178
Knowledge Area Module 2: Principles of Human Development Student: Christine Tinner, [email protected] Student ID # A00044587 Program: Ph. D. in Education Specialization: Curriculum, Instruction, and Assessment KAM Assessor: Dr. Janice Garfield, [email protected] Faculty Mentor: Dr. Janice Garfield, [email protected]

Developing Critical Literacy Skills

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Knowledge Area Module 2:Principles of Human Development

Student: Christine Tinner, [email protected] ID # A00044587

Program: Ph. D. in EducationSpecialization: Curriculum, Instruction, and Assessment

KAM Assessor: Dr. Janice Garfield, [email protected] Mentor: Dr. Janice Garfield, [email protected]

Walden UniversityJanuary 10, 2011

ABSTRACT

BREADTH

Access to higher academia requires entrance exams

such as the Accuplacer, ACT, or SAT, and successful completion of

these tests, which are designed for native English speakers,

demands exhibition of higher order thinking skills in reading and

writing. Immigrants must acquire English academic proficiency at

a much faster rate than normally takes native speakers 12 years

to acquire. Critical literacy, or the ability to exhibit higher

order thinking skills is necessary for successful completion of

college entrance tests. Consideration of theoretical adult

language acquisition and development of critical literacy plus

the limited research in this field offers instructional insights.

Assisting millions of adults in going to college will help

improve countless lives and strengthen the economy.

DEPTH

ABSTRACT

Research involving critical literacy instruction for adult

English language learners is minimal. However, existing research

mirrors the instructional implications from Cummins, Vygotsky,

Chomsky, Marzano and Kendall, and the Declarative/Procedural

model from neuroscience. Current instructional indications from

the literature include using strategy-based instruction that

encourages metacognition and motivation. Scaffolding instruction

and including cultural preferences for learning and discussing

cultural definitions of critical thinking are indicated as

effective instructional strategies. Additional information

regarding memory, motivation, and emotion pertaining to learning

helps to create powerful instructional practices for critical

literacy.

APPLICATION

ABSTRACT

A PowerPoint presentation and the metaphorical use of the outline

of a tree to guide note taking are used in this Application to

share the insights gleaned from the theory and research related

to developing critical literacy skills. Many “experts” often seem

unapproachable, and possibly rightly so when the task of

conveying a large body of information succinctly and meaningfully

is daunting. In order to make the presentation, given at the

Colorado TESOL (Teaching English to Speakers of Other Languages)

conference in November 2010, instructional strategies were

offered first, and supporting research and theory last. Attendees

were able to walk away from the presentation empowered and ready

to investigate the supporting research and theory independently.

TABLE OF CONTENTS

BREADTH.........................................................6

Adult language acquisition and thoughts.......................6

Jim Cummins: BICS/CALPS.....................................7

Critical literacy..........................................14

Vygotsky: language and thought.............................17

Chomsky: Universal Grammar.................................25

How long does it take adults to learn a language?............29

Developing critical literacy skills..........................29

Neuroscience and second language acquisition.................30

Relationship between thought and language....................32

Evaluating Theories..........................................32

Common ideas.................................................35

Language development and intelligence........................37

Implications for teaching adult ELLs second language.........39

Implications for teaching adult Ells critical literacy.......40

ii

DEPTH..........................................................42

Annotated Bibliography.......................................64

Literature review essay......................................65

CALPS and the literature...................................65

Vygotsky and the literature................................66

Marzano and Kendall and the literature.....................66

Declarative procedural model and the literature............67

Chomsky and the literature.................................67

Gaps in research and the literature........................67

Learning and memory from psycholinguistics.................69

Memory and adult critical literacy instruction.............71

Motivation and adult literacy..............................76

The immigrant experience and environmental factors.........77

Development of an inner voice..............................81

Instructional practices for adult ELLs.....................82

Instructional practices for critical literacy..............83

APPLICATION....................................................84

iii

Developing critical literacy skills..........................84

Introduction and problem...................................85

Theory.....................................................85

Instructional strategies...................................85

Sharing and conclusion.....................................86

Discussion...................................................86

Theoretical insights.......................................88

Vygotsky: thought, language, and interdependence...........90

Chomsky: Universal Grammar.................................92

Declarative procedural model...............................93

Definitions of higher order thinking.......................94

REFERENCES.....................................................96

APPENDIX......................................................102

iv

BREADTH

EDUC 8210: THEORIES OF HUMAN DEVELOPMENT

Being an English language teacher for adults is no easy task

some days. Each classroom contains a wide variety of students

with different cultural and educational experiences. In order to

understand how to best teach adult English language students

reading and writing skills that demonstrate their ability to

think critically, it is useful to explore second language

theories and how language influences the development of thoughts.

After evaluating the language acquisition theories of Lev

Vygotsky, Jim Cummins, and Noam Chomsky to see how these theories

help us understand adult second language, we will explore how

these theories shed light on the development of higher order

thinking and critical literacy skills. Additionally, the

declarative / procedural model from neuroscience will be offered

as additional information for adult language acquisition. By

defining critical literacy skills and examining the instructional

implications of the language acquisition theories of Chomsky,

Cummins, and Vygotsky, further wisdom can be gained for teaching

critical literacy skills to adult English language learners.

Adult Language Acquisition and Thoughts

The relationship between thougths and language is complex

(Vygotsky, 1978; Chomsky, 2006). Vygotsky and Chomsky are more

concerned about the origins of thought and language than Cummins.

According to Vygotsky and Chomsky, language affects thought and

thought affects language, which has some important implications

for adult language acquisition of English as a foreign language.

Cummins’ theory provides additional insight about the specific

nature of adult language acquisition. By recognizing the role

interpersonal learning plays in language acquisition and the

previous neural pathways for language in the L1, language

instructors can offer students a rich and powerful learning

experience.

Jim Cummins: BICS/CALPS

According to Cummins, most ELL students develop basic

interpersonal communication skills (BICS) within two to three

6

years of learning English but take up to seven years longer to

learn cognitive analytical language proficiency skills (CALPS)

(1984). The BICS/CALPS theory explains the problem many adult ELL

students have who experience trouble with critical literacy.

These students can have a normal conversation and express

themselves quite well, but still struggle with complex reading

and writing tasks. Research shows that it takes 7 to 10 years for

ELL students to reach grade-level-equivalent performance (1984).

Sometimes students never, or minimally, develop cognitive

analytical language proficiency skills (CALPS). Using higher

order thinking skills to express themselves orally, critically

examine reading, or produce writing that expresses higher order

thinking skills, never quite seems to happen.

Development of BICS/CALPS

Cummins, a strong proponent of bilingualism, stresses that

second language acquisition for children occurs best in context-

embedded language programs, where interaction with the new

language is stressed, rather than in direct language instruction

7

(1984). Citing the success of Canadian children in such context-

embedded French immersion programs, plus the remarkable gains of

learning disabled children in the same programs versus

traditional schools, Cummins makes a case for contextualizing

language acquisition learning (1984). Additionally, Cummins notes

the importance of a “supportive affective environment.” Because

young children naturally absorb language through their

environment without direct instruction, drills, and quizzes, do

adults also learn through contextualized absorption? How do

adults learn a second language?

Delving further into the nature of language acquisition,

Cummins references the “input” hypothesis which states that

exposure and interaction with second language learning needs to

be comprehensible, interesting or relevant, not “grammatically

sequenced,” and just enough but not too much (1984, p. 231).

Cummins states:

Underlying the principle of comprehensible input is the obvious fact that a central function of language use is meaningful communication; when this central function of language is ignored in classroom instruction, learning is

8

more likely to be by rote and supported only by extrinsic motivation. (1984, p. 231)

The importance of meaningful language instruction is additionally

clarified when Cummins references additional language acquisition

theories, such as “schema” theory, which states the importance of

including students’ background knowledge, and “interactionsist”

theory, which contends the interaction students have with the

target language is the major variable in second language

acquisition (1984).

Cummins makes the case then that language acquisition for

children is most successful when it is meaningful,

contextualized, and allows the student to interact with the

learning process rather than learn language passively. BICS/CALPS

theory evolved out of Cummins’ search to understand why some

English language learner (ELL) students were struggling and which

type of bilingual programs were most successful in helping

students with this struggle (1984). Drawing upon research from

Europe, Africa, and North America, Cummins shows how, in learning

additional languages, students learn BICS first and CALPS later

9

(1984). Language proficiency, according to Cummins, requires both

BICS and CALPS.

In addition to offering pedagogical suggestions based on

second language acquisition (SLA) theory, Cummins spent

considerable time researching the question, what is language

proficiency? Generally it has been noted that immigrant students

acquire conversational fluency within about two years after

moving through interaction with peers, the community, family, and

media (Cummins, 1984). Often students with this basic level of

communication were moved from English language instructional or

bilingual programs into regular classrooms. When these same

students failed academically, their IQ was questioned. Also tests

for English fluency varied dramatically.

Of the 46 tests examined by DeAvila & Duncan (1978), only four included a measure of phoneme production, 43 claimed tomeasure various levels of lexical ability, 34 included itemsassessing oral syntax comprehension and nine attempted to assess pragmatic aspects of language. (Cummins, 1984)

When it was obvious that significant theoretical and empirical

discrepancies existed, Cummins reexamined the concept of language

10

proficiency. Considering the four areas of language learning—

reading, writing, speaking, and listening—the literacy skills

require cognitive or academic instruction, while speaking and

listening do not (Cummins, 1984). Given the fact that most

immigrant students acquired conversational language proficiency

within two years, Cummins proposed the BICS/CALPS theory.

BICS and CALPS theory is comprable in some ways to Bloom’s

Taxonomy. BICS are learned the traditional way of teaching

language focusing on pronunciation, vocabulary, and grammar. BICS

correspond with Bloom’s levels of knowledge, comprehension, and

application. CALPS require that students develop a deeper

semantic and functional meaning, and correspond with the skills

of analysis, synthesis, and evaluation (Cummins, 1984).

The main reason for BICS and CALPS, according to Cummins, is

the difference between contextualized and non-contextualized

learning. Cummins’ emphasis on contextualized learning comes from

the seemingly easier and faster experience of learning BICS.

There are far more context clues with eyes, facial expressions,

11

body language, and tone of voice with interpersonal communication

skills. According to Cummins, most classroom learning is de-

contextualized. Tasks are broken down and taken out of normal

context to examine and practice.

Furthermore, to develop CALPS, Cummins proposes there is a

transfer principle from a native language (L1) to a second

language (L2). Language learning has been compared to an iceberg,

what you see on the surface is just a small piece of actual

language proficiency (Cummins, 1984). According to Cummins,

“developing a deeper conceptual and linguistic proficiency…is

strongly related to the development of English literacy and

general academic skills” (1984); hence, Cummins’ orientation

towards contextualized bilingualism.

In understanding adult language learning experience with

BICS and CALPS, the transfer principle from L1 to L2 allows

teachers to gauge roughly the extent of possible CALPS

development. Naturally, all adult ELLs have learned BICS in their

L1 and may have started or already learned BICS in English.

12

Following Cummins theory, the more time students have spent

developing L1literacy, the more likely they are to have developed

strong CALPS. Transferring the concepts central to CALPS or

critical literacy skills will be easier for these students than

it will be for students who have not developed CALPS in their L1

because CALPS are critical literacy skills. In order for students

to use any higher order thinking skills academically, such as

comparing and contrasting, evaluating, or hypothesizing, students

must have words and thoughts for these constructs. Cognitive

analytical language proficiency allows students to go beyond the

face value of words to question meaning, make inferences, and

develop nonliteral connections.

Adult ELLs present a continuum of literacy skills in their

L1 and in English, as well as exhibit a range of speaking and

listening skills in English. Many students enter English as a

Second Language (ESL) classes with little or no education. Some

have limited literacy skills in their first language, L1, or are

illiterate. These students may or may not speak English. The

13

following chart demonstrates the range of abilities students

exhibit in the adult ELL classroom.

Table 1 - BICS Proficiency Possibilities for Adult ELLs __________________________________________________________________________

ProficiencyLevel

Fluent– BICSL1

Fluent-

BICSL2

SomeBICS/L1

SomeBICSL2

DVLP.BICSL1

No BICsL2

Listening-

Speaking

Almostall

adultstudents arefluentin

theirL1

SomestudentswhohavelivedintheU.S.a

longtime

Students whomoveherewhentheyare

youngand

speakpidgin

Students withlimited

/inadequate

Englishtrainin

g

Children

NoEnglishexposure or

instruction

CALPSL1

CALPSL1 ?

SomeCALPS

SomeCALPS

NoCALPS

NoCALPS

Reading-Writing

Someadultstudent

s,especia

llythosewhohave

graduated fromhigh

InU.S.school

system,

studied

English incollege in

Bilingual

education,

absorptionfromhome,

limitedschooli

ng

Students withlimited/inadequate

Englishinstruction

Noliterac

yinstructionand/orlearnin

gdisability

NoEnglishinstruction

and/orlearnin

gdisabil

ity

14

schoolor

collegein

theirL1

theircountry

L1 = native language, L2= English

Student scenarios:

Pablo: Fluent in L1 speaking, Some BICS in English, and No BICS

with literacy in L1 or L2

Delihla: Fluent in L1 and L2, Fluent in L1 literacy, Some BICS in

L2 literacy

Nanci: Fluent in L1, high L2 skills, struggles with reading in L1

and L2

Teachers must work hard to accommodate adult ELLs who enter the

classroom with BICS in their L1 and a wide variation of literacy

skills in L1 and in English.

Adult ELL students with CALPS demonstrate higher order

comprehension and problem-solving skills with literacy. They make

inferences, identify fact and opinion, identify the main idea,

recognize voice, can compare and contrast information, and

15

perform other higher order thinking skills with reading and

writing. They are able to instantly recognize critical thinking

concepts when they are given an explanation of one, for example

comparing and contrasting, because they know how to compare and

contrast in L1.

The ways that students gain CALPS mirror how students fail

to gain CALPS. Good instruction, affective orientation, years of

education, and environmental factors can influence the

development of CALPS (Cummins, 1984). Students with explicit

instruction in higher order thinking skills as applied to

literacy have an advantage in learning CALPS over students with

no instruction. Certainly, modeling and contextualized embedded

learning techniques could also engage students to learn CALPS.

Affective orientation can greatly affect language acquisition

(Krashen, 2004). Students with families, friends, or communities

that support and encourage their education are more likely to

succeed (Grant-Vallone, Kelly, Umali, and Pohlert, 2003).

Environmental influences also can affect students’ affective

16

orientation (McFarlin, 2007; Sparks, 2007). Students who attend a

bilingual school and who can express their L1 culture and

language have far less stress than students who enter an English

immersion program and possibly experience prejudice or teasing

from other students (Cummins, 1986). Students with more education

have more practice with reading and writing and are more likely

to have CALPS and stronger CALPS if they have been to college

(Cummins, 1984). There may be other everyday ways students could

learn higher order thinking skills in life, debating politics,

fixing cars, or even figuring out how to efficiently mow 12 lawns

a day. However, school and direct instruction are necessary for

literacy and CALP development.

CALPS will not develop with limited instruction and exposure

to L2. Students usually will not or cannot develop CALPS if they

have not had adequate instruction (Cummins, 1984). Students who

struggle with English in school and never have the vocabulary to

advance to grade level in L2 simply can’t learn CALPS because of

lack of vocabulary and basic understanding of grammar (Cummins,

17

1984). They simply don’t have the language tools to navigate

English at a higher level. Many Latino immigrants come to the

United States with a sixth-grade education or less (Colorado,

2010) and only develop literacy skills at a very basic level in

their L1. Students with less education need English instruction

and additional educational instruction to make up for their lack

of a education from 7th through 12th grade.

If their experience learning English has been embarrassing,

frustrating, and confusing, adult ELLs may have some prohibitive

beliefs that block them from progressing with the language

(Krashen, 2004). Affective orientation can be instrumental in

failure to develop CALPS (2004). If students experience

discrimination in their new country, as most immigrants do to

some degree, and if their family struggles, then students may

struggle with their motivation to learn English. They may speak

in their L1 at home and at work and whenever possible. When

students have had stress with immigrating, or maybe if their

parents don’t read, they are likely to have some negative beliefs

18

about learning English that block them from both BICS and CALPS

(Roehssing, 2008).

Finally, physical, developmental, and learning disabilities

must be considered when adult ELLs appear to lack CALP skills.

Sometimes students need corrective lenses and can’t afford them,

or they may have hearing disorders that prevent them from hearing

certain sounds, developing phonemic awareness with written text,

and learning understandable pronunciation in English. Learning

disabilities manifest in attention problems, self-management

problems, difficulties with social interaction, memory

dysfunction, and processing problems with input and output. It is

possible that students who dropped out of school at a young age

were challenged by learning disabilities and need additional

strategies to learn English (Wamba, 2007).

Critical Literacy

Students who enroll in adult English courses and want to go

to college need to learn critical literacy skills (Marzano &

Kendall, 2007). The words critical literacy combine the idea of

19

critical thinking, or higher order thinking, and literacy.

Critical literacy skills are the skills needed to demonstrate

higher order thinking through reading and writing. Historically,

higher order thinking skills have been explained as reason and

logic by the Greeks (Paul, Elder, and Bartlett, 2010), and

enlightenment and self-mastery by Asian philosophers such as

Confucius (Huang & Brown, 2009). Benjamin Bloom, proposed a

hierarchy model of higher order thinking skills, and hence the

moniker “higher order” thinking skills, made popular with Bloom’s

Taxonomy (Marzano & Kendall, 2007). More recently, Marzano and

Kendall have expanded on Bloom’s theory with the New Taxonomy of

learning, which includes aspects from research in education,

psychology, and neuroscience. Critical literacy incorporates the

key aspects of these philosophies and theories as applied to

reading and writing.

In order to gain admission to college-level courses,

students need to demonstrate critical literacy skills on

standardized tests. Students need to show that they can know,

20

understand, apply, analyze, synthesize, and evaluate information

(College Board, 2010). Newer teaching techniques require that

students write self-reflectively and meta-cognitively (Alverman,

Phelps, & Ridgeway, 2007).

Building on the work of Bloom and of Anderson, which

proposed factual, conceptual, procedural, and metacognitive

aspects of learning, in the new taxonomy, Marzano and Kendall

added the self-system (2007). The self-system, according to

Marzano and Kendall, “contains a network of interrelated beliefs

and goals.” By adding the self-system, Marzano and Kendall

acknowledge the body of research that underscores the importance

student’s affective orientation towards learning (Krashen, 2004;

Alverman, Phelps, & Ridgeway, 2007).

The following table illustrates Marzano & Kendall’s new

taxonomy using the example of flying an airplane. The

illustration suggests possible experiences a pilot must have with

the levels of processing and the domains of knowledge.

Table 2 - New Taxonomy: Levels of Processing and Domains of Knowledgewith Example

21

______________________________________________________________________________Information Mental

ProceduresPsychomotorProcedures

6: Self-system Identify beliefs and emotions about landing and taking off

Determine underlying reasons for beliefs and emotions

Access successful emotional states with breathing priorto take off andlanding

5: Meta-cognitive system

Determine accuracy of knowledge of lift and drag

Establish goalsfor more learning about lift and drag

Monitor expertise with landing and take- off procedures

4: Knowledge Utilization (Cognitive)

Apply lift and drag to hypotheses

Solve problems with excessive lift or drag challenges

Apply landing and take-off experience withthe space shuttle

3: Analysis (Cognitive)

Compare and contrast lift and drag

Identify errorswith various lift/drag principles

Use best landing and take-off procedures in ablizzard

2: Comprehension (Cognitive)

Symbolize lift and drag with math equations

Use math to calculate lift and drag

Integrate landing and take off procedures on multiple aircraft

22

1: Retrieval (Cognitive)

Recognize lift and drag

Recall how to scan instrumentpanel

Execute landingand taking off

In the new taxonomy, all six levels of processing involve all

three domains of knowledge, illustrating that developing critical

literacy skills can be a very complex process. Educators need

more and more time, even in English language courses, to focus on

implementing learning theory such as the new taxonomy in an

attempt to bolster the American education system and be more

competitive internationally (Marzano & Kendall, 2007). Later on

in this Breadth component, the new taxonomy will be correlated

with the declarative procedural model for second language

acquisition, and implied teaching strategies for critical

literacy will be explored.

The Accuplacer, Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT), American

College Testing, ACT are standard college entrance exams that

require critical literacy (Collge Board, 2010; ACT, 2010). The

Accuplacer and the SAT require students to make inferences,

23

analyze text, synthesize information, and utilize critical

literacy skills (College Board, 2011). Similarly, the ACT

requires students to use critical literacy strategies for math,

science, and social studies in addition to reading and writing

(ACT, 2011). Whichever college entrance exam students take, they

must have and apply critical literacy skills.

The Test of English as a Foreign Language (TOEFL) exam is

the English language college entrance exam for students who speak

English as a foreign language. The TOEFL exam requires students

to summarize, paraphrase, give supported opinions, and compare

and contrast (ETS, 2010). On the TOEFL test, students must

demonstrate critical literacy through reading, writing, listening

and speaking. It is a demanding test that requires students to

combine multiple modalities, such as listening and reading, then

writing. The International English Language Test (IELTS) is

similarly demanding and designed as an English language college

entrance exam (IELTS, 2010). Whether ELL students graduate from

high school in the United States or from another country, they

24

need to pass college entrance exams with rigorous academic

vocabulary and demonstrate critical literacy the same as native

U.S. students.

Critical literacy skills are necessary to navigate new

trends in education, gain entrance to college, process

information quickly, problem-solve, and have better access to

jobs. Many argue that because of the Internet we don’t have to

memorize so many facts these days; it is more important that we

know how to evaluate information for validity and apply it wisely

(Leu, Leu, & Coiro, 2004). The current economy also requires

workers to be more skilled with problem-solving skills and

technological skills (Le and Kazis, 2009). Entry level jobs—such

as being a secretary or mechanic-- are becoming ever more

computerized, requiring more skills than before. In order to

truly be effective students or employees, adult ELL students need

critical literacy skills.

Vygotsky: Language and Thought

25

Is language necessary for thought, or is thought necessary

for language?

Lev Vygotsky, a psychologist, did not find the Behaviorist model

for language acquisition acceptable and instead argued for a more

Gestalt perspective (Vygotsky, 1986, p.4). Vygotsky pointed out

that thoughts and language are not separately evolving functions.

Language from a mother or caregiver is needed to begin inner

language or speech, i.e. first thoughts. Without the stimulus of

language from a mother or caregiver, would children develop inner

speech?

The implications for adult language acquisition are rather

obvious; most adults have already acquired semantic concepts in

their first language. Adults need to translate or link words that

mean the same concept together. This is sometimes called “re-

contextualizing” (Aukerman, 2010). Also, critical thinking skills

or critical literacy are L2 concepts that require teaching L2

vocabulary (Cummins, 1984) specifically, vocabulary relating to

thinking concepts such as compare, contrast, evaluate, summarize,

26

synthesize, create, and hypothesize, etc. If adult ELLs have

learned these concepts before they will have an easier time

acquiring the word in the second language. If they haven’t yet

learned the concept of compare, learning the concept and the

vocabulary will take longer.

Once in my class after asking comparison questions, an 18-

year-old Latina woman who emigrated to the U.S. as a child and

spoke primarily Spanish said, “I never ask those questions when

I’m reading.” She didn’t know that reading and thinking went

together. She thought that you just read and understood the

words. She was starting to develop CALPS.

Since it takes 7-10 years to develop academic language

proficiency (Cummins, 1984), teaching specific skills related to

critical literacy is one instructional strategy to deal with time

restraints. Adults may catch up more quickly than K-12 students,

because adults are more likely motivated to get a job, support

their family, and the prestige of doing well in school. Also,

27

motivation plays an important role in language acquisition

(Krashen, 2004).

Some other ways that teachers can help adult students

develop metacognition and independent learning skills are asking

various types of questions, navigating the Internet, interviewing

native speakers, problem-solving, using graphic organizers,

outlining, note-taking, and summarizing and paraphrasing

(Alverman, Phelps, & Ridgeway, 2007). Question-answer

relationships (QAR) technique helps students develop questioning

strategies and analytical skills for reading (2007). Teachers ask

factual “what” questions and “why” questions about comprehension.

QAR strategy also encourages questioning implicit information

about the author’s intentions and applying personal background

knowledge to the main idea (2007). Teaching students to ask

questions from different points of view will help them develop

their inner voice and critical literacy skills. Similarly, using

the Internet with webquests can also be a very beneficial way to

develop inner dialogue (Zhang, T., Tianguagng, Gao, & Gailring,

28

Zhang W., 2007). In particular, when students are asked to take

on roles of various professionals in a webquest, dialogue between

students helps them recognize different points of view and

develop more awareness and metacognition.

Teaching students to ask questions develop internal

dialogue, to develop metacognition, during reading is an

important piece to developing critical literacy. According to

Vygotsky, adults are not as spontaneous as children with learning

(Vygotsky, 1978). Relating the differences in children and adults

to a stimulus-response test, Vygotsky noted that children will

focus on the task at hand regardless of how complicated the task

is made, i.e. pushing the green button. Whereas, the more complex

the same task becomes,

…adults often refused even to attempt to deal with the problem, objecting that they could not remember what to do. Even after the session started they kept repeating the instructions to themselves, asked about aspects of the test they had forgotten, and generally sought to master the entire systems of relations as a whole before they settled down to the task as it is usually conceived. (Vygotsky, 1978)

29

Adults, having more thoughts to occupy their working memory

(Sanz, 2005), may need more explicit instruction and more goal-

oriented explanations. Therefore, it is important for adults to

realize that reading for facts requires basic decoding skills,

but reading for deeper semantic and inferred information requires

the reader to ask questions and dialogue about the facts using

their internal voice (Tovani, 2000).

Development of Concept Thinking

According to Vygotsky, meaning is the basic unit of the

“union of word and thought,” or inner speech (Vygotsky, 1986).

Meaning requires thought to make sense of language. “A word

without meaning is an empty sound” (Vygotsky, 1986). Even though

Vygotsky hypothesizes that thought and language have different

genetic roots, at a certain point in time “whereupon thought

becomes verbal, and speech rational” (1986) language and thought

are interdependent. Vygotsky sees the development of language

concomitant with the development of thought as primarily a social

process (Vygotsky, 1986).

30

For the adult ELL student, thought and language have already

developed to a large degree. The implications from Vygotsky’s

reasoning are that adults probably have developed problem-solving

skills, analytical skills, and other critical thinking skills.

Learning critical literacy skills is a matter of helping these

students apply their pre-existing critical thinking skills to the

activities of reading and writing.

Vygotsky asserts young children first make associations with

language but then use these associations as tools to take care of

their needs. “The cognitive and communicative functions of

language then become the basis of a new and superior form of

activity, in children distinguishing them from animals”

(Vygotsky, p.28-29, 1978). Egocentric thought for Vygotsky is

evidence that children are not really thinking critically just

using language to handle basic needs. Vygotsky finds the

practical use of language, specifically the “union of speech and

action” in children notable because adults separate these

processes (Vygotsky, 1978).

31

There is an interesting challenge adult ELL teachers face,

given that a basic function of language is communication and

using language to acquire basic needs. Indeed, the first levels

and vocabulary of any language are usually about food,

restaurants, transportation, medicine, shopping, and using the

telephone (CASAS, 2011). At higher levels of language learning

when critical literacy instruction is possible, it may be a

challenge to adult students to learn an additional language

because if they can handle their basic needs with rudimentary

English and if they speak their native language at home, they may

not feel an instinctive need to really express themselves

intellectually through reading and writing.

Also, maybe adult ELL students actually understand critical

literacy concepts when the information is inputted through

reading and listening. However, they may not be comfortable with

outputting information through writing and speaking. According to

Vygotsky, “visual perception is integral” since one sees an

entire visual field, however “speech is essentially analytical”

32

because it is a sequential process (Vygotsky, 1978). So there may

be concept recognition, but an inability or lack of practice or

comfort with expressing concepts.

Vygotsky concedes that children can think conceptually.

Though he doesn’t give ages for his stages of thought

development, he defines thinking conceptually as the point where

an association is taken apart, analyzed and synthesized, then put

back together. Offering a quote from Goethe, “Synthesis and

analysis presuppose each other as inhalation presupposes

exhalation” (1986). Vygotsky shows that children progress from

using language as a tool socially to get their needs met to using

language internally to solve problems (Vygotsky, 1986). Learning

to think conceptually and turn complex thought into conceptual

thought requires critical thinking skills (Marzano & Kendall,

2007).

In the next stage of concept development, Vygotsky writes

that teenagers don’t always make logical conclusions from

information but think as a “movement” from whole to part and back

33

again (Vygotsky, 1986). Insightfully, Vygotsky points out that

teenagers and even adults with very advanced concepts can apply

the learned concepts concretely and perfectly. However, when they

try to speak about these concepts they minimize ideas, have

trouble putting concepts into words, and make concepts more

simplistic (Vygotsky, 1986). This observation that teens and

adults initially understand complex concepts but tend to simplify

them through speech foreshadows Vygotsky’s other major theory,

the Zone of Proximal Development.

Implications for adult ELL students learning critical

literacy are firstly to learn how to learn a concept. If adult

ELL students haven’t had much formal education, or struggled in

school with little language proficiency, they may have never

learned how to break apart ideas and put them back together.

Teaching critical thinking concepts such as analysis, synthesis,

evaluation, and application may need to be done explicitly with

words and writing. Students may have a developed these critical

thinking concepts already, but they might need practice and

34

instruction to put the concepts into words and communicate

concepts.

Zone of Proximal Development

Vygotsky defines the Zone of Proximal Development, ZPD,

theory as,

the distance between actual developmental level as determined by independent problem solving and the level of potential development as determined through problem solving under adult guidance or in collaboration with more capable peers. (1978)

ZPD theory states that learning evolves into mastery in stages,

but learning doesn’t require mastery to occur. For example,

Vygotsky observed that teens and adults are able to concretely

apply concepts, but simplify these concepts with speech. This

would be an example of the development of concept mastery.

Students absorb the information about a concept and can apply it

correctly, but they haven’t practiced speaking about the concept

and thus have not developed speaking mastery about the concept.

Also, learning requires human interaction. ZPD theory

applies to all ages of learners. For teachers, ZPD theory says

35

that students are learning even if they aren’t showing signs of

mastery. So it is important to challenge students beyond their

stage of learning mastery, but also give students time and

practice to develop concept mastery. According to Vygotsky, “good

learning is that which is in advance of development.” (1978),

which may be why most people say they learn the most from their

hardest teachers.

Implications of ZPD theory for adult ELLs include exposure

to language, and social interaction. Logically, it follows that

adult ELLs should interact with L2 native language speakers as

much as possible. Students should volunteer, work, join clubs, or

take enrichment classes. In one study, ELL students who had jobs

showed more L2 gains than students without jobs (Drewring,

Murray, &Thomson, 2007). Language is a primary tool of connection

and it only makes sense that interacting with people and

challenging existing language development levels helps adults

learn a second language.

Non-speech, thought communication

36

Finally, Vygotsky offers one final point for adult ELLs

learning critical literacy. Thoroughly investigating the whole

spectrum of thought and language, Vygotsky shows how sometimes

when people are thinking the same thoughts little to no speech is

needed. Quoting Tolstoy (Appendix A) he shows how when Tolstoy

used only the first letter of words in the sentence, for example

“Way: icb, dymton…When you answered: it cannot be, did you mean

then or never?” to communicate with his future bride. She

understood what he meant and they had a communication of these

abbreviations where Tolstoy asked his wife to marry him. Mind

reading such as the Tolstoy example is in essence making

inferences, a desirable critical literacy skill.

Mind reading, assuming to know another’s thoughts, is a

common occurrence in communication and can enhance or inhibit

communication (Papafragou, 2002). Frequently, people who know

each other finish each other’s sentences. For advanced adult ELL

students who use this mind-reading strategy in their L1, they may

try to mind-read as their L2 language becomes more advanced which

37

puts them at risk for possible miscommunication. It may be very

important to remind adult ELL students to repeat or paraphrase

what other people have said to make sure they understand

(Papafragou, 2002). Mind reading may be a concept adult ELL

students know that can help them learn a concept such as making

inferences in reading and writing.

Vygotsky’s observations about thought, language, and

learning show that people are an essential component of learning

language (1978). Thought and language merge into meaning. Meaning

helps concepts develop when people learn to take complex ideas

and break them into parts analyzing and synthesizing them.

Finally, the Zone of Proximal Development theory says that

failure to master learning does not mean that learning isn’t

happening. Students are always learning more than they can

readily exhibit given sufficient challenges (1978). Mirroring

some of Vygotsky’s assumptions about natural critical thinking

skills, Chomsky provides more detail about the nature of language

and thought development.

38

Chomsky: Universal Grammar

Noam Chomsky is arguably one of the most prolific modern

linguistic theorists. Even though most of his writings are

philosophical in nature and explore the very nature of language

rather than the development of language and its acquisition, some

of Chomsky’s insights provide guidance for teaching adult ELL

students critical literacy skills. In particular, Chomsky’s

universal grammar, UG, theory helps to explain CALPS; why

students develop BICS before CALPS; and why students might have

trouble developing CALPS.

Chomsky attributes his theory of universal grammar to the

philosophical grammar discussed in the Port-Royal Grammar of 1660

(Chomsky, 2006). In the Port-Royal Grammar languages are described

as having a surface grammar and a deeper semantic grammar,

referred to as philosophical grammar (2006). According to

Chomsky, this concept is readily transferrable to all languages.

Each language can be regarded as a particular relation between sound and meaning…the grammar of a language must contain a system of rules that characterizes deep and surface structures and the transformational relationship

39

between them, and – if it is to accommodate the creative aspect of language use – that does so over an infinite domain of paired and deep structures. (Chomsky, 2006)

In English as with most languages there are many phonetic sounds

associated with the same letters. The word “egg” is pronounced

differently if you are British, American, South African,

Australian, or from New Zealand.

Sometimes, the same syntactic structures have multiple

meanings (2006), which could very well be the reason for the

existence of questions. For example, Chomsky provides the

following: “I disapprove of John’s drinking” could mean the

disapproval of John’s drinking tonight, or excessively, or of

John’s drinking beer (2006). “I know a taller man than Bill,”

could mean, I know a taller man than Bill knows, or than Bill is.

Furthermore, the sentences:

He thinks Bill is a nice guy. The woman he married thinks Bill is a nice guy.Bill thinks he is a nice guy.The woman Bill married thinks he is a nice guy. (Chomsky,

2000)

40

illustrate how syntactic combinations of words provide very

different semantic meaning.

It is not enough to be able to decode the surface meaning of

Bill, nice guy, married, woman, and think. There is a deeper

semantic meaning or grammar in these sentences, also a key

component of language that Chomsky asserts naturally exists.

The implication of universal grammar for adult ELL students

is that they already have deeper semantic knowledge in their L1.

Adult ELL students have a finely developed sense of deep grammar

and their language’s particular grammar. Developing CALPS may not

be as much related to a deficit of stimulus as much as a deficit

of literacy skills (Cummins,1984). Chomsky readily points out

that the poverty of stimulus argument, which argues language

deficits or low scores are due to lack of exposure to L2, lack of

adequate academic training, or some combination, is never applied

to other developmental biological processes (2000). However, one

could argue that environmental factors can inhibit developmental

41

language processes, for example lack of vitamin D can lead to

Ricket’s disease.

Chomsky also asserts contrary to looking at development in

terms of healing deficits, grammar has a generative quality

(2006). Generative grammar is constructivist in nature. Everyone,

all the time, is generating or developing his or her personal I-

language. Generative grammar explains why the English of

Shakespeare or of Benjamin Franklin or of Ghandi was different

than the English people speak around the world today.

Language Acquisition Device

For Chomsky knowledge of a language has been acquired when a

person has “internalized a system of rules that relate sound and

meaning in a particular way.” (Chomsky, 2006). Not distinguishing

language development for children or adults, L1 or L2, Chomsky

simply claims that language acquisition is the result of a likely

biological process called the language acquisition device, or

LAD. Chomsky reasons that language acquisition is different than

42

a cognitive or psychological process because children learn

language automatically and unconsciously (2006).

Adults and children learning a language, first or second,

always learn surface phonetic structures then deeper grammar

structures (Chomsky, 2006). Language acquisition is

constructivist and a very individual experience. Even adult

second language learners will acquire deep grammar structures

over time inexplicably whether or not they have had direct

language instruction (2006). Chomsky also refers to I-language,

meaning language is internal, individual, and intentional. I-

language is even, Chomsky asserts, a brain state (2000).

Chomsky’s theories about UG, LAD, generative grammar and I-

language indicate critical literacy teachers should make learning

personal by drawing on student background knowledge, allowing

moments of metacognition, and helping students develop and

connect every lesson with their motivation and goals. Even

though, Chomsky poses a paradoxical dilemma by claiming that all

languages have universal grammar but every human being learns

43

language mysteriously and individualistically, Chomsky’s deep

understanding of grammar helps identify universal language

learning principles.

Personal differences and I-language

Teachers can also assume from UG that adult ELL students

have acquired the ability to think conceptually and undoubtedly

use higher order thinking sometimes in their lives. Applying UG

to adult language acquisition, teachers should always focus on

language acquisition before critical literacy. Students must have

the vocabulary to build their surface grammar to utilize deep

grammar structures required in critical literacy. Therefore,

teachers must help students identify their higher order thinking

skills and apply these skills to reading and writing.

Chomsky does admit that variations occur amongst languages

semantically (Chomsky, 2000).

To ask in 1950 whether machines think is as meaningful as the question whether airplanes and people (say skyjumpers) really fly; in English airplanes do and high jumpers don’t (accept metaphorically), in Hebrew neither do, and in Japanese both do. (2000)

44

The question about flying in various languages, according to

Chomsky, underscores the nature of I-langauage. Teachers might

consider finding alternative ways of explaining concepts and not

assuming students always have exactly the same concept in their

native language.

Chomsky makes critical literacy for adult learners sound

fairly easy; it should just naturally appear without any direct

instruction (2000). Motivational factors, similar to I-language

principles, originating from the self-system and metacognitive

factors are part of Marzano and Kendall’s New Taxonomy of

critical thinking (Marzano & Kendall, 2007). Chomsky’s theories

are good reminders for teachers to recontextualize L1 higher

order thinking concepts into L2 concepts and to personalize

instruction.

How long does it take adults to learn a second language?

According to CAELA, the Center for Adult English Language

Acquisition, the length of time required to learn English as a

second language varies depending on previous educational

45

experience, age, literacy level in the native language, and

exposure to English (CAELA, 2010). CAELA reports that accepted

expectancy for English proficiency including academic proficiency

is estimated around 5-7 years. Furthermore, a study done in 1985

by the Mainstream English Language Training project indicates 500

to 1000 hours of instruction for adults with literacy skills in

their first language to acquire basic interpersonal communication

skills (2010). Additional research shows adult Asian subjects

show to have an 18% increase in hearing l’s and r’s correctly,

indicating that adult brains can be retuned to learn languages

more easily (UCL, 2010). Not only can adults learn a second

language at varying rates, but also adults can learn languages

more efficiently and faster.

Developing critical literacy skills at all ELL levels

Since it can take years to develop critical literacy skills,

and according to Chomsky and Vygotsky, adult ELLs without

literacy skills in their L1 still have developed higher order

46

thinking skills, teachers can begin offering critical literacy

skills in English at beginning levels.

For example, using a cause and effect graphic organizer in a

beginning class teachers could get students to write dirty hands

= sick stomach, fever, cough. In an intermediate class the same

cause and effect graphic organizer could be used to model

questions: Did you wash your hands? = Yes, I did. Or No, I did

not = Go wash your hands! In a higher intermediate class past

tense could be used. In advanced classes, full sentences with

conditional clauses could be requested. This is just one

scenario, but possibly CALPS could be introduced much more

earlier than imagined.

Neuroscience and second language acquisition

There are approximately 66 different SLA theories (Long,

2000). Cummins and Chomsky are regularly noted as SLA theorists

(2000). Vygotsky’s theories offer rich insights into the

development of language and thought, although he is not

considered a SLA theorist. Neuroscientists have also added a

47

great deal to the field of SLA mostly from the studies of

amnesia, Alzheimer’s and stroke patients (Sanz, 2005).

Cristina Sanz, a neuroscientist, writes that there is

“general agreement that individual differences have a greater

role in L2 acquisition than L1.” (2005). Previously it was

thought that second language was impossible after puberty;

however, now “age effects” are considered more likely. Research

shows that it becomes harder to learn a second language as we

age. Possibly younger people have more motivation, self-esteem,

and desire to learn a second language and fit into a new culture

(Sanz, 2005). Other possible factors affecting second language

acquisition, SLA, are neural plasticity, the ability of neurons

in the brain to reorganize and create new connections in the

brain faced with new information (2005).

The declarative/procedural (D/P) model is one of the more

prevalent contributions to SLA from neuroscience (Sanz, 2005). In

this model both declarative and procedural memory are needed to

acquire language. Declarative memory categorizes facts,

48

environmental information, and facts. Procedural memory is used

to learn sequential events and things that are done

automatically. Declarative memory facilitates the development of

lexical grammar, or what Chomsky would call surface grammar, and

procedural memory facilitates the development of semantic

grammar, or deep grammar (Sanz, 2005). Marzano and Kendall call

declarative knowledge the “what” or content and procedural

knowledge the “how” or process of thinking (2007).

Contextual language instruction where facts and scenarios

can be absorbed would be most appropriate, whereas procedural

memory might prefer direct instruction with clear explanations

and step-by-step instructions. Marzano and Kendall state that

declarative memory is the domain of information, one of the three

main aspects of knowledge development in their new taxonomy

theory (2007). Declarative knowledge, a “hierarchic in its own

right”, requires vocabulary that should be accurate but “not

necessarily a deep level of understanding.” (2007). Procedural

49

knowledge makes up the mental domain or the second of the three

domains of knowledge.

Children use declarative memory much more than procedural

memory, which explains why most research (Cummins, 1984), even

Vygotsky (1986) recommends that children be taught contextually.

Women tend to use declarative memory more than men, indicating

that for adults while it may be beneficial to include both direct

and contextual instruction in the classroom, women and men may

prefer more of one type of instruction. Also the older people get

the more procedural memory is used (2005). Clearly, the D/P model

provides helpful guidance for teachers of critical literacy.

Include both contextual and direct instruction in proportion to

the gender and age of the adult ELL students.

This interesting correlation between the inclusion of

declarative and procedural knowledge in Marzano and Kendall’s new

taxonomy and the D/P model for SLA is certainly noteworthy for

teachers of critical literacy. In the new taxonomy basic

principles of language acquisition provide the bulk of knowledge

50

acquisition. Adult ELL instructors could infer that it is always

a good first step to provide language instruction before critical

literacy instruction. Make sure that students have the vocabulary

and concepts of critical literacy. Show them through modeling,

scaffolded instruction, and other research-based literacy

strategies how to express themselves and their intelligence

through reading and writing.

The relationship between thought and language

Exploring the development of critical literacy is a dual

exploration of thought and language development. The theorists

mentioned in this Breadth section leave clues about the nature of

thought and development. Cummins’ research had its genesis in

questions about bilingualism and intelligence (1984). Cummins’

found that bilingual children in several languages made higher

marks than monolingual children. According to Cummins, speaking

and listening precede reading and writing proficiency (1984). So

from Cummins it can be deduced that language facilitates the

development of thought.

51

Vygotsky, ever so eloquently stating the obvious, points out

how thought develops from child to adult naturally facilitating

the development of higher order thinking. First thought requires

language from a mother or caregiver to develop (1986). Then

language becomes a child’s tool for taking care of his or her

basic needs and at some point language also becomes an internal

voice. Later on as children begin to develop and think complex

thoughts, they are capable of learning concepts. Teenagers can

apply concepts correctly, but may simplify concepts if asked to

explain them, which adults also do even at advanced levels of

academia. From Vygotsky, the beauty of the internal voice used to

express oneself and understand the world is a valuable reminder

about the interdependent nature of thought and language

development.

Chomsky’s universal grammar, and I-language illustrate how

paradoxical language and thought are. UG shows that all languages

share basic surface and deep grammar. On a vast human scale

people are learning phonemes and words then putting them together

52

in meaningful ways. Individually, each person’s thoughts, their

I-language – that internal, individual, and intentional language

process – varies infinitely. Just as Vygotsky showed with the

Tolstoy example that words are only needed as long as thoughts

are not the same. Less verbal language is needed to communicate

when people share the same thoughts. UG and I-language point out

that huge human variation colors the surface and deep grammar

structures. The new taxonomy even is more elaborate by including

the idea of metacognition and the self-system, more internal

language, influencing the development of thought.

The D/P model and Marzano & Kendall’s new taxonomy both use

the declarative and procedural cognitive systems to explain

respectively language and thought. The overlap of these theories

shows how interrelated thought and language truly are.

Evaluating the theories

Cummins’ BICS / CALPS theory has been in existence over 30

years and still is an important contribution to SLA. The

Woodcock-Munoz Language Survey – Revised, an addition to the

53

Woodcock-Johnson reading test, was developed bilingually in

Spanish and English with specific sections that test CALPS

(Riverside, 2010). Maren Aukerman, assistant professor of

Education at the University of Pennsylvania, wrote a paper called

“The Culpable CALP” (2007) in which she totally disagreed with

the BICS/CALPS theory stating instead, semantic meaning was

merely a matter of recontextualizing information or concepts ELL

students already have.

Cummins, himself, points out criticisms for BICS/CALPS on

his website as 1) based on academic language not realistic

natural language; 2) based on “test-wiseness” and not language

production; 3) promoting the idea that some students have

cognitive deficits rather than inadequate schooling (2010).

Cummins refutes these criticisms citing compelling studies that

show how academic language is a large part of natural English;

“where underachievement among subordinated students was

attributed to coercive relations of power operating in the

society at large and reflected in schooling practices”; and other

54

studies which confirmed the construct of academic language

proficiency does not necessarily depend on standardized testing

for validity (2010). All in all, Cummins BICS/CALPS theory stands

out solidly amongst SLA theories.

Vygotsky has only been criticized for the “studies” he

quotes in his writings (1986). No data or explanation of the

“studies” Vygotsky supposedly has done are offered. Nevertheless,

the zone of proximal development and Vygotsky’s explanation of

the development of thought and language are somewhat iconic in

educational theory. He is more of an educational theorist.

However, for the study of critical literacy, his insights are

very valuable.

Chomsky has consistently refuted skeptics over the years. He

is a philosopher and debater of the highest caliber. Reading his

work was very challenging because he would constantly bring in

other theories and compare and evaluate them in the middle of

presenting his theories. His sentences are often lengthy and

complex. For such simple ideas, he has literally volumes to say,

55

illustrating with highly advanced linguistics his concepts.

Universal grammar is a staple of SLA and one is grossly

misinformed without considering Chomsky’s theories.

The D/P model is, by nature of being a model, heavily

researched. Even so, Sanz admits that neuroscientific SLA

research tends to be contrived since it adheres to strict

experimental research guidelines (2005). Classroom educational

research has too many variables to control, so neuroscientific

SLA research sometimes reduces variables like multiple students,

classrooms, and teachers by using laboratories and only parts of

lessons (Sanz, 2005). Still, by virtue of its model quality, the

D/P shows promise until, if, and when new research cast shadows

of doubt.

Robert Marzano is a highly respected and prolifically

published educational researcher (2010). He has assisted Walden

University with instruction and development of the University’s

Master’s in Education program (Walden, 2010). Marzano has

synthesized and published educational research that is directly

56

applicable for classroom instruction. The new taxonomy is quite a

bit different and more complex than Bloom’s, and its importance

has yet to be seen.

Common Ideas

Amongst the theories on language and thought from Cummins,

Vygotsky, Chomsky, the D/P theory, and Marzano and Kendall, there

are a great many common ideas and a few differences. The

following chart illustrates these similarities and differences.

Table 3 – Synthesis of Theories

______________________________________________________________________________

Cummins Vygotsky Chomsky D/P Model NewTaxonomy

Languageacquisiti

on –children

- BICS 2-3 years- CALPS 7-10 years

- requires input from caregiver- eventually child develops internal voice

-LAD,languageacquisitiondevice,automatic

- children use primarilydeclarative memory

- Information domainrequires vocabulary acquisition as a first step

Language INFERRED: INFERRED: - No real - both - use all

57

acquisition –adults

- some adults may have challenges with CALPS dueto inadequate education

- adults already have and internal voice andability to think conceptually, theysimply must transfer those L1 abilitiesto L2

distinction is made between children and adults, both use the LAD

D/P 6 levels of thinking,and all 3domains of knowledgefor best results

Development of

criticalthinkingskills

- facilitated by development of CALPS

- complexthinking in children turns to conceptual thinking-all teens andadults can thinkconceptually

- thoughtand language are always developing, generative grammar

- n/a - developall 6 levels ofthinking,and all 3domains of knowledgefor best results-theory for academicsnot SLA

Implications for

Adult ELLStudents

- students with L1 CALPS will learn

- ZPD, challengeand review- assume L1

- everybodyis acquiringlanguage all the

- teach contextually and directly- women may need

- teach using thenew taxonomy model

58

critical literacy faster- help others develop critical literacy skills

critical thinking exists and help transfer to L2

time in L1 and L2- help students identify their I-language

more contextual, older people and men less

Implications forteachingcriticalliteracy

- youngerstudents need morecontextual teaching- supportL2 literacy learning with L1 literacy learning

- help students transfer L1 conceptual thinking ability to reading and writing in L2

- make sure students understand vocabulary for critical literacy then teach reading /writing strategies

- use indications for teaching language to teach critical literacy vocabulary

- use newtaxonomy model starting with level one, making sure a clear vocabulary foundation is set

Language development and intelligence

Language may or may not be a sign of intelligence. Cummins

avoids associating CALPS with intelligence and higher order

thinking, although he does point out that many intelligence tests

are inaccurate for ESL students when they have not gained

59

proficiency with either English or their native language (1986).

However, assuming that one mode of expression important to

expressing higher order thinking is literacy, developing CALPS

are essential for children and adults to proving their

development of higher order thinking.

Vygotsky is more concerned with the developmental dance

thought and language play; he also does not address intelligence.

However, according to the zone of proximal development, any

intelligence test would never show the true level of learning a

student has acquired (Vygotsky, 1978). Chomsky directly states

…one would expect that human language should directly reflect the characteristics of human intellectual capacities, that language should be a direct ‘mirror of mind’ in ways other systems of knowledge and belief cannot. (2006)

Although it appears language and intelligence are linked,

Chomsky, as a researcher, is reticent to link these variables. He

asserts “given the primitive character of the study of man and

society and its general lack of intellectual substance” science

would be irresponsible claiming to really know the relationship

60

between language and intelligence (2006). Sanz, et al.,

neuroscience researchers, are also interestingly silent on the

relationship between intelligence and second language acquisition

(2005).

Whether or not language acquisition and intelligence are

linked, teaching critical literacy may increase intelligence. The

purpose of Mazano and Kendall’s new taxonomy is to promote

learning and the development of thinking skills in the classroom

(2007); additionally, it can be inferred - develop intelligence.

By adding the metacognitive level and the self-system to Bloom’s

traditional cognitive approach to thinking, behaviors associated

with high intelligence were thrown into the schema of developing

thinking skills (2007). Including instructional practices that

utilize new taxonomy learning principles, adult ELL teachers will

undoubtedly help their students develop intelligence as well as

knowledge of English and critical thinking skills.

Implications for helping adult ELL Students acquire a second language

61

Many ideas for teaching adults English have already been

explored in this Breadth section. In summary, adults do need

contextual instruction as indicated from research with children

(Cummins, 1984), but adults also need direct instruction (Marzano

& Kendall, 2007; Sanz, 2005). Women may need more contextual

instruction and men more direct instruction. Vygotsky’s ZPD is an

indication that language should be learned collaboratively with

people in ways that challenge learners and allow learners to

practice previously learned information (1986).

Both Vygotsky and Chomsky suggest that learning for adults

should be as personalized as possible to help concepts in L1

transfer to L2; this is also called helping learners develop

background knowledge (Marzano, Pickering, and Pollock, 2001)

Vygotsky and Chomsky both presume adults have developed

conceptual thinking in their L1. They may need to identify the

specific concepts associated with critical literacy such as

comparing, contrasting, making inferences, and evaluating. Adults

probably already have these thought processes and express these

62

ways of thinking through BICS but may need to develop the

vocabulary for these thoughts in English and possibly in their

L1. Cummins’ BICS and CALPS distinction is descriptive in nature,

but by no means a static condition. In his findings, Cummins’

points out how immersion programs and bilingual literacy

instruction greatly improve students’ CALP and overall language

acquisition (1984). Cummins, Vygotsky, and Chomsky appear to

agree on the principles that language is a constructivist process

requiring human interaction and literacy exposure.

Chomsky’s LAD theory is particularly encouraging to adults

who tend to learn languages more slowly than children (Sanz,

2005). If adults are naturally generating grammar all the time

and learning with the LAD all the time, knowledge of a second

language will eventually occur.

Mind reading is a natural stage of language and thought

development (Vygotsky, 1986). Vygotsky’s example of Tolstoy’s

exchange with his wife is a wonderful reminder for adult ELL

students. In L1 language adults automatically mind read with

63

other L1 speakers (Papafragou, 2002). It might be tempting for

advanced adult ELL students to begin mind reading when they are

not quite 100% fluent. Mind reading could lead to

miscommunication problems. Instructors might want to remind adult

ELL students to ask clarifying questions when conversing in L2.

Critical literacy instruction should greatly assist advanced

adult ELLs in developing critical thinking skills with reading

and writing that help them avoid mind reading miscommunication in

speech.

Implications for teaching critical literacy

To recap, Cummins work suggests that adult ELL students

might learn critical literacy skills with primarily contextual

instruction. Also, it is important that teachers recognize if

students have CALPS in their L1. If students still need to

develop CALPS, teachers will need to demonstrate the concepts of

critical literacy as well as the skills. From Vygotsky and

Chomsky, adult ELL students probably have critical thinking

concepts in their L1. However, their critical thinking skills may

64

not have been applied to reading and writing. Vygotsky’s ZPD

suggests using collaborative learning techniques, as well as

instruction that challenges and allows for practice of previously

learned critical literacy knowledge.

Marzano and Kendall’s new taxonomy provides a detailed

numerous and sequential processes that can help adults develop

critical thinking. Even when adult ELL students enter the

classroom with little education, these theorists provide hope

that with proper guidance adults can learn the language of

critical thinking and the literacy skills to express critical

thinking as they learn English. The new taxonomy is largely

procedural in nature, suggesting how to teach, in terms of its

implications for teaching adult ELL students. Whereas, Vygotsky

and Chomsky’s theories are more declarative in nature, suggesting

what people experience as they acquire language and thought.

Conclusion

With over 66 theories about second language acquisition

there are a great many ideas about how language is acquired

65

(Long, 2000). This Breadth component examined just a few SLA

theories selected for their orientation on thought and critical

literacy development. Cummins BICS/CALPS theory clearly described

the dilemma many immigrants in English-speaking lands face as

they seek entrance to college. Their second language skills must

show cognitive academic language proficiency. Vygotsky and

Chomsky theories illuminate relationship between language and

thought acquisition for adults. Adult ELL students, having

acquired conceptual and probable critical thinking skills with

listening and speech in their L1 presumably can transfer these

skills to L2. As indicated by the D/P model and new taxonomy, the

transfer of critical thinking skills from L1 to L2 and the

expression of critical literacy may be facilitated by contextual

and direct instruction. Applying these principles in an adult ELL

classroom may yield very rich experiences for instructors and

students.

66

DEPTH

EDUC 8224: CURRENT RESESARCH IN HUMAN DEVELOPMENT AND ADULT

EDUCATION

Annotated Bibliography

Drewring, T., Murray, M., Thomson, R. (2007). A longitudinal

study of ESL learners’ fluency and comprehensibility

development. Applied Linguistics, 29(3), 359-380.

doi:10.1093/applin/amm041

Research Summary

This two-year mixed method longitudinal study assessed

fluency and comprehensibility for Mandarin and Slavic high

beginning English language students in Canada. The study also

assessed student motivation and exposure to English outside of

class. A slight increase in fluency was observed for the Slavic

students but none for Mandarin students. Also comprehensibility

67

failed to increase for either group. Based on the findings,

researchers recommended: pronunciation practice; structured

mandatory volunteer activities in English settings; and

increasing motivation for learning. Fluency exercises including

offering formulaic phrases, and paraphrasing practice. Teaching

appropriate pausing, and increasing the speaking rate and

structured encounters with native speakers were recommended as

well. Reliability and validity were established and significant

ratings were found for the quantitative rating section of the

research.

Critical Assessment

The research questions about increasing fluency and

comprehensibility might be more validly addressed if this same

study was replicated for beginning and advanced level students.

Students in intermediate levels tend to be in the “silent

period,” meaning they generally have good comprehension but tend

to have great difficulty speaking. Also basic information about

the curriculum and teachers was left out completely. No

68

statistics checking for teacher variation or reliability was

mentioned, which makes the gains Slavic students experienced

questionable. The study does not address pronunciation challenges

inherent with the L1 alphabet and phonetics, nor is specific

phonetic instruction suggested or implemented.

Professional Value

The implications for this study are that motivation and

exposure to English outside of the classroom could be very

important for advanced English language learners to learn

literacy critical thinking skills. Pronunciation is an important

piece of becoming fluent and this study shows that very possibly

failure to increase comprehensibility can inhibit adult language

acquisition. Pronunciation may be an important component of

developing deeper grammar understanding and developing critical

literacy skills.

Durkin, K. (2008). The middle way: East Asian master’s students’

perception of critical argumentation in U.K. Universities.

Journal of Studies in International Education, 12, 38. DOI:

69

10.1177/1028315307302839

Research Summary

Forty-two masters’ students were studied over four years in

this qualitative grounded theory and case methodology study in

which Durkin identified that Chinese/Asian college students in

the U.K. find the Socratic argumentative debate style of learning

uncomfortable. Chinese/Asian students are more used to direct

instruction given by the teacher to the students, where the

effects are given and the causes are implied. Chinese culture

dictates that higher order thinking involves saying more with

less communication, so Chinese/Asian students find the western

definition of critical thinking as odd and the opposite of their

traditional mode of higher thinking.

Critical Assessment

Durkin tends to idealize the “Middle Way” as being more

humane and empathetic, possibly more feminine. All the interviews

were conducted in English, which may have inhibited true

understanding and communication. However, the background

70

information and the explanation of findings thoroughly illuminate

the cultural differences between debate, academic argumentation,

and an array of discourse and writing styles that search for the

truth.

Professional Value

It is useful to discuss various cultural definitions of

critical thinking with advanced English language students

learning literacy critical thinking skills because student

concepts of critical literacy in L1 might be different than in

English. In that case not only would new vocabulary but also new

concepts would need to be taught. Students may have difficulty

comparing and contrasting in essays in English because it is

socially inappropriate in their culture to criticize. It may be

useful to talk about implicit meaning with students.

Dyson, B. (2009). Processability theory and the role of

morphology in English as a second language development: a

longitudinal study. Second Language Research 25(3), 355-376.

Research Summary

71

Processability theory (PT) asserts that learning morphology

is the main source of second language acquisition. In this

longitudinal qualitative study a girl who was 12 at the beginning

of the study and a boy of 13, were interviewed to test PT to see

if morphological attainment preceded syntactic development in

English. Both participants were Chinese, Madarin native speakers,

and beginner English language students. The participants were

interviewed orally and given communication tasks 6 times

throughout the course of a year. The study concluded that

although the boy showed evidence of English acquisition in

accordance with PT, the girl did not. Possible explanations for

this are individual learner preferences and incremental language

development.

The boy was characterized as a “standardizer” who took basic

grammatical structures and applied them to new language

structures. The girl was described as a “simplifier” who used

context to navigate new language structures. The author points

out that the boy, as a grammatical learner, gave preference to

72

learning morphology over syntax. The girl, however, showed a

preference for syntax and exhibited significant lags in

morphological development.

Critical Assessment

Being a case study it is harder to generalize these findings

to a larger population unlike quantitative studies. However, the

implications are useful in evaluating and developing PT. There

was no discussion of moderating variables such as education and

environment. Were they in the same class with the same teacher?

How much of the day did they speak English? Did their parents

speak English? Did they like Australia? Additionally, the study

only focused on speaking and listening. If the students were

primarily visual learners, their reading and writing skills in

English might be higher than their speaking skills.

Professional Value

This study is very interesting because its findings mirror

the Declarative/Procedural (D/P) model. According to the D/P

model, women tend to use the declarative memory that would favor

73

context learning focusing on syntax or content; whereas, men tend

to learn procedurally focusing on rules, order, and process or

how. The participants are not adults, but still the results show

that teaching both content focused and process, or grammar,

strategies for language acquisition are important for all

learners.

Huang, J., & Brown, K. (2009). Cultural factors affecting Chinese

ESL students’ academic learning. Education, 129(4), 643-653.

Retrieved from Education Research Complete database

Research Summary

This is a literature review rather than a report of an

actual research study. In this article Huang and Brown explain

how Confucianism has imbued Chinese culture with the principles

of harmony and respect. Elders and teachers are given great

respect in China. Students are taught to develop self-mastery,

then harmony in the family, and harmony at work and in the world.

The Chinese school system is based on standardized testing and is

74

very competitive. The testing culture goes back to Confucius who

trained state leaders to take special examinations.

Specifically, inside the classroom Chinese students are

uncomfortable with discussions versus lectures and the behavior

of American students. They question the value of discussion-led

learning and failure of instructors to follow textbooks. Chinese

students also report lacking appreciation and value for group

work. Also, a lack of lectures makes these students question the

professor’s organizational skills. Finally, Chinese students fail

to connect socially with subjects Americans enjoy, such as

sports.

Critical Assessment

Although no interviews were conducted, which would have made

this literature review more interesting and valid, the fact that

one author was Chinese gave validity to this report. The study is

specific to the Chinese culture; however, it is possible to

imagine how other adult students would have similar experiences.

Having been successful in their native land using one set of

75

behaviors, it might be difficult to understand, appreciate,

value, feel comfortable, and be motivated to express oneself in a

new language with cultural affective filters blocking a learning

experience because it is specific to American culture.

Professional Value

No figures were given for the number of Chinese students

enrolled in English-speaking universities, but the authors say

that 80% of all Chinese students in the United States are

graduate students. This information reminds adult English

language instructors to have conversations about students’

classroom expectations and preferences. It could very well

facilitate learning to offer more lecture-based classes for

Chinese students. Similarly, instruction could be modified

including the learning preferences of other cultures. Having

conversations in English language classrooms with adults will

promote tolerance, understanding, metacognition, and help create

classroom community.

76

Ismail, I. (2009). Weblog: a collaborative tool for learning

academic reading. The International Journal of Learning 16(7), ISSN

1447-9494

Research Summary

Ismail found in this qualitative study that using a weblog

as a supplemental classroom experience for 25 young adult

students whose average age was 18.6 years was a positive

community-building experience. The students were enrolled in a

college academic reading class focusing on critical thinking

skills and navigating textbooks. Participants were interviewed

and given questionnaires after 16 weeks. Ismail reported the

weblog allowed students to see how other students were “thinking”

and using critical literacy skills for their assignments. The

weblog also helped to create more community amongst the students.

Critical Assessment

The research does not use student quotes from interviews or

list any data from the questionnaire. The lack of data undermines

77

the credibility of the research report. However, the theoretical

background of the weblog concept was well researched.

Professional Value

Many students have personal computers or cell phones with

Internet service. Technological literacy is becoming just as

important if not more so than paper literacy. A weblog is a great

idea for inviting students to read and write outside of class.

Weblogs also allow all students if they wish to voice their

thoughts. Whereas, in class all students may not speak and share

what is on their mind.

Iwai, Y. (2008). The Perceptions of Japanese Students toward

Academic English Reading: Implications for Effective ESL

Reading Strategies. Multicultural Education, 15(4), 45-50.

Retrieved from EBSCOhost.

Research Summary

This qualitative study about academic reading strategies was

conducted with two Japanese graduate students who were studying

in American Universities. The interviews were conducted in

78

Japanese about the students’ experience with academic reading in

English. Major themes identified in the research included

strategies such as reading for pleasure, increasing overall

reading in English, and only using the dictionary for key words

rather than translating every unknown word. Other factors this

research identified as positively affecting reading were exposure

to many different types of reading and finding reading materials

that were of interest.

Critical Assessment

This study provides useful suggestions and perspectives from

the two Japanese students studying in American universities. The

fact that the interviews were done in the students’ native

language seems to add validity to their information, since the

students can express themselves better in Japanese than in

English. It would be interesting to conduct the interviews in

English with an English interviewer asking the same questions.

Maybe the students would think differently in English. Even

though the study was small, the methodology and conclusions

79

offered valuable suggestions for teaching critical literacy to

adult ELL students in universities. No mention of specific

quantitative data collection statistics was given.

Professional Value

The import for teaching literacy critical thinking skills for

advanced ESL students is that basic reading strategies such as

skimming, scanning, note-taking, critical reading, and using

context clues are very important for non-native readers. This

study also implicates that allowing students to self-select their

reading material increases motivation for learning.

Kovalik, D., Kovalik, L. (2007). Language simulations: the

blending space for writing and critical thinking. Simulation

Gaming, 38, 310. doi: 10.1177/1046878106298271

Research Summary

This qualitative descriptive study reported the experience of

45 college English freshman students in Canada. All students were

enrolled in a Composition 1 class and were required to

participate in six real-life activities geared towards

80

stimulating critical thinking skills. The activities included: an

in class quick response, small group game show activity, a

library assignment, a whole class debate, a debriefing session,

and at home writing. The in-class quick response and the essay

were about liberty as defined by Montesquieu. Most students did

change their opinions and supported them with better details

after the gaming activities, but some students did not.

Critical Assessment

Although the design of this research was extremely well

described and documented, the results were weakly evaluated. No

grades, quotes from students, observations, or survey results

were given. The authors wrote that the students who did not show

any change in opinion may need to have individual conversations

to understand their stagnation in opinion.

Professional Value

The activities described may represent best practices for

teaching advanced ESL students literacy critical thinking skills.

The same experiment would be interesting to duplicate with clear

81

student outcomes, rubrics, and measurements. This could become a

mixed method study with data collected from quizzes, writing

assignments, and student remarks during debates and other

collaborative learning activities.

Melles, G. (2009). Teaching and evaluation of critical appraisal

skills to postgraduate

ESL engineering students. Innovations in Education and

Teaching International, 46 (2), 161-170.

Research Summary

English for academic purposes (EAP) is a course widely taught

at English-speaking universities. In this quantitative case

study, engineering students are asked to write about their

experience in an EAP course specifically designed for their

field. In the EAP course students were required to write reviews

of scholarly articles, literature reviews, and give an oral

presentation. The responses were evaluated with NVivo software

and it was discovered that students learned how to link critical

thinking with reading and writing. Students also reported

82

recognizing the value of critical thinking for their engineering

profession. Some students reported that critical thinking was not

as much of a challenge as plagiarizing, paraphrasing, and

generally writing well in English. Other students related that

critical thinking seemed like criticism to them, and in their

native culture it was disrespectful to criticize. One Turkish

student even wrote there were probably different kinds of

critical thinking, but he could adapt to the Australian way of

critical thinking.

Critical Assessment

There is no number given for the participants in this study.

It could have been a mix of quantitative and qualitative

research. If the survey had scores as well as open-ended

responses, possibly more insight about the nature of the

students’ EAP experience could have been revealed. The written

samples exhibited in the research showed a clear need for grammar

instruction. No mention of actual English language instruction

was made, and it seems natural that as students have more command

83

of the language they can express critical literacy much more

easily.

Professional Value

This study provides evidence offering specific writing

support and critical thinking vocabulary and concept instruction

for EAP curriculum linked to specific fields. Again the topic of

cultural interpretations of critical thinking is important to

discuss with students. This is one of the only studies

specifically exploring how to help adult ELLs learn critical

literacy. Linking English language and critical literacy

instruction with specific fields of study makes perfect sense

because students will have acquired more vocabulary for their

field of study. Thus, they will be able to navigate advanced

English and critical literacy more easily.

Mok, J. (2009). From policies to realities: developing students’

critical thinking in Hong

Kong Secondary School English writing classes. RELC

Journal, 40, 262.

84

Research Summary

In this qualitative study two English writing teachers in

Hong Kong were observed for their methods to teach critical

thinking. Both writing teachers were interviewed and it was found

that they felt a great deal of pressure in the classroom. The

researcher found that teachers generally relied on the 3Ts:

teacher, text, and test, rather than engaging students in

literacy critical thinking skills. During observation, it was

observed that teachers failed to give their students wait time

before answering questions. Also, in interviews teachers admitted

to only encouraging the best students to write creatively and

felt too busy to implement critical thinking activities.

Critical Assessment

This study may show that it is difficult to teach non-fluent

language learners abstract higher order thinking skills when

students haven’t developed fluent concrete vocabulary to build

upon. Also, the study indicates teacher training for critical

literacy skills might help teachers feel more comfortable

85

teaching these skills. Mok reports that in Hong Kong businesses

are requesting students who have developed higher order thinking

skills. However, the dialogs reported in this study show that

teachers are possibly using traditional Asian dialog techniques

which Mok calls “product-oriented.”

Professional Value

Writing is a skill that involves learning the process of

writing. This study shows that it may be very difficult to take

theory into the classroom without professional development. Also,

this study indicates that businesses worldwide are interested in

graduates who have critical literacy skills. It was not mentioned

in this article if the teachers were of Asian origin and spoke

English as a L2. If they were non-native speakers they may need

additional support in teaching critical literacy concepts.

Morgan, B. 2009. Revitalising the essay in an English for

academic purposes course: critical engagement,

multiliteracies and the Internet. International Journal of Bilingual

Education and Bilingualism 12 (3), 309-324.

86

Research Summary

In this descriptive qualitative piece, the author makes a

case for teaching essay writing using written and digital media.

Using the essay of a Korean student, Morgan illustrates how video

and other types of digital media were used to help the student

develop an opinion and supporting evidence through critical

literacy. The author points out how pictures convey information

powerfully, possibly more so than written text.

Critical Assessment

In this case study the student purportedly made significant

progress with college-level essay writing using media, especially

documentaries to stimulate plans for writing. The results were

not entirely convincing due to lack of quotes from the student

and a lack of excerpts from the student’s writing. The author

does suggest that the study is a starting point and further

research is indicated.

Professional Value

This study offers wonderful insight about using writing to

87

develop literacy critical thinking skills simultaneously

including the Internet or other sources of media, such as videos.

More and more classrooms have SmartBoards. YouTube makes it easy

to offer short video clips pertaining to teaching. Using web

sites and video to supplement critical literacy instruction makes

great sense especially when students may have not developed

concepts such as synthesize in their native language.

Majid, F. (2008). The Development of an Academic Reading

Strategies Pedagogical Model for ESL Adult Learners.

International Journal of Learning, 15(1), 203-210. Retrieved from

Education Research Complete database.

Research Summary

Using a qualitative design this study employed case studies,

student diaries, think aloud protocols, and researcher

observation to investigate the relationship between adult learner

characteristics, specifically self-concept, experience, and time

perspective on academic reading strategies. Data collection was

analyzed by the constant comparison method. Halfway through the

88

study, Cohen Kappa’s test of agreement showed a “comfortable” .93

indicating that interrater agreement between researchers was

reliable taking into consideration chance agreement, so research

continued. Comparing proficient and non-proficient readers for

cognitive, metacognitive, and affective reading strategies, a

pedagogical instructional method called SRAIS was suggested.

SRAIS stands for sharing ideas orally, acknowledging the

importance of reading in and out of class, increasing

independence as a student and reader, read reflectively and

responsively, and seek to draw on background knowledge. Non-

proficient readers were also found to lack basic concepts about

the organization and schemas involved in academic texts.

Critical Assessment

This study never stated the number of participants. Even so,

the explanation of variables studied and coded and his SRAIS

curriculum suggestions actively engage students in metacognition,

the self-system, collaborative learning. Connecting with prior

knowledge to activate and build on strong neural connections.

89

Triangulating data from student diaries, think alouds, and

participant observation, plus using 24 texts offers more

convincing data than just researcher observation. However, the

author fails to substantiate claims such as the less proficient

readers’ self-concept as ‘student learner’ led them to making

mistakes with metacognitive strategies with anecdotes or student

quotes.

Professional Value

Majid mentions in this study that proficient readers were

intrinsically motivated

and able to read both contextualized and decontextualized texts.

Here may be

confirmation of teaching critical literacy to adults utilizing

Marzano & Kendall’s new

taxonomy. This study also implies that students who have had more

academic

experience probably have developed CALPS. So teaching

metacognitive skills and

90

helping students develop beliefs and motivation for academic

success may be necessary

to helping adult students develop critical literacy skills.

Lisa Pray.  (2005). How Well Do Commonly Used Language

Instruments Measure English Oral-Language

Proficiency? Bilingual Research Journal, 29(2), 387-409,500. 

Retrieved November 16, 2010, from Research Library.

(Document ID: 914809711).

Research Summary

Can CALPS be accurately measured? This question is addressed

in this quantitative study assessing the accuracy of the

measurement of English fluency in three commonly used English

proficiency tests. The Language Assessment Scales-Oral (LAS-O),

the IDEA Proficiency Test (IPT), and the WoodcockMunoz Language

Survey (WMLS) were given to 40 participants from the fourth and

fifth grades who were native English speakers. Half of the

students were Hispanic. The LAS and the IPT are commonly used to

rate English language learner progress and grant entrance into

91

regular classrooms. The WMLS is less commonly used, but the only

English language test that purportedly measures CALPS.

Using only oral language scores the research found that the

IPT and LAS-O classified the majority of students as fluent in

English; however, the WMLS classified none of the students, all

native English speakers, as fluent. The IPT results showed that

3% of the students were non-English speakers, 15% were limited

English speakers, and the majority was fluent. The LAS-O found no

students to be non-English speakers. The WMLS results classified

10% as negligible English speakers, 50% as very limited English

speakers, and 40% as limited English speakers. In sum,

comparisons of the three tests show great variation in their

reported measurements making their validity questionable,

especially since half the participants were white native English

speakers.

Critical Assessment

If the WMLS provides questionable results for white native

English speakers for its oral component, is the CALP component

92

also possibly invalid? The researcher tried to correlate socio-

economic status with the test scores for whites and Hispanics but

no significant correlation was found. The researcher was also the

only test administrator for all test applications. It is possible

there were proctor complications, although the researcher

reported being trained in the IPT and WMLS. Also, there was no

power analysis given for the sample size. Perhaps the sample was

not representative of the population to make generalizations.

Professional Value

Many financial and instructional decisions are made for

students based on test scores. It is concerning to see such great

variation in proficiency tests that are used for many students.

The WMLS is normed on thousands of people (Riverside Publishing,

2010), which is why the research reported is suspicious. However,

if the test really found white native speakers not fluent in

English, then maybe other measures for language proficiency and

CALPS need to be designed.

Rafik-Galea, S., & Bhaskaran Nair, P. (2007). Enhancing ESL

93

teacher

trainees’ critical thinking skills through

scaffolding. Journal of Pan-

Pacific Association of Applied Linguistics, 11(1), 99–113.

Research Summary

Scaffolding as an instructional technique to teach critical

thinking in literacy classes for adult ESL students is the focus

of this study. Three researchers observed 16 teacher trainees who

were also ESL students work in dyads, one high proficient student

paired with a low proficient student. Data were collected

quantitatively. The researchers evaluated narrative scripts once

alone, the second time with a partner, and thirdly with all three

researchers establishing a 93% inter-coder reliability rate. In

the areas of inferencing, analysis, evaluation, and

interpretation, evidence from student discussion was shown that

collaboratively students were able to learn and exhibit critical

thinking skills.

Critical Assessment

94

This study offers a wonderful model of utilizing Vygotsky’s

zone of proximal development theory to teach critical literacy

skills through collaborative learning. These students learned by

reading and speaking. It would have been a more powerful study if

the students had produced essays or short written responses that

exhibited critical thinking. There was no survey given to

students that could give voice to their subjective experience of

the critical thinking and reading training. A survey would have

triangulated data providing more evidence for scaffolding.

Professional Value

This study offers a model of scaffolding as a technique for

teaching critical literacy. Presumably, since all the 16

participants were already college students, they had developed

CALPS in their first language. Some may have developed CALPS

already in English. However, pairing a high proficient student

with a low proficient student is a great idea for developing

CALPS. It would be very interesting to extend this study with the

modifications suggested in the critical analysis above and using

95

adult ELLs with high and low CALP development.

Wilda Laija-Rodríguez, Salvador Hector Ochoa, & Richard

Parker. (2006). The Crosslinguistic Role of Cognitive

Academic Language Proficiency on Reading Growth in Spanish

and English. Bilingual Research Journal, 30(1), 87-106,238-239. 

Retrieved November 16, 2010, from Research Library.

(Document ID: 1122033121).

Research Summary

It is widely accepted in linguistics that literacy in L1

facilitates literacy in L2CITE.

This quasi-experimental study with 77 second and third graders

also shows a weak correlation between CALP development in L1 and

L2 using the Woodcock Munoz Language Survey (WMLS) and the ORF

standardized reading test. The participants were all native

Spanish speakers. Development in their L1 CALP scores on the WMLS

was significantly correlated with reading in L2, but not reading

in L1. These findings are consistent with other research and

theories.

96

Critical Assessment

The findings of this study might be skewed by using second

and third graders. Second graders won’t have the same experience

with literacy and language as third graders. The CALP

measurements might include too much variation in the sample

population to be significant. The study did not offer any

statistical analysis of the sample population or inter-rater

reliability measures for the 6 test administrators. The WMLS is

administered one on one so inter-rater reliability is definitely

a concern for valid test results.

Professional Value

The WMLS is currently the only professionally developed

standardized language and literacy test that measures CALPS. It

is the third most widely used language test according to Laija-

Rodríguez, Ochoa, and Parker. The WMLS has levels suitable from

pre-school to adult. If this test truly measures CALPS then it

could be a useful tool to teach critical literacy skills to adult

ELLs.

97

Additionally, this research provides further evidence for a

bilingual curriculum. With adult ELLs who are struggling to

develop CALPS in English, it might be useful to ask them to

supplement their education with reading and writing in their

native language. Possibly by strengthening previous made neural

pathways for literacy, new connections might be created more

easily.

Xiangming, L., Brand, M. (2009). Effectiveness of music on

vocabulary acquisition, language usage and meaning for

mainland Chinese ESL learners. Music Education, 36(1), 73-84.

Research Summary

In this quasi-experimental quantitative study of the effect

music has on learning vocabulary, music as an instructional

method was found to have a significantly positive effect of

learning vocabulary and retaining that knowledge. The study

separated 105 Chinese law students into three classes of 35

students each, an all music group, a half music half-traditional

instruction group, and a no music group. The students were high

98

intermediate English speakers with an average vocabulary of 5,500

words. Analysis of Variance, ANOVA results showed no significant

difference in their knowledge of vocabulary after pre-tests were

given. Post-test and delayed post-test measures were the highest

for the all music group and second highest for the no music

group. Also, student surveys assessing student satisfaction with

their classes showed the highest satisfaction among the all music

group and second highest among the no music group. Interestingly,

the half music half traditional instruction group had the lowest

post and delayed post-test measures, as well as the lowest

satisfaction rates.

The researchers attempted to control moderating variables by

using the same instructor for all three groups. Each group had

the same 9 hours of instruction for the study, 1.5 hours twice a

week for three weeks. The usual English club extracurricular

English practice was suspended during the experiment to prevent

moderating variables.

Critical Assessment

99

This study indicates that playing pop songs from the

Beatles, Led Zeppelin, Van Morrison, Cat Stevens and other famous

artists is a promising and most likely enjoyable way to teach

vocabulary development. The instructor used the texts to teach

grammar, semantics, stress, and pronunciation. In the literature

review the “song-stuck-in-your-head” phenomenon is given as a

rationale for using music for instruction. Music has also been

found to be processed in a similar part of the brain as language.

Many pop lyrics unfortunately are written as poetry with

incorrect grammar. It might be confusing to ELLs to learn grammar

solely from music.

Professional Value

Everybody loves having fun and being entertained in class.

This study very interestingly points out that using a combination

of music and traditional instruction may not be the best use of

music for teaching adult ELLs. It might be more appropriate to

teach a unit, such as the three-week span shown in this study, to

teach English language using primarily music as an instructional

100

method. Music may also be a way to help students with primarily

BICS in English start to develop CALPS because most pop lyrics,

as cited by this study, are written at about an 11 year old level

in English. Simpler vocabulary and grammar structures may allow

students to understand and identify metaphorical comparison and

inferences more easily.

Zhang, L.J. (2008). Constructivist pedagogy in strategic reading

instruction: exploring pathways to learner development in

the English as a second language (ESL) classroom. Instructional

Science, 38, 89-116. doi:10.1007/s11251-007-9025-6

Research Summary

This study indicates constructivist strategy-based pedagogy

is effective for teaching Chinese students, with an average age

of 18, reading in English for academic purposes courses required

for entrance into university. The 99 participants were chosen

for their previous levels of English education and randomly

placed into two groups of 49 and 50 students. Instructional

strategies for teaching critical literacy, metacognition, and

101

collaborative learning were used with one group and the other

group was taught with knowledge-based instructional practices.

The researcher / instructor included the students’ cultural

preference for working with the teacher and first led

collaborative learning such as reciprocal teaching. The same

instructor taught both groups using the same texts.

Students were given IELTS English proficiency tests before

and after the 2-month period of research. Additionally, students

were surveyed using a Likert scale questionnaire to find out

their background knowledge with strategies for reading. Results

showed that students often knew the word strategy but could not

name reading strategies even in their L1. Students reported being

very motivated to learn. Sixteen of 17 different reading

strategies were found to produce higher mean scores, usually

double, that of the control group and produced significant t test

results.

Critical Assessment

This is a very detailed study and provides fantastic

102

direction for teaching adult ESL students academic reading. It

would be very interesting to apply the same study to other

nationalities of students such as Latino students in the United

States, for example. Two months seems like a relatively short

time for such significant instructional gains. It would also be

useful to give students a survey asking which reading strategies

were most useful for them.

Professional Value

This study clearly indicates that utilizing the metacognitive

and self-system noted in Marzano and Kendall’s new taxonomy,

critical thinking can be taught quite efficiently. Instructors of

advanced adult ELLs should definitely take these findings into

the classroom. Also, the research here indicates the importance

of using cultural expectations as a bridge to new classroom

learning experiences.

Zhou, A. (2007). What adult ESL learners say about improving

grammar and vocabulary in their writing for academic

purposes. Language Awareness 18 (1), 31-36. Retrieved from

103

Education Research Complete database.

This is a follow-up qualitative study using data collected by Jim

Cummins and

others in 2006. The grounded theory study investigates the

experience of adult ESL

learners with academic English. Specifically, the research

focused on vocabulary and

grammar acquisition. The 15 university students reported that

although they wanted very badly to write in English flawlessly,

they had trouble with grammar and often didn’t know where to go

for help or support. University professors were said to have

focused on content and not grammar. Also, vocabulary was learned

best in context rather than through memorization. Students also

relied on media and social contact to improve their vocabulary.

Critical Assessment

The debate about teaching grammar contextually or through

direct instruction

amongst English language teachers has gone on for years,

104

chronicled by the succinct literature review in this study. The

vast array of student voices documented in this well detailed

study show that adult English language students, especially

advanced speakers admitted to English speaking universities

prefer direct grammar instruction. The study could have been

improved by offering a survey of some sort to find out what the

students would like. New students could have been included in the

study and given such a survey as an addendum to the existing

research.

Professional Value

Here there is a clear plea from adult ELLs to have direct

instruction for grammar. Presumably, the students in this study

have developed CALPS. However, in order to express themselves to

their best ability they still need vocabulary and grammar

support. This study indicates that college language labs,

learning labs, or writing centers are very important to support

the learning experience of ELL students.

Literature Review Essay

105

Language is a fascinating subject of instruction with

uncharted territory yet to be discovered. So far in this inquiry

of human development concentrating on adult foreign language

acquisition of critical literacy skills, theory, models, and

research have begun to illuminate effective instructional

practices. In this literature review essay, some additional

themes necessary for effective critical literacy instruction have

surfaced: memory, motivations, emotion and development of the

inner voice. These themes will be explored along side discussions

of the discoveries from the annotated bibliography research.

CALPS and the Literature

Cummins’ theory of BICS/CALPS helped to set the stage for

investigating adult critical literacy development in L2. Not all

researchers agree that Cummins’ definitions of CALPS are clear

(Wilda, Ochoa, & Parker, 2006). Even if CALPS exist, there is

debate about how to measure CALPS (Pray, 2005; Wilda, Ochoa &

Parker, 2006). Much research focuses on developing critical

literacy skills especially in English for academic purposes

106

courses (Durkin, 2008; Ismail, 2009; Iwai, 2008; Kovalik &

Kovalik, 2007; Melles, 2009; Mok, 2009; Morgan, 2009; Majid,

2008; Rafik-Galea & Nair, 2007; Xiangming & Brand, 2009; Zhang,

2008). Obviously, there is international interest from educators

in finding effective ways to teach critical literacy, and

consensus about the definition of critical thinking skills

applied to reading and writing.

Currently, more adult ELL instructors are concerned about

teaching critical thinking skills than CALP skills. Revisiting

Cummins’ distinction, “CALP is a reliable dimension of individual

differences in de-contextualized literacy related functions of

language which appears to be distinct from interpersonal

communicative skills in L1 and L2” (2001). This definition is

broad, but understanding the distinction of BICS and CALPS helps

instructors identify expressions of student critical literacy.

Vygotsky and the Literature

The zone of proximal development and Vygotsky’s emphasis on

social constructivist learning are cited as sources of

107

significant instructional practice and design for adult critical

literacy (Zhang, 2008; Rafik-Galea & Nair, 2007). The

collaborative social nature of the ZPD has been utilized

successfully to teach critical literacy skills (Ismail, 2009;

Kovalik & Kovalik, 2007; Majid, 2008; Rafik-Galea & Nair, 2007;

Zhang, 2008). Clearly, utilizing collaborative learning and ZPD

theory are indicated for teaching adult ELLs critical literacy.

Marzano & Kendall and the Literature

Since the new taxonomy theory is relatively new (Marzano &

Kendall, 2007), only one researcher noted this specific theory

(Mok, 2009). However, Bloom’s taxonomy, the precursor to Marzano

and Kendall’s work is cited more often probably because it has

much more longevity (Mok, 2009). Interestingly, metacgonition and

the self-system are referenced and utilized successful several

times to teach adult ELLs critical literacy (Zhang, 2008; Majid,

2008; Ismail, 2009). It appears the new taxonomy is an

appropriate theory to include instructionally yielding successful

learning experiences for adult ELLs.

108

Declarative/Procedural Model and the Literature

Several themes in the research cited indicate as the D/P

model suggests a need for both contextual learning and direct

grammar instruction. Astoundingly, Dyson’s study of the 12-year-

old girl’s preference for contextualized language production and

the 13-year-old boy’s preference for proceduralized language

production (2009) provide proof of research showing women tend to

prefer contextualized instruction using more declarative memory

processing, and men tend to prefer proceduralized instruction

utilizing the procedural memory processing (Sanz, 2005).

Additional studies show that instruction for both the

declarative, the what, and the procedural, the how, systems

facilitate adult ELLs in developing critical literacy (Drewring,

Murray, & Thomson, 2007; Melles, 2009; Morgan, 2009; Majid, 2008;

Xiangming & Brand, 2009; Zhang, 2008). All of these studies

successfully employed some type of context, literature,

discussion, music, or textbooks, the what, of English, in

addition to direct grammar instruction, the how of English.

109

Scaffolding is another instructional practice that breaks

down learning concepts into what and how. Scaffolding has been

used to teach reading and critical thinking successfully (Zhang,

2008; Morgan, 2009; Melles, 2009; Rafik-Galea & Nair, 2007;

Majid, 2008). Certainly another instructional practice indicated

for teaching adult ELLs critical literacy is scaffolding.

Chomsky and the Literature

Chomsky is not cited often in this literature selection.

Only Dyson explains the differences in the morphological versus

syntactical language acquisition discrepancy with Chomsky’s

universal grammar theory (2009). Universal grammar theory

maintains that syntactic language is acquired incrementally in

L2, which would explain why the girl chose to focus on language

syntax and later on morphology. In terms of adult critical

literacy acquisition, Dyson’s study and Chomsky’s theories remind

instructors to honor individual learning preferences and rates of

learning.

Gaps in Research Surrounding Critical Literacy

110

The relationship between CALPS and critical thinking, two

seemingly similar interdependent cognitive processes, seems

nonexistent in research. There is a lack of research regarding

CALP and critical thinking development in L1. Textbooks exist

about teaching strategies for critical literacy skills, but none

for teaching strategies for CALPS. There is still more to be

learned about instructional best practices for critical literacy

skills in a second language both for children and adults.

Vygotsky’s ideas about the development of an inner voice

appear very important to the development of metacognition and the

self-system. These relationships between language, internal

language, metacognition and the self-system raise many questions.

If these concepts intertwine developmentally as they appear to

do, can students raised in a bilingual environment with BICS in

both languages but CALPS in neither, acquire the necessary

conceptual language to develop a cognitive and affective

foundation for critical literacy? Is an inner voice a necessary

part of learning a language such as reading, speaking, listening,

111

and writing? Maybe, as Chomsky asserts, language acquisition and

subsequently thought acquisition is much more natural than we

think. Maybe developing critical literacy is simply a matter of

exposure to ideas. In any event, no clear indication from

research suggests instructional practices that help students

develop their inner voice.

Barriers to developing critical literacy skills are also

indicated by the research. Data shows that poverty and lower

academic achievement is correlated (Cummins, 1984; Levine, 2002).

Lack of vitamins, attention, and mental stimulation can lead to

cognitive challenges in adulthood and sometimes even impairment

(Amen, 2008). How are adult educators to know if their student

who went to school for six years in El Salvador has an impaired

brain or simply a lack of proper education? Many immigrants face

significant economic, social, cultural, and emotional challenges,

sometimes, even trauma. How much does the “immigrant experience”

affect learning, and how can instructors facilitate immigrants to

be more effective learners? These questions and others are ripe

112

for research that will aid millions of people. Until research

unearths suggestions for these questions, instructors can always

focus on the basics of communication, speaking, listening,

reading, and writing.

The development of adult English language acquisition and

critical literacy can be mutually beneficial processes. In order

to acquire critical literacy concepts, vocabulary is necessary.

In order to express critical literacy, grammar is necessary.

Critical literacy helps students to deepen their understanding of

English semantics or deep grammar and utilize more complex and

academic, CALP, language structures. Additional information about

second language acquisition and memory, motivation, and emotion

provides more illumination in the search to uncover effective

instructional practices for critical literacy.

Learning and Memory from Psycholinguistics

According to Dr. Lise Menn, providing repetition,

accommodating top-down and bottom-up processing, and helping

students develop mental models that increase memorization are

113

three main concepts from psycholinguistic studies can make

language acquisition instruction very powerful (2010). Menn

likens the brain to a “bustling metropolis full of traffic: but

when traffic gets heavy, the roads between them automatically get

better” (2010). Practice, practice, practice as the old adage

goes literally creates stronger and more automatic neural

connections (Menn, 2010; Ratey, 2001). This idea seems true at

all ages; now the ability of the brain to make new neural

connections even has a name – “plasticity” (Ratey, 2001; Amen,

2008). Menn states a virtual slogan in neuroscience: “what fires

together, wires together” (2010). Repetition is still a mainstay

of effective instruction for any subject.

Repetition is a foundational condition that predisposes

learners to top-down bottom-up processing. Because we interpret

the world based on all our past neural connections, our busiest

and strongest neural roads, these neural connections become the

filters for taking in information and define what we recognize

(Menn, 2010). Top-down processing is the process of taking in

114

information and filtering it through our personal metropolis of

neural activity. Instructors can best interact with top-down

processing by activating background knowledge, inviting multiple

experiences with the information (2010) and accommodating

cultural preferences in the classroom (Zhang, 2008; Huang &

Brown, 2009; Durkin, 2008). Additionally, utilizing real world

experiences and realia, or physical objects, also help to

facilitate top-down processing (Menn, 2010).

Bottom-up processing is also part of inputting information

into the brain. It is not enough just to identify the filter by

which people take in data, but the new substances, ideas,

experiences, and sounds also need to be rich and specific to the

learning outcome. Again providing information that attacks the

senses and makes us see, hear, taste, smell, feel, and remember

will have more likelihood of making sense and becoming memorable

(Menn, 2010).

Memory is such an important part of learning that is often

overlooked. Teachers focus a great deal on the input process of

115

learning, but not as much on the memorization of learning. For

adult ELLs memorization is very important to acquire vocabulary

and develop critical literacy. Mental models are internal multi-

sensory constructs that students make to facilitate learning

(Menn, 2010). When instructors attack a subject by activating old

neural pathways requiring students to use both top-down and

bottom-up processing they make learning easier, more effective,

and more efficient for students. Then when students take the new

information and mentally visualize or feel or hear or even

combine senses to imagine this new information in action, they

are more likely to store this memory into long-term memory (Menn,

2010; Ratey, 2001; Levine, 2002).

Top-down, bottom-up processing also helps to explain why

increasing knowledge in a student’s L1 has been shown to increase

their knowledge in L2 (Cummins,1984); Wilda, Ochoa, & Parker,

2006). When students are encouraged to read and write in their

native language, the skills of reading and writing in a second

language become more practiced or more heavily trafficked. If

116

students have developed BICS but have less developed CALPS,

talking and discussion would be a great way to develop critical

literacy. Using the strong L2 roads of listening and speaking,

students can begin to transfer ideas like making inferences and

analyzing texts to reading comprehension tests and writing

persuasive essays.

Based on this KAM investigation, adult English language

instructors teaching critical literacy have several instructional

practices to rely upon: strategy-based instruction for critical

thinking, collaborative learning, scaffolding, encouraging

metacognition and the self-system, developing phonemic and

syntactic language awareness, and providing continuous grammar

instruction. From psycholinguistics, additionally, including

instruction for top-down, bottom-up processing, repetition, and

offering multi-sensory real life experiences will also help

critical literacy skills come alive.

Memory and Adult Critical Literacy Instruction

117

Continuing with an exploration of memory and learning.

Various types of memory facilitate different types of learning.

Short-term memory, also called working memory holds information

for minutes or hours (Ratey, 2001). Long-term memory holds

information for longer than a day. Short-term memory allows us to

process old memories and new information, but like a computer’s ,

random access memory, or RAM,this information is forgotten once

time has passed (2001). Short-term memory is what allows students

to focus on the details of a story in class and pull up long-term

memories of subjects and verbs to analyze the sentences of the

story. After class, students would probably forget the story.

Long-term memory occurs when perceptual stimuli make new

neural connections and those neural connections are fired over

and over again (Ratey, 2001). Neighboring neurons also become

fired and the road to a memory becomes stronger and broader

(2001). Emotions and motivation can also help memories become

long-term files in the brain. However, research with trauma shows

that when events are excessively emotional people will disengage

118

from the memory, failing to remember the details of the event,

but instead remembering the sensory aspects of the memory (2001).

Educators want learning to become long-term memories.

Research from stroke victims shows that language is usually

located on the left side of the brain (Ratey, 2001). Right next

to the language area in the brain is a motor-sensory area, which

explains why when language neurons fire so do gesturing neurons;

hence we talk while moving our hands. Music is also near the

language area and these neurons may help us to use intonation and

rhythm with speech (2001). Sensory memories are also an aspect of

long-term memory (2001).

By offering students multi-sensory learning experiences,

information can be more easily connected, accessed, and practiced

because more neurons become wired together and fired

repetitively.

Another differentiation of long-term memory essential to

learning and to critical thinking is explicit and implicit

memory. Explicit memory is factual (Ratey, 2010). A student would

119

remember that in English subjects precede verbs. Implicit memory

allows a factual memory to be applied to skills and procedures

that become automatic (2010). Ideally, students would store

vocabulary as explicit memory and then use the vocabulary in a

common response or phrase that is produced automatically. For

example, a student might know the word scallions and say, “I

would prefer salad without any scallions please.”

Instructors recognize that students must first form explicit

memories. Then especially with making inferences, comparing and

contrasting, evaluating, synthesizing, and other critical

thinking skills, students must apply the explicit memories

implicitly to understand and express comprehension and deeper

thinking. Once grammar and critical thinking skills have

transferred to implicit memory, students would probably be

fluent.

Memory Development and Instructional Implications

Previously, declarative and procedural memory also an aspect

of long term memory, were discussed as important links to

120

language learning. Procedural learning has been found to develop

first in babies as they begin to use their body to interact with

the world (Ratey, 2010). Next semantic memory develops and allows

the thing the baby touches to have a name, mommy. Finally, memory

evolves into episodic memory, and babies can consciously recall

the dog that grabbed the ball out of their hands (2010).

Semantic and episodic types of memory are also lumped into

declarative memory (Marzano & Kendall, 2007). It is noteworthy

that the two main types of memory processing in the D/P model are

the first two types of memory to develop. Very possibly adult

second language acquisition mirrors Chomsky’s claim that all

babies are born with a language acquisition device (LAD) or

biological ability to learn language (2001).

Following the progression of infantile memory development,

it might be useful to offer more physical movement activities

with sequenced language structures. Then provide students with

stories and factual details to apply to the rote physical

experience. Total physical response (TPR) is a current

121

instructional model for second language acquisition that seems to

work very well with adults and children (Asher, 1967). TPR

utilizes this exact sequence of body, grammar sequence, plus

story with details.

Apparently, systematizing studying and studying before sleep

are great ways to help memories become long-term (Levin, 2002).

Making studying organized and routine would assist implicit

procedural memory to help repetition take place. Repetition

always helps neural pathways to become stronger. The brain needs

time to process information as well as take it in (2002). This is

why sleep, discussion, and reflection are all extremely important

for helping transfer learning into long-term memory.

Finally, the studies assessing adult ELLs’ perceptions of

their needs reported a need for grammar instruction (Iwai, 2008;

Majid, 2008). Other qualitative studies of advanced ELLs in

university classes demonstrated a need for grammar support

(Rafik-Galea & Nair, 2007; Melles, 2009; Zhang, 2008). Chomsky’s

explanation of language claims that language just happens whether

122

instruction is present or not (2001). However, the D/P model and

psycholinguistics research shows that repetition and help

connecting old neural pathways with new ones facilitates

knowledge. The fact that Chomsky points out how complex deep

grammar is (2001) provides an additional reason to provide

continuous grammar support and training as adults learn a second

language.

Memory makes learning possible and lasting. Short-term

memory allows learning to be processed and combine new

information with old. Transferring knowledge to usable long-term

memory is a chief goal of any form of education. Utilizing the

various aspects of long-term memory, explicit, implicit, sensory,

declarative, and procedural, instructors can help students

maximize learning. Long-term memory is not infallible,

unfortunately, and can become subjective over time (Ratey, 2010).

Emotions and Memory

Memories become weaker over time when they are not accessed,

subjective over time, and short-circuited by trauma (Ratey,

123

2010). Ratey notes that research shows people can completely

invent memories and even block them completely (2010). A whole

family believed they had all participated in satanic rituals

when, in fact, none ever had. A girl who believed for many years

that, when she was 14, her uncle found her mother who had

drowned in a swimming pool, found herself at age 45 screaming

with clear memories of viewing her dead mother floating face-down

in the pool (2010). She had blocked out these memories. Adult

ELLs may have had similarly tragic or traumatic events leaving

their countries and immigrating to the U.S. Their memory may be

compromised from being in a foreign place.

Emotion has been labeled “messy, complicated, primitive, and

undefined because it is all over the place intertwined with

physiology and cognition” (Ratey, 2001). Emotions are expressed

through the body and through language. We even call behavior that

goes with emotions “body language”. Emotion is significant in

this exploration of adult critical literacy acquisition and the

discussion of memory because the brain process for turning

124

perception into memory through the hippocampus is right next door

to the amygdala which simultaneously tags the information with

emotion, connecting emotion to the memory (Ratey, 2010).

The fact that long-term memory can be facilitated by emotion

(Ratey, 2010) is important for educators to consider. Hopefully,

teachers provide pleasant and happy experiences for students to

encode. However, sensitive use of other emotions may also provide

opportunities for students to apply background knowledge and file

new information with appropriate emotional tags. Certainly,

instructors should be wary of causing students embarrassment,

stress, humiliation, and anger. These types of emotions in the

classrooms would certainly qualify as traumatic and short-circuit

long-term memory and motivation for learning. Motivation as seen

previously in is useful in promoting adult ELL critical literacy

development.

Motivation and Adult Literacy

How does motivation affect learning and long-term memory?

Cannon proposed in the 1930’s that motivation was a result of a

125

person’s drive for equilibrium (Ratey, 2010). Maslow stated that

motivation arose from people striving to satisfy a hierarchy of

needs (2010). Motivation is a difficult concept to define and

measure, but is nevertheless a recognizable key component of

learning (Levine, 2002). Usually, people are motivated when they

have a desirable goal and believe they can achieve that goal

(2002).

Motivation, an internal state like emotion, could be defined

as an emotion like happiness, sadness, anger, or joy. With so

many competing forces inside human beings it is no wonder that

motivation can be undercut by low self-esteem, frustration,

pessimism, lack of tangible rewards, disenfranchisement, and past

failures (Levine, 2002). Motivation improves with success and

decreases with failure. In order to foster motivation, goals can

be broken down into achievable chunks (2002). Success with each

chunk must be acknowledged and celebrated.

Motivation arises from internal stimuli and external stimuli

(Levine, 2002). Simple satisfaction and pride from writing a

126

great essay might suffice for some students. Others really aren’t

motivated to write great essays unless they know their family or

peers will be impressed. Perhaps they will get a better job if

they get good grades and a diploma. Both internal and external

motivation can assist in learning.

Instructors can foster internal motivation by coaching adult

ELLs in goal-setting strategies, self-assessment of the goals,

and recognizing success. Students who get the opportunity in each

class to recognize their success and enjoy their progress become

more cognizant of their progress. Their neural connections for

positive motivation are strengthened and reinforced by meta-

cognitive strategies and intrapersonal intelligence.

Meta-cognition and self-assessment are wonderful bridges to

critical literacy skills. Using graphs, scaffolded writing

prompts, journaling, and self-reflective paragraphs, students can

get lots of practice reading and writing with the subject they

know best, themselves. In order to set goals students have to

gather information, synthesize it, evaluate the rewards and

127

decide yes, this is good for me. Looking at their progress,

students use comparison strategies and must summarize: ok, I have

learned x, y, z and next I need to learn a, b, and c. If

roadblocks occur, students can analyze and ask themselves what

isn’t working and what additional help they need. When goals are

achieved students have practiced critical thinking strategies and

are empowered to set new challenges for themselves.

Teaching metacognitive critical literacy strategies could

easily be applied to instruction for adult ELLs. Motivation and

emotion can enhance or hinder education. Both motivation and

emotion are primarily internal experiences that students can use

to their benefit. The previous section has examined internal

human processes that make learning critical literacy easier.

External forces must also be considered to fully understand the

challenge adult ELL students face in learning critical literacy.

The next section will show how the environment in America

supports and challenges adult English language learners.

The Immigrant Experience, Environmental Factors

128

Adult ELLs are generally taught in English as a Second

Language (ESL) classes, GED Preparation classes, community

college courses, and in higher education courses. According to

the Institute of International Education, in 2008/09 there were

over 95,000 international community college students (1.5% of the

total community college population), and over 671,000

international students at all post-secondary institutions (3.7%

of the total post-secondary student population (2010). In 2007,

there were over 1 million adult ESL students in the United States

and over another million students in GED Preparation courses

(National Center, 2010). There are definitely advanced ESL

students enrolled in GED Preparatory courses, so it is safe to

assume that there somewhere between 1 and 2 million ELL students

in ESL and GED courses across the United States. Estimating

around 20 adults are usually in these types of classrooms, about

100,000 teachers need to offer reading and writing instruction

that can help adult ELLs become successful.

129

Nationally, according to 2005 statistics, the United States

houses approximately over 40 million immigrants (Pew, 2010). One

in eight Americans is an immigrant or 12% of the total U.S.

population of 296 million people. Hispanics make up the largest

minority group, 29 million, followed by Asians, 12 million

(2010). It is estimated that between 2005 and 2050 the immigrant

population will see an 83% increase and become the majority

population in the U.S. with whites becoming the minority at 47%

(2010). Truly, the enormous social, political, economic, and

educational implications of the present and impending immigrant

populations require attention.

Immigrants are challenged in many ways and have

unfortunately faced discrimination. Instructors of adult ELLs can

develop compassion, patience, and respect for their immigrant

students when they consider the courage it takes to come to and

live in the United States. It is estimated that 12 million

undocumented immigrants work in the United States, 1 in every 20

workers (Rincon, 2009). These immigrants take low-paying jobs and

130

contribute significantly to industries such as construction,

agriculture, meatpacking, garment, and the service-industry. They

work as domestics, janitors, and landscapers (2009). Immigrants

contribute to the economy just as immigrants always have in the

United States.

It is estimated that immigrants contribute significantly to

the U.S. economy. The Social Security Administration collects

10%, $7 billion dollars, from the illegal use of social security

numbers (Rincon, 2009). Undocumented immigrants pay sales tax,

payroll tax, Medicare, unemployment, disability, pensions and get

nothing back. They increasingly pay income tax and property taxes

(2009). According the Institute of Boston, in 1997 immigrants

paid $133 billion in direct taxes to federal, state, and local

governments (2010). Why would they come and work so hard for

minimal income?

In 2006, estimates of $20 billion dollars were sent to

Mexico, mostly in increments of $300 via wire transfer (Gordon,

2010). The Bank of Mexico reportedly received $16.5 billion

131

dollars during the first 10 months of 2006 from wire American

wire-transfers (Gordon, 2010). The U.S. Census estimates that in

2007 between $10.6 and $12.8 billion dollars were transferred

outside of the United States from foreign-born households

(Greico, E., de la Cruz, P., Cortes, R., and Larsen, L. (2008).

The exact number of dollars earned in the United States by

immigrants that is sent out of the country may not be

identifiable, but it appears to be in the billions. One rationale

for the exodus of Mexican immigrants to the United States is that

Mexico because is a third-world country with international debt

minimizing its ability to adequately provide for its citizens

(Ricon, 2009). Many immigrants work very hard supporting two

countries. Economic stress is not the only challenge immigrants

face.

Families face separation. The 12 million undocumented

immigrants live in constant fear of deportation (Ricon, 2009).

Nearly 75% of all immigrants come to the U.S. to be with family

usually on a travel or student visa (Institute, 2010). Often

132

children are born to undocumented immigrants in the United

States, but that doesn’t guarantee freedom from deportation

(Rincon, 2009). Sometimes U.S. born children are incarcerated in

deportation centers with parents (2009). Such a threat of

separation from loved ones surely can make learning a struggle.

Any discussion of immigrant challenges would be incomplete

without noting prejudice and intolerance. My students have told

me directly about the discrimination they have faced from

landlords not fixing the heat to co-workers telling them they are

ruining America. Law enforcement violations also unfortunately

occur (Rincon, 2009; Southern, 2010). As the “Great Recession” of

the past years has taken hold of the United States, increasing

anti-immigrant sentiment has been reported (Southern, 2010).

Currently, the anti-immigrant group, Federal Immigrant Reform and

Enforcement (FIRE), has chapters in 33 states (2010). The

environment immigrants find themselves in is not only foreign

with different customs, culture, food, and language, but it can

be hostile and threatening.

133

The upside of being an immigrant in the United States is

that there are educational and financial opportunities. In 1996

ten high-tech firms in the U.S. contributed $28 billion to the

U.S. economy; these ten plus all other immigrant owned companies

are estimated to have contributed $29 billion to the United

States (Institute, 2010). Over 70% of immigrants arrive in the

United States over the age of 18, but still many go to school to

learn English or even acquire additional education (2010). For

generations America has become the new home for immigrants around

the world, and this tradition continues today.

Extrinsic factors such as the economy, familial stress,

integration, and prejudice can be both positively and negatively

motivating. Adult ELLs faced with this cornucopia of divergent

experiences enter classrooms ready to learn. Their future depends

on bettering themselves and they know it. With over $16 billion

dollars being sent to Mexico there are undoubtedly millions of

stories of people who “made it” in the U.S. They see the goal of

living a better life in America as an achievable goal since so

134

many other immigrants have succeeded. Some days might be easier,

some might be harder, and but given support and encouragement

adult ELLs can have successful classroom opportunities. The final

section of this Depth exploration will wrap up with possible

implications of developing a clear inner voice and summaries of

instructional implications.

Developing an Internal Voice

Developing critical literacy skills is necessary for

advanced language proficiency and success in higher education,

but maybe it is even more useful. Critical literacy skill

development helps the inner voice develop. When students have an

inner voice that can self-assess, set goals, analyze, synthesize,

and evaluate, they are more personally powerful. Consider that

students who have acquired higher order thinking skills will not

only be better students, but better employees/employers, wiser at

home, and become smarter citizens. It has already been discussed

how teaching meta-cognitive strategies using reading and writing

can help adult ELLs develop critical thinking skills. Perhaps

135

teaching meta-cognitive strategies and helping students expand

the capacity of their inner voice is just as empowering as

teaching English and critical literacy skills. Teaching people to

use critical thinking skills internally may be a necessary step

to learning critical literacy.

Instructional practices for adult ELLs

Learning English is absolutely essential for critical

literacy skill acquisition. As Chomsky pointed out with surface

grammar (2001) and Cummins noted with BICS (1984), words and

conversation precede deep grammar or CALPS. From the theory and

research in this KAM, instructional practices for teaching adults

English as a foreign language include multi-sensory exposure to

L1 (Menn, 2010; Ratey, 2001; Drewring, et al., 2007; Kovalik &

Kovalik, 2007; Morgan, 2009; Xiangming & Brand, 2009). Including

real-life experiences or taking the classroom into the real world

is also indicated (Zhou, 2009; Drewring, et. al., 2007; Xiangming

& Brand, 2009; Melles, 2009; Menn, 2010). Instruction that

accommodates or acknowledges cultural classroom preferences and

136

expectations helps top-down processing take place (Menn, 2010;

Durkin, 2008; Huang & Brown, 2009; Zhang, 2008).

In addition, teach to both declarative and procedural memory

systems by teaching content and process, what and how, phonics

and grammar (Dyson, 2009; Iwai, 2008; Marzano & Kendall, 2007;

Cummins, 1984; Chomsky, 2001; Zhang, 2008; Zhou, 2009). Lastly,

repetition, emotion, organized study, and sleep are useful

strategies for turning short-term memories into long-term ones

(Menn, 2010; Levine, 2002; Ratey, 2001).

Instructional practices for critical literacy

The same instructional implications for adult ELLs can be

utilized to teach critical literacy. Other strategies such as

collaborative learning (Kovalik & Kovalik, 2007; Majid, 2008;

Melles, 2009; Rafik-Galea & Nair, 2007; Zhang, 2008), scaffolding

(Zhang, 2008; Rafik-Galea & Nair, 2007; Morgan, 2009; Majid,

2008; Iwai, 2009; Menn, 2010) and developing metacognition

(Majid, 2008; Zhang, 2008; Rafik-Galea & Nair, 2007; Amen, 2008)

are equally beneficial and empowering. Instruction for critical

137

literacy is a natural extension of language learning allowing

students to express themselves more powerfully and creatively

through literacy and through life.

Conclusion

The literature found relating to critical literacy

acquisition indicated that instruction rich in scaffolding,

collaborative learning, and strategy-based content fostering

metacognition and critical thinking is successful for adult ELLs.

Research also indicated that accommodating the cultural

expectations of classroom etiquette helps students. Music, games,

debate, media, and weblogs are all great ways to engage adult

students using real life experiences. Students require both

morphological and syntactic, vocabulary and grammar instruction.

Encouraging students to read for personal interest and use

context clues instead of the dictionary were also implicated.

Additional information from psycholinguistics, memory processing

and development, and the relationship between memory and emotions

empowers teachers to provide students with effective

138

instructional practices that foster critical literacy. In the

next section these instructional implications are combined into a

power point presentation for adult educators at the Colorado

Teachers to Speakers of Other Languages conference.

139

APPLICATION

SBSF 8234: PROFESSIONAL PRACTICE, HUMAN DEVELOPMENT, AND

ADULT EDUCATION

Participants in this presentation about teaching critical

literacy skills to adult English language students will be given

an 11 x 17 white poster with an outline of a tree to use for

note-taking. The outline of a tree represents the interdependence

of language acquisition and the development of higher order

thinking. Theoretical information regarding language acquisition

from Lev Vygotsky, Noam Chomsky, Jim Cummins, and the

declarative/procedural model from neuroscience, will be

contrasted with the behavior descriptors for adult ESL language

acquisition stages. Additional theoretical background information

about higher order thinking will be provided from Benjamin Bloom,

Robert Marzano, John Kendall, and Art Costa and Bena Kallick.

Current research will be discussed which shows how cultural

140

definitions of higher order thinking may differ from our standard

definition of critical literacy and how instruction can be

differentiated so students can express higher order thinking in

various ways. Finally, tips for direct instruction of critical

literacy for beginning, intermediate, and advanced levels of

English language study, as well as supportive instructional

techniques will be discussed.

Developing Critical Literacy Skills

A PowerPoint presentation, which can be found as an appendix

to this application, entitled Developing Critical Literacy will

be used to guide participants. Since the 45-minute presentation

has been dubbed a “poster presentation”, participants will be

given 11x17 white posters with the outline of a tree. The tree

outline represents growth and development. The tree trunk is

symbolic of language acquisition, a necessary prerequisite of

thought (Vygotsky, 1978). The branches and leaves, which

participants will add to the poster, represent content necessary

for developing critical literacy skills. Language acquisition

141

represents branches, and leaves are the critical literacy

activities. Foliage, or abundant growth, is representative of

instructional strategies, or how to teach critical literacy

skills. Finally, fertilizer is offered as a symbol of

instructional best practices specific to adult English language

learners.

Introduction and Problem

After an introduction that explains the challenge and

relevance of teaching Cognitive Analytical Language Proficiency

Skills (CALPS) (Cummins, 2001), participants will be asked to

consider theories about second language acquisition (SLA) and

higher order thinking. Theory will be contrasted with generally

accepted ESL level descriptors, and participants will be invited

to discuss, evaluate, and hypothesize about adult language

acquisition.

Theory

Theories of language acquisition from Vygotsky, Chomsky, and

the declarative procedural model from neuroscience will be

142

discussed briefly and referred to from a handout. Similarly, to

save time and avoid repetitive information for this audience of

English language instructors, theories of higher order thinking

from Bloom, Marzano & Kendall, and Costa & Kallick, will also be

briefly introduced. Additionally, recent research by Durin

regarding cultural differences defining higher order thinking

skills will also be offered.

Instructional Strategies

Given background information about SLA and higher order

thinking, participants will be offered suggestions for content

and instructional strategies that develop critical literacy

skills.

Content useful for developing critical literacy skills would

include language acquisition skills, such as building vocabulary

and spelling ability by increasing phonemic and morphemic

awareness as well as increasing knowledge of many irregularly

spelled sight words (Marzano & Kendall, 2007). Building critical

literacy skills would include reading and writing for specific

143

purposes, such as making inferences, creating hypotheses,

evaluating, analyzing, applying, and synthesizing (Marzano &

Kendall, 2007). Participants will be asked to add to the list of

offering students multiple opportunities to express critical

literacy skills like multiple choice tests, projects, Power Point

presentations, essays, and graphic organizers.

Additionally, the possibility of scaffolding instruction,

using collaborative learning, modeling and think alouds, will be

offered as instructional strategies tailored to the specific

content aspects cited earlier. Literacy instruction mixing

reading and writing activities will also be discussed. Also,

pre-, during, and post- reading and writing activities will be

discussed as ways to encourage metacognition, independent

learning skills, reflective learning, and stimulate higher order

thinking (Alverman, Phelps, Ridgeway, 2007).

More instructional best practices specific for adult English

language learners will be explored. Asking questions to build

curiosity can help students gain motivation for learning and

144

develop metacognition. Reinforcing and celebrating success can

help foster confidence in learning. Building on BICS, instructors

can utilize student strengths to expand critical literacy skills.

Finally, remembering that emotions can play a large role in

language acquisition (Krashen, 2004), basic compassion is

necessary when instructors are working with shy, or tired

students, for example.

Sharing and Conclusion

If there is time left in the presentation, small groups of

participants will be encouraged to develop critical literacy

activities teaching making inferences for beginning,

intermediate, and advanced levels of English. The presentation

will conclude with group sharing and a review of the main points

of the presentation.

Handouts shall include: outline of the presentation,

presenter biography, table of language acquisition theory, ESL

level descriptors, participant evaluation rubric, the tree

outline poster, and references.

145

Discussion

Some people say the more you know, the less you know. That

axiom definitely epitomizes my experience with the theory and

research in this KAM. I chose the theorists Vygotsky and Chomsky

because not only had they written about language acquisition but

they had also written about the role language plays with thought.

The main emphasis of this KAM on human development is to explore

the relationship between language development and the development

of higher order thinking skills. Jim Cummins’ theory about

BICS/CALPS is not about the development of general language

acquisition and thought, but rather the development of specific

language and thought related to higher order thinking. Vygotsky,

Chomsky, and Cummins all offer ideas that help to explain the

development of language, higher order thinking and ultimately

critical literacy guiding instructional practices.

The theories studied in this KAM have offered more than

recent research has. Most of the research found done by educators

was not true research but reports about instructional practices

146

that seemed to have good results. Some of the research was more

qualitative and lacked honest research integrity. The two studies

that were significant were the study about cultural

interpretations of higher order thinking in Asia by Durkin

(2008), and Kovalik & Kovalik’s (2007) qualitative study of what

they called gaming to stimulate critical literacy for adult

English language learners.

Additionally, I turned to neuroscience for valid scientific

studies about second language acquisition. This turn in focus to

neuroscience uncovered a plethora of research on the subject of

thought and language that I could spend years investigating.

However, as the editor of Cristina Sanz

wrote[citation?], many of the neuroscience studies on second

language acquisition are contrived or heavily influenced to

control variables in the study. It seems people in classrooms are

difficult to study because of the multitudes of variables

affecting learning. The main find in a perusal of second language

acquisition research from neuroscience is the

147

Declarative/Procedural Model which helps to explain the debate in

English language instruction about teaching implicitly or

explicitly, as well as supports Chomsky’s theories of lexical

mind, grammar mind, and universal grammar. As discussed earlier

in the Breadth section, declarative memory facilitates facts and

learning through context. Lexical mind assimilates the

superficial meaning of grammar through fact knowledge, and

universal grammar states that language is learned through

context. Procedural memory facilitates the learning of sequence

and structure, which in turn helps the grammar mind to gain

knowledge of language sequence and interpret deep grammar.

Theoretical Insights

In sum, Cummins’ BICS/CALPS theory clearly poses the problem

this KAM seeks answers for: how do you teach people to develop

critical literacy skills when they haven’t exhibited the ability

to express higher order thinking skills in their first or second

language? In order to answer that question, Vygotsky offers

insightful awareness of the seemingly obvious that language from

148

a mother is necessary to stimulate inner speech and that language

development is necessary for thought development, but language

and thought are interdependent phenomena (Vygotsky, 1978). Also

Vygotsky’s Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD) suggests that in

order to teach critical literacy, students must be challenged at

a comfortable level and if they are truly in their ZPD, they

won’t be able to express critical literacy with 100% accuracy.

Chomsky’s theories about the lexical mind, grammar mind, and

universal grammar point mirror BICS and CALPS in certain ways

because according to Chomsky there is a superficial semantical

meaning and a deep semantical meaning when one has mastery over

the grammar of a language. To that end, students who have BICS

just haven’t yet acquired the grammar necessary to develop CALPS.

Universal grammar, which posits that all languages share aspects

of basic grammar constructs, supports the research Cummins’ puts

forth identifying BICS/CALPS in various languages (Cummins,

1984). All in all, Chomsky’s theories make a good case for more

149

language and grammar instruction in order to develop critical

literacy skills.

The D/P Model echoes Chomsky’s lexical mind and grammar mind

with scientific research done on people with a variety of brain

dysfunction affecting long-term memory, such as Alzheimer’s’

patients and amnesiacs. Studies on declarative memory show that

it is used more prevalently in children and women, and its use

decreases as people age. Declarative memory may pick up

irregularities and nuances in language and being involved in

episodic memory and factual memory, the instructional implication

would be as follows: teach contextually, teach implicitly.

Procedural memory has more to do with rote automatic

responses and sequences. Procedural memory increases in use with

age and men tend to use procedural memory more than declarative.

Explicit instruction is implied by the role procedural memory

plays in SLA, because explicit instruction is more direct and

more practicable than implicit instruction. From the D/P model we

learn that teaching critical literacy may need both implicit and

150

explicit instruction, and probably more explicit instruction with

adult learners who need to increase their command of grammar.

Vygotsky: Thought and Language Interdependence

According to Vygotsky, external speech is necessary to

produce the development of internal “verbal thought” (1986, p.

212). This idea means that teaching critical literacy is

dependent on language acquisition. If students don’t have

language to express comparing and contrasting, they probably

won’t internally compare and contrast things. With most adults,

we can assume that they have the internal speech for comparing

and contrasting in their first language (L1) and therefore if

they learn the words but and more they can express “I like corn,

but I like cake more.”

With adults, who tend to be young adults, and who have not

yet acquired the internal speech for higher order thinking, but

they have BICS and very large vocabulary, just a little direct

instruction and experience with listing foods they like and foods

they don’t like could be used to teach them academic vocabulary,

151

such as however, although, moreover, additionally, and

nevertheless. For example: “I like carrots; however, peas are my

favorite vegetable. Although I don’t like carrots, I eat them

when they are cooked. Moreover, carrots and peas together taste

ok. Additionally, I like corn but not okra. Nevertheless, when my

grandmother in New Orleans cooks gumbo, I will eat okra.”

Vygotsky reminds us that language is necessary for the

development of thought. Language and specific vocabulary

instruction is necessary for the development of critical

literacy.

Zone of Proximal Development

Another hallmark educational theory that Vygotsky is

credited for is the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD) (1978).

Most test and instruction assume that if the student accurately

performs and produces instructional objectives that the student

has learned. Vygotsky points out that the “learning” exhibited in

the production of stated outcomes is the mental development level

representing learning retrospectively. However, just because

152

students don’t produce instructional objectives, doesn’t mean

they are not learning. If they produce or exhibit the stated

objectives to some degree the student is experiencing dynamic

mental development. The implication for teaching critical

literacy for adults is that it is important to remind adults that

even when the activity like writing is difficult in the second

language (L2), they are learning. It is also useful to recognize

that partial success with expressing critical thinking in reading

or writing is still evidence of learning.

In terms of CALPS acquisition, ZPD theory reminds us that

students haven’t acquired the concept of abstract ideas like

hypothesize or infer or evaluate in their L1. In order to

activate CALPS, students must have enough L2 vocabulary to

acquire these abstract concepts in L2. Basically, it might just

take longer for students with CALP challenges to become

proficient with critical literacy, underscoring the need for

English language instruction as well as higher order thinking

activities. ZPD helps to explain why students who have acquired

153

higher order thinking skills in their L1 find it much easier to

express critical literacy because higher order thinking for them

has become mental development and their ZPD for critical literacy

in L2 requires less conceptual information than adults who are

CALP challenged.

Collaborative Learning

Another instructional strategy useful for teaching critical

literacy skills is collaborative learning (Marzano & Kendall,

2007). Vygotsky also championed the need for human interaction in

order to learn (1978). Since our internal language, or verbal

thought, originates from our mother and other people around us,

it follows that we acquire most of our information from other

people and our environment. Indeed, language is the tool of

communication so it only makes sense that we interact with other

people in order to learn. Examples of collaborative learning to

assist in the development of critical thinking include: partner

work to fill out a Venn diagram; a class debate; and creating

hypotheses about characters in small groups.

154

Chomsky and Universal Grammar

An idea that Chomsky has become famous for is universal

grammar, or the similarity of grammars amongst languages (2006,

p. 40). Chomsky argues that although superficial grammar might

vary from language to language, the deeper structures for making

meaning out of language are very similar (2006). What does this

have to do with critical literacy? Universal grammar suggests

explains that students with CALPS in their L1 will begin to

understand immediately on a deep level once they get a whiff or a

slight glimmer that the instructor wants them to infer, evaluate,

or synthesize the information. These students already have the

deep idea of synthesize and even a word for it in their L1.

Universal grammar also suggests that students who are CALP

challenged probably have a deep idea and even experiences of

synthesizing, they just might not recognize it as a literacy

skill. For instance when students go to fix a car you check out

the situation (gather information), try to change or replace

something (based on the hypothesis that a part is faulty), and

155

hopefully keep trying various things until the car runs

(evaluation and further hypotheses). Universal grammar suggests

that adult students who demonstrate CALP challenges may not have

analytical higher order thinking challenges in all aspects of

life, but maybe they do with literacy.

Surface Grammar and Deep Grammar

Chomsky also elaborates on universal grammar, theorizing

that grammar is composed of several aspects, two being surface

grammar and deep grammar (2006, p. 111). Surface grammar is the

mechanism that creates meaning from phonemic structures; deep

grammar creates semantic meaning. The correlation here between

BICS and CALPS is quite clear. Students who have BICS could be

said to have good command of surface grammar but not of deep

grammar. Examining surface grammar and deep grammar further, it

seems that critical literacy skills require possibly more deep

grammar. Still the connection between possibly undeveloped deep

grammar and lack of CALPS and critical literacy is merely

explanation.

156

Finally, two other mental abilities associated with Chomsky

are metal lexicon, the ability to learning irregularities in

language such as phrasal verbs, and mental grammar, or the

ability to learn the regular use of language, the rules of

language (Sanz, 2004). Mental lexicon and mental grammar explain

the language acquisition process. Whereas, surface grammar and

deep grammar explain how people interpret the language they

acquire. Mental lexicon and mental grammar will be important in

guiding instructional strategy for critical literacy as evidenced

by the D/P Model.

D/P Model

The Declarative/Procedural Model attributed to Ullman (Sanz,

2004), offers a possible mechanism that language is stored in

long-term memory. Both the declarative and procedural memory

systems store different types of long-term memories. According to

Ullman, the declarative system is used in low proficiency L2

acquisition more than the procedural system. However, the

procedural system is more useful in acquiring grammar at higher

157

levels (Sanz, 2004). Instructionally, one interpretation of the

D/P model is that declarative memory can be served by contextual

or implicit instruction. Procedural memory would best be

supported by explicit or direct instruction. If Ullman is correct

and declarative memory is used more prevalently at lower levels

of L2 acquisition, then beginning and intermediate levels of

English language instruction should have a significant component

of contextual learning. It follows more advanced levels of

instruction would do better with more direct or explicit

instruction.

According to the D/P model, the declarative memory system is

more active under the influence of the hormone estrogen (Sanz,

2004); therefore, women would tend to learn better with

contextual instruction. Also young female adults, this being the

main population that might not have developed CALPs, might need

more contextual instruction and males might need more explicit

instruction.

Culturally Defined Higher Order Thinking

158

While we are striving to make students here in the U.S.

think more analytically because emerging technologies are

changing and acquiring data to memorize is not seen as important

as being able to navigate data (Marzano & Kendall, 2007), other

cultures seem to have different definitions of higher order

thinking (Durkin, 2008). In Durkin’s qualitative study using

grounded theory case methodology, over 4 years in several Asian

countries, found that Chinese and Asian students found the

Socratic method of dialogue and debate uncomfortable. The Asian

students were used to respecting the teacher by listening and

experiencing the effect of the instruction using their mental

digestive properties to learn. Durkin concluded that it was

Chinese higher order thinking to say less with communication and

learn more. The point of the matter being for critical literacy

skill development that students from other cultures, particularly

Asian cultures for sure, may have trouble expressing critical

literacy in front of other students or the instructor. They may

need other outlets to demonstrate the ability to read and write

159

critically, perhaps through multiple choice tests, essays,

videos, or individual conversations.

Conclusion

Theories on second language acquisition, a model from

neuroscience, and research pointing out cultural semantics, help

give this presentation some rich ideas for instructional

strategies, which develop critical literacy skills. Language

instruction and critical literacy activities need to include

vocabulary and enhance phonemic and morphemic awareness in order

to promote superficial grammar, which is necessary for deep

grammar. Instructors of adult English language learners

challenged by CALPS would do well to check student background

knowledge, especially if they have BICS in L2, for evidence of

higher order thinking skills outside of literacy skills.

Instructors should also remember that if students are not

expressing critical literacy skills perfectly they are still

learning. Finally, by using a mix of contextual and direct

instruction instructors may optimally facilitate long-term

160

memory. Research-based instructional best practices for

developing critical literacy along with contextual and explicit

instruction is indicated for helping students develop CALPS.

161

REFERENCES

ACT. (2011, January). American College Testing

information. Retrieved from

http://www.actstudent.org/testprep/index.html

Amen, D. (2008). Magnificent mind at any age. New York: Harmony Books.

Alverman, D., Phelps, S., Ridgeway, V. (2007). Content area reading

and literacy. Boston: Pearson.

Asher, J., & Price, B. (1967). The learning strategy of the total

physical response: some age differences. Child Development,

38(4), 1219. Retrieved from Education Research Complete

database.

Aukerman, M. (2007). A culpable CALP: Rethinking the

conversational/academic language proficiency distinction in

162

early literacy instruction. Reading Teacher, 60(7), 626-635.

doi:10.1598/RT.60.7.3

CASAS. (2011, January). Comprehensive Adult Student Assessment

Systems ESL level descriptors. Retrieved from

https://www.casas.org/home/index.cfm?

fuseaction=home.showContent&MapID=1720

College Board, (2011, January). Preparing for the

test. Retrieved from

http://www.collegeboard.com/student/testing/ps

at/prep.ht

163

164

Colorado Mountain College, (2010). Update with Krisan Crow- ESL student

statistics. Aspen, CO: DES/IVS meeting.

Chomsky, N. (2006). Language and mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge

University Press.

Chomsky, N. (2000). New horizons in the study of language

and mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University

Press.

Costa, A., Kallick, B. (2000). Activating and engaging

habits of mind. Alexandria, VA: Association for

Supervision and Curriculum Development.

Cummins, J. (1984). Bilingualism and special education: Issues

in assessment and pedagogy. Clevedon, England:

Multilingual Matters.

Cummins, J. (1986). Empowering minority students: A framework for

165

intervention.  Harvard Educational Review, 56, 18-36.

Gordon, J. (2010, November 22). Working immigrants. Retrieved from

http://www.workingimmigrants.com/2006/01/how_mexicans_in_us_

send_money.html

Grant-Vallone, E., Kelly, R., Umali, C., Pohlert, E. (2003) An

analysis of the effects of self-esteem, social support, and

participation in student support services on students’

adjustment and commitment to college. Journal of College Student

Retention: Research, Theory & Practice, 5(3), 255-274. Retrieved from

EBSCOhost.

Greico, E., de la Cruz, P., Cortes, R., Larsen, L. (2008). Who in

the United States Sends and Receives Remittances? An

Initial Analysis of the Monetary Transfer Data from the

August 2008 CPS Migration Supplement. U.S. Census Bureau Working

Paper No. 87 . Retrieved from

http://www.census.gov/population/www/documentation/twps0087/

166

twps0087.pdf

International Institute of Boston. (2010, November 22). Immigrant

facts. Retrieved from

http://www.iiboston.org/resources/faq.htm

Institute of International (2010). Statistics on international

students in the U.S. Retrieved from

http://www.iie.org/search.aspx?q=international%20community

%20college%20statistics

LE, C., & KAZIS, R. (2009). Educating All Learners

for the New Economy. New England Journal of

Higher Education, 23(3), 14-15. Retrieved from

EBSCOhost.

Leno, L., & Dougherty, L. (2007). Using direct

instruction to teach content vocabulary.

Science Scope, 31(1), 63-66. Retrieved from

167

EBSCOhost.

Levine, M. (2002). A mind at a time. New York: Simon and Schuster.

Long, Michael. (2000). Linguistics. In Routledge Encyclopedia of

Language Teaching & Learning. Retrieved from

www.eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/recordDetail?accno=ED454712

Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). Linguistic and cognitive mechanisms in

foreign vocabulary acquisition. Dissertation Abstracts

International, 68, Retrieved from PsycINFO database.

Krashen, S. (2004). The power of reading. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann

Marzano Research laboratory, (2010, November 10). About Marzano.

Retrieved from http://www.marzanoresearch.com/site/

Marzano, R., Pickering, D., Pollock, J. (2001). Classroom instruction

that works. Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and

Curriculum Development.

168

McFarlin, I. (2007). Do school teacher parents make a

difference?. Economics of Education Review, 26(5), 615-628.

doi:10.1016/j.econedurev.2006.07.011

National Center for Educational Statistics. (2010). Adult ELL

student populations statistics. Retrieved from

http://nces.ed.gov/fastfacts/display.asp?id=4

Paul, R., Elder, L., Bartlett, T. (2011, January).

A Brief History of the Idea of Critical

Thinking. Retrieved from

http://www.criticalthinking.org/aboutCT/briefH

istoryCT.cfm

Papafragou, A. (2002). Mindreading and verbal

communication. Mind & Language 38 (1-2)

Feb/April, pp55-67. Retrieved from

http://papafragou.psych.udel.edu/papers/mind.p

df

169

Pew Research Center. (2010). Population projections. Retrieved from

http://pewsocialtrends.org/2008/02/11/us-population-

projections-2005-2050/

Ratey, J. (2001). A user’s guide to the brain. New York: Pantheon Books.

Rincon, A. (2009). Undocumented immigrants and higher education. New

York: LFB Scholarly Publishing.

Roessingh, H. (2008). Variability in ESL Outcomes:

The Influence of Age on Arrival and Length of

Residence on Achievement in High School. TESL

Canada Journal, 26(1), 87-107. Retrieved from

EBSCOhost.

Roessingh, H., Pat, K., & Watt, D. (2005).

Developing Cognitive Academic Language

Proficiency: The Journey. TESL Canada Journal,

23(1), 1-27. Retrieved from EBSCOhost.

170

Sparks, S. D. (2007). Public or private, parents

hold key to student success. Education Daily,

40(186), 2. Retrieved from EBSCOhost.

Southern Poverty Law Center, 2010. 2009 report on anti-

immigrant sentiment. Retrieved from

http://www.splcenter.org/get-informed/intellig

ence-report/browse-all-issues/2009/spring

Tovani, C. (2000). I read but I don’t get it. Portland, ME: Stenhouse

Publishers.

University College London (2005, June 15). Adults Can Be

Retrained To Learn Second Languages More Easily, Says UCL

Scientist.

Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in Society: The Development of

Higher Thought Processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard

University Press.

Vygotsky, L. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge,

MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

171

Wagner, T. (2008, November 12). Teaching and

Testing the Skills That Matter Most. Education

Week. p. 30. Retrieved from EBSCOhost.

Wamba, N. (2007). Students with Learning

Disabilities Leaving School: Implications for

Educational Leadership Programs. Insights on

Learning Disabilities, 4(1), 39-50. Retrieved

from EBSCOhost.

Zhang, T., Tianguang, Gao & Gailring, Zhang, W.

(2007). Using online discussion forums to

assist a traditional English class. International

Journal on E-Learning 6(4), 623-643

172

173

APPENDIX

Colorado Teachers to Speakers of Other Languages, COTESOL, Feedback

Overall, participant comments after the presentation were very positive suggesting that the depth of the presentation content illuminated knowledge of adult language acquisition. One very experienced instructor told me, “You helped remind us that we are teaching not only language acquisition but communication.”There were about 35 participants. Unfortunately, only about 8 participant rubrics were completed. All participants reported their understanding of second language acquisition and higher order thinking were somewhat improved and the majority reported improvement. A few even reported significant improvement.

Main themes of areas for improvement are timing, and more examples of instructional applications.

1. Theme: Condense presentation

Action plan for theme 1:a. Insist on 1.5 hours instead of 45 minutes. b. Provide a handout with theories. c. Ask for audience participation with theories and talk about how the theories directly relate to critical literacy. Complete by April 1 and pilot at the 2011 Colorado Mountain College DevEd Summit.

2. Theme: Increase numbers of instructional examples.

a. After the jigsaw exercise for graphic organizers where participants develop instructional plans for 4 different ELL levels, the same jigsaw leveled design will be used to discuss debate.

174

b. After discussion of scaffolding give a short matching quiz with a PowerPoint slide to match movie critique scaffolded instruction for the 4 ELL levels.

Complete by April 1 and pilot at the 2011 Colorado Mountain College DevEd Summit.