Webb Study

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    1/23

    Explain to your partner: teachers instructional practices and students

    dialogue in small groupsNoreen M. Webb*, Megan L. Franke, Tondra De, Angela G. Chan, Deanna

    Freund, Pat Shein and Doris K. Melkonian

    University of California, Los Angeles, USA

    (Received 12 June 2008; final version received 26 August 2008)

    Collaborative group work has great potential to promote student learning, andincreasing evidence exists about the kinds of interaction among students that arenecessary to achieve this potential. Less often studied is the role of the teacher inpromoting effective group collaboration. This article investigates the extent towhich teachers instructional practices were related to small-group dialogue infour urban elementary mathematics classrooms in the US. Using videotaped andaudiotaped recordings of whole-class and small-group discussions, we examinedthe extent to which teachers pressed students to explain their thinking during theirinterventions with small groups and during whole-class discussions, and weexplored the relationship between teachers practices and the nature and extent ofstudents explaining during collaborative group work. While teachers used avariety of instructional practices to structure and orchestrate students dialogue insmall groups, only probing students explanations to uncover details of theirthinking and problem-solving strategies exhibited a strong relationship withstudent explaining. Implications for future research, professional development,

    and teacher education are discussed.

    Keywords: classrooms; grouping; cooperative group learning

    Introduction

    There is little doubt about the potential of collaborative group work to promote

    student learning, and increasing evidence exists about the kinds of interaction among

    students that are necessary to achieve group works potential (ODonnell, 2006;

    Webb & Palincsar, 1996). Less often studied is the role of the teacher in promoting

    effective group collaboration. This article investigates the extent to which teachers

    instructional practices are related to the students dialogue when working in smallgroups. Specifically, we focus on teachers interventions with small groups and how

    their engagement with students during whole-class instruction relates to student

    explaining during collaborative group work.

    Empirical findings from group-work studies demonstrate the critical relationship

    between explaining and achievement (see Fuchs et al., 1997; Howe et al., 2007; Howe

    & Tolmie, 2003; King, 1992; Nattiv, 1994; Slavin, 1987; Veenman, Denessen, van

    den Akker, & van der Rijt, 2005; Webb, 1991). Moreover, complex explanations

    (e.g., giving reasons elaborated with further evidence) have been shown to be more

    strongly related with learning than less complex explanations (providing simple

    reasons: Chinn, ODonnell, & Jinks, 2000). Explaining to others can promote

    *Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

    Cambridge Journal of Education

    Vol. 39, No. 1, March 2009, 4970

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    2/23

    learning as the explainer has the opportunity to reorganize and clarify material, to

    recognize misconceptions, to fill in gaps in her own understanding, to internalize and

    acquire new strategies and knowledge, and to develop new perspectives and

    understanding (Bargh & Schul, 1980; King, 1992; Peterson, Janicki, & Swing, 1981;

    Rogoff, 1991; Saxe, Gearhart, Note, & Paduano, 1993; Valsiner, 1987). When

    explaining their problem-solving processes, students think about the salient features

    of the problem, which develops their problem-solving strategies as well as their

    metacognitive awareness of what they do and do not understand (Cooper, 1999).

    Given the relationship between explaining and student outcomes, what can

    teachers do to promote student explaining in collaborative groups? Research has

    found that providing instruction and practice in explanation-related behaviours has

    beneficial effects on group discussion. Effective training programs include

    instructing students in explaining their problem-solving strategies (instead of just

    giving the answer, Gillies, 2003, 2004; Swing & Peterson, 1982; Webb & Farivar,

    1994), giving conceptual rather than algorithmic explanations (Fuchs et al., 1997),

    justifying their own ideas and their challenges of each others ideas, and negotiatingalternative ideas (exploratory talk, Mercer, 1996; Mercer, Dawes, Wegerif, & Sams,

    2004; Mercer, Wegerif, & Dawes, 1999; Rojas-Drummond & Mercer, 2003; Rojas-

    Drummond, Perez, Velez, Gomez , & Mendoza, 2003; Wegerif , Linares, Rojas-

    Drummond, Mercer, & Velez, 2005), and engaging in argumentation (providing

    reasons and evidence for and against positions, challenging others with counter-

    arguments, weighing reasons and evidence, Chinn, Anderson, & Waggoner, 2001;

    Reznitskaya, Anderson, & Kuo, 2007). A number of effective programs combine

    many of these elements (see Baines, Blatchford, & Chowne, 2007; Baines,

    Blatchford, & Kutnick, 2008; Blatchford, Baines, Rubie-Davies, Bassett, &

    Chowne, 2006).Teachers can also require group members to assume particular roles or engage in

    specific practices during their group interaction, such as asking each other specific

    high-level questions about the material (Fantuzzo, Riggio, Connelly, & Dimeff,

    1989; King, 1999), asking questions to monitor each others comprehension

    (Mevarech & Kramarski, 1997), engaging in specific summarizing and listening

    activities (Hythecker, Dansereau, & Rocklin, 1988; ODonnell, 1999; Yager,

    Johnson, & Johnson, 1985), responding to specific prompts to explain why they

    believe their answers are correct or incorrect (Coleman, 1998; Palincsar, Anderson,

    & David, 1993), and generating questions and making predictions about text

    (Palincsar & Brown, 1989). Yet, training teachers to teach their students how toengage in group work, while it can be done in ways that help students participate,

    does not address the role of the teacher in intervening in this group work and

    supporting students as they engage together with the content.

    Many cooperative learning researchers and theorists advise teachers to monitor

    small group progress and to intervene when groups fail to progress or seem to be

    functioning ineffectively, when no group member can answer the question, when

    students exhibit problems communicating with each other, and when students

    dominate group work without allowing true dialogue (see Cohen, 1994; Ding, Li,

    Piccolo, & Kulm, 2007; Johnson & Johnson, 2008). Research by Tolmie et al. (2005)

    also suggests that teacher guidance may help counteract students tendencies to disagree

    in unproductive ways and enable children to explore ideas more effectively. How

    teachers should intervene with groups to facilitate productive small-group discussion is

    50 N.M. Webb et al.

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    3/23

    less clear. Cohen (1994) cautioned teachers to intervene minimally with small groups,

    arguing that students will be more likely to initiate ideas and take responsibility for their

    discussions if teachers provide little explicit content help. She recommended that

    teachers carefully listen to group discussions to make hypotheses about the groups

    difficulties before deciding on questions to ask or suggestions to make.

    The results of some studies examining the relationship between teachers

    interventions with small groups and the quality of group discussion support Cohens

    (1994) caution against direct teacher supervision. Chiu (2004) found that higher

    levels of teacher help (e.g., explaining a concept, or giving a solution tactic) reduced

    groups subsequent time-on-task and depth of their discussions (whether students

    provided new ideas and explanations) compared to lower levels of teacher help (e.g.,

    drawing attention to an aspect of the problem through asking questions instead of

    providing a solution strategy or telling groups how to proceed). Gillies (2004) and

    Dekker and Elshout-Mohr (2004) also confirmed the detrimental effect of giving

    groups direct help about the task content. In Gillies study, students in classrooms

    with teachers who engaged in communication behaviours such as asking open andtentative questions to probe, clarify, and focus student thinking provided more

    detailed explanations than did students in classrooms in which teachers provided

    direct instruction and explicit content help. In Dekker and Elshout-Mohrs study,

    students in classrooms in which teachers were instructed to provide only process help

    to groups (e.g., encouraging students to explain and justify their work) engaged in

    more extensive discussions and exhibited more equal participation among group

    members than students in classrooms in which teachers were instructed to provide

    only content help (e.g., hints about the mathematical content and strategies).

    Somewhat in contrast to the findings just described, Meloth and Deering (1999)

    reported that high-content help (e.g., providing direct instruction about content) wasnot necessarily detrimental to productive group discussions.

    An important qualification of these results may serve as a unifying thread

    throughout these studies, helping to resolve the apparently inconsistent results.

    Meloth and Deering found that high-content help facilitated productive group

    discussion when the teacher listened to the groups ideas (for example, finding out

    whether groups were focusing on irrelevant information) before providing specific

    instruction. Chiu (2004) also suggested that what was effective about the indirect

    help that teachers provided in his study was that teachers asked questions of students

    to elicit their suggestions about how to proceed. Similarly, Gillies (2004) examples of

    teachers who had received communications skills training showed teachersascertaining students ideas and strategies before offering suggestions or focusing

    the groups attention on aspects of the task.

    An intriguing possibility, then, is that what matters in terms of teacher

    interventions with small groups is not whether teachers provide help that focuses on

    the subject matter content of group work versus guidance about what collaborative

    processes groups should carry out, or whether teachers should provide more-explicit

    versus less-explicit content help. Rather, what may be important is whether teachers

    try to ascertain student thinking and base their interaction with the group on what

    they learn about students thinking on the task. The importance of teachers doing

    this finds much support in wider literatures on effective teaching practices (see

    Fennema et al., 1996; Franke, Carpenter, Levi, & Fennema, 2000; Franke, Kazemi,

    & Battey, 2007; Lampert, 1990; Mercer, 2000).

    Cambridge Journal of Education 51

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    4/23

    Kazemi and Stipek (2001) contrasted elementary school mathematics

    classrooms in which teachers pressed students to explain and justify their

    problem-solving strategies mathematically (they termed these practices high-

    press) with classrooms in which teachers asked students to describe steps they

    used to solve problems but did not ask students to link their strategies to

    mathematical reasons or to explain why they chose particular procedures (low-

    press). When working collaboratively, students in high-press classrooms used

    mathematical arguments to explain why and how their solutions worked and to

    arrive at mutual understanding, whereas students in low-press classrooms

    summarized, but did not explain, their steps for solving a problem and did

    not debate the mathematics involved in the problem. Webb et al. (in press) also

    found similar results in that the extent to which teachers asked students to

    elaborate on their explanations about how to solve mathematical problem

    showed a strong relationship with the nature and extent of students explaining

    to each other during collaborative conversations. Webb, Nemer, and Ing (2006)

    found that low-press teacher practices were a possible reason for the infrequentstudent explaining that occurred during cooperative group work. Teachers in

    that study did not ask students about their thinking but instead assumed most

    of the responsibility for setting up the steps in the problem (typically with little

    or no rationale given for the mathematics underlying the steps) and asked

    students simply to provide the results of specific calculations that the teachers

    themselves had posed.

    Extending previous research, this study examines how teachers practices both

    with small groups and with students in the larger classroom context may relate to

    student explaining in collaborative groups. Specifically, we examine how the extent

    to which teachers press students to explain their thinking during specificinterventions with small groups, as well as during whole-class discussions, relates

    to the accuracy and completeness of students explanations during collaborative

    group work. It should be noted that the analyses reported here address associations

    between teacher and student variables, not causal relationships; this issue is discussed

    at greater length in a later section of this paper.

    The specific research questions addressed in this paper, then:

    (1) What are the relationships between specific teacher interventions with small

    groups and the extent of student explaining in those groups?

    (2) What are the correspondences between teacher-to-teacher differences in

    their practices and classroom-to-classroom differences in student explainingduring collaborative group work?

    Method

    Sample

    Four elementary-school classrooms (Grades 2 and 3) in three schools in a large

    urban school district in Southern California are the focus of this study.1 These

    schools are large (more than 1100 students), serve predominantly Latino (with some

    African-American) students, have a high percentage of students receiving free or

    reduced lunch, have a substantial proportion of English language learners, and have

    low standardized achievement test scores.

    52 N.M. Webb et al.

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    5/23

    Teacher professional development

    The four teachers were part of a large-scale study focused on supporting teachers

    efforts to engage their students in algebraic thinking (see Carpenter, Franke, & Levi,

    2003; Jacobs, Franke, Carpenter, Levi, & Battey, 2007) and had participated in at

    least one year of on-site professional development that explored the development ofstudents algebraic reasoning and, in particular, how that reasoning could support

    students understanding of arithmetic. The professional development content, drawn

    from Thinking mathematically: Integrating arithmetic and algebra in the elementary

    school (Carpenter et al ., 2003), highlighted relational thinking, including:

    (a) understanding the equal sign as an indicator of a relation; (b) using number

    relations to simplify calculations; and, (c) generating, representing, and justifying

    conjectures about the fundamental properties of numerical operations. Teachers

    received guidance in how to engage their students in conversations in order to help

    them explain their thinking and debate their reasoning and how to encourage

    students to solve problems in their own ways. A primary focus was on teachers

    eliciting student explanations and supporting students to describe the details of their

    thinking (Franke, Kazemi, & Battey, 2007).

    Approximately 12 months after the conclusion of the professional development,

    we selected seven teachers for intensive observation who had shown a range of

    student achievement in the prior year. The four teachers analyzed here used small-

    group collaborative work as described below.

    Observation procedures

    In most cases, students worked in pairs, with the exception of a few larger groups

    ranging in size from three to five. Teachers assigned students seated adjacently intopairs (or groups). Because students seating proximity was not based on student

    characteristics (e.g., achievement level, gender), group composition can be

    considered random. Analysis of group composition showed a variety of groupings

    in each class (e.g., some same-gender pairs, some mixed-gender pairs), with no

    particular pattern predominating. The groups remained intact for the two occasions

    of observations analyzed here; otherwise, group membership was fluid and teachers

    changed group composition frequently.

    Each class was videotaped and audiotaped on two occasions within a one-week

    period. We recorded all teacher-student interaction during whole-class instruction,

    and recorded a sample of groups, selected at random, from each classroom. Thenumber of students recorded in each class ranged from 11 to 15 out of the 20

    students enrolled in each class. Comparison of the recorded students with the non-

    recorded students revealed no significant differences in gender, ethnicity, or

    performance on the achievement tests administered in this study.

    Observers recorded classroom activity as teachers taught problems of their choice

    related to the topics of equality and relational thinking. Teachers posed such problems

    as (a) 50+50525+%+50, and (b) 11+255+8 (true or false?). Consistent with their

    accustomed practice, teachers incorporated group-work time into the class during

    which students worked together to solve and discuss problems assigned by the teacher.

    Teachers introduced a problem, asked groups to work together to solve the problem

    and share their thinking, and then brought the whole class together for selected students

    to share their answers and strategies with the whole class, usually at the board.

    Cambridge Journal of Education 53

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    6/23

    We made comprehensive transcripts of each class session consisting of verbatim

    records of teacher and student talk, annotated to include the details of their non-

    verbal participation. These transcripts included whole-class discussion as well as

    group-work discussion for recorded students. We also collected student written

    work, took field notes during class sessions, and administered student-achievement

    measures.

    Measures of student achievement

    We used two measures of student achievement in this study. The written assessment

    was designed to measure relational thinking. Some items were designed to assess

    students understanding of the equal sign, and whether students held a relational

    view of the equal sign (Jacobs et al., 2007). For example, to answer the problem

    3+45%+5, students needed to know that the numbers to the left of the equal sign

    summed to the same result as the numbers to the right of the equal sign, rather than

    treating the equal sign as an operation such as the answer comes next. Other itemswere designed to assess students abilities to identify and use number relations to

    simplify calculations. For example, in 889+11821185%, students could simplify this

    problem by recognizing that 118211850.

    We also individually interviewed the students from each class who were audio or

    videotaped on the observation days. We asked students what number they would put

    in the box to make certain number sentences true, for example, 13+185%+19, and

    asked them to describe their problem-solving strategies. For this paper, we analyzed

    the accuracy of their answers. Internal consistency alpha coefficients for the written

    assessment and interview were .88 and .74, respectively.

    Coding of student and teacher participation

    Using transcripts of all classroom talk we coded teacher and student participation

    during whole-class and group-work discussions. To analyze whole-class discussions,

    we separated the interaction into segments, each of which consisted of one bounded

    interaction between the teacher and a particular student. A segment consisted of a

    minimum of two conversational turns each for the teacher and the student. When

    analyzing group interaction, we coded each group conversation on a problem as a

    single unit. The group conversation started when the group began discussing the

    assigned problem and ended when the teacher called the class together again.

    We coded teacher practices across the entire lesson (all whole-class segments and

    all group conversations). The four teacher practices coded were: (a) probes students

    explanations to uncover details or further thinking about their problem-solving

    strategies (asks specific questions about details in a students explanation); (b)

    engages with students around their work on the problem (either an answer or an

    initial explanation) but does not probe the details of student thinking about their

    problem-solving strategies (typically, repeats or revoices the students work without

    asking further questions); (c) interacts with the group only around norms for

    behaviour (typically, directing students to talk to, or share with, each other); and (d)

    makes other brief comment or suggestion (e.g., acknowledges work on a problem,

    repeats the problem assigned, makes a brief suggestion, or makes a comment

    concerning classroom management).

    54 N.M. Webb et al.

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    7/23

    We coded student participation along two dimensions. First was the highest level

    of student participation on a problem: (a) gives correct and complete explanation;

    (b) gives ambiguous, incomplete, or incorrect explanation; (c) gives answer only; and

    (d) gives neither an answer nor an explanation (see Table 1). An explanation was

    considered complete if the coder could unambiguously discern how the student

    solved the problem. This included evidence from both verbal and non-verbal

    communication (such as gesturing to different parts of the number sentence). A

    correct and complete explanation was any explanation that described in detail a

    strategy that would consistently work for the problem, and involved an

    unambiguous and appropriate solution to the problem. Any explanation not

    considered complete was coded as incomplete, which included both incomplete and

    ambiguous explanations. An incorrect explanation was an explanation that was

    mathematically inconsistent with the problem posed.

    The second student participation dimension reflected the extent of the group

    explaining during or after a teachers intervention compared to the group explaining

    prior to the teachers intervention, and was coded only for groups that had not givena correct/complete explanation prior to the teachers intervention. The three codes

    were: (a) the group gives more explanation than before the teachers intervention,

    and the explanation is correct and complete; (b) the group gives more explanation

    than before the teachers intervention, but it is not correct and complete; and (c) the

    group gives no further explanation.

    Seven members of the research team coded student and teacher participation. We

    developed and set standards for the application of the coding through an iterative

    process that occurred over many regular weekly meetings. Each research-team

    Table 1. Examples of student explanations.

    Explanation category Example

    Correct and complete Problem: 10+1021055+

    Five? Its cause look. We could do this, oh no. Hold up. Cause

    ten plus ten equals twenty, huh? And then it says minus ten

    equals five plus blank. So it gotta be equal ten, so five plus

    five equals ten. And thats how I got it. Get it?

    Ambiguous or incomplete Problem: 8+257+3 (True or false?)

    [True] because theres a two and a three and a seven and aneight. Theyre like an order. [While the answer is correct, this

    explanation does not make it clear (a) whether the student is

    considering the difference in quantity between 2 and 3 and

    between 7 and 8, (b) what the student means by order, and

    (c) how the student is using order to justify that the number

    sentence is true.]

    Incorrect Problem: 4+9556322 (True or false?)

    I thought it was false because four plus nine is thirteen, and five

    times three is fifteen. Those two do not match.

    Answer but no explanation Problem: 3755 + (3* 10)Let me see. 345?

    Cambridge Journal of Education 55

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    8/23

    member was involved in the development of the codes and had primary

    responsibility for coding particular problems and particular groups of students

    from each classroom. We resolved questions about coding through discussion, and

    these questions often led to further refinement of the teacher and student codes.

    After we refined the codes, research-team members systematically reviewed their

    own and others coding to ensure that the codes reflected the final coding scheme andto uncover inconsistencies in coding. All inconsistencies were brought to the entire

    team and were resolved through discussion and consensus.

    Results

    In the following sections, we provide results about teachers instructions for student

    behaviour during group work (to produce a picture of these classrooms climates for

    student participation), the relationship between student participation and achieve-

    ment, teacher interventions with small groups and the links between those

    interventions and student participation during group work, teacher practices inthe whole class, and classroom differences in students explanations and student

    achievement.

    Teachers instructions about student collaboration

    All of the teachers in this study gave frequent reminders to students about how to

    collaborate during group work. First, in the majority (70%) of the 40 problems

    assigned for group work across the four classrooms observed here, teachers gave

    preliminary reminders to the whole class about student participation in their groups.

    The most common reminders were for students to share with their neighbour and

    talk with your table partners. Some teacher instructions were more specific, such as

    the need to share why you think so or why you dont think so, What kind of thinking

    went on? How did you solve it? and Instead of just saying true or false, tell [your

    partners] why you think its true or why you think its false. Second, during about half

    (54%) of the teacher interventions with small groups, the teacher gave specific

    reminders for students to work together, to talk about the problem with each other, to

    discuss with their group how they were solving the problem, and to explain to each

    other. These results show that teachers frequently communicated expectations for

    students to share their work and, often, for students to explain their thinking behind

    their answers or to describe the procedures they used to solve the problem.

    Consistent with the expectations communicated about student behaviour duringgroup work, groups showed a high incidence of explaining. Of the 208 group work

    conversations observed across the four classrooms, groups gave explanations in 129

    (62%) of them. Thus, these classrooms had climates that seemed to be conducive to

    explaining during collaborative group work.

    Relationship between student participation during group work and student achievement

    We next examined the relationship between student participation during group work

    and their achievement to confirm that the previously established relationship

    between explaining and achievement held up in these classrooms. Consistent with

    previous research, Table 2 shows that explaining was significantly correlated with

    achievement.2 In particular, giving correct and complete explanations was positively

    56 N.M. Webb et al.

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    9/23

    related to achievement scores. That is, the greater the percentage of groupconversations during which students gave a correct and complete explanation, the

    higher were their achievement scores. Other kinds of student participation (giving

    ambiguous, incomplete, or incorrect explanations; giving only answers; giving

    neither answers nor explanations) were not related or were negatively related to

    achievement. The remaining sections, then, pay particular attention to the

    relationship between teacher practices and students giving correct and complete

    explanations.

    Nature of teacher interventions with small groupsAcross the 208 group-work conversations, teachers intervened in 81 of them (39%).3

    Of the 81 conversations with a teacher intervention, in 27% of them, groups had

    already produced a correct/complete explanation at the time the teacher intervened;

    in 16%, groups had engaged in some explaining but it was not correct or complete;

    and in 57%, groups had not produced any explanation. Of the 127 episodes of

    collaborative work without any teacher intervention, in 38%, groups produced a

    correct/complete explanation; in 19%, groups produced some explanation but it was

    not correct or complete; and in 43%, groups did not produce any explanation. The

    difference in explanation patterns for group episodes with a teacher intervention and

    those without a teacher intervention was not statistically significant (x2

    (2;N5208)53.74, p5.15), which suggests that teachers did not target groups for

    intervention based on students explaining behaviour.

    When they engaged with groups, teachers used a variety of practices, as shown in

    Table 3. In about half of the interventions (53%), teachers interacted with groups

    around explanations or other work (e.g., answers) on the mathematics problems. In

    many of these interventions, teachers probed students explanations to uncover

    additional details about their problem-solving strategies or further thinking

    underlying their strategies. In about half of the teacher interventions (47%), teachers

    interacted with groups only around norms for behaviour or management issues. The

    distributions of teacher practices were similar for groups that had already provided

    a correct/complete explanation when the teacher intervened (22 interventions)

    and groups that had not given a correct/complete explanation (59 interventions;

    Table 2. Correlations between student participation during group work and achievement

    scores.

    Highest level of student participation on a

    problemaWritten assessment

    scorebIndividual interview

    scoreb

    Gives explanation .46*** .27

    Correct and complete .61*** .46***

    Ambiguous, incomplete or incorrect 2.18 2.30*

    Gives no explanation 2.46*** 2.27*

    Answer only .08 .20

    No answer or explanation 2.41** 2.35*

    Notes: (a) Percent of group conversations in which a student displayed this behaviour. (b)

    Percent of problems correct. *p,.05 **p,.01 ***p,.001.

    Cambridge Journal of Education 57

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    10/23

    x2(3; N581)52.70, p5.44). Importantly, teachers were just as likely to probe

    students explanations in groups that had already given correct/complete explana-

    tions as in groups that had not.

    Teacher interventions and subsequent student explanations

    Table 4 shows, for each teacher practice with small groups, the explanations that

    were given after the teachers arrival, specifically whether groups gave more

    explanation while interacting with the teacher or afterwards than they had before the

    teacher intervened, and whether their group conversation led to a complete/correctexplanation. These results focus on the 59 instances of teacher interventions with

    groups that had not already given a correct/complete explanation by the start of the

    teacher intervention.4

    As shown in Table 4, when teachers probed students explanations, groups nearly

    always gave additional explanation and many of them produced a correct/complete

    explanation of how to solve the problem by the end of the groups discussion of that

    problem. The other teacher practices, in contrast, were less likely to be associated

    with additional explaining or to produce correct/complete explanations. The

    differences in effectiveness of teacher practices for group behaviour were statistically

    significant (Fishers exact test, p5.001).5

    Although probing students explanations was more likely than other teacher

    practices to be associated with groups producing correct/complete explanations, not

    Table 3. Teacher practices with small groups.

    Teacher practice All teacher

    interventions

    (81 group

    conversations)

    Groups gave correct/complete explanation

    prior to the teachers intervention

    Yes (22 groupconversations) No (59 groupconversations)

    Teacher probed students

    explanations to uncover

    details or further thinking

    about their problem-solving

    strategies

    26a 23 27

    Teacher engaged with students

    around their work on the

    problem but did not probe

    the details of student thinkingabout their problem-solving

    strategies

    27 32 25

    Teacher interacted with the

    group only around norms for

    behaviour

    33 41 31

    Teacher made other brief

    comment or suggestion

    14 5 17

    Note: (a) Percent of group conversations in which the teacher exhibited this practice.

    58 N.M. Webb et al.

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    11/23

    all instances of teacher probing showed the same results. The following examples

    contrast more-effective and less-effective instances of teacher probing. In the first

    example, the teacher responds to a students ambiguous explanation by asking a

    sequence of questions specific to what the student said. The questions direct the

    student to say more about particular aspects of her strategy. The teacher asks

    questions about the students strategy until the student has described an explicit

    connection between the two sides of the number sentence: 7+1 and the 1022. During

    this interchange, the student engages in additional explaining (lines 3, 5, 11, 13) beyond

    the initial explanation (line 1) and arrives at a correct/complete explanation (line 13).

    Problem: 7+15102%

    1. Student 1: Eight take away two is ten. Ten take away eight so you have to

    regroup. Ten take away eight is two. So you take the one to thezero and it would be zero but it would be a two. And this one isbecause seven plus one is eight. Ten take away two is eight.

    Table 4. Teacher interventions with student explaining during group worka.

    Group explaining during or after teachers intervention

    Group gave more explanation than

    before the teachers intervention

    Group did not give

    more explanationthan before the

    teachers intervention

    Teacher practiceb Additional

    explanation was

    correct/complete

    Additional

    explanation was not

    correct/complete

    Teacher probed students

    explanations to uncover

    details or further thinking

    about their problem-solving

    strategies

    63c 31 6

    Teacher engaged with students

    around their work on the

    problem but did not probe

    the details of student

    thinking about their

    problem-solving strategies

    20 20 60

    Teacher interacted with the

    group only around norms

    for behaviour

    33 11 56

    Teacher made other brief

    comment or suggestion

    0 20 80

    Notes: (a) Includes only teacher interventions in which groups did not give a correct/complete

    explanation by the time the teacher intervened (59 interventions). (b) Number of group

    conversations in each teacher practice category are 16, 15, 18, 10, respectively. (c) Percent of

    interventions in which teacher used this practice and in which group exhibited this category of

    explaining.

    Cambridge Journal of Education 59

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    12/23

    2. Teacher: What were you doing here?3. Student 1: Regrouping. Eight take away two4. Teacher: No, this is eight plus two.5. Student 1: Eight plus two is ten. You have to regroup. Ten take away eight

    is two. And cross this out so it would be zero so the answerwould be two.

    6. Teacher: What made you write eight plus two?7. Student 1: Huh?8. Teacher: Where did you get the two from?9. Student 1: Its cause10. Teacher: Were you doing this one here?11. Student 1: I did this one first and then I did this one.12. Teacher: Where did you get the eight and the two from?13. Student 1: Eight is cause seven plus one is eight and ten take away two it

    would be eight.14. Teacher: OK.

    In the second example, the teacher responds to a students ambiguous

    explanation (line 1) with a follow-up question (line 2) that seeks clarification ofwhat the student said. The teacher makes a claim in the follow-up question that

    extends what the student said and then asks the student if that is what he was

    thinking. The student responds by adding clarification to the teachers claim (line 3).

    In doing so, the student exhibits further thinking, but this interchange does not yield

    a correct/complete explanation, nor does this student or group ever produce a

    correct/complete explanation for this problem.

    Problem: a5b+b. True or false?

    1. Student 2: I think not true because an A needs to have a partner and a B too.

    2. Teacher: So you think that it has to be two As and two Bs?3. Student 2: And the B should be on the A side and the A should be on the B

    side.

    These examples highlight how probing student thinking can play out differently

    for students. In both instances students had not yet verbalized a correct/complete

    explanation before interacting with the teacher and the interactions elicited more

    student explaining. However, in the first example, the teacher used details of the

    students strategy given in the students initial explanation to drive her probing

    questions. Her specific questions allowed the student to clarify the specifics of the

    initial explanation and provide a correct/complete explanation. While the second

    example also showed teacher probing, the teacher interjected her own interpretationof student thinking into her probing questions, and seemed satisfied with students

    engaging in additional explaining, even if the explanations were not correct or

    complete.

    When teachers engaged with students around their work on the problem but did

    not probe the details of student thinking about their problem-solving strategies

    (Table 4), groups often gave no additional explanation. Teacher behaviour often

    consisted of repeating or revoicing something the students had said, although

    teachers sometimes evaluated students answers or strategies or, when groups were

    having difficulty, led them through the steps in the solution. Sometimes, teachers

    revoicing of students explanations left the mistaken impression that the groups

    work was correct. In the following example, the teacher asked groups to fill in the

    box to make the number sentence true, but the group misinterpreted the problem.

    60 N.M. Webb et al.

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    13/23

    The teacher repeated the students answer (line 4) and implied that their answer was

    correct. The group did not talk about the problem after line 5.

    Problem: 1000+A51000+50. A5%

    1. Student 3: I think its A because the other A dont have no partner.

    2. Student 4: I think its A because A equals A is A because it didnt have apartner at first.

    3. Student 3: I think A isnt, the A dont have no partner. So we should choosethe other

    4. Teacher: You think A equals A? OK.5. Student 4: You had it.

    In some group conversations, the teacher intervened only to remind students to

    talk to each other, to share, or to explain (interaction around norms for behaviour).

    Some of these groups went on to give a correct/complete explanation. In the

    following example, the students had given the correct answer (lines 1, 2) but did not

    give an explanation until the teacher reminded students to talk to each other (line 3).

    The students responded that they had been talking about it (lines 48), but they did

    provide explanations (lines 10, 11).

    Problem: 8+257+3. True or false?

    1. Student 5: Its true. Oh, I didnt see the three before.2. Student 6: Eight, nine, ten. True.3. Teacher: Talk to your neighbour about it.4. Student 6: I told her.5. Student 5: We did it.6. Student 6: I said it was true.7. Student 5: I know.

    8. Student 6: Its true.9. Student 5: Because10. Student 6: Because that equals ten and that equals ten.11. Student 5: Yeah. Eight plus two equals ten. Seven plus three equals three

    I mean, ten.

    Most groups receiving a norm-related statement from the teacher, however, did

    not provide additional explanation. In the following example, as in the example

    above, the group had provided an answer but no explanation (line 2) before the

    teacher intervened to remind students about the norms for behaviour (line 4).

    Despite their acknowledging the teachers statement (line 5), they did not do more

    than repeat the answer (line 8). Neither student gave any explanation.

    Problem: 10+10510+10. True or false?

    1. Student 7: I think its what do you think?2. Student 8: What do you think? Youre the person who writes.3. Student 7: I think its false. I mean true.4. Teacher: You guys need to do a better job today with communicating.5. Student 8: Yes.6. Teacher: Thank you.7. Student 7: You think its true or false?8. Student 8: True.

    Comparison of the groups that gave more explanation after the teachers

    reminder about norms for behaviour and groups that gave no further explanation

    did not provide any clues about the reasons for differences in groups behaviour. The

    Cambridge Journal of Education 61

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    14/23

    two sets of group episodes were similar in terms of the nature of student

    participation prior to the teachers intervention (e.g., the accuracy of their answers,

    the nature of any explanations they gave), their group work behaviour on previous

    problems, and the wording of teachers reminders about the norms for behaviour. To

    account for the effectiveness of these teacher reminders about group work

    behaviour, it may be necessary to consider other factors such as general norms for

    behaviour in a particular classroom, the nature of the problem, and the particular

    students who were working together (and their previous history of collaboration).

    Finally, as shown in Table 4, when teachers made brief comments or suggestions,

    groups seldom gave more explanation after the teachers intervention. Teachers

    brief comments or suggestions included acknowledging or evaluating student work

    (very creative, it looks good), repeating the problem assigned (Can you find your

    own numbers to fill in instead of letters?), suggesting a strategy (Can we use the box

    strategy?), or commenting on behaviour (quiet down, let him borrow your

    pencil). Typically, groups did little work after these teacher interjections.

    Differences between classrooms in teacher practices

    We next turned to investigating teacher practices at the classroom level to examine

    how teacher practices in general both during whole-class instruction and when

    intervening with small groups may have played a role in students explaining in

    small groups. Table 5 shows the percentage of whole-class problems and teacher

    interventions with small groups in which teachers probed student thinking and the

    percentage in which they did not (teacher interaction around norms and classroom

    Table 5. Teacher practices in whole-class and small-group comments.

    Teacher practice Teachera

    1 2 3 4

    Teacher probed students explanations to uncover details or

    further thinking about their problem-solving strategies

    Whole-class instruction 23b 25 92 71

    Small-group interventions 36c 25 77 50

    Teacher engaged with students around their work on theproblem but did not probe the details of student thinking

    about their problem-solving strategies

    Whole-class instruction 77 75 8 29

    Small-group instruction 64 75 23 50

    Note: (a) Number of problems with teacherstudent engagement in whole-class instruction for

    each teacher are 30, 8, 12, 7, respectively. Number of small-group interviews for each teacher

    are 14, 8, 13, 8, respectively. (b) Percent of teacher engagements with students in whole-class

    instruction in which the teacher exhibited this practice. (c) Percent of teacher interventions

    with small groups in which the teacher exhibited this practice (includes only teacher

    interventions around the math content; excludes teacher interventions around only norms forbehaviour or classroom management because those teacher practices did not occur during

    whole-class instruction.

    62 N.M. Webb et al.

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    15/23

    management issues are excluded from this table because they did not occur

    during engagements with students in the whole-class context). Not only did

    teachers differ from one another in terms of their practices (differences between

    teachers in the whole class in the proportion of segments in which they probed

    students explanations to uncover details or further thinking about their problem-

    solving strategies, Fishers exact test, p,.001; differences between teachers in

    small groups in the proportion of conversations in which they probed students

    explanations to uncover details or further thinking about their problem-solving

    strategies, Fishers exact test, p5.001), but their practices in the whole-class

    setting were strikingly similar to their practices when engaging with small groups.

    Teacher 3 showed a strong tendency to probe students explanations, whether in

    the whole class or in small groups. Teacher 4 probed student thinking a

    substantial proportion of the time. Teachers 1 and 2, in contrast, most often

    engaged with students around their mathematics work without probing student

    thinking beyond about the details of their strategies, both in the whole class and

    with small groups.

    Classroom differences in student explanations and student achievement

    Teacher differences in their practices when engaging with students were reflected

    in corresponding classroom differences in student explanations during group work

    and in student achievement. Table 6 gives the distribution of student explanations

    and achievement across classrooms. Significant differences appeared between

    classrooms on all variables (level of student explaining during group work,

    p,.001; written assessment, F(3, 74)55.28, p,.01; individual interview, F(3, 74)5

    2.82, p,.05). In Classrooms 3 and 4, the majority of small groups producedcorrect/complete explanations; in Classrooms 1 and 2, only a minority of small

    groups did so, with the percentage in Classroom 1 being quite low. Differences

    between classrooms in student achievement showed the same pattern, with

    achievement in Classrooms 3 and 4 being highest and achievement in Classrooms

    1 and 2 being lowest.

    Table 6. Student explaining during group work and achievement across classrooms.

    Classroom

    1 2 3 4

    Student explaining in small groups

    Group gave correct/complete explanation 16a 33 72 56

    Group did not give correct/complete explanation 84 67 28 44

    Student achievement

    Written assessment score 17b 30 47 45

    Individual interview score 13b 24 37 44

    Note: (a) Percent of group conservations in which group exhibited this behaviour; (b) Percent

    of problems correct.

    Cambridge Journal of Education 63

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    16/23

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    17/23

    distinguishing teachers was the extent to which they probed student thinking both

    when interacting with students during whole-class instruction, and when intervening

    with small groups. Not only did some teachers probe student thinking much more

    frequently than did other teachers, the tendency of teachers to probe (or not probe)

    student thinking was remarkably consistent between their interactions with students

    during whole-class interactions and when intervening with small groups. In the

    classrooms in which teachers often probed student thinking, groups showed the

    highest incidence of giving correct/complete explanations. In the classrooms in which

    teachers did not often probe student thinking, groups showed lower incidences of

    giving correct/complete explanations.

    This paper, then, uncovered positive associations between teacher probing of

    student thinking (during whole-class instruction and when interacting with small

    groups) and the nature and extent of explaining in small groups, especially whe-

    ther groups gave correct/complete explanations. How might we interpret these

    relationships?

    One interpretation is that teacher probing is an effective intervention strategy forpromoting explaining in small groups, especially for giving correct/complete

    explanations. Teachers questions may help students clarify ambiguous explanations,

    make explicit steps in their problem-solving procedures, justify their problem-solving

    strategies, and correct their misconceptions or incorrect strategies. Another

    interpretation is that frequent teacher probing of student thinking communicates

    the expectation that students should engage in extended explaining, especially

    continuing until they are able to give correct/complete explanations. This

    expectation then becomes a feature of the classroom climate that influences student

    participation. Wood, Cobb, and Yackel (1991) observed this process in action. By

    asking students to explain their methods for solving problems and refraining fromevaluating students answers, teachers helped create expectations and obligations for

    students to publicly display their thinking underlying how they solved mathematical

    problems.

    We must be cautious, however, about interpreting the direction of effects

    between teacher practice and student participation. While one interpretation of the

    positive association between teacher probing and group explaining is that teacher

    probing helped groups to explain further and give correct/complete explanations, it

    is also possible that (a) these groups would have given correct/complete explanations

    even in the absence of the teachers probing questions (the teachers practice was

    unrelated to group behaviour); (b) teachers chose certain groups for apply probingbehaviour with the expectation (perhaps based on previous experience or

    observations) that they would be able or likely to provide more complete or correct

    explanations (previous group behaviour influenced the teachers practice); (c) high-

    level student explaining (possibly due to higher student mathematical ability) elicited

    teacher probing (when students revealed details about their strategies and thinking,

    teachers had more information on which to base probing questions); or (d) teacher

    practices and student participation influenced each other in reciprocal fashion

    (teacher probing promoted student explaining, and student explaining enabled

    teacher probing).

    Similar caveats apply to the relationship observed here between teacher-to-

    teacher differences in teacher practices and classroom-to-classroom differences in

    group behaviour. For example, students in some classrooms may be more capable of

    Cambridge Journal of Education 65

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    18/23

    giving explanations than students in other classrooms, independent of their teachers

    practices (perhaps as a result of classroom experiences in previous years; or as a

    result of greater mathematical understanding to start with). To test these alternative

    interpretations, it would be important to observe small-group processes at the

    beginning of the school year before teacher practices have a chance to influence

    student behaviour and before norms are developed, and, optimally, examine changes

    in both teacher practices and student behaviour over time.

    A further qualification of the findings concerns the task and subject matter. The

    mathematical group-work tasks used here (often open-ended questions about

    mathematics conjectures) were conducive to student explaining and teacher probing

    of student thinking. Whether the details of teacher practices and student

    participation found to be important in this study will emerge with other kinds of

    group-work tasks and in other content domains remains to be investigated.

    The results of this study show the importance of paying attention to the details of

    teacher practices and student participation during group work. In this study, more

    significant than asking students to give explanations was teachers probing of theparticulars in student thinking; and more significant than whether students gave

    explanations was the accuracy and completeness of students explanations.

    Our approach and findings have important methodological implications for future

    research on teachers instructional practices and students learning in small groups.

    Close attention to what students say and do in relation to what a teacher says helps us

    understand the details of practice that matter for student learning. It is imperative not

    only to analyze the dialogue among students, but also to examine student participation

    in relation to teacher participation and the context of the classroom. This type of

    analysis is difficult as one cannot strip what teachers say from the context in which it

    happens or from how students engage with each other and with the teacher. Yet, thistype of analysis, in conjunction with a variety of student outcomes, can help us

    understand the ways in which teachers can support students understanding through

    dialogue that supports students in explaining their thinking.

    The findings also have important implications for teaching and professional

    development of teachers in connection with small-group work. Teachers questions

    shape what happens for students. More than simply asking students to explain their

    thinking, it is important that teachers focus on what students say in relation to

    critical ideas (here, in mathematics) and help students make the details of their

    thinking explicit. There is a great deal of knowledge and skill embedded in these

    practices, including making sense of what students are saying, drawing out thesubject-matter ideas, and supporting students in detailing that thinking. These

    principles can and should be embedded in professional development for teachers to

    help them develop their intervention practices with small groups. The findings of this

    study provide good exemplars to include in such professional development.

    Acknowledgements

    This work was supported in part by the Spencer Foundation; the National Science

    Foundation (MDR-8550236, MDR-8955346); the Academic Senate on Research, Los

    Angeles Division, University of California; and the Diversity in Mathematics EducationCenter for Learning and Teaching (DIME). Funding to DIME was provided by grant number

    ESI-0119732 from the National Science Foundation.

    66 N.M. Webb et al.

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    19/23

    We would like to thank Marsha Ing for her helpful comments on an earlier version of this

    article.

    Notes

    1. Analyses of teacher practices and student participation in three of these classrooms were

    reported in Webb et al. (in press). The current study is larger and more comprehensive than

    the previous one: it uses a larger sample of classrooms, considers all instances of

    collaborative group work in all classrooms, uses more in-depth (and finer grained) coding

    of teacher practices and student activity, and analyzes links between teacher practices and

    student activity during the same group episodes.

    2. For some classes prior achievement scores (standardized test scores from the previous

    spring) were not available. Consequently, we could not compute partial correlations to

    control for prior achievement.

    3. All recorded students except one (n550) experienced a teacher intervention.

    4. All recorded students except one (n550) were members of groups that had not already

    given a correct/complete explanation by the start of the teacher intervention.

    5. For contingency tables with small expected cell counts, we used a Fishers exact test (Fisher,

    1935).

    6. Unless otherwise indicated, all significance levels are the results of Fishers exact tests of

    contingency tables.

    Notes on contributors

    Noreen M. Webb is a Professor in the Department of Education, Graduate School of

    Education & Information Studies, University of California, Los Angeles. Her research spans

    domains in learning and instruction, especially the study of teaching and learning processesand performance of individuals and groups in mathematics and science classrooms, and

    educational and psychological measurement.

    Megan L. Franke is a Professor in the Department of Education, Graduate School of

    Education & Information Studies, University of California, Los Angeles. Her work focuses on

    understanding and supporting teacher learning through professional development,

    particularly within elementary mathematics.

    Tondra De is a doctoral student in the Department of Education, Graduate School of

    Education & Information Studies, University of California, Los Angeles.

    Angela G. Chan is a doctoral student in the Department of Education, Graduate School of

    Education & Information Studies, University of California, Los Angeles. Her researchinterests include issues of equity in the development of elementary mathematics teachers, with

    a particular focus on classroom practice and teacher identity.

    Deanna Freund is a doctoral student in the Department of Education, Graduate School of

    Education & Information Studies, University of California, Los Angeles.

    Pat Shein is a doctoral student in the Department of Education, Graduate School of

    Education & Information Studies, University of California, Los Angeles. Her interests include

    exploring ways to support English Learner students in mathematics learning.

    Doris K. Melkonian is a doctoral student in the Department of Education, Graduate

    School of Education & Information Studies, University of California, Los Angeles. Her

    interests include collaborative learning, and gender related issues in mathematics and science

    education.

    Cambridge Journal of Education 67

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    20/23

    References

    Baines, E., Blatchford, P., & Chowne, A. (2007). Improving the effectiveness of collaborative

    group work in primary schools: Effects on science attainment. British Educational

    Research Journal, 33, 663680.

    Baines, E., Blatchford, P., & Kutnick, P. with Chowne, A., Ota, C., & Berdondini, L. (2008).

    Promoting effective group work in the classroom. London: Routledge.

    Bargh, J.A., & Schul, Y. (1980). On the cognitive benefit of teaching. Journal of Educational

    Psychology, 72, 593604.

    Blatchford, P., Baines, E., Rubie-Davies, C., Bassett, P., & Chowne, A. (2006). The effect of a

    new approach to group work on pupil-pupil and teacher-pupil interactions. Journal of

    Educational Psychology, 98, 750765.

    Carpenter, T.P., Franke, M.L., & Levi, L. (2003). Thinking mathematically: Integrating

    arithmetic and algebra in elementary school. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

    Chinn, C.A., Anderson, R.C., & Waggoner, M.A. (2001). Patterns of discourse in two kinds of

    literature discussion. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 378411.

    Chinn, C.A., ODonnell, A.M., & Jinks, T.S. (2000). The structure of discourse in

    collaborative learning. The Journal of Experimental Education, 69, 7797.

    Chiu, M.M. (2004). Adapting teacher interventions to student needs during cooperative

    learning: How to improve student problem-solving and time on-task. American

    Educational Research Journal, 41, 365399.

    Cohen, E.G. (1994). Designing group work. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.

    Coleman, E.B. (1998). Using explanatory knowledge during collaborative problem solving in

    science. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 7, 387427.

    Cooper, M.A. (1999). Classroom choices from a cognitive perspective on peer learning. In A.

    M. ODonnell & A. King (Eds.), Cognitive perspectives on peer learning (pp. 215234).

    Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Dekker, R., & Elshout-Mohr, M. (2004). Teacher interventions aimed at mathematical level

    raising during collaborative learning. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 56, 3965.Ding, M., Li, X., Piccolo, D., & Kulm, G. (2007). Teacher interventions in cooperative-

    learning mathematics classes. Journal of Educational Research, 100, 162175.

    Fantuzzo, J.W., Riggio, R.E., Connelly, S., & Dimeff, L.A. (1989). Effects of reciprocal peer

    tutoring on academic achievement and psychological adjustment: A component analysis.

    Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 173177.

    Fennema, E., Carpenter, T.P., Franke, M.L., Levi, L., Jacobs, V.A., & Empson, S.B. (1996).

    A longitudinal study of learning to use childrens thinking in mathematics instruction.

    Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27, 403434.

    Fisher, R.A. (1935). The logic of inductive inference. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society,

    98, 3954.

    Franke, M.L., Carpenter, T.P., Levi, L., & Fennema, E. (2001). Capturing teachers generativegrowth: A follow-up study of professional development in mathematics. American

    Educational Research Journal, 38, 653689.

    Franke, M.L., Kazemi, E., & Battey, D. (2007). Understanding teaching and classroom practice

    in mathematics. In F.K. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching

    and learning(pp. 225256). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.

    Fuchs, L.S., Fuchs, D., Hamlett, C.L., Phillips, N.B., Karns, K., & Dutka, S. (1997).

    Enhancing students helping behavior during peer-mediated instruction with conceptual

    mathematical explanations. Elementary School Journal, 97, 223249.

    Gillies, R.M. (2003). Structuring cooperative group work in classrooms. International Journal

    of Educational Research, 39, 3549.

    Gillies, R.M. (2004). The effects of communication training on teachers and students verbalbehaviors during cooperative learning. International Journal of Educational Research, 41,

    257279.

    68 N.M. Webb et al.

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    21/23

    Gillies, R.M., & Ashman, A.F. (1998). Behavior and interactions of children in cooperative

    groups in lower and middle elementary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90,

    746757.

    Hogan, K., Nastasi, B.K., & Pressley, M. (2000). Discourse patterns and collaborative

    scientific reasoning in peer and teacher-guided discussions. Cognition and Instruction, 17,

    379432.Howe, C., Tolmie, A., Thurston, A., Topping, K., Christie, D., Livingston, K., Jessiman, E.,

    & Donaldson, C. (2007). Group work in elementary science: Towards organisational

    principles for supporting pupil learning. Learning and Instruction, 17, 549563.

    Howe, C., & Tolmie, A. (2003). Group work in primary school science: Discussion, consensus

    and guidance from experts. International Journal of Educational Research, 39, 5172.

    Hythecker, V.I., Dansereau, D.F., & Rocklin, T.R. (1988). An analysis of the processes

    influencing the structured dyadic learning environment. Educational Psychologist, 23,

    2327.

    Jacobs, V.R., Franke, M.L., Carpenter, T.P., Levi, L., & Battey, D. (2007). Professional

    development focused on childrens algebraic reasoning in elementary schools. Journal for

    Research in Mathematics Education, 38(3), 258288.Johnson, D.W., & Johnson, R.T. (2008). Social interdependence theory and cooperative

    learning: The teachers role. In R.M. Gillies, A. Ashman & J. Terwel (Eds.), The teachers

    role in implementing cooperative learning in the classroom (pp. 936). New York, NY:

    Springer.

    Kazemi, E., & Stipek, D. (2001). Promoting conceptual thinking in four upper-elementary

    mathematics classrooms. Elementary School Journal, 102, 5980.

    King, A. (1992). Facilitating elaborative learning through guided student-generated

    questioning. Educational Psychologist, 27, 111126.

    King, A. (1999). Discourse patterns for mediating peer learning. In A.M. ODonnell & A.

    King (Eds.), Cognitive perspectives on peer learning(pp. 87116). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Lampert, M. (1989). Choosing and using mathematical tools in classroom discourse. Advancesin Research on Teaching, 1, 223264.

    Meloth, M.S., & Deering, P.D. (1999). The role of the teacher in promoting cognitive processing

    during collaborative learning. In A.M. ODonnell & A. King (Eds.), Cognitive perspectives

    on peer learning (pp. 235256). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Mercer, N. (1996). The quality of talk in childrens collaborative activity in the classroom.

    Learning and Instruction, 6, 359377.

    Mercer, N. (2000). Words and minds: How we use language to think together. New York:

    Routledge.

    Mercer, N., Wegerif, R., & Dawes, L. (1999). Childrens talk and the development of

    reasoning in the classroom. British Educational Research Journal, 25, 95111.

    Mercer, N., Dawes, L., Wegerif, R., & Sams, C. (2004). Reasoning as a scientist: Ways ofhelping children to use language to learn science. British Educational Research Journal,

    30, 359377.

    Mevarech, Z.R., & Kramarski, B. (1997). IMPROVE: A multidimensional method for teahing

    mathematics in heterogeneous classrooms. American Educational Research Journal, 34,

    365394.

    Nattiv, A. (1994). Helping behaviors and math achievement gain of students using cooperative

    learning. Elementary School Journal, 94, 285297.

    ODonnell, A.M. (1999). Structuring dyadic interaction through scripted cooperation. In

    A.M. ODonnell & A. King (Eds.), Cognitive perspectives on peer learning (pp. 179196).

    Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    ODonnell, A.M. (2006). The role of peers and group learning. In P.A. Alexander & P.H.Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 781802). Mahwah, NJ:

    Erlbaum.

    Cambridge Journal of Education 69

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    22/23

  • 7/31/2019 Webb Study

    23/23