14
ORIGINAL PAPER Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum Tyler L. Renshaw Shane R. Jimerson Published online: 8 December 2011 Ó Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2011 Abstract Bullying is a salient challenge for children and schools around the world, appearing to be especially pre- valent at the middle-school level. Contemporary research reveals an exigent need for systematic programing that aims to reduce and prevent bullying via promoting awareness, attitudes, and skills that facilitate the develop- ment of positive peer relationships and positive school climates. Considering this need, the present quasi-experi- mental study examined the effects of a relatively new, brief, universal-level bullying prevention curriculum on middle-school students’ general attitudes toward bullying and perceptions of their school bullying supports. Results indicated statistically significant enhancement of prosocial attitudes for intervention-group students compared to control-group students, with small effect sizes. Feasibility and social validity outcomes indicated that the intervention was implemented with adequate fidelity and that its goals, procedures, and outcomes were perceived as appropriate. The interpretation, limitations, and implications of these results for practice are discussed herein. Keywords Bullying Á Universal-level intervention Á Attitudes Á Social validity Á Aggression Introduction Given that most severe forms of aggression and victim- ization (e.g., assault with a weapon) take place in the home and other community settings, schools are often considered one of the safest places for youth (Jimerson, Morrison, Pletcher, & Fulrong, 2006). Nevertheless, given the amount of mildly aggressive acts perpetrated on campus, schools have also been conceptualized as epicenters for lower-level, non-severe violence (e.g., verbal harassment; Furlong, Pavelski, & Saxton, 2002). Among these less-severe-yet- more-common forms of violence perpetrated at school, bullying has received increasing attention during the past decade, as it is becoming more prevalent, is associated with a host of deleterious outcomes for students and school climates, and is posited to serve as a developmental path- way toward more severe forms of antisocial behavior (Spivak & Prothrow-Stith, 2001). In response, researchers from various disciplines have undertaken bullying schol- arship in an attempt to better understand the nature of the phenomenon and to develop interventions that mitigate its negative effects on youth and schools (e.g., Espelage & Swearer, 2004; Jimerson, Swearer, & Espelage, 2010). Bullying is generally operationalized as a three-dimen- sional construct: an (1) intentional, malicious form of victimization that (2) occurs repeatedly over time (3) between parties wherein there exists a relative power dif- ferential (Olweus, 2010). Students may participate in bul- lying relationships via several roles: as bullies, victims, bully-victims, and bystanders (Alsaker and Gutzwiller- Helfenfinger 2010). To date, each of these participatory roles has been identified in every nation that has under- taken a descriptive study of this three-dimensional con- struct, suggesting that bullying is problematic for students and schools around the world (Jimerson et al., 2010). For T. L. Renshaw Á S. R. Jimerson Department of Counseling/Clinical/School Psychology, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, USA T. L. Renshaw (&) Special Education Department, Alpine School District, 575 N. 100 E., American Fork, UT 84003, USA e-mail: [email protected] 123 School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128 DOI 10.1007/s12310-011-9069-2

Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

ORIGINAL PAPER

Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level BullyingPrevention Curriculum

Tyler L. Renshaw • Shane R. Jimerson

Published online: 8 December 2011

� Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2011

Abstract Bullying is a salient challenge for children and

schools around the world, appearing to be especially pre-

valent at the middle-school level. Contemporary research

reveals an exigent need for systematic programing that

aims to reduce and prevent bullying via promoting

awareness, attitudes, and skills that facilitate the develop-

ment of positive peer relationships and positive school

climates. Considering this need, the present quasi-experi-

mental study examined the effects of a relatively new,

brief, universal-level bullying prevention curriculum on

middle-school students’ general attitudes toward bullying

and perceptions of their school bullying supports. Results

indicated statistically significant enhancement of prosocial

attitudes for intervention-group students compared to

control-group students, with small effect sizes. Feasibility

and social validity outcomes indicated that the intervention

was implemented with adequate fidelity and that its goals,

procedures, and outcomes were perceived as appropriate.

The interpretation, limitations, and implications of these

results for practice are discussed herein.

Keywords Bullying � Universal-level intervention �Attitudes � Social validity � Aggression

Introduction

Given that most severe forms of aggression and victim-

ization (e.g., assault with a weapon) take place in the home

and other community settings, schools are often considered

one of the safest places for youth (Jimerson, Morrison,

Pletcher, & Fulrong, 2006). Nevertheless, given the amount

of mildly aggressive acts perpetrated on campus, schools

have also been conceptualized as epicenters for lower-level,

non-severe violence (e.g., verbal harassment; Furlong,

Pavelski, & Saxton, 2002). Among these less-severe-yet-

more-common forms of violence perpetrated at school,

bullying has received increasing attention during the past

decade, as it is becoming more prevalent, is associated with

a host of deleterious outcomes for students and school

climates, and is posited to serve as a developmental path-

way toward more severe forms of antisocial behavior

(Spivak & Prothrow-Stith, 2001). In response, researchers

from various disciplines have undertaken bullying schol-

arship in an attempt to better understand the nature of the

phenomenon and to develop interventions that mitigate its

negative effects on youth and schools (e.g., Espelage &

Swearer, 2004; Jimerson, Swearer, & Espelage, 2010).

Bullying is generally operationalized as a three-dimen-

sional construct: an (1) intentional, malicious form of

victimization that (2) occurs repeatedly over time (3)

between parties wherein there exists a relative power dif-

ferential (Olweus, 2010). Students may participate in bul-

lying relationships via several roles: as bullies, victims,

bully-victims, and bystanders (Alsaker and Gutzwiller-

Helfenfinger 2010). To date, each of these participatory

roles has been identified in every nation that has under-

taken a descriptive study of this three-dimensional con-

struct, suggesting that bullying is problematic for students

and schools around the world (Jimerson et al., 2010). For

T. L. Renshaw � S. R. Jimerson

Department of Counseling/Clinical/School Psychology,

University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA,

USA

T. L. Renshaw (&)

Special Education Department, Alpine School District, 575 N.

100 E., American Fork, UT 84003, USA

e-mail: [email protected]

123

School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128

DOI 10.1007/s12310-011-9069-2

Page 2: Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

example, within the United States alone, it is estimated that

around 30% of students in sixth through tenth grades are

involved in bullying—either as a bully, victim, or both (Nansel

et al., 2001). Moreover, within the past decade, several

initiatives studying bullying from a cross-cultural perspec-

tive continue to reveal increasing prevalence rates within

both Western and Eastern nations (e.g., Murray-Harvey,

Slee, & Taki, 2010). Such international studies indicate

that school bullying in all countries, including the United

States, is highly associated with pervasive, negative school

climate effects, including an environment characterized

by increased interpersonal tension and intimidation as

well as decreased school connectedness and academic

engagement (e.g., Kosciw, Greytak, Diaz, & Bartkiewicz,

2010).

Beyond elevated prevalence rates, both national and

cross-national studies reveal that bullying is highly asso-

ciated with deleterious physical, social, educational, and

psychological outcomes for victims, perpetrators, and

bystanders. Specifically, being bullied has been shown to

be related to enhanced likelihood for acquiring generalized

and socialized anxiety, depression, loneliness, posttrau-

matic stress, low self-esteem and self-competence, poor

school adjustment and academic performance, poor phys-

ical health, self-perceived unpopularity, externalizing

problem behaviors (e.g., intentional rule-breaking), and

attention difficulties (Dioguardi & Theodore, 2006).

Moreover, in a review of research investigating the nega-

tive correlates for all parties involved in bullying (Swearer,

Grills, Haye, & Cary, 2004), several noteworthy psycho-

social outcome trends were highlighted: (a) both bullies

and victims report lower levels of self-esteem than unin-

volved students; (b) bully-victims report lower levels of

self-esteem than all other involved parties; (c) youth

involved in bullying more frequently, no matter what their

role, report lower self-esteem than those who are involved

less often; (d) depression is highly associated with all roles

in the bullying process, with bully-victims showing the

highest levels; (e) bully-victims are the most at-risk for

suicide of all involved parties; and (f) victims and bully-

victims manifest greater general anxiety than bullies and

uninvolved parties. Taken together, these data suggest that

bullying involvement at any level is associated with dele-

terious psychosocial outcomes, while bully-victims are at

the highest risk for mental health problems.

Given the negative effects of bullying on school climates

and youth’s well-being, a large portion of bullying schol-

arship has focused on evaluating the effectiveness of sys-

tematic prevention and intervention efforts employed

within school settings. Although all bullying prevention

programs share a similar overarching aim—namely,

reducing bullying behavior—they seek to accomplish this

aim via different means. For example, some programs focus

on identifying, targeting, and remediating bullies, while

others focus on identifying and empowering victims or

bystanders; some focus on improving the social skills of all

students, while others focus primarily on changing the

school culture and environment. Within the past 5 years,

several meta-analyses have synthesized the national and

international state-of-the-art surrounding these efforts.

Taken together, these studies have indicated mixed find-

ings, suggesting that, overall, bullying prevention and

intervention efforts may have negligible to strong effects on

student outcomes, depending on what indicators are being

evaluated. For example, Smith, Schneider, Smith and

Ananiadou (2004) reported negligible effects for both self-

reported victimization and perpetration outcomes, while

Ttofi, Farrington and Baldry (2008) reported large, positive

effects for observed bullying and self-reported emotional

and attitudinal outcomes. Yet another meta-analysis, con-

ducted by Merrell, Guelder, Ross and Isava (2008), reported

small to moderate, positive effects for attitudinal outcomes

but primarily negligible effects for behavioral outcomes.

Although the types of bullying interventions evaluated

in these meta-analyses seek to reduce bullying via different

means, and although there is little agreement among pro-

fessionals regarding the best approach for effectively pre-

venting bullying (Rigby & Bauman, 2010), a review of the

most popular intervention programs suggests that current

prevention efforts—and thus those evaluated within the

meta-analyses described above—tend to share several core

administrative characteristics: large-scale year-to-multi-

year implementation periods, general resource intensive-

ness (i.e., constant assistance from extra-school support

stuff and substantial training needed for local educators), a

school-ecology intervention approach (i.e., involving

individual, classroom, and schoolwide components, which

target staff, student, and policy changes), and an orientation

toward antisocial behavior and attitude reduction (Jimerson

& Huai, 2010). Given this administrative homogeneity

among bullying prevention programs, and considering the

mixed effectiveness of such efforts to date, there appears to

be a substantial warrant for the development and evaluation

of different kinds of bullying interventions, including those

characterized by smaller-scale and briefer implementation

periods, lower resource intensiveness (e.g., little involve-

ment from outside support staff as well as little training

requirements), reduced and isolated intervention compo-

nents (e.g., classroom-only efforts), and an orientation

toward prosocial behavior and attitude development. In

addition, there is also a warrant for the development and

evaluation of bullying interventions that are informed by

contemporary research regarding the core characteristics of

effective prevention programs.

As a result of the mixed effectiveness of bullying

interventions, much scholarship has also been devoted to

116 School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128

123

Page 3: Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

identifying the core characteristics of successful bullying

prevention programing. But similar to the meta-analyses,

empirical studies in this area have also revealed differing

findings. For instance, Ttofi et al. (2008) noted eight core

components associated with programs that demonstrated

effective reductions in reported bullying behavior: parent

training, improved playground supervision, enhanced dis-

ciplinary methods, frequent school conferences, provision

of information for parents, enhanced classroom rules,

improved classroom management, and inclusion of video-

based instructional materials—and five core components

associated with programs that demonstrated effective

reductions in reported victimization experiences: enhanced

disciplinary methods, initiated work with peers, parent

training, cooperative group work, and improved play-

ground supervision. Moreover, Rigby (2006) observed that

effective bullying interventions are typically comprised of

at least three core components: the development of uni-

versal-level antibullying policies; the integration of a

social-cognitive classroom curriculum to raise awareness

of bullying, promote prosocial attitudes, and teach social-

emotional learning skills; and the establishment of proce-

dures for handling bullying incidences at school. Further-

more, Hazler and Carney (2006) concluded that the most

effective bullying interventions seem to share several

characteristics: enhancing the empathic involvement of

students, increasing school connectedness, raising aware-

ness regarding bullying and its deleterious effects, estab-

lishing anti-bullying school policies, and developing

student social skills.

In addition to centering the development and imple-

mentation of interventions around the characteristics of

effective bullying programs (described above), scholars

also acknowledge that consideration of contextual and

feasibility elements—which are typically ignored in studies

identifying effective program characteristics—is equally

important, as striking a balance between empirical rigor

and social validity is necessary to promote the sustain-

ability of bullying interventions at the local site level

(Larson & Busse, 2006). To date, as noted above, the most

widely used bullying interventions are characterized by

administrative characteristics that are likely to negatively

impact social validity. Specifically, large-scale, multi-year,

resource-intensive, multicomponent prevention efforts are

likely to tax the motivation of school personnel and

diminish the overall implementation fidelity of the inter-

vention. This is especially likely in school contexts wherein

substantial extra-school resources, such as monetary sup-

port and additional administrative personnel, are limited or

absent. Therefore, in addition to research evaluating

empirically promising interventions that are grounded in

contemporary theory and evidence, there is also a sub-

stantial warrant for research evaluating the social validity

and feasibility of these efforts (Olweus, 2010). Without this

sort of scholarship, the literature will remain ambiguous

regarding the capacity of school personnel to effectively

sustain effective bullying prevention programing at the

local site level, without the assistance of significant extra-

school resources. Such local sustainability seems necessary

if bullying prevention is ever to take root as a core com-

ponent of educators’ practice and schools’ curriculum.

Beyond the social validity and feasibility of bullying

interventions, there is another conspicuous characteristic

that has yet to receive much empirical attention: dosage-

response effects (Merrell et al., 2008). In the greater world

of educational research, dosage-response effects have been

frequently investigated and demonstrated to be a significant

moderator of intervention effectiveness. For example,

several studies show that measures of cognitive and aca-

demic achievement increase as intervention duration

increases (e.g., Breit-Smith, Justice, McGinty, & Kaderavek,

2009), while other studies indicate that increased inter-

vention duration is significantly associated with increased

positive behavioral outcomes in the classroom and other

school-based settings (e.g., Zelezny, 1999). Furthermore,

educational scholarship demonstrates that the maintenance

of positive academic and behavioral outcomes is also sig-

nificantly moderated by the original intervention duration

(e.g., Reynolds, Temple, Robertson, & Mann, 2001).

Generalizing from these and similar findings, it seems

reasonable that intervention duration may be a key mod-

erating characteristic of the effectiveness of bullying

prevention programing. So far, however, most studies

reporting on bullying prevention span long intervention

durations—ranging from 1 year to multiple years—while

other educational interventions, such as those described

above, are typically characterized by multi-week to multi-

month durations. Moreover, there is no evidence to indicate

that bullying interventions need to be this long, nor that

briefer interventions (i.e., multi-week to multi-month) may

not be just as effective. In fact, given the strain on

resources and social validity that can accompany long-term

programing efforts, and recognizing the mixed findings

currently yielded by efforts of this nature, there appears to

be a substantial warrant for investigating the influence of

dosage-response effects on the utility of briefer bullying

interventions in school settings.

That said, the purpose of the present study was to further

bullying intervention scholarship by investigating the ini-

tial effects of a relatively new, brief, empirically promis-

ing, universal-level antibullying curriculum: Promoting

Positive Peer Relationships (P3R; Faull, Swearer, Jimer-

son, Espelage, & Ng, 2008). P3R is grounded in a trans-

actional-ecological model of youth’s development, which

views all behavior, including antisocial acts such as bul-

lying, within a functional-contextual framework—where

School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128 117

123

Page 4: Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

habits of action are engaged and maintained because they

help youth better adapt to their environments (Sameroff,

2000). Working from this theoretical perspective, P3R

seeks to alter youth’s developmental trajectories toward

more adaptive and healthier outcomes by raising awareness

regarding bullying-related issues and promoting prosocial

cognitive-behavioral habits (Faull et al., 2008). Unlike the

majority of current universal-level bullying interventions,

P3R is amenable to implementation within smaller-scale

and briefer implementation periods (i.e., 5 weeks to

2 months), is characterized by low-resource intensiveness

(i.e., requires little involvement from outside support staff

as well as little training and support materials), is amenable

to investigation of isolated intervention components (i.e.,

classroom-based intervention only), and is characterized by

an orientation toward prosocial behavior and attitude

development (i.e., cultivation of knowledge, empathy, and

problem-solving skills). Considering the various areas of

warranted investigation within bullying prevention

research (as noted above), the present study aimed to

evaluate more than just general intervention efficacy—

extending the inquiry to target dosage-response effects and

social validity considerations. Specifically, the following

research questions were investigated by utilizing P3R

within a junior-high school context:

1. Is P3R a feasible and socially valid intervention when

used within a general education classroom and imple-

mented by a general education teacher?

2. Is P3R an effective intervention for enhancing stu-

dents’ general attitudes toward bullying as well as their

perceptions of their school’s bullying supports?

3. Does the implementation duration of P3R (i.e., 1-, 5-,

and 8-week iterations) have dosage-response effects on

students’ attitudinal outcomes?

Method

School Context and Participants

The public junior-high school targeted in this study was

located on the south-central coast of California, near a public

university with which both of the authors were affiliated.

During the beginning of the academic school year in which

the study was conducted, the administration of the junior-

high school and the two authors formed an applied research

partnership. The administration noted that the school was

experiencing increased bullying-related behavior, as evi-

denced by the nature of rising disciplinary referrals, and

requested that the two authors help facilitate an appropriate

schoolwide intervention. The authors agreed to provide the

school with the necessary resources to implement and

evaluate P3R during the coming year, given that the

administration allowed the authors to use the de-identified

data for research purposes. Therefore, during the year in

which the study was conducted, the junior-high school

adopted P3R as a mandatory element of the exploratory

Health class taken by all seventh-grade students. Thus, all

intervention-group students were seventh-graders who

received the curriculum as part of their exploratory Health

class curriculum during one of three trimesters (i.e., fall,

winter, or spring), while all control-group students were 8th

graders enrolled in alternative elective courses during the

same trimesters.

Given this school context, all participants attended the

same public junior-high school described above. The total

sample was comprised of 636 seventh-grade and eighth-

grade students, of which 320 were in the intervention group

and 316 were in the control group. Although the entire

student population was targeted as part of either the inter-

vention group or the control group, only data from

approximately 75% of student body were used herein—due

to unusable data resulting from failure to complete surveys,

incomplete survey responses, or questionable survey

response patterns. As part of this study, demographics for

each participating student were not collected; however,

given that the majority of the school’s students participated

in the study and that no particular demographics were

biased through participant selection, schoolwide demo-

graphics were deemed to be an appropriate proxy measure.

At the time of this study, then, 46% of the student popula-

tion at this particular school were identified as White, 40%

as Hispanic or Latino, 6% as Asian, 2% as African Amer-

ican, and 6% as multiple or other ethnicities. Furthermore,

31% of students were classified as socioeconomically dis-

advantaged—as evidenced by their qualification for free or

reduced-price lunch—16% as English-language learners,

and 15% as students with disabilities. Demographics for

participating students were assumed to be similar.

P3R Curriculum

P3R is comprised of a series of psychoeducational film-

based resources for supporting students, educators, and the

surrounding community in addressing the problem of bul-

lying in schools. The full curriculum is comprised of three

separate yet interrelated programs—including classroom,

professional development, and community education

resources—that can be used in tandem or in isolation.

Development of these resources was a multiyear project

resulting from the collaborative efforts of a professional

film director and a team of academic school psychologists

(of which the second author was a member), who were

guided by empirical literature, best practices, and consul-

tative feedback from various students, teachers, school

118 School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128

123

Page 5: Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

administrators, parents, and other child-service providers

from across the United States (Faull et al., 2008). Given

that P3R was created specifically for school-based use, its

program features are designed to be feasibly implemented

within school settings by either school-based mental health

professionals (e.g., school psychologists) or educators (e.g.,

general and special education teachers). Since the public

release of P3R in 2008, several small-scale empirical

studies have been undertaken in various locations around

the United States to evaluate the effects of the intervention

on various students’, parents’, and educators’ bullying-

related attitudes (e.g., empathy toward victims) and

behaviors (e.g., bullying perpetration). To date, however,

none of these studies have been published in professional

journals; therefore, there are no public results available

regarding the preliminary effects of P3R. (For more com-

prehensive information regarding P3R and its develop-

ment, see www.storiesofus.com.)

Although P3R is a multicomponent and ecologically

oriented intervention, the present study examined only the

effects of the Classroom Resource on middle-school stu-

dents’ prosocial attitudes. The limited scope of this

investigation was constrained by practical considerations,

as the P3R Classroom Resource is the only schoolwide

intervention component and, at the time of the study, there

were not enough human resources available to sponsor

robust implementations of the other P3R component pro-

grams. The P3R Classroom Resource is an intervention

comprised of lessons designed to be implementable by

educators and flexibly integrated within classroom struc-

tures and schedules. Each lesson is planned to last

approximately 30–50 min and is accompanied by the P3R

Teacher’s Guide (Faull et al., 2008), which is a standard-

ized, semi-scripted curriculum that serves to structure each

lesson implementation. For each lesson, this guide provides

the teacher with a bullet-pointed outline of the lesson’s

core objectives, the materials needed to conduct the lesson,

and a detailed overview of the various classroom activities

to be carried out during the lesson.

All P3R lessons follow the same basic pedagogical

format, consisting of a few core components: (a) viewing

film segments portraying the initiation and development of

bullying relationships for one male and one female victim

within a middle-school context; (b) facilitating class dis-

cussions regarding these film segments—considering the

students’ interpersonal relationship dynamics, identifying

and working through related emotions, and employing

group problem-solving strategies to develop positive

behavioral solutions; and (c) providing psychoeducational

information before, during, and following class activities to

inform students of current school policy as well as supports

available to assist them in handling bullying situations in a

prosocial manner (e.g., seeking mediation via local school

administrators or counselors). Taken together, these lessons

provide youth with opportunities to cultivate greater

empathic responsiveness and prosocial cognitive-behav-

ioral habits by, first, exposing them to realistic portrayals of

bullying and its deleterious effects; second, facilitating a

safe space for students to explore their emotional, cogni-

tive, and behavioral reactions to these portrayals as well as

their real-life experiences; and, third, providing guidance

and supervision for students as they work individually and

collectively to synthesize their emotional, cognitive, and

behavioral experiences into prosocial responses that can be

enacted in the real-world. In this way, the Classroom

Resource lessons facilitate the development of empathy

and positive cognitive-behavioral habits through a combi-

nation of semi-structured reflective (i.e., past oriented),

experiential (i.e., present oriented), and forward-looking

(i.e., future oriented) activities that compel students to

confront and engage their emotions, cognitions, and

behavior. To compliment the classroom activities, each

lesson is accompanied by suggestions for optional home-

work assignments (none of which were employed in the

present study). Moreover, the P3R Teacher’s Guide con-

tains both five-lesson-plan and eight-lesson-plan versions

of the curriculum, allowing for more or less intensive

implementations, depending on need and context.

The P3R Classroom Resource is grounded in a trans-

actional-ecological model of youth’s development of

antisocial behavior, which views student development

within a contextual framework wherein all behaviors,

including negative ones, serve a functional purpose for the

individual—helping one better adapt to one’s environment

(Sameroff, 2000). The transactional-ecological model rec-

ognizes that youth development is a complex process,

comprised of macroregulations (i.e., environmental chan-

ges during key developmental periods), miniregulations

(i.e., daily interactions between the child and her caregiv-

ers, siblings, or peers, which shape the development of

behavior), and microregulations (i.e., brief interactions

between the child and caregiver during which their

responses are in sync; Sameroff, 1995). Given that this

model emphasizes the dynamic nature of development,

‘‘children are not viewed as sponges, merely soaking up

information, but rather as individuals who give and receive

information that subsequently influences future develop-

mental opportunities’’ (Jimerson et al., 2006, p. 10). Thus,

there is a reciprocal relationship between children and

contexts: contexts shape their behaviors and, in turn, their

behaviors reshape those contexts (Sameroff, 2009).

Grounded in a transactional-ecological perspective, the

P3R Classroom Resource aims to raise students’ awareness

regarding the functional nature of bullying-related behav-

iors, how those behaviors are shaped by youth’s contexts,

and how they negatively impact their and other’s well-

School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128 119

123

Page 6: Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

being. In addition to establishing such awareness, as

mentioned above, the Classroom Resource also provides

youth with opportunities to develop empathy and cogni-

tive-behavioral skills that serve to prevent bullying-related

behavior, promote prosocial behaviors, and therefore

enhance the well-being of all students (Faull et al., 2008).

Thus, by promoting positive internal and external actions,

the Classroom Resource aims to alter youth’s develop-

mental trajectories toward more adaptive and healthier

outcomes.

Viewed in light of the scholarship regarding key char-

acteristics of effective bullying prevention programs (dis-

cussed above), P3R’s program features appear promising.

In particular, P3R’s film segments, class discussions, and

pscyhoeducational elements are designed to promote the

empathic involvement of students and emphasize the

importance of school connectedness (cf. Hazler & Carney,

2006), include cooperative groupwork elements that aim to

empower students with social problem-solving skills (cf.

Rigby, 2006), and provide engaging opportunities for stu-

dents to learn via an interactive, video-based medium (cf.

Ttofi et al., 2008). However, it is noteworthy that over-

arching intention of P3R is fundamentally different from

the majority of other current bullying prevention programs

on the market. While other programs seek to directly and

immediately reduce the prevalence of bullying behavior

and victimization present within a school, P3R aims to

raise awareness, enhance knowledge, and improve attitudes

that are likely to facilitate more positive peer relationships

and increased well-being at school. This scope appears

both appropriate and reasonable in light of previous find-

ings regarding the effectiveness and potential effects of

bullying programing (e.g., Merrell et al., 2008) as well as

developmental considerations regarding children’s cogni-

tive and behavioral maturation (Jimerson et al., 2010).

Research Design and Variables

The questions and associated hypotheses of this study were

investigated via a quasi-experimental, pretest–posttest

research design, in which the seventh-graders in the Health

classes served as the intervention group (n = 320) and the

eighth-graders in the other elective courses served as the

control group (n = 316). Overall, the study was designed

to test one main effect as well as one interaction effect: the

main effect of Group (i.e., intervention vs. control status)

and the interaction effect of Group 9 Duration (i.e.,

1-week vs. 5-week vs. 8-week implementation iterations,

as a function of group status). Thus, P3R and its various

duration iterations were conceptualized as the independent

variables, while two attitudinal outcomes were conceptu-

alized as the dependent variables: students’ general atti-

tudes toward bullying and their perceptions of school

bullying supports. Intervention feasibility and teacher

perceptions of social validity were also conceptualized as

independent variables; however, these outcomes were

linked with the teacher, not the students, and were therefore

only evaluated descriptively.

Measures

Bullying Attitudinal Scale: Short Form

The Bullying Attitudinal Scale (Song, Swearer, Haye, &

Bandalos, 2001) is one of the few existing measures for

assessing student attitudes related to bullying. It is com-

prised of 14 items arranged on a five-point Likert scale

(i.e., ‘‘totally false’’ to ‘‘totally true’’) and is scored by

summing the responses to produce a composite score, with

higher scores indicating more prosocial attitudes toward

bullying. At the time of the present study, limited psy-

chometric support for this measure was available, with only

two studies reporting acceptable levels of internal consis-

tency (Cronbach’s a = .71 and .78) and only one study

reporting a moderate, 1-year test–retest reliability coeffi-

cient (r = .54; Song et al.). Given the sparse psychometric

data, and considering that the BAS had yet to be used for

evaluating attitudinal change in response to intervention,

the scale was descriptively analyzed for problematic items.

As a result of this critical review, seven of the BAS items

were deemed to be potentially problematic, as they did not

appear to measure prosocial attitudinal change and there-

fore could not be expected to have an optimal response

(e.g., ‘‘I would be friends with a bully’’). Thus, these seven

items were deemed inappropriate for the purposes of the

present study and were selected out, while the seven

remaining items were retained and used as a single, short-

form measure: the Bullying Attitudinal Scale: Short Form

(BAS-SF).

Perceptions of School Bullying Supports Scale

Prior to this study, there were no available measures for

assessing students’ perceptions of their local school bul-

lying supports, a construct conceptualized as a proxy for

school climate regarding bullying issues. Thus, for the

purposes of this study, a brief 4-item measure was devel-

oped by the authors to assess this particular construct: the

Perceptions of School Bullying Supports Scale (PSBSS).

The few items comprising the PSBSS were designed to

have practical utility and face validity for evaluating the

underlying construct of student perceptions of school bul-

lying supports (i.e., perceptions of efforts, policies, and

relationships). Similar to the BAS-SF, each item was

arranged on the same five-point Likert scale, and the

responses were summed to produce a composite score, with

120 School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128

123

Page 7: Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

higher scores indicating more positive perceptions of local

school climate regarding bullying. No pilot testing was

conducted with this measure prior to its use in the present

study.

P3R Implementation Fidelity Checklists

The P3R Implementation Fidelity Checklists (P3R-IFC)

were created by the authors for the present study to assess

the Health teacher’s implementation fidelity of P3R and to

serve as a proxy for intervention feasibility. Because dif-

ferent lesson plan versions were implemented during dif-

ferent trimesters, separate checklists were created for both

the five-lesson and eight-lesson versions. One checklist

was developed per lesson, consisting of brief restatements

of the briefing procedures, lesson activity components, and

debriefing components—summarized directly from the

P3R Teacher’s Guide (Faull et al., 2008). Directions on

each checklist instructed raters to mark ‘‘X’’ if the proce-

dure or component was observed during the intervention

session and ‘‘O’’ if the procedure or component was not

observed.

Intervention Rating Profile for Teachers: P3R Adaptation

Intervention social validity has generally been conceptu-

alized as a threefold evaluation of the social significance of

the goals, social appropriateness and feasibility of the

procedures, and social importance of the outcomes of an

intervention, as perceived by its consumers (i.e., imple-

menters or clients; Wolf, 1978). Given that P3R is a uni-

versal, social-cognitive intervention targeting groups of

students, none of the currently available self-report social

validity measures were deemed appropriate for assessing

its social validity. Thus, the Intervention Rating Profile for

Teachers, which has been deemed to have high internal

consistency (Cronbach’s a = .91) and one primary factor

loading (15-item loadings ranging from .82 to .95; Martens,

Witt, Elliot, & Darveaux, 1985), was modified by the

authors for use in the present study. The scale’s original

6-point Likert rating scale, ranging from ‘‘strongly dis-

agree’’ to ‘‘strongly agree,’’ was maintained, but item

wording was altered to align with the aims of the P3R

program (i.e., focus on attitudinal as opposed to behavioral

changes.) This new, modified measure was called the

Intervention Rating Profile for Teachers: P3R Adaptation

(IRP-P3R).

P3R Social Validity Questionnaire

To provide a more robust assessment of social validity, the

P3R Social Validity Questionnaire (P3R-SVQ) was created

by the authors to assess the Health teacher’s perceptions of

the goals, procedures, and outcomes of the P3R imple-

mentations. Specifically, the questionnaire was comprised

of eight open-ended questions that surveyed the teacher’s

perceptions of (a) the need for bullying intervention for the

target students, (b) the feasibility of intervention proce-

dures and durations, (c) the outcomes produced by the

interventions, and (d) her general compliments and criti-

cisms regarding the various intervention implementations

as well as the overall P3R intervention.

Intervention and Data Collection Procedures

Given that the P3R Classroom Resource was officially

adopted by the school’s administration as a mandatory part

of the exploratory Health class curriculum, the adminis-

tration chose to test three different iterations of the inter-

vention—one during each trimester—to see whether one

proved more effective than another, and thus to inform

their future usage of the curriculum. Such iteration testing

was amenable to the design of the Classroom Resource, as

the curriculum included two different lesson plan versions

within the Teacher’s Guide, both of which could be

implemented via various durations. The administration and

the authors decided, therefore, that the 5-lesson plan ver-

sion would be implemented during a 1-week duration,

followed by 5-lesson plan implementation during a 5-week

duration, and concluding with an 8-lesson plan imple-

mentation during an 8-week duration. This implementation

schedule would provide data on the comparative effec-

tiveness of approximately 1-week, 1-month, and 2-month

P3R interventions, respectively, and would run throughout

the academic school year.

During the fall trimester of 2008, the five-lesson plan

version of P3R was implemented in the exploratory Health

course, which consisted of four class periods comprised of

22–28 students each (n = 107). One lesson was adminis-

tered per day, Monday through Friday, and therefore the

entire intervention was completed within 1 week. Follow-

ing, during the winter trimester, the five-lesson plan ver-

sion of P3R was again implemented in the exploratory

Health course, with similar class sizes (n = 113). This

time, however, only one lesson was administered per week,

and thus the entire intervention was completed within

5 weeks. Finally, during the spring trimester, the eight-

lesson plan version of P3R was implemented in the

exploratory Health course, again with similar class sizes

(n = 100). Similar to the previous trimester, one lesson

was administered per week, and therefore the intervention

was completed in 8 weeks. Control-group students

(n = 105, 108, and 103 for the fall, winter, and spring

trimesters, respectively) received ‘‘treatment as usual’’

during the same time periods in their exploratory elective

courses. During each of these P3R Classroom Resource

School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128 121

123

Page 8: Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

implementations, the Health teacher facilitated the inter-

vention using the Teacher’s Guide (described above). Prior

to each trimester’s implementation, the first author met

with the teacher, checked to see whether she was oriented

to the correct section of the curriculum that would be

administered during the present trimester and answered any

questions regarding procedures or content. The teacher

typically had few, if any, questions—reporting that the

Teacher’s Guide was self-explanatory and comprehensible.

As previously described, the present study employed a

quasi-experimental research design, with repeated-mea-

sures (i.e., pre-posttest) used to assess students’ general

attitudes toward bullying and perceptions of their local

school bullying supports. To accomplish this, all items

from the BAS-SF and the PSBSS were combined into a

single outcome measure—entitled Quick Questions About

Bullying—that was administered to both control-group and

intervention-group students the day prior to beginning the

intervention (i.e., pretest) as well as the day following the

completion of the intervention (i.e., posttest) during each

trimester. The teacher of the exploratory Health class

administered these assessments for the intervention-group

students, while the teachers of the various elective courses

administered these assessments for the control-group stu-

dents. During the course of the study, the control-group

elective courses consisted of art, keyboarding, and wood-

shop—none of which included curriculum related to social,

emotional, or behavioral development in youth. To

enhance administration fidelity of the surveys and prevent

against confounding teacher effects, all teachers were

provided with a one-page, scripted handout consisting of an

administration protocol, which was reviewed with them by

the first author. The teachers reported that they followed

the protocol appropriately; however, no observational

implementation fidelity was taken. For both pretest and

posttest administrations, and across control and interven-

tion groups, all students completed these assessments in

less than 10 min. Following each test administration (i.e.,

pre and post) during each trimester, the assessments were

retrieved from all participating teachers and entered into an

electronic database.

To measure implementation fidelity and therefore feasi-

bility of the P3R, the Health teacher was provided with the

appropriate P3R-IFC for each implementation, depending

on the duration (i.e., 5-lesson or 8-lesson versions),

and completed self-ratings immediately following each

intervention session. Given that four classes received the

intervention each trimester, the teacher completed 20

checklists the first trimester (for five lessons), 20 more the

second trimester (for five lessons), and 32 the last trimester

(for eight lessons)—resulting in a total of 72 self-completed

fidelity checklists. To enhance the validity of the teacher’s

self-reports, the first author conducted implementation

fidelity observations using the P3R-IFC for approximately

25% of the intervention lessons during each trimester.

Moreover, following the conclusion of all interventions, the

Health teacher completed the IRP-P3R and the P3R-SVQ.

Data Analyses

Preliminary Analyses

Given that the BAS-SF and PSBSS were original with this

study, preliminary analyses were conducted to explore their

initial psychometric properties, using a subsample of 105

students from the first-trimester control group. One-week

test–retest reliability was assessed for each of the seven

items comprising the BAS-SF and each of the four items

comprising the PSBSS, to ensure that the items amassing

both composites were tenable. Following, test–retest reli-

ability was also assessed for the BAS-SF and PSBSS

composites. Internal consistency analyses were also con-

ducted for the items comprising each measure, to ensure

that it represented a unified underlying construct. Finally,

to further confirm the reliability results, exploratory factor

analyses—employing the Maximum Likelihood extraction

method with a Promax (oblique) rotation—were conducted

to determine the statistical dimensionality of both

measures.

Beyond these analyses, the appropriateness of the pro-

jected usage of two-way factorial ANCOVA for some of

the primary analyses was investigated by checking the

assumptions for this particular analysis against the

parameters of the current data set. Also, given that two

separate ANCOVA analyses were projected, a Bon-

feronni’s adjustment was performed to prevent inflation of

family-wise error, and power analyses were carried out to

ascertain whether sufficient power would be obtained to

identify statistically significant effects, if present.

Primary Analyses

The primary analyses of this study were conducted to

investigate the three aforementioned research questions.

Implementation fidelity for each intervention iteration was

assessed via a two-stage quantitative descriptive analysis:

first, fidelity for each lesson was assessed via summing the

number of implemented components and dividing that by

the total number of components per lesson, resulting in a

percentage; second, a fidelity subtotal for each iteration

implementation was ascertained and reported by summing

all of the resulting percentages from each lesson within

each duration iteration and then determining the mean

percentage for each. Moreover, teacher perceptions of

social validity were assessed by quantitative and qualitative

122 School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128

123

Page 9: Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

means: first, all responses to items on the IRP-P3R were

summed to produce a composite score, which was then

judged against an associated descriptor indicating a level of

perceived social validity (e.g., composite scor-

e [75 = high social validity); second, a general qualitative

descriptive analysis of the content of the teacher’s

responses to the P3R-SVQ was used to provide evidence

for or against positive perceptions of the goals, procedures,

and outcomes of the three intervention iterations as well as

the overall intervention.

To assess the general intervention efficacy as well as

possible dosage-response effects on students’ general atti-

tudes toward bullying, an ANCOVA was conducted using

the BAS-SF pretest composite group means as a covariate

(i.e., to neutralize any confounding variables that may have

effected baseline reports between seventh- and eighth-

graders or between students in the health class and those in

the other exploratory classes), the BAS-SF posttest com-

posite group means as a dependent variable, and the Group

(i.e., control or intervention) and Duration (i.e., 1-, 5-,

8-week) statuses as the between-subjects fixed-factor

independent variables. Specifically, a Group main effect

and a Duration 9 Group interaction effect were evaluated

for potentially statistically significant difference between

BAS-SF posttest means, followed by a descriptive mean

evaluation to confirm the directionality of any noted effect

as well as an effect size analysis to determine the educa-

tional import of the effect. Similarly, to assess the general

intervention efficacy as well as possible dosage-response

effects on students’ perceptions of their school’s bullying

supports, a separate ANCOVA was conducted using the

PSBSS pretest composite group means as a covariate, the

PSBSS posttest composite group means as a dependent

variable, and the Group and Duration statuses as the

between-subjects fixed-factor independent variables.

Again, a Group main effect and a Duration 9 Group

interaction effect were evaluated for potentially statistically

significant difference between PSBSS posttest means, fol-

lowed by a descriptive mean evaluation to confirm the

directionality of any noted effect as well as an effect size

analysis to determine the educational import of the effect.

Results

Preliminary Outcomes

Psychometric Explorations

For both the BAS-SF and the PSBSS, results from the test–

retest reliability analyses revealed at least moderate, sta-

tistically significant bivariate correlations for all items,

r [ .50, as well as both composites, r [ .65, suggesting

adequate overall reliability for attitudinal measurements

that may be somewhat inherently variable. Furthermore,

the internal consistency analysis indicated acceptable

results for both the BAS-SF and the PSBSS, Cronbach’s

a = .88 and .73, respectively, suggesting each composite

adequately represents a single underlying attitudinal con-

struct. Moreover, results from the BAS-SF exploratory

factor analysis indicated that only one factor was extracted,

which accounted for approximately 50% of the total vari-

ance (Eigen value = 3.94) and had moderate to high

loadings across all seven items, [.55, suggesting unidi-

mensionality. Similarly, results from the PSBSS explor-

atory factor analysis indicated that only one factor was

extracted, which accounted for approximately 42% of the

total variance and had moderate to high loadings across all

four items, [.57, also suggesting unidimensionality. Taken

together, results from these analyses suggest that the BAS-

SF and the PSBSS had adequate psychometric properties to

justify using their composite scores as representations of

attitudinal constructs.

ANCOVA Assumption Checks

All ANCOVA assumptions—including (a) independence

of observations, (b) homogeneity of variance, (c) multi-

variate normality across each group distribution, (d) ade-

quate and equal group sample sizes, (e) low measurement

error for the covariate, (f) a linear relationship between the

covariate and the dependent variable, (g) homogeneity of

covariate regression coefficients, (h) lack of substantial

covariate outliers, (g) general additivity, and (h) indepen-

dence of the error term—were checked and deemed to be

met or, if not met, deemed to be robust in spite of such

violations. For example, given that the present study was

quasi-experimental and thus lacked random group assign-

ment, a significant Group main effect was found at baseline

for both the BAS-SF composite distribution, F(1) = 48.27,

p \ .001, and the PSBSS composite distribution,

F(1) = 18.41, p \ .001. Thus, the error term could not be

considered independent of the covariates and the categor-

ical independent variables because systematic, statistically

significant difference was identified between the groups at

baseline. Nevertheless, despite this violation, ANCOVA

remains the most appropriate analysis for repeated-mea-

sures, pre-post quasi-experimental designs (cf. Tabachnick

and Fidell 1996) and was thus considered appropriate for

the primary analyses.

Bonferonni’s Adjustment and Power Analyses

Bonferonni’s adjustment, which divided the original sig-

nificance level, a = .05, by the total number of planned

analyses (i.e., two), resulted in a modified significance

School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128 123

123

Page 10: Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

level of a = .025. Considering this adjusted significance

level with the total number of groups within each projected

analysis (i.e., six), and assuming that at least a moderate

effect size (partial g2 C .06) would be obtained for each

analysis, Power analyses indicated that all current group

sample sizes, n C 100, were more than adequate for

securing adequate power, C.80, for all F-tests.

Primary Outcomes

Intervention Feasibility and Social Validity

Results from the quantitative descriptive analyses indicated

that the teacher’s mean self-reported fidelity rating for

proportion of intervention components administered per

lesson was high: 87% for the 1-week implementation, 92%

for the 5-week implementation, 95% for the 8-week

implementation, and 91% for total lesson administrations.

The trustworthiness of these ratings was corroborated via

high inter-rater reliability established between the teacher’s

and independent observer’s fidelity ratings, Cohen’s

j = .90, suggesting adequate overall implementation

fidelity. Furthermore, the teacher’s total IRP-P3R com-

posite score was 79, putting the average item response in

the ‘‘agree’’ to ‘‘strongly agree’’ range, M = 5.27, indi-

cating a very favorable perception of the overall social

validity of P3R as a classroom intervention within the

particular school and classroom context. Moreover, the

teacher’s qualitative responses on the P3R-SVQ confirmed

her very favorable perception of goals, procedures, and

outcomes of the P3R intervention. Specifically, she

remarked that (a) students in her classes needed the inter-

vention because they lacked awareness and knowledge

related to bullying dynamics; (b) the overall intervention

was feasibly implemented within her classes under typical

classroom conditions; (c) the content of the intervention

was perceived as realistic by both herself and the students;

and (d) P3R appeared to enhance students’ mindfulness of

their actions, words, and attitudes related to bullying—as

evidence by informal observations of changes in student

social interactions in the classroom during and following

the intervention. Furthermore, the Health teacher also

remarked on the P3R-SVQ that (e) all intervention dura-

tions seemed equally feasible to implement and that (f) she

did not perceive any dosage-response effects—or differ-

ences in student attitude or behavior changes—between the

1-, 5-, and 8-week iterations.

General Intervention Efficacy

Results from the two-way factorial ANCOVA using the

BAS-SF pretest scores as a covariate and the BAS-SF

posttest scores as the dependent variable indicated a

statistically significant Group main effect, F(1) = 9.93,

p = .002, with a small effect size, partial g2 = .02. A

descriptive evaluation of the resulting estimated marginal

means from this analysis indicated that the total posttest

marginal mean for the intervention group was approxi-

mately two mean points higher than the total posttest

marginal mean for the control group, indicating positive

improvements in intervention-group students’ general

attitudes toward bullying. Moreover, results from the two-

way factorial ANCOVA using the PSBSS pretest scores as

a covariate and the PSBSS posttest scores as the dependent

variable also revealed a significant Group main effect,

F(1) = 7.25, p = .007, with a small effect size, partial

g2 = .01. A descriptive evaluation of the resulting esti-

mated marginal means from this analysis indicated that the

total posttest marginal mean for the intervention group was

approximately one mean point higher than the total posttest

marginal mean for the control group, indicating positive

attitudinal improvements in intervention-group students’

perceptions of their school’s bullying supports.

Dosage-Response Effects

Results from the same two-way factorial ANCOVAs

described above, using the BAS-SF and PSBSS pretest

scores as covariates and the BAS-SF and PSBSS posttest

scores as the dependent variables, indicated nonsignificant

Group 9 Duration interaction effects for both general

attitudes toward bullying, F(2) = .67, p = .51, as well as

perceptions of the school’s bullying supports, F(2) = .85,

p = .43, which suggests no dosage-response effects were

observed for either outcome measure (see Table 1 for a

summary of all primary ANCOVA results).

Discussion

Interpretation of Results

The present study investigated the feasibility and social

validity, general intervention efficacy, and dosage-response

effects of teacher-implemented P3R Classroom Resource

Table 1 Summary of primary ANCOVA results

Measure Source df F p g2

BAS-SFa Group 1 9.93 .002 .02

Group 9 duration 2 .67 .51 .00

PSBSSb Group 1 7.25 .007 .01

Group 9 duration 2 .85 .43 .00

a Bullying attitudinal scale—short formb Perceptions of school bullying supports scale

124 School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128

123

Page 11: Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

interventions on attitudinal outcomes of junior-high school

students. Findings from the analyses of resulting data

suggest that the intervention was feasible for the Health

teacher to implement independently and that it was socially

valid within the particular school and classroom contexts—

as evidenced by high implementation fidelity and positive

teacher perceptions of P3R’s goals, procedures, and out-

comes across implementations. Moreover, findings from

this study suggest that P3R marginally enhanced students’

attitudes toward bullying as well as their perceptions of

their school’s bullying supports, resulting in more prosocial

attitudes overall—as evidenced by statistically significant

changes in mean attitudinal scores accompanied by small

effect sizes. Furthermore, findings from this study suggest

no dosage-response effects among the 1-, 5-, and 8-week

iterations of P3R, indicating relatively equivalent effec-

tiveness of implementations—as evidenced by statistically

non-significant differences in mean attitudinal scores

across durations. Taken together, these findings suggest

that, as used in the present study, the P3R Classroom

Resource was feasibly implemented, socially valid, and

marginally effective as an intervention for enhancing stu-

dents’ general attitudes toward bullying as well as their

attitudes toward their school’s bullying supports.

As noted previously herein, the P3R Classroom

Resource differs from the majority of mainstream uni-

versal-level bullying interventions (cf. Jimerson & Huai,

2010), as it is amenable to implementation within smaller-

scale and briefer implementation periods (e.g., 1 week to

2 months), is characterized by low-resource intensiveness

(i.e., requires little involvement from outside support staff

as well as little training and support materials), is amenable

to investigation of isolated intervention components (i.e.,

classroom-based intervention only), and is characterized by

an orientation toward prosocial behavior and attitude

development (i.e., cultivation of knowledge, empathy, and

problem-solving skills). Considering this, the positive

findings resulting from the present study enhance the

warrant for further research into a new wave of bullying

intervention and prevention programing, which, similar to

P3R, can be feasibly implemented by educators at the local

site level and still facilitate positive student outcomes.

Intervention efforts of this nature are likely to be highly

sustainable (cf. Larson & Busse, 2006), but much empirical

research will be needed to prove their efficacy.

Nevertheless, given the current dearth of studies inves-

tigating the feasibility and social validity of bullying

interventions (Olweus, 2010), findings from the present

study are encouraging: providing a case example of a

teacher who accurately and effectively implemented P3R

amidst her typical school duties. Yet given the dearth of

studies investigating possible dosage-response effects for

bullying interventions (Merrell et al., 2008), findings from

this study are essentially neutral: providing no evidence for

preferring shorter implementation durations (i.e., 1-week)

over longer ones (i.e., 5-, 8-week). Moreover, given the

preponderance of evidence indicating only marginal

changes in student outcomes resulting from bullying

interventions (e.g., Smith et al., 2004), findings from the

present study serve to strengthen the status quo of student

outcomes: providing another example of small, positive

intervention effects on students’ social attitudes. Given that

the content and method of P3R is comprised of several

empirically promising program features (e.g., video-based

curriculum, social problem-solving orientation, empathic

involvement of students; e.g., Hazler & Carney, 2006), this

last finding may seem surprising. However, a reconsider-

ation of the bullying intervention literature suggests at least

four possible limitations that may account for such mar-

ginal effects: (a) an internal-potency limitation of the

intervention itself; (b) an overall dosage-response limita-

tion; (c) a problematic measurement limitation; and

(d) sampling bias and research design limitations.

Limitations and Future Research

Internal-Potency

The effectiveness of the P3R Classroom Resource to

enhance students’ prosocial attitudes may have been lim-

ited by its internal-potency, meaning that the intervention’s

contents and methods may have not been potent enough to

facilitate more remarkable student outcomes. A critical

evaluation of P3R’s content and methods in relation to the

literature regarding effective characteristics of bullying

programs suggests that although its method is characterized

by several empirically promising features (e.g., video-

based instruction and a social problem-solving orientation),

its content is lacking one fundamental component that has

been associated with stronger student outcomes: direct

social-skills instruction (cf. Hazler & Carney, 2006). Thus,

the potency of P3R may be limited by the exclusion of a

skills acquisition component. To test this hypothesis, future

studies could investigate the effectiveness of the interven-

tion as is—or without social-skills training—compared to

the effectiveness of the intervention enhanced—or with a

social-skills training component. However, by adding a

social-skills training component to P3R, the program is

likely to become more similar to other preexisting bullying

interventions and may, as a result, become more difficult

for general educators and other school-based practitioners

to implement accurately and independently. Recognizing

that ‘‘any method, regardless of its empirical merit, is only

as valid as the probability that school-based personnel will

use it in a reliable and effective manner’’ (Scott et al., 2004,

p. 190), we recommend that future research seek to strike a

School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128 125

123

Page 12: Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

balance between empirical rigor and social validity—

aiming for sustainability at the local site level.

Dosage-Response

The present study may also be limited by an overall dos-

age-response threshold. Although part of the original

intention of the study was to investigate various brief

dosage-response effects (i.e., 1-, 5-, 8-week), it is possible

that each intervention duration was too brief to facilitate

more remarkable student outcomes, failing to meet a dos-

age-response threshold. Thus, the same intervention, if

administered over longer periods of time—such as

3–5 months, as opposed to 1 or 2 months—may be capable

of facilitating more remarkable student outcomes. This

hypothesis is supported by a large body of educational

research indicating the significant role of duration in

enhancing the effectiveness of interventions (e.g., Zelezny,

1999); however, it remains largely uninvestigated as per-

taining to bullying interventions (Olweus, 2010). Thus, we

recommend that future research is warranted to investigate

the possibility of a duration-response threshold for the P3R

Classroom Resource and similar bullying interventions,

which may require stretching their recommended imple-

mentation periods.

Problematic Measurement

Another possible limitation of the present study is prob-

lematic measurement. In the present study, post hoc anal-

yses revealed that baseline results from both measures

revealed significantly non-normal, positively skewed dis-

tributions. Such results suggest substantial ceiling effects

for baseline responding, in which large proportions of

students were already responding prosocially to both out-

come indicators prior to receiving the intervention—

resulting in a significantly lower likelihood of identifying

statistically significant results with moderate to large effect

sizes at posttest (cf. McHorney & Tarlov, 1995). Thus,

although it is possible that students in the present study

already possessed extremely prosocial attitudes toward

bullying and positive perceptions of their local school

bullying supports at baseline, it is also possible that the

suggested measurement ceiling effect is attributable to a

social-desirability response effect. Given all of the items in

both measures used some form of the word ‘‘bullying,’’

which carries an inherently negative connotation, students

may have been primed to respond in an overly prosocial

manner at baseline. If this was the case, it could have

blurred the descriptive comparison between posttest

responses, which may have been more honest or accurate

than those at baseline and thus lessened the integrity of

construct validity (cf. Furlong, Sharkey, Felix, Tanigawa,

& Green, 2010). Given that concerns with problematic

measurement have been described as the ‘‘Achilles heel of

bullying prevention efforts’’ (Cornell, Sheras, & Cole,

2006, p. 193), we recommend that future research is war-

ranted to develop psychometrically sound measures with

high comparative and practical utility.

Sampling Bias and Research Design

Finally, a general sampling bias and other unintended

research design features may have also limited the present

study. For example, given that this study utilized the nat-

ural classroom groupings of students to serve as the bases

for determining group membership, it is possible that such

nonprobabilistic or convenience sampling may be con-

founded by other characteristics unique to students in the

Health class intervention group (cf. Heppner, Wampold, &

Kivlighan, 2007). Specifically, because the P3R Classroom

Resource was presented in a Health class, it is possible that

class content or activities presented prior to or in con-

junction with P3R may have confounded the intervention

effects. Although such sampling bias is undesirable, it was

largely unavoidable in the present study, as the school’s

administration selected the intervention for inclusion with

the exploratory Health class only. Nevertheless, future

studies within similar lines of research may compensate for

this possible confound by arranging for implementations of

P3R or other bullying interventions across multiple class-

room settings (e.g., Mathematics, Language Arts, and

Technology), thereby creating a categorical classroom

variable that can be investigated as a random-effect within

a larger general linear model (cf. Hancock & Mueller,

2010). Moreover, given that the present study lacked fol-

low-up measurements of students’ attitudes post-interven-

tion, we recommend that future bullying intervention

studies utilize longitudinal research designs to test whether

student outcomes decline, maintain, or improve following

intervention paired with typical developmental maturation.

Implications for Practitioners

A couple of important implications for school-based

practice can be derived from the present study. First,

findings from the present study suggest that classroom

teachers and, by extension, other educational professionals

(e.g., school psychologists) may be capable of imple-

menting P3R with high fidelity and resulting in high social

validity as a brief, universal-level bullying intervention.

However, because only one teacher’s implementations

were evaluated in the present study, more studies with

more teachers in different contexts are needed prior to

making any substantive generalizations. Second, findings

from the present study suggest that P3R had positive

126 School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128

123

Page 13: Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

effects on students’ prosocial attitudes regarding bullying

and school climate and thus may be an empirically prom-

ising school-based intervention. Nevertheless, because this

is one of the first empirical investigations of P3R and only

small effect sizes resulted in the present study, more

studies with more compelling evidence are needed before

P3R can be recommended as an evidence-based practice.

Moreover, given current educational policy and the general

scientific zeitgeist regarding selecting best practices (e.g.,

Slavin, 2002), if P3R is chosen for school-based use as a

pilot intervention, we strongly recommend that school-

based practitioners inform all involved parties of the status

of its current empirical validation.

Conclusion

The present study investigated the effects of a new, brief,

universal-level bullying intervention—the P3R Classroom

Resource. Findings indicated that the intervention had small,

positive effects on student attitudes and was feasibly

implemented by the classroom teacher, with high percep-

tions of social validity. Given the preliminary nature of these

findings, we hope that scholars and school-based practitio-

ners will collaborate in validating P3R and other similar

bullying interventions—continually striving to balance

empirical rigor with social validity in pursuit of enhancing

youth’s mental health and well-being through school-based

prevention and intervention efforts. Ultimately, we hope that

efforts in this line of applied research will increasingly move

toward developing and evaluating bullying interventions

that are sustainable at the local site level—empowering

school communities with the tools and skills necessary to

efficiently prevent bullying-related problems.

References

Alsaker, F. D., & Gutzwiller-Helfenfinger, E. (2010). Social behavior

and peer relationships of victims, bully-victims, and bullies in

kindergarten. In S. R. Jimerson, S. M. Swearer, & D. L. Espelage

(Eds.), Handbook of bullying in schools (pp. 87–100). New

York, NY: Routledge.

Breit-Smith, A., Justice, L. M., McGinty, A. S., & Kaderavek, J.

(2009). How often and how much? Intensity of print referencing

intervention. Topics in Language Disorders, 29, 360–369.

Cornell, D., Sheras, P. L., & Cole, J. C. (2006). Assessment of

bullying. In S. R. Jimerson & M. J. Furlong (Eds.), Handbook ofschool violence and school safety: From research to practice(pp. 191–210). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

DioGuardi, R. J., & Theodore, L. A. (2006). Understanding and

addressing peer victimization among students. In S. R. Jimerson

& M. J. Furlong (Eds.), Handbook of school violence and schoolsafety (pp. 339–352). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Espelage, D. L., & Swearer, S. M. (Eds.). (2004). Bullying inAmerican schools. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Faull, C., Swearer, S. M., Jimerson, S. R., Espelage, D. L., & Ng, R.

(2008). Promoting positive peer relationships: Middle school

bullying prevention program—Classroom resource. Adelaide,

Australia: Readymade Productions.

Furlong, M. J., Pavelski, R., & Saxton, J. (2002). The prevention of

school violence. In S. E. Brock, P. J. Lazarus, & S. R. Jimerson

(Eds.), Best practices in school crisis prevention and interven-tion (pp. 131–149). Bethesda, MD: National Association of

School Psychologists.

Furlong, M. J., Sharkey, J. D., Felix, E., Tanigawa, D., & Green, J. G.

(2010). Bullying assessment: A call for increased precision of

self-reporting procedures. In S. R. Jimerson, S. M. Swearer, & D.

L. Espelage (Eds.), Handbook of bullying in schools (pp.

329–346). New York, NY: Routledge.

Hancock, G. R., & Mueller, R. O. (2010). The reviewer’s guide toquantitative methods in the social sciences. New York, NY:

Routledge.

Hazler, R. J., & Carney, J. V. (2006). Critical characteristics of

effective bullying prevention programs. In S. R. Jimerson & M.

J. Furlong (Eds.), Handbook of school violence and school safety(pp. 275–292). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Heppner, P. P., Wampold, B. E., & Kivlighan, D. M. (2007).

Research design in counseling (2nd ed.). New York, NY:

Brooks.

Jimerson, S. R., & Huai, N. (2010). International perspectives on

bullying prevention and intervention. In S. R. Jimerson, S.

M. Swearer, & D. L. Espelage (Eds.), Handbook of bullying inschools: An international perspective (pp. 571–592). New York,

NY: Routledge.

Jimerson, S. R., Morrison, G. M., Pletcher, S. W., & Fulrong, M. J.

(2006). Youth engaged in antisocial behaviors: Who are they? In

S. R. Jimerson & M. J. Furlong (Eds.), Handbook of schoolviolence and school safety (pp. 3–20). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Jimerson, S. R., Swearer, S. M., & Espelage, D. L. (Eds.). (2010).

Handbook of bullying in schools. New York, NY: Routledge.

Kosciw, J. G., Greytak, E. A., Diaz, E. M., & Bartkiewicz, M. J.

(2010). The 2009 school climate survey: The experiences oflesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender youth in our nation’sschools. Washington, DC: Gay, Lesbian and Straight Education

Network. Retrieved from http://www.glsen.org.

Larson, J., & Busse, R. T. (2006). A problem-solving approach to

school violence prevention. In S. R. Jimerson & M. J. Furlong

(Eds.), Handbook of school violence and school safety: Fromresearch to practice (pp. 73–88). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Martens, B. K., Witt, J. C., Elliot, S. E., & Darveaux, D. X. (1985).

Teacher judgments concerning the acceptability of school-based

interventions. Professional psychology: Research and practice,16, 191–198.

McHorney, C. A., & Tarlov, A. R. (1995). Individual-patient

monitoring in clinical practice: Are available health status

surveys adequate? Quality of Life Research, 4, 293–307.

Merrell, K. W., Guelder, B. A., Ross, S. W., & Isava, D. M. (2008).

How effective are school bullying intervention programs? A

meta-analysis of intervention research. School PsychologyQuarterly, 23, 26–42.

Murray-Harvey, R., Slee, P. T., & Taki, M. (2010). Comparative and

cross-cultural research on school bullying. In S. R. Jimerson, S.

M. Swearer, & D. L. Espelage (Eds.), Handbook of bullying inschools (pp. 35–48). New York, NY: Routledge.

Nansel, T. R., Overpeck, M., Pilla, R. S., Ruan, W. J., Simons-

Morton, B., & Scheidt, P. (2001). Bullying behaviors among US

youth: Prevalence and association with psychosocial adjustment.

Journal of the American Medical Association, 285, 2094–2100.

Olweus, D. (2010). Understanding and researching bullying. In S.

R. Jimerson, S. M. Swearer, & D. L. Espelage (Eds.), Handbookof bullying in schools (pp. 9–34). New York, NY: Routledge.

Reynolds, R. J., Temple, J. A., Robertson, D. L., & Mann, E. A.

(2001). Long-term effects of an early childhood intervention on

School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128 127

123

Page 14: Enhancing Student Attitudes via a Brief, Universal-Level Bullying Prevention Curriculum

educational achievement and juvenile arrest. The Journal of theAmerican Medical Association, 285, 2339–2346.

Rigby, K. (2006). What we can learn from evaluated studies of

school-based programs to reduce bullying in schools. In S.

R. Jimerson & M. J. Furlong (Eds.), Handbook of schoolviolence and school safety (pp. 335–338). Mahwah, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Rigby, K., & Bauman, S. (2010). How school personnel tackle cases

of bullying: A critical examination. In S. R. Jimerson, S.

M. Swearer, & D. L. Espelage (Eds.), Handbook of bullying inschools: An international perspective (pp. 455–468). New York,

NY: Routledge.

Sameroff, A. J. (1995). General systems theories and developmental

psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), Develop-mental psychopathology (pp. 659–689). New York, NY: Wiley.

Sameroff, A. J. (2000). Dialectical processes in developmental

psychopathology. In A. J. Sameroff, M. Lewis, & S. M. Miller

(Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychopathology (2nd ed.,

pp. 23–40). New York, NY: Plenum.

Sameroff, A. J. (Ed.). (2009). The transactional model of develop-ment: How children and contexts shape each other. Washington,

DC: American Psychological Association.

Scott, T. M., Bucalos, A., Liaupsin, C. J., Nelson, C. M., Jolivette, K.,

& DeShea, L. (2004). Using functional behavioral assessment in

general education settings: Making a case for effectiveness and

efficiency. Behavioral Disorders, 29, 189–201.

Slavin, R. E. (2002). Evidence-based education policies: Transform-

ing educational practice and research. Educational Researcher,31(2), 15–21.

Smith, J. D., Schneider, B. H., Smith, P. K., & Ananiadou, K. (2004).

The effectiveness of whole-school antibullying programs: A

synthesis of evaluation research. School Psychology Review, 33,

547–560.

Song, S., Swearer, S. M., Haye, K. M., & Bandalos, D. L. (2001,

August). Development of a comprehensive survey for bullyingand victimization: Psychometric analyses. Poster presented at

the annual conference of the American Psychological Associa-

tion, San Francisco, CA.

Spivak, H., & Prothrow-Stith, D. (2001). The need to address bullying:

An important component of school violence prevention. Journalof the American Medical Association, 285, 2131–2132.

Swearer, S. M., Grills, A. E., Haye, K. M., & Cary, P. T. (2004).

Internalizing problems in students involved in bullying and

victimization: Implications for intervention. In D. L. Espelage &

S. M. Swearer (Eds.), Bullying in American schools: A social-ecological perspective on prevention and intervention (pp.

63–84). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariatestatistics (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Harper Collins.

Ttofi, M. M., Farrington, D. P., & Baldry, A. C. (2008). Effectivenessof programmes to reduce school bullying: A systematic review.

The Swedish National Council for Crime Prevention. Retrieved

from http://www.bra.se/extra/faq/?module_instance=2&action=

question_show&id=474&category_id=9.

Wolf, M. M. (1978). Social validity: The case for subjective

measurement or how applied behavior analysis is finding its

heart. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 203–214.

Zelezny, L. C. (1999). Educational interventions that improve

environmental behaviors: A meta-analysis. The Journal ofEnvironmental Education, 31, 5–14.

128 School Mental Health (2012) 4:115–128

123