12
Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity: Implications for biodiversity management and conservation Anke Fischer a, *, Juliette C. Young b a Macaulay Land Use Research Institute, Socio-Economics Group, Craigiebuckler, Aberdeen AB15 8QH, UK b Centre for Ecology and Hydrology, Hill of Brathens, Glassel, Banchory AB31 4BW, UK ARTICLE INFO Article history: Received 15 September 2006 Received in revised form 17 November 2006 Accepted 27 November 2006 Available online 18 January 2007 Keywords: Biodiversity Attitudes Values Understanding Management Public ABSTRACT The participation of the public in environmental decision-making and management is increasingly seen as essential for the success of conservation initiatives. Ecological scientists and conservation practitioners have, however, argued that a lack of understanding of biodi- versity issues by the public is a barrier to their effective participation in decision-making pro- cesses. These arguments are often based on studies where scientific knowledge is used as the sole measure of public understanding of biodiversity, and therefore fail to account for individ- uals’ constructs of biodiversity and related issues such as biodiversity management. We examined individuals’ mental constructs of biodiversity, and their conceptual con- texts, through a series of focus group discussions with members of the general public in Scot- land. To gain a fuller picture of public understanding of biodiversity, we distinguished between mental associations with the term ‘biodiversity’, and the meanings associated with biodiversity-related concepts independent of scientific terminology. We found participants to express rich mental concepts of biodiversity, irrespective of their scientific knowledge. These included notions of balance, food chains and human–nature interactions, and showed strong normative dimensions that were used to define desirable or ideal states of nature. These concepts of biodiversity were, in turn, strongly related to their attitudes towards how best to manage biodiversity. This study highlights that a better understanding of individuals’ mental constructs of bio- diversity, which are linked to their attitudes towards biodiversity management, is essential for the design of biodiversity-related policies that are supported by the public. Ó 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction With the tailwind of the sustainability debate, views of the general public on the management of species and habitats have recently been gaining importance in the development of environmental policies (UNECE, 1998; Renn, 2006). Public consultation and participatory approaches are now fre- quently considered as central elements in identifying conser- vation priorities (SNH, 2006; Stewart, 2006) and in the management of protected areas (Martin et al., 2000; Mulongoy and Chape, 2004; Barber, 2004; CNPA, 2006). However, despite increasing efforts in the social and interdisciplinary sciences to shed light on the way members of the public perceive and evaluate biodiversity-related issues (Hull et al., 2001; Stoll- Kleemann, 2001; Kaczensky et al., 2004; Christie et al., 2006), the understanding of public views on biodiversity manage- ment remains limited, often leading to serious doubts about the significance of public opinion (Parsons and Daniel, 2002). Previous studies have often been limited to an analysis of public knowledge and concern about biodiversity against the 0006-3207/$ - see front matter Ó 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.biocon.2006.11.024 * Corresponding author: Tel.: +44 1224 498200; fax: +44 1224 311556. E-mail addresses: a.fi[email protected] (A. Fischer), [email protected] (J.C. Young). BIOLOGICAL CONSERVATION 136 (2007) 271 282 available at www.sciencedirect.com journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biocon

Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity: Implications for

  • Upload
    dodieu

  • View
    216

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity: Implications for

B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E R V A T I O N 1 3 6 ( 2 0 0 7 ) 2 7 1 – 2 8 2

. sc iencedi rec t . com

ava i lab le a t www

journal homepage: www.elsevier .com/ locate /b iocon

Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity:Implications for biodiversity management and conservation

Anke Fischera,*, Juliette C. Youngb

aMacaulay Land Use Research Institute, Socio-Economics Group, Craigiebuckler, Aberdeen AB15 8QH, UKbCentre for Ecology and Hydrology, Hill of Brathens, Glassel, Banchory AB31 4BW, UK

A R T I C L E I N F O

Article history:

Received 15 September 2006

Received in revised form

17 November 2006

Accepted 27 November 2006

Available online 18 January 2007

Keywords:

Biodiversity

Attitudes

Values

Understanding

Management

Public

0006-3207/$ - see front matter � 2006 Elsevidoi:10.1016/j.biocon.2006.11.024

* Corresponding author: Tel.: +44 1224 498200E-mail addresses: [email protected].

A B S T R A C T

The participation of the public in environmental decision-making and management is

increasingly seen as essential for the success of conservation initiatives. Ecological scientists

and conservation practitioners have, however, argued that a lack of understanding of biodi-

versity issues by the public is a barrier to their effective participation in decision-making pro-

cesses. These arguments are often based on studies where scientific knowledge is used as the

sole measure of public understanding of biodiversity, and therefore fail to account for individ-

uals’ constructs of biodiversity and related issues such as biodiversity management.

We examined individuals’ mental constructs of biodiversity, and their conceptual con-

texts, through a series of focus group discussions with members of the general public in Scot-

land. To gain a fuller picture of public understanding of biodiversity, we distinguished

between mental associations with the term ‘biodiversity’, and the meanings associated with

biodiversity-related concepts independent of scientific terminology.

We found participants to express rich mental concepts of biodiversity, irrespective of their

scientific knowledge. These included notions of balance, food chains and human–nature

interactions, and showed strong normative dimensions that were used to define desirable

or ideal states of nature. These concepts of biodiversity were, in turn, strongly related to their

attitudes towards how best to manage biodiversity.

This study highlights that a better understanding of individuals’ mental constructs of bio-

diversity, which are linked to their attitudes towards biodiversity management, is essential

for the design of biodiversity-related policies that are supported by the public.

� 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction

With the tailwind of the sustainability debate, views of the

general public on the management of species and habitats

have recently been gaining importance in the development

of environmental policies (UNECE, 1998; Renn, 2006). Public

consultation and participatory approaches are now fre-

quently considered as central elements in identifying conser-

vation priorities (SNH, 2006; Stewart, 2006) and in the

management of protected areas (Martin et al., 2000; Mulongoy

er Ltd. All rights reserved

; fax: +44 1224 311556.uk (A. Fischer), [email protected]

and Chape, 2004; Barber, 2004; CNPA, 2006). However, despite

increasing efforts in the social and interdisciplinary sciences

to shed light on the way members of the public perceive and

evaluate biodiversity-related issues (Hull et al., 2001; Stoll-

Kleemann, 2001; Kaczensky et al., 2004; Christie et al., 2006),

the understanding of public views on biodiversity manage-

ment remains limited, often leading to serious doubts about

the significance of public opinion (Parsons and Daniel, 2002).

Previous studies have often been limited to an analysis of

public knowledge and concern about biodiversity against the

.c.uk (J.C. Young).

Page 2: Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity: Implications for

272 B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E R V A T I O N 1 3 6 ( 2 0 0 7 ) 2 7 1 – 2 8 2

yardstick of scientific intelligence (Spash and Hanley, 1995;

DEFRA, 2002; Hunter and Brehm, 2003), qualifying individuals’

knowledge as either ‘correct’ or ‘incorrect’ in relation to scien-

tific definitions. In contrast, we follow here the lines of socio-

logical and psychological research on related issues such as

wildlife and landscape management that suggests that public

views are best understood in their cultural, social and individ-

ual contexts (Kellert, 1979; Schultz, 2001; Skogen, 2001; Erics-

son and Heberlein, 2003). In particular, our research is

influenced by the theory of concepts (e.g., Graf, 1989; Medin,

2005). We assume that mental concepts as held by individuals

are complex constructs that may include (i) specific terms

that label a concept, (ii) definitions of a concept, and (iii) pro-

totypical images that encapsulate typical examples for repre-

sentatives of the concept. While some of these elements may

often be missing, the main body of a construct will consist in

its other connotations. The term ‘connotations’ is here used

to refer to mental associations that relate a concept to other

ideas and evaluations. These can take various forms and

may include normative aspects that capture what should or

ought to be, own experiences, and emotional evaluations.

For example, previous studies suggest that members of the

general public may hold rich mental concepts of biodiversity

although they might not be familiar with the scientific termi-

nology (Elder et al., 1998; Menzel, 2004; Holl, 2005). In particu-

lar, there is some evidence that paradigms such as

‘interconnectedness’ and systemic thinking are important

elements of mental constructs related to biodiversity; how-

ever, this has so far not been analysed in detail (Elder et al.,

1998; Menzel, 2004). Other potential components of mental

constructs related to biodiversity include the value judge-

ments implicit to conservation biology (Harrison, 1993; Barry

and Oelschlaeger, 1996; Matsuda, 1997; Fischer and Van der

Wal, 2007) and individuals’ ideas of nature, which have been

found to underlie attitudes towards wildlife and landscape

management (Fulton et al., 1996; Buijs et al., 2006; Van der

Windt et al., in press). However, little is still known about

the way in which individuals’ concepts of biodiversity and

nature are related to their attitudes with regard to these is-

sues (Henwood and Pidgeon, 2001; Clayton and Brook, 2005),

a crucial aspect with regard to public acceptance and support

of biodiversity management measures.

The aim of our study is thus to contribute to a better

understanding of the ways members of the general public

reason about issues of biodiversity change and management,

to shed light on the factors that determine public acceptance

of concrete biodiversity management measures, and to facil-

itate the development of suitable ways to communicate these.

We applied qualitative group discussions that involved

members of the general public from the Cairngorms area in

Scotland, Britain’s largest national park, to obtain insights into

the ways in which individuals form their attitudes towards bio-

diversity in relation to their constructs of biodiversity, particu-

larly focussing on the values they hold and their views on

nature. Following the concept theoretical approach mentioned

above, we first describe individuals’ mental associations with

the term ‘biodiversity’, and then focus on their connotations

with biodiversity issues independent from technical terminol-

ogy, addressing (a) participants’ own perceptions of diversity in

nature, (b) connotations relating to other concepts, (c) norma-

tive evaluations of these connotations, and (d) connotations

concerning the relationship between humans and nature. We

then provide evidence on how these constructs are linked to

individuals’ attitudes towards biodiversity management.

2. Methods

2.1. Study site

Our study centred on the Cairngorms National Park in central

Scotland, established in 2003 and currently undergoing a

process of public consultation to develop a park management

plan. The Cairngorms National Park covers an area of 3800

km2, and is Britain’s newest national park. The Cairngorms

massif constitutes one of the largest and most unspoilt upland

areas in Britain (Warren, 2002) and is considered to be the

most important mountain area in Britain for nature conserva-

tion. It contains a number of rare habitats, including the larg-

est expanse of Old Caledonian pine forest in the British Isles

(Curry-Lindahl, 1990), covering approximately 12,000 ha and

home to the endemic Scottish crossbill (Loxia scotica), the

crested tit (Parus cristatus) and capercaillie (Tetrao urogallus)

(Dennis, 2002). Other priority habitats include Northern Atlan-

tic wet heath containing Erica tetralix, European dry heaths, Al-

pine and Boreal heaths, blanket bogs and bog woodland (JNCC,

2006). The National Park was chosen as a thematic focus and

common reference of our group discussions to allow partici-

pants to relate to their own experiences of biodiversity in

the area, and to gather information on their attitudes towards

biodiversity management. Discussions thus centred on local

biodiversity rather than on global biodiversity issues.

2.2. Focus group approach

We used a qualitative approach to explore the context in

which biodiversity-related issues are perceived by members

of the general public. Qualitative research methods include,

for example, ethnographic or participant observation, quali-

tative interviews, focus groups, and discourse and conversa-

tion analysis. These do not aim to produce quantitative data

and test hypotheses in a strict sense but to explore phenom-

ena in depth.

Focus group discussions are a form of group interview

using a relatively open, non-directive qualitative technique

to explore topics as perceived by the participants (Merton,

1987; Fontana and Frey, 2000; Madriz, 2000). As well as under-

standing interactions between the members of the group, an

important aspect of the focus group is to examine the joint

construction of meaning (Bryman, 2004).

This technique is increasingly applied to understanding

participants’ views on environmental issues (Gobster, 2001;

Henwood and Pidgeon, 2001; Hull et al., 2001). We adopted

this method to allow an identification of the issues important

to participants and a qualitative analysis of their understand-

ings of biodiversity and attitudes towards biodiversity man-

agement and change. As the aim of the study was to

understand the way in which the participants reasoned

rather than to obtain a representative overview of attitudes

held or to compare the views of sub-groups with statistical

means, this approach was considered most appropriate.

Page 3: Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity: Implications for

Table 2 – Focus group discussion guide

1. What are your personal experiences in the Cairngorms? Probing,

for example: Do you have a favourite plant or animal in the

region? How often have you been in this area? What is your gen-

eral impression of the area? What were your expectations before

you came?

2. Have you heard about the Cairngorms National Park?

3. Have you ever come across the term ‘‘biodiversity’’, or biological

diversity? If not, provide information: ‘‘Biodiversity means the vari-

ety of life. Biodiversity includes all living things and the environ-

ment of which they are part.’’a

4. What does ‘biological diversity’ mean to you? What first comes

to your mind?

5. Please draw a symbol or a diagram representing your idea of bio-

logical diversity.

6. How important is biological diversity to human beings? How

important is biological diversity for your everyday life?

7. How do you think biological diversity could best be maintained

or managed?

8. Would you like to add anything?

a http://www.cairngorms.co.uk/park/wildlife/index.php.

B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E R V A T I O N 1 3 6 ( 2 0 0 7 ) 2 7 1 – 2 8 2 273

Consequently, sampling was designed to cover a cross-sec-

tion of the general public from a wide range of backgrounds

including urban and rural dwellers, laypeople, citizen-stake-

holders and professionals in relevant fields. In order to ensure

that a wide range of views on biodiversity was captured by the

focus groups, we adopted their ‘‘experience of biodiversity’’ as

our main stratifying criterion.

Overall, eight group discussions with a total of 43 individ-

uals were held from May to October 2005. Discussions lasted

between one and two hours. Participants’ ages ranged be-

tween 19 and 76. Group sizes varied between two and ten par-

ticipants (Table 1), with six of the groups mixed in gender

(groups T1 and T2 contained only men). The local residents

(T5) were a mixed group with participants from a variety of

professional backgrounds that were all living in the National

Park area, whereas all other discussants were either residents

of adjacent areas or (potential) visitors of the Cairngorms.

Most discussion groups were recruited from pre-existing

groups which either participated as an entire group (T1, T8)

or in parts (T2, T6, T7), while groups T3–T5 included individ-

uals recruited through posters and local newspaper

announcements that asked for volunteers to participate in a

group discussion on nature in the Cairngorms. By these

means, we aimed to reduce participant self-selection with re-

gard to prior knowledge specifically relating to biodiversity.

Readers unused to qualitative research may be surprised by

the seemingly small sample sizes (Table 1), but it should be

borne in mind that not statistical and quantitative analysis,

but an exploration of the diversity of mental constructs of

biodiversity within the general public, here represented by a

broad range of interest groups, are the aim of this study.

As previous research suggests that respondents’ insecurity

with regard to technical terminology can hamper and con-

strain the elicitation of individual perceptions (Christie

et al., 2006), we used both verbal and visual methods to elicit

responses. In addition to the verbal debate that revolved

around the open-ended questions in our discussion guideline

(Table 2), participants were also asked to draw a picture of

what they had in mind when they heard the word ‘‘biodiver-

sity’’. The drawings (n = 33) provided participants with the

opportunity to individually reflect upon their mental concepts

and represent them through a non-verbal approach that al-

lowed an expression of thoughts independent from the confi-

dence individuals might or might not feel with regard to the

scientific terminology (Matthews, 1985; Gobster, 1998; Alerby,

Table 1 – Focus group participants: group sizes and main char

Group code n Groups

T1 6 Mountaineers on a training co

T2 4 Mountaineers resident in adja

T3 2 Tourists (from England)

T4 2 Tourists (from Germany)

T5 10 Local residents

T6 4 Foresters resident in adjacent

T7 6 Birdwatchers resident in adjac

T8 9 Farmers (Agricultural college

2000). We also asked the respondents to explain their draw-

ings briefly, in order to interpret them as they were intended

(Alerby, 2000).

2.3. Data coding and processing

All focus group discussions were digitally recorded and subse-

quently verbatim transcribed and checked. Broad coding cat-

egories were defined according to initial research questions

that had also been used to design the discussion guidelines.

These categories were modified, refined and specified in an

iterative process to obtain a hierarchically organised coding

scheme (Ryan and Bernard, 2000; Rogan et al., 2005), while

concurrently being validated through comparisons and dis-

cussions with colleagues from other European countries

working on related qualitative studies. Final main coding cat-

egories included the understanding of biodiversity, concepts

of nature, views on the role of humans in nature, values re-

lated to nature and biodiversity, attitudes towards biodiver-

sity management measures and the perception of changes

and threats to biodiversity. The transcripts were coded using

these categories by both authors with intermittent exchanges

and discussions (Hull et al., 2001).

acteristics

Age groups

19–39 40–59 60+

urse 3 3

cent areas 1 3

2

2

1 4 5

areas 2 2

ent areas 0 3 3

students) 9 0 0

Page 4: Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity: Implications for

274 B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E R V A T I O N 1 3 6 ( 2 0 0 7 ) 2 7 1 – 2 8 2

3. Results: constructs of biodiversity

3.1. Associations with the term ‘biodiversity’ – ‘‘It’s whatit says on the tin, isn’t it?’’ [T7]

In the first part of the discussions, we asked participants di-

rectly for their understanding of and their mental associa-

tions with the term ‘biodiversity’. We identified three types

of reactions from participants, (i) confusion, (ii) definitions

and (iii) critical comments. Whereas some participants, espe-

cially among the tourists and the young farmers, articulated

their lack of knowledge (I don’t really know, I have only heard

the word [Group T3, see Table 1]), others, especially among

the mountaineers, offered definitions such as:

[Biodiversity means] the number of species and variety of spe-

cies that are present. Species right across the board, not just veg-

etation [T2].

Many responses by local residents and foresters took a meta-

perspective and commented on the strategic use of the term:

It has become a buzzword [T5];

I tend to think biodiversity is just a fancy name for nature [T6].

Although these individuals assessed the term biodiversity in

a critical way, its positive impacts on political support for con-

servation were also recognised:

I personally don’t like the word, but I think it’s a good rallying

call because I think it was something that was getting extremely

neglected internationally [T6].

Others observed that in their view, the biodiversity concept

was being misunderstood by land managers and conservation

organisations and used as a label to gain public support for

the preservation of particular species:

I was in [a forest] the other day and it was actually full of

bird boxes and you think, what is the point of a bird box?

It’s a forest that has more dead wood oak trees than any other

forest, I think. What’s the point? You just felt like saying ‘‘No,

that’s not the point, that’s not what biodiversity is about, it’s

not about sticking up bloody bird boxes. It’s about getting the

base right’’[T6].

This critical stance was mainly taken by individuals with a

professional background in land management issues, for

example by a retired gamekeeper, a ranger, and a geographer

in the group of local residents. Already in 1996, Takacs found

similar reactions for many of the conservation biologists that

he interviewed. However, while at that time the concept of

biodiversity was widely unknown to the public (Elder et al.,

1998), the responses of the mountaineers in our study suggest

that the term ‘biodiversity’ has been communicated recently

from science to parts of the interested public, who now make

use of the term. Individuals who deal with these terms on a

more professional basis, in contrast, view the terminology

in a more ambivalent manner and emphasise the strategic

use of the word.

3.2. Perception and appreciation of biological diversity –‘‘Open your eyes’’ [T4]

During the course of the discussions, numerous statements

that were independent from our direct questions on the def-

inition of ‘biodiversity’ (Table 2) revealed that many partici-

pants had rich concepts in mind that addressed issues of

diversity not necessarily linked to the technical term. Many

participants, among them both ‘‘experts’’ and ‘‘non-experts’’

in natural history, indicated that they perceived and appreci-

ated diversity in their surroundings:

The grasses are very different here; there are lots of different

grasses [T3];

Great secrets that are hidden away, small pockets of particular

species, just a different atmosphere, just a mixture of the hills,

the water. . . [T2].

Especially the tourists, but also the young farmers, mountain-

eers and some of the birdwatchers appreciated diversity for

aesthetic reasons:

Let’s imagine there were only heather and broom, it surely

wouldn’t look that nice. . . for example, up in the mountains we

saw funny plants, very small things, I don’t know, (. . .) but it

was very nice [T4];

If everything was of the same colour that would be boring [T4].

On a larger scale diversity was perceived to turn a site into a

unique and recognisable place, indicating that diversity in its

spatial patterns can contribute to individuals’ sense of place

(Rogan et al., 2005):

You could be helicoptered blindfold to a Scottish hill and if you

could look down at the vegetation you could say roughly what

part of Scotland you were in [T2].

This was also reflected in the drawings, in which participants

highlighted the habitats and species they perceived as repre-

sentative of the region (Figs. 1, 2 and 4). These drawings often

included iconic elements of the environments, usually sub-

jectively representative of Scotland such as thistles, red deer

(Fig. 1), or deer grass Scirpus cespitosus (Fig. 2). Elements of cul-

tural diversity were also portrayed in these drawings, particu-

larly by the tourists who also saw castles and stone walls as a

part of the biodiversity of the area.

The tourists also emphasised how their perception of the

diversity of plant and animal life depended on their mindset

and the attention directed to their natural environment:

Countryside-wise and scenery-wise it is absolutely fantastic all

the way, but the diversity of plant life is huge as well, but it is

not that obvious, until you start to think about it [T3].

However, some mountaineers and farmers also expressed

that they did not know or did not care about species diversity,

as it had no relevance to them personally:

In terms of vegetation I know nothing about it other than it is all

heather. I have rarely looked at what I have been stepping on [T1].

Page 5: Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity: Implications for

Fig. 1 – ‘‘Nature, peace, tranquillity, variety and colour different throughout the year’’ [T7].

B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E R V A T I O N 1 3 6 ( 2 0 0 7 ) 2 7 1 – 2 8 2 275

Others raised questions on the relevance of conservation for

themselves as non-experts, as they felt that they personally

did not notice and did not suffer from any changes in

biodiversity:

As a recreationalist I don’t know much about some of these

aspects [loss of biodiversity]; it has never detracted me from

my enjoyment of the Cairngorms, so it does beg the question,

is it important? [T2].

The way biodiversity and its changes were perceived thus dif-

fered considerably between the participants. Although we

Fig. 2 – ‘‘That was my little pink flower, do you know what it

would be, this plant?’’ [T3].

could not find any links between these perceptions and the

definitions individuals had in mind as described above, an

analysis of the context of the participants’ perceptions helps

to understand their differences.

3.3. Biodiversity in context: associations with otherconcepts – ‘‘to keep the natural flow’’ [T5]

Numerous statements showed that participants’ under-

standings of biological diversity were deeply rooted in a

variety of other, related concepts. Some of these state-

ments emerged during the discussions as responses to di-

rect questions about the implications of biodiversity loss

and the reasons for conservation, while others were part

of descriptions of experiences and perceptions of nature

in the Cairngorms.

Among these concepts, ‘food chains’ were frequently and

used in almost all the groups to argue for the conservation

of diversity:

. . .because not every animal likes every plant I guess, a few feed

on this, and a few on others [T4];

It is just to keep the natural flow! That is why it needs to be pro-

tected! If you do away with one species it affects another species

[T5].

Closely related to the notion of food chains was the idea of

‘balance’:

[Biodiversity] should be preserved under all circumstances. To

keep the equilibrium, the natural balance [T4];

It is the balance that keeps the environment healthy [T1].

Notions of balance and food chains were again echoed in the

drawings (Fig. 3):

Page 6: Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity: Implications for

276 B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E R V A T I O N 1 3 6 ( 2 0 0 7 ) 2 7 1 – 2 8 2

I was much more interested in the interaction of everything. . . the

key behind the whole system was that everything was intercon-

nected, overlapping and more importantly inter-dependent. On

top of that there was superimposed the influence of man [T2].

‘Balance’, perceived imbalance and the dominance of certain

species played a major role when participants described situ-

ations that they perceived as negative:

I think it is the imbalance. There is nothing that eats rhododen-

dron, so you end up with rhododendron everywhere. They don’t

eat mink so there is mink everywhere [T2];

It can, things can dominate. [. . .] Look at rhododendron. If that

takes over that takes over and there’s nothing else that grows

near it [T6].

3.4. Normative dimensions of biodiversity – ‘‘it looks likeit should look’’ [T2]

Many participants had implicit states of reference in mind

when arguing for biodiversity conservation:

And when you go to the bits that do still have the native wood-

land, they are the most attractive places to be and there is things

to see and it looks like it should look. It just seems more correct in

some indefinable way [T2].

For these respondents, there was clearly a normative dimen-

sion to the state of habitats and ecosystems. Our analysis

thus also addressed the issue of how individuals judged the

‘right’ and the ‘wrong’ states or species, and the reasons be-

hind these judgements. For some participants, ‘what should

Fig. 3 – ‘‘In biodiversity, everything is connected and

contained in the same environment, but with no hierarchy’’

[T1].

be there’ seemed to be somehow naturally determined by cli-

mate and habitat, preferably without any human

intervention:

You need to look at the whole, and maybe survey what is in

there, and maybe think what isn’t there, and could be there,

and maybe should be there.

[Moderator: So it’s defined by the habitat? By the ecosystem?]

Well, yes I think it is, by the climate and by the habitat. It cer-

tainly shouldn’t be defined by the number of people who are

clomping through it! [T7].

Consequently, some participants explicitly argued that

humans should not interfere with nature:

It’s nature’s way, and evolution and that of species getting wiped

out and changed and that, and we shouldn’t be meddling with

that. There are too many do-gooders that are trying to save

things and that. They should leave it and let it get on with its

natural way [T8].

Here, nature, untouched by humans, defined the ‘right’ state

of diversity. However, the participants also used other criteria

to assess states and species. Whilst many participants, espe-

cially tourists, tended to consider native species that ap-

peared unique and typical for the area as positive,

(illegitimately) introduced species as well as overly dominant

ones were evaluated as negative. However, the perceptions of

what was dominant (Gorse! It’s everywhere here! [T3]), and what

was unique and typical (The flowering of the gorse, that’s the rea-

son why we came here this time [T4]) diverged among the

participants.

Here again, the notion of ‘balance’ as opposed to the dom-

inance of single species played an important role, but cultural

and historical aspects were also linked to antipathy felt

against non-native plant species:

That has to do with history as well. . . these ferns haven’t been

here always and have spread quite a lot, it was the Englishmen

[T4].

Examples from other discussion groups underscore these

observations:

We did a survey [. . .] and asked people what they thought of the

forest at [P.] and what they suggested was that we should reduce

the amount of Sitka spruce [. . .] because they hate that whole

hillside being all Sitka spruce. Well, it’s all Scots pine. It’s just

planted and grows. It’ll look good, but again it’s people’s percep-

tions. . . [T6].

In this case, the public claimed to dislike Sitka spruce on

the hillside, a species introduced for reforestation, as it

was perceived to be too monotonous and dominant. In fact

the native Scots pine, as a mature tree perceived usually as

a charismatic species, was growing on that hill but in this

case seen as unaesthetic and thus labelled as Sitka spruce,

a species associated with negative characteristics. This

observation illustrates again how individual perceptions of

diversity are informed by the participants’ beliefs and values.

Page 7: Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity: Implications for

B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E R V A T I O N 1 3 6 ( 2 0 0 7 ) 2 7 1 – 2 8 2 277

Several respondents recognised these relationships and

pointed out how their own knowledge influenced the eval-

uation of a certain landscape or habitat, referring to intro-

duced species and to grazing impacts on the native

woodlands:

Itself, it looks quite attractive, but when you start to understand

the process that is going on, it just doesn’t belong there and that

is what offends [T2].

These normative aspects were especially obvious in the draw-

ings. Out of the 33 drawings, 20 showed an explicitly positive

state of biodiversity, or were commented on in a way that

pointed to the positive image that they tried to convey (Figs.

1 and 4). Ten drawings could not be classified as they seemed

to be schematic or iconic with no particular normative impli-

cations (Fig. 2). Only two drawings included explicitly nega-

tive elements such as the extinction of species, while one

drawing included both positive and negative elements. Other

elements which were verbally described as negative, such as

the non-native species mentioned frequently, were not

depicted.

In those drawings that appeared to capture positive

images of biodiversity, many participants – mainly mountain-

eers and birdwatchers – drew what they saw as an ‘‘undis-

turbed’’ or ‘‘wild’’ state of biodiversity. This contrasts

strongly with the discussions, in which only one participant

described the Cairngorms as one of the last, really THE last, wil-

derness left in Britain [T7], while the vast majority of partici-

pants concurred on the fact that wilderness no longer

existed in Western Europe:

I think we would all agree that there is no true wildness any-

where in Britain, there has been Homo sapiens and non-sapiens

on every bit of it [T5].

Fig. 4 – ‘‘Just creating dif

In most drawings (n = 21) anthropogenic influence was omni-

present (Fig. 4), but their images seemed to be equally idealis-

tic in a rather Arcadian, i.e., idyllic and peaceful type of

landscape where humans and nature are in harmony (Buijs

et al., 2006; Van der Windt et al., in press). Those individuals

who saw human influence as beneficial for biodiversity con-

servation reflected this clearly in their drawings. In contrast,

those participants who felt that humans were a direct threat

to biodiversity through farming, tourism or invasive species,

chose, for the majority, to omit these threats they so vividly

exposed verbally from their visual representations of biodi-

versity. Normative aspects of biodiversity thus seemed to be

a fundamental part of the participants’ biodiversity construct,

which, as will be shown later, influenced participants’ atti-

tudes towards biodiversity management.

3.5. The role of humans in biodiversity management –‘‘And it can be a forgiving hand, or it can be a detrimentalhand’’ [T7]

Views on the role of humans in nature appeared to be a dis-

tinct component of individuals’ constructs of biodiversity.

These were closely related to the notions described above

and had both descriptive and normative dimensions. In gen-

eral, all groups involved in the study readily discussed per-

ceived threats to biodiversity brought about by

anthropogenic factors. In line with the notions of food chains

and balance that many individuals had in mind, there

seemed to be a general feeling that loss of a particular species

would have knock-on effects on other species including hu-

mans, but the exact effects were unclear.

As I see it if you lose your biodiversity then you lose. . . you’ve

lost all things, forget it! [T7];

Without it [biodiversity] we are nothing [T3].

ferent habitats’’ [T8].

Page 8: Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity: Implications for

278 B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E R V A T I O N 1 3 6 ( 2 0 0 7 ) 2 7 1 – 2 8 2

Despite the unclear consequences of biodiversity loss, most

participants held strong opinions on biodiversity manage-

ment, generally supporting the view that biodiversity per se

needed to be conserved. Participants did however differ con-

siderably in their attitudes towards how management

should take place. These were found to be closely linked to

their notions of diversity and their views on the role of

humankind in nature. We identified three main views on

the role of humans in biodiversity management, including

(i) humans as potential enemies of nature, (ii) humans as

users of nature and (iii) humans as active managers of

nature.

For many participants humans were seen as a harmful

influence on biodiversity. This viewpoint was particularly

apparent in the birdwatchers’ group who viewed recreational

activities and conservation as wholly incompatible:

They are all concerned about the erosion on the footpaths and the

erosion of the slopes. Now why is that? Is it because the climbers

go up? It is not. The tourist buses all stop there and all these dod-

dering old tourists go pouring up there. . . The general public as a

whole make a mess of things, because they don’t have an interest

in where they are going [T7].

Interestingly, there seemed to be a distinction in the birdwat-

chers’ group of who should be ‘‘allowed’’ into the Cairngorms,

depending on the level of knowledge and appreciation of the

area and its biodiversity:

Now the area is full of people who in my opinion shouldn’t be

there at all; the trouble is with these things, when you open them

the mass of the people who come in have no interest in the wild-

life [T7].

The tourists, however, had a very different view, highlighting

the benefits of managing biodiversity for recreation and other

purposes, and expressing a view of humans as legitimate

users of biodiversity:

If people stay on the paths, which they normally do, most of the

time, then it is helpful for nature, and moreover money flows into

the area which they can use for the park, which is very important

– without money you can’t do anything, that’s evident [T4].

They stressed that biodiversity should be preserved for the peo-

ple [T3], not for its own sake.

Most participants saw humans as an integral part of biodi-

versity, required for the appropriate management of

biodiversity:

It is as if we [human and nature] are all rubbing shoulders

against each other [T1];

Because the impact on all these things is man’s hand, and it acts

on everything. And it can be a forgiving hand, or it can be a det-

rimental hand [T7].

This view of humans as the active managers of nature in-

cluded very different notions. Some participants saw humans

as responsible for restoring the balance that they had dis-

turbed or even destroyed previously:

If you want to get a proper balance back and bring back wood-

land and things like that you would have to introduce a large car-

nivore, or several large carnivores or whatever, to ape that [T2];

I think it depends on how far it has gone. If it has gone too far

then people have to come in to bring it back. . . [T5].

The young farmers perceived humans as actively shaping and

promoting biodiversity in the rural environment (Fig. 4):

If you didn’t have farming you wouldn’t have the different hab-

itats [T8].

But despite the need for management expressed by partici-

pants, some warned against the risks of over-managing biodi-

versity, emphasising its resilience, and took a critical stance

towards a forced maintenance of the status quo:

But it’s not fragile. Everybody seems to think it’s terribly fragile

[. . .]. It’s not fragile, it’s extremely robust and if something does

go something else will take its place [T6];

Just give it time. Forget looking at the clock. . .You don’t want it to

stand still. In a lot of ways you want the area to move forward

[T6].

Thus, for land managers such as the foresters and the young

farmers, biodiversity management was often seen as a con-

straint, interfering with agricultural and sylvicultural prac-

tices and contradicting their own view of nature as a

resilient body interacting with humankind:

There could be regulations for people that are wrecking habitats,

but I don’t think we have to go to the extent of having to reintroduce

habitats. Because there’s still enough to maintain the wildlife as it

is, without having to start spoiling good farmland really [T8].

4. Discussion

Through both verbal contributions and drawings, we found

the participants of our group discussions to express rich

views and concepts on biodiversity in their local environment

irrespective of their knowledge about scientific definitions

and technical terminologies. The drawings proved to be a par-

ticularly useful tool in eliciting these concepts. While many

participants found themselves put on the spot with a direct

question of what they understood by biodiversity, the drawing

exercise enabled them to have a personal space for thought

and time to represent their interpretation visually and devel-

op it subsequently through dialogue.

At first sight participants sometimes seemed to have

‘‘strongly held but poorly defended concerns’’ especially with

regard to biodiversity loss, resulting threats to humankind,

and biodiversity management (Hull et al., 2001) – a phenome-

non that might reflect the current controversial scientific de-

bate on the role of biodiversity in ecosystem functioning

(Naeem et al., 1995; Loreau et al., 2001; Hooper et al., 2005).

An in-depth analysis of individuals’ verbal and visual contri-

butions, however, revealed that these concerns were in-

formed by very distinctive factors. The normative

Page 9: Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity: Implications for

B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E R V A T I O N 1 3 6 ( 2 0 0 7 ) 2 7 1 – 2 8 2 279

connotations particularly proved to be very influential com-

ponents of individuals’ constructs of biodiversity and nature.

Our analysis suggests that individuals have certain, often

concurrent, characteristics of biodiversity in mind that are

clearly evaluated as either positive or negative. While balance

was almost unequivocally considered as explicitly positive

with food-webs being closely associated to the concept of

equilibrium, dominance of single species was evaluated as

negative. These notions might reflect the importance of equi-

librium theories for ecological thinking in past decades that

has now reached educational curricula and popular scientific

programmes and publications as well as conservation prac-

tice (Wallington et al., 2005). Naturalness, i.e., the absence of

human influence on biodiversity, was a particularly positive

feature of nature and biodiversity for groups such as the

mountaineers and birdwatchers. Wilderness was conse-

quently also discussed as a potentially positive quality of nat-

ure, though the majority of the respondents did not consider

the Cairngorms as true wilderness. In contrast, wilderness is

a feature often attributed to Scotland and used in the market-

ing of some of Scotland’s tourist areas. The Cairngorms na-

tional park website for example boasts that the national

park includes unique ‘‘mountainous areas of wild lands’’,

and Habron (1998) found that 89% of Scottish residents sur-

veyed in a questionnaire (n = 459) thought that wild lands ex-

isted in Scotland.

The normative components of individuals’ concepts of

biodiversity became even clearer when comparing drawings

with verbal expressions, as we identified a gap between the

ideal pictures of biodiversity that the respondents chose to

show in their drawings, and the much more critical assess-

ments of the actual state of biodiversity they expressed ver-

bally. This reinforces the notion that drawings were, for

many participants, a method of conveying their ideal image

of biodiversity, a wilderness or idyllic Arcadian landscape

where anthropogenic threats were absent, an observation

that Alerby (2000) also made in her study on children’s views

of their environment.

Takacs (1996) found in his interviews with professional

conservation biologists that these evaluative aspects that

were seen to have considerable normative and emotional

power were clearly perceived as present, but confusing and

unclear, thus hindering practical biodiversity management.

Unravelling the value judgments that are part of individuals’

concepts helped to improve our understanding of how these

normative associations relate to individuals’ attitudes to-

wards biodiversity management.

Indeed, we found that the concepts individuals associated

to biodiversity were closely linked to their attitudes towards

biodiversity management in general. Here, we can distinguish

three different stances, each informed by the participants’

constructs of biodiversity.

A first stance advocated biodiversity issues as the highest

priority for land use considerations. Many of the mountain-

eers and birdwatchers in our sample knew a great deal about

biodiversity. They tended to see humankind as separate from

nature, natural systems as very fragile, and humans as gener-

ally harmful to their environment. They thus opted for an

exclusion of humans from large natural areas. Their attitude

towards the conservation of biodiversity was a protectionist,

biodiversity-centric one, requiring a strong top-down ap-

proach for environmental governance:

We have no concept of management, we tinker with things.

Somebody should draw the line and say ‘This is how we are

going to do it’. Yeah well we should keep people out because peo-

ple are just a bloody nuisance! [T7].

A second group of individuals, mainly tourists, often set bio-

diversity conservation on an equal par with other land uses,

such as agriculture and recreation, and tended to see man

as a user of nature who could potentially, but not necessarily,

have negative impacts on nature. Aesthetic aspects of nature

were emphasised alongside economic benefits of nature,

while acknowledging that natural systems could be severely

disturbed by human overexploitation:

I think environmental considerations should stand as an equal

partner to financial considerations because you can’t continue

to raid the landscape because it is the landscape that wins people

back into the hills [T1].

A third position was held by many foresters and farmer stu-

dents who saw man as the manager of nature. While some

farmer students expressed that biodiversity issues should

not be considered as important as they currently were, the

foresters saw their own influence on biodiversity as positive

and an integral part of their professional remit. Natural sys-

tems were seen as robust. At the same time, change and evo-

lution were considered as a constitutive part of nature.

Correspondingly, biodiversity was considered a very dynamic

and resilient entity:

I think it does a pretty good job itself. We don’t necessarily have

to go to the lengths that we are at the moment [T6].

The more ‘‘functional’’ (Buijs et al., 2006) view of the foresters

and farmers therefore adopted a more pragmatic approach to

biodiversity conservation, acknowledging the need for con-

servation, while allowing for flexibility to incorporate farming

and sylvicultural activities.

Our analysis shows that individuals’ attitudes towards bio-

diversity management in their environment are complex con-

structs that appear to be informed and characterised by their

mental constructs of biodiversity, including normative associ-

ations, with regard to (a) humankind, (b) natural systems and

(c) the interactions between humans and nature. While the

participants’ views on nature and human–nature interactions

were the explicit focus of this study, their concepts of human-

kind were only occasionally touched upon. However, this

seems to be an important factor determining individuals’ atti-

tudes towards biodiversity management and should thus be

given more attention in future research.

Interestingly however, the group of local residents [T5] did

not contribute to the debate of these general issues of biodi-

versity in the Cairngorms as much as the other groups in

our sample did, and expressed their personal feelings with re-

gard to certain species and habitats to a lesser degree. In con-

trast, institutional issues related to the National Park

implementation dominated the discussion among these local

Page 10: Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity: Implications for

280 B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E R V A T I O N 1 3 6 ( 2 0 0 7 ) 2 7 1 – 2 8 2

residents who felt most affected by what they viewed as a

flawed top-down approach. The majority of local residents

who attended the focus group had lived in the area for a long

time, and had gradually seen a decrease in local species num-

bers, with many of the more charismatic species such as

mountain hare and wild cat disappearing over the years.

The discussions were often full of anger and resentment

aimed at the institutional level:

The Cairngorms is the worst managed area there is probably in

Europe [T5];

I think it is really vicious and the National Park are sponsoring

the extermination of the wild cat [T5].

Local residents seemed dismayed at the effects of measures

in the park. An integration of local knowledge and concerns

was in their view non-existent at present, alienating them

for the process of conservation in the Cairngorms. Procedural

aspects of biodiversity management took centre stage in the

debate, and participants’ values with regard to biodiversity

management in general were hardly expressed. While an im-

proved understanding of public concepts of biodiversity,

including their normative notions, can thus be very helpful

to predict and improve public support for biodiversity man-

agement in general, in specific local cases procedural aspects

come into play and might be the dominant factors determin-

ing public attitudes. These have to be carefully taken into

consideration to ensure the acceptance and ownership of

management measures.

5. Conclusion

We examined public views on biodiversity and biodiversity

management in the Cairngorms National Park in Scotland

and found the participants to express complex mental con-

cepts that included notions such as balance, food chains,

dominance and the irreversible nature of biodiversity loss,

as well as the role of humans in nature. Normative evalua-

tions of these notions were directly related to individuals’

attitudes towards biodiversity management. To cover the

complexity of these issues, a combination of methods,

namely focus group discussions together with drawing exer-

cises, proved useful.

Previously, public understanding of biodiversity has often

been measured against knowledge of scientific terminology

with regard to biodiversity, and the lack of scientific knowl-

edge in members of the general public has been used to ar-

gue against public participation in decision-making and

policy development. However, our results suggest that this

is an unsuitable argument: Independent from scientific ter-

minology, individuals expressed attitudes towards biodiver-

sity management measures that were well grounded in

complex mental concepts and corresponding normative

evaluations.

We therefore argue that approaches that reveal such value

judgments and their conceptual contexts are essential instru-

ments for an improved design and communication of biodi-

versity policies that will be more likely to find public

support. By the same token, critical reflection, communica-

tion and an open debate of the values implicit in biodiversity

policies and expert opinions might also help to gain public

acceptance (Matsuda, 1997; Fischer and Van der Wal, 2007).

Consequently, discursive approaches that recognise and em-

brace the public’s multi-faceted and well-embedded views

rather than educational programmes that impose a dominant

environmental paradigm, are required to improve public sup-

port for biodiversity management and to reduce conflicts.

Our analysis also highlights the importance of two other

aspects that are closely related to individuals’ attitudes to-

wards biodiversity management. First, and so far neglected

in social scientific research on environmental issues, individ-

uals’ views of humankind appear to inform their perspectives

on biodiversity management. Second, institutional and proce-

dural aspects of environmental management play an influen-

tial role especially in those cases where the local population is

directly affected by management measures. Studies that inte-

grate both aspects are still lacking, but essential to under-

stand the widespread lack of public acceptance for

biodiversity management and to develop more sustainable

approaches to biodiversity conservation.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all participants of the group discussions for

their contributions. We would also like to thank Jamie Watt

and Lynne Robinson for their help with the transcriptions,

as well as Kirsty Blackstock, Rene van der Wal, Susanne Men-

zel, Adam Vanbergen, Dave Raffaelli and an anonymous re-

viewer for their helpful comments on earlier versions of the

manuscript, and Arjen Buijs, Jana Sedlakova and Dieter Rink

for inspiring discussions. This work was supported by AL-

TER-Net (A Long-term Biodiversity, Ecosystem and Awareness

Research Network), a Network of Excellence funded by the 6th

Framework Programme of the European Commission

(www.alter-net.info).

R E F E R E N C E S

Alerby, E., 2000. A way of visualising children’s and young people’sthoughts about the environment: a study of drawings.Environmental Education Research 6, 205–222.

Barber, C.V., 2004. Parks and people in a world of changes:Governance, participation and equity. In: Barber, C.V., Miller,K.R., Boness, M. (Eds.), Securing Protected Areas in the Face ofGlobal Change: Issues and Strategies. IUCN, Gland,Switzerland and Cambridge, UK, pp. 97–136.

Barry, D., Oelschlaeger, M., 1996. A science for survival: values andconservation biology. Conservation Biology 10, 905–911.

Bryman, A., 2004. Social Research Methods. Oxford UniversityPress, Oxford.

Buijs, A., Pedroli, B., Luginbuhl, Y., 2006. From hiking throughfarmland to farming in a leisure landscape: changing socialperceptions of the European landscape. Landscape Ecology 21,375–389.

Christie, M., Hanley, N., Warren, J., Murphy, K., Wright, R., Hyde,T., 2006. Valuing the diversity of biodiversity. EcologicalEconomics 58, 304–317.

Clayton, S., Brook, A., 2005. Can psychology help save the world?A model for conservation psychology. Analyses of SocialIssues and Public Policy 5, 87–102.

Page 11: Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity: Implications for

B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E R V A T I O N 1 3 6 ( 2 0 0 7 ) 2 7 1 – 2 8 2 281

CNPA, 2006. A draft plan for the future: looking to 2030.Grantown-on-Spey, Cairngorms National Park Authority.<http://www.cairngorms.co.uk/resource/docs/publications/CNPA.Paper.210.Looking%20to%202030%20-%20Full%20Report.pdf>.

Curry-Lindahl, K., 1990. The Cairngorms National Nature Reserve(NNR), the foremost British conservation area of internationalsignificance. In: Conroy, J.W.H., Watson, A., Gunson, A.R. (Eds.),Caring for the High Mountains – Conservation of theCairngorms. Centre for Scottish Studies, Aberdeen, pp. 61–90.

DEFRA, 2002. Survey of Public Attitudes to Quality of Life and theEnvironment – 2001. DEFRA, London.

Dennis, R., 2002. Birds and mammals. In: Gimingham, C.H. (Ed.),The Ecology, Land Use and Conservation of the Cairngorms.Packard Publishing, Chichester, pp. 43–53.

Elder, J., Coffin, C., Farrior, M., 1998. Engaging the public onbiodiversity – a road map for education and communicationstrategies. Madison, The Biodiversity Project. <http://www.biodiversityproject.org/roadmap.pdf>.

Ericsson, G., Heberlein, T., 2003. Attitudes of hunters, locals andthe general public in Sweden now that the wolves are back.Biological Conservation 111, 149–159.

Fischer, A., Van der Wal, R., 2007. Invasive plant suppressescharismatic seabird – the construction of attitudes towardsbiodiversity management options. Biological Conservation135, 256–267.

Fontana, A., Frey, J., 2000. The interview: from structuredquestions to negotiated text. In: Denzin, N.K., Lincoln, Y. (Eds.),Handbook of Qualitative Methods. Sage Publications,Thousand Oaks, pp. 645–672.

Fulton, D.C., Manfredo, M.J., Limpscomb, J., 1996. Wildlife ValueOrientations: A Conceptual and Measurement Approach.Human Dimensions of Wildlife 1, 24–47.

Gobster, P.H., 1998. Nearby neighborhood residents’ images andperceptions of the river. In: Gobster, P.H., Westphal, L.M. (Eds.),People and the River: Perception and Use of ChicagoWaterways for Recreation. US Department of Interior,Milwaukee, pp. 5–48.

Gobster, P.H., 2001. Visions of nature: conflict and compatibility inurban park restoration. Landscape and Urban Planning 56,35–51.

Graf, D., 1989. Begriffslernen im Biologieunterricht derSekundarstufe I – Empirische Untersuchungen undHaufigkeitsanalysen. Frankfurt/Main, Peter Lang.

Habron, D., 1998. Visual perception of wild land in Scotland.Landscape and Urban Planning 42, 45–56.

Harrison, C., 1993. Nature conservation, science and popularvalues. In: Goldsmith, F., Warren, A. (Eds.), Conservation inProgress. John Wiley and Sons, Chichester.

Henwood, K., Pidgeon, N., 2001. Talk about woods and trees:threat of urbanization, stability, and biodiversity. Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 21, 125–147.

Holl, A., 2005. Narrating diversity: plants, personal knowledge andlife stories in German home gardens. In: Markussen, M., Buse,R., Garrelts, H., Manez Costa, M., Menzel, S., Marggraf, R. (Eds.),Valuation and Conservation of Biodiversity – InterdisciplinaryView on the Convention on Biological Diversity. Marburg,Metropolis, pp. 221–248.

Hooper, D.U., Chapin, F.S., Ewel, J.J., Hector, A., Inchausti, P.,Lavorel, S., Lawton, J.H., Lodge, D.M., Loreau, M., Naeem, S.,Schmid, B., Setala, H., Symstad, A.J., Vandermeer, J., Wardle,D.A., 2005. Effects of biodiversity on ecosystem functioning: aconsensus of current knowledge. Ecological Monographs 75,3–35.

Hull, R.B., Robertson, D.P., Kendra, A., 2001. Publicunderstandings of nature: a case study of local knowledgeabout ‘‘natural’’ forest conditions. Society and NaturalResources 14, 325–340.

Hunter, L., Brehm, J., 2003. Qualitative insight into publicknowledge of and concern with biodiversity. Human Ecology31, 309–320.

JNCC, 2006. Cairngorms Natura 2000 Data Form. Version 2.1.<http://www.jncc.gov.uk/ProtectedSites/SACSelection/n2kforms/UK0016412.pdf>.

Kaczensky, P., Blazic, M., Gossow, H., 2004. Public attitudestowards brown bears (Ursus arctos) in Slovenia. BiologicalConservation 118, 661–674.

Kellert, S.R., 1979. Public Attitudes Toward Critical Wildlife andNatural Habitat Issues. US Department of the Interior; US Fishand Wildlife service, Washington, DC.

Loreau, M., Naeem, S., Inchausti, P., Bengtsson, J., Grime, J.P.,Hector, A., Hooper, D.U., Huston, M.A., Raffaelli, D., Schmid, B.,Tilman, D., Wardle, D.A., 2001. Ecology – biodiversity andecosystem functioning: current knowledge and futurechallenges. Science 294, 804–808.

Madriz, E., 2000. Focus Groups in feminist research. In: Denzin,N.K., Lincoln, Y. (Eds.), Handbook of Qualitative Research. SagePublications, Thousand Oaks, pp. 835–850.

Martin, W.E., Wise Bender, H., Shields, D.J., 2000. Stakeholderobjectives for public lands: rankings of forest managementalternatives. Journal of Environmental Management 58, 21–32.

Matsuda, B., 1997. Conservation biology, values, and advocacy.Conservation Biology 11, 1449–1450.

Matthews, M.H., 1985. Young children’s representations of theenvironment: a comparison of techniques. Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 5, 261–278.

Medin, D.L., 2005. Concepts and conceptual structure. In:Hamilton, D.L. (Ed.), Social Cognition. Psychology Press, NewYork, pp. 115–129.

Menzel, S., 2004. Der okonomische Wert der Erhaltung vonBiodiversitat – die Herausforderung seiner empirischenErfassung zur Abschatzung internationalerTransferzahlungen. Berlin, dissertation.de.

Merton, R., 1987. Focused interviews and focus group: continuitiesand discontinuities. Public Opinion Quarterly 51, 550–566.

Mulongoy, K.J., Chape, S., 2004. Protected areas and biodiversity.UNEP-WCMC Biodiversity Series No 21. <http://www.unep-wcmc.org/resources/publications/pa_biodiv/>.

Naeem, S., Thompson, L.J., Lawler, S.P., Lawton, J.H., Woodfin,R.M., 1995. Empirical-evidence that declining species-diversitymay alter the performance of terrestrial ecosystems.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of LondonSeries B – Biological Sciences 347, 249–262.

Parsons, R., Daniel, T.C., 2002. Good looking: in defense of sceniclandscape aesthetics. Landscape and Urban Planning, 43–56.

Renn, O., 2006. Participatory processes for designingenvironmental policies. Land Use Policy 23, 34–43.

Rogan, R., O’Connor, M., Horwitz, P., 2005. Nowhere to hide;awareness and perceptions of environmental change, andtheir influence on relationship with place. Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 25, 147–158.

Ryan, G., Bernard, H.R., 2000. Data management and analysismethods. In: Denzin, N.K., Lincoln, Y. (Eds.), Handbook ofQualitative Research. Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, pp.769–803.

Schultz, P.W., 2001. The structure of environmental concern:concern for self, other people, and the biosphere. Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology, 327–339.

Skogen, K., 2001. Who’s afraid of the big, bad wolf? Young people’sresponses to the conflicts over large carnivores in EasternNorway. Rural Sociology 6, 203–226.

SNH, 2006. Making a difference for Scotland’s Species: AFramework for Action. <http://www.snh.org.uk/pdfs/publics/SFpublicconsultation.pdf>.

Spash, C., Hanley, N., 1995. Preferences, information, andbiodiversity preservation. Ecological Economics 12, 191–208.

Page 12: Understanding mental constructs of biodiversity: Implications for

282 B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E R V A T I O N 1 3 6 ( 2 0 0 7 ) 2 7 1 – 2 8 2

Stewart, D., 2006. Scottish Biodiversity List Social Criterion:Results of a survey of the Scottish population. In: ResearchFindings 26/2006. Environment Social Research, ScottishExecutive. <http://www.scotland.gov.uk/Publications/2006/03/27152321/0>.

Stoll-Kleemann, S., 2001. Barriers to nature conservation inGermany: a model explaining opposition to protected areas.Journal of Environmental Psychology 21, 369–385.

Takacs, D., 1996. The Idea of Biodiversity. John Hopkins UniversityPress, Baltimore.

UNECE, 1998. Aarhus Convention – Convention on Access toInformation, Public Participation in Decision-making and

Access to Justice in Environmental Matters. United NationsEconomic Commission for Europe. <http://www.unece.org/env/pp/>.

Van der Windt, H., Swart, J.A.A., Keulartz, J., in press. Nature andlandscape planning: exploring the dynamics of valuation, thecase of the Netherlands. Landscape and Urban Planning, inpress.

Wallington, T.J., Hobbs, R.J., Moore, S.A., 2005. Implications ofcurrent ecological thinking for biodiversity conservation: areview of the salient issues. Ecology and Society 10, 15.

Warren, C., 2002. Managing Scotland’s Environment. EdinburghUniversity Press, Edinburgh.