150
i

Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    5

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

i

Page 2: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

ii

ABSTRACT

Numerous studies have attempted to identify a relationship between personality traits and

workplace performance. Workplace performance in this study was construed as

including perceptions of congruence with the type of work and performance on the job.

The Five-Factor Model (FFM) has been used to predict workplace performance across a

number of vocations including aviation and the Revised NEO Personality Instrument

(NEO-PI-R) is the most widely used FFM-based instrument. The NEO-PI-R has been

used with commercial and military aviators to identify personality trait levels that are

distinct from the general public and to predict performance in cockpit situations such as

during training. U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators have not been included in previous

studies using the NEO-PI-R. A sample of 75 experienced or career U.S. Army rotary-

wing aviators was given the NEO-PI-R in order to identify their personality profiles and

to see if personality trait levels varied when they were grouped according to the mission

platform that contained their preferred aircraft. Findings revealed a personality profile

consisting of average levels of Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness.

Neuroticism and Openness were in the low range. Scores between aviators when

grouped by mission platform revealed that only Agreeableness was significantly different

and this difference was found between Attack and Utility aviators. Limitations of this

study and implications for future research are discussed.

Page 3: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

iii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I would like to thank my dissertation committee for their assistance in completing

this dissertation. I want to thank Dr. Fredrick Milacci for serving as Chair of my

dissertation committee and the direction he provided through the development of this

study and through the writing process. I want to thank Dr. Lawrence Katz for serving on

my committee and his mentoring influence throughout the writing process. Appreciation

is also expressed to Dr. Kenneth Reeves for serving on my dissertation committee and for

providing guidance based upon his career experience in the U.S. Army.

I want to thank my family for their support during my years in school and the

writing of this dissertation. I want to thank my wife, Denita, for her love and support and

for believing in me through my graduate school experience. I want to thank my children -

Heather, Melissa, Taylor, and Robbie - for their willingness to adjust to the absence of

their father through much of the PhD program. I want to thank my parents and in-laws

for their support over the years.

Appreciation is expressed to others that made this dissertation possible. First,

appreciation is expressed to the staff at the U.S. Army Research Institute located at Fort

Rucker, Alabama. Special appreciation is expressed to Robert Antoskow of U.S. Army

Research Institute for his invaluable assistance. Second, I would like to express

appreciation to Dr. Robert Ruskin and the Consortium of Universities for their assistance

through my doctoral studies. Third, appreciation is expressed to Gary Waddell and the

staff at Providence Christian School for their support during my doctoral studies.

Page 4: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

iv

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

LIST OF TABLES…………………………………………………………………. viii

LIST OF APPENDICES…………………………………………………………… ix

Chapter

I. THE PROBLEM…………………………………………………………........... 1

Introduction……………………………………………………………………... 1

Problem Background…………………………………………………………… 3

Purpose of the Study………….………………………………………………… 7

Research Questions………...…………………………………………………… 7

Limitations/Delimitations……………………….……………………………… 7

Definitions……………………………………………………………………… 9

Importance of the Study………….……………………………………………... 12

Implications……………..……………………………………………………… 13

II. REVIEW OF LITERATURE………………………………………………….. 14

Introduction…………..………………………………………………………… 14

Theoretical Assumptions…………...…………………………………………... 15

Trait Personality Theory……………………………………………………….. 15

Traits Defined……………………………………………………………….. 15

Trait Theory………………………………………………………………… 16

Trait Quantity………………………………………………………………. 18

Trait Stability……………………………………………………………….. 18

Page 5: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

v

Chapter Page

Trait and Job Fit……………………………………………………………. 24

Five-Factor Model…………………………………………………………….. 26

History………………………………………………………..……………. 28

Factors…………………………..…………………………………………. 29

Neuroticism…………………………………...………………………… 29

Extraversion…………...………………………………………………... 30

Openness…………...…………………………………………………… 30

Agreeableness…………...……………………………………………… 31

Conscientiousness……………..……………………………………….. 31

Facets………...…………………………………………………………….. 33

Neuroticism…………...………………………………………………… 33

Extraversion…………...………………………………………………... 34

Openness………...……………………………………………………… 35

Agreeableness…………...……………………………………………… 36

Conscientiousness…………...………………………………………….. 38

NEO-PI-R………...………………………………………………………… 39

Research Applications……………...………………………………………. 40

Aviator Personality Research………………...........…………………………... 42

Anecdotal Descriptions……………………………………………………... 42

Empirical Research…………...……………………………………………. 43

FFM and Aviation Research…………………………………………………... 49

Page 6: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

vi

Chapter Page

Commercial Aviator Research…………...………………………………… 49

U.S. Navy/Marine Corps Aviator Research…………..……………………. 50

U.S. Air Force Aviator Research………………..…………………...…….. 51

U.S. Army Rotary-Wing Aviator Research…………………..……………. 53

Summary……………….……………………………………………………… 58

III. METHOD…………………………………………………………………… 61

Research Design…………………………….……………………….………… 61

Selection of Sample……………………….……………………………….. 61

Instrumentation………….…………………………………………………. 62

Structure………………………………………………………………… 62

Reliability……………………………………………………………….. 63

Validity………………………………………………………………….. 64

Assumptions/Limitations…………………………………………………... 66

Procedures……………………………………………………………………... 67

Data Analysis………………………………………………………………….. 69

Research Question 1………………………………………………………... 69

Research Question 2………………………………………………………... 70

IV. FINDINGS…………………………………………………………………… 71

Purpose of Study………………………………………………………………. 71

Sample……………………………………………………………………… 71

Page 7: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

vii

Chapter Page

Findings………………………………………………………………………... 72

Demographics…………………………………………………………... 72

Research Question 1………………………………………………………... 73

Total Sample……………………………………………………………. 74

Research Question 2………………………………………………………. 75

Statistical Comparisons………………………...………………………. 76

Additional Findings…………………….…………………………………... 77

Summary……………………………………………………………………….. 78

V. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS……………. 80

Summary………………………………………………………………………. 80

Conclusions……………………………………………………………………. 80

Research Question 1…………………………………………………...…… 80

Research Question 2……………………………………………………..… 82

Additional Findings……………….…………..……………………………. 83

Implications…………………………...………………………………………. 85

Implications for Practice……………………..…………………………….. 85

Implications for Research…………………………………..……………… 86

Recommendations…………………..…………………………………………. 86

Summary………………………………………………………………………. 88

REFERENCES………………………………………………………....……… 90

APPENDICES………………………………………………..………..……… 115

Page 8: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

viii

LIST OF TABLES

Table Page

1. Descriptive Statistics of Total Sample NEO-PI-R Factor Scores………... 118

2. Comparison of Total Sample and Mission Platform Scores……………... 119

3. Ranking of Total Sample Scores…………………………………………. 120

4. Multivariate Analysis of Platform and Personality Domains……………. 121

5. Analysis of Variance for the Five-Factor Model Factors………………… 122

6. Pairwise Comparisons Between Mission Platforms……………………… 123

7. Comparisons of Agreeableness Facet Scores…………………………….. 130

8. Pairwise Comparisons of Trust Between Mission Platforms…………….. 139

9. Comparison of U.S. Air Force and U.S. Army Aviators NEO-PI-RScores…………………………………………………………………… 141

Page 9: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

ix

LIST OF APPENDICES

Appendix Page

A. DEMOGRAPHIC QUESTIONNAIRE……………………………………... 115

B. INFORMED CONSENT……………………………………………….. 116

Page 10: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

1

CHAPTER I: THE PROBLEM

Introduction

Movies such as Top Gun and The Right Stuff have popularized the idea that

aviators possess certain personality trait levels that distinguished them from the general

population. Research lends support to the assumption that aviators often exhibit

personality trait levels that distinguish them from the general public (Callister, King,

Retzlaff, & Marsh, 1997; 1999; Fitzgibbons, Davis, & Schutte, 2004) and that personality

trait levels can predict cockpit performance (e.g., mission success, teamwork, risk-

taking), especially during flight training (Anesgart & Callister, 2001). The possibility

that a link exists between personality traits and performance as an aviator suggests that

certain personality traits are better-suited for aviation and argues for further personality

research with aviators.

United States Army aviation maintains an ongoing commitment to improving

aviator performance in the cockpit as well as an interest in retaining high-performing

aviators past an initial enlistment. For instance, the Selection Instrument for Flight

Training (SIFT) research project began in 2004 and is focused on reviewing and

redeveloping the criteria used in selecting future U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators and in

aircraft assignment decisions that occur in an aviator’s career. Personality is one factor

under review that might warrant consideration as one of the criteria in initial aviator

selection to flight school or aircraft assignment (i.e. classification) decisions that occur

following flight school.

Page 11: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

2

The intent of this study was to investigate the possibility that U.S. Army rotary-

wing aviators might exhibit a unique personality profile as a total sample in comparison

to the general public and to see if personality trait levels fluctuated in comparisons

between the four mission platforms where these aviators are assigned. Mission platform

is used to denote the general task or role that is performed by each aircraft. For instance,

Attack aircraft are primarily used in combat environments while Cargo aircraft primarily

move large-scale supplies or large numbers of personnel. Utility aircraft primarily move

small groups of personnel or lightweight supplies and provide combat support roles while

Scout/Observation aircraft gather reconnaissance information in combat environments.

The sample for this study was drawn from experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing

aviators on active flight status and who possessed the military rank of Chief Warrant 3

(CW3), Chief Warrant 4 (CW4) or Chief Warrant 5 (CW5). Warrant Officer aviators

were used because they typically accumulate more actual flight time in the cockpit that

commissioned officers (Marshburn & Rollin, 2005). The rationale for using experienced

aviators was based on the likelihood that (a) these aviators had demonstrated sufficient

performance in the past to achieve their current military rank and (b) their tenure

suggested they perceived enough satisfaction and congruence with the lifestyle of an U.S.

Army aviator to make U.S. Army aviation their career.

The Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992)

was used in this study. The NEO-PI-R is based upon the Five-Factor Model (FFM; Costa

& McCrae, 1985) of personality which measures personality traits across the factor

dimensions of Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and

Conscientiousness. Though the NEO-PI-R has been used with other types of aviators to

Page 12: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

3

predict performance (e.g., Anesgart & Callister, 2001), this was the first reported study to

use this instrument with U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators.

The lack of U.S. Army aviation research using the NEO-PI-R has not gone

unnoticed. For example, Callister et al. (1997) expressed confidence in generalizing their

NEO-PI-R findings with U.S. Air Force aviators to U.S. Navy/Marine Corps aviators, but

refused to generalize to U.S. Army aviators. They reasoned, “Generalizing to U.S. Army

aviators should be done only with considerable care since many U.S. Army aviators are

Warrant Officers, many without college degrees” (p. 6). One of the goals of this study

was to address the lack of findings with U.S. Army aviators by making an initial step in

determining if a particular NEO-PI-R profile seems to be most indicative of career U.S.

Army aviators. If so, this might lend support to the notion that certain personality traits

should be considered in the selection and classification of future U.S. Army aviators.

Problem Background

Approximately 1,200 new U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators are trained each year at

Fort Rucker, Alabama at a cost of approximately $225,000 per student (Colucci, 2002).

The journey for new aviators begins at the selection phase with the completion of a

written test that measures aviation relevant skills, general spatial and mathematical

ability, and attitudes. Applicants must complete an interview with a current U.S. Army

aviator and successfully pass a flight physical. Applicants are selected for flight school

based upon their overall scores in the different components of the selection process.

Applicants can transfer from other U.S. Army job specialties or from other branches of

the military. One feature unique to U.S. Army aviation is that applicants are not required

to have a college degree.

Page 13: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

4

Aviators selected for rotary-wing flight school or Initial Entry Rotary-Wing

(IERW) attend flight training at Fort Rucker, Alabama. Flight training lasts for

approximately 9 to 10 months depending upon the length of time required for advanced

flight training at the end of IERW. Successful graduates from flight training move to the

next phase of their initial training which involves aircraft assignment or classification.

The U.S. Army determines aircraft assignments for the majority of new aviators

based upon the projected needs of the U.S. Army. Advanced training in an assigned

aircraft lasts from one to two months. The classification phase ends once the aviator has

successfully completed advanced training and has become qualified or rated to pilot that

aircraft. Each aircraft performs a different role or function known as mission platform in

U.S. Army aviation. Many aviators are rated in more than one aircraft during their

careers.

Personnel can be assigned to fly one or more aircraft in their career from four

primary mission platforms: Attack, Scout/Observation, Utility, and Cargo. Attack

aircraft, including the AH-64A, Apache, and AH-64D, Apache Longbow, are primarily

designed for offensive combat missions. They primarily provide air artillery support for

ground troops using air-to-ground missiles. Scout/Observation aircraft, such as the OH-

58D, Kiowa, provide information-gathering functions in the field. These aircraft are small

and are designed so they are not that easily seen by the enemy. Utility aircraft, such as

the UH-60A, Black Hawk, provide transportation of light-weight supplies and small

groups of personnel. Cargo aircraft, such as the CH-47D, Chinook, are larger aircraft

capable of moving heavy supplies or transporting larger groups of personnel.

Page 14: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

5

Person-environment fit research suggests that matching individual characteristics

such as personality with the task demands of a vocational environment will likely

increase performance and tenure (e.g., Edwards, 1996; Holland, 1985; Assouline & Meir,

1987). U.S. Army aviation would appear to benefit from personality findings that might

predict aviator performance and tenure given the significant investment it makes in

aviation training each year. Experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators would appear

to be the best population to sample in order to identify these personality traits even

though there has been some caution expressed about attempting to generalize one

personality profile to all aviators (Chidester, Helmreich, Gregorich, & Geis, 1991;

Retzlaff & Gibertini, 1987).

An initial step in measuring the personality traits of U.S. Army rotary-wing

aviators is determining the appropriate instrument to use with this population.

Personality instruments such as the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory

(MMPI) (Hathaway & McKinley, 1943), the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality

Inventory – 2 (MMPI-2) (Butcher, Dahlstrom, Graham, Tellegen, & Kaemmer, 1989),

and the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory (MCMI) (Millon, 1977) with its subsequent

editions may provide a means for measuring aviator personality traits. However, their

use may be inappropriate with this population because they are not normed for high-

functioning aviators, but are designed for clinical populations (Retzlaff & Gibertini,

1987).

A more promising tactic appears to be the use of a trait-based approach to

personality that measures normal or non-pathological personality trait levels and which

Page 15: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

6

has been used successfully in the past to predict performance in vocational settings

(Barrick & Mount, 1991; Callister et al., 1997; Mount, Barrick, & Stewart, 1998).

The NEO-PI-R is a trait-based instrument that has a history of use in studies attempting

to link personality with workplace performance including with aviators (Anesgart &

Callister, 2001; Fitzgibbons et al., 2004). For instance, Anesgart and Callister (2001)

found that scores on the Neuroticism factor could predict attrition from flight training for

U.S. Air Force aviators.

The NEO-PI-R has received widespread support for its comprehensive coverage

of the general domains of “normal” personality traits based upon the factors and facets of

the FFM (Bernard & Walsh, 2004; Callister, King, Lanier, & Etterlie, 1995; Callister et

al., 1997; Costa & McCrae, 1992; Paunonen & Ashton, 2001). The FFM’s

conceptualization of personality has received validation across vocations including with

aviators. For instance, the factors of the FFM have received empirical support for their

ability to identify the personality traits of aviators from studies utilizing other trait-based

instruments (Helton & Street, 1993; Street & Helton, 1993). The FFM has been

identified as the best approach to use in making U.S. Air Force aircrew selection

decisions (Pedersen, Allan, Laue, Johnson, & Siem, 1992).

The volume of personality research with U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators is small

which limits the comparisons that can be made with previous findings. A search of the

primary academic search engines EBSCOHost, Annual Reviews of Psychology, and

PsycINFO as well as the Defense Technical Information Center (DTIC) search engine of

military technical reports yielded three studies of U.S. Army aviator personality

(Caldwell, O’Hara, Caldwell, Stephens, & Krueger, 1993; Geist & Boyd, 1980; Picano,

Page 16: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

7

1991). Two (Caldwell et al.; Geist & Boyd, 1980) of these studies used the MMPI and

one (Picano, 1991) used the Occupational Personality Questionnaire (OPQ). Only Picano

(1991) considered experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators and identified his sample

with similar criteria as was used in this study.

Purpose of the Study

The purpose of this study was to identify the personality profiles of experienced

U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators based upon the personality factors of the FFM. The

factor scores were measured by using the NEO-PI-R. Additionally, the study sought to

determine if the personality trait levels of these aviators were significantly different

across the U.S. Army’s four rotary-wing mission platforms (i.e. Attack,

Scout/Observation, Cargo, and Utility).

Research Questions

• Research Question #1: What are the personality profiles of experienced U.S.

Army rotary-wing aviators?

• Research Question #2: Do experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviator

personality profiles differ significantly across the U.S. Army’s four mission

platforms?

Limitations/Delimitations

There are two limitations associated with this study. First, the sample size was

less than 100 (n=75) and sample sizes fluctuated once the aviators were grouped

according to mission platform which could raise concerns about the level of confidence

that should be extended to the findings of this study. Second, there is an absence of

Page 17: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

8

studies using the NEO-PI-R with U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators. Steps have been taken

to mitigate the limitations cited.

First, the sample size was citied as a limitation because it is generally assumed

that larger samples increase the level of confidence placed in findings (Portney &

Watkins, 2000). An increase in sample size could cause trait levels to fluctuate and

change their ranking of importance. For example, a larger sample’s scores could move

their ranking in a factor from low to average or from average to high.

A problem in studying populations such as experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing

aviators is accessibility. A solution for the accessibility dilemma was to survey the

experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators that attended advanced leadership training at

the Warrant Officer Career Center (WOCC) located at Fort Rucker, Alabama from

October 2005 to January 2006. Ninety surveys were provided WOCC personnel based

upon estimates of the number of aviators likely to attend this training. Experienced U.S.

Army rotary-wing aviators serving as WOCC training personnel were also included in the

study resulting in a return of 77 survey packets. Two surveys were incomplete and were

not used resulting in a sample size of 75.

Second, the absence of studies using the NEO-PI-R with this population is a

limitation because there are no findings from previous research to use for comparison.

The usage of the NEO-PI-R in this study was based upon a review of the literature and

the conclusion that it was appropriate to use with U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators

because; (a) it focuses on general domains of personality and measures normal

personality traits rather than narrow aspects or psychopathology; (b) it is commonly used

in studies examining the fit between individual personality and job characteristics (e.g.,

Page 18: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

9

Mount et al., 1998; Tett & Burnett, 2003); and (c) it has been effective in measuring the

personality profiles of military aviators (Callister et al., 1997), and commercial pilots

(Fitzgibbons et al., 2004).

Definitions

Agreeableness. Agreeableness is the personality characteristic or disposition

exemplified by behavioral descriptions such as courteous, good-natured, flexible,

cooperative, and tolerant. Agreeableness is an indicator of how well a person will belong

to groups engaged in tasks because task completion by teams typically requires high

levels of effective social interaction.

Classification. Classification involves successful completion of flight training

and becoming rated or qualified to pilot an aircraft. In the U.S. Army, classification is

typically the result of an aviator being assigned to an aircraft based upon the needs of

U.S. Army aviation during IERW training and reclassification in a different aircraft can

occur at different points in an aviator’s career based upon U.S. Army needs or some

reclassifications can occur due to the expressed desires of the aviator.

Conscientiousness. Conscientiousness is the personality characteristic or

disposition exemplified by descriptions such as responsible, orderly, dependable, and

persevering. Conscientiousness indicates the amount of forethought and level of

commitment a person gives to task completion.

Domains. “Domain” can be used synonymously with “factors” in the FFM and

describes dimensions of personality (e.g., Digman, 1990). In the FFM framework this

would include the five factors that represent the broad dimensions of personality.

Page 19: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

10

Experienced U.S. Army Rotary-Wing Aviators. Experienced rotary-wing aviators

in this study are defined as those U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators having achieved the

minimum rank of CW3. Promotion to CW3 generally requires a minimum of 10 years

and possibly longer to achieve. Promotion to CW4 and CW5 typically occur around 20

years of service. These aviators are qualified or rated to fly at least one U.S. Army

rotary-wing aircraft and are likely flying this aircraft in some capacity (e.g., supervisor,

instructor pilot) at this time. These aviators are assumed to have achieved a record of

high performance and sufficient adaptation to U.S. Army aviation that tenure is possible

and desired.

Extraversion. Extraversion is the personality characteristic or disposition

exemplified by descriptions such as sociable, assertive, talkative, and achievement-

oriented. Extraversion can be an indicator of the level of teamwork commitment a person

possesses including their willingness to communicate and assume prominent roles within

a team.

Five Factor Model (FFM). The FFM developed out of over a half-century of trait

personality research. Costa and McCrae (1985) offered the contemporary labels for the

FFM. Their labels are Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience (or

Openness), Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness.

Mission Platform. Mission platform is used to denote the type of tasks or primary

role a particular aircraft performs for U.S. Army aviation. The four primary mission

platforms are Attack, Scout/Observation, Cargo, and Utility.

Page 20: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

11

• Attack Aircraft. Attack aircraft such as the AH-64A Apache and the AH-64D

Apache Longbow are designed for offensive combat roles on the battlefield and

provide air artillery support for troops on the ground.

• Scout/Observation. The observation aircraft (i.e. OH-58D Kiowa) primarily

function as a scout to gather reconnaissance information on enemy activity. These

aircraft are small in order to reduce the likelihood of detection.

• Cargo Aircraft. Cargo aircraft such as the CH-47D Chinook are primarily tasked

with cargo movement of supplies (e.g., ammunition, artillery) and can transport

large numbers of personnel. These large aircraft are not designed for direct

combat functions so they often operate as a combat support behind the frontlines

of battle.

• Utility Aircraft. The utility aircraft such as the UH-60A Black Hawk is a light

utility transport aircraft that can be called upon to move light-weight supplies or

small groups of personnel in both combat and non-combat environments.

Neuroticism. Neuroticism is the personality characteristic or disposition

exemplified by descriptions such as anxious, depressed, insecure, and angry. This factor

can share the label Emotional Stability since low Neuroticism is assumed to indicate

stable emotionality and affect.

Openness. Openness is the personality characteristic or disposition exemplified

by descriptions such as imaginative, artistic, curious, and tolerant. Openness is the

willingness to look for novel solutions to problems and to resist narrow procedures

determined by others.

Page 21: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

12

Performance. Performance is defined as the work outcomes of U.S. Army

aviators related to training scores and supervisor ratings among factors such as technical

proficiency and teamwork commitment. The goals of performance are mission success

and aircrew safety. Performance is a key component of promotion decisions.

Selection. Selection is the process of choosing new aviation students from a large

pool of applicants based upon: (a) test battery scores of general knowledge, skills, and

attributes (e.g., personality); (b) successfully passing a flight physical; and (c) receiving

endorsement based upon an interview with an active U.S. Army aviator.

Trait Personality. Trait personality is the theoretical approach that identifies

broad personality characteristics with descriptive labels such as extraverted (e.g.,

sociable, outgoing), agreeable (e.g., cooperative), and conscientious (e.g., responsible,

orderly). Traits typically include personality dimensions of dispositions and interests.

Importance of the Study

The importance of this study applies primarily to the assignment or classification

of U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators to rotary-wing aircraft during their careers. The FFM

has been identified as the best approach to use in making aircrew selection decisions

(Pedersen et al., 1992). Research has found the NEO-PI-R can reveal distinct levels of

personality traits in aviators that are not shared by the general public (Callister et al.,

1997; Fitzgibbons et al., 2004). However, as Callister et al. noted, caution should be

exercised in generalizing these findings to U.S. Army aviators since allowing WOs to be

aviators is a policy unique to U.S. Army Aviation. Findings such as those generated by

this study might possibly provide a strategy for mitigating the record loss of WO aviators

to other aviation organizations (Marshburn & Rollin, 2005).

Page 22: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

13

Implications

The findings of this study received a two-fold examination. First, the findings

from the NEO-PI-R were used to compile a personality profile for the experienced U.S.

Army rotary-wing aviators sampled in this study. This was the first reported study that

has offered a personality profile of U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators based upon the FFM.

Second, personality trait levels were evaluated to see if they were significantly different

across the four primary types of missions (i.e. mission platforms) suggesting that certain

personality trait levels are more congruent with characteristics of one mission than the

other three. Experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators could provide helpful

information in understanding the personality traits of aviators and possibly indicate

personality traits that could predict aviation performance.

Page 23: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

14

CHAPTER II: REVIEW OF LITERATURE

Introduction

U.S. Army aviation has a proud history of service dating back to the Korean

Conflict. The roles of U.S. Army rotary-wing aircraft have evolved over the years to

include transportation, attack, and reconnaissance. Each role is defined as a mission

platform, namely Attack, Scout/Observation, Utility, and Cargo. Aircraft are specifically

designed and built to perform these missions and aviators for each type of aircraft require

their own specialized training.

Individual characteristics such as personality traits might influence the level of

congruence perceived by aviators with their aircraft and the type of missions they

perform. Individual characteristics include personality traits, especially when traits are

construed as including interests, dispositions, and preferences (Barrick, Mount, & Gupta,

2003). Person-environment (P-E) fit theory posits that workplace performance will

increase when individuals perceive congruence or a match between their traits and the

tasks of the vocation as well as with the ancillary factors (e.g., culture, values, incentives)

of the workplace environment (e.g., Edwards, 1996; Kieffer, Schinka, & Curtiss, 2004),

Intano, Howse, and Lofaro (1991) suggested that matching individual traits with the task

demands of each mission platform might indicate “the aircraft for which the [aviation]

candidate might be expected to have the highest probability of successfully completing

flight training and of having a successful flight career” (p. 15).

Page 24: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

15

A trait-based approach, such as the Five-Factor Model (FFM; Costa & McCrae,

1985), has become a common approach in studies attempting to link personality traits

with job performance (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Mount et al., 1998). The Revised NEO

Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992) has emerged as the most

commonly used of the FFM measures (Bernard & Walsh, 2004). Some of the proposed

outcomes of achieving congruence between personal traits like personality and workplace

characteristics are better job performance, higher job satisfaction, and a greater likelihood

of tenure (Holland, 1985; 1992).

There are no reported studies using the FFM or the NEO-PI-R with U.S. Army

rotary-wing aviators. Studies have supported the ability of the FFM and the NEO-PI-R to

identify distinctive personality traits levels with other types of aviators (Anesgart &

Callister, 2001; Fitzgibbons et al., 2004)). Helton and Street (1993) found that using

other trait-based personality instruments with U.S. Navy aviators produced results

consistent with the domains of the FFM. A rationale can be offered for using the NEO-

PI-R based upon (a) the theoretical assumptions of trait personality theory, (b) an

examination of the FFM and the NEO-PI-R, and (c) a review of past aviator personality

research where the FFM was used.

Theoretical Assumptions

Trait Personality Theory

Traits Defined. Traits have been described as the “building blocks of personality”

(Ryckman, 1989, p. 274), as the set of factors within a person that explains their behavior

(e.g., temperaments and genetically controlled dispositions), and as a person’s

interpersonal characteristics that generally remain constant across situations (Burger,

Page 25: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

16

1997; Johnson, 1999). Personality traits have also been referred to as an individual’s

social reputation and inner nature (Hogan, 1991) that may influence their responses to a

wide array of circumstances (Wortman & Loftus, 1988). McCrae and Costa (1990)

defined traits as “dimensions of individual differences in tendencies to show consistent

patterns of thoughts, feelings, and actions” (p. 23). Personality traits include dispositions,

interests, and temperaments as well as specific personality characteristics (Barrick, et al.,

2003) and they generally manifest in response to situational or environmental cues that

occur during person-environment interactions (Tett & Guterman, 2000). More relevant

for this study is the approach to personality that stresses the interchangeability of

personality with other concepts such as traits, states, interests, attitudes, and generalized

dispositions (Milgram, 1991).

Trait Theory. Trait-based personality theory has a rich history dating back to

Francis Galton’s (1884) scan of a dictionary and subsequent proposition of a list of 1,000

words that could describe personality. McDougall (1932) raised interest in identifying

personality traits and concluded that, “Personality may be broadly analyzed into five

distinguishable but separate factors, namely, intellect, character, temperament,

disposition, and temper…each of these is highly complex [and] comprises many

variables” (p. 15). Allport (1937; Allport & Obert, 1936) used the dictionary to identify

terms that described personality traits resulting in 4,504 terms that “designate generalized

and personalized determining tendencies” (Allport, p. 306). Allport (1937) described

personality as, “the dynamic organization with the individual of those psychophysical

systems that determine his unique adjustments to his environment” (p. 48). The

Page 26: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

17

production of such a lengthy list of traits motivated numerous studies to determine if the

list could be shortened and still provide comprehensive coverage of general personality.

Cattell (1943, 1946, 1947, 1948) produced a list of descriptive labels that

identified the various dimensions of personality according to broadly defined factors.

Cattell’s taxonomy consisted of 16 primary factors and eight second-order factors which

came to serve as the basis for the 16PF personality measure. Cattell (Cattell, 1965;

Cattell, Eber, & Tatsuoka, 1970) offered that traits, such as those he identified, would

predict what a person will do when placed in a given situation. Concerns over the

complexity and reliability of Cattell’s model motivated further research in the field of

trait theory to see if Cattell’s labels were a sufficient representation of general personality

traits (Fiske, 1949; Tupes 1957).

The studies of Fiske (1949) and Tupes and Christal (Tupes, 1957; Tupes &

Christal, 1961) failed to replicate Cattell’s findings. Instead, they found that personality

can be adequately represented across five dimensions rather than 16. Fiske (1949) put

forward five broad dimensions (Social Adaptability, Conformity, Conscientiousness,

Emotional Control, and Inquiring Intellect). Four subsequent studies corroborated the

findings of Fiske and Tupes and Christal (Borgatta, 1964; Hakel, 1974; Norman, 1963;

Smith, 1967). Norman’s findings were probably the most notable because he proposed

the five domains of Extraversion, Emotional Stability, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness,

and Culture as providing an adequate conceptualizing of the broad dimensions of normal

personality traits. Norman’s labels became known as the “Big Five” and served as the

standard five-factor approach for over 20 years.

Page 27: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

18

Trait Quantity. Though much of the early research (e.g., Fiske, 1949; Norman,

1963; Tupes & Christal, 1961) and more contemporary studies (e.g., Barrick & Mount,

1991; Costa & McCrae, 1992; 1995; 1997; Digman, 1989; 1990) seem to support a five-

factor approach to conceptualizing broad domains of normal personality, there has been

disagreement expressed over what should be the number of personality traits. Hogan

(1983) suggested six dimensions of personality which include Ambition, Sociability,

Likeability, Prudence, Adjustment, and Intellectance. Eysenck (1970) offered that human

personality can be described with the factors labels of Psychoticism, Extraversion, and

Neuroticism. Others have offered a six-factor approach to personality (Ashton, Lee, &

Son, 2000), but there is general disagreement over what a sixth factor should be (Cellar,

Miller, Doverspike, & Klawsky, 1996). Hogan resolved the issue by borrowing from the

Big Five dimension of Extraversion and dividing it into Ambition and Sociability.

Since Norman (1963) proposed his Big Five taxonomy, numerous studies have

supported a five-factor description of personality (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Costa &

McCrae, 1985; Digman, 1989; 1990; Goldberg, 1981). Concerns over clarifying

Norman’s traits such as Culture has led to the suggestion that labels such as Intellect or

Intellectence (Digman & Takemoto-Chock, 1981; Hogan, 1983) and Openness to

Experience (Costa & McCrae, 1985) might provide a better explanation of the Culture

factor since it includes being imaginative, cultured, intelligent, and broad-minded. Costa

and McCrae’s (1985) factor labels became the standard identifiers of the FFM and the

theoretical basis for Costa and McCrae’s NEO-PI-R.

Trait Stability. The stability of personality trait levels across the lifespan has been

a matter of research interest for much of the past century (e.g., Helson, 1999, Soldz &

Page 28: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

19

Vaillant, 1999). Trait personality theorists in the early 1980’s supported the stability of

personality trait levels across the lifespan (Costa & McCrae, 1985; Terracciano, McCrae,

Brant, & Costa, 2005). Trait theories such as the FFM construed personality traits as

being biologically-based and stable across the lifespan (Costa & McCrae, 1985; 1992;

McCrae & Costa, 1996; Sheldon, Ryan, Rawsthorne, & Ilardi, 1997). McCrae and Costa

(1994) proposed that by age 30 personality trait levels are set and remain stable through

adulthood if cognitive functions remain intact.

Proponents of trait fluctuations posit that trait levels fluctuate over the lifespan

due to the experiences related to developmental changes and person-environment

interactions such as family roles and social/vocational expectations that typically come

with age (Caspi & Roberts, 1999; 2001; Fleeson & Heckhausen, 1997; Hogan, 1996;

Srivastava, John, Gosling, & Potter, 2003). For instance, Srivastava et al. put forward

that increases in Conscientiousness and Agreeableness into adulthood might be due to the

increased demands for order, responsibility, and sociability that typically come with

assuming adult roles related to work and family. The difference in Conscientiousness

and Agreeableness has been noted between adults and college-aged populations as well

as between college-aged respondents and adolescents (Sherry, Henson, & Lewis, 2003).

Even past proponents of trait stability such as Costa and McCrae acknowledge that some

flexibility in their trait stability stance might be in order due to natural human maturation

or aging (Costa, Herbst, McCrae, & Siegler, 2000; McCrae & Costa, 2003).

McCrae and Costa (2003) reported that based upon their review of longitudinal

and cross-sectional studies, the FFM factors do seem to fluctuate over time with

Neuroticism and Extraversion decreasing between late adolescence and age 30 while

Page 29: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

20

Agreeableness and Conscientiousness were found to increase. Openness was found to

first increase and then decrease. Longitudinal and cross-sectional studies of individuals

over 30 years of age have found that Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Openness

experience a normative decline of 1 to 2 T score points per decade (Costa et al., 2000;

Terracciano et al., 2005). Terracciano et al. reported in their review of longitudinal

studies between 1989 and 2004 that there was a decline in Neuroticism up to age 80

while levels of Extraversion and Openness remained stable across much of this period.

Scores in Agreeableness and Conscientiousness increased up to age 70.

Environmental influences on personality trait levels can come from a number of

sources. For example, adapting to the workplace and self-monitoring in social

interactions may influence trait levels over time. Workplace environments often

necessitate the need to adapt personal characteristics such as personality trait levels to

match a vocation as well as the organizational culture of the workplace (Caspi, Roberts,

& Shiner, 2005; Low, Yoon, Roberts, & Rounds, 2005). The desire to adapt individual

characteristics to the match the expectations or requirements of the workplace is typically

accomplished through self-monitoring (Barrick, Parks, & Mount, 2005).

Self-monitoring is the desire to present a self-image in social interactions that is

positive and acceptable in the workplace (Barrick et al., 2005). The image created and

maintained through self-monitoring can conflict with a desire to maintain a sense of well-

being and authenticity (e.g., Barrick, Mount, & Judge, 2001; Caspi et al., 2005; Fleeson

& Heckhausen, 1997; Mischel & Shoda, 1998; Roberts, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2003).

Personality can be one aspect influencing perceptions of authenticity. The resolution

between a desire to be authentic and the social/vocational expectations an individual

Page 30: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

21

experiences could involve adjustments in perceptions of authenticity. For instance, the

demand of organizational skills in the workplace might necessitate high

Conscientiousness and the maintenance of a self-image at work that demonstrates high

Conscientiousness might influence Conscientiousness scores for individual, who would

otherwise not score high on this FFM factor.

Human development may influence trait fluctuations (Caspi & Roberts, 2001;

Fleeson & Heckhausen, 1997). Changing goals and priorities are not uncommon with

age and new roles (Feji, van der Velde, Taris, & Taris, 1999; Fleeson & Heckhausen,

1997; Roberts & Robins, 2004; Srivastava et al., 2003). Erikson (1950) described human

development as occurring in response to an individual’s negotiation and mastery of stage-

specific life tasks with mastery at one stage initiating a new maturation stage involving

new tasks, roles, and priorities. It seems reasonable to assume that developmental

changes such as these would impact personality, at least to some degree.

Explanations can be offered to resolve the disagreement over the stability of

personality traits. One explanation is that the disagreement is semantic. It could be that

the significant changes in personality over time touted by some are actually

representative of fluctuations in narrow personality traits or facets, rather than broad

dimensional categories related to general trait levels and dispositions such as with the

five factors of the FFM. Narrow facets or traits might be more reactive to environmental

or human development influence than will be the overall personality profile of a person.

For example, a person typically scoring high in a personality dimension such as

Conscientiousness could experience a range of fluctuations at any given moment among

Page 31: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

22

the facets of Conscientiousness, but he or she will likely score consistently high in this

dimension over time as part of the overall personality profile.

A second explanation for trait fluctuations is the possibility that personality traits

manifest in response to psychodynamic processes that are a normal part of human

development. For example, Adler (1979) proposed that feelings of inferiority are a

common feature of early personality. Corey (1996) commented:

Indeed, at around 6 years of age, our fictional vision of ourselves as perfect is

formed into a life goal. The life goal unifies the personality and becomes the

source of human motivation; every striving and every effort to overcome

inferiority is now in line with this goal (p. 135).

Early interactions with environmental factors such as the family of origin can result in

either healthy psychological adjustment or personality maladjustment (Mozak, 2000).

If Adler is correct, it seems possible that striving for mastery, superiority, and

success would influence personality trait levels. At the same time, it might be that

personality trait levels influence the strategies employed in seeking to achieve mastery.

For instance, a child high on Extraversion might find social interactions to be less

stressful than those scoring low on Extraversion and so may pursue more opportunities to

engage in social activities that reinforce a sense of confidence and competence. High

scorers on Conscientiousness might discover they experience success and superiority by

being high achievers.

A third explanation for trait fluctuations over time is the influence of social

pressure to adjust personality trait levels. Research (e.g., Caspi & Roberts, 2001; Low et

al., 2005) has demonstrated that individuals can adapt their personalities to fit within

Page 32: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

23

social situations. Incongruence between the personality traits that are desired by a

social/vocational situation and an individual’s personality trait levels creates pressure to

either adjust to the expectations of the social group or to seek a different situation that is

more congruent with the personality of the individual. It seems reasonable to assume that

the conscious decision to adjust personality trait levels to fit within a particular

social/vocational situation could result in more permanent changes over time.

The possibility that trait levels fluctuate over time does not detract from the value

of trait-based personality instruments such as the NEO-PI-R. The NEO-PI-R might

reveal a match between current trait-related interests and job characteristics. This

information could prove helpful in identifying possible links between personality and job

outcomes such as performance (Kieffer et al., 2004), satisfaction (Edwards, 1996), and

tenure (O’Reilly, Chatman, & Caldwell, 1991). The best strategy for mitigating the

impact of trait level changes over time appears to be periodic personality reassessments

(Thoresen, Bradley, Bliese, & Thoresen, 2004).

The value of reassessment is the ability to monitor potential changes in vocational

interests that accompany trait-level fluctuations. Failure to recognize when a change of

interest has occurred can negatively impact job performance and increase the likelihood

of attrition (Assouline & Meir, 1987; Caplan, 1987; de Jong, van der Velde, & Jansen,

2001, p. 350). Pervin (1968) noted nearly four decades ago that when people begin to

lose interest in their current vocation it leads to a perception of “lack of fit” which will

eventually result in decreased performance and dissatisfaction. For example, the possible

loss of interest in flying one aircraft or mission platform could reduce performance or

safety-consciousness and potentially prompt attrition from U.S. Army aviation.

Page 33: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

24

Traits and Job Fit. Trait theory has proven helpful in vocational research as a

method for predicting workplace performance. The focus of person-environment fit

theory is that congruence between personal traits such as personality, interests,

dispositions, and values and workplace characteristics such as tasks, work environment,

and workplace values will likely result in desired workplace outcomes. For instance,

congruence between the individual and the workplace is believed to improve workplace

performance (Kieffer et al., 2004) and result in greater motivation (Schmitt, Cortina,

Ingerick, & Wiechmann, 2003), higher job satisfaction (Edwards, 1996; Latham &

Pinder, 2005) and tenure (Assouline & Meir, 1987; O’Reilly et al., 1991). In other

words, greater performance and higher job satisfaction are likely to increase when

individuals perceive that their personality, skills, dispositions, interests, values, and goals

match job characteristics like task requirements and organizational values (Barrick et al.,

2003; Ostroff & Rothausen, 1997; Schneider, 1983; 1987). Perceptions of incongruence

with the workplace can lower performance (Dawis, 1996).

The relationship between personality traits and the workplace embodies not only a

trait to trait relationship, but personality also contributes to the creation of vocational

interests that can determine perceptions of congruence between the individual and the

workplace (Holland, 1985). A relationship seems to form between personality traits and

vocational interests and the workplace environment. A change in one part of the

relationship (e.g., personality, interests) can result in changes in other parts of this

person-environment relationship such as reduced productivity and job satisfaction (Low

et al., 2005). Perceptions of workplace dynamics and the level of personal congruence

Page 34: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

25

believed to exist with the workplace will shape how positively or negatively a person

interprets workplace experiences (Lent, Brown, & Hackett, 1994).

The relationship that appears to exist between personality and interests has been

investigated. Ackerman and Heggestad (1997) found that interests provide the

motivation for engaging in particular types of activities and personality will likely

influence performance in these activities. Lubinski and Benbow (2000) offered that

interests and personality motivate people to pursue certain occupations and the

development of occupational skills enables them to create occupational niches.

Swanson and Fouad (1999) explained the essence of P-E fit theory. First,

individuals seek out environments that are congruent with their personal characteristics.

Second, the degree of fit between person and environment is associated with important

outcomes for both the person and the environment such as satisfaction, achievement,

performance, stability, retention, and tenure. The degree of fit may be viewed along a

continuum with perceptions of greater congruence leading to better outcomes. Third, the

process of fit is reciprocal in that the person shapes the environment, and the environment

shapes the person. If the reciprocal relationship begins to breakdown with negativity the

result will be lower job satisfaction, decreased performance, and a greater likelihood of

attrition.

Holland (1973; 1978; 1985; 1992) offered that vocations possess characteristics

much like human personality traits. Holland’s proposition was that matching personality

and vocational traits will often result in greater workplace performance and higher job

satisfaction. While Holland’s depiction of vocational traits provides helpful information

for individuals making vocational choices, his approach is not necessarily the best for use

Page 35: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

26

by organizations in applicant selection and job placement decisions. De Fruyt and

Mervielde (1999) suggest that Holland’s taxonomy is most helpful for individuals

making vocational decisions, but the FFM is more helpful for organizations seeking to

select and place employees within the organization. Further, research has linked the FFM

with work outcomes related to career success such as promotions, salary, and career

satisfaction (Barrick et al., 2003; Ostroff & Rothausen, 1997; Seibert & Kraimer, 2001).

The FFM seems to be an effective approach to conceptualizing personality for

U.S. Army aviation’s classification decisions for a number of reasons. First, the FFM has

become the dominant trait-based approach to personality (Saucier & Goldberg, 1998;

Young & Schinka, 2001). Second, the FFM factors have been found to predict

performance across different types of vocations (Barrick et al., 2001; Saucier &

Goldberg, 1998). Third, the factors of the FFM have predicted performance for aviators.

For instance, high scores on Neuroticism have been found to predict the emergence of

undiagnosed psychological problems related to motivation and attrition during U.S. Air

Force flight training (Anesgart & Callister, 2001; Callister et al., 1997). Fourth, a review

of FFM-based and non-FFM-based instruments found the FFM-based to be better

predictors of performance than other types of instruments (Salgado, 2003).

Five-Factor Model

The FFM has gained widespread support for its ability to provide a

comprehensive description of human personality across five broad factors and thirty

facets (e.g., Barrick & Mount, 1991; Barrick et al., 2001; Costa & McCrae, 1985; 1989;

1992; 1995; 1997; Digman, 1989; 1990; Goldberg, 1981; McCrae & Costa, 1997; Mount

et al., 1998). The FFM is a trait-centered approach based upon factorial analysis rather

Page 36: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

27

than any specific theory of personality (Digman, 1990; McCrae & Costa, 1989).

Digman’s (1990) comprehensive examination of personality resulted in his conclusion:

At a minimum, research on the five-factor model has given us a useful set of very

broad dimensions that characterize individual differences. These dimensions can

be measured with high reliability and impressive validity. Taken together, they

provide a good answer to the question of personality structure (p. 436).

The growing acceptance of FFM established it as a standard by which competing

trait models of personality were measured (Barrick et al., 2001; Costa & McCrae, 1985;

Digman, 1990; McCrae & John, 1992). Barrick and Mount (1991) reported meta-

analytical support for the FFM by linking the factors and facets of the FFM with job

performance. They concluded:

We believe that the robustness of the 5-factor model provides a meaningful

framework for formulating and testing hypotheses relating individual differences

in personality to a wide range of criteria in personnel psychology, especially in

the subfields of personnel selection, performance appraisal, and training and

development (p. 23).

Ozer and Reise (1994) emphasized this point by comparing the use of personality

measures not grounded in the FFM to geographers reporting the discovery of new lands

without locating them on a map.

The FFM has received empirical support for its ability to measure personality

traits across cultures. For instance, personality factors similar to those found in the FFM

have emerged in studies with participants from Holland, Spain, Italy, Canada, Finland,

Poland, China, and the Philippines (Avdeyeva & Church, 2005; Caprara, Barbaranelli,

Page 37: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

28

Borgogni, & Perugini, 1993; John, Goldberg, & Angleitner, 1984; Paunonen, Jackson,

Trzebinski, & Forsterling, 1992: Trull & Geary, 1997; Tsaousis & Nikolaou, 2001). The

applicability of the FFM across cultures appears to diminish concerns over cultural bias.

Costa and McCrae (1992) did not offer NEO-PI-R norms based upon culture, but they did

provide separate norms tables based upon gender.

A rationale for using the FFM and the NEO-PI-R seems possible with a review of

the model’s origins, design, and usage. First, a review of the history of the FFM

highlights its growth out of trait-personality theory. Second, the design of the FFM is

discussed with a description of the factors and facets that comprise the model. Third, a

review of research will be offered demonstrating the applicability of the FFM as a

predictor of performance across vocational environments including aviation.

History

The FFM has a rich history dating back to the findings of Fiske (1949), Tupes

(1957), Tupes and Christal (1961), and Norman (1963). Their attempts to validate the

personality factors identified by Cattell (1943; 1946) revealed that personality can instead

be described across five broad factor domains (e.g., McCrae & Costa, 1997). Despite the

misgivings of some (e.g., Eysenck, 1991; Hogan, 1983; Shedler & Westen, 2004), a five-

factor approach has been successful in providing a comprehensive coverage of

personality traits (a) across different theoretical frameworks (Goldberg, 1981); (b) using

different instruments (Conley, 1985; Costa & McCrae, 1988; McCrae, 1989), (c) across

cultures (Kallasmaa, Allik, Realo, & McCrae, 2000; McCrae, Costa, del Pilar, Rolland, &

Parker, 1998; Montag & Levin, 1994; Ployhart, Lim, & Chan, 2001; Salgado, 1997), and

(d) with a variety of settings (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Digman, 1990).

Page 38: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

29

Factors

Costa and McCrae (1985) provided the contemporary labels of the FFM:

Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience (or Openness), Agreeableness, and

Conscientiousness. Numerous studies have since validated these factors in vocational

applications such as predicting performance (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Barrick et al.,

2001; Digman, 1990; Mount & Barrick, 1998). The growing empirical support and the

versatility of the FFM as a measure of normal personality trait levels, especially as a

predictor of performance in vocational settings, has made it “the norm against which

different personality trait taxonomies are tested” (Kallasmaa et al., 2000, p. 266). The

theoretical assumption of the FFM is that individuals can be very low, low, average, high,

or very high in each of the five factors and 30 facets.

Neuroticism. Neuroticism contrasts emotional adjustment and stability with

maladjustment such as a frequent depression or anxiety. High Neuroticism scores likely

indicate aspects of emotional instability such as psychological distress and difficulties

with impulse control as well as poor stress management (Endler, Rutherford, & Denisoff,

1997). For instance, high Neuroticism can result in (a) diminished productivity, (b)

absenteeism (Judge, Martocchio, & Thoresen, 1997), (c) simplistic thinking styles (Zhang

& Jiafen-Huang, 2001), (d) lower career success (Bozionelos, 2004), (e) nonconformity

(Griffin & McDermott, 1998), and (f) and low performance motivation (Judge & Ilies,

2002). Average levels of Neuroticism indicate a person is generally emotionally stable

and able to mitigate stress, but they periodically experience anxiety, depression, or guilt.

Low Neuroticism (i.e. Emotional Stability) suggests good coping skills and a positive

Page 39: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

30

disposition to life. Neuroticism has been linked with vocational performance such as

with military leaders (Lim & Ployhart, 2004).

Extraversion. Extraversion contrasts aspects of sociability with a disposition

towards introversion and independence. High Extraversion can be described as showing

interpersonal warmth and enthusiasm (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Individuals high in

Extraversion have been found to (a) provide more input in group activities (Littlepage,

Schmidt, Whisler, & Frost, 1995), (b) emerge as leaders and to have more influence on

team outcomes than introverts (Barry & Stewart, 1997; Judge, Bono, Ilies, & Gerhardt,

2002), and (c) outperform introverts in occupations requiring frequent social interaction

(Barrick & Mount, 1991) resulting in more success (e.g., salary level, promotions) than

those low in Extraversion (Salgado, 2003; Seibert & Kraimer, 2001). Average

Extraversion indicates a moderate desire for activity and social interaction, but also

privacy. Low Extraversion indicates a desire for privacy and to work alone.

Openness. Openness contrasts aspects of imagination and curiosity with

conventionality and obeying the rules. High scorers on Openness are often described as

creative, inquisitive, and attentive to feelings (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Openness

correlates with full scale IQ (Harris, Vernon, Olson, & Jang, 1999), creative thinking

(Gelade, 2002; Zhang & Jiafen-Huang, 2001), problem solving (Ferguson, 1998), and

willingness to engage in conflict or debate (Bono, Boles, Judge, & Lauver, 2002). Such

traits can be helpful in team problem-solving and maintaining quality communication,

especially during high stress conditions when communication quality often diminishes.

Another benefit of having high Openness within a team is its positive correlation with

training proficiency (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Salgado, 1997). Average Openness

Page 40: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

31

indicates a disposition for practicality, but with a willingness to consider new ideas in a

way that balances the old and new. Low Openness indicates a disposition for being

down-to-earth, practical, and traditional.

Agreeableness. Agreeableness contrasts aspects of altruism and compliance with

aspects of antagonism and egocentrism. High Agreeableness is generally associated with

an ability and desire to cooperate with team efforts. Agreeableness has been identified as

a predictor of performance with job environments requiring teamwork and interpersonal

interactions (Barrick, Stewart, Neubert, & Mount, 1998). Similar findings suggest

Agreeableness is strongly related to cooperation and negatively related to

hypercompetition (Ross, Rausch, & Canada, 2003). Effective teamwork appears to rely

upon significant levels of Agreeableness among team members. Individuals with average

Agreeableness can be described as warm and trusting, but can be stubborn and

competitive. Low Agreeableness indicates being competitive, direct, and proud.

Conscientiousness. Conscientiousness contrasts aspects commonly associated

with character such as self-disciplined and organized with impulsivity and

disorganization. Conscientiousness can be associated with quality of work issues such as

job completion, task commitment, and goals accomplishment. Conscientiousness is

possibly the most common correlate with job performance from among the FFM factors

and has been found to predict performance across occupation types and performance

criteria (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Barrick et al., 2001). Average Conscientiousness

indicates a disposition for being well-organized and goal-oriented, but the commitment to

task accomplishment is kept in balance with personal activities. Low Conscientiousness

indicates characteristics such as being easygoing, disorganized, and careless.

Page 41: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

32

Other research has found positive correlations between Conscientiousness and

academic performance (Gray & Watson, 2002), time management (Kelly & Johnson,

2005) and career success (Judge, Higgins, Thoresen, & Barrick, 1999). An inverse

relationship has been reported between Conscientiousness and hap-hazard behaviors such

as accident proneness (Arthur & Graziano, 1996). Judge and Ilies’ (2002) meta-analytic

review of personality research with goal setting theory, self-efficacy theory, and

expectancy theory indicated that Conscientiousness was the strongest positive correlate of

performance motivation across all three theoretical orientations.

These factors seldom function independently of each other. For instance,

Conscientiousness and Agreeableness have been shown to increase throughout early and

middle adulthood for men and women at varying rates while Neuroticism declined among

women (Srivastava et al., 2003). Conscientiousness and emotional stability (low

Neuroticism) were positively correlated with job performance (Anderson & Viswesvaran,

1998; Barrick et al., 2001; Salgado, 1997; Tett, Jackson, & Rothstein, 1991) especially

when high levels of organizational politics are perceived by employees (Hochwarter,

Witt, & Kacmar, 2000). Mount et al.’s (1998) meta-analysis found Conscientiousness,

Agreeableness, and Emotional Stability (low Neuroticism) to be strongly related to

performance in jobs requiring frequent interpersonal interactions while Agreeableness

and Emotional Stability were strongly related to jobs requiring interdependent teamwork.

Ployhart et al. (2001) found that among military leaders Openness was most predictive of

maximum performance, Neuroticism was most predictive of typical performance, and

Extraversion was predictive of both.

Page 42: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

33

Facets

Each FFM factor consists of six facets. Responses on FFM-related instruments

such as the NEO-PI-R result in scores on each of the thirty facets. The facet scores

comprising each factor combine to produce the factor score. Facet scores may increase

the ability of the FFM factors to predict performance (Costa, McCrae, & Kay, 1995;

Paunonen & Ashton, 2001).

Neuroticism. The Neuroticism facets include Anxiety, Angry Hostility,

Depression, Self-Consciousness, Impulsiveness, and Vulnerability. Anxiety measures a

disposition towards apprehension, worry, and phobias. Descriptions of this facet include

anxious, fearful, and nervous. High scorers are apprehensive and are likely to report

phobias or free-floating anxiety. Low scorers are calm and do not dwell on what could

go wrong.

Angry Hostility represents the disposition towards experiencing anger and

frustration. Descriptions of this facet include irritable, impatient, and moody. High

scorers possess a high readiness to experience anger, but their willingness to express that

anger is probably linked to their level of Agreeableness (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Low

scorers are generally slow to anger.

Depression is a measure of differences between the tendencies of normal people

to experienced depressive affect. Descriptions of this facet include worrying, pessimistic,

and moody. High scorers are more prone to sadness and hopelessness and are easily

discouraged. Low scorers rarely experiences depressed affect, but they may not be

cheerful or joyful either.

Page 43: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

34

Self-Consciousness measures a disposition towards embarrassment and shame.

Descriptions of this facet include shy, timid, and inhibited. High scorers are

uncomfortable in social situations and are sensitive to criticism because they struggle

with feelings of inferiority. Low scorers may not demonstrate high social skills, but they

are better able to mitigate the negative emotionality experienced in awkward social

situations.

Impulsiveness is the level of self-control an individual exercises in response to

fulfilling needs and desires. Descriptions of this facet include sarcastic, self-centered,

and hasty. High scorers demonstrate low self-control in their choices so they often

experience regret in response to the consequences of their actions. Low scorers find it

easier to resist temptation and are better able to manage frustration over unmet needs or

desires.

Vulnerability measures an individual’s response to stress. Descriptions of this

facet include panicked, incompetent, and careless. High scorers are prone to become

dependent on others or panicked when facing emergency situations. Low scorers

perceive themselves as competent to handle emergency situations.

Extraversion. The facets composing extraversion include Warmth,

Gregariousness, Assertiveness, Activity, Excitement-Seeking, and Positive Emotions.

Warmth measures interpersonal intimacy. Descriptions of this facet include friendly,

sociable, and affectionate. High scorers are affectionate and friendly. Low scorers are

formal, reserved, and distant.

Gregariousness measures the level of preference an individual has for the

company of others. Descriptions of this facet include pleasure-seeking, talkative, and

Page 44: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

35

spontaneous. High scorers enjoy being in groups and social interaction. Lower scorers

are loners who avoid social interaction.

Assertiveness measures the willingness to speak without hesitation and to assume

leadership roles. Descriptions of this facet include aggressive, forceful, and enthusiastic.

High scorers are dominant and forceful. Low scorers prefer to remain in the background

and let others assume dominant roles.

Activity denotes energetic movement and a need to keep busy. Descriptions of

this facet include hurried, quick, and determined. High scorers are energetic and busy.

Low scorers are more leisurely and relaxed.

Excitement-Seeking measures sensation seeking. Descriptions of this facet

include daring, adventurous, and clever. High scorers enjoy excitement and stimulation.

Low scorers prefer low risks and predictability.

Openness. The facets comprising the Openness factor include Fantasy,

Aesthetics, Feelings, Actions, Ideas, and Values. Fantasy usually describes people who

have a vivid imagination and often daydream. Common descriptors of Fantasy include

dreamy, imaginative, and idealistic. High scorers believe their imagination contributes to

a better quality of life. Low scorers prefer to keep their minds on the task before them.

Aesthetics measure an appreciation for art and beauty. Descriptors of this facet

include artistic, original, and versatile. High scorers enjoy poetry, music, and art whether

or not they have artistic talent. Their appreciation for the arts has often motivated them

to gain more knowledge about the arts than the average person. Low scorers are

uninterested in the arts.

Page 45: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

36

Feelings measure an individual’s receptivity of one’s inner emotions and the

belief that emotions are an important part of life. Descriptions of this facet include

excitable, spontaneous, affectionate, and outgoing. High scorers recognize and accept a

full range of emotionality and may experience happiness and sadness more than the

average person. Low scorers have blunted affect and do not consider emotions to be of

much importance.

Actions measure a willingness to go new places and to try new things.

Descriptions of this facet include adventurous, optimistic, and imaginative. High scorers

prefer novelty to routine and often engage in many hobbies. Low scorers resist change

and prefer the routine.

Ideas measure intellectual curiosity and a willingness to try unconventional ideas.

Descriptions of this facet include idealistic, inventive, and original. High scorers enjoy

philosophical arguments and solving complicated problems and can contribute to high

intelligence. Low scorers have limited curiosity and focus their resources on a narrow

range of topics.

Values measure a willingness to reexamine social, political, and religious values.

Descriptions of this facet include unconventional and flirtatious. High scorers are more

liberal in their views and less accepting of honoring traditions. Low scorers are more

conservative and more accepting of authority.

Agreeableness. The facets of Agreeableness include Trust, Straightforwardness,

Altruism, Compliance, Modesty, and Tender-Mindedness. Trust measures a disposition to

believe people. Descriptions of this facet include forgiving, trusting, and peaceable.

Page 46: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

37

High scorers believe that others are honest, reliable, and well-intentioned. Low scorers

are more skeptical, cynical, and suspicious of people.

Straightforwardness is the disposition towards being unguarded or open in

communications with others. Descriptions of this facet include frank and sincere. High

scorers are open in communication and without consideration for the feelings of others.

They can appear rude, obnoxious, or autocratic. Low scorers will frequently resort to

communication techniques such as flattery, charm, or deception in communication.

Costa and McCrae (1992) warn that a low score on this facet does not necessarily

connote a deceitful person nor should it be regarded as a lie scale that would question the

validity of the NEO-PI-R. This facet is simply an interpersonal style relative to other

people.

Altruism measures a willingness to help others. Descriptions of this facet include

warm, soft-hearted, and generous. High scorers will have an active concern for others

and will demonstrate that concern through generosity and by providing assistance to

those in need. Low scorers are reluctant to get involved in the lives of others and may

appear selfish or self-centered.

Compliance measures reaction styles to interpersonal conflict. Descriptions of

this facet include deference, forgiving, and mild-mannered. High scorers frequently

choose to defer to others and to not appear aggressive or domineering. Low scorers are

aggressive and prefer competence over cooperation.

Modesty measures humility. Descriptions of this facet include humble and self-

effacing. High scorers express humility even though they may possess healthy levels of

Page 47: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

38

self-confidence and self-esteem. Low scorers present as being arrogant, superior, and

condescending to others.

Tender-Mindedness measures levels of sympathy and concern for others.

Descriptions of this facet include sympathetic, soft-hearted, and friendly. High scorers

are prompted to action by the needs of others and see the human side of policy decisions

in organizations and society. Low scorers are more resistant to the needs of others and

avoid emotional responses to dealing with stress and crisis.

Conscientiousness. The facets composing the Conscientiousness factor include

Competence, Order, Dutifulness, Achievement Striving, Self-Discipline, and Deliberation.

Competence refers to a sense that one is capable, sensible, and effective. Descriptions of

this facet include efficient, self-confident, and resourceful. High scorers feel well-

prepared and competent to deal with life. Low scorers often feel incompetent, lack self-

confidence, or feel inept.

Order measures a disposition towards neatness and being well-organized.

Descriptions of this Order facet include organized, precise, and methodical. High scorers

are neat, tidy, and prefer structure. Low scorers are often unable to get organized and see

themselves as unmethodical.

Dutifulness involves being “governed by conscience” (Costa & McCrae, 1992, p.

18). Descriptions of this facet include defensive, fault-finding, and thorough. High

scorers adhere to ethical standards and fulfill their moral obligations. Low scorers are

more prone to being undependable and unreliable.

Achievement Striving measures the drive to succeed and meet goals.

Descriptions of this facet include descriptions ambitious, industrious, and persistent.

Page 48: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

39

High scorers are diligent and purposeful and have a sense of direction in life and can

become workaholics. Low scorers are lackadaisical and lack ambition which often

results in a low need for achievement.

Self-Discipline measures the ability to carry tasks to completion regardless of

distractions. Descriptions of this facet include energetic, organized, and industrious.

High scorers have the ability to motivate themselves to complete tasks. Low scorers

procrastinate and are distracted easily from tasks.

Deliberation is the inclination to think before acting. Descriptions of this facet

include thorough and patient. High scorers are cautious and deliberate. Low scorers are

hasty and impulsive often acting without forethought of consequences.

NEO-PI-R

The NEO-PI-R is the most commonly used instrument measuring FFM factor and

facet levels (Bernard & Walsh, 2004). The NEO-PI-R is “highly regarded for its ability

to gauge normal personality functioning” (King & Flynn, 1995, p. 955). The NEO-PI-R

measures the five factors and 30 facets of the FFM. The suggestion has been made that

the facets might cover the ancillary features of personality that fall beyond the coverage

of the five FFM factors (Paunonen & Ashton, 2001).

The NEO-PI-R is a self-report instrument consisting of 240 statements. A 5-point

Likert scale ranging from 0 to 4 is used and each facet is represented by eight statements.

Each factor consists of six facets and the combined facet scores for each factor produce

the factor scores. Scores are normed based upon the general population and are separated

by gender. Instrument norms do not differ based on ethnicity.

Page 49: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

40

Research Applications

The versatility of the FFM to predict performance across vocational environments

(Barrick & Mount, 1991; Mount et al., 1998) and across performance criteria such as job

selection and satisfaction has been demonstrated (e.g. Digman, 1989, 1990; Tett &

Burnett, 2003; Waldman, Atwater, & Davidson, 2004). This is significant because past

personality research has proven disappointing at times in predicting job performance

(e.g., Ghiselli, 1973; Guion & Gottier, 1965; Schmitt, Gooding, Noe, & Kirsch, 1984).

Barrick et al. (2001) proposed that the difficulty in finding a link between personality and

job performance was because there was no classification system that reduced the number

of personality traits to a quantity that provided a comprehensive coverage of broad

dimensions of personality while also being small enough to effectively manage.

The FFM appears to provide a comprehensive coverage of the general dimensions

of normal personality traits while also offering a manageable taxonomy of five factors. A

framework such as the FFM can be readily applied to vocational settings as part of job

placement decisions and FFM-based personality instruments have been found to be the

better predictors of performance over other types of instruments (Salgado, 2003). For

example, the FFM has been linked to the likelihood of promotion, salary and career

satisfaction (Seibert & Kraimer, 2001).

Studies have found correlations between the NEO-PI-R and Holland’s vocational

assessments (e.g., Self-Directed Search). Modest correlations have been found between

FFM factors and facets and Holland’s taxonomy (Schinka, Dye, & Curtiss, 1997;

Sullivan & Hansen, 2004). Findings such as these suggest that a complimentary

relationship might exist between the FFM and Holland’s vocational taxonomy which has

Page 50: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

41

prompted the recommendation that employee selection might be best served by including

instruments from both vocational modalities (De Fruyt & Mervielde, 1999).

Barrick et al. (2001) examined FFM findings from 15 meta-analytic studies and

reported that: (a) Conscientiousness is a valid predictor across all performance measures;

(b) Emotional Stability (low Neuroticism) was significant when work performance was

the criterion; and (c) the remaining factors (Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Openness)

were predictors of success in specific occupations such as those involving teamwork.

Mount et al. (1998) found that the FFM factors of Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, and

Emotional Stability (i.e. low Neuroticism) were positively related to jobs requiring

interpersonal interactions, and Emotional Stability and Agreeableness were strongly

related to jobs requiring teamwork. The assumption seems reasonable that findings such

as these could have implications for U.S. Army aircrews since a high degree of

interaction and teamwork is required in the cockpit.

Military research has considered the viability of using the FFM approach to

personality in leadership performance, personnel placement, and as a predictor of training

success. The FFM factors have been applied to military leadership research. For

instance, Vickers (1995) used the NEO-PI-R with U.S. Navy personnel and identified

positive leadership qualities including competence, effort, achievement striving, self-

discipline, frankness, trust, assertiveness, and aggressiveness. In all, four of the five FFM

factors (excluding Neuroticism) are represented in Vickers’ findings. Vickers concluded

that the NEO-PI-R is a suitable instrument for selection purposes.

The FFM has been applied in military studies seeking to improve personnel

placement. The use of the NEO-PI-R improved the selection decisions of air traffic

Page 51: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

42

control specialists especially with the Conscientiousness factor (King, Retzlaff, Detwiler,

Schroeder, & Broach, 2003). NEO-PI-R findings were predictive of success for Landing

Craft Air Cushion Vehicle training (Street, Helton, Nontasak, 1994). Openness exhibited

the greatest correlation with performance. A possible explanation is that Openness may

contribute to academic performance and increase the likelihood of successfully mastering

the detailed procedures involved in training.

The FFM has been used to predict training success with military aviators.

Personality has been identified as a factor that could influence the effectiveness of

aircrew teamwork training also called aircrew coordination (Chidester et al., 1991).

Chidester and colleagues comment, “Selecting individuals on the basis of optimizing the

fit between personality characteristics and desired performance may prove highly

beneficial” (p. 41). The importance of considering personality in aviator selection was

further supported by research showing that trait-levels across certain FFM factors such as

Neuroticism and Extraversion could predict training performance and attrition from flight

training (Anesgart & Callister, 2001; Callister et al., 1997).

Aviator Personality Research

Anecdotal Descriptions

Personality research is not limited to organizations outside of aviation. Attempts

have been made to describe the seemingly unique personalities of those choosing aviation

as a career since the early days of aviation. Anecdotal descriptions of pilot personalities

have long been offered attempting to distinguish personality traits from the general

public. For example, as far back as 1918, Rippon and Manuel described aviators as

“outgoing” and “risk-taking.” Dockery and Issacs (1921) shared a different view and

Page 52: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

43

possibly present a different aspect of aviator personality, suggesting that they were

“methodical” and “quiet.”

More recently, Dillinger (2000) provided an anecdotal categorization of military

aviators as being Normal, Rogue Pilot, Failing Aviator, or the Distressed Aviator. The

determination of which label applies to an aviator is based upon personality factors such

as a high achievement-striving orientation, a commitment to teamwork, and the ability to

manage stress. These traits would be similar to elevated FFM levels of

Conscientiousness and Extraversion and low levels of Neuroticism. Anecdotal reports

such as Dillinger’s (2000) can be helpful because they do supply a perspective on aviator

personality traits that can serve to provide hypotheses for empirical research.

Empirical Research

The anecdotal descriptions of aviators can be helpful, but empirical research

offers a mechanism to determine the validity and reliability of the description. The

development of personality instruments that measured trait levels introduced the

possibility of being able to go a step beyond subjective opinions in describing aviators.

For instance, Lambirth, Dolgin, Rentmeister-Bryant, and Moore (2003) used the

Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire and the Hand Test with U.S. Navy aviators and

found that they are (a) goal-oriented, (b) status-seeking, (c) achievement-oriented, (d)

calm, and (e) willing to take risks.

New instruments such as the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory

(MMPI) and the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory (MCMI) enabled the measurement

of traits so findings could be validated by subsequent research. For example, studies

using instruments such as the MMPI, MMPI-II, or the MCMI have resulted in

Page 53: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

44

descriptions of aviators as being “self-assertive,” and “action oriented,” with “high

achievement needs,” and “stable self-identity” (Hormann & Maschke, 1996; Shinar,

1995). While findings from these instruments can be confusing to interpret in relation to

job performance (Retzlaff & Gibertini, 1987), they have made possible the creation of

generalized profiles of aviators.

Empirical research has revealed a possible relationship between aviator

personality traits and training (Chidester et al., 1991; Retzlaff & Gibertini, 1987; Siem &

Murray, 1994). Personality traits can potentially influence cockpit performance such as

in decision-making and aircrew coordination or teamwork training (Chidester et al.;

Murray, 1999). Personality traits have been linked with the likelihood of experiencing an

aviation mishap (Lardent, 1991). The relevance of personality’s influence in training is

illustrated by Chidester et al.’s (1991) comment, “Although it is encouraging that efforts

are now underway to improve both the technical and interpersonal skills dimensions,

considering the impact of other dimensions such as stable personality characteristics, may

make an additional contribution to optimizing crew performance” (Chidester et al., p. 26).

Studies have reported that aviators possess personality trait levels that distinguish

them from the general public (Callister et al., 1997; Fitzgibbons, 2004). Among the

differences are higher scores on Extraversion and lower scores on Neuroticism (Bartram,

1995). Some support has been reported for a variation in trait levels across types of

aircraft, suggesting that the aviator’s personality differences may need to be considered in

aircraft assignment decisions (Boyd, Patterson, & Thompson, 2005; Chidester et al.,

1991).

Page 54: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

45

Despite the advantages of incorporating aviator personality traits into selection,

aircraft placement, and training, confusion still remains over what traits constitute an

aviator’s personality profile. Different personality instruments have yielded results that

can be confusing to interpret especially when instruments are designed for a clinical

population with psychopathology (Dolgin, Kay, Langelier, Wasel, & Hoffman, 2002;

King, McGlohn, & Retzlaff, 1997). For instance, studies using the Edwards Personal

Preference Survey found aviators to be more achievement-oriented, dominant, and

aggressive while reporting less nurturance, affiliation, and abasement than their non-

aviation peers (Ashman & Telfer, 1983; Reinhardt, 1970). Other studies using the

Eysenck Personality Inventory (Bucky & Ridley, 1972; Jessup & Jessup, 1971) found

successful aviators were introverts, dependable, practical, and pragmatic. Results from

studies using the MMPI have suggested that aviators were social, hysteric, aggressive,

self-confident, and intellectually-striving (e.g., Culpepper, Jennings, & Perry, 1972).

One solution to addressing the confusion over aviator personality traits is to move

away from attempting to identify one personality profile that applies to all aviators.

Retzlaff and Gibertini (1987) concluded that the lack of agreement over which

personality instrument is most appropriate for this population is to blame for the

confusion and disagreement in identifying the personality traits that typify what they

believed to be the “right stuff.” Their suggestion was to view aviator personalities as

being diverse as is found in the general public rather than seeking to identify one set of

personality factors or a single profile to generalize to all aviators. They offered that

personality traits would likely cluster differently suggesting “types” of aviators. Further,

Page 55: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

46

they proposed that personality trait levels can fluctuate over time, so periodic

reassessments of personality are warranted during an aviator’s career.

Retzlaff and Gibertini (1987) administered the Personality Research Form (PRF)

and the MCMI to 350 male students entering United States Air Force Undergraduate

Pilot Training at Reese Air Force Base, Texas. They labeled the three aviator personality

profiles they found “right stuff,” “company-man,” and “wrong stuff.” The first group

was identified as having the “right stuff,” comprised 21% of the sample and was

described as (a) aggressive, (b) dominant, (c) exhibitionistic, (d) impulsive, and (e)

playful. They appeared as “self-possessed to the point of arrogance, dramatic, excitable,

easily bored with routine tasks, and at times erratic and impulsive” (Retzlaff & Gibertini,

1987, p. 397).

The second and largest cluster (58%) was the “company-man” group. The

“company-man” aviators were concerned about their level of performance in the cockpit,

but they were as concerned and motivated to maintain a positive image to others and to

positively represent their employer. These aviators tended towards (a) dominance, (b)

endurance, (c) achievement, (d) order, and (e) affiliation. “Thrill-seeking and playfulness

tend to be low and there appears to be a matter-of-fact, highly structured approach to

need-gratification and coping skills” (Retzlaff & Gibertini, 1987, p. 397). This group is

composed of stable, pragmatic, compliant aviators who value the comradeship of peers.

These aviators are believed to seek the non-combative aircraft suggesting a personality

difference with aviators in the first cluster.

The third cluster (21%) was characterized as aviators having the “wrong stuff.”

The “wrong stuff” aviators were cautious, conforming, polite, compulsive, and retiring.

Page 56: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

47

These aviators preferred stable environments and were likely to prefer the security of

military life over a desire to fly. This group was least affiliative and lowest in

achievement. Retzlaff and Gibertini (1987) concluded that this group was more likely to

be motivated by the perceived job security related to military aviation than actually

flying.

The findings of Retzlaff and Gibertini (1987) offer interesting descriptions of the

personality traits found within active-duty U.S. Air Force aviators. Their three categories

have been adapted by others with minor modifications such as changing “company-man”

to “no stuff” (Gregorich, Helmreich, Wilhelm, & Chidester, 1989). The same descriptive

categories of traits were supported in a study with successful applicants undergoing

astronaut training (Musson, Sandal, & Helmreich, 2004). Though not empirically linked,

similarities exist between Musson et al.’s three categories of traits and the FFM. For

instance, Callister et al. (1997) found that male and female U.S. Air Force student

aviators reported high Extraversion scores, which they interpreted to be traits similar to

Retzlaff and Gibertini’s “right stuff” such as aggressive, exhibitionistic, and dominant.

The same student aviators scored low on Agreeableness. Characteristics associated with

Agreeableness would be similar to those identified as representing the “wrong stuff” such

as being too conforming and polite.

A second approach to addressing the confusion over aviator personality traits is to

assume no relationship exists between personality and performance. Attempts to link

personality with aviator performance have not always proven successful (Davis, 1989;

Hilton & Dolgin, 1991). Dolgin and Gibb’s (1988) meta-analysis of a dozen personality

instruments found that some trait-based instruments merited further research in linking

Page 57: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

48

personality traits with performance, but none of them offered overwhelming support for a

relationship between personality and aviator performance. Helton and Street (1993) offer

that the problem might be, “researchers were addressing only a portion of a more

comprehensive description of personality” (p. 9). A bright spot in the research has been

the creation of positive, performance-based measures, such as the NEO-PI-R, that have

proven to be more accurate descriptors and predictors of personality (Dolgin et al., 2002;

King, 1994; King et al., 1997).

A third approach to addressing the confusion over aviator personality traits is to

adopt a valid theoretical methodology that accurately measures aviator personality traits

in a comprehensive manner with manageable data. The FFM offers a positive,

performance-based approach to personality and has been found to link personality with

performance (e.g., Barrick et al., 2003; Mount et al., 1998). The FFM has received meta-

analytical support as the best modality for linking personality with performance in

aviators (Pedersen et al., 1992). Pedersen et al. (1992) reviewed 13 popular personality

theories including the FFM. They found the FFM to be the “most viable option to guide

research on the selection and classification of aircrew members” (Pedersen et al., 1992, p.

16).

FFM data is manageable because it is generated from self-report instruments such

as the NEO-PI-R. Responses are given to eight statements per facet. The scores of each

statement are totaled and result in the facet scores. The next step is to total the six facet

scores that comprise each factor, which will result in the factor score. Studies can

highlight factor and/or facet scores depending upon the level of analysis desired. For

example, aviators may be found to be high on Extraversion. A deeper analysis of the

Page 58: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

49

facets comprising Extraversion should identify aspects of the factor that are high for

aviators and that produced a high Extraversion score overall.

FFM and Aviation Research

Aviation studies have been supportive of using the FFM with aviators in

commercial and military aviation (Anesgart & Callister, 2001; Callister et al., 1997;

Fitzgibbons et al., 2004; Helton & Street, 1993). For instance, the FFM can be predictive

of flight school performance and attrition from flight training (Anesgart & Callister,

2001; Callister et al., 1997). A discussion of the FFM research with different types of

aviators provides some indication of the personality trait levels that are most likely to

adapt to cockpit demands and find aviation to be a satisfying vocational choice.

Commercial Aviator Research. The NEO-PI-R has a history of use with

commercial aviators (Fitzgibbons et al., 2004; Herold, Davis, Fedor, & Parsons, 2002).

For example, Herold et al. found that scores on Conscientiousness, Emotional stability

(low Neuroticism), and Openness to Experience predicted how long it would take for a

flight student to receive an initial pilot’s license in general aviation. Research has sought

to identify a personality profile for commercial aviators (Fitzgibbons et al., 2004).

Fitzgibbons et al. (2004) surveyed 93 commercial aviators to determine if they

reported unique trait levels in comparison with the general public. They identified two

distinctions in the personality trait levels of this sample. First, nearly two-thirds (60%)

scored very low or low on Neuroticism. Second, over half (58%) scored high or very

high on Conscientiousness. Commercial aviators were described as emotionally stable

persons who are “low in anxiety, vulnerability, angry hostility, impulsiveness, and

depression” (p. 5). Further, they conclude by offering a pilot profile based upon their

Page 59: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

50

study describing aviators as: (a) emotionally stable; (b) low in anxiety; (c) very

conscientious; (d) trusting; (e) straightforward; (f) active; and (g) assertive.

U.S. Navy/Marine Corps Aviator Research. Helton and Street (1993) explored

the feasibility of using a model such as the FFM with U.S. Navy/Marine aviators to

predict who would complete aviation training. The researchers utilized the Edwards

Personal Preference Schedule (EPPS) and the Pilot Personality Questionnaire (PPQ) with

158 U.S. Navy and U.S. Marine Corps male aviator candidates. They found that the

EPPS and the PPQ revealed descriptors that could be correlated with FFM factor and

facet characteristics. For instance, characteristics considered in association with

Agreeableness included “accept blame for wrongdoing” and “self-centered.” Descriptors

related to Openness included “can handle change” and “do new and different things.”

They found that in several instances the descriptors reported revealed intercorrelations

between factors such as positive correlations between Agreeableness and

Conscientiousness and between Neuroticism and Openness. Helton and Street

concluded, “A robust five-factor solution may describe the underlying personality testing

constructs in U.S. Navy/Marine Corps student aviators” and “the five-factor model of

personality identified in this research may prove to be a useful tool for the selection of

Navy/Marine Corps aviators” (p. 9).

U.S. Air Force Aviator Research. Several studies have surveyed the personalities

of U.S. Air Force aviators using different theoretical approaches. For instance, the NEO-

PI-R has been found to suggest some distinction in trait levels across types of aircraft

(Boyd et al., 2005). Siem and Murray (1994) surveyed 100 U.S. Air Force aviators using

Page 60: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

51

the NEO-PI-R and found Conscientiousness was reported as the most important

personality dimension in terms of flight performance across aircraft types.

The NEO-PI-R was used by Anesgart and Callister (2001) with U.S. Air Force

flight students to determine if attrition from flight training could be predicted based upon

personality profiles. They found that combinations of domain scores seemed to

successfully predict whether the student completed Enhanced Flight Screening. For

example, individuals scoring high on Neuroticism and low on Extraversion were found to

be 10 times more likely to self-eliminate from flight school. Those scoring very high on

Neuroticism, low on Extraversion, and low on Openness were found to be 50 times more

likely to self-eliminate. They concluded that in light of the tremendous cost associated

with flight training, “the NEO-PI-R many have great utility (savings in costs and man-

hours) as part of a battery of tools for screening potential pilots” (p. 10).

Callister et al. (1997) administered the NEO-PI-R to 1301 student aviators

attending Air Force Enhanced Flight Screening Programs at Hondo, Texas. Their

motivation was to determine if some personality factor might predict who will likely

experience psychological problems that impede cockpit performance that are not

otherwise identified by the current personality instruments used during the student

selection phase. They decided that using a personality instrument designed to measure

“normal” personality traits and to predict performance might prove most beneficial. For

example, the non-phobic fear of flying some students report due to an inability to manage

the stress of high workload in the cockpit was found to have some degree of relationship

with low scores on the Conscientiousness facet Achievement Striving. Low Achievement

Page 61: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

52

Striving can indicate low motivation, which will impact cockpit performance under

pressure.

Callister et al. (1997) addressed the confusion that exists between the trait-based

personality instruments that are available. One problem can be that the personality

factors measured are irrelevant in studies seeking to predict workplace performance. For

instance, the value of using instruments designed to measure psychopathology (MCMI,

MMPI) with high functioning aviators has been brought into question (King, 1994).

Another problem can be complexity in scoring or interpreting findings. They selected the

NEO-PI-R because it provided a comprehensive approach to personality in relationship to

performance and enabled them to establish a baseline of personality data that could be

drawn upon later (Callister et al., 1995).

Callister et al.’s (1997) study provides an interesting picture of U.S. Air Force

pilot personality traits. Both male and female aviators were found to be higher on

Extraversion than their counterparts in the general public. They concluded that using an

instrument such as the NEO-PI-R can augment selection decisions as well as provide

assistance in providing clinical services to aviators experiencing psychological problems

during training. Specifically, Callister and his associates (1997) found that high

Neuroticism could predict performance in flight training. They concluded that their

findings would likely generalize to U.S. Navy aviators since the selection criteria with the

U.S. Air Force are similar in terms of basic requirements. However, they expressed

concern over generalizing their findings to U.S. Army aviators since “many US Army

aviators are Warrant Officers, many without a college education” (p. 6).

Page 62: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

53

U.S. Air Force research has resulted in further support for the applicability of the

FFM and the NEO-PI-R with its aviators. Pedersen et al. (1992) provided meta-

analytical support for basing aircrew selection decisions on FFM assessments of

applicants. They found the FFM to provide the best approach to reveal a predictive

relationship between personality and performance among 13 different personality

theories (e.g., self-efficacy, 16 PF). Specifically, the FFM represented the only

personality theory that addressed the three criteria of their investigation, which were

compatibility with the Basic Attitudes Test currently used to select aviators,

comprehensiveness, and scientific acceptance. Pedersen et al. concluded:

As a result of the preceding evaluation, the Five-Factor theory was selected as the

most viable option to guide research on the selection and classification of aircrew

members. The Five-Factor theory provides a structural description of the basic

dimensions of personality. As such, this theory serves as a scientific framework

for the development of predictor constructs for selection and classification (p. 16).

U.S. Army Rotary-Wing Aviator Research

Only three published personality studies of U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators were

found in a literature search of EBSCOhost, PsychINFO, Annual Reviews of Psychology,

and the Department of Defense (DTIC) search engines (Caldwell et al., 1993; Geist &

Boyd, 1980; Picano, 1991). These studies found personality characteristics that seem to

distinguish U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators from other military personnel and the general

public (Geist & Boyd, 1980). These studies also indicated some personality differences

between types of U.S. Army aviators such as with those seeking Special Operations

training and aviators assigned to general aviation duties (Caldwell et al., 1993).

Page 63: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

54

Unfortunately, the one study (Picano, 1991) seeking to identify personality differences

among aviators across types of aircraft proved disappointing using the Occupational

Personality Questionnaire (OPQ). Significant differences between types of aviators were

more evident when findings were sorted by tenure rather than the type of aviator. One

exception was that instructor pilots shared similar trait levels as a group than other types

of aviators.

Three shortcomings appear to befall what is currently known about U.S. Army

rotary-wing aviator personalities. First, reported personality studies involving U.S. Army

rotary-wing aviators are over a decade old. Changes in aviation technology and in the

operational conditions where U.S. Army aviators are called upon to fly might indicate

that a different set of characteristics such as personality traits are needed today. Second,

there are a minimal number of reported studies available. The present study was

designed to contribute to the limited amount of reported personality research with U.S.

Army aviators and was the first to use the NEO-PI-R. Third, no single approach to

personality has been identified as the most comprehensive to use with U.S. Army rotary-

wing aviators. The findings of this study might be the first step in suggesting a

theoretical approach that holds merit for future use.

Geist and Boyd (1980) offered the oldest personality study involving U.S. Army

aviators. They administered the MMPI to 15 male U.S. Army helicopter aviators and 16

male non-aviation U.S. Army officers. The aviators were found to report more pathology

than non-aviators, with higher scores in hypochondriasis (HS), depression (D),

psychasthenia (Pt), hysteria (Hy), and social introversion (Si). While this study provides

a snap-shot of U.S. Army rotary-wing aviator personality traits, it fails to suggest grounds

Page 64: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

55

for creating a sense of congruence between identified personality traits and mission

platform characteristics.

Caldwell et al. (1993) found differences between U.S. Army aviators applying to

Special Operations training and those remaining in general aviation using the MMPI.

They compared the personality traits of conventional helicopter aviators with those

seeking Special Operations duty. Their findings indicated overall personality differences

for both groups in comparison with the general public in descriptions such as personal

defensiveness, extraversion, nonconformity, friendliness, and sociability. The authors

reported that personality findings between those choosing conventional aviation and

those applying for Special Operations training were of limited interpretive value. For

example, there were differences reported between the two groups on the MMPI scales of

Hysteria (Hy), Psychopathic Deviant (Pd), Hypomania (MA), and Social Introversion

(Si). However, the authors offered that intra-group differences on the clinical

Masculinity-Femininity (Mf ) scale and the test-taking (K) validity scale were probably

the most helpful in identifying the personality differences they were seeking between

these two groups of aviators.

The conventional aviators scored higher on the Mf scale than the Special

Operations sample suggesting perceptions of a greater sense of balance between

characteristics associated with masculinity and femininity for those pursuing

conventional aviation. The authors concluded that conventional aviators are similar to

college-educated males in that they are often more passive, aesthetically-oriented, and

indirect in problem solving than those pursuing Special Operations training. The Special

Page 65: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

56

Operations group was found to more like normative males being somewhat traditional

and inflexible.

Differences on the K scale were linked with Mf scores in that they were

interpreted in terms of the level of traditional masculinity. Caldwell et al. found that the

Special Operations group answered questions “less deviantly and in a more

stereotypically male fashion, although they appeared less impulsive, slightly more

extraverted, and more defensive” (p. 197). The authors proposed that the combination of

scales K and Mf were the best predictors of group membership for these aviators because

of the traits measured by these scales. For instance, the high K scores for the Special

Operations group were compared to the characteristics of aviators posited by Ursano

(1980) that included (a) avoiding introspection, (b) denying internal emotional events,

and (c) being self-sufficient.

The same is true for lower Mf scores from the Special Operations group since

these indicate a greater propensity for behaviors associated with traditional perceptions of

masculinity. The authors suggested that it seems reasonable to assume that Special

Operations aviators would demonstrate these characteristics more than conventional

aviators. While these findings are helpful in understanding the differences in masculinity

between types of aviators, their benefit is limited in providing detailed insight into the

personality profiles of U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators.

Picano (1991) surveyed 170 U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators and was the only

reported study to consider the possibility of personality traits clustering differently across

the four mission platforms. Picano was seeking to validate the OPQ with U.S. Army

aviators and found three personality clusters across subjects similar to Retzlaff and

Page 66: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

57

Gibertini (1987). The largest cluster (48%) resembled those traits stereotypically

ascribed to military aviators. This group corresponded to Retzlaff and Gibertini’s

“company-man.” That is, they were outgoing and used a structured problem-solving

approach emphasizing planning, logical analysis, and attention to detail.

The second cluster (36%) were emotionally controlled, inhibited, and appeared

apprehensive. They tended to prefer stability and predictability, and were uncomfortable

in social situations. These would be indicative of the “wrong stuff” identified by Retzlaff

and Gibertini (1987).

The final cluster was the smallest group (16%) of aviators and corresponded with

Retzlaff and Gibertini’s “right stuff.” Aviators in this cluster were described as highly

independent, competitive, and decisive. In terms of low scores they were found to be

least emotionally sensitive and exhibited the lowest concern for making a good

impression. Picano expressed some degree of surprise that many instructor pilots (IP)

were part of this cluster. His explanation is that IPs likely represent high competitiveness

and are achievement-oriented because it is required for them to achieve this status.

Picano’s (1991) study did not prove beneficial in revealing marked differences in

personality across mission platforms. However, he recommended that further personality

research with U.S. Army aviators is needed. He concluded:

The typological approach to studying pilot personality may establish an

empirical foundation for new practical applications of personality assessment in

aviation including selection and classification, instructor/student matching and

crew composition which might serve to reduce training costs, improve

performance, and enhance aviation safety (p. 520).

Page 67: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

58

Picano determined that he was unable to provide an overall aviator personality profile

based upon his findings.

The next step in personality research with U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators

appears to be identifying a theoretical methodology that is appropriate for this population.

The FFM has proven effective in identifying personality traits with other types of aviators

(Anesgart & Callister, 2001; Fitzgibbons et al., 2004; Helton & Street, 1993) and in

predicting performance (Anesgart & Callister et al., 2001; Pedersen et al., 1992). There

is also support for the usage of the NEO-PI-R due to its broad measurement of normal

personality domains and versatile application across organizational contexts including

with aviators (e.g., Callister et al., Dolgin et al., 2002). The vast research support given

to the FFM and the NEO-PI-R suggest it is an approach worthy of consideration in

assessing the personality profiles of U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators.

Summary

Organizations have a natural interest in seeking ways to enhance productivity and

to retain well-trained workers. U.S. Army aviation shares this desire and launched the

Selection Instrument for Flight Training (SIFT) research effort to improve the criteria by

which new aviators are selected and assigned to aircraft. The criteria for selection are

designed to identify applicable knowledge, skills, and attributes such as personality that

will likely predict optimum cockpit performance. A secondary issue is the possibility

that characteristics such as certain personality traits will indicate which aviators will best

“fit” into the four U.S. Army aviation mission types.

The rationale underlying this current study was found in P-E fit theory. The

assumption of P-E fit theory is that achieving congruence between personal

Page 68: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

59

characteristics and work environment will likely result in better workplace performance

(Kieffer et al., 2004) and other positive outcomes such as greater motivation (Schmitt et

al., 2003), higher job satisfaction (Latham & Pinder, 2005) and tenure (Assouline &

Meir, 1987). Studies have found that the domains of the FFM can predict workplace

performance across various occupations (Barrick & Mount, 1991 Barrick et al., 2001;

Digman, 1990; Mount & Barrick, 1998). The NEO-PI-R is the primary instrument that

measures FFM factors and the facets that comprised each dimension.

Personality studies with aviators have attempted to determine if they possess

distinct trait levels from the general public. The findings from studies using the NEO-PI-

R have found that military and commercial aviators report distinct personality traits

levels. For example, Callister et al. (1997) found that U.S. Air Force aviators reported

high Extraversion and Openness, but low Agreeableness. Fitzgibbons et al. (2004) found

that a large percentage of commercial aviators scored high on Conscientiousness and

Extraversion, but low on Openness. Both studies found low scores on Neuroticism.

The FFM approach to personality can impact performance by predicting training

success for aviators. For instance, Helton and Street (1993) found that a five-factor

approach to personality is probably the best approach to selecting new U.S. Navy/Marine

Corps aviators. Similar support for the FFM was found with U.S. Air Force aviators as

NEO-PI-R findings predicted which aviators would likely complete flight training and

which ones would self-eliminate (Anesgart & Callister, 2001; Callister et al., 1997).

Conscientiousness and Agreeableness have been linked with aviator performance

regardless of aircraft type (Siem & Murray, 1994). Pedersen et al. (1992) found the FFM

to be the best approach for use in aircrew selection.

Page 69: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

60

Research with U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators has not included the use of the

NEO-PI-R. Rather, the three published studies put forward descriptions of U.S. Army

rotary-wing aviators based upon MMPI and OPQ scales. While descriptions vary among

these three studies, findings based upon the NEO-PI-R could improve the manageability

of the information and provide greater clarity in describing the personalities of U.S.

Army rotary-wing aviators because they sort across a minimal number of factors.

The NEO-PI-R was administered to experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators

in this study. These aviators are experienced because they have generally accumulated

numerous years of experience in aviation and have demonstrated sufficient cockpit

performance to achieve the minimal rank of Chief Warrant 3 (CW3). Aviators achieving

the rank of Chief Warrant 4 (CW4) and Chief Warrant 5 (CW5) have obtained senior

leadership positions in U.S. Army aviation. All three groups have found sufficient job

satisfaction to have made U.S. Army rotary-wing aviation their career.

The focus of this study was to see if FFM traits levels are distinct for this group in

comparison with the general population and if these traits levels vary according to

mission platform assignment. Experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators provide a

type of end product in terms of what U.S. Army aviation hopes to receive in return for the

substantial resources invested in aviation training. Identifying the personality trait levels

of these aviators could provide a rationale for further research to determine if NEO-PI-R

results should be included in the classification decisions of future U.S. Army rotary-wing

aviators.

Page 70: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

61

CHAPTER III: METHOD

Research Design

The purpose of this study was to identify the personality profiles of experienced

U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators based upon the Five-Factor Model (FFM). Additionally,

the study sought to determine if the personality trait levels of these aviators were

significantly different across the U.S. Army’s four rotary-wing mission platforms (i.e.

Attack, Scout/Observation, Cargo, and Utility). Personality was measured with the

Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R).

Mission platform denotes the type of tasks or missions a particular aircraft

performs for U.S. Army aviation. The four primary mission platforms are Attack,

Scout/Observation, Cargo, and Utility. Based on results from past research with other

types of aviators, findings could indicate a personality profile that distinguishes this

group from the general public. The scores generated by this sample are categorized by

mission platform according to the type of aircraft they cited as their preference.

Selection of Sample

Sample selection involved experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators having

achieved the rank of Chief Warrant 3 (CW3), Chief Warrant 4 (CW4), or Chief Warrant

5 (CW5) and who are currently on active flight status. Accessibility to these aviators was

obtained through the cooperation of personnel at the Warrant Officer Career Center

(WOCC) located at Fort Rucker, Alabama. Subjects were sampled during the training

cycle from October 2005 to January 2006. The four classes of WOs attending leadership

Page 71: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

62

training at the WOCC during this period were comprised of 90 individuals with U.S.

Army aviation experience. Survey packets were distributed to these aviators and 77 were

returned. Two survey packets were returned incomplete and were discarded from the

study resulting in a sample size of 75.

Experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators typically represent years of

experience in military aviation and have assumed leadership or training roles in U.S.

Army aviation. The combination of time in service and experience suggests this

population has achieved sufficient motivation and job satisfaction to remain in the U.S.

Army and has likely achieved sufficient status to exercise some degree of self-selection

in aircraft classification. Research suggests that sampling experienced or “high-time,”

successful aviators is essential to obtain the necessary information to create personality

profiles suitable for new aviator selection and classification (Intano et al., 1991).

Instrumentation

The NEO-PI-R is the most widely used instrument for measuring FFM domains

(Bernard & Walsh, 2004). The NEO-PI-R is “highly regarded for its ability to gauge

normal personality functioning” (King & Flynn, 1995, p. 955). Costa and McCrae (1997)

revisited the factors and facets of the NEO-PI-R to consider if the instrument needed

large-scale changes in structure or terminology and concluded, “We do not imagine that

the FFM is the last word in personality structure, but we do believe that it will remain the

basis of personality assessment for many years” (p. 87).

Structure. The NEO-PI-R is a 240-item self-report questionnaire to which

subjects respond to statements on a scale of 0 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree)

and scores range from low to high with descriptors representing each end of the

Page 72: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

63

spectrum. Factor and facet scores were generated by the sample. Facet scores can

increase the richness of findings (Paunonen & Ashton, 2001), but the focus of this study

was on factor-level findings. Studies exploring facet-level differences appear to hold

merit as future research.

Reliability. Costa and McCrae (1992) reported high test-retest and internal

consistency reliability. For example, reliability for the NEO-PI-R is reported based upon

the findings of Costa, McCrae, and Dye’s (1991) employment study consisting of 1,800

male and female employees. Internal consistency was supported with coefficient alphas

of the FFM domains ranging from .86 to .92. Internal consistency for facet scales ranged

from .56 to .81.

Test-retest reliability has been reported by Kurtz and Parrish (2001). They

examined the test-retest scores on the NEO-PI-R among three groups of respondents

categorized as low, moderate, or high in inconsistent responding (INC) during the initial

administration of the NEO-PI-R. This determination was based upon one of the NEO-PI-

R validity scales offered by Schinka, Kinder, and Kremer (1997). Retest interclass

correlations for each group were found to be high across FFM domains based upon self-

report and informant data. Low INC group correlations ranged from .92 to .95 on self-

report data and from .75 to .93 on informant data. The moderate INC group reported

correlations ranging from .85 to .95 on self-report data and informant data correlations

ranged from .73 to .82. The high INC group reported correlations ranging from .71 to .94

on self-report data and ranged from .66 to .92 on informant data.

Kurtz, Lee, and Sherker (1999) provided support for the reliability for the NEO-

PI-R. Their test-retest protocol with undergraduate students over a 6-month period

Page 73: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

64

resulted in high correlations. Initial domain coefficient alphas ranged from .89 to .96,

with a median of .80 on the facet scales. Retest Pearson correlations exceeded .70 for

each domain.

Validity. The NEO-PI-R has received some criticism for not including validity

scales (Ben-Porath & Waller, 1992; Bernard & Walsh, 2004; Butcher & Rouse, 1996;

Schinka, et al., 1997). Research generally supports the validity and reliability of the

NEO-PI-R even in the absence of validity scales (McCrae & Allik, 2002; McCrae et al.,

1998; McCrae, Zonderman, Costa, Bond, & Paunonen, 1996). The authors (Costa &

McCrae, 1992) of the NEO-PI-R contend that validity scales are not necessary because

validity checks are in place including (a) the proper administration of the instrument, (b)

judicious interpretation of responses should result in valid test results, and (c) three

statements at the bottom of the answer sheet query respondents if they answered honestly,

completely, and correctly.

Much of the concern expressed over a lack of validity scales focuses on test-

faking and non-purposeful responding. While the motivations (e.g., boredom, fatigue)

for non-purposeful responding can vary, test-faking is generally linked to impression

management. Faking-good is the desire to give what is perceived to be the desirable

responses to create a positive impression with the test administrator. Faking-bad is the

desire to respond in a way that creates a negative impression with the test administrator.

Findings thus far do not indicate that test-faking is a significant problem with the

NEO-PI-R. For instance, a comparison between groups taking the NEO-PI-R under

“fake-good” and normal conditions found the only notable difference was that “fake-

good” respondents tended to score higher on Extraversion and lower on Neuroticism

Page 74: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

65

(Bagby & Marshall, 2003). An international study involving samples subgrouped to

respond naturally or socially desirable found the NEO-PI-R factor structure remained

stable across samples (Marshall, De Fruyt, Rolland, & Bagby, 2005). The suggestion has

been made that concerns over test-faking should not be exaggerated because impression

management is likely part of normal personality (Costa & McCrae, 1992; Morey,

Quigley, Sanislow, Skodol, McGlashan, Shea, Stout, Zanarini, & Gunderson, 2002).

Further, Piedmont, McCrae, Riemann and Angleitner (2000) contend that rather than

laboring to create validity scales for self-report measures such as the NEO-PI-R, energy

could be better spent in seeking to improve the quality of personality assessments.

Schinka et al. (1997) responded to concerns over the lack of validity scales and

the possibility of erroneous responding by creating three validity scales. Their scales

measure positive presentation management (e.g., denial of common faults), negative

presentation management (e.g., denial of common virtues), and inconsistency

(thoughtless responding). Young and Schinka (2001) reported empirical support for the

internal consistency and interscale relations of Schinka et al.’s validity scales.

Validity for the NEO-PI-R has been supported in correlational studies with other

validated personality instruments. These studies have found at least partial correlations

between the NEO-PI-R and other popular personality instruments such as the Myers-

Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI; Myers & McCaulley’s, 1985), the MMPI, Wechler Adult

Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R), and Holland’s (1985) vocational inventories (e.g.,

Self Directed Search) (Furnham, Moutafi, & Crump, 2003; Schinka et al., 1997).

Furnham et al. (2003) administered the MBTI and the NEO-PI-R to 900 people and

found significant correlations between the NEO-PI-R factors of Extraversion, Openness,

Page 75: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

66

Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness and the MBTI factors of Extraversion-

Introversion, Sensing-Intuition, Thinking-Feeling, and Judging-Perceiving respectively.

Costa, Busch, Zonderman, & McCrae (1986) found significant correlations between

MMPI factors and the FFM domains measured in the NEO-PI-R, especially within a

normal sample. Significant correlations are also reported between Openness and the

WAIS-R IQ scores (Holland, Dollinger, Holland, & MacDonald, 1995).

Assumptions/Limitations

There are three limitations associated with this study. First, the sample size of 75

respondents could influence findings. Second, the unequal sample sizes (i.e. Cargo

[n=8], Scout/Observation [n=11], Attack [n=16], and Utility [n=40]) across the mission

platforms can be a limitation. Third, there is an absence of studies using the NEO-PI-R

with this population so comparisons with a similar sample of U.S. Army rotary-wing

aviators are not possible.

First, the size of the sample can influence findings. Larger sample sizes are

generally desired because they are presumed to increase confidence in findings (Portney

& Watkins, 2000). A larger sample could result in significant fluctuations in trait levels.

A problem in studying populations such as experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators

is accessibility since they can be assigned around the world and compliance cannot be

enforced in most cases. Surveying WOs attending training at the WOCC mitigated the

accessibility problem, however it necessarily limited the sample size for this study.

Second, the unequal sample sizes across the four mission platforms are a

limitation. Sample sizes ranging from 8 to 40 can raise concerns about the findings when

comparisons are made between groups. The comparisons of scores between the four

Page 76: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

67

mission platforms were tested at the .01 level in an effort to mitigate concerns over the

unequal sample sizes.

Third, the absence of studies using the NEO-PI-R with this population has made

comparisons with previous findings impossible except with other types of aviators (e.g.,

commercial, U.S. Air Force). Based upon a review of studies with other types of aviators

the NEO-PI-R was used in this study because: (a) it focuses on general domains of

personality and measures normal personality traits rather than focusing on narrow facets

or psychopathology (e.g., Callister et al., 1997; Costa & McCrae, 1992); (b) it is

commonly used in studies examining the fit between individual personality and job

characteristics (e.g., Barrick et al., 2001; Mount et al., 1998; Tett & Burnett, 2003); and

(c) it has been effective in capturing the personality profiles of military aviators (Callister

et al., 1997; 1999), and commercial pilots (Fitzgibbons et al., 2004).

Procedures

Initial contact was made with the director of training at the U.S. Army Warrant

Officer Career Center (WOCC) in July 2005. WOCC compliance was gained since this

study will be considered as part of current U.S. Army aviation efforts to improve rotary-

wing aviator selection and classification. Survey packets were assembled by the

researcher and delivered to WOCC personnel for distribution to all experienced U.S.

Army rotary-wing aviators attending the advanced leadership course during the sampling

period. Completed surveys were collected the week following their completion.

Survey packets consisted of a copy of the NEO-PI-R test booklet and an answer

sheet. A demographic questionnaire (Appendix A) was included that sought information

for the purposes of sorting data such as flight experience, years of service, current

Page 77: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

68

classification, gender, and level of satisfaction. No identifying information was sought

such as names or social security numbers in order to maintain confidentiality. Two

informed consent forms (See Appendix B) were provided. One informed consent form

was to be signed and returned while the other was to be kept by the subject. The

informed consent form addressed the purpose of the study, confidentiality, potential risks,

and how findings will be computed and managed.

The completed surveys were assigned a number by the researcher to enable the

matching of demographic information with NEO-PI-R scores. No other identifying

information was made available to the center personnel or to the researcher so

confidentiality could be ensured. Incomplete demographic forms and incomplete NEO-

PI-R answer sheets that exceed 40 responses were excluded from this study. The NEO-

PI-R manual (Costa & McCrae, 1992) allows for the inclusion of incomplete answer

sheets that do not exceed 40 unanswered items or more than 3 unanswered items on one

facet.

Survey packets were distributed to all experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing

aviators attending advanced leadership training at the WOCC during the survey period.

Surveys were distributed and collected by WOCC training personnel. Survey

information was managed by the researcher and stored in the Fort Rucker branch of U.S.

Army Research Institute.

Surveying began in October 2005 for CW3s and CW4s attending the Warrant

Officer Staff Course. A second data collection occurred in November 2005 and the third

administration of the survey occurred in December 2005. Final data collection occurred

Page 78: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

69

in January 2006 for experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators attending Warrant

Officer Senior Staff Courses at the WOCC.

The hand-scored NEO-PI-R answer sheets were chosen due to budgetary concerns

and the need for an answer sheet format that was versatile, so that they could be

completed at home. The answer sheets were hand-scored by the researcher. Two

techniques were employed to reduce the risk of errors. First, scores were entered from

the answer sheet to a SPSS spreadsheet created for this study. Second, a random sample

of 30 answer sheets was rescored. The raw scores initially produce the facet scores. The

six facet scores comprising each factor are combined to form the factor scores.

Participation in this study was voluntary and no coercive tactics were utilized.

WOCC personnel encouraged compliance but no rewards or punishment was

administered to subjects. Failure to return completed packets was interpreted as a refusal

to participate in the study.

Data Analysis

Two research questions were investigated in this study. The first question

represents an initial effort to produce a personality profile for experienced U.S. Army

aviators based on the factors of the FFM. The identification of this profile was the focus

of the first research question.

• Research Question #1: What are the personality profiles of experienced U.S.

Army rotary-wing aviators?

Findings were reported as raw scores at the factor level and presented in a means

table. The total sample means were provided to allow for a comparison with the general

public and to provide an overall description of the typical experienced U.S. Army rotary-

Page 79: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

70

wing aviator’s personality represented in this study. Factor means were also presented by

mission platform to allow for the identification of disparities between these aviators. The

recognition of disparities prompted the second research question concerning the

significance of these differences in mean scores between the aviators from the four

mission platforms.

• Research Question #2: Do experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviator

personality profiles differ significantly across the U.S. Army’s four mission

platforms?

The rationale for this question is that experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators

have likely exercised some degree of self-selection in their career in terms of mission

platform. The demographic questionnaire allows for their identification of the mission

platform they prefer as their primary platform. Furthermore, the fact that they have

remained in U.S. Army aviation for a number of years assumes they have achieved

sufficient job satisfaction to pursue tenure. Trait level differences across the four mission

platforms might indicate that some trait levels are more congruent with the mission and

aircraft characteristics of one mission platform than with others.

Findings for this question were calculated with a one-way multivariate analysis of

variance (MANOVA) to determine the effect of mission platform on FFM personality

factors. Analysis of variances (ANOVA) were conducted on each dependent variable as

follow-up procedures to the MANOVA. A significance level of .01 was used in this

study to avoid Type 1 errors. Post hoc analysis identified the source of significance at the

facet level.

Page 80: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

71

CHAPTER IV: FINDINGS

Purpose of Study

The purpose of this study was to identify the personality profiles of experienced

U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators based upon the personality factors of the FFM. The

factor scores were measured by using the NEO-PI-R. Additionally, the study sought to

determine if the personality trait levels of these aviators were significantly different

across the U.S. Army’s four rotary-wing mission platforms (i.e. Attack,

Scout/Observation, Cargo, and Utility).

Sample

The sample for this study consisted of rotary-wing aviators that had achieved the

military rank of Chief Warrant 3 (CW3), Chief Warrant 4 (CW4), or Chief Warrant 5

(CW5), because, typically, they have accrued years of military service and represent the

type of aviators the U.S. Army would like to recruit and retain. The aviators sampled

were attending advanced U.S. Army leadership training at the Warrant Officer Career

Center (WOCC) located at Fort Rucker, Alabama. Respondents were surveyed from

October 2005 to January 2006.

Accessibility to these aviators was the primary reason these experienced U.S.

Army rotary-wing aviators were chosen. Ninety survey packets were distributed

containing: (a) one informed consent form to be signed and returned; (b) a consent form

for their records; (c) a demographic form; and (d) a copy of the Revised NEO Personality

Inventory (NEO-PI-R). Seventy-seven packets were returned. Two of the returned

packets were incomplete and were not used, resulting in a sample size of 75.

Page 81: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

72

Findings

Demographics

The demographic form was designed to elicit basic information concerning the

background and experience of the respondents. The demographic form consisted of

items seeking information related to age, years of military service and aviation service,

gender, and time in rank. Additionally, the form probed whether the respondent had

qualified to fly aircraft other than the one to which they were initially assigned and to

identify the aircraft they preferred to fly. The selection of preferred aircraft enabled those

who were qualified to fly more than one aircraft to identify the one they perceived to be

most congruent with their interests. Respondents reporting changes in aircraft

classification were asked to cite the reason for the change. Ethnicity was not identified

because the FFM factors and the NEO-PI-R have enjoyed considerable support across

cultures and scoring norms are differentiated by gender rather than ethnicity.

Findings revealed a mean age of 45 for the sample. Respondents reported a mean

of 24 years of military service and a mean of 20 years of aviation experience. The mean

years at current rank was six years. The majority (53.6%; n = 37) of the sample chose

their initial aircraft while the remaining (46.4%; n = 32) respondents were assigned to

their initial aircraft by the U.S. Army or they did not respond to the statement. The most

common initial aircraft chosen or assigned by the U.S. Army was Utility (61.6%; n =45)

followed by Attack (21.9%; n = 16), Scout/Observation (13.7%; n = 10), and finally

Cargo (2.7%; n = 2).

The majority of respondents (69%; n = 48) reported acquiring additional

qualifications to fly a different aircraft at some point in their career. This additional

Page 82: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

73

aircraft qualification occurred, on average, around seven years into their aviation careers.

The most common reason for the change of aircraft was involuntary reassignment by the

U.S. Army (37.5%; n = 18). This was followed by a desire for career advancement

(33.3%; n = 16) and a desire to fly a different aircraft (29%; n = 14).

Job satisfaction was almost unanimously reported (n =74) by this sample. Only

one respondent reported not being satisfied with his current aircraft assignment. Subjects

were asked to identify their preferred type of aircraft or mission platform and this

selection determined which mission platform would be their designation for this study.

The most frequently chosen “preferred aircraft” was Utility aircraft (53.3%; n = 40),

followed by Attack (21.3%; n = 16), Scout/Observation (14.7%; n = 11), and Cargo

(10.7%; n = 8).

Research Question #1

The first research question (i.e. What are the personality profiles of experienced

U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators?) yielded the identification of a personality profile for

the total sample of experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators. The personality profiles

are based upon the NEO-PI-R factor scores collected in this study. Table 1 presents the

total descriptive statistics for this sample. Table 2 presents total sample factor scores and

scores across mission platforms. Mean scores of the sample were compared with NEO-

PI-R norms (Costa & McCrae, 1992) to determine if they ranked “low,” “average,” or

“high.” Table 3 provides the ranking of total sample scores across mission platforms.

Descriptions or characteristics are provided with the NEO-PI-R and help clarify

factor score rankings. For example, low Neuroticism would involve characteristics such

as being emotionally stable and calm under stress. Average Neuroticism is described as

Page 83: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

74

being typically emotionally calm and able to mitigate stress, but there can be occasional

experiences with guilt or anxiety. High Neuroticism is described as typically

experiencing feelings of anxiety or symptoms of depression.

Total Sample. Based upon the characteristics associated with the rankings of low,

average, and high, characteristics can be suggested for the total sample and aviators in

each mission platform. The total sample scored low to average across the five factors.

The low scores were near the average range, suggesting that this sample of aviators was

similar to the personality trait levels of the general public. For instance, NEO-PI-R

norms begin the average range for males on the Neuroticism scale at 65 and on the

Openness scale at 101. Aviators in this sample were in the low range on Neuroticism (M

= 62.77; SD =18.54) and on Openness (M = 98.47; SD =17.43).

Low to average Neuroticism would indicate this group is emotionally stable and

able to manage stress. . Low to average Openness would suggest these aviators are

practical and conventional, but are willing to consider new ideas. Low Neuroticism

coupled with low Openness would seem to suggest that these aviators are able to mitigate

the influence of stress in the cockpit and prefer to obey standard operating procedures

rather than engage in efforts to create novel approaches to cockpit decision-making.

The remaining three factor scores were in the average range. Average

Extraversion indicates these aviators are moderately social and active, but value their

privacy Average Agreeableness suggests these aviators are generally warm and

cooperative, but can be competitive. Average Conscientiousness indicates these aviators

are dependable and moderately goal-oriented, but are able to set work aside for other

activities.

Page 84: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

75

Research Question #2

The second research question (i.e., Do experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing

aviator personality profiles differ significantly across the U.S. Army’s four mission

platforms?) revealed that this sample of aviators differed on the Agreeableness factor.

There were variations in factor score rankings in the aviators across the four mission

platforms. One exception was that Openness was low across all four mission platforms,

indicating that these aviators are conventional and adhere to procedures rather than

actively challenging the status quo with alternative ideas.

It should be noted, however that one of the limitations involved in discussing

aviator differences across mission platforms is the wide range in sample size across the

four groups (i.e. Cargo [n=8], Scout/Observation [n=11], Attack [n=16], and Utility

[n=40]). It should be noted that while the overall statistical outcome indicated strong

differences across mission platforms on the Agreeableness factor (p = .001), there were

variations in group sizes among platforms. There were only eight Cargo aviators, and

one is reluctant to base conclusions with small group sizes. Larger group sizes would

have been preferable. However, Analysis of Variance is an extremely power analysis,

and capable of handling wide discrepancies in group sizes. One of the cautions

mentioned by Tabachnick & Fidell (2001) relates to unequal cell sizes and they state that

it is necessary to have more cases than dependent variables in every cell (p. 329). This

study met and surpassed their assumption. It is expected that greater uniformity in

sample size across the four groups (i.e. mission platforms) of U.S. Army aviators could

influence findings in future research.

Page 85: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

76

However, based upon this sample the following descriptions are offered.

Scout/Observation aviators (n = 11) scored in the average range on all but the Openness

factor, indicating they do not possess personality trait levels that distinguish them from

the general public based upon NEO-PI-R norms. Cargo (n = 8) and Utility aviators

(n=40) were low on Neuroticism, indicating they tend to be emotionally stable, calm, and

secure. Conscientiousness was high for Attack (n = 16) and Utility (n = 40) aviators,

indicating that these respondents are inclined to be self-disciplined, well-organized,

detailed, and goal-oriented. Attack aviators (n = 16) were the only ones to deviate from

the average ranking on Agreeableness, indicating they might be prone to being

hardheaded, direct, and competitive.

Statistical Comparisons. A one-way multivariate analysis of variance

(MANOVA) was used to compare factor scores across mission platforms (Table 4). The

Wilks’ Lambda (Tatsuoka, 1971) was used and a simple main effect was found between

platform and personality domain, [Wilk’s Λ =.684, F(15, 185.359)=1.824,p=.034]. The

multivariate η2 = .119, is weak, and indicates only about 10% of the multivariate variance

of the personality domains are associated with the platform factor.

Post hoc analysis was conducted through generating analysis of variances

(ANOVA) on each dependent variable as follow-up procedures to the significant

MANOVA outcome (Table 5). Using the Bonferroni method (correcting the alpha level

to guard against Type I errors), each ANOVA was tested at the .01 level. Only

Agreeableness was found to be significantly different among aviators across the four

mission platforms [f(3,71)=6.571, p=.001]

Page 86: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

77

An examination of factor scores reveals that Attack pilots scored lower than the

others on Agreeableness. Since these aviators differed on the domain of Agreeableness,

follow-up analysis of the Agreeableness facet scores was conducted, using an exploratory

multivariate analysis. Findings revealed a significant interaction between aviator

platform and the scores on the Agreeableness domain [Wilk’s Λ =.619, F(18,

187.161)=1.923, p=.016]. Post hoc analysis for the MANOVA consisted of pairwise

comparisons to determine differences among the mission platforms at the factor level

(Table 6).

Under the variable of Agreeableness, the Attack pilots were significantly different

from the Utility pilots (p = .001), but not significantly different from Scout/Observation

or Cargo aviators at the factor level. The source of difference on Agreeableness was

identified on the facets of Trust (Table 7). Attack aviators were found to score lowest

among the four mission platforms on these facets. For instance, the mean score for

Attack aviators (M = 17.75; SD = 5.03) on Trust was significantly lower (p = .001) than

the scores of Utility aviators (M = 22.50; SD = 3.43) (Table 8).

Additional Findings

This study originated in response to Callister et al.’s (1997) concern that they

could not generalize their findings with U.S. Air Force aviators to U.S. Army aviators

due to the limited findings with U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators. Though some

discrepancies in NEO-PI-R scores are reported, three differences should be noted with

Callister et al.’s study that merit caution when comparing the scores between U.S. Air

Force and U.S. Army aviators. First, Callister et al. surveyed aviators during initial flight

training while this study surveyed experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators. Second,

Page 87: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

78

Callister et al. were attempting to identify traits that predicted performance in flight

training while this study examined the possibility that distinctive trait levels might be

indicative of experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators across the four mission

platforms. Third, Callister et al. surveyed over 1,300 aviators; the sample in this present

study (n = 75) was smaller.

Comparisons (Table 9) between U.S. Air Force and U.S. Army aviators reveal

similarities on Agreeableness and Conscientiousness, with Army aviators scoring slightly

higher on both factors. Differences are more pronounced in the Neuroticism,

Extraversion, and Openness scales for the total sample. Comparisons between U.S. Air

Force aviators and U.S. Army aviators across the four mission platforms revealed

variations of scores. For instance, U.S. Air Force aviators (M = 71; SD = 19.60) scored

higher on Neuroticism than the Utility aviators (M = 59.78; SD = 19.47) in this study.

Summary

The findings of this study revealed that this sample of experienced U.S. Army

rotary-wing aviators was similar to the general public in terms of NEO-PI-R factor

scores. While the total sample’s factor scores ranked low on Neuroticism (M = 62.77; SD

= 18.54) and Openness (M = 98.47; SD = 17.43), their scores on these factors were near

the average range (less than five points on each scale). Based upon these findings a

personality description for these aviators would include that they are likely (a)

emotionally stable, (b) able to remain calm under stress, and (c) likely to abide by

standard procedures.

Page 88: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

79

The only significant difference between these aviators was on Agreeableness.

Specifically, Attack (M = 17.75; SD = 5.03; p=.01) and Utility (M = 22.50; SD = 3.43;

p=.01) aviators reported significant differences on the Agreeableness facet of Trust, with

Attack aviators scoring lower. A comparison between NEO-PI-R factor scores in this

sample and the scores presented by Callister et al. (1997) revealed differences between

the two groups at the factor level, but further research is needed to determine the meaning

and facet-level source of these differences.

Page 89: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

80

CHAPTER V: SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS

Summary

The purpose of this study was to identify personality profiles of experienced U.S.

Army rotary-wing aviators based upon the factorial domains of the Five-Factor Model

(FFM). Additionally, findings were analyzed to determine if trait levels were

significantly different between aviators from the four mission platforms of U.S. Army

aviation. The Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) was used because of its

past success in identifying distinct personality trait levels and in predicting performance

with other types of aviators. Though the sample size (n= 75) was less than preferred,

some conclusions can still be drawn that merit mention and suggest further research.

Conclusions

Research Question #1

Four conclusions can be offered based upon the first research question (i.e. What

are the personality profiles of experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators?) First, the

findings of this study indicated that the personality profiles of the U.S. Army rotary-wing

aviators sampled were similar to males in the general public except for slightly lower

scores on Neuroticism and Openness. These findings were surprising given that military

aviators are often stereotyped with personality characterizations such as being highly

extraverted and mavericks (low Agreeableness). Overall, these findings suggest that

those who might demonstrate the best performance over time and will likely pursue

Page 90: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

81

tenure in U.S. Army aviation may be those whose personality traits are near the average

range on the NEO-PI-R.

Second, the differences across mission platforms suggest that the NEO-PI-R

might be of more value in assigning aviators to aircraft or aircraft classification than

aviator selection. Even though Neuroticism and Openness were low for this sample and

high Neuroticism has been linked to flight school performance (Anegart & Callister,

2001; Callister et al., 1997), the variations across mission platforms suggest that future

research might find that more than Agreeableness is significantly different for these

aviators. While NEO-PI-R findings would not be a sole determinant in classification

decisions, recognizing how traits are represented across the mission platforms among

experienced aviators could suggest personality considerations among the other decision-

making criteria.

Third, the low total sample scores found on Neuroticism and Openness could

indicate some of the characteristics that are necessary to have a successful career as an

U.S. Army rotary-wing aviator. Low Neuroticism is typically associated with being more

emotionally stable and calm under pressure than the average person. Low Openness

would suggest a commitment to following the rules and procedures of U.S. Army

aviation, rather than the maverick stereotype of “making their own rules” in the cockpit.

Fourth, this sample was within two points of being high on Conscientiousness.

High Conscientiousness was reported for Attack and Utility aviators. A larger sample

might clarify if experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators as a whole are higher in

Conscientiousness than the general public.

Page 91: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

82

Research Question #2

Three conclusions are suggested based upon results from research question #2

(i.e. Do experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviator personality profiles differ

significantly across the U.S. Army’s four mission platforms?). First, variations in scores

were found between the aviators across the four mission platforms, though Agreeableness

was the only factor found to be significantly (p = .01) different. For instance, Attack (M

= 137.06; SD = 14.78) and Utility (M = 134.38; SD = 17.82) scored high on

Conscientiousness while Scout/Observation and Cargo were in the average range. A

larger sample could help to clarify the level of significance that exists between FFM

factors across the four mission platforms.

Second, the significant difference in Agreeableness between Attack and Utility

aviators may reflect stereotypical characterizations of these two types of U.S. Army

aviators. For example, Utility aviators were described in interviews with instructor pilots

as being more interpersonal and cooperative in the cockpit than aviators from the other

mission platforms. The Agreeableness factor measures the level of priority associated

with maintaining interpersonal relationships and commitment to cooperation as in

teamwork. While this finding might merit attention, more research is needed to

determine if this difference in Agreeableness continues to emerge and the source of this

difference.

Third, the facet level differences in the Agreeableness facet of Trust may indicate

a need to compare U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators at the facet level in future research.

The significant difference between the scores of Attack and Utility aviators in Trust was

not surprising since they were significantly different at the factor level. However, the

Page 92: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

83

difference between Attack and Scout/Observation aviators in Compliance could highlight

the need to compare these two groups of aviators again with a larger sample to see if

differences might prove to be significantly different.

The difference in Trust could indicate that the Utility aviators sampled are likely

to be trusting and cooperative with others, while Attack aviators in this sample are more

skeptical or cynical about people and the need for high levels of cooperation. The

difference in Compliance implies that Utility and Scout/Observation aviators sampled

might show deference to others and do not typically appear aggressive or domineering.

The Attack aviators sampled may be more prone to aggressiveness, competition, and

prefer to emphasize competence over cooperation.

Investigating the origins of these facet level differences could prove interesting.

It is possible that these aviators came into U.S. Army aviation with the trait levels as

measured by this study and gradually migrated to their current aircraft in an effort to

achieve a sense of congruence with their interests. It might also be true that these

aviators have adapted their personality to fit the nature and requirements of their

particular mission platform. Future research in the form of longitudinal studies could

prove helpful in determining which of these scenarios might be more accurate.

Additional Findings

This study provides a response to the comments of Callister et al. (1997)

concerning their reluctance to generalize their personality findings with U.S. Air Force

aviators to U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators since little is known about this population. It

was, in fact, this depiction of U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators that originally provided the

impetus for this study. The results from comparisons between Callister et al. and the

Page 93: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

84

scores obtained in this study resulted in similarities and differences between the two

groups. For instance, U.S. Air Force aviators (M = 126.13, SD = 18.01) were high on

Extraversion while the U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators (M = 116.55, SD = 18.59) were in

the average range. Another comparison revealed that the U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators

(M = 98.47, SD = 17.43) were low on Openness while the U.S. Air Force aviators (M =

114. 39, SD = 18.96) were average.

Some similarities were found to exist between the ranking of scores for U.S. Air

Force aviators and the total sample of U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators in this study. For

example, scores on Agreeableness and Conscientiousness were in the average range for

both groups. Differences were found, however, on the remaining factors. Extraversion,

for example, was high for U.S. Air Force aviators but average for U.S. Army aviators.

Openness was average for U.S. Air Force aviators and low for U.S. Army aviators.

Neuroticism was average for U.S. Air Force aviators and low for U.S. Army aviators.

The comparisons between U.S. Air Force aviators and U.S. Army rotary-wing

aviators in this study might identify similarities or differences that could prove significant

in future research. Caution should be exercised in interpreting the significance of these

findings at this point since further research investigation is needed. It would appear,

based on these comparisons, the caution expressed by Callister et al. in generalizing their

findings with U.S. Air Force aviators to U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators is merited until

additional research findings are available.

Page 94: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

85

Implications

Implications for Practice

One implication for practice is that the near-average and average scores reported

by the experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators sampled appears to support the need

for additional research to determine if these findings are replicated with larger samples.

Based on this sample, it would seem that aviator personalities may be similar to the

average person rather than high in factors like Extraversion that might be more consistent

with stereotypical descriptions of military aviators such as being prone to risk-taking or

maverick-type attitudes and behavior. NEO-PI-R scores might help to predict cockpit

performance as well as indicate which aviators are likely to pursue tenure as a U.S. Army

rotary-wing aviator. While an instrument such as the NEO-PI-R would not serve as a

primary determinant in selecting new U.S. Army aviators, further research might find that

it could augment future selection procedures.

A second implication for practice is that differences in aviator personality trait

levels across mission platforms seems to suggest that the NEO-PI-R might warrant

consideration in aircraft assignment or classification decisions. More research would be

needed to determine if factors other than Agreeableness are significantly different among

aviators across mission platforms. A larger sample would be helpful in revealing the

differences that may be present. If person-environment fit theory is correct, the

identification of these differences and their inclusion in classification decisions should

result in maximum cockpit performance and tenure in U.S. Army aviation.

Page 95: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

86

Implications for Research

Numerous suggestions for future research have already been made largely due to

the lack of current personality research with U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators and the

absence of studies using the NEO-PI-R with these aviators. Future research could

employ a similar research design as used in this study but with a larger sample. A larger

sample might change the rankings of factor scores or increase the significance that exists

between aviators across the four mission platforms.

Future research could also examine facet-level differences among U.S. Army

rotary-wing aviators (i.e. between experienced and new U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators

including correlations within mission platforms). Facets represent different dimensions

within the five personality factors that can provide clarification in interpreting a factor

score (Paunonen & Ashton, 2001). For example, facet scores for the total sample in this

study indicated this sample was low on the Vulnerability facet and high on Competence.

Low Vulnerability would suggest these aviators likely feel competent to handle

emergency situations, while High Competence implies feelings of being capable,

sensible, and effective. Significant differences at the facet level could increase what is

known about U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators in general and if facet levels vary among

aviators across the four mission platforms.

Recommendations

Two recommendations are made based on the findings of this study. First,

personality should be considered in aircrew training. Second, findings from this study

could serve as an initial step in validating the consideration of personality in

classification decisions.

Page 96: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

87

Regarding the first recommendation, personality should be considered in aircrew

training. U.S. Army rotary-wing aircraft are typically manned by two and three aviators

that function as an aircrew. Aircrews rely upon interpersonal dynamics such as (a) clear

communication, (b) quality decision-making, and the (c) coordination of team member

skills and input in accomplishing team goals. The U.S. Army provides periodic training

designed to improve aviator performance as a member of an aircrew.

Personality can influence how effective this training will be with individuals. For

instance, individuals with high Conscientiousness are likely to be more attentive and

responsive to training designed to improve performance than those with low

Conscientiousness. Individuals with high Agreeableness are more likely to be

cooperative in using new skills that are emphasized by the organizations than someone

who is low in Agreeableness. Individuals with high Neuroticism will likely be difficult

to incorporate into a team because of their unstable emotionality. Individuals with high

Extraversion are likely to be interactive in the training process and seek to influence the

group’s response to training.

The basic dynamics of successful aircrews have been identified in aircrew

training programs such as Crew Resource Management (Wiener, Kanki, & Helmreich,

1993). However, factor scores such as those comprising Agreeableness and

Conscientiousness could influence the effectiveness of teamwork training. For example,

Attack aviators in this study scored the lowest in Agreeableness, which may indicate that

they would view the importance of teamwork training differently than do aviators who

fly the other mission platforms. Attack and Utility aviators scored high on

Conscientiousness which might indicate that these aviators are motivated to improve their

Page 97: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

88

flight-related skills. Aircrew training could benefit from acknowledging the differences

that exist in aviator personalities, including addressing specific ways personality

dispositions can impact aircrew performance.

The second recommendation is that findings from this study could serve as an

initial step in validating the consideration of personality in classification decisions with

future U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators. Agreeableness was the only factor found to be

significantly different among the aviators sampled across the four mission platforms.

Future research might expand on this finding by identifying other areas of significant

difference that could be incorporated into classification decision-making.

Summary

Information about the personality traits of U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators

remains shrouded in some mystery; a point that has been noted in research with other

aviators (Callister et al., 1997). This study provides some clarity by identifying the

personality profiles of experienced U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators through scores on the

NEO-PI-R. Though the sample size in this study was small (n=75), findings were

generated for the total sample and among aviators across the different mission platforms.

The aviators in this study reported personality trait levels similar to the general

public though they scored slightly lower on Neuroticism and Openness. A personality

profile such as this would suggest these aviators are similar to the general population

even though they may share a greater disposition for emotional stability and remaining

calm under pressure. They may also share a higher lever of commitment to following

rules and procedures than is found in the general public.

Page 98: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

89

Trait-level fluctuations were reported by the aviators from the different mission

platforms indicating that all U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators do not share the same

personality traits, but the significance of these differences appears to argue for further

research. The findings in this study are an initial step in understanding the personality of

U.S. Army rotary-wing aviators, but much more research is needed to determine the role

personality should play in the selection and classification of future U.S. Army rotary-

wing aviators.

Page 99: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

90

References

Ackerman, P.L. (1996). A theory of adult intellectual development: Process,

personality, interests, and knowledge. Intelligence, 22, 227-257.

Adler, A. (1979). Superiority and social interest: A collection of later writings (3rd Rev.

ed.) (H.L. Ansbacher & R.R. Ansbacher, Eds.). New York: Norfolk.

Allport, G.W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Holt,

Rinehart, & Winston.

Allport, G.W., & Obert, H.S. (1936). Trait names: A psycho-lexical study.

Psychological Monographs, 47, No. 211.

Anderson, G., & Viswesvaran, C. (1998). An update of the validity of personality scales

in personnel selection: A meta-analysis of studies published after 1992. Paper

presented at the 13th Annual Conference of the Society of Industrial and

Organizational Psychology, Dallas.

Anesgart, M.N., & Callister, J.D. (2001). Predicting training success with the NEO-PI-

R: The use of logistic regression to determine the odds of completing a pilot

screening program. (AFRL-HE-WP-TR-2001-0074). Wright-Patterson AFB, OH:

United States Air Force Research Laboratory Human Effectiveness Directorate.

Arthur, W., Jr., & Graziano, W.G. (1996). The five-factor model, conscientiousness, and

driving accident involvement. Journal of Personality, 64(3), 593-628.

Ashman, A., & Tefler, R. (1983). Personality profiles of pilots. Aviation Space and

Environmental Medicine, 54, 940-943.

Page 100: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

91

Ashton, M.C., Lee, K., & Son, C. (2000). Honesty as the sixth factor of personality:

Correlations with Machiavellianism, primary psychopathy, and social adroitness.

European Journal of Personality, 14, 359-368.

Assouline, M., Meir, E.I. (1987). Meta-analysis of the relationship between congruence

and well-being measures. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 31, 319-332.

Avdeyeva, T.V., & Church, A.T. (2005). The cross-cultural generalizability of

personality types: A Philippine study. European Journal of Personality, 19, 475-499.

Bagby, R.M., & Marshall, M.B. (2003). Positive impression management and its

influence on the Revised NEO Personality Inventory: A comparison of analog and

differential prevalence group designs. Psychological Assessment, 15(3), 333-339.

Barrick, M.R., & Mount, M.K. (1991). The big five personality dimensions and job

performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1-26.

Barrick, M.R., Mount, M.K., & Gupta, R. (2003). Meta-analysis of the relationship

between the five-factor model of personality and Holland’s occupational types.

Personnel Psychology, 56, 45-74.

Barrick, M.R., Mount, M.K., & Judge, T.A. (2001). Personality and performance at the

beginning of the new millennium: What do we know and where do we go next?

Personality and Performance, 9(1/2), 9-30.

Barrick, M.R., Parks, L., & Mount, M.K. (2005). Self-monitoring as a moderator of the

relationship between personality traits and performance. Personnel Psychology,

58(3), 745-767.

Page 101: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

92

Barrick, M. R., Stewart, G. L., Neubert, M., & Mount, M. K. (1998). Relating member

ability and personality to work team processes and team effectiveness. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 83, 377-391.

Barry, B., & Stewart, G.L. (1997). Composition, process, and performance in self-

managed groups: The role of personality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 62-78.

Bartram, D. (1995). The predictive validity of the EPI and 16PF for military flying

training. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 68, 219-236.

Ben-Porath, Y.S., & Waller, M.K. (1992). Five big issues in clinical personality

assessment: A rejoinder to Costa and McCrae. Psychological Assessment, 4, 23-25.

Bernard, L.C., & Walsh, P. (2004). Socially desirable and non-purposeful responding on

the Neuroticism Extraversion Openness Personality Inventory-Revised. Counseling

and Clinical Psychology Journal, 1(1), 4-16.

Bono, J.E., Boles, T.L., Judge, T.A., & Lauver, K.J. (2002). The role of personality in

task and relationship conflict. Journal of Personality, 70(3), 311-344.

Borgatta, E.F. (1964). The structure of personality characteristics. Behavioral Science,

12, 8-17.

Boyd, J.E., Patterson, J.C., & Thompson, B.T. (2005). Psychological test profiles of

USAF pilots before training vs. type aircraft flown. Aviation, Science, &

Environmental Medicine, 76(5), 463-468.

Bozionelos, N. (2004). The relationship between disposition and career success: A

British study. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 77, 403-420.

Bucky, S.F., & Ridley, S.L. (1972). California Psychological Inventory as a predictor of

success in the Naval flight program. Aerospace Medicine, 43, 971-973.

Page 102: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

93

Burger, J.M. (1997). Personality. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing.

Butcher, J.N., Dahlstrom, W.G., Graham, J.R., Tellegen, A., & Kaemmer, B. (1989).

MMPI-2: Manual for administering and scoring. Minneapolis: University of

Minnesota Press.

Butcher, J.N., & Rouse, S.V. (1996). Personality: Individual differences and clinical

assessment. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 87-111.

Caldwell, J.A., Jr., O’Hara, C., Caldwell, J.L., Stephens, R.L., Krueger, G.P. (1993).

Personality profiles of U.S. Army helicopter pilots screened for special operations

duty. Military Psychology, 5(3), 187-209.

Callister, J.D., King, R.E., Lanier, D.C., & Etterle, P.M. (1995). Neuropsychiatrically

enhanced flight screening: A pilot baselining and validation effort. In R.S. Jensen &

L.A. Rakovan (Eds.), Proceedings of the Eighth International Symposium on Aviation

Psychology (pp. 1127-1131). Columbus, OH: Ohio State University.

Callister, J.D., King, R.E., Retzlaff, P.D., & Marsh, R.W. (1997). Using the NEO-PI-R

to assess the personality of U.S. Air Force pilots. (AL/AO-TR-1997-0097). Brooks

Air Force Based, TX: Aerospace Medicine Directorate Clinical Sciences Division

Neuropsychiatry Branch.

Callister, J.D., King, R.E., Retzlaff, P.D., & Marsh, R.W. (1999). Revised NEO

personality inventory profiles of male and female U.S. Air Force pilots. Military

Medicine, 164, 885-890.

Caplan, R.D. (1987). Person-environment fit theory and organizations: Commensurate

dimensions, time perspectives, and mechanisms. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 44,

248-267.

Page 103: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

94

Caprara, G.V., Barbaranelli, C., Borgogni, L., & Perugini, M. (1993). The “Big-Five

Questionnaire”: A new questionnaire to assess the five factor model. Personality and

Individual Differences, 15, 281-288.

Caspi, A., & Roberts, B.W. (1999). Personality continuity and change across the life

course. In L.A. Pervin & O.P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and

research (2nd ed., pp. 300-326). New York: Guilford Press.

Caspi, A., & Roberts, B.W. (2001). Personality development across the life course: The

argument for change and continuity. Psychological Inquiry, 12(2), 49-66.

Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., Shiner, R. (2005). Personality development. Annual Review

of Psychology, 56, 453-484

Cattell, R.B. (1943). The description of personality: Basic traits resolved into clusters.

Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 38, 476-506.

Cattell, R.B. (1946). The description and measurement of personality. Yonkers, NY:

World Book.

Cattell, R.B. (1947). Confirmation and clarification of primary personality factors.

Psychometrika, 12, 197-220.

Cattell, R.B. (1948). The primary personality factors in women compared with those in

men. British Journal of Psychology, 1, 114-130.

Cattell, R.B. (1965). The scientific analysis of personality. Chicago: Aldine.

Cattell, R.B., Eber, H.W., & Tatsuoka, M.M. (1970). Handbook for the Sixteen

Personality Factor Questionnaire. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and

Ability Testing.

Page 104: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

95

Cellar, D.F., Miller, M.L., Doverspike, D.D., & Klawsky, J.D. (1996). Journal of

Applied Psychology, 81(6), 694-704.

Chidester, T., Helmreich, R., Gregorich, S., & Geis, C. (1991). Pilot personality and

crew coordination: Implications for training and selection. The International Journal

of Aviation Psychology, 1, 25-44.

Colucci, F. (2002). Army to outsource rotary-wing pilot training: Flight School XXI is

expected to improve overall aviation skills and combat readiness. National Defense

Magazine. Retrieved 6/5/05 from

www.nationaldefensemagazine.org/issues/2002/Nov/Army_to_Outsource.htm.

Conley, J.J. (1985). Longitudinal stability of personality traits: A multitrait-multimodal-

multioccasion analysis. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 49, 1266-1282.

Corey, G. (1996). Theory and practice of counseling and psychotherapy (5th ed.).

Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing.

Costa, P.T., Jr., Busch, C.M., Zonderman, A.B., & McCrae, R.R. (1986). Correlations of

the MMPI factor scales with measures of the five factor model of personality. Journal

of Personality Assessment, 50, 640-650.

Costa, P.T., Jr., Herbst, J.H., McCrae, R.R., & Siegler, I.C. (2000). Personality at

midlife: Stability, intrinsic maturation, and response to life events. Assessment, 7,

365-378.

Costa, P.T., Jr., & McCrae, R.R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory manual.

Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Costa, P.T., Jr., & McCrae, R.R. (1988). From catalog to classification: Murray’s needs

and the five-factor model. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 55, 258-265.

Page 105: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

96

Costa, P.T., Jr., & McCrae, R.R. (1989). The NEO-PI/NEO-FFI manual supplement.

Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Costa, P.T., Jr., & McCrae, R.R. (1992). Professional manual Revised NEO Personality

Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI). Odessa, FL:

Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

Costa, P.T., Jr., & McCrae, R.R. (1995). Trait explanations in personality psychology.

European Journal of Personality, 9(4), 231-252.

Costa, P.T., Jr., & McCrae, R.R. (1997). Stability and change in personality

assessment: The revised NEO Personality Inventory in the year 2000. Journal of

Personality Assessment, 68(1), 86-94.

Costa, P.T., Jr., McCrae, R.R., & Dye, D.A. (1991). Facet scales for Agreeableness and

Conscientiousness: A revision of the NEO Personality Inventory. Personality and

Individual Differences, 12, 887-898.

Costa, P.T., Jr., McCrae, R.R., & Kay, G.G. (1995). Persons, places, and personality:

Career assessment using the Revised NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Career

Assessment, 3, 123-139.

Culpepper, B., Jennings, C., & Perry, C. (1972). Psychiatric and psychometric

predictability of test pilot school performance. Aerospace Medicine, 43, 1257-1260.

Davis, R.A. (1989). Personality: Its use in selecting candidates for US Air force

undergraduate pilot training (AU-ARI-88-8). Maxwell Air Force Base, TX: Air

University.

Page 106: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

97

Dawis, R.V. (1996). The theory of work adjustment and person-environment

correspondence counseling. In D. Brown & L. Brooks (Eds.), Career choice and

development (3rd ed., pp. 75-120). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

De Fruyt, F., & Mervielde, I. (1999). RIASEC types and big five traits as predictors of

employment status and nature of employment. Personnel Psychology, 52, 701-727.

de Jong, R.D., van der Velde, M.E.G., & Jansen, P.G.W. (2001). Openness to

experience and growth need strength as moderators between job characteristics and

satisfaction. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 9(4), 350-356.

Digman, J.M. (1989). Five robust trait dimensions: Development, stability, and utility.

Journal of Personality, 57, 195-214.

Digman, J.M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model.

Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 417-440.

Digman, J.M., & Takemoto-Chock, N.K. (1981). Factors in the natural language of

personality: Re-Analysis, comparison, and interpretation of six major studies.

Multivariate Behavioral Research, 16, 149-170.

Dillinger, T. (2000). The aviator personality. Flying Safety, June, 8-11.

Dockery, F.C., & Isaacs, S. (1921). Psychological research in aviation in Italy, France,

England, and the American Expeditionary Forces. Journal of Comparative

Psychology, 1, 115-148.

Dolgin, D.L., & Gibb, G.D. (1988). A review of personality measurement in aircrew

selection. (NAMRL Monograph 36). Pensacola, FL: Naval Aerospace Medical

Research Laboratory.

Page 107: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

98

Dolgin, D.L., Kay, G.G., Langelier, M.K., Wasel, B.D., & Hoffman, C. (2002).

Identification of the cognitive, psychomotor, and psychosocial skill demands of

uninhabited combat air vehicle (UCAV) operators. Space and Flight Equipment

Journal, 30, 219-225.

Edwards, J.R. (1996). An examination of competing versions of the person-environment

fit approach to stress. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 292-339.

Endler, N.S., Rutherford, A., & Denisoff, E. (1997). Neuroticism: How does one slice

the PI(e)? European Journal of Personality, 11, 133-145.

Erikson, E.H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.

Eysenck, H.J. (1970). The structure of human personality (3rd ed.). London: Methuen.

Eysenck, H.J. (1991). Dimensions of personality: 16, 5, or 3? – Criteria for a taxonomic

paradigm. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 773-790.

Feij, J.A., van der Velde, M.E.G., Taris, R., & Taris, T.W. (1999). The development of

person-vocation fit: A longitudinal study among young employees. International

Journal of Selection and Assessment, 7(1), 12-25.

Ferguson, E. (1998). The five factor model of personality: Openness as distinct but

related construct. Personality & Individual Differences, 24(6), 789-796.

Fiske, D.W. (1949). Consistency of the factorial structures of personality ratings from

different sources. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 44, 329-344.

Fitzgibbons, A., Davis, D., & Schutte, P.C. (2004). Pilot personality profile using the

NEO-PI-R. (NASA/TM-2004-213237). Hampton, VA: National Aeronautics and

Space Administration Langley Research Center.

Page 108: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

99

Fleeson, W., & Heckhausen, J. (1997). More or less ‘me’ in past, present, and future:

Perceived lifetime personality during adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 12(1), 125-

136.

Furnham, A., Moutafi, J., & Crump, J. (2003). The relationship between the Revised

NEO-Personality Inventory and the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Social Behavior

and Personality, 31(6), 577-584.

Galton, S.F. (1884). Measurement of character. Fortnightly Review, 42.

Geist, C. R. & Boyd, S. T. (1980). Personality characteristics of Army helicopter pilots.

Perceptual & Motor Skills, 51, 253–254

Gelade, G.A. (2002). Creative style, personality, and artistic endeavor. Genetic, Social,

and General Psychology Monographs, 128(3), 213-234.

Ghiselli, E.E. (1973). The validity of aptitude tests in personnel selection. Personnel

Psychology, 26, 461-477.

Goldberg, L.R. (1981). Language and individual differences: The search for universals

in personality lexicons. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and social

psychology (pp. 141-165). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Gray, E.K., & Watson, D. (2002). General and specific traits of personality and their

relation to sleep and academic performance. Journal of Personality, 70(2), 177-206.

Gregorich, S., Helmreich, R.L., Wilhelm, J.A., & Chidester, T. (1989). Personality

based clusters as predictors of aviator attitudes and performance. In R.S. Jensen (Ed.),

Proceedings of the 5th International Symposium on Aviation Psychology (Vol. II; pp.

686-691). Columbus, OH: Ohio State University.

Page 109: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

100

Griffin, M., & McDermott, M.R. (1998). Exploring a tripartite relationship between

rebelliousness, openness to experiences, and creativity. Social Behavior &

Personality: An International Journal, 26(4), 347-356.

Guion, R.M., & Gottier, R.F. (1965). Validity of personality measures in personnel

selection. Personnel Psychology, 18, 135-164.

Hakel, M.D. (1974). Normative personality factors recovered from ratings of personality

descriptors: The beholder’s eye. Personnel Psychology, 27, 409-421.

Harris, J.A., Vernon, P.A., Olson, J.M., & Jang, K.L. (1999). Self-rated personality and

intelligence: A multivariate genetic analysis. European Journal of Personality, 13,

121-128.

Hathaway, S.R., & McKinley, J.C. (1943). Manual for administering and scoring the

MMPI. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Helson, R. (1999). A longitudinal study of creative personality in women. Creativity

Research Journal, 12, 89-101.

Helton, K.T., & Street, D.R., Jr. (1993). The five-factor personality model and naval

aviation candidates. (NAMRL-1379). Pensacola, FL: Naval Aerospace Medical

Research Laboratory.

Herold, D.M., Davis, W., Fedor, D.B., & Parsons, C.K. (2002). Dispositional influences

on transfer of learning in multistage training programs. Personnel Psychology, 55,

851-869.

Hilton, T.R., & Dolgin, D.L. (1991). Pilot selection in the military of the free world. In

R. Gal and A.D. Mangelsdorff (Eds.), Handbook of Military Psychology (pp. 88-101).

Sussex, England: John Wiley and Sons.

Page 110: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

101

Hochwarter, W.A., Witt, L.A., & Kacmar, K.M. (2000). Perceptions of organizational

politics as a moderator of the relationship between conscientiousness and job

performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(3), 472-478.

Hogan, R. (1983). Socioanalytic theory of personality. In M.M. Page (Ed.), 1982

Nebraska symposium on motivation: Personality-current theory and research (pp. 55-

89). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

Hogan, R. (1991). Personality and personality measurement. In M.D. Dunnette &

L.M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, (Vol. 2).

Palto Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Hogan, R. (1996). A socioanalytic perspective on the five-factor model. In J.S. Wiggins

(Ed.), The five-factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 180-207).

New York: Guilford Press.

Holland, J.L. (1973). Making vocational choices: A theory of career. Englewood Cliff,

NJ: Prentice Hall.

Holland, J.L. (1978). Manual for the vocational preferences inventory. Palo Alto, CA:

Consulting Psychologist Press.

Holland, J.L. (1985). Manual for the self-directed search. Odessa, FL: Psychological

Assessment Resources.

Holland, J.L. (1992). Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities

and work environments (2nd ed.). Odessa, FL: PAR.

Holland, D.C., Dollinger, S.J., Holland, C.J., & MacDonald, D.A. (1995). The

relationship between psychometric intelligence and the five-factor model of

personality in a rehabilitation sample. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(1), 79-88.

Page 111: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

102

Hormann, H., & Maschke, P. (1996). On the relation between personality and job

performance of airline pilots. The International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 6(2),

171-178.

Intano, G.P., Howse, W.R., & Lofaro, R.J. (1991). The selection of an experimental test

battery for aviator cognitive, psychomotor abilities and personal traits. (USARI

Research Note 91-21). Fort Rucker, AL: US Army Research Institute.

Jessup, G., & Jessup, H. (1971). Validity of the Eysenck Personality Inventory in pilot

selection. Occupational Psychology, 45, 111-123.

John, O.P., Goldberg, L.R., & Angleitner, A. (1984). Better than the alphabet:

Taxonomies of personality-descriptive terms in English, Dutch, and German. In

H.J.C. Bonarious, G.L.M. van Heck, & N.G. Smid (Eds.), Personality psychology in

Europe: Theoretical and empirical developments (pp. 83-100). Lisse, Switzerland:

Swets & Zeitlinger.

Johnson, W.H. (1999). Personality characteristics of future military leaders. Military

Medicine, 164, 906-910.

Judge, T.A., Bono, J.E., Ilies, R., & Gerhardt, M.W. (2002). Personality and leadership:

A qualitative and quantitative review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(4), 765-780.

Judge, T.A., Higgins, C.A., Thoresen, C.J., & Barrick, M.R. (1999). The big five

personality traits, general mental stability, and career success across the life span.

Personnel Psychology, 52, 621-652.

Judge, T.A., & Ilies, R. (2002). Relationship of personality to performance motivation:

A meta-analytic review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(4), 797-807.

Page 112: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

103

Judge, T.A., Martocchio, J.J., & Thoresen, C.J. (1997). Five-factor model of personality

and employee absenteeism. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(5), 745-755.

Kallasmaa, T., Allik, J., Realo, A., & McCrae, R.R. (2000). The Estonian version of the

NEO-PI-R: An examination of universal and culture-specific aspects of the five-factor

model. European Journal of Personality, 14, 265-278.

Kelly, W.E., & Johnson, J.L. (2005). Time use efficiency and the five-factor model of

personality. Education, 125(3), 511-515.

Kieffer, K.M., Schinka, J.A., & Curtiss, G. (2004). Person-environment congruence and

personality domains in the prediction of job performance and work quality. Journal of

Counseling Psychology, 51(2), 168-177.

King, R.E. (1994). Assessing aviators for personality pathology with the Millon Clinical

Multiaxial Inventory (MCMI). Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 65(3),

227-231.

King, R.E., & Flynn, C.F. (1995). Defining and measuring the “Right Stuff”:

Neuropsychiatrically enhanced flight screening (N-EFS). Aviation, Space, and

Environmental Medicine, 66, 951-956.

King, R.E., McGlohn, S.E., & Retzlaff, P.D. (1997). Female United States Air Force

pilot personality: The new right stuff. Military Medicine, 162, 695-697.

King, R.E., Retzlaff, P.D., Detwiler, C.A., Schroeder, D.J., & Broach, D. (2003). Use of

personality assessment measures in the selection of air traffic control specialists.

(DOT/FAA/AM-03/20 Technical Report). Washington, DC: Office of Aerospace

Medicine.

Page 113: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

104

Kurtz, J.E., Lee, P.A., & Sherker, J.L. (1999). Internal and temporal reliability estimates

for informant ratings of personality using the NEO-PI-R and IAS. NEO Personality

Inventory. Interpersonal Adjective Scales. Assessment, 6(2), 103-113.

Kurtz, J.E., & Parrish, C.L. (2001). Semantic response consistency and protocol validity

in structured personality assessment: The case of the NEO-PI-R. Journal of

Personality Assessment, 76(2), 315-332.

Lambirth, T.T., Dolgin, D.L., Rentmeister-Bryant, H.K., & Moore, J.L. (2003). Selected

personality characteristics of student naval aviators and student naval flight officers.

The International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 13(4), 415-427.

Lardent, C.L., Jr. (1991). Pilots who crash: Personality constructs underlying accident

prone behavior of fighter pilots. Multivariate Experimental Clinical Research, 10(1),

1-25.

Latham, G.P., & Pinder, C.C. (2005). Work motivation theory and research at the dawn

of the twenty-first century. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 485-516.

Lent, R..W., Brown, S.D., & Hackett, G. (1994). Toward a unifying social cognitive

theory of career and academic interest, choice, and performance. Journal of

Vocational Behavior, 45, 79-122.

Lim, B., & Ployhart, R.E. (2004). Transformational leadership: Relations to the Five-

Factor Model and team performance in typical and maximum contexts. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 89(4), 610-621.

Littlepage, G.E., Schmidt, G.W., Whisler, E.W., & Frost, A.G. (1995). An input-

process-output analysis of influence and performance in problem-solving groups.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 877-889.

Page 114: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

105

Low, K.S., Yoon, M., Roberts, B.W., & Rounds, J. (2005). The stability of vocational

interests from early adolescence to middle adulthood: A quantitative review of

longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 131(5), 713-737.

Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C.P. (2000). States of excellence. American Psychologist, 55,

137-150.

Marshall, M.B., De Fruyt, F., Rolland, J.P., & Bagby, R.M. (2005). Socially desirable

responding and the factorial stability of the NEO-PI-R. Psychological Assessment,

17(3), 379-384.

Marshburn, T.H. & Rollin, S.A. (2005). The motivational orientation of Army aviators.

Military Psychology, 17(2), 69-87.

McCrae, R.R. (1989). Why I advocate the five-factor model: Joint factor analysis of the

NEO-PI with other instruments. In D.M. Buss & N. Cantor (Eds.), Personality

psychology: Recent trends and emerging directions. New York: Springer-Verlag.

McCrae, R.R., & Allik, J. (2002). The Five-Factor Model of Personality across

cultures. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

McCrae, R.R., & Costa, P.T., Jr. (1989). Reinterpreting the Myers-Briggs Type

Indicator from the perspective of the five-factor model of personality. Journal of

Personality, 57(1), 17-40.

McCrae, R.R., & Costa, P.T., Jr. (1990). Personality in adulthood. New York:

Guildford.

McCrae, R.R., & Costa, P.T., Jr. (1994). The stability of personality: Observations and

evaluations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(6), 173-175.

Page 115: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

106

McCrae, R.R., & Costa, P.T., Jr. (1996). Toward a new generation of personality

theories: Theoretical contexts for the five-factor model. In J.S. Wiggins (Ed), The

five-factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 51-87). New York:

Guilford Press.

McCrae, R.R., & Costa, P.T., Jr. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal.

American Psychologist, 52, 509-516.

McCrae, R.R., & Costa, P.T., Jr. (2003). Personality in adulthood: A Five-Factor

Theory perspective (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.

McCrae, R.R., Costa, P.T., Jr., del Pilar, G., Rolland, J.P., & Parker, W.D. (1998).

Cross-cultural assessment of the Five-Factor model: The Revised NEO Personality

Inventory. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 171-188.

McCrae, R.R., & John, O.P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its

implications. Journal of Personality, 60, 175-215.

McCrae, R.R., Zonderman, A.B., Costa, P.T., Jr., Bond, M.H., & Paunonen, S.V. (1996).

Evaluating replicability of factors in the Revised NEO Personality Inventory:

Confirmatory factor analysis versus Procrustes rotation. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 70, 552-566.

McDougall, W. (1932). Of the words character and personality. Character Personality,

1, 3-16.

Milgram, N.A. (1991). Personality factors in military psychology. Ottawa: John Wiley

and Sons.

Millon, T. (1977). Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory, manual. Minneapolis:

National Computer inventory and computer systems.

Page 116: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

107

Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1998). Reconciling processing dynamics and personality

dispositions. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 229-258.

Montag, I., & Levin, J. (1994). The five-factor personality model in applied settings.

European Journal of Personality, 8, 1-11.

Morey, L.C., Quigley, B.D., Sanislow, C.A., Skodal, A.E., McGlashan, T.H., Shea, M.T.,

Stout, R.L., Zanarini, M.C., & Gunderson, J.G. (2002). Substance or style? An

investigation of the NEO-PI-R validity scales. Journal of Personality Assessment,

79(3), 583-599.

Mount, M.K., & Barrick, M.R. (1998). Five reasons why the “Big Five” article has been

frequently cited. Personnel Psychology, 51, 849-857.

Mount, M.K., Barrick, M.R., & Stewart, G.L. (1998). Five-factor model of personality

and performance in jobs involving interpersonal interactions. Human Performance,

11(2/3), 145-165.

Mozak, H.H. (2000). Adlerain psychotherapy. In R.J. Corsini & D. Wedding (Eds.),

Current psychotherapies (6th ed.). Itasca, IL: F.E. Peacock Publishers.

Murray, S.R. (1999). FACE: Fear of loss of face and the five hazardous attitudes

concept. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 9(4), 403-411.

Musson, D.M., Sandal, G.M., & Helmreich, R.L. (2004). Personality characteristics and

trait clusters in final stage astronaut selection. Aviation, Space, and Environmental

Medicine, 75(4), 342-349.

Myers, I.B., & McCauley, M.H. (1985). Manual: A guide to the development and use of

the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Page 117: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

108

Norman, W.T. (1963). Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality attributes:

Replicated factor structure in peer nomination personality ratings. Journal of

Abnormal and Social psychology, 66, 574-583.

O’Reilly, C., Chatman, J., & Caldwell, D.F. (1991). People and organizational culture:

A profile comparison approach to assessing person-organization fit. Academy of

Management Journal, 34, 487-516.

Ostroff, C., & Rothausen, T.J. (1997). The moderating effect of tenure in person-

environment fit: A field study in educational organizations. Journal of Occupational

and Organizational Psychology, 70, 173-188.

Ozer, D.J., & Reise, S.F. (1994). Personality assessment. Annual Review of Psychology,

45, 357-388.

Paunonen, S.V., & Ashton, M.C. (2001). Big Five Factors and facets and the prediction

of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(3), 524-539.

Paunonen, S.V., Jackson, D.N., Trzebinski, J., & Forsterling, F. (1992). Personality

structure across cultures: A multimethod evaluation. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 62, 447-456.

Pedersen, L.A., Allan, K.E., Laue, F.J., Johnson, J.R., & Siem, F.M. (1992). Personality

theory for aircrew selection and classification. (USAF Technical Report No. AL-

TR-1992-0021). Brooks Air Force Base, TX: Armstrong Laboratory.

Pervin, L.A. (1968). Performance and satisfaction as a function of individual-

environment fit. Psychological Bulletin, 69, 56-68.

Picano, J.J. (1991). Personality types among experienced military pilots. Aviation,

Space, and Environmental Medicine, June, 517-520.

Page 118: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

109

Piedmont, R.L., McCrae, R.R., Riemann, R., & Angleitner, A. (2000). On the invalidity

of validity scales: Evidence from self-reports and observer ratings in volunteer

samples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(3), 582-593.

Ployhart, R.E., Lim, B., & Chan, K. (2001). Exploring relations between typical and

maximum performance ratings and the five factor model of personality. Personnel

Psychology, 54, 809-843.

Portney, L.G., & Watkins, M.P. (2000). Foundations of clinical research: Applications

to practice (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Reinhardt, R.F. (1970). The outstanding jet pilot. American Journal of Psychiatry, 127,

732-736.

Retzlaff, P.D. & Gibertini, M. (1987). Air Force pilot personality: Hard data on the

right stuff. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 22(4), 383-389.

Rippon, T.S., & Manuel, E.G. (1918). The essential characteristics of successful and

unsuccessful aviators. The Lancet, September, 411-415.

Roberts, B.W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T.E. (2003). Work experiences and personality

development in young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

84(3), 582-593.

Roberts, B.W., & Robins, R.W. (2004). Person-environment fit and its implications for

personality development: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality, 72(1), 89-

110.

Ross, S.R., & Rausch, M.K., & Canada, K.E. (2003). Competition and cooperation in

the five-factor model: Individual differences in achievement orientation. The Journal

of Psychology, 137(4), 323-337.

Page 119: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

110

Ryckman, R.M. (1989). Theories of personality (4th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA:

Brooks/Cole Publishing.

Salgado, J.F. (1997). The five factor model of personality and job performance in the

European Community. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 30-43.

Salgado, J.F. (2003). Predicting job performance using FFM and non-FFM personality

measures. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76, 323-346.

Saucier, G., & Goldberg, I.R. (1998). What is beyond the Big Five? Journal of

Personality, 66, 495-524.

Schinka, J.A., Dye, D.A., & Curtiss, G. (1997). Correspondence between five-factor and

RIASEC models of personality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 68(2), 355-368.

Schinka, J.A., Kinder, B., & Kremer, T. (1997). Research validity scales for the NEO-

PI-R: Development and initial validation. Journal of Personality Assessment, 68,

127-138.

Schmitt, N., Cortina, J.M., Ingerick, M.J., & Wiechmann, D. (2003). Personnel selection

and employee performance. In W.C. Borman, D.R. Ilgin, & R.J. Klimoski (Eds.),

Handbook of Psychology (Vol. 12; pp. 77-106). New York: Wiley.

Schmitt, N., Gooding, R.Z., Noe, R.A., & Kirsch, M. (1984). Meta-analysis of validity

studies published between 1964 and 1982 and the investigation of study

characteristics. Personnel Psychology, 37, 407-422.

Schneider, B. (1983). Interactional psychology and organizational behavior. In I.I.

Cummings & B.M. Staw (Eds.), Research in Organizational Behavior, (Vol. 5).

Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Schneider, B. (1987). The people make the place. Personnel Psychology, 40, 437-453.

Page 120: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

111

Seibert, S.E., & Kraimer, M.L. (2001). The five-factor model of personality and career

success. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58(1), 1-21.

Shedler, J., & Westen, D. (2004). Dimensions of personality pathology: An alternative

to the five-factor model. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1743-1754.

Sheldon, K.M., Ryan, R.M., Rawsthorne, L.J., & Ilardi, B. (1997). Trait self and true

self: Cross-role variation in the big-five personality traits and its relations with

psychological authenticity and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 73(6), 1380-1393.

Sherry, A., Henson, R.K., & Lewis, J.G. (2003). Evaluating the appropriateness of

college-age norms for use with adolescents on the NEO Personality Inventory-

Revised. Assessment, 10(1), 71-78.

Shinar, Y. (1995). Personality as the key factor in the competence of a pilot. In R.S.

Jensen (Ed.), Proceedings of the Eighth International Symposium on Aviation

Psychology (pp. 1137-1141). Columbus, OH: The Ohio State University.

Siem, F.M., & Murray, M.W. (1994). Personality factors effecting pilot combat

performance: A preliminary investigation. Aviation, Space, & Environmental

Medicine, 65(5, Sect 2, Suppl), A45–A48.

Smith, G.M. (1967). Usefulness of peer ratings of personality in educational research.

Educational and Psychological Measurement, 27, 967-984.

Soldz, S., & Vaillant, G.E. (1999). The Big Five personality traits and the life course: A

45-year longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Personality, 33, 208-232.

Page 121: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

112

Srivastava, S., John, O.P., Gosling, S.D., & Potter, J. (2003). Development of

personality in early and middle adulthood: Set like plaster or persistent change.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(5), 1041-1053.

Street, D., & Helton, K. (1993). The ‘right stuff’: Personality tests and the five factor

model in landing craft air cushion crew training. Proceedings of the Human Factors

and Ergonomics Society 37th annual meeting. (920-924).

Street, D.R., Jr., Helton, K.T., & Nontasak, T. (1994). An evaluation of personality

testing and the five-factor model in the selection of landing craft air cushion vehicle

crew members. (NAMRL-1385). Pensacola, FL: Naval Aerospace Medical Research

Laboratory.

Sullivan, B.A., & Hansen, J.C. (2004). Mapping associations between interests and

personality: Toward a conceptual understanding of individual differences in

vocational behavior. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51(3), 287-298.

Swanson, J.L., & Fouad, N.A. (1999). Applying theories of person-environment fit to

the transition from school to work. The Career Development Quarterly, 47, 337-347.

Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Boston:

Allyn and Bacon.

Tatsuoka, M.M. (1971). Multivariate analysis: Techniques for educational and

psychological research. New York: Wiley.

Terracciano, A., McCrae, R.R., Brant, L.J., & Costa, P.T., Jr. (2005). Hierarchical linear

modeling analysis of the NEO-PI-R scales in the Baltimore longitudinal study of the

aging. Psychology and Aging, 20, 3, 493-506.

Page 122: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

113

Tett, R.P., & Burnett, D.D. (2003). A personality trait-based interactionist model of job

performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(3), 500-517.

Tett, R. P., & Guterman, H. A. (2000). Situation trait relevance, trait expression, and

cross-situational consistency: Testing a principle of trait activation. Journal of

Research in Personality, 34, 397-423.

Tett, R.P., Jackson, D.N., & Rothstein, M. (1991). Personality measures as predictors of

job performance: A meta-analytic review. Personnel Psychology, 44, 703-742.

Thoresen, C.J., Bradley, J.C., Bliese, P.D., & Thoresen, J.D. (2004). The big five

personality traits and individual performance growth trajectories in maintenance and

transition job stages. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(5), 835-853.

Trull, T.J., & Geary, D.C. (1997). Comparison of the Big-Five Factor structure across

samples of Chinese and American adults. Journal of Personality Assessment, 69(2),

324-341.

Tsaousis, I., & Nikolaou, I.E. (2001). The stability of the Five-Factor Model of

personality in personnel selection and assessment in Greece. International Journal of

Selection and Assessment, 9(4), 290-301.

Tupes, E.C. (1957). Personality traits related to effectiveness of junior and senior Air

Force officers. (USAF Personnel Training Research, No. 57-125). Lackland Air

Force Base, TX: Aeronautical Systems Division, Personnel Laboratory.

Tupes, E.C., & Christal, R.E. (1961). Recurrent personality factors based on trait

ratings (USAF ASD Tech Report No. 61-97). Lackland Air Force Base, TX:

Aeronautical Systems Division, Personnel Laboratory.

Page 123: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

114

Ursano, R.J. (1980). Stress and adaptation: The interaction of the pilot personality and

disease. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 51, 1245-1249.

Vickers, R.R., Jr. (1995). Using personality assessment for leadership selection.

(NHRC Report No. 95-16). San Diego, CA: Naval Health Research Center.

Waldman, D.A., Atwater, L.E., & Davidson, R.A. (2004). The role of individualism and

the five-factor model in the prediction of performance in a leaderless group

discussion. Journal of Personality, 72(1), 1-25.

Wiener, E., Kanki, B., & Helmreich, R. (1993). Cockpit resource management. San

Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Wortman, C.B., & Loftus, E.L. (1988). Psychology (3rd ed.). New York: Alfred Knopf.

Young, M.S., & Schinka, J.A. (2001). Research validity scales for the NEO-PI-R:

Additional evidence for reliability and validity. Journal of Personality Assessment,

76(3), 412-420.

Zhang, L., & Jiafen-Huang, A. (2001). Thinking styles and the five-factor model of

personality. European Journal of Personality, 15(6), 465-476.

Page 124: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

115

APPENDIX A

DEMOGRAPHIC QUESTIONNAIRE

1. Age ______________ 2. Years of military service _______

3. Gender (Circle): Male Female 4. Years of rated aviation service ___

5. Rank ________________ 6. Time in rank _________________

Classification Information

7. What aircraft was your initial classification? _______________________

**IF YOUR PMOS IS ROTARY-WING ANSWER QUESTIONS 8 – 13**

8. Have you switched PMOS in your aviation career? Yes or No. (Circle one)

9. If you have switched PMOS, which explanation most closely explains why? (Circleone)

A. Desire to fly a different aircraft

B. Involuntary reassignment

C. Personal choice career improvement

10. If you have switched PMOS, after how many years as a rated Army aviator did thisoccur?

11. Did you choose your initial aircraft? (Circle one)

A. Yes

B. No

12. Which rotary-wing aircraft would you prefer as your primary classification?

13. Are you satisfied with your current aircraft classification MOS? (Circle one)

A. Yes

B. No

Page 125: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

116

APPENDIX B

INFORMED CONSENT

INTRODUCTION: This study is designed to collect data regarding aviator personalityprofiles. This study is conducted under the supervision of Army Research Institute and ispart of the Selection Instrument for Army Flight Training (SIFT) project. Findings willassist in the creation of a new aircraft classification instrument for future Army aviators.

DISCLOSURE: You will be asked to complete a demographic questionnaire andprovide information concerning gender, rank, years of service, and past training. You willalso be asked to complete the NEO-PI-R.

RISKS: No physical or emotional risks have been identified in this research protocol.The level of stress generated by participation in this research is expected to be minimal.There are no hidden measures or hidden purposes within this study, nor has anydeception been used in this research protocol.

CONFIDENTIALITY: All information will be kept in strictest confidence. Only groupsummary results will be discussed or reported. No personally identifiable informationwill be used in reporting the results of this project to any agency, either within or outsideof the Army. Individuals will remain anonymous. YOU HAVE THE RIGHT TOREFUSE TO PROVIDE ANY OR ALL INFORMATION WITHOUT RISK OF ANYNEGATIVE CONSEQUENCE TO YOU. This right is protected under provisions of AR70-25 Use of Volunteers as Subjects of Research.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT: By returning this packet I acknowledge that I have beeninformed that I have the right to refuse to provide any or all information requested of me.I further acknowledge that I have been informed that any and all information that Ichoose to provide will be kept anonymous.

POINT OF CONTACT: Dr. Larry KatzARI-RWARUBldg 5100Fort Rucker, AL 36362334-255-2385 DSN 558-2385

Please sign below to acknowledge your agreement to participate in this study

__________________________________________________ __________________

(RETURN THIS COPY WITH YOUR COMPLETED SURVEY PACKET)

Page 126: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

117

INFORMED CONSENT

INTRODUCTION. This study is designed to collect data regarding aviator personalityprofiles. This study is conducted under the supervision of Army Research Institute and ispart of the Selection Instrument for Army Flight Training (SIFT) project. Findings willassist in the creation of a new aircraft classification instrument for future Army aviators.

DISCLOSURE: You will be asked to complete a demographic questionnaire andprovide information concerning gender, rank, years of service, and past training. You willalso be asked to complete the NEO-PI-R.

RISKS: No physical or emotional risks have been identified in this research protocol.The level of stress generated by participation in this research is expected to be minimal.There are no hidden measures or hidden purposes within this study, nor has anydeception been used in this research protocol.

CONFIDENTIALITY: All information will be kept in strictest confidence. Only groupsummary results will be discussed or reported. No personally identifiable informationwill be used in reporting the results of this project to any agency, either within or outsideof the Army. Individuals will remain anonymous. YOU HAVE THE RIGHT TOREFUSE TO PROVIDE ANY OR ALL INFORMATION WITHOUT RISK OF ANYNEGATIVE CONSEQUENCE TO YOU. This right is protected under provisions of AR70-25 Use of Volunteers as Subjects of Research.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT: By returning this packet I acknowledge that I have beeninformed that I have the right to refuse to provide any or all information requested of me.I further acknowledge that I have been informed that any and all information that Ichoose to provide will be kept anonymous.

POINT OF CONTACT: Dr. Larry KatzARI-RWARUBldg 5100Fort Rucker, AL 36362334-255-2386 DSN 558-2385

**YOUR COPY. KEEP FOR YOUR RECORDS**

Page 127: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

118

Table 1

Descriptive Statistics of Total Sample NEO-PI-R Factor Scores

N E O A C

N Valid

Missing

75

2

75

2

75

2

75

2

75

2

Mean 62.77 116.55 98.47 116.51 132.91

Median 63.00 117.00 99.00 117.00 133.00

Mode 65.00 105.00 82.00 121.00 133.00

Std. Dev. 18.54 18.59 17.43 16.25 18.09

Skewness .54 -.18 .080 -.24 -.11

Note. Factor names: N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion; O = Openness; A =

Agreeableness; C = Conscientiousness. Average range of scores: Neuroticism (65-86);

Extraversion (99-118); Openness (101-119); Agreeableness (112-128);

Conscientiousness (115-133).

Page 128: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

119

Table 2

Comparison of Total Sample and Mission Platform Scores

Factor A

(n-16)

S/O

(n=11)

C

(n=8)

U

(n=40)

Total Sample

(n=75)

M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

Neuroticism 68.75 18.73 66.55 14.85 60* 17.11 59.78* 19.47 62.77* 18.54

Extraversion 114.38 14.45 114.09 20.82 118 21.53 118.03 19.60 116.55 18.59

Openness 94.88* 15.41 97.73* 17.38 97.75* 16.98 100.13* 18.66 98.47* 17.43

Agreeableness 103.81* 17.95 114.45 10.22 113.50 13.15 122.70 14.53 116.51 16.25

Conscientiousness 137.06** 14.78 127.55 20.41 129.13 19.22 134.38** 17.82 132.91 18.09

Note. * denotes “low” and ** denotes “high” ranking of scores based upon NEO-PI-R norms. Mission Platform names: A = Attack;

S/O = Scout/Observation; C = Cargo; U = Utility.

Page 129: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

120

Table 3

Ranking of Total Sample Scores

Factor A S/O C U

Total

Sample

n 16 11 8 40 75

Neuroticism Average Average Low Low Low

Extraversion Average Average Average Average Average

Openness Low Low Low Low Low

Agreeableness Low Average Average Average Average

Conscientiousness High Average Average High Average

Note. Mission Platform names: A = Attack; S/O = Scout/Observation; C = Cargo; U =

Utility. Rankings based upon NEO-PI-R norms provided by Costa and McCrae (1992).

Page 130: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

121

Table 4

Multivariate Analysis of Platform and Personality Domains

Effect Value FHypothesis

df Error df Sig.

Partial

Eta

Squared

Intercept Pillai’sTrace

.996 3112.071 5.00 67.00 .000 .996

0Wilk’sLambda .004 3112.071 5.00 67.00 .000 .996

0

Platform Pillai’sTrace

.338 1.75 15.00 207.000 .044 .113

2

Wilk’sLambda .684 1.82 15.00 185.359 .034 .119

4

Page 131: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

122

Table 5

Analysis of Variance for the Five-Factor Model Factors

DependentVariable

Sum ofSquares df

MeanSquare F Sig.

PartialEta

Squared

Neuroticism Contrast 1124.569 3 374.856 1.095 .357 .044

Error 24316.577 71 342.487

Extraversion Contrast 230.078 3 76.693 .215 .886 .009

Error 25344.509 71 356.965

Openness Contrast 322.485 3 107.495 .345 .793 .014

Error 22152.182 71 312.003

Agreeableness Contrast 4219.682 3 1406.561 6.517 .001 .216

Error 15325.065 71 215.846

Conscientiousness Contrast 1227.432 3 409.144 1.264 .293 .051

Error 22988.915 71 323.788

Page 132: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

123

Table 6

Pairwise Comparisons Between Mission Platforms

Factor

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference Std. Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

N A S/O 2.205 7.248 .762 -16.981 21.390

C 8.125 8.014 .314 -13.086 29.336

U 8.975 5.474 .016 -5.515 23.465

S/O A -2.205 7.248 .762 -21.390 16.981

C 5.920 8.599 .493 -16.840 28.681

U 6.770 6.301 .286 -9.906 23.447

C A -8.125 8.014 .314 -29.336 13.086

S/O -5.920 8.599 .493 -28.681 16.840

Page 133: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

124

Table 6 (cont.).

Factor

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference Std. Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

N U .850 7.167 .906 -18.121 19.821

U A -8.975 5.474 .106 -23.465 5.515

S/O -6.770 6.301 .286 -23.447 9.906

C -.850 7.167 .906 -19.821 18.121

E A S/O .284 7.400 .969 -19.303 19.871

C -2.500 8.181 .761 -24.154 19.154

U -3.650 5.589 .516 -18.443 11.143

S/O A -.284 7.400 .969 -19.303 19.871

C -2.784 8.779 .752 -26.021 20.453

U -3.934 6.432 .543 -18.443 11.143

Page 134: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

125

Table 6 (cont.)

Factor

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference Std. Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

E C A 2.500 8.181 .761 -19.154 24.154

S/O 2.784 8.779 .752 -20.453 26.021

U -1.150 7.317 .876 -20.518 18.218

U A 3.650 5.589 .516 -11.143 18.443

S/O 3.934 6.432 .543 -13.092 20.960

C 1.150 7.317 .876 -18.218 20.518

O A S/O -2.852 6.918 .681 -21.164 15.460

C -3.500 7.649 .649 -23.745 16.745

U -5.250 5.225 3.18 -19.080 8.580

S/O A 2.852 6.918 .681 -15.460 21.164

Page 135: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

126

Table 6 (cont.).

Factor

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference Std. Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

O C -.648 8.208 .937 -22.372 21.077

U -2.398 6.014 .691 -18.315 13.520

C A 3.500 7.649 .649 -16.745 23.745

S/O .648 8.208 .937 -21.077 22.372

U -1.750 6.841 .799 -19.857 16.357

U A 5.250 5.225 .318 -8.580 19.080

S/O 2.398 6.014 .691 -13.520 18.315

C 1.750 6.841 .799 -16.357 19.857

AG A S/O -10.642 5.754 .069 -25.873 4.589

Page 136: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

127

Table 6 (cont.)

Factor

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference Std. Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

AG C -9.938 6.392 .123 -26.776 6.901

U -18.888 4.346 .000 -30.390 -7.385

S/O A 10.642 5.754 .069 -4.589 25.873

C .705 6.827 .918 -17.365 18.774

U -8.245 5.002 .104 -21.485 4.994

C A 9.938 6.362 .123 -6.901 26.776

S/O -.705 6.827 .918 -18.774 17.365

U -8.950 5.690 .120 -24.011 6.111

U A 18.888 4.346 .000 7.385 30.390

S/O 8.245 5.002 .104 -4.994 21.485

Page 137: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

128

Table 6 (cont.).

Factor

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference Std. Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

AG U C 8.950 5.690 .120 -6.111 24.011

CO A S/O 9.517 7.048 .181 -9.138 28.172

C 12.438 7.792 .115 -8.186 33.061

U 2.688 5.323 .615 -11.401 16.776

S/O A -9.517 7.048 .181 -28.172 9.138

C 2.920 8.361 .728 -19.210 25.051

U -6.830 6.126 .269 -23.045 9.386

C A -12.438 7.792 .115 -33.061 8.186

S/O -2.920 8.361 .728 -25.051 19.210

U -9.750 6.969 .166 -28.196 8.969

Page 138: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

129

Table 6 (cont.)

Factor

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference Std. Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

CO U A -2.688 5.323 .615 -16.776 11.401

S/O 6.830 6.126 .269 -9.386 23.045

C 9.750 6.969 .166 -8.696 28.196

Note. Mission Platform names: A = Attack; S/O = Scout/Observation; C = Cargo; U = Utility. Factor labels: N = Neuroticism; E =

Extraversion; O = Openness; AG = Agreeableness; CO = Conscientiousness.

Page 139: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

130

Table 7

Comparisons of Agreeableness Facet Scores

Dependent

Variable

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference

Std.

Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

A1 Trust A S/O -1.6136 1.57875 1.000 -5.8987 2.6714

C -3.7500 1.74538 .210 -8.4873 .9873

U -4.7500* 1.19232 .001 -7.9862 -1.5138

S/O A 1.6136 1.57875 1.000 -2.6714 5.8987

C -2.1364 1.87294 1.000 -7.2199 2.9471

U -3.1364 1.37229 .152 -6.8610 .5883

C A 3.7500 1.74538 .210 -.9873 8.4873

S/O 2.1364 1.87294 1.000 -2.9471 7.2199

Page 140: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

131

Table 7 (cont.)

Dependent

Variable

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference

Std.

Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

A1 Trust (cont.) U -1.0000 1.56111 1.000 -5.2372 3.2372

U A 4.7500* 1.19232 .001 1.5138 7.9862

S/O 3.1364 1.37229 .152 -.5883 6.8610

C 1.0000 1.56111 1.000 -3.2372 5.2372A2Straightforwardness A S/O -1.9261 1.65625 1.000 -6.4215 2.2693

C -.0625 1.83106 1.000 -5.0323 4.9073

U -2.7125 1.25085 .201 -6.1075 .6825

S/O A 1.9261 1.65625 1.000 -2.5693 6.4215

C 1.8636 1.96488 1.000 -3.4694 7.1967

Page 141: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

132

Table 7 (cont.)

Dependent

Variable

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference

Std.

Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

A2Straightforwardness(cont.) U -.7864 1.43966 1.000 -4.6939 3.1211

C A .0625 1.83106 1.000 -4.9073 5.0323

S/O -1.8636 1.96488 1.000 -7.1967 3.4694

U -2.6500 1.63775 .660 -7.0952 1.7952

U A 2.7125 1.25085 .201 -.6825 6.1075

S/O .7864 1.43966 1.000 -3.1211 4.6939

C 2.6500 1.63775 .660 -1.7952 7.0952

A3 Altruism A S/O -.5795 1.50908 1.000 -4.6755 3.5164

Page 142: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

133

Table 7 (cont.)

Dependent

Variable

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference

Std.

Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

A3 Altruism (cont.) C -1.3750 1.66835 1.000 -5.9032 3.1532

U -2.1250 1.13970 .398 -5.2184 .9684

S/O A .5795 1.50908 1.000 -3.5164 4.6755

C -.7955 1.79028 1.000 -5.6546 4.0637

U -1.5455 1.31173 1.000 -5.1057 2.0148

C A 1.3750 1.66835 1.000 -3.1532 5.9032

S/O .7955 1.79028 1.000 -4.0637 5.6546

U -.7500 1.49222 1.000 -4.8002 3.3302

U A 2.1250 1.13970 .398 -.9684 5.2184

Page 143: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

134

Table 7 (cont.)

Dependent

Variable

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference

Std.

Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

A3 Altruism (cont.) S/O 1.5455 1.31173 1.000 -2.0148 5.1057

C .7500 1.49222 1.000 -3.3002 4.8002

A4 Compliance A S/O -4.9886* 1.63332 .019 -9.4218 -.555

C -2.1250 1.80571 1.000 -7.0260 2.7760

U -3.7500* 1.23353 .020 -7.0980 -.4020

S/O A 4.9886* 1.63332 .019 .5555 9.4218

C 2.8636 1.93768 .863 -2.3956 8.1229

U 1.2386 1.41973 1.000 -2.6148 5.0920

C A 2.1250 1.80571 1.000 -2.7760 7.0260

Page 144: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

135

Table 7 (cont.)

Dependent

Variable

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference

Std.

Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

A4 Compliance (cont.) S/O -2.8636 1.93768 .863 -8.1229 2.3956

U -1.6250 1.61507 1.000 -6.0086 2.7586

U A 3.7500* 1.23353 .020 .4020 7.0980

S/O -1.2386 1.41973 1.000 -5.0920 2.6148

C 1.6250 1.61507 1.000 -2.7586 6.0086

A5 Modesty A S/O -1.2614 1.84719 1.000 -6.2750 3.7523

C -1.7500 2.04214 1.000 -7.2928 3.7928

U -2.6250 1.39505 .384 -6.4114 1.1614

S/O A 1.2614 1.84719 1.000 -3.7523 6.2750

Page 145: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

136

Table 7 (cont.)

Dependent

Variable

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference

Std.

Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

A5 Modesty (cont.) C -.4886 2.19140 1.000 -6.4365 5.4592

U -1.3636 1.60562 1.000 -5.7216 2.9943

C A 1.7500 2.04214 1.000 -3.7928 7.2928

S/O .4886 2.19140 1.000 -5.4592 6.4365

U -.8750 1.82655 1.000 -5.8326 4.0826

U A 2.6250 1.39505 .384 -1.1614 6.4114

S/O 1.3636 1.60562 1.000 -2.9943 5.7216

C .8750 1.82655 1.000 -4.0826 5.8326

A6 Tender-Mindedness A S/O -.2727 1.51583 1.000 -4.3870 3.8415

Page 146: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

137

Table 7 (cont.)

Dependent

Variable

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference

Std.

Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

A6 Tender-Mindedness (cont.) C -.6250 1.67582 1.000 -5.1735 3.9235

U -2.6250 1.14480 .149 -5.7322 .4822

S/O A .2727 1.51583 1.000 -3.8415 4.3870

C -.3523 1.79830 1.000 -5.2332 4.5286

U -2.3523 1.31760 .471 -5.9285 1.2240

C A .6250 1.67582 1.000 -3.9235 5.1735

S/O .3523 1.79830 1.000 -1.5286 5.2332

U -2.000 1.49890 1.000 -6.0683 2.0683

Page 147: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

138

Table 7 (cont.)

Dependent

Variable

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference

Std.

Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

A6 Tender-Mindedness (cont.) U A 2.6250 1.14480 .149 -.4822 5.7322

S/O 2.3523 1.31760 .471 -1.2240 5.9285

C 2.0000 1.49890 1.000 -2.0683 6.0683

* denotes the mean difference is significant at the .05 level

Page 148: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

139

Table 8

Pairwise Comparisons of Trust Between Mission Platforms

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference Std. Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

A S/O -1.6136 1.57875 1.00 -5.8987 2.6714

C -3.7500 1.74538 .210 -8.4873 .9873

U -4.7500 1.19232 .001 -7.9862 -1.5138

S/O A 1.6136 1.57875 1.000 -2.6714 5.8987

C -2.1364 1.87294 1.000 -7.2199 2.9471

U -3.1364 1.37229 .152 -6.8610 .5883

C A 3.7500 1.74538 .210 -.9873 8.4873

S/O 2.1364 1.87294 1.000 -2.9471 7.2199

Page 149: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

140

Table 7 (cont.).

(I)

Mission

Platform

(J)

Mission

Platform

(I-J)

Mean

Difference Std. Error Sig.

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound Upper Bound

U -1.0000 1.5611 1.000 -5.2372 3.2372

U A 4.7500 1.19232 .001 1.5138 7.9862

S/O 3.1364 1.37229 .152 -.5883 6.8610

C 1.0000 1.56111 1.000 -3.2372 5.2372

Note. Mission Platform names: A = Attack; S/O = Scout/Observation; C = Cargo; U = Utility.

Page 150: Personality Profiles of Experienced US Army Rotary-Wing Aviators Across Mission

141

Table 9

Comparison of U.S. Air Force and U.S. Army Aviators NEO-PI-R Scores_____________________________________________________________

Factor U.S. Air Force U.S. Army_____________________________________________________________

M SD M SD

Neuroticism 71.00 19.60 62.77* 18.54

Extraversion 126.13** 18.01 116.55 18.59

Openness 114.39 18.96 98.47* 17.43

Agreeableness 112.89 18.51 116.51 16.25

Conscientiousness 128.24 19.15 132.91 18.09

Source: Air Force scores from “Using the NEO-PI-R to Assess the Personality of US Air

Force Pilots” by J.D. Callister, R.E. King, P.D. Retzlaff, and R.W. Marsh, 1997,

(Technical Report AL/AO-TR-1997-0097), Brooks Air Force Base, TX: United States

Air Force Armstrong Laboratory. (*) denotes low and (**) denotes high score rankings

based upon NEO-PI-R norms.