7
Actu Psychlutr Scund 1995: 92: 453-459 Printed in Belgium - ull rights reserved Copyright 0 Munksgaurd 1995 ACTA PSYCHIATRICA SCANDINAVICA ISSN 0001-690X Suicide among New Zealand Maori: is history repeating itself? Skegg K, Cox B, Broughton J. Suicide among New Zealand Maori: is history repeating itself’? Acta Psychiatr Scand 1995: 92: 453-459. 0 Munksgaard 1995. Suicide rates for New Zealanders identified as Maori were analysed for the period 1957-91 and compared with those for non-Maori people. Overall, Maori men had about half the risk of suicide of non-Maori men, and Maori women one-third the risk of non-Maori women. Nevertheless, there was a sharp increase in suicide rates for Maori aged 15-24 years during the period studied, with rates for the 1987-91 time period of 35.2/100,000 for men and 6.0/100,000 for women. These were similar to the high suicide rates of young non-Maori New Zealanders. Suicide among Maori in pre-European times appears to have been embedded in traditional culture and may have occurred particularly among bereaved women; today the pattern is one of high rates in young men who are likely to have been alienated I from their culture. A common stereotype of the suicide patterns of col- onised indigenous peoples is of low suicide rates before colonisation, followed by very high contem- porary rates resulting from the impact of colonisa- tion on the traditional way of life. For the Maori people of New Zealand there were no written records before the arrival of the first Europeans, but the documented evidence on Maori suicide by early Eu- ropean writers suggests that, far from being rare, suicide was very much part of traditional Maori so- ciety. Marsden (l), referred in a journal written in 1819 to suicide as occurring commonly among Maori people. Maning (2), writing under the nom-de-plume “a Pakeha Maori” to denote simultaneously his close association with the Nga Puhi people and his Eu- ropean (pakehu) origin, claimed that “In the first years of my residence in the country (suicide) was of almost daily occurrence”. Dieffenbach (3) and oth- ers described the ritual suicide of widows on be- reavement, often by hanging, and suicide because of shame or disgrace. White (4) and Gudgeon (5) gave details of cases in which death was seen as prefer- able to life without honour. According to Johnstone (6), another motive for suicide was frustrated love. Clearly the comments of early European writers are a very limited basis on which to build a picture of suicide in pre-European Maori society, and it is possible that some writers could have highlighted K. Skegg’, B. Cox’, J. Broughton2 Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Otago Medical School, Dunedin, New Zealand Department of Preventive and Social Medicine, Key words: suicide; epidemiology; ethnic groups Keren Skegg, Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Otago Medical School, Dunedin, New Zealand Accepted for publication July 2, 1995 I aspects of Maori life that they regarded as sensa- tional. Another source of information is Maori oral tradition. The existence of stories about suicides coming from a variety of iwi (tribes) supports the impression of the early settlers that suicide (whaku- rnornori) was an accepted feature of Maori society. All the motives described above are echoed in the traditional stories of Puhi-Huia, a grieving widow, Pare, who was frustrated in love (7), Te Aohuruhuru, who was shamed publicly (8) and Tikawe, whose husband forsook her for another (9). It is notewor- thy that all of the above were women. The story of Tikawe includes mention of Motutawa, a “famous suicide cliff’, and Oppenheim (10) tells of how Whangaroa harbour is said to have taken its name (Whakamomori) from a suicide. Maning (2) believed that suicide was declining rapidly during his time in New Zealand. “I do not believe that one case of suicide occurs now for twenty when I first came into the country”. According to Polack (11) the custom of suicide following bereavement was becoming less common during the late 1830s. It is impossible to know the actual suicide rates that lay behind these historical accounts. Suicide among Maori people today, however, is more sus- ceptible to objective analysis. Has the stereotype of suicide in colonised indigenous people any more va- lidity for New Zealand today than in the past? To identify recent patterns of suicide among Maori 453

Suicide among New Zealand Maori: is history repeating itself?

  • Upload
    k-skegg

  • View
    219

  • Download
    4

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Suicide among New Zealand Maori: is history repeating itself?

Actu Psychlutr Scund 1995: 92: 453-459 Printed in Belgium - ull rights reserved

Copyright 0 Munksgaurd 1995 ACTA PSYCHIATRICA

SCANDINAVICA ISSN 0001-690X

Suicide among New Zealand Maori: is history repeating itself?

Skegg K, Cox B, Broughton J. Suicide among New Zealand Maori: is history repeating itself’? Acta Psychiatr Scand 1995: 92: 453-459. 0 Munksgaard 1995.

Suicide rates for New Zealanders identified as Maori were analysed for the period 1957-91 and compared with those for non-Maori people. Overall, Maori men had about half the risk of suicide of non-Maori men, and Maori women one-third the risk of non-Maori women. Nevertheless, there was a sharp increase in suicide rates for Maori aged 15-24 years during the period studied, with rates for the 1987-91 time period of 35.2/100,000 for men and 6.0/100,000 for women. These were similar to the high suicide rates of young non-Maori New Zealanders. Suicide among Maori in pre-European times appears to have been embedded in traditional culture and may have occurred particularly among bereaved women; today the pattern is one of high rates in young men who are likely to have been alienated

I from their culture.

A common stereotype of the suicide patterns of col- onised indigenous peoples is of low suicide rates before colonisation, followed by very high contem- porary rates resulting from the impact of colonisa- tion on the traditional way of life. For the Maori people of New Zealand there were no written records before the arrival of the first Europeans, but the documented evidence on Maori suicide by early Eu- ropean writers suggests that, far from being rare, suicide was very much part of traditional Maori so- ciety.

Marsden (l), referred in a journal written in 1819 to suicide as occurring commonly among Maori people. Maning (2), writing under the nom-de-plume “a Pakeha Maori” to denote simultaneously his close association with the Nga Puhi people and his Eu- ropean (pakehu) origin, claimed that “In the first years of my residence in the country (suicide) was of almost daily occurrence”. Dieffenbach (3) and oth- ers described the ritual suicide of widows on be- reavement, often by hanging, and suicide because of shame or disgrace. White (4) and Gudgeon ( 5 ) gave details of cases in which death was seen as prefer- able to life without honour. According to Johnstone (6 ) , another motive for suicide was frustrated love.

Clearly the comments of early European writers are a very limited basis on which to build a picture of suicide in pre-European Maori society, and it is possible that some writers could have highlighted

K. Skegg’, B. Cox’, J. Broughton2 ’ Department of Psychological Medicine,

University of Otago Medical School, Dunedin, New Zealand

Department of Preventive and Social Medicine,

Key words: suicide; epidemiology; ethnic groups

Keren Skegg, Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Otago Medical School, Dunedin, New Zealand

Accepted for publication July 2, 1995 I

aspects of Maori life that they regarded as sensa- tional. Another source of information is Maori oral tradition. The existence of stories about suicides coming from a variety of iwi (tribes) supports the impression of the early settlers that suicide (whaku- rnornori) was an accepted feature of Maori society. All the motives described above are echoed in the traditional stories of Puhi-Huia, a grieving widow, Pare, who was frustrated in love (7), Te Aohuruhuru, who was shamed publicly (8) and Tikawe, whose husband forsook her for another (9). It is notewor- thy that all of the above were women. The story of Tikawe includes mention of Motutawa, a “famous suicide cliff’, and Oppenheim (10) tells of how Whangaroa harbour is said to have taken its name (Whakamomori) from a suicide. Maning (2) believed that suicide was declining rapidly during his time in New Zealand. “I do not believe that one case of suicide occurs now for twenty when I first came into the country”. According to Polack (11) the custom of suicide following bereavement was becoming less common during the late 1830s.

It is impossible to know the actual suicide rates that lay behind these historical accounts. Suicide among Maori people today, however, is more sus- ceptible to objective analysis. Has the stereotype of suicide in colonised indigenous people any more va- lidity for New Zealand today than in the past? To identify recent patterns of suicide among Maori

453

Page 2: Suicide among New Zealand Maori: is history repeating itself?

Skegg et al.

people, we analysed suicide rates for people identi- fied as Maori in New Zealand for the last 35 years and compared them with rates for non-Maori people.

Material and methods

The numbers of deaths from suicide (International Classification of Diseases, Ninth Revision [ ICD 91, rubrics E 950-959) in people identified as Maori and non-Maori for the period 1957-91 were obtained from the annual publications of the National Health Statistics Centre. Age-specific suicide rates for the two ethnic groups were derived for men and women separately for successive decades and the most re- cent five-year period.

Cumulative suicide rates for ages 0-74 years were calculated for each quinquennium for Maori and non-Maori men and women. The cumulative suicide rate provides an estimate of the risk an individual would have of committing suicide before 75 years of age if no other causes of death were in operation. A cumulative rate is an age-standardised rate, enabling direct comparisons between different populations.

Methods used by Maori people for committing suicide were examined for the two sexes separately, for successive decades and the most recent five-year period. For each period, cumulative rates (0-74 years) were calculated for the two sexes for poison- ing with solid or liquid substances (ICD 9 rubric E 950), hanging, strangulation and suffocation (E 953), firearms and explosives (E 955) and for all other methods combined.

Cumulative suicide rates for the age range avail- able for successive generations (or birth cohorts) were also calculated for Maori and non-Maori men and women respectively. The ratios (Maori to non- Maori) of these cumulative suicide rates for different generations of men and women represent the relative risk of suicide between Maori and non-Maori for different generations over the ages represented.

Results

The numbers and rates of Maori male and female suicides from 1957 to 1991 are shown in Table 1. For Maori men and women aged 15-34 years, sui- cide rates increased significantly over the 35 years. While for Maori women in this age group the risk of suicide has almost doubled, it has increased three- fold among Maori men. The most dramatic increase among Maori men aged 15-34 years has occurred since 1986, whereas among Maori women aged 15-34 years suicide rates increased most in the 1977-86 time period.

Overall, Maori men have half the risk of suicide of non-Maori men and Maori women have about one-third the risk of non-Maori women (Fig. 1). The cumulative suicide rate (0-74 years of age) has in- creased significantly over the 35-year period for Maori men but not for Maori women. These over- all trends are similar to those among non-Maori men and women.

The age-specific suicide rates for Maori and non- Maori men and women for the decade 1982 to 1991

Table 1. Number of Maori male and female suicides, age-specific suicide rates (per 100,000) and cumulative suicide rates (0-74 years of age) in successive time periods.

Age Group (years) Time CSR’ Total period 0-14 15-24 25-34 35-44 45-54 55-64 65-74 75-84 8 5 t ( % I Number

Men

1957-66 1 0.2

1967-76 2 0.4

1977-86 1 0.2

1987-9 1 4 1.6

Women

1957-66 0 0.0

1967-76 0 0.0

1977-86 4 0.7

1987-9 1 0 0.0

10 6.4 17 7.2 40

11.6 61

35.2

4 2.6 6

2.6 17 5.0 10 6.0

14 12.4 13 8.8 28

13.6 26

19.4

3 2.6 5

3.4 9

4.3 6

4.3

11 14.7 13

11.9 16

11.6 18

21.0

0 0.0 5

4.6 2

1.4 1

1.1

8 4 15.2 13.3

4 9 5.9 22.1 18 7

18.3 12.9 6 3

10.2 7.9

2 4.2 3

4.5 3

3.0 1

1.7

0 0.0 2

5.3 1

1.8 0

0.0

2 15.2

3 16.4

2 7.9

1 7.0

1 0 51 23.1 0.0 0.86

1 0 62 21.1 0.0 0.70

1 0 113 14.3 0.0 0.79

0 0 119 0.0 0.0 1.02

1 0 9.1 0.0

1 0 5.9 0.0 0 0 0.0 0.0 0 0

0.0 0.0

0 10 0.0 0.22 0 22

0.0 0.25 0 36

0.0 0.16 0 18

0.0 0.13

’ Cumulative suicide rate.

454

Page 3: Suicide among New Zealand Maori: is history repeating itself?

Suicide among New Zealand Maori

45 -

40 -

35 -

Cumulative suicide rate (per cent)

9 Non-Maori male Maori male

Maori female #'

---+-- ,

----&-- Non-Maori female

#'

0- Non-Maori male a

0.6 -

0.5 -

0.4 -

0.3 -

0.2 -

0.1 -

1.61 - Maori male ---*-- Non-Maori female

5 -

0

0.0 J 0

1957-61 1962-66 1967-71 1972-76 1977-81 1982-86 1987-91

Time period

Fig. I . Trends in Maori and non-Maori male and female cumu- lative suicide rates (0-74 years of age) for five-year time periods from 1957 to 1991.

e V ,' ,'

I I I I I 1 - a - 1

.d

5 15 25 35 45 55 65 75 85

Age (years)

Fig. 2. Maori and non-Maori male and female age-specific sui- cide rates for the 1982-91 time period.

are shown in Fig. 2. Maori men have lower suicide rates than non-Maori at all ages and, unlike non- Maori men, their risk of suicide does not appear to increase beyond 55 years of age. The recent increase in suicide rates for Maori men and women aged 15 to 24 years has produced rates similar to non-Maori in this age group. Among those aged over 25 years, Maori women experienced declining suicide rates with increasing age, whereas non-Maori women ex- perienced a slow increase in suicide rates up to the age of retirement.

Hanging, strangulation and suffocation (E 953) has increasingly been the predominant method of suicide chosen by Maori men, followed by firearms or explosives (E 955), as shown in Fig. 3. Other methods of suicide have been used much less fre- quently. The suicide rate from hanging has increased substantially over the 35 years among Maori men aged 15-34 years. Among Maori women, poisoning by solid or liquid substances (E 950) has become the commonest method, whereas methods of suicide other than hanging (E 953), poisoning (E 950) or firearms (E 955) have declined, as can be seen in Fig. 4.

Fig. 5 and 6 show the Maori to non-Maori ratios of cumulative suicide rates (relative risks) for suc- cessive generations (or birth cohorts) indicated by their median year of birth for men and women, re- spectively. While the risk of suicide has been lower

Cumulative suicide rate (per cent)

,A Hanging

,,'

/ Poisoning

0.0 ! 1957-66 1967-76 1977-86 1987-91

Time period

Fig. 3. Trends in Maori male cumulative suicide rates (0-74 years of age) according to method.

455

Page 4: Suicide among New Zealand Maori: is history repeating itself?

Skegg et al.

Cumulative suicide rate (per cent)

0.20 1

Relative risk

0.15-

0.10-

0.05 -

0.004 1957-66 1967-76 1977-86 1987-91

Time period

Fig. 4 . Trends in Maori female cumulative suicide rates (0-74 years of age) according to method.

Relative risk

l o /

0.1 1 ----@-- Upper 95%CI

Relative risk

Lower 95%CI

- ----&--

0 . 0 1 1 . I ' I ' I ' I ' I ' I ' I

1902 1912 1922 1932 1942 1952 1962 1972

Median year of birth

Fig. 5. Ratio of Maori to non-Maori cumulative suicide rates (relative risk) for males by birth cohort, as indicated by median year of birth.

for Maori than non-Maori for most generations, it appears to have increased to that of non-Maori for men born since about 1966. For Maori women the

1 1

0.1

1902 1912 1922 1932 1942 1952 1962 1972

Median year of birth

Fig. 6. Ratio of Maori to non-Maori cumulative suicide rates (relative risk) for females by birth cohort, as indicated by median year of birth.

risk of suicide compared to non-Maori women ap- pears to have increased gradually for successive gen- erations and is now the same as for non-Maori women for those born since about 1966.

Discussion

The pattern of Maori suicide has changed greatly in New Zealand since the 1950s. The extent of this is not immediately evident, because changes in cumu- lative suicide rates for Maori men and women were modest, and rates remained substantially lower than for non-Maori, particularly among women. Age- specific rates, however, showed a different picture. Suicide rates for Maori youth increased sharply so that by 1987-91 they were little different from those of their non-Maori counterparts, thus sharing in na- tional youth suicide rates among the highest in the developed world (12).

Maori rates are believed to be underestimated in mortality statistics because of the methods of ascer- tainment of ethnicity used for death registrations and for censuses. Some Maori deaths are classified as non-Maori by default, because the recording of Maori ethnicity on a death certificate depends on the undertaker ascertaining that the person had 50% or more of Maori biological origin. Brown (13) esti- mated the understating of Maori deaths to be of the order of 28%. In an Auckland study of coronary heart disease mortality (14), Maori mortality was

456

Page 5: Suicide among New Zealand Maori: is history repeating itself?

Suicide among New Zealand Maori

found to have been understated by 82% when na- tional death registrations were compared with a reg- ister that used a close relative’s assessment of the cultural affiliation of the deceased to determine eth- nicity. Understatement on death certificates is un- likely to have been as pronounced for suicide as for coronary heart disease, because the distressing cir- cumstances would probably have resulted in fuller information emerging about the victim, but under- statement could still have been substantial. Another issue concerns the denominators used in calculating mortality rates. Whereas death certificates have al- ways used a biological definition of ethnicity, the quinquennial census shifted from a biological defi- nition to self-identification of ethnicity in 1986. Brown (13) had estimated that the use of a cultural affiliation question instead of a biological question in the census would result in an increase of the Maori population of only about 3 % . This was because many Maori census respondents had probably al- ready been answering the ethnic question on the basis of self-identification rather than biological ori- gin. The denominators used for calculating Maori suicide rates were thus based on a broader definition of Maori ethnicity (particularly from 1986 onwards) than the numerators. This is likely to have added to the underestimation of Maori suicide rates resulting from misclassification on death certificates.

The true Maori cumulative suicide rates could therefore have been as high as those of non-Maori for men, although they would have still been lower for women. The Maori youth suicide rates, already a cause for concern, might in reality have been even higher than those of non-Maori young people.

The Maori and non-Maori cumulative suicide rates shown in Fig. 1 were not adjusted for social class. Smith and Pearce (15), in a study of determi- nants of Maori mortality, calculated the relative risk for suicide in Maori men aged 15-64 years of age (compared with non-Maori men). Their adjustment for social class had the effect of slightly increasing the difference between Maori and non-Maori risks of suicide. In the present study, adjustment for so- cial class would also be expected to widen somewhat the difference between Maori and non-Maori.

Methods used by Maori people for committing suicide changed over the 35 years studied. Hanging became the main method of suicide for men. Many of these hangings would have occurred in custody: 71 % of suicides by hanging in Maori men aged 15-49 years in New Zealand in the years 1980-88 occurred in custody (16). For Maori women, poi- soning (which did not even feature as a method of suicide in 1957-66) became the commonest method, tending to replace methods other than hanging or firearms.

These patterns of suicide differed somewhat from

those of the total New Zealand population. Among the total New Zealand male population, hanging was increasingly used as a method of suicide over the same period, but did not have the same dominance as a method because firearms and carbon monoxide were also used increasingly for suicide (17). The adoption of poisoning as a method of suicide by Maori women echoed on a smaller scale the increase in total female suicide by poisoning in New Zealand in the 1960s. For the total female population, how- ever, this increase was followed by a marked decline which coincided with reducing the prescription of barbiturates (17), whereas for Maori women the fre- quency of suicide by poisoning declined only slightly.

For both sexes the current Maori pattern of de- clining suicide rates with increasing age bears little resemblance to the non-Maori patterns, which were characterised by an increase after 55 years of age in men and a steady increase until about the age of retirement in women. At present it is only possible to speculate on the reasons for this. In Maori so- ciety, older people are held in great esteem and carry much tribal responsibility. The valued roles available to them as they mature, and their greater involve- ment in Maori cultural life compared with young people (18) may enhance their sense of security and wellbeing, and this may partly explain the persis- tently low rates at older ages for Maori people. The birth-cohort analysis showed that the risk of suicide for Maori born since about 1966 was about the same as for non-Maori whereas it used to be lower. It cannot yet be discerned whether the recent genera- tion of young Maori will carry risks of suicide similar to those of non-Maori throughout life or whether their risks will be lower in later life. If a secure Maori cultural identity is a protective factor for Maori old people, then future suicide rates in present genera- tions of young Maori may depend partly on the ex- tent to which they embrace this identity.

The steep rise in Maori youth suicide paralleled the rise in suicide for all young New Zealanders. Like Australia (and to a lesser extent Canada and the USA), New Zealand has witnessed the worrying development of youth suicide rates that are unusu- ally high in relation to total suicide rates, compared with most Western countries (19). The dispropor- tion of youth suicide compared with total suicide rate is much more striking within the Maori popu- lation, as can be seen in Fig. 2. This type of pattern has been observed in a number of indigenous peoples, for example, among certain native North American tribes (20, 21), in Micronesia (22), among Aboriginal people in Australia (23), and in Western Samoa (24). The youth suicide rates in some of these communi- ties were much higher than those of Maori young people - for example, Rubinstein (22) found young male suicide rates of well over 150/100,000 in some

457

Page 6: Suicide among New Zealand Maori: is history repeating itself?

Skegg et al.

Micronesian islands in 1975-79, and in 1986 the suicide rate for native Canadian men aged 15-29 years was over 100/100,000 (25). Disintegration of the traditional way of life, particularly family life, substance abuse, and a feeling of being stranded between two cultures are factors thought to under- lie high youth suicide rates among some indigenous peoples (20-22, 26).

It will be important to ascertain to what extent suicide among Maori youth has increased because of factors related to their ethnic background, and to what extent the rise in suicide is because Maori youth increasingly resemble non-Maori youth in their be- haviour, including suicidal behaviour. Although the similarity in rates between young Maori and non- Maori New Zealanders might lead one to imagine that similarities in problems and behaviour underlie these results, it is notable that non-fatal suicidal be- haviour has been very different in the two ethnic groups. In 1976-85, young Maori men and women had much higher rates of hospital admission for non- fatal self-inflicted injury than young non-Maori people (27).

Maori youth have experienced much more ad- verse social conditions than those of non-Maori youth. They have been educationally disadvantaged, with 37% of Maori school leavers in 1991 leaving without a qualification compared with 16% of all school leavers (28). Their unemployment rates have also been much higher - in 1991, Maori men and women aged 20-24 years were twice as likely as non-Maori to be unemployed (29). In 1991, first ad- mission rates for alcohol and drug dependence for Maori men and women aged 15-24 years were con- siderably higher than for non-Maori men and women (30). Some problems of young non-Maori people are thus being experienced in greater severity by young Maori people. An important point of difference is the proportion of suicides that occurred in custody. In 1980-88, 23% of all suicides among Maori men aged 15-24 years occurred in prison or police cus- tody, compared with only 2.2% among non-Maori men (16). Although the suicide rates of Maori and non-Maori youth are similar, it cannot be assumed that reasons behind the rates are similar. Cultural factors cannot be ignored.

Belonging to the group is very important for Maori. Durie (3 1) observed that traditionally Maori did not seek to own or possess anything, but to belong. One belonged to a family that belonged to a h a p that belonged to an iwi. For young Maori today the sense of belonging to their social group remains a key element of their personal identity. Therefore to be ostracised from the group or to participate in risk taking behaviour in order to maintain their status within the group may lead the individual to become vulnerable to suicide. Conversely, the security af-

forded by acceptance within a group may protect the young Maori from suicide when everything seems against them. The unity within the group may pro- vide a feeling of wellbeing and mutual support that mitigates against suicide.

The cultural alienation of many Maori young people must be taken seriously as a possible factor underlying Maori youth suicide. Eckersley (32) has stressed the importance of culture with its “ability to provide a sense of belonging and purpose, and so a sense of meaning and self-worth, and a moral frame- work to guide our conduct”. He suggested that just as the consequences for indigenous people are seen when their culture is undermined, so countries like Australia and New Zealand may be seeing the con- sequences of the failure of modern Western culture to meet such fundamental human requirements. Young Maori and non-Maori New Zealanders may therefore each be experiencing cultural deprivation of different kinds, which contributes to their high rates of suicide.

Whereas many traditional societies are believed to have had low suicide rates, it appears that suicide was not uncommon among the Maori people in pre- European times, for example when honour was lost or a wife was bereaved. For Maori people in recent decades, suicide has been less common than for non-Maori people, especially at older ages. The re- cent increase in suicide among young Maori men and women to very high levels gives concern that history might be about to repeat itself, particularly if these high rates carry through to older ages. Never- theless, the emerging pattern is very different. Whereas in the past, the act of suicide was embed- ded in traditional culture and may have occurred particularly among bereaved women, today it occurs mainly among young men who are likely to have been alienated from their culture.

References

1.

2.

3.

4.

5.

6. 7.

8.

9.

10.

ELDER JR (ed). The Letters and Journals of Samuel Mars- den, 1765-1838. Dunedin: Coulls Somerville Wilkie & A.H. Reed, 1932. MANINC FE. Old New Zealand. Auckland: Creighton & Scales, 1863. DIEFFENBACH E. Travels in New Zealand. London: John Murray, 1843. WHITE J. Te Rou. London: Sanipson Low, Marston, Low & Searle, 1874. GUDGEON WE. Whaka-momore. Journal of the Polynesian Society 1906: 15: 163-174. JOHNSTONE JC. Maoria. London: Chapman & Hall, 1874. WHITE J . The Ancient History of the Maori. Vols I1 and IV. Wellington: Government Printer, 1887-90. ORBELL M. Traditional Maori Stories. Auckland: Reed, 1992. POMARE M, COWAN J. Legends of the Maori. Wellington: Fine Arts, 1930. OPPENHEIM RS. Maori Death Customs. Wellington: A.H. & A.W. Reed, 1973.

458

Page 7: Suicide among New Zealand Maori: is history repeating itself?

Suicide among New Zealand Maori

11 . POLACK JS. Manners and Customs of the New Zealanders. Vol 1. London: Madden, 1840.

12. BARWICK H. Youth Suicide Prevention Project: Workshop report and literature review. Wellington: Department of Health, 1992.

13. BROWN PG. An Investigation of Official Ethnic Statistics. Occasional Paper No 5. Wellington: Department of Statis- tics, 1983.

14. GRAHAM P, JACKSON R, BEAGLEHOLE R, DE BOER G. The validity of Maori mortality statistics. New Zealand Medi- cal Journal 1989: 102: 124-126.

15. SMITH AH, PEARCE NE. Determinants of differences in mortality between New Zealand Maoris and non-Maoris aged 15-64. New Zealand Medical Journal 1984: 97: 101-108.

16. SKEGG K, COX B. Suicide in custody: occurrence in Maori and non-Maori New Zealanders. New Zealand Medical Jour- nal 1993: 106: 1-4.

17. SKEGG K, COX B. Suicide in New Zealand 1957-1986: the influence of age, period and birth-cohort. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 1991: 25: 181-190.

18. MURCHIE E. Rapuora: Health and Maori Women. Welling- ton: Maori Women’s Welfare League, 1984.

19. PRITCHARD C. Youth suicide and gender in Australia and New Zealand compared with countries of the western world 1973-1987. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychia- try 1992: 26: 609-617.

20. Ross CA, DAVIS B. Suicide and parasuicide in a northern Canadian native community. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry

21. KETTL PA, BIXLER EO. Suicide in Alaska natives, 1979-

22. RUBINSTEIN DH. Epidemic suicide among Micronesian ado-

1986: 31: 331-334.

1984. Psychiatry 1991: 54: 55-63.

lescents. Social Science and Medicine 1983: 17: 657-665.

23. CLAYER JR, CZECHOWKZ AS. Suicide by Aboriginal people in South Australia: comparison with suicide deaths in the total urban and rural populations. Medical Journal of Aus- tralia 1991: 154: 683-685.

24. BOWLES JR. Suicide and attempted suicide in contemporary Western Samoa. In: Hezel FX et al., ed. Culture, youth and suicide in the Pacific. Honolulu: University of Hawaii 1985:

25. Canadian Task Force on the Periodic Health Examination. Periodic health examination, 1990 update: 2. Early detection of depression and prevention of suicide. Canadian Medical Association Journal 1990: 142: 1233-1238.

26. LAFROMBOISE T D , BIGFOOT DS. Cultural and cognitive considerations in the prevention of American Indian adoles- cent suicide. Journal of Adolescence 1988: 11: 139-153.

27. SKEGG K. Suicide and self-inflicted injury 1976-1985. In: National Health Statistics Centre, ed. Contemporary health issues. Wellington: Department of Health, 1989.

28. New Zealand Official Yearbook. Auckland: Statistics New Zealand, 1994.

29. Public Health Commission. Our health, our future. Welling- ton: Public Health Commission, 1993.

30. National Health Statistics Centre. Mental health data. Welling- ton: Department of Health, 1991.

31. DURIE E. The law and the land. In: Phillips J, ed. Te whenua te iwi: the land and the people. Wellington: Allen & Unwin,

32. ECKERSLEY R . Failing a generation: the impact of culture on the health and well-being of youth. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health 1993: 29: Suppl. 1, S16-Sl9.

15-35.

1987: 78-81.

459