Transcript
Page 1: Peer Effects and Individual Performance: Evidence From Swimming Competitions

http://jse.sagepub.com/Journal of Sports Economics

http://jse.sagepub.com/content/early/2014/01/29/1527002514521429The online version of this article can be found at:

 DOI: 10.1177/1527002514521429

published online 30 January 2014Journal of Sports EconomicsWen-Jhan JaneCompetitions

Peer Effects and Individual Performance: Evidence From Swimming  

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of: 

EconomistsThe North American Association of Sports EconomistsThe North American Association of Sports

can be found at:Journal of Sports EconomicsAdditional services and information for    

  http://jse.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

 

http://jse.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:  

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:  

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:  

What is This? 

- Jan 30, 2014OnlineFirst Version of Record >>

at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 26, 2014jse.sagepub.comDownloaded from at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 26, 2014jse.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 2: Peer Effects and Individual Performance: Evidence From Swimming Competitions

Article

Peer Effects andIndividual Performance:Evidence FromSwimming Competitions

Wen-Jhan Jane1

AbstractThis article addresses the issue of peer effects on a swimmer’s performance. TheNational Database of Student Athletes in Taiwan from 2008 to 2010 is employed.The results show that a swimmer performs better when his or her competitors arefaster. The evidence shows that peer effects are positive. As to the relative quality ofswimmers in a competition, dispersed-quality competitors make a swimmer faster.The evidence also shows that older and taller boys swim faster. The regressions ofthe Heckman selection model support these conclusions.

KeywordsHeckman selection model, National Database of Student Athletes, peer effects onindividual performance

Introduction

There is growing literature that stresses the importance of the environment in deter-

mining the outcomes of individuals. Most of this literature is concerned with exam-

ining how peers and environmental factors affect youth behavior with regard to their

educational achievements, health, criminal involvement, work status, and other

1Department of Economics, Shih Hsin University, Taipei, Taiwan

Corresponding Author:

Wen-Jhan Jane, Department of Economics, Shih Hsin University, No. 111, Sec.1, Mujha Rd., Wunshan

District, Taipei 116, Taiwan.

Email: [email protected]

Journal of Sports Economics1-9

ª The Author(s) 2014Reprints and permission:

sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navDOI: 10.1177/1527002514521429

jse.sagepub.com

at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 26, 2014jse.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 3: Peer Effects and Individual Performance: Evidence From Swimming Competitions

economic variables. This article differs from previous studies by looking at the inter-

action of individual performance in swimming competitions.

It has long been recognized by psychologists that an individual’s performance

might be influenced by his peers. The first study to show evidence of such peer

effects was that of Triplett (1898), who noted that cyclists raced faster when they

were pitted against one another and slower when they raced only against a clock.

While Triplett’s study showed that the presence of others could facilitate perfor-

mance, others found that the presence of others inhibited performance. In particular,

Allport (1920) found that people in a group setting wrote more refutations of a logi-

cal argument, but that the quality of the work was lower than when they worked

alone. Zajonc (1965) resolved these paradoxical findings by pointing out that the

task in these experimental setups varied in a way that confounded the results. In par-

ticular, he argued that for well-learned or innate tasks, the presence of others

improves performance. For complex tasks, however, he argued that the presence

of others worsens performance.

The growing body of empirical studies on peer effects consistently find the pos-

itive impact of high-ability workers on their peers.1 Mas and Moretti (2009), for

example, showed that under hourly wages, high-ability grocery checkers increase

coworker efforts through social processes. Ichino and Maggi (2000) found that

absenteeism and episodes of misconduct are considerably more frequent in the

southern branches of the bank. They showed the impact of peers on negative produc-

tivity (absenteeism) in Italian bank workers and that an individual’s shirking level

increased with his coworkers’ average shirking level. However, their study was

based on cultural norms rather than ability. Azoulay, Zivin, and Wang (2010) found

that deaths of academic superstars lead to declines in coauthors’ publication rates.

Depken and Haglund (2011) found that team member quality improves team per-

formance, but at a decreasing rate in the National Collegiate Athletic Association

4 � 400 m men’s relay teams.

This article investigates whether positive or negative peer effects exist for swim-

mers in the National High School Athletic (NHSA) Games in Taiwan. To assess

whether swimmers experience peer effects, we estimated various empirical models

that relate an absolute and relative performance to the average quality of their com-

petitors in a race. The results suggest that as the average competitor quality

increases, that is, competitors’ average time decreases, individual performance

improves. However, as the standard deviation of the competitors’ quality increases,

that is, competitors’ standard deviation of time increases, individual performance

decreases. The former supports the positive peer effects. The latter indicates that a

disparity of quality in a competition reduces a swimmer’s performance.

The goal of this article is to employ the data of swimming competitions from

NHSA Games to investigate peer effects in a race. The remainder of this article is

organized as follows: The data and the empirical methodology are presented in the

second section. The results are discussed in the third section, and the article ends

with a summary of the main conclusions.

2 Journal of Sports Economics

at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 26, 2014jse.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 4: Peer Effects and Individual Performance: Evidence From Swimming Competitions

Empirical Methodology and Data Description

In Taiwan, the National Database of Student Athletes has accumulated approximately

1,582,000 athletes’ demographic data and performance records from 2007 to 2013,

forming a rare set of micro data that we use as the basis for this study. The database

includes all swimming, track and field, gymnastic, table tennis, badminton, tennis,

kickboxing, judo, archery, karate, and soft tennis athletes in the NHSA Games.2

For the NHSA swimming, individual races consist of freestyle, breaststroke,

backstroke, and butterfly races. Races cover 50, 100, 200, 400, 800, and 1,500 m.

The 800 m is for females and the 1,500 m is for males only. The butterfly, back-

stroke, and breaststroke races each cover 100 and 200 m. All four strokes are used

in the 200 m and 400 m individual medley events. The data cover 14 types of races

and were provided by the Ministry of Education for research purposes. Our data con-

tain extensive personal characteristics and yearly performance information on com-

petitors from 2008 to 2010. Demographic data include age, gender, height, and

weight. Competition dates and locations, as well as athlete’s schools, are also

included in the data set.

In order to consider peer effect on performance in a traditional labor market, two

challenges need to be overcome. One is the measure of individual performance and the

other is the definition of peers. This is the main reason why there are still few articles

discussing this issue. Swimming races present an ideal case where the performance of

each player, that is, the time, is easily measured in a uniform way. In addition, peers

for a player are easily defined in a game, that is, the rest of the competitors.

To test for a positive or negative peer effect, we control for both the average and

the standard deviation of participant quality in a race. The swimmer’s production

function estimated model can be specified as:

Timejt ¼ b0þ b1AvgTime�jt�1 þ b1SDTime�jt�1 þ FXit þ e; ð1Þ

where Timejt represents jth player’s seconds in a race. bs and F are estimated para-

meters, and e is an error term. The explanatory variables include the average seconds

of competitors which excluded player j in the previous race (AvgTime�jt�1); and the

standard deviation of competitors’ seconds which excluded player j in the previous

game (SDTime�jt�1).3 Control variables (Xjt) are the swimmer’s characteristics and

environmental factors. The former consists of swimmer’s height (Height), weight

(Weight), age (Age), and gender (Gender). The latter are dummies for the race types

(GameType), year dummy (Year), dummies for whether the race was a preliminary

(PRELIMS, yes ¼ 1, otherwise ¼ 0), and a final against a clock (CFINALS

yes ¼ 1, otherwise ¼ 0). Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics of the data.

The variable AvgTime�jt�1 is expected to have a positive relationship with

Timejt, if there exists a positive peer externality on a swimmer. Conversely, if the

peer externality on a swimmer’s performance is negative, the variable AvgTime�jt�1

is expected to be negative.

Jane 3

at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 26, 2014jse.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 5: Peer Effects and Individual Performance: Evidence From Swimming Competitions

The parameter of SDTimejt�1, on the other hand, could be either positive or neg-

ative since the dispersion degree of time for the competitors can result from any com-

bination of swimmers. For example, the participants in a race could consist of eight

average swimmers, or of three above-average swimmers, three below-average swim-

mers, and two average swimmers. However, when comparing two races with the same

potential (i.e., participants’ average time in a race), the sign of the parameter on

SDTimejt�1 provides evidence as to whether the average member in a race suffers

from positive peer effects, ceteris paribus. If the coefficient is positive, it means that

a dispersed human capital race induces more seconds. Therefore, a race with averagely

talented members has better individual performance. This represents that an average

human capital race suffers positive peer effects. Conversely, if the coefficient is neg-

ative, an average human capital race suffers negative peer effects.

Table 1. Descriptive Statistics of the Data.

Variable Description M SD Min. Max.

Time Player’s seconds in a game 163.8458 177.6139 24.43 1,220.95Proxies of peer effects

AvgTime Average seconds ofcompetitors

161.2749 174.0081 25.2075 1,126.244

SDTime Standard deviation ofcompetitors’ seconds

18.6576 27.3796 .3707 257.8379

AvgTimeP Average seconds ofcompetitors inpreliminaries

124.7666 75.8428 25.2475 333.0688

SDTimeP Standard deviation ofcompetitors’ seconds inpreliminaries

5.8531 4.2992 .3500 21.5017

AvgTime(t � 1)

Average seconds ofcompetitors last year

181.9861 234.1711 27.3015 1,119.079

SDTime(t � 1)

Standard deviation ofcompetitors’ seconds lastyear

139.3349 767.0914 1.40498 4,678.039

Control variablesAge Swimmer’s age 19.8242 1.8767 16 24Height (cm) Swimmer’s height 167.9784 8.4398 116 201Weight (kg) Swimmer’s weight 60.4344 9.9606 33 100Gender Swimmer’s gender (male¼

1, otherwise ¼ 0).5366 .4987 0 1

CFINALS Dummy of final raced onlyagainst a clock (yes ¼ 1,otherwise ¼ 0)

.0701 .2553 0 1

PRELIMS Dummy of preliminary(yes ¼1, otherwise ¼ 0)

.6580 .4745 0 1

Note. n ¼ 4,210.

4 Journal of Sports Economics

at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 26, 2014jse.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 6: Peer Effects and Individual Performance: Evidence From Swimming Competitions

According to the literature concerning the relationship between a swimmer’s soma-

totype and performance, the coefficients of Height and Gender are expected to be neg-

ative, the coefficient of Age is expected to be negative, and the coefficient of Weight is

insignificant.4 Cyclists raced faster when they were pitted against one another and

slower when they raced only against a clock (Triplett, 1898). The comparison base for

PRELIMS and CFINALS is the finals, so both coefficients are expected to be positive.

Empirical Results

The empirical results of Equation 1 for pooled ordinary least squares (OLS) regressions

are regressed. The w2 values of the Breusch–Pagan (B-P) test (¼9303.39) rejects the null

hypothesis of homoscedasticity. Therefore, a robust regression using iteratively

reweighted least squares is employed in the following estimations. Moreover, unob-

served individual-specific heterogeneity and sample selection biases induced by the non-

random process for competitors in a race are considered in the regressions. The Breusch

and Pagan Lagrangian multiplier test (¼622.92) rejects the null hypothesis of the absence

of an unobserved effect, and the Hausman (1978) test cannot reject the null hypothesis

that the difference in coefficients is not systematic. The random effects (RE) model is

supported. In a test of the selectivity effect, however, Mills’ ratio (l) does not support the

results in the Heckman selection model. The results are presented in Table 2.

The parameters on the measures of peer effects are of most interest in this article.

Both coefficients of AvgTimet�1 and SDTimet�1 are significant. The former is con-

sistently and positively related to a swimmer’s seconds, and the latter is consistently

and negatively related to a swimmer’s seconds in OLS, weighted least squares

(WLS), and Heckman selection regressions.

The evidence here indicates that a swimmer’s speed is influenced by the compet-

itors’ average and relative quality. The evidence of AvgTimet�1 supports a positive

peer effect on individual performance. A one-unit decrease in the average time of

the participants represents better competitors in a game and it will result in shorter

seconds (e.g., 0.31 s in the Heckman selection model with clustering race) for a

swimmer in a race. This part of the results corresponds to Depken and Haglund

(2011). Moreover, the evidence of SDTime indicates that a dispersed human capital

race induces a shorter time. A race with average quality members leads to a swim-

mer achieving longer seconds. A one-unit decrease in SDTime increases a swim-

mer’s time by 0.056 s in the Heckman selection model with clustering race,

ceteris paribus. This is in contrast to Brown’s (2011) findings that ‘‘large ability dif-

ferences in golfers is associated with lower performance.’’

As for the control variables, the coefficients of age, height, and gender are nega-

tively significant. For these students, older and taller boys induce faster times. Boys

are faster than girls. According to the RE Model, a 1-year increase in age decreases a

swimmer’s time by 1.76 s, and a 1-cm increase in height decreases a swimmer’s time

by 0.176 s, ceteris paribus.

Jane 5

at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 26, 2014jse.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 7: Peer Effects and Individual Performance: Evidence From Swimming Competitions

Tab

le2.

Tim

ean

dPee

rEffec

tsR

egre

ssio

nR

esults

(Dep

enden

tV

aria

ble

:T

ime

inSe

conds)

.

Poole

dO

LSPoole

dW

LSR

EM

odel

Hec

kman

Sele

ctio

nM

odel

Hec

kman

Sele

ctio

nM

odel

(Clu

ster

ing

Rac

e)

Var

iable

sse

conds

seco

nds

seco

nds

Seco

nd

stag

eSe

lect

Seco

nd

stag

eSe

lect

Avg

Tim

e(t�

1)

.360**

*.7

13**

*.1

26

.311**

*.3

10**

*(.048)

(.026)

(.155)

(.058)

(.059)

SDT

ime(

t�

1)

�.0

64**

*�

.128**

*�

.023

�.0

56**

*�

.056**

*(.009)

(.005)

(.028)

(.011)

(.011)

Age

�1.3

33**

*�

.760**

*�

1.7

6**

*.0

43**

*.0

43

(.146)

(.079)

(.208)

(.012)

(.031)

Hei

ght

�.1

45**

�.1

49**

*�

.176**

*�

.135**

.004

�.1

33**

*.0

04

(.047)

(.025)

(.060)

(.067)

(.004)

(.042)

(.007)

Wei

ght

�.0

30

.002

�.0

54

�.0

15

.004

�.0

11

.004

(.038)

(.021)

(.059)

(.060)

(.003)

(.048)

(.005)

Gen

der

�10.6

56**

*�

8.8

87**

*�

9.9

73**

*�

11.9

69**

*�

.22**

*�

12.1

05**

*�

.217**

(.607)

(.328)

(.825)

(1.3

58)

(.055)

(1.9

38)

(.084)

CFI

NA

LS5.6

23**

1.3

11**

*�

.991

4.0

49*

4.0

60

(1.7

06)

(.922)

(1.8

26)

(2.1

31)

(3.5

39)

PR

ELI

MS

5.4

20**

*3.1

55**

*2.1

57**

*4.5

21**

*4.5

25**

*(.585)

(.316)

(.673)

(.694)

(.739)

Gam

eTyp

eY

esY

esY

esY

esY

esY

ear

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

Const

ant

99.1

25**

64.0

44**

*133.8

47**

*74.7

75**

*�

2.2

6**

*73.3

64**

*�

2.2

61*

(7.8

37)

(4.2

35)

(13.3

32)

(14.9

83)

(0.6

1)

(8.9

05)

(1.2

68)

Obse

rvat

ions

3,0

26

3,0

26

3,0

26

4,2

10

4,2

10

Cen

sore

dobs

2,9

08

2,9

08

Num

ber

ofid

924

(Pse

udo)

R2

.995

.9948

B-P

test

(w2)

9303.3

9**

*H

ausm

ante

st11.0

4LM

test

622.9

2**

*M

ills’

ratio

(l)

�1.0

43

�.2

21

(6.5

35)

(1.0

22)

Not

e.O

LS¼

ord

inar

yle

astsq

uar

es;L

Lagr

angi

anm

ultip

lier;

RE¼

random

effe

ct;W

LS¼

wei

ghte

dle

ast

squar

es.V

alues

inpar

enth

eses

are

the

stan

dar

der

rors

.**

*Den

ote

ssi

gnifi

cance

atth

e1%

leve

l.**

Den

ote

ssi

gnifi

cance

atth

e5%

leve

l.an

d*D

enote

ssi

gnifi

cance

atth

e10%

leve

l.

6 at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 26, 2014jse.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 8: Peer Effects and Individual Performance: Evidence From Swimming Competitions

The coefficients of PRELIMS and CFINALS are positively and significantly

related to a swimmer’s performance. Compared with finals, preliminaries and finals

against a clock increase a swimmer’s race time by 4.52 and 4.05 s on average in the

estimation of the Heckman selection model. This indicates that swimmers raced

faster when they swum against one another and slower when they raced only against

a clock. The results reinforce the evidence for peer effects, and it corresponds to the

findings of Triplett (1898) and Depken and Haglund (2011).

Conclusion

While most research examining how peers affect youth behavior are concerned with

educational achievements, health, and economic variables, this article looks at the inter-

action of individual performance in swimming competitions. The results of this study

confirm that the peer effect from the average quality of competitors on a swimmer’s per-

formance in a NHSA race is positive. The peer effect from the relative quality of com-

petitors on a swimmer’s performance in an NHSA race is negative. Large differences in

the ability of swimmers are associated with higher performance. Moreover, the results

of regressions in the Heckman selection model reinforce the evidence of peer effects.

These findings suggested three important implications. First, peer performance is

an important factor for a swimmer’s performance. This means that if the swimmers

in a race are faster, and if they are racing against one another (as opposed to against a

clock), better performances will be stimulated. Understanding peer effect is the first

step toward learning how to best structure situations in which competition exists

among players of heterogeneous ability. Second, the evidence of peer effects from

the relative quality of competitors suggests a positive learning effect from the top

swimmer in a race. While it is a substantial leap to transfer the findings on swim-

mers’ race performances to children’s school behavior, our results suggest that there

may be a potential upside to introducing a learning effect into the classroom by hav-

ing a superstar pupil. Finally, physical characteristics are important. The signifi-

cance of the Height coefficients confirms and reinforces the relationship between

a swimmer’s height and performance. These estimations of marginal effects can pro-

vide valuable information for swimmers and coaches.

Acknowledgements

Jane would like to thank the editor and the anonymous referees for their helpful comments on

the manuscript. All remaining errors are my own.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, author-

ship, and/or publication of this article.

Jane 7

at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 26, 2014jse.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 9: Peer Effects and Individual Performance: Evidence From Swimming Competitions

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship,

and/or publication of this article: Jane is grateful to the National Science Council for its finan-

cial support (NSC101-2410-H-128-006-MY2).

Notes

1. To the best of our knowledge, the only exceptions are two studies using data from profes-

sional golf tournaments in which players’ compensation is tournament based. Guryan,

Kroft, and Notowidigdo (2009) find no evidence that a player’s performance is affected

by the existence of star players, while Brown (2011) documents that the presence of a

superstar (Tiger Woods) is associated with reduced performance from peers. On average,

higher skill PGA golfers first-round scores are approximately 0.2 strokes higher when

Tiger Woods participates, relative to when Woods is absent. The overall superstar effect

for tournaments is approximately 0.8 strokes.

2. The National High School Athletic Games is the largest multisport event for junior and

senior high school players in Taiwan. The Games started in 1952, under the name of

Taiwan Provincial High School Games. It is now hosted by the Ministry of Education and

the National Sports Council, Executive Yuan. The host city changes every year.

3. Both square terms of AvgTime�jt�1 and SDTime�jt�1 are also included in the regressions,

and the conclusions are similar. The results of estimation are provided by the author if

needed.

4. For discussion of the relationship between a swimmer’s somatotype and swimming perfor-

mance, see Helmuth (1980), Blanksby, Bloomfield, Ponchard, and Ackland (1986), Mei

(1989), Chollet, Pelayo, Delaplace, Tourny, and Sidney (1997), and Geladas, Nassis, and

Pavlicevic (2005).

References

Allport, F. H. (1920). The influence of the group upon association and thought. Journal of

Experimental Psychology, 3, 159–182.

Azoulay, P., Zivin, J. G., & Wang, J. (2010). Superstar extinction. Quarterly Journal of Eco-

nomics, 125, 549–589.

Blanksby, B. A., Bloomfield, J., Ponchard, M., & Ackland, T. R. (1986). The relationship

between anatomical characteristics and swimming performance in state age-group cham-

pionship competitors. Journal of Swimming Research, 2, 30–36.

Brown, J. (2011). Quitters never win: The (Adverse) incentive effects of competing with

superstars. Journal of Political Economy, 119, 982–1013.

Chollet, D., Pelayo, P., Delaplace, C., Tourny, C., & Sidney, M. (1997). Stroking character-

istic variations in the 100-M freestyle for male swimmers of differing skill. Perceptual and

Motor Skills, 85, 167–177.

Depken, C. A., & Haglund, L. E. (2011). Peer effects in team sports: Empirical evidence from

NCAA relay teams. Journal of Sports Economics, 12, 3–19.

8 Journal of Sports Economics

at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 26, 2014jse.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 10: Peer Effects and Individual Performance: Evidence From Swimming Competitions

Geladas, N. D., Nassis, G. P., & Pavlicevic, S. (2005). Somatic and physical traits affecting

sprint swimming performance in young swimmers. International Journal of Sport and

Medicine, 26, 139–144.

Guryan, J., Kroft, K., & Notowidigdo, M. J. (2009). Peer effects in the workplace: Evidence

from random groupings in professional golf tournaments. American Economic Journal:

Applied Economics, 1, 34–68.

Hausman, J. A. (1978). Specification tests in econometrics. Econometrica, 46, 1251–1271.

Helmuth, H. S. (1980). Anthropometric survey of young swimmers. Anthropologischer

Anzeiger, 38, 17–34.

Ichino, A., & Maggi, G. (2000). Work environment and individual background: Explaining

regional shirking differentials in a large Italian firm. Quarterly Journal of Economics,

115, 1057–1090.

Mas, A., & Moretti, E. (2009). Peers at work. American Economic Review, 99, 112–145.

Mei, X. (1989). The influence of anthropometric measurements and physical qualities on

short distance swimming performance. Sports Science, 9, 21–24.

Triplett, N. (1898). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competition. American

Journal of Psychology, 9, 507–533.

Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation: A solution is suggested for an old unresolved social

psychological problem. Science, 149, 269–274.

Author Biography

Wen-Jhan Jane, PhD, is an associate professor in the Department of Economics. His current

research focuses on the applied microeconometrics, especially the topics of peer effects,

superstar effects, and discrimination in professional sports.

Jane 9

at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 26, 2014jse.sagepub.comDownloaded from


Recommended