology, 25, 457–466.

Hertzog, C., Dixon, R.A., & Hultsch, D.F. (1990).Relationships between metamemory, memory pre-dictions, and memory task performance in adults.Psychology and Aging, 5, 215–227.

Hertzog, C., McGuire, C.L., & Lineweaver, T.T.(1998). Aging, attributions, perceived control andstrategy use in a free recall task. Aging, Neuropsy-chology and Cognition, 5, 85–106.

Hertzog, C., McGuire, C.L., Powell-Moman, A., &York, A. (2001). Implicit theories and self-referentbeliefs regarding control over memory. Paper pre-sented at the Annual Convention of the AmericanPsychological Association, San Francisco, CA.

Howard, D. (1980). Category norms: A comparison ofthe Battig and Montague (1969) norms with theresponses of adults between the ages of 20 and 80.Journal of Gerontology, 35, 225–231.

Hultsch, D.F., Hertzog, C., & Dixon, R.A. (1987). Agedifferences in metamemory: Resolving the incon-sistencies. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41,193–208.

Jarvis, B. (2000). MediaLab2000 Research Software.Philadelphia, PA: Empirisoft.

Kenny, D.A., Kashy, D.A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Dataanalysis in social psychology. In G. Lindzey (Ed.),The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1,pp. 233–265). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.

Lachman, M.E. (1990). When bad things happen toolder people: Age differences in attributional style.Psychology and Aging, 5, 607–609.

Lachman, M.E. (1991). Perceived control over mem-ory and aging: Developmental and interventionperspectives. Journal of Social Issues, 47, 159–175.

Lachman, M.E. (2004, in press). Development inmidlife. Annual Review of Psychology, 55.

Lachman, M.E., & Jelallian, E. (1984). Self-efficacyand attributions for intellectual performance inyoung and elderly adults. Journal of Gerontology,39, 577–582.

Lachman, M.E., & McArthur, L.Z. (1986). Adulthoodage differences in causal attributions for cognitive,physical, and social performance. Psychology andAging, 1, 127–132.

Lachman, M.E., Steinberg, E.S., & Trotter, S.D. (1987).Effects of control beliefs and attributions onmemory self-assessments and performance. Psy-chology and Aging, 2, 266–271.

Lineweaver, T.T., & Hertzog, C. (1998). Adults’efficacy and control beliefs regarding memory andaging: Separating general from personal beliefs.Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 5,264–296.

McCracken, J.E., Hayes, J.A., & Dell, D. (1997).Attributions of responsibility for memory problemsin older and younger adults. Journal of Counselingand Development, 75, 385–391.

Miller, L.S., & Lachman, M.E. (2000). Cognitiveperformance and the role of control beliefs inmidlife. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 7,69–85.

Roenker, D.L., Thompson, C.P., & Brown, S.C. (1971).Comparison of measures for the estimation ofclustering in free recall. Psychological Bulletin,76, 45–48.

78 ILANA BLATT-EISENGART & MARGIE E. LACHMAN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UQ

Lib

rary

] at

01:

14 2

4 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 13: Attributions for Memory Performance in Adulthood: Age Differences and Mediation Effects

Weaver, S.L., & Lachman, M.E. (1990). When memoryfails: Adulthood age differences in attribution formemory. Boston, MA: American PsychologicalAssociation.

Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achieve-ment motivation and emotion. PsychologicalReview, 92, 548–573.

Weiner, B., Frieze, I., Kukla, A., Reed, L., Rest, S., &Rosenbaum, R.M. (1972). Perceiving the causes ofsuccess and failure. In D.E. Kanouse (Ed.), Attribu-

tion: Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 95–120). Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.

West, R.L., & Thorn, R.M. (2001). Goal-setting, self-efficacy, and memory performance in older andyounger adults. Experimental Aging Research, 27,41–65.

Wrosch, C., & Heckhausen, J. (2002). Perceivedcontrol of life regrets: Good for young and badfor old adults. Psychology and Aging, 17,340–350.

ATTRIBUTIONS FOR MEMORY 79

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UQ

Lib

rary

] at

01:

14 2

4 N

ovem

ber

2014