12
Journal of Applied Psychology Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1998, Vol. 83, No. 4, 654-665 0021-9010/98/$3.00 Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to Learn During the Learning Process" A Longitudinal Study Jason A. Colquitt and Marcia J. Simmering Michigan State University Conscientiousness and goal orientation were examined as (a) predictors of motivation to learn and (b) moderators of reactions to performance levels during the learning process, using an Expectancy × Valence framework. Learners (N = 103) participated in a 6- week course in which an objective performance goal was assigned. Results indicated that conscientiousness and learning orientation were positively related to motivation to learn both initially and after performance feedback was given, whereas performance orientation was negatively related to motivation to learn at those 2 time periods. In addition, learning and performance orientation moderated the relationships between performance levels during the learning process and subsequent expectancy and valence. The increasing complexity of the tasks to be performed in the future workplace is likely to place an even greater emphasis on training programs, as evidenced by the fact that no less than six chapters in Howard's (1995) The Changing Nature of Work noted the need for improved training approaches. The increased emphasis on training will necessitate a commitment on the part of selection programs to provide motivated trainees. Such a commit- ment could create internal fit and synergy among human resources practices, potentially improving firm perfor- mance (Becker & Gerhart, 1996; Delery & Doty, 1996). One stream of literature that can inform practitioners in this area is the research on motivation to learn, defined here as a desire on the part of trainees to learn the content of the training program (Noe, 1986). Research has consistently shown a positive relationship between motivation to learn and learning across a variety of settings, but several important gaps remain. For exam- ple, research linking dispositional personality variables to motivation to learn has been limited and often atheoretical. In addition, researchers have not adequately explored what types of learners continue to show high motivation Jason A. Colquitt and Marcia J. Simmering, Eli Broad Gradu- ate School of Management, Michigan State University. We wish to thank John R. Hollenbeck, Raymond A. Noe, Linn Van Dyne, and J. Kevin Ford for their helpful comments and suggestions on an earlier draft of this article. An earlier version of this article was presented at the 1997 meeting of the Academy of Management, Boston, Massachusetts. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jason A. Colquitt, Eli Broad Graduate School of Management, N475 North Business Complex, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan 48824. Electronic mail may be sent to colquitt @ pilot.msu.edu. levels throughout the learning process, particularly in the wake of early difficulty (Fedor, 1991; Phillips & Gully, 1997). This issue is critical because many learners strug- gle during the early phases of the training program and often fall short of the objective performance goals that are typically used (Farr, Hofmann, & Ringenbach, 1993). Our study addressed both of these gaps by integrating two personality variables, conscientiousness and goal ori- entation, with motivation to learn to assess their role in learning. We examined these issues in the context of a 6- week academic course, which provided an opportunity to study the learning process using a more comprehensive, longitudinal approach than many past investigations (e.g., Baldwin, Magjuka, & Loher, 1991; Martocchio & Web- ster, 1992; Phillips & Gully, 1997; Quinones, 1995). We gave a difficult but achievable performance goal to each learner as part of the course, matching the type of goal normally assigned to employees in training programs and organizational settings (Farr et al., 1993; Locke & La- tham, 1990). We then examined personality relationships at two time periods during the learning process: the begin- ning of the course and directly after learners were given feedback relating their current performance levels with their assigned goal (approximately halfway through the course). Gagne and Medsker (1996) noted that these two time periods are critical junctures in the learning process. We cast conscientiousness and goal orientation as distal variables that influenced learning through the more proxi- mal mechanism of motivation to learn. Kanfer (1991) advocated using this distal-proximal framework for ex- amining personality effects because it places those effects in a more theoretical context (see also Judge & Martoc- chio, 1995). We predicted that feedback regarding one's performance levels vis-?a-vis the assigned goal would alter subsequent motivation levels. In addition, we examined 654

Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to ...media.terry.uga.edu/socrates/publications/2013/06/ColquittSimmering1998.pdf · Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    7

  • Download
    3

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to ...media.terry.uga.edu/socrates/publications/2013/06/ColquittSimmering1998.pdf · Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation

Journal of Applied Psychology Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1998, Vol. 83, No. 4, 654-665 0021-9010/98/$3.00

Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to Learn During the Learning Process" A Longitudinal Study

Jason A. Colquitt and Marcia J. Simmering Michigan State University

Conscientiousness and goal orientation were examined as (a) predictors of motivation to learn and (b) moderators of reactions to performance levels during the learning process, using an Expectancy × Valence framework. Learners (N = 103) participated in a 6- week course in which an objective performance goal was assigned. Results indicated that conscientiousness and learning orientation were positively related to motivation to learn both initially and after performance feedback was given, whereas performance orientation was negatively related to motivation to learn at those 2 time periods. In addition, learning and performance orientation moderated the relationships between performance levels during the learning process and subsequent expectancy and valence.

The increasing complexity of the tasks to be performed in the future workplace is likely to place an even greater emphasis on training programs, as evidenced by the fact that no less than six chapters in Howard's (1995) The Changing Nature of Work noted the need for improved training approaches. The increased emphasis on training will necessitate a commitment on the part of selection programs to provide motivated trainees. Such a commit- ment could create internal fit and synergy among human resources practices, potentially improving firm perfor- mance (Becker & Gerhart, 1996; Delery & Doty, 1996). One stream of literature that can inform practitioners in this area is the research on motivation to learn, defined here as a desire on the part of trainees to learn the content of the training program (Noe, 1986).

Research has consistently shown a positive relationship between motivation to learn and learning across a variety of settings, but several important gaps remain. For exam- ple, research linking dispositional personality variables to motivation to learn has been limited and often atheoretical. In addition, researchers have not adequately explored what types of learners continue to show high motivation

Jason A. Colquitt and Marcia J. Simmering, Eli Broad Gradu- ate School of Management, Michigan State University.

We wish to thank John R. Hollenbeck, Raymond A. Noe, Linn Van Dyne, and J. Kevin Ford for their helpful comments and suggestions on an earlier draft of this article. An earlier version of this article was presented at the 1997 meeting of the Academy of Management, Boston, Massachusetts.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jason A. Colquitt, Eli Broad Graduate School of Management, N475 North Business Complex, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan 48824. Electronic mail may be sent to colquitt @ pilot.msu.edu.

levels throughout the learning process, particularly in the wake of early difficulty (Fedor, 1991; Phillips & Gully, 1997). This issue is critical because many learners strug- gle during the early phases of the training program and often fall short of the objective performance goals that are typically used (Farr, Hofmann, & Ringenbach, 1993).

Our study addressed both of these gaps by integrating two personality variables, conscientiousness and goal ori- entation, with motivation to learn to assess their role in learning. We examined these issues in the context of a 6- week academic course, which provided an opportunity to study the learning process using a more comprehensive, longitudinal approach than many past investigations (e.g., Baldwin, Magjuka, & Loher, 1991; Martocchio & Web- ster, 1992; Phillips & Gully, 1997; Quinones, 1995). We gave a difficult but achievable performance goal to each learner as part of the course, matching the type of goal normally assigned to employees in training programs and organizational settings (Farr et al., 1993; Locke & La- tham, 1990). We then examined personality relationships at two time periods during the learning process: the begin- ning of the course and directly after learners were given feedback relating their current performance levels with their assigned goal (approximately halfway through the course). Gagne and Medsker (1996) noted that these two time periods are critical junctures in the learning process.

We cast conscientiousness and goal orientation as distal variables that influenced learning through the more proxi- mal mechanism of motivation to learn. Kanfer (1991) advocated using this distal-proximal framework for ex- amining personality effects because it places those effects in a more theoretical context (see also Judge & Martoc- chio, 1995). We predicted that feedback regarding one's performance levels vis-?a-vis the assigned goal would alter subsequent motivation levels. In addition, we examined

654

Page 2: Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to ...media.terry.uga.edu/socrates/publications/2013/06/ColquittSimmering1998.pdf · Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation

RESEARCH REPORTS 655

ExV

INITIAL MOTIVATION

TO LEARN

PERSONALITY VARIABLES Conscientiousness

Learning Orientation Performance Orientation

I ExV

w -('-~ Pre-Feedback E x V MOTIVATION Learning TO LEARN

PERFORMANCE FEEDBACK DELIVERED

I 3 WEEKS t I 3 WEEKS

~ Post-Feedback Lesming

Figure 1. Conceptual model illustrating the relationships among personality variables, motiva- tion to learn, and learning. E x V = Expectancy x Valence variables.

the degree to which learners' conscientiousness and goal orientation levels moderated these effects, which are sum- marized in our conceptual model presented in Figure 1.

The Relat ionship Be tween Conscient iousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to Learn

Noe (1986) presented a conceptual model detailing antecedents and outcomes associated with trainee motiva- tion to learn. The primary antecedents in Noe's model were job attitudes, reactions to feedback, contextual fac- tors, and individual perceptions of self-efficacy or expec- tancy (i.e., effort-performance contingencies). 1 In the decade since then, other researchers have argued that mo- tivation to learn should be examined from both an expec- tancy and a valence perspective, consistent with Vroom's (1964) expectancy theory (Baldwin & Karl, 1987; Ma- thieu, Tannenbaum, & Salas, 1992). This perspective as- serted that trainees can desire to learn the training content (i.e., have high levels of motivation to learn) for two reasons: (a) They see a relationship between their effort and actual learning progress (i.e., expectancy), and (b) the outcomes that can be attained from such progress are valued (i.e., valence).

Unfortunately, motivation to learn research has largely ignored this approach, particularly in terms of valence (e.g., Noe & Schmitt, 1986). Furthermore, even when Expectancy x Valence approaches have been used, only correlations with an overall summed index have been re- ported (Baldwin & Karl, 1987; Mathieu et al., 1992). As a result, studies that yielded correlations between motiva- tion to learn and attitudinal predictors (i.e., job involve- ment or organizational commitment) or situational pre- dictors (e.g., the provision of choice, the framing of the training assignment, or the presence of situational con- straints) were less able to empirically show why the corre-

lation was there (Baldwin et al., 1991; Hicks & Klimoski, 1987; Martocchio & Webster, 1992; Mathieu et al., 1992; Noe & Schmitt, 1986; Quinones, 1995; Ryman & Biersner, 1975; Tannenbaum, Mathieu, Salas, & Cannon- Bowers, 1991). An explicit treatment of relevant media- tors, lacking in the extant literature, can help answer the " w h y " questions that are a central component of good theory (Whetten, 1989).

Personality variables have also been overlooked in this research, except for locus of control, which has failed to yield significant correlations, particularly when ability is controlled for (Lied & Pritchard, 1976; Noe & Schmitt, 1986). Conspicuous by their absence are conscientious- ness, which "may be the most important trait-motivation variable in the work domain" (Barrick, Mount, & Strauss, 1993, p. 721 ), and goal orientation, a variable that holds untapped promise for training applications (Button, Ma- thieu, & Zajac, 1996; Farr et al., 1993). An Expectancy X Valence approach would be helpful in linking these personality variables to motivation to learn, as evidenced by similar efforts in the goal commitment literature (Gel- latly, 1996; Hollenbeck & Klein, 1987; Hollenbeck, Wil- liams, & Klein, 1989). Goal commitment researchers have

1Most researchers distinguish between self-efficacy (e.g., Bandura & Cervone, 1986) and expectancy (Vroom, 1964). Gist and Mitchell (1992) noted that expectancy is a probabilistic judgment regarding effort-performance contingencies that is driven by self-efficacy, a person' s estimate of his or her "capac- ity to orchestrate performance on a specific task" (p. 183). However, it is difficult to separate the two in terms of relation- ships to motivation to learn, possibly explaining why they were integrated in Noe's model (e.g., Noe & Schmitt, 1986). Simi- larly, our study discusses both expectancy and self-efficacy as examples of a larger expectancy component of motivational theories (e.g., Kanfer, 1991).

Page 3: Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to ...media.terry.uga.edu/socrates/publications/2013/06/ColquittSimmering1998.pdf · Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation

656 RESEARCH REPORTS

used Expectancy × Valence approaches for personality effects, thereby placing those effects in a more theoretical context and responding to past criticisms of personality research (Davis-Blake & Pfeffer, 1989; Judge & Martoc- chio, 1995; Kanfer, 1991). Thus, in our study, we examine how conscientiousness and goal orientation relate to moti- vation to learn through the mechanisms of expectancy and valence, as shown in Figure 1.2

Conscientiousness

Conscientiousness, a trait reflecting qualities such as being reliable, hardworking, self-disciplined, and perse- vering (McCrae & Costa, 1987), has been linked to a variety of positive work outcomes (Barrick & Mount, 1991). Meta-analytic evidence found conscientiousness to be a predictor of job performance across many occupa- tional groups, and other research has linked it to goal commitment and self-set goal setting (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Barrick et al., 1993; Gellatly, 1996). It is surprising that no published research has used conscientiousness as a predictor of motivation to learn. However, recent research suggests a link between conscientiousness and expec- tancy. Martocchio and Judge (1997) demonstrated a sig- nificant relationship between conscientiousness and self- efficacy in a computer software training setting. In a similar manner, Gellatly linked conscientiousness and ex- pectancy using an arithmetic task.

These results can be explained theoretically by examin- ing the antecedents of self-efficacy reviewed by Gist and Mitchell (1992). For example, the authors suggested that self-efficacy is driven in part by one's assessment of per- sonal resources and constraints. In reflecting on their per- sonal resources, conscientious individuals are likely to perceive that they are diligent and hardworking (McCrae & Costa, 1987) and should consider that a favor- able resource. Another antecedent of self-efficacy is an individual's analysis of task requirements. Conscientious individuals are more likely to accurately assess task re- quirements because of their tendency to be organized and systematic (McCrae & Costa, 1987) and should have more confidence in their ability to meet those require- ments given their traditionally high performance levels (Barrick & Mount, 1991).

There are also reasons to expect a positive relationship between conscientiousness and valence given that consci- entiousness subsumes the more specific dimension of need for achievement. Hollenbeck and Klein (1987) predicted a link between need for achievement and goal commitment because individuals with high levels of need for achieve- ment are more likely to value high performance. Similarly, conscientious individuals are more likely to set challeng- ing goals for themselves and stay committed to those goals (Barrick et al., 1993). However, although a conscientious- ness-valence linkage would be consistent with this re-

search, Gellatly (1996) failed to demonstrate such a rela- tionship. Thus, although existing research is equivocal, we propose the following:

Hypothesis 1. Conscientiousness will be positively related to motivation to learn, both initially and after performance feedback is given, an effect that will be mediated by expec- tancy and valence.

Goal Orientation

Goal orientation is a relativelystable dispositional vari- able that assumes two forms: (a) a learning orientation in which increasing competence by developing new skills is the focus and (b) a performance orientation in which demonstrating competence by meeting normative-based standards is deemed critical. The construct was originally derived from Dweck' s work with children in the education domain (e.g., Dweck, 1986, 1989). Her research detailed adaptive and maladaptive behavioral patterns that arose on the basis of children's beliefs about ability (Dweck, Hong, & Chiu, 1993). Entity theorists believed that ability levels were fixed, whereas incremental theorists believed that ability levels were malleable and could be increased. These implicit theories affect the kinds of goals individu- als choose to pursue, with entity theorists holding strong performance goals and weak learning goals and incremen- tal theorists showing the opposite tendency.

Much of the goal orientation research in the education literature has used child samples (e.g., Elliott & Dweck, 1988; Seifert, 1995; Stipek & Gralinski, 1996). The extent to which the findings of these studies generalize to adults is still at issue, but, in general, adult samples have shown convergent results (e.g., Archer, 1994; Bouffard, Boisvert, Vezeau, & Larouche, 1995; Duda & Nicholls, 1992; El- liot & Harackiewicz, 1994; Greene & Miller, 1996; Har- ackiewicz & Elliot, 1993; Schraw, Horn, Thorndike- Christ, & Bruning, 1995). Only recently has goal orienta- tion become a focus of organizational researchers, but most results have supported previous research (e.g., But- ton et al., 1996; Fisher & Ford, in press; Hofmann, 1993; Phillips & Gully, 1997).

Button et al. noted that goal orientation holds great

2 Vroom's (1964) conceptualization of valence differentiated the valence of the first-order outcome, performance, and second- order outcomes tied to performance (e.g., pay or promotions). In an academic setting, the distinction between first- and second- order outcomes is less clear, given that grades are outcomes received because of performance and are direct measures of performance itself. Thus, we referenced valence toward getting a good grade, doing well in the course, and achieving success in the class. Vroom also posited that valence and expectancy are independent, a stance that contrasted with Atkinson and Birch's (1964) theory of achievement motivation that suggested that more difficult tasks have higher valences.

Page 4: Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to ...media.terry.uga.edu/socrates/publications/2013/06/ColquittSimmering1998.pdf · Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation

RESEARCH REPORTS 657

promise for organizational applications and provided sev- eral recommendations for future research. First, they pro- vided factor analytic evidence suggesting that goal orien- tation is best conceptualized as two independent dimen- sions (learning and performance orientation) rather than a unidimensional continuum. That is, individuals can be simultaneously high or low on both learning and perfor- mance orientations, contrary to the conceptualization of Dweck and her colleagues (e.g., Dweck, 1986, 1989). The extent to which past findings in this area can be generalized to the two-dimensional case is as yet un- known, although early research is consistent with Dweck's predictions (Button et al., 1996; Phillips & Gully, 1997). Finally, Button et al. further argued that individuals have dispositional goal orientations that pre- dispose them to react to situations in specific ways but that situational cues can impact those predispositions. Thus, the construct has both trait and state qualities, sug- gesting that test-retest reliabilities may be lower than those of other trait measures such as conscientiousness (Button et al., 1996; DeShon & Fisher, 1996).

Because of the implicit theories from which they are derived, there are reasons to expect relationships between both goal orientations and expectancy. The higher one's performance orientation, the more one should react to difficult tasks with doubts about ability levels (e.g., E1- liott & Dweck, 1988). This relationship would explain the finding that highly performance-oriented individuals often avoid difficult tasks in favor of more achievable ones (Dweck & Leggett, 1988). In contrast, the stronger one's learning orientation, the more one should continue to believe that ability and skill can be increased to achieve success (e.g., Dweck, 1986, 1989). This connection sug- gests a positive relationship between learning orientation and expectancy and a negative relationship between per- formance orientation and expectancy. Indeed, Phillips and Gully (1997) recently demonstrated both of these pre- dicted relationships in the context of an undergraduate course.

There are also reasons to expect relationships between both goal orientations and valence. Highly learning-ori- ented individuals are more likely to seek challenges and view high performance as indicative of increased mastery (Dweck & Leggett, 1988). One would therefore expect that the more learning oriented a person is, the more he or she would place value on high performance levels. In contrast, Elliott and Dweck (1988) noted that, when performance orientations are highlighted, individuals avoid difficult tasks in favor of moderate ones (when perceived ability is high) or easy ones (when perceived ability is low). This finding suggests that, because the learners in our sample were given difficult goals, perfor- mance orientation should be negatively related to valence. Performance-oriented individuals also respond to difficult tasks with negative affect (Dweck & Leggett, 1988; El-

liott & Dweck, 1988), which would also suggest a nega- tive relationship with valence given that valence is based on anticipated satisfaction. Therefore,

Hypothesis 2. Learning orientation will be positively re- lated to motivation to learn, both initially and after perfor- mance feedback is given, an effect that will be mediated by expectancy and valence.

Hypothesis 3. Performance orientation will be negatively related to motivation to learn, both initially and after perfor- mance feedback is given, an effect that will be mediated by expectancy and valence.

Trait Variables as Moderators of Reactions to P e r f o r m a n c e Levels

Not all learners succeed in every facet of the learning process--many experience some difficulty in learning the material (Farr et al., 1993). Whether learners internally perceive this difficulty or receive feedback regarding it, reactions to performance levels are likely to have im- portant motivational consequences (Ilgen, Fisher, & Tay- lor, 1979; Kluger & DeNisi, 1996; Phillips, Hollenbeck, & Ilgen, 1996; Podsakoff & Farh, 1989; Taylor, Fisher, & Ilgen, 1984). It is important to understand these conse- quences to maximize the effectiveness of goal-directed learning settings. In general, researchers have shown that low performance levels decrease expectancy and self-ef- ficacy levels (e.g., Bandura & Cervone, 1986; Gist & Mitchell, 1992). Relationships with valence may also be negative, but they depend on complex cognitive and self- reactive influences such as self-satisfaction, attribution, and referent comparison (Bandura, 1991).

In our study, we gave learners feedback halfway through the course that related their current performance to their assigned goal. Gagne and Medsker (1996) empha- sized the importance of giving feedback during the learn- ing process, identifying it as an instructional event that facilitates learning. We were interested in learners' moti- vation in the wake of this feedback, in particular how those effects differed according to learners' levels of con- scientiousness and goal orientation. Fedor (1991) noted that empirical research addressing the role played by per- sonality in responding to performance feedback is needed.

Conscientiousness

Martocchio and Judge (1997) noted that conscientious individuals have a tendency to engage in self-deception, a notion also supported by Barrick and Mount (1996). That is, conscientious individuals are more likely to be- lieve that they are doing better than they truly are. This self-deception should prevent large decreases in expec- tancy perceptions because such individuals may refuse to accept the fact that poor performance levels are indicative of true ability. Indeed, self-deceivers often reach high

Page 5: Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to ...media.terry.uga.edu/socrates/publications/2013/06/ColquittSimmering1998.pdf · Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation

658 RESEARCH REPORTS

achievement levels for this reason (Martocchio & Judge, 1997). Also, conscientious individuals, who are by defini- tion persevering and disciplined (McCrae & Costa, 1987), should refuse to give up in the face of difficulty. Indeed, they should continue to hold high perceptions of valence given their tendency to commit to assigned goals (Barrick et al., 1993). Thus, we predict the pattern shown in Figure 2 and posit the following:

Hypothesis 4. The relationship between performance levels at the time feedback is given and subsequent (a) expectancy and (b) valence will be moderated by conscientiousness, such that lower performance will be less associated with lower expectancy or valence for highly conscientious learners.

Goal Orientation

Goal orientation is often discussed in the context of responses to feedback regarding performance (e.g., Dweck, 1986, 1989; Farr et al., 1993). Research has sug- gested that learning-oriented individuals should be less likely to respond to poor progress during learning with lowered expectancy. Button et al. (1996) demonstrated that the more learning oriented individuals are, the more they subscribe to the incremental theorists' position that ability levels can be increased to correct poor progress. Such beliefs should buffer these individuals against large decreases in expectancy (Bobko & Colella, 1994). More- over, individuals with strong learning orientations are more likely to engage in problem solving and altering of strategies when they encounter low performance levels (Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Elliott & Dweck, 1988). In contrast, the more performance oriented people are, the more they believe the entity theorists' position that ability is fixed (Button et al., 1996). Thus, these individuals should meet difficulty with marked decreases in expec-

- 7

13-

- 6

-O

-4

-3

-2

-1

I I HIGH LOW

Pre-Feedback Learning Levels

High Conscientiousness High Learning Orientation

Low Performance Orientation

Low Conscientiousness Low Learning Orientation

High Performance Orientation

Figure 2. Predicted patterns of Personality × Learning interac- tions for subsequent expectancy and valence.

tancy as they shift their focus to ability inadequacies and off-task thoughts (Bobko & Colella, 1994).

In terms of valence, research has shown that learning- oriented individuals react to challenges with positive af- fect, pride, and intrinsic motivation (Dweck & Leggett, 1988). The more challenging a task becomes, the more it is perceived as an opportunity to build competence. This relationship suggests that highly learning-oriented indi- viduals should be buffered against the tendency to associ- ate poor performance with lower valence. Indeed, such individuals may find themselves immersed in the task be- cause of increased cognitive engagement (Duda & Ni- cholls, 1992; Greene & Miller, 1996). Performance-ori- ented individuals, on the other hand, traditionally do not react to poor progress in a constructive manner. Indeed, Fisher and Ford (in press) showed a link between perfor- mance orientation and off-task attention, and Dweck and Leggett (1988) related performance orientation to anxiety and negative affect. Such results suggest that these learn- ers lack self-leadership skills (Manz, 1986) because they focus on unpleasant aspects of the task (e.g., pressure or uncertainty) rather than pleasant ones (e.g., challenge or learning). Thus, the more difficulty one experiences, the more the desire to perform well should decline. We there- fore predict the pattern shown in Figure 2 and posit the following:

Hypothesis 5. The relationship between performance at the time feedback is given and subsequent (a) expectancy and (b) valence will be moderated by learning orientation, such that lower performance will be less associated with lower expectancy or valence for highly learning-oriented learners.

Hypothesis 6. The relationship between performance at the time feedback is given and subsequent (a) expectancy and (b) valence will be moderated by performance orientation, such that lower performance will be less associated with lower expectancy or valence for less performance-oriented learners.

The Relat ionship Between Motivation to Learn and Learning

Linking trait variables to motivation to learn is only valuable to the degree that motivation levels are connected to actual learning differences. Fortunately, the relationship between motivation to learn and learning has been robust, predicting both cognitive and skill-based outcomes (e.g., Baldwin et al., 1991; Martocchio & Webster, 1992; Ma- thieu et al., 1992; Noe & Schmitt, 1986; Quinones, 1995; Tannenbaum et al., 1991). On the basis of this research,

Hypothesis 7. Initial motivation to learn will be positively related to prefeedback learning; postfeedback motivation to learn will be positively related to postfeedback learning.

Method

Sample

The participants were 103 undergraduates enrolled in two sections of a management course. The 6-week course was the

Page 6: Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to ...media.terry.uga.edu/socrates/publications/2013/06/ColquittSimmering1998.pdf · Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation

RESEARCH REPORTS 659

learning program, with performance at the midpoint of the course (i.e., when performance feedback was given) and perfor- mance during the remainder of the course serving as the learning measures. This paradigm has been used successfully by re- searchers in the areas of motivation to learn and goal setting (Hollenbeck, Williams, & Klein, 1989; Mathieu et al., 1992; Phillips & Gully, 1997). Hollenbeck et al. (1989) examined goal commitment in an undergraduate management course, Phil- lips and Gully examined self-set goal setting in a similar setting, and Mathieu et al. examined motivation to learn in an undergrad- uate bowling course. Participation was voluntary, and we awarded extra credit points (6% of the total points) for involve- ment. All students participated.

P r o c e d u r e

The authors served as instructors for the classes included in the sample. Each author served as experimenter for the other author's class; thus, no author collected data, had access to data, or even discussed the study with his or her own students. We asked students to participate in a term-long study involving personality variables, and we informed them that they would be asked to provide some academic information and to complete several surveys. We gave the students feedback on the correlates of various personality variables and told them how such infor- mation could be used in the workplace during the final week of the course.

Given that reactions to learning levels were to occur as a result of performance feedback, we had to create an objective achievement standard, which we did by assigning each student a goal for class performance. We collected the current cumulative grade point average (GPA) for each participant and used it to create a grade goal on the basis of the method used by Hollen- beck, Williams, & Klein (1989). Specifically, we added .25 points to each participant's GPA, and the resulting GPA was then expressed as a percentage of total points on the basis of the course's grading scale. For example, a student with a cumu- lative GPA of 3.25 was given a GPA goal of 3.5, which was expressed as a grade goal of 86%. 3 Thus, these goals represented a difficult but achievable goal for student learning (Hollenbeck et al., 1989). We distributed grade goals to the students during the second class period in sealed envelopes marked with identi- fication numbers. Each participant received the following expla- nation with the grade goal:

Goal setting theory suggests that people perform well when they are assigned difficult and specific goals. Research has found that a difficult goal for a student's performance in a course is approximately .25 points higher than his or her cumulative grade point average. Based on your grade point average, we have given you a difficult and specific goal for this course.

Your goal for a grade in this course is: _ _ %

Please note that this goal will in no way prevent you from attaining a higher grade in this course. It is not an indication of the grade you are capable of receiving, only a guideline that you can use to track your performance. Your instructor has no knowledge of your goal, and achievement of the goal (or lack of achievement) will have no bearing on your grade whatsoever. Good luck this semester!

After receiving the grade goal, each student completed the first of two in-class surveys measuring expectancy, valence, and motivation to learn. We measured goal orientation and goal commitment (used as a control variable) concomitantly, and we also assessed goal orientation at the conclusion of the term. Note that we examined goal orientation as a dispositional trait, one that, like conscientiousness, could be used in selection bat- teries. However, goal orientation has been shown to have situa- tional variability (Button et al., 1996; Dweck, 1975). Assessing goal orientation three separate times allowed us to account for this variance source. Specifically, we measured dispositional goal orientation by averaging the initial, midpoint, and conclu- sion scores. This procedure provided us with one disposition score because it collapsed across the contaminating influence of situational goal orientation variance, which was outside the scope of our study. The average correlation among the three learning orientation scores was .62; the average correlation among the three performance orientation scores was .60. This level is consistent with other uses of Button et al.'s measure (DeShon & Fisher, 1996).

The first course examination was given 2 weeks (four class periods) after students received their grade goals. We recorded performance on this examination in percentage terms, and parti- cipants received performance feedback during the class period immediately following the exam. This feedback read as follows: "Your grade goal at the beginning of the semester was a %. Your current grade in the course is a %. That means you are currently % (above/below) your grade goal." During the subsequent class meeting, students completed the second in- class survey, which again assessed expectancy, valence, motiva- tion to learn, goal orientation, and goal commitment.

We gave the final examination for the course 3 weeks (five class periods) after the administration of the grade goal feed- back, and we used it to assess learning at the conclusion of the learning process. We gave the final goal orientation measure at the beginning of this class period. We also gave participants a separate out-of-class survey that assessed conscientiousness and asked them to complete the scale on their own time. We collected all scales at least 2 weeks before the end of the course.

M e a s u r e s

Motivation to learn. We used three items adapted from Noe and Schmitt (:1986) to measure motivation to learn. One exam- ple included, "I will exert considerable effort in learning this material" ( 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree; as = .75 initially and .83 postfeedback).

Expectancy. We used both expectancy-based and self-effi- cacy-based measures to tap this construct. The expectancy mea- sure consisted of two items from Lawler and Suttle (1973). We asked participants to indicate how true it was that the first phrase of a pair leads to the second phrase of a pair, for them personally, in the context of the course. Pairs included, (a) "trying hard --*

3 Eight students had GPAs of 3.76 of higher, which created a ceiling for their grade goals because students could not earn more than 100% in the course. Thus, all analyses were run with and without this group. In no cases did the results differ in either significance level or effect size.

Page 7: Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to ...media.terry.uga.edu/socrates/publications/2013/06/ColquittSimmering1998.pdf · Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation

660 RESEARCH REPORTS

learning the material" and (b) "putting forth effort --* gaining an understanding of the material." We assessed self-efficacy with three items from Mclntire and Levine (1984). One exam- ple included, " I do well in activities where I have to remember lots of information ' '4 (1 = never to 7 = always; as = .66 initially and .70 postfeedback).

Valence. We used three items adapted from Lawler and Sut- tle (1973) to assess valence. We asked participants to indicate "how desirable" (in the context of the course) they considered (a) "getting a good grade," (b) "doing well in the course," and (c) "achieving success in the class" to be ( 1 = very unde- sirable to 7 = very desirable; as = .63 initially and .64 postfeedback).

Conscientiousness. We used the 12-item scale from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory, a shorter version of the NEO Per- sonality Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992), to measure consci- entiousness (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree; a = .79 ). Costa and McCrae (1992) reported test-retest reliabilities of .83 for this scale, suggesting that the variable has a definite trait quality.

Goal orientation. We used Button et al.'s (1996) eight-item dispositional goal orientation measures. Learning orientation items included, "I prefer to work on tasks that force me to learn new things" and "The opportunity to do challenging work is important to me." Performance orientation items included, "Once I find the right way to do something, I stick to i t" and "The things I enjoy the most are the things I do best." (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree; as = .83 for leaming orientation and .80 for performance orientation). Button et al. provided reliability and construct validity evidence for these scales. We computed coefficient alphas by treating each of the three learning orientation and performance orientation scores (i.e., initial, midpoint, and conclusion) as one item in a three- item scale.

Prefeedback learning. We assessed prefeedback learning through the first course examination. This examination consisted of 60 items (100 points) that assessed declarative knowledge.

Postfeedback learning. We assessed postfeedback learning through the final course examination, which again consisted of 60 items (100 points) that assessed declarative knowledge.

Control variables. Three control variables were used. GPA served as a control to ensure that performance or motivation variance due to ability could be captured. This control was especially critical because the .25-point increase in grade seemed more difficult to learners with low GPAs. We also coded class membership because it was likely a contaminant that ex- plained performance and motivation variance due to nonrandom assignment of students to classes. A score of 0 was given to students in Jason A. Colquitt's class; a score of 1 was given to students in Marcia J. Simmering's class. Finally, we controlled for goal commitment to statistically ensure that all learners were equally committed to their assigned goals, given that such com- mitment should relate to performance and motivation, particu- larly after feedback is delivered. Indeed, Bobko and Colella (1994) noted that reactions to performance standards should depend on the degree to which those standards are accepted by the individual. Thus, we used Hollenbeck, Klein, O'Leary, and Wright's (1989) four-item goal commitment scale ( 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree; as = .69 initially and .69

postfeedback), and it should be noted that commitment levels were generally high (M = 5.11 out of 7; SD = 1.02).

Re su l t s

Descr ip t ive Statistics

The means, s tandard deviations, and zero-order inter- correlat ions of all variables are repor ted in Table 1. Coef- ficient alphas are shown in the diagonal of the correlat ion matrix. Several correlat ions of variables never included together in a single study are notable. For example , consci- entiousness was posit ively associated with learning orien- tation levels and negatively associated with performance orientation levels. Goal commitment was posit ively re- lated to motivation to learn, as well as valence and expec- tancy. Finally, conscient iousness had a significant posit ive correlat ion with goal commitment and learning, whereas performance orientation was negatively corre la ted with learning (pos t feedback) .

Tests o f Hypotheses

Hypotheses 1 - 3 predicted that the relat ionships be- tween the personal i ty variables and motivation to learn would be mediated by valence and expectancy. The results are shown in Table 2. Al l three personal i ty variables ex- pla ined significant incremental variance in motivation to learn beyond the effects of the control variables. Consis- tent with our predict ions, the personal i ty variables ex- p la ined less variance in motivation to learn when the Ex- pectancy X Valence variables were statist ically controlled. However, conscient iousness and learning orientation still explained significant incremental variance, suggesting that their relat ionships with motivation to learn were only part ial ly mediated. 5 The direct ions of these relat ionships were all as predic ted (see Table 1): Conscient iousness and learning orientation were posit ively correlated with expectancy, valence, and motivation to learn, and perfor- mance orientation was negatively correlated with expec- tancy and motivation to learn. Contrary to predict ions, performance orientation was unrelated to valence.

To complement the above analyses, we examined the

4 We conducted a principal components factor analysis with varimax rotation to test the unidimensionality of the larger ex- pectancy variable. A one-factor solution fit the data, suggesting that the expectancy and self-efficacy items could in fact be combined.

5 The fact that the relationships between conscientiousness and goal orientation and motivation to learn were only partially mediated by Expectancy x Valence variables suggests that sig- nificant correlations with motivation to learn are not simply a function of common method variance. Presumably, controlling for other self-report variables would remove variance due to common methods and same-source bias.

Page 8: Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to ...media.terry.uga.edu/socrates/publications/2013/06/ColquittSimmering1998.pdf · Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation

RESEARCH REPORTS 661

t ,~

00

r---

o l

r.~

;>

I

. - . . . . = . ~ I

I I

I

~ 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1

. . . . . ~ . . ~ . . . ~ 1 I

. . . . . . . . . . . ~ .

~ - - ~ . ~ . ~

.,= ~ ~= ~.=

o= ~ =~.~ ~ ~ - ~ ~<~

"a

II

=<

e! o

rl

o

H

o

.,= II

g

o

i g

ca,

I

II ~ > .

,~2~v 0

relationship between the three personality variables (en- tered simultaneously) and motivation to learn. Personality variables explained an incremental 28% of the variance in prefeedback motivation to learn (p < .001 ), with both conscientiousness (/~ = .33, p < .001 ) and learning orien- tation (/~ = .38, p < .001 ) having significant independent relationships. Personality variables explained an incre- mental 27% of the variance in postfeedback motivation to learn (p < .001 ), again with both conscientiousness (fl = .28, p < .01 ) and learning orientation (/~ = .40, p < .001 ) having significant independent relationships. These results have practical significance in addition to statistical significance. To express this, we classified learners into positive and negative trait patterns, where a positive pat- tern represented high learning orientation, high conscien- tiousness, and low performance orientation, and expressed the correlation between this pattern and motivation to learn using a binomial effects size display (Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1991). Compared with learners with a negative pattern, learners with a positive pattern were 38% more likely to be motivated before feedback and 40% more likely to be motivated after feedback. They were also 30% more likely to perform well on the first exam and 28% more likely to perform well on the second. 6

Hypotheses 4 - 6 predicted interactions among the per- sonality variables and prefeedback learning levels for postfeedback expectancy and valence. We assessed these hypotheses using moderated multiple regression; results are presented in Table 3. No interactions were demon- strated for conscientiousness. However, interactions were demonstrated for learning orientation with postfeedback expectancy and performance orientation with postfeed- back valence, with the pattern matching that shown in Figure 2.

Hypothesis 7 predicted that motivation to learn would be positively related to pre- and postfeedback learning. Initial motivation to learn explained an additional 2% of the variance in prefeedback learning (/~ = .14, p < .05) beyond the three control variables, which accounted for 45% of the learning variance. Postfeedback motivation to learn added 4% of the variance explained for postfeedback

6 Rosenthal and Rosnow (1991) expressed practical signifi- cance by putting a criterion into dichotomous terms such as "perform well" or "perform poorly" and "motivated" or "un- motivated" using median splits. This convention is applied here because it is congruent with the context of many training pro- grams in which some cutoff must be achieved to fulfill a require- ment or move on to a subsequent course. Relationships with this dichotomous criterion are then expressed in percentage terms, where the percentages are calculated by 50% _+ r/2. Thus, the correlation between our positive trait pattern and pre- feedback learning of r = .30 translates into 65% (50 + 15) and 35% (50 - 15), or a 30% difference (65 - 35) in the likelihood of performing well based on trait patterns.

Page 9: Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to ...media.terry.uga.edu/socrates/publications/2013/06/ColquittSimmering1998.pdf · Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation

662 RESEARCH REPORTS

Table 2

Variance Explained in Motivation to Learn by Personality Variables

Personality variable

Initial motivation to learn (DV) Postfeedback motivation to learn (DV)

Learning Performance Learning Performance Predictor Conscientious oriented oriented Conscientious oriented oriented

1. Class a .00 .00 .00 .02 .02 .02 2. GPA a .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 3. Goal commitment" .15"** .15"** .15"** .11 ** .11"* .11 ** 4. Personality variable .28*** .35*** .17* .10"* .20*** .04* Controlling for E × V

1. Class" .00 .00 .00 .02 .02 .02 2. GPAa .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 3. Goal commitment ~ .15" * * .15"** .15 *** .11 ** .11 * * .11 ** 4. Expectancy .05** .05** .05** .02 .02 .02 5. Valence .10"** .10"** .10"** .22*** .22*** .22*** 6. Personality variable .08** .13*** .02 .05** .09*** .02

Note. Values represent AR2s. N = 103. All equations had an overall F-statistic significance o f p < .001. GPA = cumulative grade point average; E × V = Expectancy × Valence; DV = dependent variable. "Control variables. * p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

learning (/3 = .13, p < .05) beyond the three control variables, which accounted for 54% of the learning vari- ance. Thus Hypothesis 7 was supported. The fact that the hypothesis was supported despite the marginal signifi- cance of the motivation-to-learn correlations in Table 1 illustrates the importance of the control variables.

Table 3 Regression Results for Moderation Hypotheses

Personality variable

Learning Performance Predictor Conscientious oriented oriented

DV = Postfeedback expectancy a

1. Class h ,00 .00 .00 2. GPA b .04* .04* .04* 3. Goal commitment b .04" .04* .04" 4. Initial expectancy .30"** .30"** .30*** 5. Personality variable ,01 .02 .00 6. Learning .04* .03* .03* 7. Personality × Learning .01 .04** .00

DV = Postfeedback valence ~

1. Class ~ .00 .00 .00 2. GPA b .00 .00 .00 3. Goal commitment b .03 .03 .03 4. Initial valence .53*** .53*** .53*** 5. Personality variable .01 .00 .00 6. Learning .00 .00 .00 7. Personality × Learning .00 .00 .02*

Note. Values represent ARes. N = 103. All equations had an overall F-statistic significance of p < .001. GPA = cumulative grade point average; DV = dependent variable. "Hypotheses 4, 5, and 6--expectancy. b Control variables, c Hy- potheses 4, 5, and 6--valence. * p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Discuss ion

The positive relationships of learning orientation and conscientiousness with motivation to learn stand out as the most important contributions of this study. Learners who had high levels of these personality variables exhib- ited higher motivation levels, both initially and in the wake of feedback during the learning process. Furthermore, we elucidated the underlying mechanisms for these effects using an Expectancy x Valence framework, placing these relationships in a more established theoretical context. Specifically, the relationships of conscientiousness and motivation to learn were partially mediated by expectancy and valence. In other words, individuals who were reli- able, self-disciplined, and persevering were more likely to perceive a link between effort and performance and were more likely to value high performance levels. These results therefore complement past research by identifying mechanisms for positive manifestations of this personality variable (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Barrick et al., 1993).

Learning orientation was also related to motivation- to-learn levels, with highly learning-oriented individuals exhibiting higher expectancy and valence levels. In con- trast, performance orientation was negatively related to motivation to learn by means of expectancy. Thus, this study is among the first to demonstrate a significant link between goal orientation and two of the most visible moti- vational constructs, expectancy and valence. Our work therefore places goal orientation (and conscientiousness) in a larger, metatheoretical framework of Expectancy × Valence theories, allowing their relationships to be com- pared and contrasted with those of other trait and motiva- tion variables (Kanfer, 1991).

Page 10: Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to ...media.terry.uga.edu/socrates/publications/2013/06/ColquittSimmering1998.pdf · Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation

RESEARCH REPORTS 663

Besides predicting motivation levels, the personality variables we examined also moderated individuals' re- sponses to performance levels during the learning process. Although the role of personality in reacting to perfor- mance levels and feedback has been much discussed, em- pirical work is lacking (Fedor, 1991). Our work begins to fill this gap by showing that the tendency for low perfor- mance levels to be associated with lower expectancy lev- els was less prevalent for highly learning-oriented individ- uals. This finding is consistent with past goal orientation research suggesting that learning orientations buffer learn- ers against the negative effects of early difficulty (Button et al., 1996; Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Elliott & Dweck, 1988). Furthermore, the tendency for low performance to be associated with lower valence was not as prevalent for less performance-oriented individuals, an effect that has not been demonstrated in past research. Thus, in situations in which difficulty in mastering training content is ex- pected, our results suggest that highly learning-oriented individuals (and less performance-oriented individuals) should remain motivated. We observed no interactions for conscientiousness; rather, its effects were uniformly positive across performance levels.

Taken together, these results have many practical impli- cations. As noted above, learners with a trait pattern that was highly conscientious, highly learning oriented, and less performance oriented were 30% more likely to per- form well on the first exam and 28% more likely to per- form well on the second using Rosenthal and Rosnow's (1991) convention. Because conscientiousness is used in many selection batteries and has shown considerable sta- bility (test-retest r = .83; Costa & McCrae, 1992), infor- mation about the conscientiousness of learners could be a valuable component of the person analysis phase of needs assessment (Goldstein, 1991). Goal orientation measures could be used in a similar fashion, or the training context could be manipulated to enhance one orientation or the other. Dweck (1975) and Kraiger, Ford, and Salas (1993) noted that situational cues can affect a learner's orientation at a given point in time, giving a state compo- nent to the construct (Button et al., 1996; DeShon & Fisher, 1996). Thus, in situations in which training materi- als are particularly complex and learning difficulty is ex- pected, conscientious and learning-oriented individuals could be selected for training, or a learning-oriented em- phasis could be created by illustrating task experimenta- tion and exploration of multiple learning strategies. In situations in which conscientious individuals are lacking or a learning-oriented emphasis is impossible, training may need to be lengthened and learning rate slowed so students can avoid early difficulty. Also, given the lower valence levels among such individuals, external incentives may be necessary to improve their motivation to learn.

A potential limitation of this study is that the learning occurred in a classroom setting rather than in an actual

organizational training environment. However, because we were interested in the effects of personality variables in an achievement-oriented setting, the classroom did af- ford a high degree of fidelity. Specifically, Farr et al. (1993) noted that most of the goals advanced in organiza- tions are normative performance goals. Such goals are also most frequently used in training programs, which often culminate in objectively scored achievement tests. Nonetheless, future research is needed to replicate these findings in other types of learning environments, including those in which explicit rewards are tied to goal achieve- ment. Although many goals in organizational settings are only loosely tied to objective rewards, the presence of such rewards could increase the motivational ramifications of outcome valence. In the current study, learners received no specific awards for goal achievement. However, to the extent that awards increase goal commitment (e,g., Hol- lenbeck & Klein, 1987), controlling for goal commitment in all our analyses suggests that our results may be gener- alizable to such contexts.

In addition, researchers should replicate these findings with other types of goals (e.g., mastery goals) and with other forms of feedback. Fedor ( 1991 ) notes that feedback is multidimensional, including facets such as sign, accu- racy, timing, quantity, and specificity. To this we would add that feedback can be subjective or objective, process or outcome oriented, task or person generated, interactionally fair or unfair (Bies & Moag, 1986), written or oral, individ- ual or group level, or surprising or expected. Fedor notes that individual differences affect preferences for certain dimensions of feedback. It may be that personality variables moderate reactions to feedback differently according to these dimensions. For example, learning orientation may be a more significant moderator when process feedback is given or when feedback is task generated. Conscientious- ness may be more necessary when subjective, interac- tionally unfair feedback is given. Performance orientation may be more deleterious to individuals who receive nega- tive feedback when it is surprising to them. Moreover, our results may become less significant when feedback is gen- eral, brief, delayed, or inaccurate because the information needed to alter behavior is not provided. These types of feedback occur across a variety of training settings, de- pending on whether procedural or declarative knowledge is emphasized (Kraiger et al., 1993), whether tasks amena- ble to objective feedback are used, and whether trainers are experienced in feedback delivery. Future research on personality effects in these domains is warranted.

References

Archer, J. (1994). Achievement goals as a measure of motivation in university students. Contemporary Educational Psychol- ogy, 19, 430-446.

Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. (1964). An introduction to motiva- tion. Princeton, NJ: Van Norstrand.

Page 11: Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to ...media.terry.uga.edu/socrates/publications/2013/06/ColquittSimmering1998.pdf · Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation

664 RESEARCH REPORTS

Baldwin, T.T., & Karl, K. (1987, August). The development and empirical test of a measure for assessing motivation to learn in management education. Paper presented at the 47th Annual Meeting of the Academy of Management, New Or- leans, LA.

Baldwin, T. T., Magjuka, R. J., & Loher, B. T. ( 1991 ). The perils of participation: Effects of learner choice on motivation and learning. Personnel Psychology, 44, 51-66.

Bandura, A. ( 1991 ). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 248-287.

Bandura, A., & Cervone, D. (1986). Differential engagement of self-reactive influences in cognitive motivation. Organiza- tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 38, 92-113.

Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. ( 1991 ). The Big Five personal- ity dimensions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Person- nel Psychology, 44, 1-26.

Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1996). Effects of impression management and self-deception on the predictive validity of personality constructs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 500-509.

Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Strauss, J. P. (1993). Consci- entiousness and performance of sales representatives: Test of the mediation effects of goal setting. Journal of Applied Psy- chology, 78, 715-722.

Becket, B., & Gerhart, B. (1996). The impact of human resource management on organizational performance: Progress and prospects. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 779-801.

Bies, R. J., & Moag, J. E (1986). Interactional justice: Commu- nication criteria of fairness. In R.J. Lewicki, B.H. Shep- pard, & M. H. Bazerman (Eds.), Research on negotiations in organizations (Vol. 1, pp. 43-55) . Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Bobko, E, & Colella, A. (1994). Employee reactions to perfor- mance standards: A review and research propositions. Person- nel Psychology, 47, 1-29.

Bouffard, T., Boisvert, J., Vezeau, C., & Larouche, C. (1995). The impact of goal orientation on self-regulation and perfor- mance among college students. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 317-329.

Button, S. B., Mathieu, J. E., & Zajac, D. M. (1996). Goal ori- entation in organizational research: A conceptual and empiri- cal foundation. Organizational Behavior and Human Deci- sion Processes, 67, 26-48.

Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). The NEO PI-Rprofes- sional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Davis-Blake, A., & Pfeffer, J. (1989). Just a mirage: The search for dispositional effects in organizational research. Academy of Management Review, 14, 385-400.

Delery, J.E., & Doty, D.H. (1996). Modes of theorizing in strategic human resource management: Tests of universalistic, contingency, and configurational performance predictions. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 802-835.

DeShon, R. P., & Fisher, S. L. ( 1996, April). Goal orientation: Weaving the nomological net. Symposium conducted at the 12th Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, St. Louis, MO.

Duda, J. L,, & Nicholls, J. G. (1992). Dimensions of achieve-

ment motivation in schoolwork and sport. Journal of Educa- tional Psychology, 84, 290-299.

Dweck, C. S. (1975). The role of expectations and attributions in the alleviation of learned helplessness. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 31, 674-685.

Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41, 1040-1048.

Dweck, C, S. (1989). Motivation. In A. Lesgold & R. Glaser (Eds.), Foundations for a psychology of education (pp. 87- 136). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Dweck, C. S., Hong, Y., & Chiu, C. (1993). Implicit theories: Individual differences in the likelihood and meaning of dispo- sitional inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- tin, 19, 644-656.

Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive ap- proach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256-273.

Elliot, A.J., & Harackiewicz, J.M. (1994). Goal setting, achievement orientation, and intrinsic motivation: A media- tional analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 968-980.

Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and So- Cial Psychology, 54, 5-12.

Farr, J. L., Hofmann, D. A., & Ringenbach, K. L. (1993). Goal orientation and action control theory: Implications for indus- trial and organizational psychology. International Review of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 8, 193-232.

Fedor, D. B. ( 1991 ). Recipient responses to performance feed- back: A proposed model and its implications. In G.R. Ferris & K. M. Rowland (Eds.), Research in personnel and human resource management (Vol. 9, pp. 73-120). Green- wich, CT: JAI Press.

Fisher, S.L., & Ford, J. K. (in press). Differential effects of learner effort and goal orientation on two learning outcomes. Personnel Psychology.

Gagne, R.M., & Medsker, K.L. (1996). The conditions of learning: Training applications. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace.

Gellatly, I. R. (1996). Conscientiousness and task performance: Test of a cognitive process model. Journal of Applied Psy- chology, 81, 474-482.

Gist, M. E., & Mitchell, T. R. (1992). Self-efficacy: A theoreti- cal analysis of its determinants and malleability. Academy of Management Review, 17, 183-211.

Goldstein, I .L. (1991). Training in work organizations. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of indus- trial and organizational psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 507-620). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Greene, B. A., & Miller, R. B. (1996). Influences on achieve- ment: Goals, perceived ability, and cognitive engagement. Contemporary Educational Measurement, 21, 181 - 192.

Harackiewicz, J. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1993). Achievement goals and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 904-915.

Hicks, W. D., & Klimoski, R. (1987). The process of entering training programs and its effect on training outcomes. Acad- emy of Management Journal, 30, 542-552.

Hofmann, D. A. (1993). The influence of goal orientation on task performance: A substantively meaningful suppressor

Page 12: Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation to ...media.terry.uga.edu/socrates/publications/2013/06/ColquittSimmering1998.pdf · Conscientiousness, Goal Orientation, and Motivation

RESEARCH REPORTS 665

variable. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 1827- 1846.

Hollenbeck, J. R., & Klein, H. J. (1987). Goal commitment and the goal-setting process: Problems, prospects, and proposals for future research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 212- 220.

Hollenbeck, J. R., Klein, H. J., O'Leary, A. M., & Wright, P. M. (1989). Investigation of the construct validity of a self-report measure of goal commitment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 1-6.

Hollenbeck, J. R., Williams, C. R., & Klein, H. J. (1989). An empirical examination of the antecedents of commitment to difficult goals. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 18-23.

Howard, A. (1995). The changing nature of work. San Fran- cisco: Jossey-Bass.

Ilgen, D.R., Fisher, C.D., & Taylor, M.S. (1979). Conse- quences of individual feedback on behavior in organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 64, 349-371.

Judge, T. A., & Martocchio, J. J. ( 1995, May). Dispositions and work outcomes. Symposium conducted at the annual meeting of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Orlando, FL.

Kanfer, R. ( 1991 ). Motivation theory and industrial and organi- zational psychology. In M.D. Dunnette & L.M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychol- ogy (Vol. 1, pp. 75-170). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychol- ogists Press.

Kluger, A. N., & DeNisi, A. (1996). The effects of feedback interventions on performance: A historical review, a meta- analysis, and a preliminary feedback intervention theory. Psy- chological Bulletin, 119, 1-31.

Kraiger, K., Ford, J. K., & Salas, E. (1993). Application of cognitive, skill-based, and affective theories of learning out- comes to new methods of training evaluation. Journal of Ap- plied Psychology, 78, 311-328.

Lawler, E. E., & Suttle, J. L. (1973). Expectancy theory and job behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 9, 482-503.

Lied, T. R., & Pritchard, R. D. (1976). Relationships between personality variables and components of the expectancy- value model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 463-467.

Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Manz, C. C. (1986). Self-leadership: Toward an expanded the- ory of self-influence processes in organizations. Academy of Management Review, 11, 585-600.

Martocchio, J. J., & Judge, T. A. (1997). Relationship between conscientiousness and learning in employee training: Mediat- ing influences of self-deception and self-efficacy. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 764-773.

Martocchio, J.J., & Webster, J. (1992). Effects of feedback and cognitive playfulness on performance in microcomputer software training. Personnel Psychology, 45, 553-578.

Mathieu, J. E., Tannenbaum, S. I., & Salas, E. (1992). Influ- ences of individual and situational characteristics on measures of training effectiveness. Academy of Management Journal, 35, 828-847.

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, E T, Jr. (1987). Validation of the five- factor model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 81-90.

McIntire, S.M., & Levine, E.L. (1984). Task specific self- esteem: An empirical investigation. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 25, 290-303.

Noe, R. A. (1986). Trainee attributes and attitudes: Neglected influences on training effectiveness. Academy of Management Review, 11, 736-749.

Noe, R.A., & Schmitt, N. (1986). The influence of learner attitudes on training effectiveness: Test of a model. Personnel Psychology, 39, 497-523.

Phillips, J. M., & Gully, S. M. (1997). Role of goal orientation, ability, need for achievement, and locus of control in the self- efficacy and goal-setting process. Journal of Applied Psychol- ogy, 82, 792-802.

Phillips, J. M., Hollenbeck, J. R., & Ilgen, D. R. (1996). Preva- lence and prediction of positive discrepancy creation: Exam- ining a discrepancy between two self-regulation theories. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 498-511.

Podsakoff, P.M., & Farh, J.L. (1989). Effects of feedback sign and credibility on goal setting and task performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 44, 45-67.

Quinones, M. A. (1995). Pretraining context effects: Training assignment as feedback. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 226-238.

Rosenthal, R. & Rosnow, R. L. ( 1991 ). Essentials ofbehavioral research: Methods and data-analysis. New York: McGraw- Hill.

Ryman, D. H., & Biersner, R. J. (1975). Attitudes predictive of diving training success. Personnel Psychology, 28, 181 - 188.

Schraw, G., Horn, C., Thorndike-Christ, T., & Bruning, R. (1995). Academic goal orientations and student classroom achievement. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20, 359-368.

Seifert, T L. (1995). Academic goals and emotions: A test of two models. Journal of Psychology, 129, 543-552.

Stipek, S., & Gralinski, J. H. (1996). Children's beliefs about intelligence and school performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 397-407.

Tannenbaum, S. I., Mathieu, J. E., Salas, E., & Cannon-Bowers, J.A. (1991). Meeting trainees' expectations: The influence of training fulfillment on the development of commitment, self-efficacy, and motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 759-769.

Taylor, M. S., Fisher, C. D., & Ilgen, D. R. (1984). Individuals' reactions to performance feedback in organizations: A control theory perspective. In K. M. Rowland & G. R. Ferris (Eds.), Research in personnel and human resource management (Vol. 2, pp. 81-124). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley. Whetten, D. A. (1989). What constitutes a theoretical contribu-

tion? Academy of Management Review, 14, 490-495.

Received June 23, 1997 Revision received January 20, 1998

Accepted January 20, 1998 •