9
British Journal of Social Psychology (2013), 52, 377–385 © 2013 The British Psychological Society www.wileyonlinelibrary.com Brief report Through the looking glass: Focusing on long-term goals increases immanent justice reasoning Mitchell J. Callan 1 *, Annelie J. Harvey 1 , Rael J. Dawtry 2 and Robbie M. Sutton 2 1 Department of Psychology, University of Essex, UK 2 School of Psychology, University of Kent, UK Immanent justice reasoning involves causally attributing a negative event to someone’s prior moral failings, even when such a causal connection is physically implausible. This study examined the degree to which immanent justice represents a form of motivated reasoning in the service of satisfying the need to believe in a just world. Drawing on a manipulation that has been shown to activate justice motivation, participants causally attributed a freak accident to a man’s prior immoral (vs. moral) behaviour to a greater extent when they first focused on their long-term (vs. short-term) goals. These findings highlight the important function believing in a just world plays in self-regulatory processes by implicating the self in immanent justice reasoning about fluke events in the lives of others. Immanent justice reasoning involves causally attributing a negative outcome to someone’s prior misdeeds, even when such a causal connection is physically implausible. When we infer that a man came down with a mysterious illness because he robbed many people of their money, we are engaging in immanent justice reasoning (Raman & Winer, 2004). Although Piaget (1932/1965) proposed that the use of immanent justice reasoning declines with age, recent research has shown that adults, at times, also entertain notions of immanent justice (Callan, Ellard, & Nicol, 2006; Callan, Sutton, & Dovale, 2010; Maes, 1998; Raman & Winer, 2002, 2004; Young, Morris, Burrus, Krishnan, & Regmi, 2011). In fact, Raman and Winer (2004) call in to question developmental stage accounts of immanent justice reasoning by demonstrating that immanent justice responses to events are more common among adults than school children. But why would adults indulge in causal explanations for events that do not jibe with the physical laws of cause-and-effect? Several explanations for why adults sometimes engage in immanent justice reasoning have been identified. Raman and Winer (2002, 2004) suggested that culturally normative *Correspondence should be addressed to Mitchell J. Callan, Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester CO4 3SQ, UK (e-mail: [email protected]). DOI:10.1111/bjso.12022 377

Through the looking glass: Focusing on long-term goals increases immanent justice reasoning

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Through the looking glass: Focusing on long-term goals increases immanent justice reasoning

British Journal of Social Psychology (2013), 52, 377–385

© 2013 The British Psychological Society

www.wileyonlinelibrary.com

Brief report

Through the looking glass: Focusing on long-termgoals increases immanent justice reasoning

Mitchell J. Callan1*, Annelie J. Harvey1, Rael J. Dawtry2 and RobbieM. Sutton2

1Department of Psychology, University of Essex, UK2School of Psychology, University of Kent, UK

Immanent justice reasoning involves causally attributing a negative event to someone’s

prior moral failings, even when such a causal connection is physically implausible. This

study examined the degree to which immanent justice represents a form of motivated

reasoning in the service of satisfying the need to believe in a just world. Drawing on a

manipulation that has been shown to activate justice motivation, participants causally

attributed a freak accident to a man’s prior immoral (vs. moral) behaviour to a greater

extent when they first focused on their long-term (vs. short-term) goals. These findings

highlight the important function believing in a just world plays in self-regulatory processes

by implicating the self in immanent justice reasoning about fluke events in the lives of

others.

Immanent justice reasoning involves causally attributing a negative outcome to

someone’s prior misdeeds, evenwhen such a causal connection is physically implausible.

When we infer that a man came down with a mysterious illness because he robbed many

people of their money, we are engaging in immanent justice reasoning (Raman &Winer,2004).

Although Piaget (1932/1965) proposed that the use of immanent justice reasoning

declineswith age, recent research has shown that adults, at times, also entertain notions of

immanent justice (Callan, Ellard, & Nicol, 2006; Callan, Sutton, & Dovale, 2010; Maes,

1998; Raman &Winer, 2002, 2004; Young, Morris, Burrus, Krishnan, & Regmi, 2011). In

fact, Raman and Winer (2004) call in to question developmental stage accounts of

immanent justice reasoning by demonstrating that immanent justice responses to events

are more common among adults than school children. But why would adults indulge incausal explanations for events that do not jibe with the physical laws of cause-and-effect?

Several explanations for why adults sometimes engage in immanent justice reasoning

have been identified. Raman andWiner (2002, 2004) suggested that culturally normative

*Correspondence should be addressed to Mitchell J. Callan, Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park,Colchester CO4 3SQ, UK (e-mail: [email protected]).

DOI:10.1111/bjso.12022

377

Page 2: Through the looking glass: Focusing on long-term goals increases immanent justice reasoning

principles of justice, such as religious teachings (e.g., ‘Whatsoever a man soweth, that

shall he also reap’), might exert an influence on adults’ causal reasoning via enculturation.

They also suggested that immanent justice accounts of events might result from adults’

capacity to entertain various models of causal explanation, of which immanent justice isone possible choice. This explanation resonates with work on magical thinking more

generally, where contrary to replacement/stage models (cf. Piaget, 1932/1965), coexis-

tence theories suggest that science-based explanatory frameworks and supernatural or

magical explanatory frameworks coexist across the lifespan (Legare & Gelman, 2008;

Subbotsky, 2004; Woolley, Cornelius, & Lacy, 2011).

A further explanation, explored in this research, is that immanent justice reasoning is

one means of satisfying the need to believe in a just world (Lerner, 1980). According to

just-world theory, people are motivated to sustain the functional belief that the world is ajust, fair, and non-random place where people get what they deserve. From this

perspective, immanent justice represents a form of motivated reasoning engaged to

construe events as consistent with the belief that people get what they deserve, thus

serving the same functional purpose as other strategies aimed at maintaining a

commitment to justice and deservingness (e.g., victim rejection; Hafer & B�egue, 2005).In other words, immanent justice reasoning allows people to maintain the assumption

that bad things happen for a reason, and the reason can be located in the priormisdeeds of

an unfortunate victim (‘bad outcomes are caused by bad people’).Offering some support for a just-world theory account, Callan et al. (2006, 2010) found

that immanent justice reasoning is related to people’s concerns about deservingness, and

that immanent justice reasoning isheightenedwhenobservershavewitnessed anunrelated

injustice (Callan et al., 2006). Specifically, Callan et al. (2006) found that participants

causally attributed a man’s misfortune (car accident) to his past behaviour more when he

was perceived to deserve his outcome (i.e., when he was a bad vs. good person).

Although this research shows that perceived deservingness is related to immanent

justice attributions as just-world theory predicts, it is not clear whether a concern forjustice subserves immanent justice attributions or is a justification for them because these

constructs were assessed simultaneously. Moreover, experimental work examining

whether the adoption of immanent justice accounts of events fluctuates as a function of

people’s concern for justice is limited. The objective of this research, then,was to provide

further experimental evidence for the notion that one of the reasons why people engage

in immanent justice reasoning is the need to believe in a just world. To do so, we adopted

an experimental paradigm that has been shown to activate the need to perceive theworld

as just: long-term (vs. short-term) goal focus (Hafer, 2000).According to Lerner and colleagues (1977; Lerner, Miller, & Holmes, 1976; Long &

Lerner, 1974), people need to believe in a just world because doing so enables them to

commit to long-term goal pursuits with confidence. Believing the alternative – that the

world is a random, capricious, and unfair place – would likely discourage investing in

anything but themost immediate outcomes, because longer term investmentsmade by an

individual may not pay off. Research supports the notion that the belief in a just world is

related to people’s long-term goal pursuits (e.g., Bal & van den Bos, 2012; Callan, Shead, &

Olson, 2009, 2011; Hafer, 2000; Hafer, B�egue, Choma, & Dempsey, 2005; Laurin,Fitzsimons, & Kay, 2011). For example, beliefs in a just world have been positively linked

to people’s tendencies to plan for and invest in future goals (Xie, Liu, & Gan, 2011),

considerations of the consequences of future behaviour (Hafer, 2000), and confidence in

achieving life goals (Sutton&Winnard, 2007).Most relevant to the current research, Hafer

(2000) experimentally examined the effects of a long-term goal focusmanipulation on the

378 Mitchell J. Callan et al.

Page 3: Through the looking glass: Focusing on long-term goals increases immanent justice reasoning

strategies people adopt to maintain a belief in a just world. She found that participants

derogated a victim whose fate threatened their just-world beliefs more strongly among

those who focused on their future plans after university than those who focused on their

current extracurricular activities. In a recent conceptual replication of this effect, Bal andvan den Bos (2012) found that individual differences in future orientation (Zimbardo,

1990) positively predicted victim derogation and blame when the victim was innocent,

but not when the victim was non-innocent (i.e., when their just-world beliefs were

threatened vs. not threatened).

Taken together, this research shows that a concern with one’s long-term (vs. short-

term) goals increases the use of strategies to maintain a commitment to justice. Thus, we

adapted a modified version of Hafer’s (2000) paradigm to test the notion that immanent

justice reasoning is a social-cognitive strategy used to construe events as being consistentwith a justworld. After focusing on their long-term (vs. short-term) goals, participants read

about a freak accident that occurred either to a good person (respected swimming coach)

or a bad person (thieving swimming coach). Participants were asked to rate the extent to

which they believed the accidentwas a result of the person’s prior conduct. If the belief in

a just world is more essential when people are focused on their long-term goals (Hafer,

2000), then we should expect immanent justice attributions for a random bad outcome

that occurred to a bad (vs. good) person to increase when people are asked to think about

and list their long-term goals. Thus, we predicted that the effect of the target person’smoral worth (good vs. bad) on immanent justice attributions would occur more strongly

when participants were first asked to think about their long-term (vs. short-term) goals.

Method

ParticipantsParticipants were recruited across two mediums (paper-based and online). One sample

consisted of 116 staff and students from the University of Essex, United Kingdom, who

were approached on campus to complete a survey in exchange for a candy bar (62%

females; Mage = 29.18, SDage = 13.10). The other sample consisted of 251 participants

recruited via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk to complete an online survey (see Buhrmester,

Kwang, & Gosling, 2011; 65% females; Mage = 32.41, SDage = 11.46). Participants from

both sampleswere told that the studyconcerned ‘LifeActivities andPerceptionsofNews’.1

Materials and procedure

We informed participants that they would complete two separate studies involving ‘a

content analysis of the different types of goals/activities people carry out in their lives’ and

‘how people make sense out of and find meaning in the things that happen to others’.

Long-term goal focus manipulation and validation study

Participants were first presented with an open-ended questionnaire that introduced the

goal focus manipulation (entitled either ‘Life Goals Study’ or ‘Daily Activities Study’). We

told participants that the studywas a content analysis of the different types of goals people

1Weexcluded 15 (3.9%) additional participants because they incorrectly answered a simplemanipulation check question (‘In thearticle you read, was Keith someone who stole from his students?’).

Immanent justice 379

Page 4: Through the looking glass: Focusing on long-term goals increases immanent justice reasoning

set for themselves (or activities they carry out). Participants in the long-term goal focus

conditionwere asked to think about and list up to four goals theywanted to accomplish in

their lives for each of three extended time periods (1–5, 5–10, and 10–15 years).

Participants in the short-term goal focus condition were asked to list up to four activitiesthey would carry out on that day within each of three time periods (next 1, 1–12, and 12–24 hr). Within the description of the questionnaire, participants were given examples of

the sort of goals/activities they could provide (e.g., interpersonal goals or activities, such

as getting married or meeting with a friend that day). Participants in each condition were

given four lines for each of the time periods to list their goals (activities).2 Thus, one group

of participants was asked to focus on their ‘here-and-now’ activities, whereas the other

group was asked to consider their longer term goals.

Using a separate sample of participants from Mechanical Turk (N = 70; 53% femaleparticipants;Mage = 30.42, SDage = 9.63), we tested the previously unexamined assump-

tion that believing in a just world is more crucial and important to people in light of

achieving their long-term versus short-term goals. Participants completed the long-term/

short-term goal manipulation described above and then, across three items, rated how

much they believed that to achieve their goals, it was crucial, important, and essential that

they live in a just and fair world (a = .89; ‘In order for you to achieve the things you listed

above, how crucial is it to you that the world you live in is a just and fair place’?; ‘How

important is it to you that people get what they deserve in life in order for you to achievethe things you listed above’?; and ‘In terms of accomplishing the things you mentioned

above, how essential is it to you that the world you live in treats people fairly’?). These

items were rated on scales ranging from 1 (crucial/important/essential) to 7 (extremely

crucial/important/essential). Consistent with our conceptual analysis, in terms of

achieving their goals, living in a just world was rated as more important for participants

who focused on their long-term goals (M = 5.08, SD = 1.21) than for participants who

focused on their short-term goals (M = 3.97, SD = 1.80), t(68) = 3.06, p = .003, d = .74.

Immanent justice scenario

Next, adapted from a real news report (Razaq, 2008), participants read a news article

describing a freak accident in which a swimming coach, fictitiously named Keith

Murdoch, was seriously injured when a tree collapsed on his vehicle during high winds

(see Appendix). The content of the article was identical in both conditions except for the

title and the last paragraph. To vary the moral value of the target character, participants

either read, ‘Sources confirm that KeithMurdoch volunteered as a swimming coach at theBitterne Leisure Centre and is a valued and beloved member of the community’ (‘good

person’ condition) or ‘Sources confirm that Keith Murdoch volunteered as a swimming

coach at the Bitterne Leisure Centre and is awaiting sentencing for theft. He had used his

master keys to steal money, jewellery, and cell phones from swimmers attending their

classes’ (‘bad person’ condition).

Following Callan et al. (2006), we then asked participants to provide their immanent

justice attributions within a ‘sense-making survey’ by answering the item, ‘to what extent

2Of the 1,921 long-term goals listed by participants (not all participants provided 12 answers), 51% concerned career, financial,or material goals (e.g., ‘graduate college’); 22% concerned social, family, or relationship goals (e.g., ‘get married and start afamily’); 22% concerned self-development/self-experience goals (e.g., ‘learn a new language’); and 5% concerned physical/healthgoals (e.g., ‘lose weight’). The short-term goals people listed largely involved mundane activities (e.g., eat lunch, watch television,sleep).

380 Mitchell J. Callan et al.

Page 5: Through the looking glass: Focusing on long-term goals increases immanent justice reasoning

do you feel that what happened to Keith Murdoch was a result of his conduct as a

swimming coach’. For the paper-based sample, this item was answered using a 7-point

scale (1 = Not at all, 7 = A great deal), whereas for the online sample, this question was

presented with a 6-point scale (1 = Not at all, 6 = A great deal). Thus, scores on thismeasure from the online sample were rescaled for a common 7-point scale.3

Results

Immanent justice attributions were analysed using a 2 (Medium: online vs. paper) 9 2

(Goal Focus: long-term vs. short-term) 9 2 (Moral Worth: respected vs. thief) between-subjects ANOVA. Analyses revealed no significant main or interaction effects of

recruitment medium (all ps > .11). Most important, the predicted Moral Worth 9 Goal

Focus two-way interactionwas not significantly different between samples (i.e., therewas

no three-way interaction), F(1, 359) = 1.12, p = .29.

Conceptually replicating Callan et al. (2006) findings, analyses revealed a significant

main effect of the moral value of the swim coach’s prior behaviour on immanent justice

attributions, F(1, 359) = 41.64, p < .001, d = .73, such that participants causally

attributed the freak accident to his conduct more when he was a thief than when hewas a respected swim coach. Crucially, analyses revealed the predicted Moral

Worth 9 Goal Focus interaction, F(1, 359) = 6.84, p = .009, gp2 = .02.4 Shown in

Figure 1, the effect of the coach’s moral worth on immanent justice attributions was

stronger for participants who first focused on their long-term goals, t(359) = 6.51,

p < .001, d = .86, than for participants who first focused on their short-term activities, t

Figure 1. Effect of the protagonist’s moral conduct (respected coach vs. thieving coach) on immanent

justice attributions as a function of goal focus (long-term vs. short-term). Error bars show standard errors

of the means.

3 Standardizing the scores within samples results in the same conclusions.4 Because immanent justice attributions tend to be positively skewed, we conducted resampling analyses that do not requirenormality of the data (Manly, 2007; described by Howell, 2009). These analyses used 5,000 random permutations of the dataand yielded the same Moral Worth 9 Goal Focus interaction effect (p = .02).

Immanent justice 381

Page 6: Through the looking glass: Focusing on long-term goals increases immanent justice reasoning

(359) = 3.39, p < .001, d = .60. Looking at the interaction in a different way, when the

coach was a thief, immanent justice attributions were higher when participants focused

on their long-term (vs. short-term) goals, t(359) = 3.42, p < .001, d = .39. Thus, these

results show that a long-term goal focus, which has been shown previously to enhancejustice motive effects (Hafer, 2000), increased the extent to which participants were

willing to causally attribute a fluke accident to a person’s prior misdeeds.

Discussion

Early treatments of immanent justice reasoning regarded it as a na€ıve, childlike, heuristicway of understanding events. More recently, it has been conceptualized as motivated

cognition designed to uphold the apparent justice of theworld (Callan et al., 2006, 2010).

The present result adds weight to this interpretation of immanent justice reasoning by

demonstrating a novel effect: long-term (vs. short-term) goal focus, which theoretically

activates the need to perceive the world as just (Hafer, 2000; Lerner, 1977), increases

immanent justice reasoning.5

The current findings resonate with a growing body of research interested in the

interplay between people’s concerns about justice for the self and justice for others (e.g.,De Cremer & Van Hiel, 2010; Loseman & van den Bos, 2012; van Prooijen & van den Bos,

2009). By revealing that the effects of thinking about one’s own life goals impacts

immanent justice reasoning, these findings highlight the importance of justice for the self

on how people make sense out of the fluke events in the lives of others. Thus, the effects

reported here highlight the important function that believing in a just world plays in self-

regulatory processes (Callan et al., 2009; Laurin et al., 2011; Lerner, 1977).

One limitation of these findings is that we did not ask participants to provide

explanations or rationales for their immanent justice attributions. Taking this approach,Callan et al. (2006) found that participants’ invoked notions of justice (e.g., ‘what goes

around comes around’) compared to other possible explanations (e.g., fate, chance,

naturalistic processes) more strongly when they learned that a bad chance outcome

happened to a ‘bad’ person and when their belief in a just world was previously

threatened. One avenue for future research, then, might be to examine how people’s

causal explanations for fluke events might be qualitatively different depending on the

degree to which they are focused on their long-term versus short-term investments.

The effects we report might also be limited to people who hold Western or Christianworldviews. Using a scenario much like the one in this research, Young et al. (2011)

found that Christian participants inferred that a target person’s misfortune was fated only

if that person performed a prior misdeed, whereas Hindu participants, who generally

believe that people have past lives and ‘karmic’ payback comes in the next lifetime,

inferred that the person’s misfortune was fated regardless of her misdeeds. The Hindu

5 Long-term focus can alter construal level, so we tested whether construal level might have influenced the present effects (Trope& Liberman, 2010). We conducted a similar study in the laboratory (N = 152; 62% female; Mage = 21.83, SDage = 4.53)using our good vs. bad ‘swim coach’ scenario preceded by a validated construal-level manipulation (‘how’ or ‘why’ one maintainsgood physical health vs. a no construal control condition; see Freitas, Salovey, & Liberman, 2001). In this study, participantsprovided their immanent justice attributions across four items (the ‘result of’ item above along with items assessing the extent towhich participants believed it was worth considering, plausible, and possible that the accident was a result of Keith’s past conduct;a = .94). A 2 (moral worth) 9 3 (construal level) ANOVA showed that the effect of the coach’s worth on immanent justiceattributions, F(1, 146) = 6.09, p = .015,d = .42, was notmoderated by construal level, F(2, 146) = .20,p = .82. The resultsof this study suggest that construal level does not impact immanent justice attributions in the same way that long-term (vs. short-term) goal focus does.

382 Mitchell J. Callan et al.

Page 7: Through the looking glass: Focusing on long-term goals increases immanent justice reasoning

participants’ responses to such a misfortune are similar to ‘ultimate justice’ reasoning

(Anderson, Kay, & Fitzsimons, 2010; Lerner, 1980; Maes, 1998), a form of which Christian

cosmologies also ascribe to (e.g., heaven and hell). The challenge for future research will

be to determine the interacting role that justice motivation and different cultural andreligious principles of justice play in how people find meaning in and make sense out of

the misfortunes that befall themselves and others.

Acknowledgements

We thank Clare Hyland for her assistance with data collection and Will Matthews for his help

with statistical analyses.

References

Anderson, J. E., Kay, A. C., & Fitzsimons, G. M. (2010). In search of the silver lining: The justice

motive fosters perceptions of benefits in the later lives of tragedy victims.Psychological Science,

21, 1599–1604. doi:10.1177/0956797610386620Bal, M., & van den Bos, K. (2012). Blaming for a better future: Future orientation and associated

intolerance of personal uncertainty lead to harsher reactions toward innocent victims.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 835–844. doi:10.1177/0146167212442970Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s mechanical Turk: A new source of

inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5. doi:10.1177/1745691610393980

Callan, M. J., Ellard, J. H., & Nicol, J. E. (2006). The belief in a just world and immanent justice

reasoning in adults. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1646–1658. doi:10.1177/0146167206292236

Callan, M. J., Shead, N.W., &Olson, J. M. (2009). Foregoing the labor for the fruits: The effect of just

world threat on the desire for immediate monetary rewards. Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 45, 246–249. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.08.013Callan,M. J., Shead,N.W.,&Olson, J.M. (2011). Personal relative deprivation, delaydiscounting, and

gambling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 955–973. doi:10.1037/a0024778Callan, M. J., Sutton, R., & Dovale, C. (2010). When deserving translates into causing: The effect of

cognitive load on immanent justice reasoning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46,

1097–1100. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.05.024DeCremer,D.,&VanHiel, A. (2010). Becoming angrywhenother is treated fairly:Onunderstanding

when own and other’s fair treatment influences negative reactions. British Journal of

Management, 21, 280–298. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8551.2009.00653.xFreitas, A. L., Salovey, P., & Liberman, N. (2001). Abstract and concrete self-evaluative goals. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 410–424. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.410Hafer, C. L. (2000). Investment in long term goals and commitment to just means drive the need to

believe in a just world. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1059–1073. doi:10.1177/01461672002611004

Hafer, C., & B�egue, L. (2005). Experimental research on just-world theory: Problems, developments,

and future challenges.PsychologicalBulletin,131, 128–167. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.128Hafer, C. L., B�egue, L., Choma, B. L., & Dempsey, J. L. (2005). Belief in a just world and commitment

to long-term deserved outcomes. Social Justice Research, 18, 429–444. doi:10.1007/s11211-005-8569-3

Howell, D. C. (2009). Permutation tests for Factorial ANOVA designs. Retrieved from http://www.

uvm.edu/~dhowell/StatPages/More_Stuff/Permutation%20Anova/PermTestsAnova.html

Laurin, K., Fitzsimons, G., &Kay, A. C. (2011). Social disadvantage and the self-regulatory function of

justice beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 149–171. doi:10.1037/a0021343

Immanent justice 383

Page 8: Through the looking glass: Focusing on long-term goals increases immanent justice reasoning

Legare, C. H., & Gelman, S. A. (2008). Bewitchment, biology, or both: The co-existence of natural

and supernatural explanatory frameworks across development.Cognitive Science,32, 607–642.doi:10.1080/03640210802066766

Lerner, M. J. (1977). The justice motive. Some hypotheses as to its origins and forms. Journal of

Personality, 45, 1–32. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1977.tb00591.xLerner, M. J. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion. New York: Plenum.

Lerner,M. J.,Miller,D. T., &Holmes, J.G. (1976).Deserving and the emergence of formsof justice. In

L. Berkowitz & E.Walster (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 9, pp. 133–162). New York: Academic Press.

Long, G. T., & Lerner, M. J. (1974). Deserving, the “personal contract”, and altruistic behavior by

children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 551–556. doi:10.1037/h0036207Loseman, A., & van den Bos, K. (2012). A self-regulation hypothesis of coping with an unjust world:

Ego-depletion and self-affirmation as underlying aspects of blaming of innocent victims. Social

Justice Research, 25, 1–13. doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0152-0Maes, J. (1998). Immanent and ultimate justice: Twoways of believing in justice. In M. J. Lerner & L.

Montada (Eds.), Responses to victimizations and belief in a just world (pp. 9–40). New York:

Plenum Press.

Manly, B. F. J. (2007). Randomization, bootstrap, and Monte Carlo methods in biology. (3rd ed.)

London, UK: Chapman & Hall.

Piaget, J. (1932/1965). The moral judgment of the child. London, UK: Kegan, Paul, Trench,

Trubner, & Co.

Raman, L., & Winer, G. A. (2002). Children’s and adults’ understanding of illness: Evidence in

support of a coexistence model. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 128,

325–355.Raman, L., & Winer, G. A. (2004). Evidence of more immanent justice responding in adults than

children: A challenge to traditional developmental theories. British Journal of Developmental

Psychology, 22, 255–274. doi:10.1348/026151004323044609Razaq, R. (2008). Passenger’s luck escape as tree kills van man. Retrieved from http://www.

standard.co.uk/news/passengers-lucky-escape-as-tree-kills-van-man-6871601.html

Subbotsky, E. (2004).Magical thinking in judgments of causation: Can anomalous phenomena affect

ontological causal beliefs in children and adults? British Journal of Developmental Psychology,

22, 123–152. doi:10.1348/026151004772901140Sutton, R.M., &Winnard, E. J. (2007). Looking ahead through lenses of justice: The relevance of just-

world beliefs to intentions and confidence in the future. British Journal of Social Psychology,

46, 649–666. doi:10.1348/014466606X166220Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal level theory of psychological distance. Psychological

Review, 117, 440–463. doi:10.1037/a0018963van Prooijen, J.-W., & van den Bos, K. (2009). We blame innocent victims more than I do: Self-

construal level moderates responses to just-world threats. Personality and Social Psychology

Bulletin, 35, 1528–1539. doi:10.1177/0146167209344728Woolley, J. D., Cornelius, C. A.,& Lacy,W. (2011).Developmental changes in the useof supernatural

explanations for unusual events. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 11, 311–337. doi:10.1163/156853711X591279

Xie, X., Liu, H., & Gan, Y. (2011). Belief in a just world when encountering the 5/12 Wenchuan

earthquake. Environment and Behavior, 43, 566–586. doi:10.1177/0013916510363535Young, M. J., Morris, M. W., Burrus, J., Krishnan, L., & Regmi, M. P. (2011). Deity and destiny:

Patterns of thinking in Christian and Hindu cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42,

1030–1053. doi:10.1177/0022022110381123Zimbardo, P. G. (1990). The Stanford time perspective inventory. Stanford, CA: StanfordUniversity.

Received 25 July 2012; revised version received 29 November 2012

384 Mitchell J. Callan et al.

Page 9: Through the looking glass: Focusing on long-term goals increases immanent justice reasoning

Appendix

Text for the thieving swim coach condition is presented in italics

Volunteer Swim Coach Critically Injured by Falling Tree [ThievingSwim Coach Critically Injured by Falling Tree]

Southampton – In an unlikely turn of events, volunteer swim coach Keith Murdoch, 37, was critically

injured Thursday morning when a tree fell on his van on Mountbatten Way in Southampton.

Commenting on the incident, one nearby office worker said; ‘I heard an almighty crash and I ran to the

window and saw the tree was lying in the road. It was chaos. People were standing around watching’.

Southampton Transport, which is responsible for the safety of trees along the road, said they are

maintained regularly and that an investigation will be launched, with other trees checked.

‘There were strong, gusty winds and rain in the early morning. Trees are full of leaves at this time of

year so are more likely to be uprooted. Even so, we must remind people that this is a freak accident

and they should not be unduly concerned about their safety’, said a spokesman from Southampton

Transport.

The tree struck the van where the windshield and roof meet, crashing into the front-seat area.

Emergency crews were eventually able to remove Mr. Murdoch from the vehicle. He was rushed to

Royal Southampton Hospital where he is in critical condition.

Sources confirm that Keith Murdoch volunteered as a swimming coach at the Bitterne Leisure Centre

and is a valued and beloved member of the community.

Sources confirm that Keith Murdoch volunteered as a swimming coach at the Bitterne Leisure Centre and is

awaiting sentencing for theft. He had used his master keys to steal money, jewellery, and mobile phones from

swimmers attending their classes.

Immanent justice 385