Stressing Out the Poor

  • Upload
    asanij

  • View
    215

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/30/2019 Stressing Out the Poor

    1/6

    16 Pathways Winter 2011

    Stressing Outthe Poor

    Chronic Physiological Stress andthe Income-Achievement Gap

    By Gary W. Evans, JEannE Brooks-Gunn,

    and PamEla kato klEBanov

  • 7/30/2019 Stressing Out the Poor

    2/6

    Pathways Winter 2011

    It is well known that economic depriva-

    tion early in lie sets children on a tra-

    jectory toward diminished educational

    and occupational attainment. But why is

    early-childhood poverty so harmul? I we

    cant answer that question well, our reorm

    eorts are reduced to shots in the dark.

    In this article, we oer a new perspective on this

    question. We suggest that childhood poverty is

    harmul, in part, because it exposes children to

    stressul environments. Low-income children

    ace a bewildering array o psychosocial and

    physical demands that place much pressure on

    their adaptive capacities and appear to be toxic

    to the developing brain. Although poor childrenare disadvantaged in other ways, we ocus our

    analysis here on the new, underappreciated path-

    way depicted in Figure 1. As shown in this g-

    ure, children growing up in poverty demonstrate

    lower academic achievement because o their

    exposure to a wide variety o risks. These risks,

    in turn, build upon one another to elevate levels

    o chronic (and toxic) stress within the body. And

    this toxic stress directly hinders poor childrens

    academic perormance by compromising theirability to develop the kinds o skills necessary to

    perorm well in school.

  • 7/30/2019 Stressing Out the Poor

    3/6

    18 Pathways Winter 2011

    accumulation occurs in various ways; or example, children who

    score poorly at age six may be tracked into low-achievement

    school groups, which in turn exposes them to lower expecta-

    tions, to less rigorous curricula, and to less capable peers, all o

    which urther disadvantage them and generate ever more sub-

    stantial between-group gaps. The RiskStress Model, to whichwe turn later, suggests that such splaying may also be attributed

    to the cognitive decits and poorer health that chronic stress

    generates. Both cognitive decits and ill health then repeatedly

    disadvantage poverty-stricken children in one educational set-

    ting ater another.

    Pathway #1: Parenting PracticesWhat types o orces have social scientists conventionally under-

    stood as explaining the achievement gaps illustrated in Figure

    2? One reason poor children lag behind their more afuent

    peers is that their parents interact with them in ways that arent

    conducive to achievement. For example, psychologist Kathryn

    Grant and her colleagues have documented a strong and con-

    sistent relation between socioeconomic disadvantage and harsh,

    unresponsive parenting. In one national dataset, 85 percent

    o American parents above the poverty line were shown to be

    responsive, supportive, and encouraging to their children dur-

    ing inancy and toddlerhood, whereas only 75 percent o low-

    income parents had the same achievement-inducing parenting

    style. While most low-income parents (i.e., 75 percent) do pro-

    vide adequate levels o support and encouragement, these data

    reveal, then, a nontrivial dierence across income levels in the

    chances that children will experience a problematic parenting

    style. There is considerable evidence that at least a portion o the

    cognitive developmental consequences o early childhood pov-erty is due to this dierence.

    Pathway #2: Cognitive StimulationIts also well known that children rom low-income households

    tend to receive less cognitive stimulation and enrichment. For

    example, a child rom a low-income amily who enters rst

    grade has been exposed on average to just 25 hours o one-on-

    one picture book reading, whereas an entering middle-income

    child has been exposed on average to more than 1,000 hours

    o such reading. Likewise, during the rst three years o lie, a

    child with proessional parents will be exposed to three times as

    many words as a child with parents on welare.

    And its not just simple parental eects that account or theachievement decit. I a child is born into a high-income amily,

    he or she may also benet rom high-quality stimulation and

    enrichment rom extended amily, rom siblings and riends,

    and rom more ormal care providers. All o this redounds to the

    benet o higher-income children while urther handicapping

    low-income children.

    So much or the well-known pathways by which disadvan-

    tage is transmitted. We turn now to another and less-appreciated

    aspect o low-income environments that may also harm cogni-

    We will unpack this new RiskStress Model in the balance

    o our article. However, beore doing so, its useul to rst go

    over the evidence regarding the relation between poverty and

    achievement and then to present some o the well-known path-

    ways through which this relationship is generated. With that

    background in place, we can then describe the RiskStress

    Model, as represented in Figure 1.

    Poverty and AchievementIt is well known that children born into low-income amilies

    lag behind their middle- and upper-income counterparts on

    virtually all indices o achievement. To provide one example, a

    national study o elementary school children shows that childrenin the poorest quarter o American households begin kindergar-

    ten nearly 10 percent behind their middle-income and afuent

    classmates in math (Figure 2). Six years later, as they are about

    to enter middle school, the poorest quarter o American children

    have allen even urther behind, with the gap between them-

    selves and their most afuent schoolmates nearly doubling.

    The splaying pattern revealed here, a general one that holds

    across various outcomes, may be attributed to the tendency

    or advantage and disadvantage to accumulate over time. This

    figure 2 Average percentile rank on Peabody Individual AchievementMath.

    35

    6 8 10 12

    40

    Highest income quartile

    Third income quartile

    Second income quartile

    Lowest income quartile

    Age of child

    Scorepercentile

    45

    50

    55

    60

    65

    figure 1 A new pathway to account for the income-achievement gap.

    Low Achievement

    Source: James J. Heckman (2006). Skill Formation and the Economics o Investing in Disadvantaged

    Children. Science, 312(5782): 1900-1902.

    chronic StreSSPoverty cumuLAtive riSk exPoSure

  • 7/30/2019 Stressing Out the Poor

    4/6

    Pathways Winter 2011

    tive development. The key concern here: Children rom impover-

    ished households ace a wide array o physical and psychosocial

    stressors. Their homes, schools, and neighborhoods are much

    more chaotic than the settings in which middle- and upper-

    income children grow up. Such conditions can, in turn, produce

    toxic stress capable o damaging areas o the brain known tounderlie cognitive processessuch as attention, memory, and

    languagethat all combine to undergird academic success. In

    the pages that remain, we document each o the steps in the

    RiskStress Model.

    Poverty and Cumulative Risk ExposureThe stressors that poor children ace take both a physical and

    psychosocial orm. The physical orm is well documented; poor

    children are exposed to substandard environmental conditions

    including toxins, hazardous waste, ambient air and water pollu-

    tion, noise, crowding, poor housing, poorly maintained school

    buildings, residential turnover, trac congestion, poor neigh-

    borhood sanitation and maintenance, and crime. The psycho-

    social orm is also well documented; poor children experience

    signicantly higher levels o amily turmoil, amily separation,

    violence, and signicantly lower levels o structure and routine

    in their daily lives.

    An important aspect o early, disadvantaged settings may be

    exposure to more than one risk actor at a time. A powerul way

    to capture exposure to such multiple sources o stress and strain

    is the construct o cumulative risk. Although there are various

    ways to quantiy cumulative risk, one common approach is to

    simply count the number o physical or psychosocial risks to

    which a child has been exposed. In one UK study, the authors

    counted how oten children were exposed to such stresses as(a) living with a single parent, (b) experiencing amily discord,

    (c) experiencing oster or some other orm o institutional care,

    (d) living in a crowded home, and (e) attending a school with

    high turnover o both classmates and teachers. It was ound in

    this study that inner-city children experienced ar more o these

    stresses than did the better-o working-class children. The same

    result holds in the United States (see Figure 3). In rural New

    England, only 12 percent o middle-income nine-year-olds expe-

    rienced three or more physical and psychosocial risk actors,

    whereas nearly 50 percent o low-income children crossed this

    same threshold (o three risk actors).

    In a national U.S. sample o premature and low birth weight

    inants, Brooks-Gunn and colleagues similarly ound thatinants born into low-income amilies experienced nearly three

    times more risk actors than their middle-income counterparts

    by the time they were toddlers. These same low-income toddlers

    were seven times more likely than their afuent counterparts

    to experience a very high number o risk actors (> 6). The pat-

    tern is overwhelmingly clear: Being born into early poverty oten

    means exposure to many more physical and psychosocial risk

    actors.

    figure 4 Resting blood pressure in nine-year-old, White rural children.

    figure 5 Overnight stress hormones in nine-year-old, White rural childr

    figure 3 Cumulative risk exposure among low- and middle-incomerural nine-year-olds.

    0

    0 1 2 5 63 4

    10

    number of cumulAtive riSkS

    percentAgeofSchoolchildren

    expoSed

    20

    30

    40

    50

    60

    Poverty

    Middle-Income

    Note: Cumulative risks include amily turmoil, violence, child separation rom amily, noise, crowding

    and housing quality.

    Source: Gary W. Evans and Kimberly English. 2002. The Environment o Poverty: Multiple Stressor

    Exposure, Psychophysiological Stress, and Socioemotional Adjustment. Child Development, 73(4):1

    Source: Gary W. Evans and Kimberly English. 2002. The Environment o Poverty: Multiple Stressor E

    sure, Psychophysiological Stress, and Socioemotional Adjustment. Child Development, 73(4):1238-

    Source: Gary W. Evans and Kimberly English. 2002. The Environment o Poverty: Multiple Stressor E

    sure, Psychophysiological Stress, and Socioemotional Adjustment. Child Development, 73(4):1238-

    10099.5

    99

    100.5

    101

    102.5

    102

    101.5

    poverty middle income

    58

    57

    59

    57.5

    58.5

    59.5

    61

    60.5

    60

    poverty middle income

    diastolic

    cortisol epinephrine norepinephrine

    systolic

    mm/hg

    ug/mgc

    reAtine

    ng/mgc

    reAtine

    ng/mgc

    reAtine

    .01

    0

    .02

    .005

    .015

    .025

    .035

    .03

    poverty middle

    income

    6

    5

    4

    3

    2

    1

    0

    poverty middle

    income

    33

    32

    31

    30

    29

    poverty middle

    income

  • 7/30/2019 Stressing Out the Poor

    5/6

    20 Pathways Winter 2011

    Cumulative Risk Exposure and Chronic StressBut does such dierential exposure indeed result in higher

    stress levels among poor children? The simple answer is that it

    does. In cross-sectional analyses o 9- and 13-year-old children,

    Evans and colleagues ound that the risk exposure described

    in Figure 3 elevated baseline, resting blood pressure as well as

    overnight indices o such stress hormones as cortisol. At age 13,

    when challenged by mental arithmetic problems, children with

    higher levels o cumulative risk exposure did not show a typi-

    cal healthy response, instead exhibiting a muted rise in blood

    pressure. These same children also didnt recover as successully

    rom the mental challenge posed by these arithmetic problems(as indexed by the longer time it took their blood pressure to

    return to pre-stressor baseline levels). The evidence thus sug-

    gests that children exposed to high levels o cumulative risk are

    less ecient both in mobilizing and then shutting o physi-

    ological activity.

    The RiskStress Model, as represented in Figure 1, implies

    that the eect o amily poverty on stress is mediated by risk

    exposure. Although one would ideally like to test that mediation,

    its also important to simply document the association between

    poverty and stress (thereby ignoring the mediating actor). Many

    investigators have indeed documented that disadvantaged chil-

    dren have higher chronic physiological stress levels, as indicated

    by elevated resting blood pressure. A smaller number o studies

    have also uncovered higher levels o chronic stress hormones,

    such as cortisol, among disadvantaged children. To provide just

    a ew examples, Figures 4 and 5 show elevated resting blood

    pressure as well as higher overnight urinary stress hormones in

    a sample o nine-year-old rural children.

    The oregoing data, which pertain to nine-year-olds, dont

    tell us when such stress symptoms emerge. Do poverty-stricken

    children show evidence o elevated stress early on in their lives?

    Or do such symptoms only emerge later? With support rom

    the Stanord Center or the Study o Poverty and Inequality, we

    sought to answer this question by reanalyzing a national data set

    o very young at-risk children. The Inant Health and Develop-

    ment Program (IHDP) is a representative sample o low birth

    weight (< 2500 grams) and premature (< 37 weeks gestational

    age) babies born in 1985 at eight medical centers throughout

    the country. This sample o nearly 1,000 babies is racially and

    economically diverse (52 percent Black, 37 percent White, 11 per-

    cent Hispanic).

    We assessed resting blood pressure and childs height and

    weight at 24, 30, 36, 48, 60, and 78 months o age. The collec-tion o physical health data at such young ages and over time

    provided us with an unprecedented opportunity to examine the

    early trajectories o chronic stress among a high-risk sample

    o babies. Both baseline blood pressure levels and Body Mass

    Index (BMI) refect wear and tear on the body and are precur-

    sors o lielong health problems. The ormer is indicative o car-

    diovascular health and the latter o metabolic equilibrium. BMI,

    which refects at deposition, is measured as height divided by

    weight (kg/m2).

    We sought to assess whether these two measures o stress

    are elevated in poverty-stricken neighborhoods. Low-income

    neighborhoods, as dened in our study, have median household

    incomes below $30,000 (in 1980 dollars), while middle income

    neighborhoods have median income levels exceeding $30,000

    per household. As is evident in Figures 6 and 7, babies grow-

    ing up in low-income neighborhoods have health trajectories

    indicative o elevated chronic stress. Additional statistical con-

    trols or inant birth weight, health, and demographic character-

    istics did not alter these trajectories. These gures also reveal,

    even more importantly, that elevated stress emerges very early

    or children growing up in low-income neighborhoods. BMI,

    figures 67 Developmental trajectories in chronic stress in relation to neighborhood poverty.

    15

    24 36 48 60 72

    16

    17

    Afuent Neighbors Near Poor & Poor Neighbors

    Age in monthS

    bmi

    50

    55

    24 36 48 60 72

    60

    65

    70

    dbp

    Age in monthS

    Afuent Neighbors Near Poor & Poor Neighbors

    BMI rom 24 to 78 Months by Neighborhood Poverty Diastolic Blood Pressure rom 24 to 78 Months by Neighborhood Poverty(with controls or blood pressure method & childs state)

  • 7/30/2019 Stressing Out the Poor

    6/6

    Pathways Winter 2011

    ConclusionChildhood socioeconomic disadvantage leads to decits in aca-

    demic achievement and occupational attainment. Its long been

    argued that such decits arise because poor children are exposed

    to inadequate cognitive stimulation and to parenting styles that

    dont encourage achievement. We dont dispute the important

    role o these two variables. But we have outlined here evidence

    or a new, complementary pathway that links early childhood

    poverty to high levels o exposure to multiple risks, which in

    turn elevates chronic toxic stress. This cascade can begin very

    early in lie. Even young babies growing up in low-income

    neighborhoods already evidence elevated chronic stress. Thisstress then accounts or a signicant portion o the association

    between poverty and working memory, a critical cognitive skill

    involved in language and reading acquisition.

    The RiskStress Model suggests that the povertyachievement

    link can be broken by addressing (a) the tendency o poverty to be

    associated with physical or psychosocial risks (e.g., environmen-

    tal toxins, amily turmoil), (b) the eects o such risks on stress,

    and (c) the eects o stress on achievement. I this model bears

    up under urther testing, it would be useul to explore which o

    these pathways is most amenable to intervention.

    Gary W. Evans is the Elizabeth Lee Vincent Professor of Human

    Ecology at Cornell University. Jeanne Brooks-Gunn is the Virginia

    & Leonard Marx Professor of Child Development & Education and

    Co-Director of the National Center for Children and Families at

    Teachers College, Columbia University. Pamela Kato Klebanov is

    a Senior Research Scientist at the National Center for Children and

    Families at Teachers College, Columbia University and a Visiting

    Research Collaborator at the Center for Research on Child Well-

    being at Princeton University.

    or example, proves to be unusually low among poor children

    under ve years old, but it then takes o as these children grow

    older. The blood pressure measure, by contrast, registers high

    among low-income children rom almost the very beginning o

    our measurements (i.e., 24 months). This research conrms,

    then, that low-income children are more likely than others to

    develop dangerous stress trajectories very early on in their child-hood. As we discuss below, this has proound consequences or

    their likelihood o success in school and beyond.

    Chronic Stress and the Achievement GapThe next and nal step in our chain model pertains to the eects

    o chronic stress on achievement. Here we turn to an important

    longitudinal program on poverty and the brain at the University

    o Pennsylvania conducted by Martha Farah and her colleagues.

    In a series o studies with multiple samples drawn rom lower-

    and middle-class Black amilies in Philadelphia, Farah and col-

    leagues show that several areas o the brain appear vulnerable

    to early childhood deprivation. Using batteries o neurocogni-

    tive tests o brain unction and brain imaging studies, Farah andother neuroscientists can map the areas o the brain that are

    recruited by neurocognitive tasks. As shown in Figure 8, among

    the areas o the brain most sensitive to childhood SES are lan-

    guage, long-term memory (LTM), working memory (WM), and

    executive control. What the graph depicts is the separation,

    in standard deviation units, between a low- and middle-SES

    sample o 11-year-old Black children rom Philadelphia. For this

    sample, one standard deviation represents about one-th o the

    total distribution o scores. Samples diering by 3.5 or more

    standard deviations are virtually non-overlapping. Given that the

    samples dier by about 3.5 standard deviations or all our areas

    o brain unctioning, this means that there is virtually no overlap

    between poor and middle-class Black children when it comesto language, long-term memory, working memory, or executive

    control. Eleven-year-old Black children rom lower SES amilies

    reveal dramatic decits in multiple, basic cognitive unctions

    critical to learning and eventual success in society. These results

    reveal the starkly cognitive oundation to the poor perormance

    o low-income children.

    But is this achievement gap attributable to cumulative risk and

    chronic stress? With a recent ollow-up o the sample depicted

    in Figures 4 and 5, Evans and colleagues have now provided the

    rst test o the nal link in the RiskStress Model. The base-

    line nding rom their research is that working memory in early

    adulthood (i.e., age 17) deteriorated in direct relation to the num-

    ber o years the children lived in poverty (rom birth through age

    13). I, in other words, a child lived in poverty continuously, his

    or her working memory was greatly compromised. The main

    result o interest, however, was that such deterioration occurred

    only among poverty-stricken children with chronically elevated

    physiological stress (as measured between ages 9 and 13). That

    is, chronic early childhood poverty did not lead to working mem-

    ory decits among children who somehow avoided experiencing

    the stress that usually accompanies poverty.

    0

    1

    .5

    1.5

    2.5

    2

    3

    3.5

    4

    lAnguAge ltm Wm executive

    control

    SdSepArAtor

    figure 8 Effect sizes measured in standard deviationsof separation between low- and middle-SES

    10- to 12-year-old, African American children.

    Source: Martha J. Farah, David M. Shera, Jessica H. Savage, Laura Betancourt, Joan M.

    Giannetta, Nancy L. Brodsky, Elsa K. Malmud, and Hallam Hurt. 2006. Childhood Poverty:

    Specifc Associations with Neurocognitive Development. Brain Research,1110(1): 166-74.