14
Stress, glucocorticoids and liquorice in human pregnancy: Programmers of the offspring brain K. RA ¨ IKKO ¨ NEN 1 , J. R. SECKL 2 , A.-K. PESONEN 1 , A. SIMONS 3 , & B. R. H. VAN DEN BERGH 3 1 Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland, 2 Centre for Cardiovascular Science, The Queen’s Medical Research Institute, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK, and 3 Developmental Psychology, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands (Received 18 January 2011; revised 4 April 2011; accepted 4 May 2011) Abstract A suboptimal prenatal environment may induce permanent changes in cells, organs and physiology that alter social, emotional and cognitive functioning, and increase the risk of cardiometabolic and mental disorders in subsequent life (“developmental programming”). Although animal studies have provided a wealth of data on programming and its mechanisms, including on the role of stress and its glucocorticoid mediators, empirical evidence of these mechanisms in humans is still scanty. We review the existing human evidence on the effects of prenatal maternal stress, anxiety and depression, glucocorticoids and intake of liquorice (which inhibits the placental barrier to maternal glucocorticoids) on offspring developmental outcomes including, for instance, alterations in psychophysiological and neurocognitive functioning and mental health. This work lays the foundations for biomarker discovery and affords opportunities for prevention and interventions to ameliorate adverse outcomes in humans. Keywords: Child, glucocorticoids, human, liquorice, mood, offspring Introduction Mounting epidemiological evidence suggests that smaller body size at birth and/or a shorter length of gestation increase the risk of poorer physical health later in life (Barker et al. 1989; Eriksson et al. 2006, 2007). The brain is particularly affected, manifesting as altered cognitive and affective functioning and as an increased risk of mental health disorders. These associations may not be linear, as children born at the upper end of the birth weight distribution may also be at a higher risk for poorer health later in life (Gunnell et al. 2003). These findings can be best understood within the framework of prenatal “programming” or developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD; Barker 1998). According to the DOHaD paradigm, adverse environmental experiences during critical periods of early development can permanently alter or programme the structure and function of cells, organs and physiological systems. Recent research has shown that the influences of programming may not be limited to the prenatal period, but may extend across the entire human growth period from preconception to adulthood (Barker et al. 2005). The effects of programming are not limited to development in childhood, but are influential across the life span (Barker et al. 1989; Eriksson et al. 2006, 2007; Ra ¨ikko ¨nen et al. 2007, 2008a, 2009a). Recently, much interest has been focused on whether developmental programming—as reflected in alterations in body size at birth and length of gestation—plays a role in determining individual differences in cognitive, social and emotional func- tioning, including characteristic traits of temperament and personality, in neuroendocrine and autonomic Correspondence: K. Ra ¨ikko ¨nen, Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, PO Box 9, 00014 University of Helsinki, Finland. Tel: 358 9 19129501. Fax: 358 9 19129521. E-mail: katri.raikkonen@helsinki.fi Stress, November 2011; 14(6): 590–603 q Informa Healthcare USA, Inc. ISSN 1025-3890 print/ISSN 1607-8888 online DOI: 10.3109/10253890.2011.602147 Stress Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Universiteit Van Tilburg on 03/13/13 For personal use only.

Stress, glucocorticoids and liquorice in human pregnancy: Programmers of the offspring brain

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Stress, glucocorticoids and liquorice in human pregnancy:Programmers of the offspring brain

K. RAIKKONEN1, J. R. SECKL2, A.-K. PESONEN1, A. SIMONS3,

& B. R. H. VAN DEN BERGH3

1Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland, 2Centre for Cardiovascular Science, The Queen’s

Medical Research Institute, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK, and 3Developmental Psychology, Tilburg University,

Tilburg, The Netherlands

(Received 18 January 2011; revised 4 April 2011; accepted 4 May 2011)

AbstractA suboptimal prenatal environment may induce permanent changes in cells, organs and physiology that alter social, emotionaland cognitive functioning, and increase the risk of cardiometabolic and mental disorders in subsequent life (“developmentalprogramming”). Although animal studies have provided a wealth of data on programming and its mechanisms, including onthe role of stress and its glucocorticoid mediators, empirical evidence of these mechanisms in humans is still scanty. We reviewthe existing human evidence on the effects of prenatal maternal stress, anxiety and depression, glucocorticoids and intake ofliquorice (which inhibits the placental barrier to maternal glucocorticoids) on offspring developmental outcomes including,for instance, alterations in psychophysiological and neurocognitive functioning and mental health. This work lays thefoundations for biomarker discovery and affords opportunities for prevention and interventions to ameliorate adverseoutcomes in humans.

Keywords: Child, glucocorticoids, human, liquorice, mood, offspring

Introduction

Mounting epidemiological evidence suggests that

smaller body size at birth and/or a shorter length of

gestation increase the risk of poorer physical health

later in life (Barker et al. 1989; Eriksson et al. 2006,

2007). The brain is particularly affected, manifesting

as altered cognitive and affective functioning and as

an increased risk of mental health disorders. These

associations may not be linear, as children born at the

upper end of the birth weight distribution may also be

at a higher risk for poorer health later in life (Gunnell

et al. 2003). These findings can be best understood

within the framework of prenatal “programming” or

developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD;

Barker 1998). According to the DOHaD paradigm,

adverse environmental experiences during critical

periods of early development can permanently alter

or programme the structure and function of cells,

organs and physiological systems. Recent research has

shown that the influences of programming may not be

limited to the prenatal period, but may extend across

the entire human growth period from preconception

to adulthood (Barker et al. 2005). The effects of

programming are not limited to development in

childhood, but are influential across the life span

(Barker et al. 1989; Eriksson et al. 2006, 2007;

Raikkonen et al. 2007, 2008a, 2009a).

Recently, much interest has been focused on

whether developmental programming—as reflected

in alterations in body size at birth and length of

gestation—plays a role in determining individual

differences in cognitive, social and emotional func-

tioning, including characteristic traits of temperament

and personality, in neuroendocrine and autonomic

Correspondence: K. Raikkonen, Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, PO Box 9, 00014 University of Helsinki, Finland.Tel: 358 9 19129501. Fax: 358 9 19129521. E-mail: [email protected]

Stress, November 2011; 14(6): 590–603q Informa Healthcare USA, Inc.ISSN 1025-3890 print/ISSN 1607-8888 onlineDOI: 10.3109/10253890.2011.602147

Stre

ss D

ownl

oade

d fr

om in

form

ahea

lthca

re.c

om b

y U

nive

rsite

it V

an T

ilbur

g on

03/

13/1

3Fo

r pe

rson

al u

se o

nly.

nervous system (ANS) functioning and in mental

health. A series of studies have shown that smaller

body size at birth and/or shorter length of gestation

associate, for instance, with alterations in hypothala-

mic–pituitary–adrenocortical (HPA) axis and ANS

functioning during daily living and under psychosocial

stress (Reynolds et al. 2001; IJzerman et al. 2003;

Wust et al. 2005; Jones et al. 2006, 2007; Phillips and

Jones 2006; Feldt et al. 2007; Kajantie et al. 2007) and

with increased risks of depression (Raikkonen et al.

2007, 2008b), suicide (Barker et al. 1995), schizo-

phrenia (Wahlbeck et al. 2001), personality disorders

(Lahti et al. 2010b), schizotypal personality traits

(Lahti et al. 2009), hostility (Raikkonen et al. 2008a),

behavioural symptoms of attention deficit/hyperactiv-

ity disorder (Lahti et al. 2006; Strang-Karlsson et al.

2008b; Heinonen et al. 2010), poorer cognitive

functioning (Gale et al. 2004; Heinonen et al. 2008;

Broekman et al. 2009; Raikkonen et al. 2009a)

internalizing and externalizing behaviour problems

(Schlotz et al. 2008), poorer sleep (Strang-Karlsson

et al. 2008a; Pesonen et al. 2009) and negative and

positive affectivity characteristics of temperament and

personality (Pesonen et al. 2006, 2008a, Schlotz et al.

2008, Schmidt et al. 2008; Lahti et al. 2010a). These

associations may not be linear as children born with

higher birth weights may display higher levels of

anxiety (Lahti et al. 2010a) and distress (Cheung

2002), and be at an increased risk for psychosis later in

life (Gunnell et al. 2003). The extent to which the

associations with higher birth weight reflect maternal

obesity, poorer health and pregnancy disorders, such

as maternal diabetes/glucose intolerance, is not clear.

In addition to the original empirical studies, several

papers exist that have reviewed the evidence linking

smaller body size at birth and/or shorter length of

gestation with psychological and psychophysiological

functioning and mental health later in life (Shenkin

et al. 2004; Gluckman et al. 2005, 2008, 2009; Seckl

and Meaney 2006; Seckl and Holmes 2007; Hanson

and Gluckman 2008; Kajantie 2008; Seckl 2008;

Raikkonen and Pesonen 2009; Kajantie and Raikko-

nen 2010).

Despite the increasing evidence pointing to the

importance of early life programming in inducing

alterations in psychological and psychophysiological

functioning and mental health, the mechanisms

through which these prenatal influences operate

remain largely unknown. Two major hypotheses have

been advanced to explain the link between events

in utero and the later risk of neuropsychiatric and

cardiometabolic disorders: maternal malnutrition and

foetal overexposure to glucocorticoid stress hormones

(Barker 1991; Edwards et al. 1993). These notions

have been extensively explored in preclinical studies.

In a range of experimental species, maternal malnu-

trition (global undernutrition or selective protein

deficiency) reduces birth weight and reliably leads to

higher blood pressure, glucose levels, altered beha-

viour and HPA axis function in adult offspring

(Warner and Ozanne 2010). Similarly, maternal stress

or administration of glucocorticoids, such as dexa-

methasone or betamethasone that freely crosses the

placenta, also reduces birth weight and produces a

similar or identical phenotype in the adult offspring

(Seckl and Holmes 2007). Similar effects occur in

non-human primates exposed to glucocorticoids in

the last half of gestation (de Vries et al. 2007). The two

hypotheses may be linked. Thus, circulating levels of

physiological glucocorticoids (cortisol and corticos-

terone) are much higher in the maternal than in the

foetal blood. This gradient is ensured by a placental

enzyme, 11b-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2

(11b-HSD2), that catalyzes the rapid inactivation of

glucocorticoids to their inert 11-keto forms (corti-

sone, 11-dehydrocorticosterone), thus forming a

physiological “barrier” to maternal glucocorticoids.

Liquorice contains compounds, such as glycyrrhizic

acid and glycyrrhetinic acid, that potently inhibit 11b-

HSDs. Maternal administration of these agents or the

derived drug carbenoxolone (Lindsay et al. 1996a,b)

or genetic knock out of the enzyme (Holmes et al.

2006) reduces birth weight and generates similar

programmed CNS and peripheral outcomes in the

adult offspring. Moreover, maternal protein malnu-

trition selectively lowers placental 11b-HSD2 (Lang-

ley-Evans et al. 1996), affording a possible link

between the proposed mechanisms. Maternal stress in

rodents also downregulates placental 11b-HSD2

(Mairesse et al. 2007), perhaps delimited by genotype

(Lucassen et al. 2009), suggesting a double hit of

elevated maternal glucocorticoids and reduced pla-

cental barrier function.

Despite this increasingly coherent body of evidence

in model organisms, understanding the key mechan-

isms of programming in humans has been much less

explored. Figure 1 outlines the mechanisms that are

potentially involved. A series of human studies have

demonstrated that prenatal environmental adversities,

such as exposure to malnutrition (Roseboom et al.

2001), maternal pre-pregnancy overweight (Rodri-

guez 2010), pregnancy disorders, such as hyperten-

sion (Tuovinen et al. 2010, 2011), tobacco (Cornelius

and Day 2009; Espy et al. 2011; Heinonen et al.

2011), cannabis (El Marroun et al. 2009) and alcohol

exposure (Hellemans et al. 2010), have the potential

to induce alterations in the offspring growth, birth

anthropometry, development and adult functions. In

this paper, we concentrate on reviewing the existing

human evidence of programming induced by prenatal

maternal stress, depression and anxiety, maternal

glucocorticoid therapy and intake of liquorice during

pregnancy.

Stress, glucocorticoids and liquorice in human pregnancy 591

Stre

ss D

ownl

oade

d fr

om in

form

ahea

lthca

re.c

om b

y U

nive

rsite

it V

an T

ilbur

g on

03/

13/1

3Fo

r pe

rson

al u

se o

nly.

Exposure to prenatal maternal stress, depression and

anxiety: Overview of results of studies, examining offspring

behaviour with questionnaires, standardized observations,

neurocognitive tasks, EEG, MRI and fMRI

Prenatal maternal stress, depression and anxiety may

exert effects on offspring development through

multiple pathways. These pathways, which include

alterations, for instance, in maternal lifestyle, physio-

logical stress-regulatory mechanisms and placental

function, are outlined in Figure 1 and described in

more detail in previous reviews (O’Regan et al. 2001;

Seckl 2001, 2008; Seckl and Meaney 2004b; Van den

Bergh et al. 2005b; Seckl and Holmes 2007).

In humans, prospective follow-up studies have

provided accumulating evidence for an association

between prenatal exposure to high levels of maternal

stress, anxiety and depression and altered neurodeve-

lopment in the offspring at stages ranging from birth

up to 20 years of age. Importantly, these associations

persisted after controlling for postnatal maternal

Fetal genomeand epigenome

Maternalnutrition

Maternalstress

Pregnancydisorders

Placentalfunction

Fetal programming leading to life-long changes in:

Organ structureand function- Muscle tissue- Fat tissue- Pancreas- Liver- Brain

Hormonalfunction/axes - HPAA- Thyroid

Autonomicnervoussystem

Neurotransmittersystems

Glucose metabolismBody compositionLipid metabolismInsulin sensitivity

Stress responsiveness

Other factors(e.g., hormonal,growth factors)

Nutrient andoxygen

availability

Sympathoadrenalfunction

Glucocorticoidexposure

HPAA andAutonomic

nervous systemfunction

Physical health, e.g.,CHD

StrokeType 2 diabetes

Mental health Other stress-related bodily

disorders

Other healthoutcomes

- Social, emotional, cognitive functioning- Temperemant- Personality- Behavior

Figure 1. Mechanisms involved in the offspring programming. (Modified from Raikkonen et al. 2008b).

K. Raikkonen et al.592

Stre

ss D

ownl

oade

d fr

om in

form

ahea

lthca

re.c

om b

y U

nive

rsite

it V

an T

ilbur

g on

03/

13/1

3Fo

r pe

rson

al u

se o

nly.

mood and/or other potentially important pre- and

postnatal confounders, such as alcohol consumption

and smoking, during pregnancy, birth weight and

socio-economic status, and in some studies also use of

medication, such as antidepressants, have been taken

into account. Results of the studies reviewed here are

described in Table I. The studies are organized by the

age of the offspring at the final follow-up assessment.

In newborn babies, maternal prenatal stress was

associated with neurodevelopmental alterations

manifesting as less optimal scores on the Brazelton

Neonatal Assessment Scale (Brouwers et al. 2001;

Rieger et al. 2004; Hernandez-Reif et al. 2006) and on

quantitative neurological examination (Lou et al.

1994). The newborn babies also displayed higher

cardiac vagal tone, lower Apgar score (Ponirakis et al.

1998), less optimal behavioural states (Van den Bergh

1990, 1992) and higher cortisol levels 1 week

postpartum (Diego et al. 2004). In the study of

Harvison et al. (2009), neonates of mothers scoring

lower on perinatal anxiety displayed more negative

frontal slow wave amplitudes in response to their

mother’s voice compared to a female stranger’s voice,

while neonates of mothers scoring higher on perinatal

anxiety showed the opposite pattern. The latter results

indicate that maternal anxiety may induce neurophy-

siologically based differences in attentional allocation,

processes known to be crucial to an infant’s ability to

maintain homoeostasis.

Infants of mothers scoring higher on prenatal stress

were rated by an observer as having poorer inter-

actions with their mother (Field et al. 1985), being

more irritable (DiPietro et al. 2008) and reactive

(Davis et al. 2004, 2007), having problems with

regulation of attention (Huizink et al. 2002, 2003) and

having poorer language abilities and a lower IQ

(Laplante et al. 2004, 2008). Their mothers rated

them as having more sleeping, feeding and activity

problems (Van den Bergh 1990, 1992), and being

more irritable, difficult and showing more negative

affectivity characteristics of temperament (Vaughn

et al. 1987; Van den Bergh 1990, 1992; Huizink et al.

2002, 2003; Austin et al. 2005; Pesonen et al. 2005;

McGrath et al. 2008; Henrichs et al. 2009) and crying

more excessively (van der Wal et al. 2007). Scores on

the Bayley Scales of Infant Development were worse at

8 and 24 months (Brouwers et al. 2001; Huizink et al.

2002, 2003; Laplante et al. 2004), but not in another

study of 7-month-old infants (Van den Bergh 1990,

1992). Methodological differences, such as differ-

ences in sample size and variables that were used as

covariates, may explain the conflicting findings

between the studies using the Bayley Scales. Dawson

and colleagues found that during mother–infant

interaction, children of depressed mothers showed

higher than normal heart rates and higher levels of

cortisol, and reduced activity in brain regions that

mediate positive approach behaviour. The authors

indicate that there is suggestive evidence from their

follow-up study that the postnatal experience with the

mother may have had more effect on infant frontal

EEG than on prenatal factors (Dawson and Ashman

2000; Dawson et al. 2001).

Pre-school children and children were rated by their

mothers (Martin et al. 1999; O’Connor et al. 2002,

2003; Niederhofer and Reiter 2004; Van den Bergh

and Marcoen 2004), teachers (Niederhofer and Reiter

2004; Rodriguez and Bohlin 2005), an external

observer (Van den Bergh and Marcoen 2004) or

themselves (Van den Bergh and Marcoen 2004) as

showing poorer attention, hyperactivity, behavioural

and emotional problems, and they were rated by their

teacher as having lower school grades and problem

behaviour (Niederhofer and Reiter 2004). O’Connor

et al. (2002) found that the effects of antenatal anxiety

were stronger than the effects of antenatal depression.

Finally, the results of Obel et al. (2003)indicated that

stressful life events in the mother during pregnancy

increased the risk for ADHD problems in pre-

adolescence between ages, 9–11 years.

Adolescents showed problems with cognitive control

when performing computerized cognitive tasks

measuring prefrontal cortex functioning and scored

lower on intelligence subtests at the age of 14–15 and

17 years (Mennes et al. 2006; Van den Bergh et al.

2005a, 2006). Girls displayed higher levels of

depressive symptoms (Van den Bergh et al. 2008).

Mennes et al. (2009) showed that 17-year-old

children of mothers who were more anxious between

12 and 22 weeks of gestation displayed changes in

event-related potentials (ERP) measured with EEG,

when performing a gambling task, requiring endogen-

ous control. Functional magnetic resonance (fMRI)

measures revealed that in the 20-year-old offspring,

differences related to the level of antenatal maternal

anxiety were found in the activation patterns of several

important prefrontal regions (Mennes 2008). Buss

et al. (2009) showed in a MRI study that high

pregnancy-specific anxiety in mid gestation, but not

later, is associated with decreased grey matter density

in specific brain areas, in 8-year-old children.

Some evidence also suggests that the major life

events inducing severe prenatal stress, such as death of

a close relative during pregnancy, may be associated

with stillbirth (Wisborg et al. 2008), low birth weight

and smallness for gestational age status (Khashan et al.

2008b), cerebral palsy (Li et al. 2009b) and

schizophrenia (Khashan et al. 2008a) in the offspring.

One study has linked such events also with the risk of

autism in the offspring (Kinney et al. 2008), but

another study failed to replicate this association (Li

et al. 2009a).

In humans, birth anthropometry associates with

subsequent HPA axis function. Higher plasma and

urinary glucocorticoid levels are found in children and

adults who were of lower birth weight (Clark et al.

Stress, glucocorticoids and liquorice in human pregnancy 593

Stre

ss D

ownl

oade

d fr

om in

form

ahea

lthca

re.c

om b

y U

nive

rsite

it V

an T

ilbur

g on

03/

13/1

3Fo

r pe

rson

al u

se o

nly.

Table

I.S

tud

ies

test

ing

ass

oci

ati

on

sbet

wee

npre

nata

lm

ate

rnal

stre

ss,

an

xie

tyan

dd

epre

ssio

nw

ith

off

spri

ng

dev

elopm

ent.

Age

of

child

Mea

sure

or

type

of

pre

nata

lex

posu

re

Pre

nata

lti

min

g

wee

ks

ges

tati

on

Ou

tcom

em

easu

re(s

)R

efer

ence

(s)

2–

3d

ays

An

xie

ty(s

tate

&tr

ait

);S

tres

s(P

SS

)20;

30

NB

AS

:habit

uati

on

,so

cial

inte

ract

ion

,m

oto

r

syst

em,

state

org

an

izati

on

,st

ate

regu

lati

on

,

au

ton

om

icsy

stem

Her

nan

dez

-Rei

fet

al.

(2008)

3–

5d

ays

Tota

ld

istr

ess

,20;

30

–34

Reg

ula

tion

pro

ble

ms

(NB

AS

)R

ieger

etal.

(2004)

1w

eek

Per

inata

ld

epre

ssio

n26

Cort

isol

leve

lD

iego

etal.

(2004)

Neo

nate

An

xie

ty(g

ener

al)

40*

Au

dit

ory

evoked

resp

on

ses

(AE

R)

Harv

ison

etal.

(2009)

12

day

sD

epre

ssio

nN

ot

rep

ort

edA

tten

tiven

ess

(NB

AS

)H

ern

an

dez

-Rei

fet

al.

(2006)

4–

14

day

sL

ife

even

tsst

ress

Mid

ges

tati

on

Bir

thw

eight,

hea

dci

rcu

mfe

ren

ce,

Pre

chtl

’s

neu

rolo

gic

al

obse

rvati

on

Lou

etal.

(1994)

Bir

th,

1d

ay,

3–

4w

eeks

An

xie

ty(s

tate

an

dtr

ait

)#

16;

32

–34

Med

ical

reco

rds

(e.g

.A

pgar

scale

),ca

rdia

c

vagal

ton

e(E

KG

Porg

es’

met

hod

)

Pon

irakis

etal.

(1998)

6w

eeks

Foet

al

react

ivit

yto

mate

rnal

stim

ula

tion

(vie

win

gof

vid

eo)

32

Infa

nt

neg

ati

vere

act

ivit

yd

uri

ng

a

stan

dard

ized

lab

pro

ced

ure

DiP

ietr

oet

al.

(2008)

3m

on

ths

Dep

ress

ion

15

–28;

29

–40

Infa

nt

cort

isol

leve

lp

rean

dp

ost

infa

nt

stre

ss

test

Ober

lan

der

etal.

(2008)

4m

on

ths

An

xie

ty(s

tate

)an

dd

epre

ssio

n32

Beh

avio

ura

lre

act

ivit

y(H

IBR

P)

Dav

iset

al.

(2004)

2,

6m

on

ths

Dep

ress

ion

Th

ird

trim

este

rT

emp

eram

ent

rati

ngs

McG

rath

etal.

(2008)

3–

5m

on

ths

Str

ess

an

dd

epre

ssio

n28

–40

Face

-to-f

ace

pla

yin

tera

ctio

ns

(CC

TI)

Fie

ldet

al.

(1985)

3–

6m

on

ths

Dep

ress

ive

sym

pto

ms,

an

xie

ty

(pre

gn

an

cy-s

pec

ific)

,pare

nti

ng

stre

ss;

job

stra

in

7–

40

Exce

ssiv

ecr

yin

gva

nd

erW

al

etal.

(2007)

4–

6m

on

ths

An

xie

ty(t

rait

);d

epre

ssio

n,

life

even

tsst

ress

Thir

dtr

imes

ter

(32)

Dif

ficu

ltte

mper

am

ent

(SIT

Q)

Au

stin

etal.

(2005)

6m

on

ths

Dep

ress

ion

an

dan

xie

ty26

Infa

nt

cort

isol

leve

lpre

an

dpost

infa

nt

stre

ss

test

Bre

nn

an

etal.

(2008)

6m

on

ths

Str

ess

exper

ien

ce1st

wee

kpost

part

um

Tem

per

am

ent

(IB

Q);

neg

ati

ve

an

dover

all

tem

per

am

enta

lre

act

ivit

y

Pes

on

enet

al.

(2005)

6m

on

ths

An

xie

ty(p

regn

an

cy-s

pec

ific;

gen

eral)

12;

20

Tem

per

am

ent

(IB

Q-R

);in

fan

td

iffi

cult

ies

Hen

rich

set

al.

(2009)

1w

eek,

12

wee

ks,

7m

on

ths

An

xie

ty(s

tate

an

dtr

ait

)12

–22;

23

–31;

32

–40

Pre

chtl

’sn

euro

logic

al

obse

rvati

on

,b

eha-

vio

ura

lst

ate

ob

serv

ati

on

,fe

edin

gsc

ore

,

moth

er–

infa

ntin

tera

ctio

n(I

TQ

,IC

Q,B

SID

)

Van

den

Ber

gh

,(1

990;1

992)

7m

on

ths

An

xie

ty36

–39*

Cort

isol

leve

lof

the

child

,p

rean

dp

ost

still

face

pro

ced

ure

Gra

nt

etal.

(2009)

4–

8m

on

ths

An

xie

ty(t

rait

)21;

26

–34,

35

Tem

per

am

ent

(IT

Q-r

evis

ed)

Vau

gh

net

al.

(1987)

8m

on

ths

Daily

hass

les

15

–17,

27

–28,

37

–38

Men

tal

dev

elopm

ent

(BS

ID-M

DI)

Hu

izin

ket

al.

(2002,

2003)

9m

on

ths

9/1

1N

ot

rep

ort

edT

emp

eram

ent

(IB

Q)

Bra

nd

etal.

(2006)

9m

on

ths

9/1

128

–40

Cort

isol

leve

lof

the

child

Yeh

ud

aet

al.

(2005)

1.5

yea

rsL

ife

even

tsst

ress

(i.e

.re

lati

on

ship

pro

ble

ms)

14

mon

ths

post

part

um

*C

ogn

itiv

ed

evel

opm

ent

(BS

ID-M

DI)

,fe

ar

react

ivit

y(L

ab

-TA

B)

Ber

gm

an

etal.

(2007)

2yea

rsA

nxie

ty(g

ener

al:

PO

MS

an

dS

TA

I),st

ress

(DS

I,

PS

S);

dep

ress

ion

(PO

MS

,C

ES

-D);

pre

gn

an

cy-s

pec

ific

stre

ss(P

ES

)

24,

28,

32

BS

ID:

MD

I,P

DI;

IBR

Dip

ietr

oet

al.

(2006)

3w

eeks;

1yea

r;2

yea

rsA

nxie

ty(s

tate

an

dtr

ait

)32

Ori

enta

tion

(NB

AS

),C

ogn

itiv

ed

evel

opm

ent

(BS

ID-M

DI)

,T

ask

ori

enta

tion

an

dm

oto

r

co-o

rdin

ati

on

(IB

R)

Bro

uw

ers

etal.

(2001)

K. Raikkonen et al.594

Stre

ss D

ownl

oade

d fr

om in

form

ahea

lthca

re.c

om b

y U

nive

rsite

it V

an T

ilbur

g on

03/

13/1

3Fo

r pe

rson

al u

se o

nly.

TA

BL

EI

–continued

Age

of

child

Mea

sure

or

typ

eof

pre

nata

lex

posu

re

Pre

nata

lti

min

g

wee

ks

ges

tati

on

Ou

tcom

em

easu

re(s

)R

efer

ence

(s)

2yea

rsP

erce

ived

stre

sspre

gn

an

cy-r

elate

dan

xie

ty15

–38

Beh

avio

ura

lpro

ble

ms

(CB

CL

)te

mper

am

ent

(IC

Q),

att

enti

on

regu

lati

on

(BS

ID)

Gu

ttel

ing

etal.

(2005b)

13

–15

an

d42

mon

ths

Dep

ress

ion

Mon

thly

,fr

om

con

cepti

on

on

ward

s

Fro

nta

lan

dp

ari

etal

EE

Gasy

mm

etry

score

sD

awso

nan

dA

shm

an

(2000);

Daw

son

etal.

(2001)

14

–54

mon

ths

An

xie

ty,

dep

ress

ion

Each

trim

este

rB

ehav

iou

ral

an

dem

oti

on

al

pro

ble

ms

(CB

CL

);sa

liva

cort

isol

aft

erst

ress

ors

de

Bru

ijn

etal.

(2009a,b

)

6m

on

ths,

5yea

rsP

sych

olo

gic

al

dis

tres

s1

–16;

17

–28;

29

–40

Tem

per

am

ent

(IT

Q,

PT

Q)

Mart

inet

al.

(1999)

2yea

rsIc

est

orm

4–

24

Cogn

itiv

ed

evel

opm

ent

(BS

ID-M

DI)

,

lan

gu

age

pro

du

ctio

n(M

CD

I)

Lap

lan

teet

al.

(2004)

5.5

yea

rs–

IQ(W

ISC

)L

ap

lan

teet

al.

(2008)

6m

on

ths,

6yea

rsP

regn

an

cyri

sks

16

–20

Tem

per

am

ent

(IT

Q),

sch

ool

gra

des

an

d

mark

s

Nie

der

hofe

ran

dR

eite

r(2

004)

4–

6ye

ars

Daily

hass

les,

an

xie

ty,

per

ceiv

ed

stre

ss,

life

even

ts

16

Cort

isol

level

of

the

child

at

firs

tsc

hool

day

;

aft

erin

ocu

lati

on

mem

ory

(TO

MA

L)

Gu

ttel

ing

etal.

(2004,

2005a,

2006)

4an

d7

years

An

xie

ty18,

32

Em

oti

on

al

an

db

ehav

iou

ral

pro

ble

ms

(SD

Q)

O’C

on

nor

etal.

(2002,

2003)

7–

8ye

ars

Per

ceiv

edst

ress

10,

12,

20,

28,

32,

36

AD

HD

(DS

M-I

V)

Rod

rigu

ezan

dB

ohlin

(2005)

6–

9ye

ars

An

xie

ty(p

regn

an

cysp

ecifi

c)19,2

5,3

1G

rey

matt

erd

ensi

ty(M

RI

stu

dy)

Bu

sset

al.

(2009)

10

yea

rsS

elf

rate

dan

xie

ty18,

32

Cort

isol

level

of

the

child

O’C

on

nor

etal.

(2005)

10

yea

rs!

Dea

thof

acl

ose

rela

tive

0–

12

Sch

izophre

nia

(IC

D8/I

CD

10)

Khash

an

etal.

(2008)

14

yea

rsC

her

nob

yl

14

!D

epre

ssio

n/M

DD

(C-S

SA

GA

-A:

DS

M-I

II-

R)

AD

HD

Hu

izin

ket

al.

(2007)

8–

9ye

ars

,14

–15,

17

yea

rs

An

xie

ty(s

tate

an

dtr

ait

)12

–22

AD

HD

(CB

CL

,T

RF

),co

mp

ute

rize

d

enco

din

gta

skan

dst

op

task

,vo

cabu

lary

an

d

blo

ckd

esig

n(W

ISC

-R),

sust

ain

edatt

enti

on

(CP

T),

dep

ress

ion

(CD

I),

cued

att

enti

on

,

N-b

ack

,G

o/N

oG

o,

du

al

task

(ER

P),

resp

on

se-s

hif

tin

g.

Van

den

Ber

gh

an

dM

arc

oen

(2004);

Van

den

Ber

gh

etal.

(2005,

2006,

2008),

Men

nes

etal.

(2006,

2009)

25

yea

rsS

ever

elife

even

tsO

nave

rage

25

years

post

part

um

*

Cort

isol

an

dA

CT

Hle

vel

sE

ntr

inger

etal.

(2009)

–H

urr

ican

e24

–40

Au

tism

(DS

M-I

I-R

/DS

M-I

V)

Kin

ney

etal.

(2008)

Note

:A

DH

D,

att

enti

on

defi

cit

hyper

act

ivit

yd

isord

er;

AE

R,

Au

dit

ory

Evo

ked

Res

pon

ses;

BS

ID,

Bay

ley

Sca

les

of

Infa

nt

Dev

elopm

ent;

CB

CL

,C

hild

Beh

avio

ur

Chec

klist

;C

CT

I,C

olo

rad

oC

hild

Tem

per

am

ent

Inven

tory

;C

DI,

child

dep

ress

ion

inve

nto

ry;C

PT

,co

nti

nu

ou

sper

form

an

ceta

sk;C

-SS

AG

A-A

-,ch

ild

sem

i-st

ruct

ure

dass

essm

ent

of

gen

eti

csan

dalc

oholism

;D

SI,

Daily

Str

ess

Inven

tory

;

DS

M-R

,D

iagn

ost

ican

dS

tati

stic

al

Man

ual-

revis

ed;

EC

G,

elec

troca

rdio

gra

m;

ER

P,

even

t-re

late

dpote

nti

al;

HIB

RP,

Harv

ard

Infa

nt

Beh

avio

ura

lR

eact

ivit

yP

roto

col;

IBQ

,In

fan

tB

ehav

iou

r

Qu

esti

on

nair

e;IB

R,

Infa

nt

Beh

avio

ur

Rec

ord

;IC

D,

Inte

rnati

on

al

Cla

ssifi

cati

on

of

Dis

ease

s;IC

Q,

Infa

nt

Chara

cter

isti

csQ

ues

tion

nair

e;IT

Q,

Infa

nt

Tem

per

am

ent

Qu

esti

on

nair

e;IQ

,In

tellig

ence

Qu

oti

ent;

MC

DI,

MacA

rth

ur

Com

mu

nic

ati

veD

evel

opm

ent

Inven

tory

;M

DD

,M

ajo

rD

epre

ssiv

eD

isord

er;

MD

I,M

enta

lD

evel

opm

ent

Ind

ex;

Lab

-TA

B,

lab

ora

tory

tem

per

am

ent

ass

essm

ent

Batt

ery;

NB

AS

,B

raze

lton

neo

nata

lbeh

avio

ura

lass

essm

ent

scale

;P

ES

,P

regn

an

cyE

xper

ien

ceS

cale

;P

OM

S,P

rofi

leof

Mood

sS

cale

;P

SS

,P

erce

ived

Str

ess

Sca

le;P

DI,

Psy

chom

oto

rD

evel

opm

ent

Ind

ex;P

TQ

,

Pre

school

Tem

per

am

ent

Qu

esti

on

nair

e;S

DQ

,S

tren

gth

san

dD

iffi

cult

ies

Qu

esti

on

nair

e;S

ITQ

:S

hort

Infa

nt

Tem

per

am

ent

Qu

esti

on

nair

e;T

OM

AL

,T

est

of

Mem

ory

an

dL

earn

ing;

TR

F,

Tea

cher

’s

Rep

ort

Form

;W

ISC

,W

ech

sler

Inte

llig

ence

Sca

lefo

rC

hild

ren

;W

PP

SI,

Wec

hsl

erP

resc

hool

an

dP

rim

ary

Sca

leof

Inte

llig

ence

;*

Ret

rosp

ecti

ve.

Stress, glucocorticoids and liquorice in human pregnancy 595

Stre

ss D

ownl

oade

d fr

om in

form

ahea

lthca

re.c

om b

y U

nive

rsite

it V

an T

ilbur

g on

03/

13/1

3Fo

r pe

rson

al u

se o

nly.

1996; Phillips et al. 1998). Cortisol responses to

ACTH stimulation are exaggerated in those of low

birth weight (Levitt et al. 2000; Reynolds et al. 2001,

2007). This occurs in myriad populations (Phillips

et al. 2000; Kajantie and Raikkonen 2010) and

precedes overt adult disease (Levitt et al. 2000). It is

well known that psychological stress causes activation

of the HPA axis with a consequent elevation in the

levels of cortisol. Elevated levels of cortisol are also

among the most consistently demonstrated biological

abnormalities found in depression (Knorr et al. 2010),

and in some contexts in anxiety (Roelofs et al. 2009).

As maternal and foetal levels of cortisol are correlated

(Gitau et al. 1998; O’Keane et al. 2011), the

experience of stress and higher levels of depression

and anxiety during pregnancy may directly result in

higher foetal exposure to maternal cortisol. In a recent

study, Sarkar et al. (2008) demonstrated that the

correlation between amniotic fluid cortisol (reflecting

foetal exposure) and maternal plasma cortisol

becomes stronger the higher with levels of maternal

prenatal anxiety, while maternal prenatal anxiety

correlated with higher plasma cortisol levels of the

mother, it was not directly related to amniotic fluid

cortisol (Glover et al. 2009).

As a consequence of this biology, a number of

studies have explored the effects of maternal

psychopathology on offspring HPA axis function,

mainly observing the effects by comparing the

offspring of high vs. low anxiety, stressed or depressed

pregnant women (Glover et al. 2010). Prenatal

maternal anxiety and depression are in general

associated with raised cortisol in the offspring, but

infants of mothers exposed to the trauma of 9/11 who

themselves developed symptoms of posttraumatic

stress disorder (PTSD) had lower cortisol levels,

though this is a common finding in PTSD (Yehuda

et al. 2005). One study has provided evidence that an

altered cortisol diurnal profile in the offspring

associated with prenatal maternal anxiety was also

associated with an altered behavioural phenotype, i.e.

with depressed mood (Van den Bergh et al. 2008).

Differences have been reported throughout the

lifespan from infancy, ages 1 week to 9 months

(Diego et al. 2004; Yehuda et al. 2005; Brennan et al.

2008; Oberlander et al. 2008; Grant et al. 2009),

4–15 years of age (Gutteling et al. 2004, 2005;

O’Connor et al. 2005; Huizink et al. 2008; Van den

Bergh et al. 2008) and in adults (Entringer et al.

2009). These data suggest that, as in animal models,

the HPA axis is a prime target for prenatal influences

and programming.

The studies on prenatal maternal stress, anxiety and

depression, even when controlling for postnatal

maternal stress, anxiety and depression, cannot

overrule a possibility that a shared genetic basis may

underlie the associations with offspring neurodevelop-

mental outcomes. Most evidence pointing to a

hereditary component comes from the studies of

depression (Belmaker and Agam 2008). It is, however,

unlikely that individual differences in offspring

neurodevelopmental outcomes could be attributed

to a single genetic, epigenetic or pre- or postnatal

environmental factor. Multiple mechanisms do exist

(see Figure 1), and these may act in concert. Further

studies on the exact underlying mechanisms through

which maternal stress, depression and anxiety induce

programming of the offspring are clearly warranted.

Maternal glucocorticoid therapy

In humans, therapeutic treatment with synthetic

glucocorticoids is common in women at risk of

preterm delivery, serving to accelerate foetal lung

maturation and thus reduce neonatal morbidity and

mortality (Seckl and Meaney 2004a). The long-term

effects of this treatment are not fully determined,

although there are suggestions that foetal growth and

subsequent development may be impaired (for a

review see Bolt et al. 2001). Glucocorticoid treatment

during pregnancy typically reduces birth weight (for a

review see Sloboda et al. 2005), but long-term follow

up studies are few. Antenatal glucocorticoid admin-

istration has been linked with higher blood pressure in

adolescence (Doyle et al. 2000) and higher insulin

levels in adulthood (Dalziel et al. 2005). Studies

aimed at establishing the long-term neurological and

developmental effects of antenatal glucocorticoid

therapy are complicated by the frequency of neuro-

logical sequelae common in such children anyway.

However, in a group of 6-year-old children, antenatal

glucocorticoid exposure is associated with subtle

effects on neurological function, including reduced

visual closure and visual memory (MacArthur et al.

1982). Multiple doses of antenatal glucocorticoids

given to women at risk of preterm delivery reduced

birth weight and head circumference in the offspring

(French et al. 1999). There are also effects on

behaviour: three or more courses of glucocorticoids

associate with an increased risk of externalizing

behaviour problems, distractibility and inattention

(Yeh et al. 2004), and children exposed to a longer

(.24 h to delivery) relative to a shorter duration

(,24 h to delivery) of a single, repeat dose of

betamethasone were rated by their mothers as more

impulsive at the age of 3 years (Pesonen et al. 2009a).

Children exposed to dexamethasone in early preg-

nancy because of risk of congenital adrenal hyperpla-

sia and born at term showed increased emotionality,

unsociability, avoidance and behavioural problems

(Trautman et al. 1995). Postnatally, dexamethasone

in premature babies lowers later IQ and other higher

brain functions (Yeh et al. 2004). Overall, whilst far

from complete, evidence indicates that exposure of the

foetus (or premature neonate) to excess glucocorti-

coids that readily cross the placental and foetal tissue

K. Raikkonen et al.596

Stre

ss D

ownl

oade

d fr

om in

form

ahea

lthca

re.c

om b

y U

nive

rsite

it V

an T

ilbur

g on

03/

13/1

3Fo

r pe

rson

al u

se o

nly.

barriers to physiological cortisol (because they are

poor substrates for 11b-HSD2) may induce altera-

tions in the developing CNS.

Maternal intake of liquorice

If glucocorticoid exposure underpins some develop-

mental effects on the foetus, what does 11b-HSD2

do? Rare children homozygous for deleterious

mutations of the HSD11B2 gene encoding 11b-

HSD2 are of substantially lower birth weight than

their siblings, many of whom will be heterozygous for

the mutations (Dave-Sharma et al. 1998). In humans,

as in rodents (Benediktsson et al. 1993), placental

11b-HSD2 activity correlates with birth weight in

some (Stewart et al. 1995), but not all studies

(Rogerson et al. 1997). However, such correlations

are weak and mechanisms can only be inferred.

A handful of recent studies have focused on prenatal

maternal intake of liquorice confectionery as a natural

experimental platform to address mechanisms of

programming. These have been conducted in Finland

where consumption of large quantities of liquorice

confectionery is common among young women:

nearly half of the pregnant women consume at least

some liquorice (Raikkonen et al. 2009b; Strandberg

et al. 2001). Glycyrrhizin (3b-D-diglucuronyl-18b-

glycyrrhetinic or -glycyrrhizic acid) is a natural

constituent of liquorice. Its hemisuccinate synthetic

analogue, carbenoxolone, was a clinical drug formerly

used to treat peptic ulcers. Its water solubility makes it

attractive for use in preclinical and human studies

(Welberg et al. 2000). These agents are potent (low

nanomolar Ki) inhibitors of 11b-HSD2. Though they

also potently inhibit 11b-HSD type 1, this is little

expressed in the foetus at least in rodents (Speirs et al.

2004). In normal circumstances, this placental

enzyme metabolizes up to 80–90% of maternal active

cortisol to inactive cortisone, a function inhibited by

carbenoxolone, at least in intact placenta ex vivo

(Benediktsson et al. 1997).

In 1998, a project was initiated to test whether

varying levels of maternal consumption of glycyrrhizin

in liquorice during pregnancy was associated

with body size at birth and length of gestation. In

over 1000 pregnant women, prenatal exposure to high

(.500 mg/wk) compared to zero-low (0–249 mg/wk)

or moderate (250–499 mg/wk) glycyrrhizin in

liquorice was associated with a slightly shorter

duration of gestation (Strandberg et al. 2001).

Glycyrrhizin intake during pregnancy was not

significantly associated with birth anthropometry.

The findings on shorter length of gestation were

replicated in another Finnish cohort: high intake of

glycyrrhizin during pregnancy was associated with

over a twofold increased risk in the rate of preterm

delivery (Strandberg et al. 2002).

In a follow-up study, the long-term effects of

maternal intake of liquorice during gestation on

8-year-old offspring psychological development,

mental health and psychophysiological functioning

were determined. In comparison to the group whose

mothers had zero-low glycyrrhizin exposure, those

with high exposure scored significantly lower on verbal

and visuo-spatial abilities and in narrative memory. In

addition, children in the high-exposure group had

significant 2.4–3.0-fold increased risk of externalizing

symptoms, attention, rule breaking, aggression pro-

blems and DSM IV-based symptoms of obsessive

defiant disorder (Raikkonen et al. 2009b). The effects

on cognitive performance appeared dose-related.

None of the associations were affected by birth weight

or duration of gestation and other pre- and perinatal,

maternal and child characteristics implicated as risks

for pregnancy and/or cognitive and psychiatric

outcomes.

The HPA axis of these children was also affected.

Children of mothers consuming high levels of

glycyrrhizin had a higher salivary cortisol peak and

area under the curve upon awakening and higher

overall salivary cortisol throughout a Trier Social

Stress Test for Children (TSST-C; Raikkonen et al.

2010c). The associations with salivary cortisol peak

and salivary cortisol baseline during the TSST-C were

dose-related. As far as could be ascertained, the results

were not due to confounding factors such as maternal

health during pregnancy, including blood pressure

levels, obesity and pregnancy disorders, birth anthro-

pometry and length of gestation, maternal social class

at birth and in a follow-up at the child’s age of 8 years,

maternal smoking and alcohol consumption during

pregnancy, and child characteristics, such as sex,

growth in height, head circumference and difficulties

in cognitive functioning that might interfere with

cognitive performance, associate with psychiatric

symptomatology, and alter performance in the

TSST-C. We have previously reported, in this

same sample of 8-year-old children, that shorter

sleep duration and lower sleep efficiency (percent time

spent asleep whilst in bed) are associated with

alterations in the diurnal salivary cortisol pattern and

in salivary cortisol responses during the TSST-C

(Raikkonen et al. 2010b). Poorer sleep may thus

introduce another factor that may confound the

associations between liquorice consumption and

HPA axis activity. Maternal intake of liquorice during

pregnancy is, however, not associated with the

children’s sleep patterns (Raikkonen, unpublished

data). Thus, when we further controlled the associ-

ations of maternal liquorice intake during pregnancy

and their children’s HPA axis function for the

children’s sleep duration and sleep efficiency, the

associations remained identical.

These findings suggest that high maternal liquorice

consumption during pregnancy may exert deleterious

Stress, glucocorticoids and liquorice in human pregnancy 597

Stre

ss D

ownl

oade

d fr

om in

form

ahea

lthca

re.c

om b

y U

nive

rsite

it V

an T

ilbur

g on

03/

13/1

3Fo

r pe

rson

al u

se o

nly.

effects upon cognitive and psychiatric outcomes and

HPAA functioning in children paralleling the effects

seen in preclinical models. Whilst it remains to be

ascertained whether the dose of liquorice consumed

affects placental 11b-HSD2 function appreciably

in vivo (mice lacking one copy of the gene encoding

11b-HSD2 have intermediate reductions in birth

weight), it is plausible to suggest that this may occur

and that placental glucocorticoid “leakiness” may

have an impact on foetal brain development during

periods critical for affective and cognitive develop-

ment with persisting impacts. If humans resemble

rodents, then maternal stress-related disorders may

further lower placental glucocorticoid barrier

function.

Conclusion

This review shows, first, that accumulating evidence

exists suggesting that maternal negative emotions

during pregnancy are linked with the long-term

behaviour and physiology of her child, even after

controlling for relevant covariates. The range of

maternal “stressors” that predict the child outcomes

is quite wide. Moreover, a wide range of different

outcomes have been found to be affected by these

maternal stressors (Glover et al. 2010). This should

come as no surprise. In terms of mechanisms, we are

far from understanding how and when the early

hormonal environment may affect the refinement of

neural circuits in specific brain layers and areas which

will later determine the way in which sensory-

cognitive, motor, arousal and emotional structure–

function relationships are affected (Van den Bergh

et al. 2005a; Fox et al. 2010).

Second, this review shows that there are links

between prenatal maternal intake of glucocorticoids

and of liquorice confectionery and long-term beha-

viour and physiology of her child, even after

controlling for relevant covariates. These findings

may not generalize merely to intake of liquorice

confectionery.

Because of its sweetening (50–200 times sweeter

than refined sugar) and flavouring capacity, glycyr-

rhizin is also used as a natural sweetener and is also

found in other foodstuffs, including some candies and

chewing gum, herbal teas, alcoholic and non-alcoholic

drinks, tobacco and some traditional (e.g. cough

medicine) as well as some herbal medicine (to treat

stomach ulcers, sore throat and viral infections).

Hence, not surprisingly, according to some estimates

the daily consumption levels of glycyrrhizin range

from 1.6 to 215.2 mg (Isbrucker and Burdock 2006).

This estimate was derived from the USA where

consumption of liquorice confectionery is not popular.

Glycyrrhizin is generally recognized as safe for use

in foods, though the European Community’s

Scientific Committee on Food and the FAO/WHO

Expert Committee (http://ec.europa.eu/food/fs/sc/scf/

out186_en.pdf) have considered that a consumption

of 100 mg/day may be a reasonable upper limit for the

majority of the population, and the FDA recommends

that if glycyrrhizin-containing foods are not consumed

in excess or by sensitive individuals, these foods do not

pose a health hazard (http://www.accessdata.fda.gov/

scripts/cdrh/cfdocs/cfcfr/CFRSearch.cfm?fr ¼ 184.

1408). The findings reviewed here suggest that

consumption of large quantities of glycyrrhizin in

foodstuffs may alter offspring’s brain development and

subsequent function, and therefore, it may be prudent

to inform pregnant women of the potential hazards of

eating large quantities of liquorice.

Future studies should try to increase knowledge

about mechanisms underlying the effect of prenatal

influences on social-emotional, cognitive and beha-

vioural problems, particularly about whether prenatal

stress and gene-environment interactions affecting the

HPA-axis play a role in early developmental processes.

It has become increasingly clear that epigenetic

changes, including DNA methylation and histone

modifications, are causally involved in a range of

developmental processes by affecting transcriptional

activity. For the future, it is important that these

epigenetic processes, that take place also in the

placenta, are integrated in the studies of prenatal

stress. They provide “a physical basis for the influence

of the perinatal environmental signals over the life of

the individual” (Meaney 2010). Moreover, the

eventual moderating influence of other environmental

factors, such as the postnatal caregiving environment,

should also be studied. There is evidence that severe

early life stress may carry consequences on a wide

range of developmental outcomes, including physical

and mental health, psychophysiological functioning

and cognitive abilities and attained social class in

adulthood (Pesonen et al. 2007, 2008b, 2010, 2011;

Alastalo et al. 2009; Raikkonen et al. 2011). Whether

the postnatal environmental influences buffer or add

to the prenatal influences is little studied. Results of

these research efforts would open the way for the

development of targeted pre- and perinatal prevention

and intervention strategies that could reduce the risk

that prenatal environmental adversities carry for early

functioning and later mental health of children, and

have significant consequences for the long-term health

and well-being of the children.

Declaration of interest: This work was sponsored by

the grants from the European Science Foundation,

Stress and Mental Health programme (EuroS-

TRESS), the Finnish Academy, the Medical Research

Council (UK), The Netherlands Organisation for

Scientific Research (NWO). The authors report no

conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible

for the content and writing of the paper.

K. Raikkonen et al.598

Stre

ss D

ownl

oade

d fr

om in

form

ahea

lthca

re.c

om b

y U

nive

rsite

it V

an T

ilbur

g on

03/

13/1

3Fo

r pe

rson

al u

se o

nly.

References

Alastalo H, Raikkonen K, Pesonen A-K, Osmond C, Barker DJ,

Kajantie E, Heinonen K, Forsen TJ, Eriksson JG. 2009.

Cardiovascular health of Finnish war evacuees 60 years later.

Ann Med 41:66–72.

Austin MP, Hadzi-Pavlovic D, Leader L, Saint K, Parker G. 2005.

Maternal trait anxiety, depression and life event stress in

pregnancy: Relationships with infant temperament. Early Hum

Dev 81:183–190.

Barker DJ. 1991. The intrauterine environment and adult

cardiovascular disease. Ciba Found Symp 156:3–10, discussion

10–6.

Barker DJ. 1998. In utero programming of chronic disease. Clin Sci

(Lond) 95:115–128.

Barker DJ, Winter PD, Osmond C, Margetts B, Simmonds SJ.

1989. Weight in infancy and death from ischaemic heart disease.

Lancet 2:577–580.

Barker DJ, Osmond C, Rodin I, Fall CH, Winter PD. 1995. Low

weight gain in infancy and suicide in adult life. Br Med J 311:

1203.

Barker DJ, Osmond C, Forsen TJ, Kajantie E, Eriksson JG. 2005.

Trajectories of growth among children who have coronary events

as adults. N Engl J Med 353:1802–1809.

Belmaker RH, Agam G. 2008. Major depressive disorder. N Engl J

Med 358:55–68.

Benediktsson R, Lindsay RS, Noble J, Seckl JR, Edwards CR. 1993.

Glucocorticoid exposure in utero: New model for adult

hypertension. Lancet 341:339–341.

Benediktsson R, Calder AA, Edwards CR, Seckl JR. 1997. Placental

11 beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase: A key regulator of fetal

glucocorticoid exposure. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 46:161–166.

Bergman K, Sarkar P, O’Connor TG, Modi N, Glover V. 2007.

Maternal stress during pregnancy predicts cognitive ability and

fearfulness in infancy. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 46:

1454–1463.

Bolt RJ, van Weissenbruch MM, Lafeber HN, Delemarre-van de

Waal HA. 2001. Glucocorticoids and lung development in the

fetus and preterm infant. Pediatr Pulmonol 32:76–91.

Brand SR, Engel SM, Canfield RL, Yehuda R. 2006. The effect of

maternal PTSD following in utero trauma exposure on behavior

and temperament in the 9-month-old infant. Ann N Y Acad Sci

1071:454–458.

Brennan PA, Pargas R, Walker EF, Green P, Newport DJ, Stowe Z.

2008. Maternal depression and infant cortisol: Influences of

timing, comorbidity and treatment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry

49:1099–1107.

Broekman BF, Chan YH, Chong YS, Quek SC, Fung D, Low YL,

Ooi YP, Gluckman PD, Meaney MJ, Wong TY, Saw SM. 2009.

The influence of birth size on intelligence in healthy children.

Pediatrics 123:e1011–e1016.

Brouwers EPM, van Baar AL, Pop VJM. 2001. Maternal anxiety

during pregnancy and subsequent infant development. Inf Behav

Dev 24:95–106.

Buss C, Davis EP, Muftuler LT, Head K, Sandman CA. 2009. High

pregnancy anxiety during mid-gestation is associated with

decreased gray matter density in 6–9-year-old children.

Psychoneuroendocrinology 35:141–153.

Cheung YB. 2002. Early origins and adult correlates of psychoso-

matic distress. Soc Sci Med 55:937–948.

Clark PM, Hindmarsh PC, Shiell AW, Law CM, Honour JW, Barker

DJP. 1996. Size at birth and adrenocortical function in

childhood. Clin Endocrinol 45:721–726.

Cornelius MD, Day NL. 2009. Developmental consequences of

prenatal tobacco exposure. Curr Opin Neurol 22:121–125.

Dalziel SR, Walker NK, Parag V, Mantell C, Rea HH, Rodgers A,

Harding JE. 2005. Cardiovascular risk factors after antenatal

exposure to betamethasone: 30-year follow-up of a randomised

controlled trial. Lancet 365:1856–1862.

Dave-Sharma S, Wilson RC, Harbison MD, Newfield R, Azar MR,

Krozowski ZS, Funder JW, Shackleton CH, Bradlow HL, Wei

JQ, Hertecant J, Moran A, Neiberger RE, Balfe JW, Fattah A,

Daneman D, Akkurt HI, De Santis C, New MI. 1998.

Examination of genotype and phenotype relationships in 14

patients with apparent mineralocorticoid excess. J Clin

Endocrinol Metab 83:2244–2254.

Davis EP, Snidman N, Wadhwa PD, Glynn LM, Schetter CD,

Sandman CA. 2004. Prenatal maternal anxiety and depression

predict negative behavioral reactivity in infancy. Infancy 6:

319–331.

Davis EP, Glynn LM, Schetter CD, Hobel C, Chicz-Demet A,

Sandman CA. 2007. Prenatal exposure to maternal depression

and cortisol influences infant temperament. J Am Acad Child

Adolesc Psychiatry 46:737–746.

Dawson G, Ashman SB. 2000. On the origins of a vulnerability to

depression: The influence of the early social environment on the

development of psychobiological systems related to risk for

affective disorder. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Dawson G, Ashman SB, Hessl D, Spieker S, Frey K, Panagiotides

H, Embry L. 2001. Autonomic and brain electrical activity in

securely- and insecurely-attached infants of depressed mothers.

Inf Behav Dev 24:135–149.

de Bruijn ATCE, van Bakel HJA, van Baar AL. 2009a. Sex

differences in the relation between prenatal maternal emotional

complaints and child outcome. Early Hum Dev 85:319–324.

de Bruijn ATCE, van Bakel HJA, Wijnen H, Pop VJM, van Baar AL.

2009b. Prenatal maternal emotional complaints are associated

with cortisol responses in toddler and preschool aged girls. Dev

Psychobiol 51:553–563.

de Vries A, Holmes MC, Heijnis A, Seier JV, Heerden J, Louw J,

Wolfe-Coote S, Meaney MJ, Levitt NS, Seckl JR. 2007. Prenatal

dexamethasone exposure induces changes in nonhuman primate

offspring cardiometabolic and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal

axis function. J Clin Invest 117:1058–1067.

Diego MA, Field T, Hernandez-Reif M, Cullen C, Schanberg S,

Kuhn C. 2004. Prepartum, postpartum, and chronic depression

effects on newborns. Psychiatry 67:63–80.

DiPietro JA, Ghera MM, Costigan KA. 2008. Prenatal origins of

temperamental reactivity in early infancy. Early Hum Dev 84:

569–575.

Doyle LW, Ford GW, Davis NM, Callanan C. 2000. Antenatal

corticosteroid therapy and blood pressure at 14 years of age in

preterm children. Clin Sci 98:137–142.

Edwards CR, Benediktsson R, Lindsay RS, Seckl JR. 1993.

Dysfunction of placental glucocorticoid barrier: Link between

fetal environment and adult hypertension? Lancet 341:355–357.

El Marroun H, Tiemeier H, Steegers EA, Jaddoe VW, Hofman A,

Verhulst FC, van den Brink W, Huizink AC. 2009. Intrauterine

cannabis exposure affects fetal growth trajectories: The

generation R study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry

48(12):1173–1181.

Entringer S, Kumsta R, Hellhammer DH, Wadhwa PD, Wust S.

2009. Prenatal exposure to maternal psychosocial stress and

HPA axis regulation in young adults. Horm Behav 55:292–298.

Eriksson JG, Osmond C, Kajantie E, Forsen TJ, Barker DJ. 2006.

Patterns of growth among children who later develop type 2

diabetes or its risk factors. Diabetologia 49:2853–2858.

Eriksson JG, Forsen TJ, Kajantie E, Osmond C, Barker DJ. 2007.

Childhood growth and hypertension in later life. Hypertension

49:1415–1421.

Espy KA, Fang H, Johnson C, Stopp C, Wiebe SA, Respass J. 2011.

Prenatal tobacco exposure: Developmental outcomes in the

neonatal period. Dev Psychol 47:153–156.

Feldt K, Raikkonen K, Eriksson JG, Andersson S, Osmond C,

Barker DJ, Phillips DI, Kajantie E. 2007. Cardiovascular

reactivity to psychological stressors in late adulthood is predicted

by gestational age at birth. J Hum Hypertens 21:401–410.

Stress, glucocorticoids and liquorice in human pregnancy 599

Stre

ss D

ownl

oade

d fr

om in

form

ahea

lthca

re.c

om b

y U

nive

rsite

it V

an T

ilbur

g on

03/

13/1

3Fo

r pe

rson

al u

se o

nly.

Field T, Sandberg D, Garcia R, Vega-Lahr N, Goldstein S, Guy L.

1985. Pregnancy problems, postpartum depression, and early

mother–infant interactions. Dev Psychol 21:1152–1156.

Fox SE, Levitt P, Nelson CA, 3rd. 2010. How the timing and quality

of early experiences influence the development of brain

architecture. Child Dev 81:28–40.

French NP, Hagan R, Evans SF, Godfrey M, Newnham JP. 1999.

Repeated antenatal corticosteroids: Size at birth and subsequent

development. Am J Obstet Gynecol 180:114–121.

Gale CR, O’Callaghan FJ, Godfrey KM, Law CM, Martyn CN.

2004. Critical periods of brain growth and cognitive function in

children. Brain 127:321–329.

Gitau R, Cameron A, Fisk NM, Glover V. 1998. Fetal exposure to

maternal cortisol. Lancet 352:707–708.

Glover V, Bergman K, Sarkar P, O’Connor TG. 2009. Association

between maternal and amniotic fluid cortisol is moderated by

maternal anxiety. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34:430–435.

Glover V, O’Connor TG, O’Donnell K. 2010. Prenatal stress and

the programming of the HPA axis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35:

17–22.

Gluckman PD, Hanson MA, Pinal C. 2005. The developmental

origins of adult disease. Matern Child Nutr 1:130–141.

Gluckman PD, Hanson MA, Cooper C, Thornburg KL. 2008.

Effect of in utero and early-life conditions on adult health and

disease. N Engl J Med 359:61–73.

Gluckman PD, Hanson MA, Bateson P, Beedle AS, Law CM,

Bhutta ZA, Bhutta ZA, Anokhin KV, Bougneres P, Chandak

GR, Dasgupta P, Smith GD, Ellison PT, Forrester TE, Gilbert

SF, Jablonka E, Kaplan H, Prentice AM, Simpson SJ, Uauy R,

West-Eberhard MJ. 2009. Towards a new developmental

synthesis: Adaptive developmental plasticity and human disease.

Lancet 373:1654–1657.

Grant KA, McMahon C, Austin MP, Reilly N, Leader L, Ali S.

2009. Maternal prenatal anxiety, postnatal caregiving and

infants’ cortisol responses to the still-face procedure. Dev

Psychobiol 51:625–637.

Gunnell D, Rasmussen F, Fouskakis D, Tynelius P, Harrison G.

2003. Patterns of fetal and childhood growth and the

development of psychosis in young males: A cohort study. Am

J Epidemiol 158:291–300.

Gutteling BM, de Weerth C, Buitelaar JK. 2004. Maternal prenatal

stress and 4–6 year old children’s salivary cortisol concentrations

pre- and post-vaccination. Stress 7:257–260.

Gutteling BM, de Weerth C, Buitelaar JK. 2005. Prenatal stress and

children’s cortisol reaction to the first day of school.

Psychoneuroendocrinology 30:541–549.

Gutteling BM, de Weerth C, Willemsen-Swinkels SH, Huizink AC,

Mulder EJ, Visser GH, Buitelaar JK. 2005b. The effects of

prenatal stress on temperament and problem behavior of

27-month-old toddlers. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 14:41–51.

Gutteling BM, de Weerth C, Zandbelt N, Mulder EJ, Visser GH,

Buitelaar JK. 2006. Does maternal prenatal stress adversely

affect the child’s learning and memory at age six? J Abnorm

Child Psychol 34:789–798.

Hanson MA, Gluckman PD. 2008. Developmental origins of health

and disease: New insights. Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol 102:

90–93.

Harvison KW, Molfese DL, Woodruff-Borden J, Weigel RA. 2009.

Neonatal auditory evoked responses are related to perinatal

maternal anxiety. Brain Cogn 71:369–374.

Heinonen K, Raikkonen K, Pesonen AK, Kajantie E, Andersson S,

Eriksson JG, Niemela A, Vartia T, Peltola J, Lano A. 2008.

Prenatal and postnatal growth and cognitive abilities at 56

months of age: A longitudinal study of infants born at term.

Pediatrics 121:e1325–e1333.

Heinonen K, Raikkonen K, Pesonen AK, Andersson S, Kajantie E,

Eriksson JG, Wolke D, Lano A. 2010. Behavioural symptoms of

attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in preterm and term

children born small and appropriate for gestational age: A

longitudinal study. BMC Pediatr 15:10–91.

Heinonen K, Raikkonen K, Pesonen AK, Andersson S, Kajantie E,

Eriksson JG, Wolke D, Lano A. 2011. Longitudinal study of

smoking cessation before pregnancy and children’s cognitive

abilities at 56 months of age. Early Hum Dev 87(5):353–359.

Hellemans KG, Sliwowska JH, Verma P, Weinberg J. 2010. Prenatal

alcohol exposure: Fetal programming and later life vulnerability

to stress, depression and anxiety disorders. Neurosci Biobehav

Rev 34:791–807.

Henrichs J, Schenk JJ, Schimdt HG, Velders FP, Hofman A, Jaddoe

VWV, Verhulst FC, Tiemeier H. 2009. Maternal pre- and

postnatal anxiety and infant temperament. The generation R

study. Inf Child Dev 18:556–572.

Hernandez-Martinez C, Arija V, Balaguer A, Cavalle P, Canals J.

2008. Do the emotional states of pregnant women affect

neonatal behaviour? Early Hum Dev 84:745–750.

Hernandez-Reif M, Field T, Diego M, Ruddock M. 2006. Greater

arousal and less attentiveness to face/voice stimuli by neonates of

depressed mothers on the Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral

Assessment Scale. Inf Child Dev 29:594–598.

Holmes MC, Abrahamsen CT, French KL, Paterson JM, Mullins

JJ, Seckl JR. 2006. The mother or the fetus? 11beta-

hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2 null mice provide evidence

for direct fetal programming of behavior by endogenous

glucocorticoids. J Neurosci 26:3840–3844.

Huizink AC, de Medina PG, Mulder EJ, Visser GH, Buitelaar JK.

2002. Psychological measures of prenatal stress as predictors of

infant temperament. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 41:

1078–1085.

Huizink AC, Robles de Medina P, Mulder EJ, Visser GH, Buitelaar

JK. 2003. Stress during pregnancy is associated with develop-

mental outcome in infancy. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 44:

810–818.

Huizink AC, Bartels M, Rose RJ, Pulkkinen L, Eriksson CJ, Kaprio

J. 2008. Chernobyl exposure as stressor during pregnancy and

hormone levels in adolescent offspring. J Epidemiol Community

Health 62:e5.

IJzerman R, Stehouwer CD, de Geus EJ, van Weissenbruch MM,

Delemarre-van de Waal HA, Boomsma DI. 2003. Low birth

weight is associated with increased sympathetic activity:

Dependence on genetic factors. Circulation 108:566–571.

Isbrucker RA, Burdock GA. 2006. Risk and safety assessment on the

consumption of Licorice root (Glycyrrhiza sp.), its extract and

powder as a food ingredient, with emphasis on the pharmacology

and toxicology of glycyrrhizin. Regul Toxicol Pharmacol 46:

167–192.

Jones A, Godfrey KM, Wood P, Osmond C, Goulden P, Phillips DI.

2006. Fetal growth and the adrenocortical response to

psychological stress. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 91:1868–1871.

Jones A, Beda A, Ward AM, Osmond C, Phillips DI, Moore VM,

Simpson DM. 2007. Size at birth and autonomic function

during psychological stress. Hypertension 49:548–555.

Kajantie E. 2008. Early-life events. Effects on aging. Hormones

(Athens) 7:101–113.

Kajantie E, Raikkonen K. 2010. Early life predictors of the

physiological stress response later in life. Neurosci Biobehav Rev

35:23–32.

Kajantie E, Feldt K, Raikkonen K, Phillips DI, Osmond C,

Heinonen K, Pesonen AK, Andersson S, Barker DJ, Eriksson

JG. 2007. Body size at birth predicts hypothalamic–pituitary–

adrenal axis response to psychosocial stress at age 60 to 70 years.

J Clin Endocrinol Metab 92:4094–4100.

Khashan AS, Abel KM, McNamee R, Pedersen MG, Webb RT,

Baker PN, Kenny LC, Mortensen PB. 2008a. Higher risk of

offspring schizophrenia following antenatal maternal exposure to

severe adverse life events. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65:146–152.

Khashan AS, McNamee R, Abel KM, Pedersen MG, Webb RT,

Kenny LC, Mortensen PB, Baker PN. 2008b. Reduced infant

K. Raikkonen et al.600

Stre

ss D

ownl

oade

d fr

om in

form

ahea

lthca

re.c

om b

y U

nive

rsite

it V

an T

ilbur

g on

03/

13/1

3Fo

r pe

rson

al u

se o

nly.

birthweight consequent upon maternal exposure to severe life

events. Psychosom Med 70:688–694.

Kinney DK, Miller AM, Crowley DJ, Huang E, Gerber E. 2008.

Autism prevalence following prenatal exposure to hurricanes and

tropical storms in Louisiana. J Autism Dev Disord 38:481–488.

Knorr U, Vinberg M, Kessing LV, Wetterslev J. 2010. Salivary

cortisol in depressed patients versus control persons: A

systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology

35:1275–1286.

Lahti J, Raikkonen K, Kajantie E, Heinonen K, Pesonen AK,

Jarvenpaa AL, Strandberg T. 2006. Small body size at birth and

behavioural symptoms of ADHD in children aged five to six

years. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 47:1167–1174.

Lahti J, Raikkonen K, Sovio U, Miettunen J, Hartikainen AL, Pouta

A, Taanila A, Joukamaa M, Jarvelin MR, Veijola J. 2009. Early-

life origins of schizotypal traits in adulthood. Br J Psychiatry 195:

132–137.

Lahti J, Raikkonen K, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K, Kajantie E,

Forsen T, Osmond C, Barker DJ, Eriksson JG. 2010a. Prenatal

growth, postnatal growth, and trait anxiety in late adulthood—

the Helsinki Birth Cohort Study. Acta Psychiatr Scand 121:

227–235.

Lahti M, Raikkonen K, Wahlbeck K, Heinonen K, Forsen T,

Kajantie E, Pesonen AK, Osmond C, Barker DJ, Eriksson JG.

2010b. Prenatal origins of hospitalization for personality

disorders: The Helsinki birth cohort study. Psychiatry Res 179:

226–230.

Langley-Evans SC, Phillips GJ, Benediktsson R, Gardner DS,

Edwards CR, Jackson AA, Seckl JR. 1996. Protein intake in

pregnancy, placental glucocorticoid metabolism and the

programming of hypertension in the rat. Placenta 17:169–172.

Laplante DP, Barr RG, Brunet A, Galbaud du Fort G, Meaney ML,

Saucier JF, Zelazo PR, King S. 2004. Stress during pregnancy

affects general intellectual and language functioning in human

toddlers. Pediatr Res 56:400–410.

Laplante DP, Brunet A, Schmitz N, Ciampi A, King S. 2008.

Project Ice Storm: Prenatal maternal stress affects cognitive and

linguistic functioning in 5 1/2-year-old children. J Am Acad

Child Adolesc Psychiatry 47:1063–1072.

Levitt NS, Lambert EV, Woods D, Hales CN, Andrew R, Seckl JR.

2000. Impaired glucose tolerance and elevated blood pressure in

low birth weight, nonobese, young south african adults: Early

programming of cortisol axis. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 85:

4611–4618.

Li J, Vestergaard M, Obel C, Christensen J, Precht DH, Lu M,

Olsen J. 2009a. A nationwide study on the risk of autism after

prenatal stress exposure to maternal bereavement. Pediatrics

123:1102–1107.

Li J, Vestergaard M, Obel C, Precht DH, Christensen J, Lu M,

Olsen J. 2009b. Prenatal stress and cerebral palsy: A nationwide

cohort study in Denmark. Psychosom Med 71:615–618.

Lindsay RS, Lindsay RM, Edwards CR, Seckl JR. 1996a. Inhibition

of 11-beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase in pregnant rats and

the programming of blood pressure in the offspring. Hyperten-

sion 27:1200–1204.

Lindsay RS, Lindsay RM, Waddell BJ, Seckl JR. 1996b. Prenatal

glucocorticoid exposure leads to offspring hyperglycaemia in the

rat: Studies with the 11 beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase

inhibitor carbenoxolone. Diabetologia 39:1299–1305.

Lou HC, Hansen D, Nordentoft M, Pryds O, Jensen F, Nim J,

Hemmingsen R. 1994. Prenatal stressors of human life affect

fetal brain development. Dev Med Child Neurol 36:826–832.

Lucassen PJ, Bosch OJ, Jousma E, Kromer SA, Andrew R, Seckl JR,

Neumann ID. 2009. Prenatal stress reduces postnatal neurogen-

esis in rats selectively bred for high, but not low, anxiety: Possible

key role of placental 11beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type

2. Eur J Neurosci 29:97–103.

MacArthur BA, Howie RN, Dezoete JA, Elkins J. 1982. School

progress and cognitive development of 6-year-old children

whose mothers were treated antenatally with betamethasone.

Pediatrics 70:99–105.

Mairesse J, Lesage J, Breton C, Breant B, Hahn T, Darnaudery M,

Dickson SL, Seckl J, Blondeau B, Vieau D, Maccari S, Viltart O.

2007. Maternal stress alters endocrine function of the feto-

placental unit in rats. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 292:

E1526–E1533.

Martin RP, Noyes J, Wisenbaker J, Huttunen M. 1999. Prediction of

early childhood negative emotionality and inhibition from

maternal distress during pregnancy. Merrill-Palmer Q 45:

370–391.

McGrath JM, Records K, Rice M. 2008. Maternal depression and

infant temperament characteristics. Infant Behav Dev 31:71–80.

Meaney MJ. 2010. Epigenetics and the biological definition of

gene £ environment interactions. Child Dev 81:41–79.

Mennes M. 2008. Longitudinal study on the effects of maternal

anxiety during pregnancy: Neuropsychological and neurophy-

siological examination of cognitive control in the adolescent

offspring. Leuven: Katholieke Universiteit Leuven.

Mennes M, Stiers P, Lagae L, Van den Bergh BR. 2006. Long-term

cognitive sequelae of antenatal maternal anxiety: Involvement of

the orbitofrontal cortex. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30:1078–1086.

Mennes M, Van den Bergh BR, Lagae L, Stiers P. 2009.

Developmental brain alterations in 17 year old boys are related

to antenatal maternal anxiety. Clin Neurophysiol 120:

1116–1122.

Niederhofer H, Reiter A. 2004. Prenatal maternal stress, prenatal

fetal movements and perinatal temperament factors influence

behavior and school marks at the age of 6 years. Fetal Diagn

Ther 19:160–162.

Obel C. 2003. Epidemiological studies of stress during pregnancy

and fetal brain development. Denmark: University of Aarhus.

Oberlander TF, Weinberg J, Papsdorf M, Grunau R, Misri S, Devlin

AM. 2008. Prenatal exposure to maternal depression, neonatal

methylation of human glucocorticoid receptor gene (NR3C1)

and infant cortisol stress responses. Epigenetics 3:97–106.

O’Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J, Beveridge M, Glover V. 2002.

Maternal antenatal anxiety and children’s behavioural/emotional

problems at 4 years. Report from the Avon Longitudinal Study

of Parents and Children. Br J Psychiatry 180:502–508.

O’Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J, Glover V. 2003. Maternal

antenatal anxiety and behavioural/emotional problems in

children: A test of a programming hypothesis. J Child Psychol

Psychiatry 44:1025–1036.

O’Connor TG, Ben-Shlomo Y, Heron J, Golding J, Adams D,

Glover V. 2005. Prenatal anxiety predicts individual differences

in cortisol in pre-adolescent children. Biol Psychiatry 58:

211–217.

O’Keane V, Lightman S, Marsh M, Pawlby S, Papadopoulos AS,

Taylor A, Moore R, Patrick K. 2011. Increased pituitary–

adrenal activation and shortened gestation in a sample of

depressed pregnant women: A pilot study. J Affect Disord 130:

300–305.

O’Regan D, Welberg LL, Holmes MC, Seckl JR. 2001.

Glucocorticoid programming of pituitary–adrenal function:

Mechanisms and physiological consequences. Semin Neonatol

6:319–329.

Pesonen AK, Raikkonen K, Strandberg TE, Jarvenpaa AL. 2005.

Continuity of maternal stress from the pre- to the postnatal

period: Associations with infant’s positive, negative and overall

temperamental reactivity. Inf Behav Devel 28:36–47.

Pesonen AK, Raikkonen K, Kajantie E, Heinonen K, Strandberg

TE, Jarvenpaa AL. 2006. Fetal programming of temperamental

negative affectivity among children born healthy at term. Dev

Psychobiol 48:633–643.

Pesonen AK, Raikkonen K, Heinonen K, Kajantie E, Forsen T,

Eriksson JG. 2007. Depressive symptoms in adults separated

from their parents as children: A natural experiment during

World War II. Am J Epidemiol 166:1126–1133.

Stress, glucocorticoids and liquorice in human pregnancy 601

Stre

ss D

ownl

oade

d fr

om in

form

ahea

lthca

re.c

om b

y U

nive

rsite

it V

an T

ilbur

g on

03/

13/1

3Fo

r pe

rson

al u

se o

nly.

Pesonen AK, Raikkonen K, Heinonen K, Andersson S, Hovi P,

Jarvenpaa AL, Eriksson JG, Kajantie E. 2008a. Personality of

young adults born prematurely: The Helsinki study of very low

birth weight adults. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 49:609–617.

Pesonen AK, Raikkonen K, Heinonen K, Kajantie E, Forsen T,

Eriksson JG. 2008b. Reproductive traits following a parent-child

separation trauma during childhood: A natural experiment

during World War II. Am J Hum Biol 20:345–351.

Pesonen AK, Raikkonen K, Matthews K, Heinonen K, Paavonen

JE, Lahti J, Komsi N, Lemola S, Jarvenpaa AL, Kajantie E,

Strandberg T. 2009. Prenatal origins of poor sleep in children.

Sleep 32:1086–1092.

Pesonen AK, Raikkonen K, Feldt K, Heinonen K, Osmond C,

Phillips DI, Barker DJ, Eriksson JG, Kajantie E. 2010.

Childhood separation experience predicts HPA axis hormonal

responses in late adulthood: A natural experiment of World War

II. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35:758–767.

Pesonen AK, Raikkonen K, Kajantie E, Heinonen K, Osmond C,

Barker DJ, Forsen T, Eriksson JG. 2011. Intergenerational social

mobility following early life stress. Ann Med 43(4):320–328.

Phillips DI, Jones A. 2006. Fetal programming of autonomic and

HPA function: Do people who were small babies have enhanced

stress responses? J Physiol 572:45–50.

Phillips DI, Fall CHD, Whorwood CB, Seckl JR, Wood PJ, Barker

DJP, et al. 1998. Elevated plasma cortisol concentrations: A link

between low birth weight and the insulin resistance syndrome?

J Clin Endocrinol Metab 83:757–760.

Phillips DI, Walker BR, Reynolds RM, Flanagan DE, Wood PJ,

Osmond C, Barker DJ, Whorwood CB. 2000. Low birth weight

predicts elevated plasma cortisol concentrations in adults from 3

populations. Hypertension 35:1301–1306.

Ponirakis A, Susman EJ, Stifter CA. 1998. Negative emotionality

and cortisol during adolescent pregnancy and its effects on infant

health and autonomic nervous system reactivity. Dev Psychobiol

33:163–174.

Raikkonen K, Pesonen AK. 2009. Early life origins of psychological

development and mental health. Scand J Psychol 50:583–591.

Raikkonen K, Pesonen AK, Kajantie E, Heinonen K, Forsen T,

Phillips DI, Osmond C, Barker DJ, Eriksson JG. 2007. Length of

gestation and depressive symptoms at age 60 years. Br J

Psychiatry 190:469–474.

Raikkonen K, Pesonen A-K, Heinonen K, Lahti J, Kajantie E,

Forsen T, Osmond C, Barker DJ, Eriksson JG. 2008a. Infant

growth and hostility in adult life. Psychosom Med 70:306–313.

Raikkonen K, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K, Kajantie E, Hovi P,

Jarvenpaa AL, Eriksson JG, Andersson S. 2008b. Depression in

young adults with very low birth weight: The Helsinki study of

very low-birth-weight adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65:290–296.

Raikkonen K, Forsen T, Henriksson M, Kajantie E, Laaksonen I,

Leskinen JT, Laaksonen I, Osmond C, Barker DJ, Eriksson JG.

2009a. Growth trajectories and intellectual abilities in young

adulthood: The Helsinki Birth Cohort Study. Am J Epidemiol

15:447–455.

Raikkonen K, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K, Lahti J, Komsi N,

Eriksson JG, Seckl JR, Jarvenpaa AL, Strandberg TE. 2009b.

Maternal licorice consumption and detrimental cognitive and

psychiatric outcomes in children. Am J Epidemiol 170:

1137–1146.

Raikkonen K, Lahti M, Heinonen K, Pesonen AK, Wahlbeck K,

Kajantie E, Osmond C, Barker DJP, Eriksson JG. 2011. Risk of

severe mental disorders in adults separated temporarily from

their parents in childhood: The Helsinki birth cohort study.

J Psychiatr Res 45:332–338.

Raikkonen K, Matthews KA, Pesonen AK, Pyhala R, Paavonen EJ,

Feldt K, Jones A, Phillips DI, Seckl JR, Heinonen K, Lahti J,

Komsi N, Jarvenpaa AL, Eriksson JG, Strandberg TE, Kajantie

E. 2010b. Poor sleep and altered hypothalamic–pituitary–

adrenocortical and sympatho–adrenal–medullary system

activity in children. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 95:2254–2261.

Raikkonen K, Seckl JR, Heinonen K, Pyhala R, Feldt K, Jones A,

Pesonen AK, Phillips DI, Lahti J, Jarvenpaa AL, Eriksson JG,

Matthews KA, Strandberg TE, Kajantie E. 2010c. Maternal

prenatal licorice consumption alters hypothalamic–pituitary–

adrenocortical axis function in children. Psychoneuroendocri-

nology 35:1587–1593.

Reynolds RM, Walker BR, Syddall HE, Andrew R, Wood PJ,

Whorwood CB, Phillips DI. 2001. Altered control of cortisol

secretion in adult men with low birth weight and cardiovascular

risk factors. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 86:245–250.

Reynolds RM, Godfrey KM, Barker M, Osmond C, Phillips DI.

2007. Stress responsiveness in adult life: Influence of mother’s

diet in late pregnancy. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 92:2208–2210.

Rieger M, Pirke KM, Buske-Kirschbaum A, Wurmser H, Papousek

M, Hellhammer DH. 2004. Influence of stress during pregnancy

on HPA activity and neonatal behavior. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1032:

228–230.

Rodriguez A. 2010. Maternal pre-pregnancy obesity and risk for

inattention and negative emotionality in children. J Child

Psychol Psychiatry 51:134–143.

Rodriguez A, Bohlin G. 2005. Are maternal smoking and stress

during pregnancy related to ADHD symptoms in children?

J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46:246–254.

Roelofs K, van Peer J, Berretty E, Jong P, Spinhoven P, Elzinga BM.

2009. Hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis hyperresponsive-

ness is associated with increased social avoidance behavior in

social phobia. Biol Psychiatry 65:336–343.

Rogerson FM, Kayes KM, White PC. 1997. Variation in placental

type 2 11beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase activity is not

related to birth weight or placental weight. Mol Cell Endocrinol

128:103–109.

Roseboom TJ, van der Meulen JH, Ravelli AC, Osmond C, Barker

DJ, Bleker OP. 2001. Effects of prenatal exposure to the Dutch

famine on adult disease in later life: An overview. Mol Cell

Endocrinol 185:93–98.

Sarkar P, Bergman K, O’Connor TG, Glover V. 2008. Maternal

antenatal anxiety and amniotic fluid cortisol and testosterone:

Possible implications for foetal programming. J Neuroendocrinol

20:489–496.

Schlotz W, Jones A, Godfrey KM, Phillips DI. 2008. Effortful

control mediates associations of fetal growth with hyperactivity

and behavioural problems in 7- to 9-year-old children. J Child

Psychol Psychiatry 49:1228–1236.

Schmidt LA, Miskovic V, Boyle MH, Saigal S. 2008. Shyness and

timidity in young adults who were born at extremely low birth

weight. Pediatrics 122:e181–e187.

Seckl JR. 2001. Glucocorticoid programming of the fetus; adult

phenotypes and molecular mechanisms. Mol Cell Endocrinol

185:61–71.

Seckl JR. 2008. Glucocorticoids, developmental “programming”

and the risk of affective dysfunction. Prog Brain Res 167:17–34.

Seckl JR, Meaney MJ. 2004a. Glucocorticoid programming

Biobehavioral stress response: Protective and damaging effects,

New York: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,

Vol. 1032. pp 63–84.

Seckl JR, Meaney MJ. 2004b. Glucocorticoid programming. Ann N

Y Acad Sci 1032:63–84.

Seckl JR, Meaney MJ. 2006. Glucocorticoid “programming” and

PTSD risk. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1071:351–378.

Seckl JR, Holmes MC. 2007. Mechanisms of disease: Glucocorti-

coids, their placental metabolism and fetal “programming” of

adult pathophysiology. Nat Clin Pract Endocrinol Metab 3:

479–488.

Shenkin SD, Starr JM, Deary IJ. 2004. Birth weight and cognitive

ability in childhood: A systematic review. Psychol Bull 130:

989–1013.

Sloboda DM, Challis JR, Moss TJ, Newnham JP. 2005. Synthetic

glucocorticoids: Antenatal administration and long-term impli-

cations. Curr Pharm Des 11:1459–1472.

K. Raikkonen et al.602

Stre

ss D

ownl

oade

d fr

om in

form

ahea

lthca

re.c

om b

y U

nive

rsite

it V

an T

ilbur

g on

03/

13/1

3Fo

r pe

rson

al u

se o

nly.

Speirs HJ, Seckl JR, Brown RW. 2004. Ontogeny of glucocorticoid

receptor and 11beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type-1 gene

expression identifies potential critical periods of glucocorticoid

susceptibility during development. J Endocrinol 181:105–116.

Stewart PM, Whorwood CB, Mason JI. 1995. Type 2 11 beta-

hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase in foetal and adult life. J Steroid

Biochem Mol Biol 55:465–471.

Strandberg TE, Jarvenpaa AL, Vanhanen H, McKeigue PM. 2001.

Birth outcome in relation to licorice consumption during

pregnancy. Am J Epidemiol 153:1085–1088.

Strandberg TE, Andersson S, Jarvenpaa AL, McKeigue PM. 2002.

Preterm birth and licorice consumption during pregnancy. Am J

Epidemiol 156:803–805.

Strang-Karlsson S, Raikkonen K, Kajantie E, Andersson S, Hovi P,

Heinonen K, Pesonen AK, Jarvenpaa AL, Eriksson JG,

Paavonen EJ. 2008a. Sleep quality in young adults with very

low birth weight—the Helsinki study of very low birth weight

adults. J Pediatr Psychol 33:387–395.

Strang-Karlsson S, Raikkonen K, Pesonen AK, Kajantie E,

Paavonen EJ, Lahti J, Hovi P, Heinonen K, Jarvenpaa AL,

Eriksson JG, Andersson S. 2008b. Very low birth weight and

behavioral symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

in young adulthood: The Helsinki study of very-low-birth-weight

adults. Am J Psychiatry 165:1345–1353.

Trautman PD, Meyer-Bahlburg HFL, Postelnek J, New MI. 1995.

Effects of early prenatal dexamethasone on the cognitive and

behavioral development of young children: Results of a pilot

study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 20:439–449.

Tuovinen S, Raikkonen K, Kajantie E, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K,

Osmond C, Barker DJ, Eriksson JG. 2010. Depressive

symptoms in adulthood and intrauterine exposure to pre-

eclampsia: The Helsinki Birth Cohort Study. Br J Obstet

Gynaecol 117:1236–1242.

Tuovinen SH, Raikkonen K, Kajantie E, Lehtinen J, Henriksson M,

Pesonen AK, Heinonen K, Osmond C, Barker D, Eriksson JG.

2011. Hypertensive disorders in pregnancy and intellectual

abilities in the offspring in young adulthood: The Helsinki birth

cohort study. Ann Med. Apr 15. [Epub ahead of print].

Van den Bergh BR. 1990. The influence of maternal emotions

during pregnancy on fetal and neonatal behavior, Win, 1990.

J Prenatal Perinatal Psychol Health 5:119–130.

Van den Bergh BR. 1992. Maternal emotions during pregnancy and

fetal and neonatal behavior. Oxford University Press: Oxford,

UK.

Van den Bergh BR, Marcoen A. 2004. High antenatal maternal

anxiety is related to ADHD symptoms, externalizing problems,

and anxiety in 8- and 9-year-olds. Child Dev 75:1085–1097.

Van den Bergh BR, Mennes M, Oosterlaan J, Stevens V, Stiers P,

Marcoen A, Lagae L. 2005a. High antenatal maternal anxiety is

related to impulsivity during performance on cognitive tasks in

14- and 15-year-olds. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:259–269.

Van den Bergh BR, Mulder EJ, Mennes M, Glover V. 2005b.

Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and the neurobehavioural

development of the fetus and child: Links and possible

mechanisms. A review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:237–258.

Van den Bergh BR, Mennes M, Stevens V, van der Meere J, Borger

N, Stiers P, Marcoen A, Lagae L. 2006. ADHD deficit as

measured in adolescent boys with a continuous performance task

is related to antenatal maternal anxiety. Pediatr Res 59:78–82.

van der Wal MF, van Eijsden M, Bonsel GJ. 2007. Stress and

emotional problems during pregnancy and excessive infant

crying. J Dev Behav Pediatr 28:431–437.

Van den Bergh BR, Van Calster B, Smits T, Van Huffel S, Lagae L.

2008. Antenatal maternal anxiety is related to HPA-axis

dysregulation and self-reported depressive symptoms in

adolescence: A prospective study on the fetal origins of depressed

mood. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:536–545.

Vaughn B, Bradley C, Joffe L, Seifer R, Barglow P. 1987. Maternal

characteristics measured prenatally are predictive of ratings of

temperamental “difficulty” on the Carey Infant Temperament

Questionnaire. Dev Psychol 23:152–161.

Wahlbeck K, Forsen T, Osmond C, Barker DJ, Eriksson JG. 2001.

Association of schizophrenia with low maternal body mass index,

small size at birth, and thinness during childhood. Arch Gen

Psychiatry 58:48–52.

Warner MJ, Ozanne SE. 2010. Mechanisms involved in the

developmental programming of adulthood disease. Biochem J :

427.

Welberg LA, Seckl JR, Holmes MC. 2000. Inhibition of 11beta-

hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase, the foeto-placental barrier to

maternal glucocorticoids, permanently programs amygdala GR

mRNA expression and anxiety-like behaviour in the offspring.

Eur J Neurosci 12:1047–1054.

Wisborg K, Barklin A, Hedegaard M, Henriksen TB. 2008.

Psychological stress during pregnancy and stillbirth: Prospective

study. BJOG: Int J Obstet Gynaecol 115:882–885.

Wust S, Entringer S, Federenko IS, Schlotz W, Hellhammer DH.

2005. Birth weight is associated with salivary cortisol responses

to psychosocial stress in adult life. Psychoneuroendocrinology

30:591–598.

Yeh TF, Lin YJ, Lin HC, Huang CC, Hsieh WS, Lin CH, Tsai CH.

2004. Outcomes at school age after postnatal dexamethasone

therapy for lung disease of prematurity. N Engl J Med 350:

1304–1313.

Yehuda R, Engel SM, Brand SR, Seckl J, Marcus SM, Berkowitz

GS. 2005. Transgenerational effects of posttraumatic stress

disorder in babies of mothers exposed to the World Trade Center

attacks during pregnancy. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 90:

4115–4118.

Stress, glucocorticoids and liquorice in human pregnancy 603

Stre

ss D

ownl

oade

d fr

om in

form

ahea

lthca

re.c

om b

y U

nive

rsite

it V

an T

ilbur

g on

03/

13/1

3Fo

r pe

rson

al u

se o

nly.