3
Introduction Tools, TV, and trust: Introduction to the special issue on imitation in typically-developing children Man is a toolmaking animal. Certainly, as with other traits once considered to be solely human do- mains, we cannot claim exclusive right to the ‘‘toolmaker” moniker. Yet we use tools with a depth, breadth, and remarkable inventiveness that no other animal comes close to showing. Our environ- ment, no matter where that might be, is saturated with tools, and one way or another, children must learn to use these tools. One of the most efficient ways they can do so is by copying others. It thus came as no surprise to us that the vast majority of manuscripts that were submitted for publication in our special issues on the functions and mechanisms of imitation in childhood presented studies of children learning to use tools and other artifacts. Some manuscripts were more explicitly about this topic than others, but it is clear that learning to use tools and artifacts is inextricably linked to the developmental study of imitation. In the first special issue we introduced a series of papers focused on imitation in children with aut- ism (Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(2) November 2008). The present issue comprises three papers on imitation in typically-developing children. Each of these papers offers novel insights into how young children learn about the objects they are confronted with, day in and day out. Each touches on some or all of three main trends that have emerged in the developmental imitation liter- ature in recent years. One of the more fascinating and perhaps the most pervasive of these trends is the growing body of experimental evidence that characterizes children as selective, flexible imitators on the one hand (e.g., Carpenter, Akhtar, & Tomasello, 1998; Gergely, Bekkering, & Király, 2002; Meltzoff, 1995) but as high- fidelity over-imitators on the other (e.g., Lyons, Young, & Keil, 2007; Nagell, Olguin, & Tomasello, 1993; see Nielsen, 2006). Thus one of the primary challenges currently facing imitation researchers is to determine when children will copy others’ actions and when they will choose to perform their own alternative actions instead. This leads us to a second trend. Picking up on Uz ˇgiris’ (1981) distinction between the two functions of imitation, recently several researchers (ourselves included) have begun to focus on less instrumen- tal, more social reasons for copying others (e.g., Carpenter, 2006; Nielsen, 2008; see Rogers & Williams, 2006). Both are themes that run through the articles in this issue. A third trend is that demonstrations in imitation studies are being increasingly presented on video, and this is happening for a number of reasons. There is an obvious practical utility to using video: It is a mechanism by which demonstra- tions can be standardized across participants while also allowing specific aspects of the demonstration to be edited (e.g., Huang & Charman, 2005). Further, because the demonstrator cannot be interacted with, video affords a means of investigating social functions of imitation more directly (e.g., see Nielsen, Simcock, & Jenkins, 2008). Finally, documenting what and how children learn from television and videos has become critical in certain cultures where children spend considerable portions of their day watching these (e.g., see Anderson & Pempek, 2005). This, too, is a theme that runs through two of 0022-0965/$ - see front matter Ó 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.09.005 Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 101 (2008) 225–227 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

Tools, TV, and trust: Introduction to the special issue on imitation in typically-developing children

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 101 (2008) 225–227

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Experimental ChildPsychology

Introduction

Tools, TV, and trust: Introduction to the special issue onimitation in typically-developing children

Man is a toolmaking animal. Certainly, as with other traits once considered to be solely human do-mains, we cannot claim exclusive right to the ‘‘toolmaker” moniker. Yet we use tools with a depth,breadth, and remarkable inventiveness that no other animal comes close to showing. Our environ-ment, no matter where that might be, is saturated with tools, and one way or another, children mustlearn to use these tools. One of the most efficient ways they can do so is by copying others. It thuscame as no surprise to us that the vast majority of manuscripts that were submitted for publicationin our special issues on the functions and mechanisms of imitation in childhood presented studiesof children learning to use tools and other artifacts. Some manuscripts were more explicitly about thistopic than others, but it is clear that learning to use tools and artifacts is inextricably linked to thedevelopmental study of imitation.

In the first special issue we introduced a series of papers focused on imitation in children with aut-ism (Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(2) November 2008). The present issue comprisesthree papers on imitation in typically-developing children. Each of these papers offers novel insightsinto how young children learn about the objects they are confronted with, day in and day out. Eachtouches on some or all of three main trends that have emerged in the developmental imitation liter-ature in recent years.

One of the more fascinating and perhaps the most pervasive of these trends is the growing body ofexperimental evidence that characterizes children as selective, flexible imitators on the one hand (e.g.,Carpenter, Akhtar, & Tomasello, 1998; Gergely, Bekkering, & Király, 2002; Meltzoff, 1995) but as high-fidelity over-imitators on the other (e.g., Lyons, Young, & Keil, 2007; Nagell, Olguin, & Tomasello, 1993;see Nielsen, 2006). Thus one of the primary challenges currently facing imitation researchers is todetermine when children will copy others’ actions and when they will choose to perform their ownalternative actions instead.

This leads us to a second trend. Picking up on Uzgiris’ (1981) distinction between the two functionsof imitation, recently several researchers (ourselves included) have begun to focus on less instrumen-tal, more social reasons for copying others (e.g., Carpenter, 2006; Nielsen, 2008; see Rogers & Williams,2006). Both are themes that run through the articles in this issue. A third trend is that demonstrationsin imitation studies are being increasingly presented on video, and this is happening for a number ofreasons. There is an obvious practical utility to using video: It is a mechanism by which demonstra-tions can be standardized across participants while also allowing specific aspects of the demonstrationto be edited (e.g., Huang & Charman, 2005). Further, because the demonstrator cannot be interactedwith, video affords a means of investigating social functions of imitation more directly (e.g., seeNielsen, Simcock, & Jenkins, 2008). Finally, documenting what and how children learn from televisionand videos has become critical in certain cultures where children spend considerable portions of theirday watching these (e.g., see Anderson & Pempek, 2005). This, too, is a theme that runs through two of

0022-0965/$ - see front matter � 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.09.005

226 Introduction / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 101 (2008) 225–227

the articles in this special issue. Below we briefly summarize each of the articles; then, we finish byspeculating on the role of children’s imitative behavior in the development of our propensity for tooluse and the transmission of cultural information.

In the first article, Flynn and Whiten explore children’s ability to imitate in a complex tool-use taskwith multiple component actions. Expanding on their previous studies (e.g., Whiten, Flynn, Brown, &Lee, 2006), Flynn and Whiten investigate developmental differences in 3- and 5-year-old children’sability to imitate hierarchical action structure, and they look for relations between this kind of imita-tion and imitation of the demonstrator’s action styles. They use a video demonstration in this studyand compare their results to those of a previous study that used a live demonstration. Their findingsemphasize several key points. The first is that children are not slavish copiers, mindlessly reproducingeverything they see an adult do. Rather, they are capable of making judgments about what to replicatebased on what they consider to be the overall structure of the demonstration. This work also showsthat children are able to acquire relatively complex skills from a videotaped model, though the fidelityof their reproduction is diminished when compared with learning from a live model.

In the second article, DiYanni and Kelemen investigate children’s imitation of an adult’s tool usewhen the adult’s choice of tool is not an optimal one for achieving her goal. This study thus pits chil-dren’s understanding of others’ intentional action and their motivation to copy others against theirunderstanding of causal efficiency and design. DiYanni and Kelemen find interesting developmentaldifferences in children’s choices of tools in 2-, 3-, and 4-year-olds across their three studies. These re-sults contribute not only to the imitation literature, in highlighting once again the tension betweencopying others and doing things one’s own way, but also to the growing literature on reliability andselective trust (see Harris, 2007, for a review) and children’s understanding of the design stance(e.g., see German & Johnson, 2002).

Finally, in the third article, Strouse and Troseth investigate the conditions under which 2-year-oldsare able to learn from video demonstrations. In a series of three studies, Strouse and Troseth examinecontextual factors (i.e., whether the video was filmed in the same versus a different setting as that inwhich the test took place), whether ‘‘cuts” in the videos negatively affect children’s imitation, whetherthe duration and number of repetitions of the demonstration affect children’s performance, andwhether children learn equally well from video demonstrations when they are presented in theirown home versus in a laboratory. In all cases, children’s responses to video demonstrations are com-pared with their responses to live demonstrations. These results provide important information forresearchers considering using video demonstrations in imitation tasks.

Together, these studies show that children do not copy others without processing a host of vari-ables that determine exactly what they will reproduce. These papers document how the hierarchicalstructure of the modeled actions, the logic of the actions given their intended outcomes, and the nat-ure of the presentation medium affect what children copy. Clearly, children do not blindly mimic. If wereturn to the issue of human tool use, this may prove a crucial point.

Consider the suggestion that tool use, per se, is not what separates us from other species but ratherit is the proliferation, adaptation, complexity, and variation of our tools and the ways we use them thatdistinguish us. The seemingly contradictory findings of selective imitation and overimitation in chil-dren—a combination that our nearest primate relatives lack—may help explain our species-specific ap-proach to the use and development of tools. That is, past research has established a strong propensityin children for faithfully imitating others’ object-directed actions, sometimes even when this results ina failure to bring about the demonstrated outcome (for a review see Flynn & Whiten, this issue).Though at first glance such behavior may seem maladaptive it likely reflects the value of adopting imi-tation as a default strategy: It affords the rapid acquisition of novel behaviors while at the same timeavoiding the potential pitfalls and false end-points that can come from trial-and-error learning. Alongwith its instrumental skill-acquisition function, imitation also has a crucial social–cultural function:Faithfully copying others is absolutely indispensible in the acquisition of arbitrary, conventional/cul-tural skills and when identifying with and aligning oneself with one’s cultural in-group. Overimitationthus serves many important functions for human children. However, if children only overimitated,opportunities for adaptation, reinvention, and improvement would be limited. In this context, selec-tive imitation is crucial for prior information to be refined and built upon. As previously alluded to,the papers included in this issue show that children do not always blindly imitate. They are keen

Introduction / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 101 (2008) 225–227 227

replicators, but they are flexible too. They attend to and prioritize different aspects of a demonstrationand adjust their imitative behavior accordingly. This combination of selective imitation and overimi-tation is precisely the kind of approach we would want if we were to build a premium tool-usingorganism.

The study of how and why children copy others’ object-directed actions is critical to our broaderunderstanding of children’s social and cognitive development. It can tell us much about strategiesfor learning, teaching, communicating, and much more. It may also provide insight into the originsof our astonishing propensity for using and developing tools, a trait that has no doubt impactedimmeasurably on our colonization of the planet. The papers featured in this special edition make a finefoundation for work that can continue to do this.

Acknowledgments

We once again thank David Bjorklund for inviting us to edit these special issues and for his guid-ance and patience in helping us to get them together. We again also thank all the reviewers who tookthe time to provide such helpful comments on the submissions.

References

Anderson, D. R., & Pempek, T. A. (2005). Television and very young children. The American Behavioral Scientist, 48, 505–522.Carpenter, M. (2006). Instrumental, social, and shared goals and intentions in imitation. In S. J. Rogers & J. H. G. Williams (Eds.),

Imitation and the social mind: Autism and typical development (pp. 48–70). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen- to 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and

accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315–330.Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.German, T., & Johnson, S. (2002). Function and the origins of the design stance. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3, 279–300.Harris, P. L. (2007). Trust. Developmental Science, 10, 135–138.Huang, C., & Charman, T. (2005). Gradations of emulation learning in infants’ imitation of actions on objects. Journal of

Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 276–302.Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of

Sciences, United States of America, 104, 19751–19756.Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children.

Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850.Nagell, K., Olguin, R., & Tomasello, M. (1993). Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and

human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107, 174–186.Nielsen, M. (2006). Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology,

42, 555–565.Nielsen, M. (2008). The social motivation for social learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 33.Nielsen, M., Simcock, G., & Jenkins, L. (2008). The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds’ imitation from live and

televised models. Developmental Science, 11, 722–731.Rogers, S. J., & Williams, J. H. G. (2006). Imitation and the social mind: Autism and typical development. New York, NY: Guilford

Press.Uzgiris, I. C. (1981). Two functions of imitation during infancy. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 1–12.Whiten, A., Flynn, E., Brown, K., & Lee, T. (2006). Imitation of hierarchical structure in actions by young children. Developmental

Science, 9, 575–583.

Malinda CarpenterDepartment of Developmental and Comparative Psychology,

Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology,Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig, Germany

Fax: +49 341 3550 444.E-mail address: [email protected]

Mark NielsenUniversity of Queensland, School of Psychology,

Brisbane, QLD 4072, AustraliaFax: +61 3365 4466.

E-mail address: [email protected]