30
Institute of International Education Department of Education Testing and Examining Intercultural Transfer: Case Study of an International University Cohort Assignment for the course Introduction to Educational Research Methods Aljaz Kovac (May 2015)

Testing and Examining Intercultural Transfer

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

 

 

 

Institute  of  International  Education  

Department  of  Education  

 

 

Testing  and  Examining  Intercultural  

Transfer:  Case  Study  of  an  International  

University  Cohort  

 

Assignment  for  the  course  

Introduction  to  Educational  Research  Methods  

Aljaz Kovac

(May  2015)  

 

  2  

Table  of  Contents  

INTRODUCTION & BACKGROUND 3

MULTICULTURALISM & GLOBALIZATION 3

MULTICULTURAL VS. INTERCULTURAL 6

INTERCULTURAL TRANSFER (ICT) & EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE (EI) 8

PSYCHOMETRIC TESTING OF “INTERCULTURAL TRANSFER” 9

AIMS AND RESEARCH QUESTIONS 10

LIMITATIONS AND DELIMITATIONS OF THE RESEARCH 13

SIGNIFICANCE OF THE RESEARCH 14

ETHICAL CONSIDERATIONS 15

REVIEW OF RELEVANT CONCEPTS 17

MULTICULTURALISM & ICT 17

PSYCHOMETRIC TESTING: TEIQUE, BIG 5 PERSONALITY TEST, STANFORD-BINET

IQ TEST, RAVEN’S MATRICES TEST 18

RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 19

QUANTITATIVE RESEARCH 20

QUANTITATIVE DATA ANALYSIS 22

QUALITATIVE RESEARCH 22

QUALITATIVE ANALYSIS 23

CONCLUSION & RESEARCH SCHEDULE 24

REFERENCES 25

 

  3  

Introduction  &  Background  

Multiculturalism  &  Globalization    

In 1962 Marshall McLuhan, the man who predicted the Word Wide Web and coined

the term “global village”, said that our world is now a world of “simultaneous events

and overall awareness” (McLuhan, 1962, p. 40) where the distinction between

cultures, nationalities or “us” and “them” has become blurred. This has happened due

to communication and transportation networks that have brought various cultures,

races and religions into a web of “interdependence and a common fate.” (Kim, 2008,

p. 359) This, of course, presents us with specific challenges: the democratic vision of

a diverse and peaceful world is constantly challenged by particular group identities

dominating the notion of cosmopolitan citizenry. Often overlooked in the identity

polemics are the ideals of multiculturalism that promote a coexistence and a mutual

understanding with the ability to learn from each other rather than encroach on

specific forms of knowledge that the individual groups may possess.

Nowhere do these struggles come to the fore more than in a true multicultural

context “where individuals from multiple cultures or nations are operating in a foreign

culture, and their actions or behaviors in this context are referred to as international

performance.” (Burke, Watkins, & Guzman, 2009, p. 475) This type of performance

differs significantly from cross-cultural performance where individuals from the same

background or culture work and perform in a host environment. A multi-cultural

performance entails an additional aspect to simply working in a foreign country:

individuals need to interact with and adjust to other similar individuals who are also

  4  

foreigners in their common host culture. Interestingly enough, “empirical evidence

demonstrates a negative relationship between visible cultural differences and

adaptation to a new living environment” (ib.) There are several other definitions of

multiculturalism: Dolce describes multiculturalism as “a reflection of a value system

which emphasizes acceptance of behavioral differences deriving from differing

cultural systems and an active support of the right of such differences to exist.”

(Dolce, 1973, p. 283) Here is another, more comprehensive definition:

Firstly, multiculturalism can refer to a demographic feature, more specifically the poly-ethnic

composition of a society. Secondly, the concept is used by policymakers to denote a specific type of

policy about cultural diversity . . . Thirdly, multiculturalism as a psychological concept is an attitude

related to the political ideology, which refers to the acceptance of, and support for, the culturally

heterogeneous composition of the population of a society.” (Vijver, R., Breaugelmans, Schalk-Soekar,

& G., 2008, p. 93)

But what enables us to assume a psychological stance of multiculturalism and to build

such interpersonal relationships? Researchers mention a specific type of empathy, a

“culturally oriented empathy”, meaning “the ability of taking another person’s

ethnocultural perspective and sharing others’ experiences and feelings of being

discriminated.” (Albiero & Matricardi, 2013, p. 648) Here we arrive at an intersection

between culture theory and psychology: how can we investigate the difference

between a multicultural setting and the psychological factors that determine the

outcome of such a setting? A good starting point would be to make the distinction

between multiculturalism and interculturalism, the two terms frequently used in

literature on cultural diversity. Although a clear distinction between them does not

exist, they are clearly not synonyms either: their semantic meaning and usage in

  5  

scientific literature show that “multicultural” denotes “cultural plurality in a particular

context”, while “intercultural” denotes “a form of exchange or interaction between

cultures or cultural perspectives.” (Arasaratnam, 2013, p. 2) In other words,

“multicultural” describes a pluralistic state, whereas “intercultural” describes a

process that occurs, to a greater or lesser extent, in such a state:

For example, Halualani (2010) speaks of intercultural interactions in a multicultural university, in her

study of students and how they define intercultural interactions. Similarly, Ippolito (2007) refers to

intercultural education in multicultural universities, making the distinction between cultural plurality in

an environment (multicultural university) and the process of understanding how to socially operate in

that environment (intercultural learning).” (ib., p. 2)

Intercultural interaction is therefore a prerequisite for multicultural coexistence! It

would thus make sense to try and understand the dynamics and factors of intercultural

interaction before applying ourselves to political agendas that promote a vague notion

of multiculturalism. A multicultural setting in itself will not be enough to ensure the

promotion of multicultural ideals: several studies show that what tends to happen in a

multicultural setting instead of intercultural interaction is actually plain and simple

acculturation where the majority favor the minority to adapt and the minority favors

cultural autonomy: a genuine cul-de-sac!

Based on findings from three studies conducted in the United Kingdom, Tip et al. (2012) found that

when members of the majority group perceived that minority group members want to maintain their

original culture, they (the majority group) feel threatened by it, resulting in unfavorable attitudes

toward multiculturalism. The converse was also found to be true.” (ib., p. 4)

  6  

But what happens in a multicultural environment that is set on equal ground, for

example, among an international group of equal members? Studies show that more

equality among members or the extent to which both or all groups are willing to give

up some of their ground “plays an important role in the construction of a truly

multicultural society.” (Liu, 2007, p. 771)

This is a vital piece of information for the implementation of the romantic

ideals of multiculturalism! Translated into psychological lingo, it means that

personality or emotional intelligence traits, as well as several other factors, such as

self-motivation; expectation and cultural background; gender and educational level;

etc. (Dandy & Pepua, 2010) play a key part in what I call “intercultural transfer”. I

define “intercultural transfer” or ICT as the “transfer of cultural and personal

knowledge”.

Multicultural  vs.  Intercultural    

The main reason why I posit “intercultural transfer” as a notion of its own is that the

results from several studies show that while individuals might live in a multicultural

community and thus by default take part in intercultural interaction, this in no way

means that this interaction will increase their sense of diversity or lead to “the

development of richer cognitive frameworks with which to relate to others.”

(Arasaratnam, 2013, p. 5) Furthermore, there seems to be a need for several factors to

combine in order for individuals to form identities that will allow them to “develop

attitudes that are associated with effective intercultural communication.” (ib., p. 7)

This process of developing such an identity or, due to the financial terminology

employed, the observable end-result of such an identity, I call intercultural transfer or

  7  

ICT. One of the aims of this paper will be to examine the factors that contribute to

ICT and the psychological dynamics of the very process.

There seems to be considerable confusion or vagueness in the field that would

grant such an exploratory examination: a search on the prominent ScienceDirect.com

for terms “differential psychology” and “intercultural” yields 1390 results: together

they combine into a plethora of theories, concepts and interpretations, with little

consensus! This is partly because intercultural communication is a relatively new

domain and it thus needs to establish itself with a flow of original theories “grounded

in detailed case research.” (Howell, 1979) The research that this paper proposes is

very much in that vein, aiming to ground the theory of intercultural transfer with a

detailed case study of a first-year university cohort at the department of International

and Comparative Education at Stockholm University.

Such a cohort seems to be the perfect setting for a detailed examination of the

factors of intercultural transfer. The department website states:

“IIE is an "international" institute, in the true meaning of the word. It has successfully created a multi-

cultural and multi-disciplinary research and teaching environment which attracts a number of visiting

scholars and lecturers as well as students from many parts of the world.” (University)

The actors on this “multicultural and multi-disciplinary” stage are equals among each

other, meaning that the process of acculturation will not be a factor. The controlled

environment of a university setting enables us to measure and observe the nature of

intercultural transfer. Intercultural communication has long been part of the academic

offer at business schools because “intercultural communication can be envisioned as a

life skill which allows one to function effectively in his or her day-to-day activities.”

(Chiper, 2013, p. 1641)

  8  

Intercultural  Transfer  (ICT)  &  Emotional  Intelligence  (EI)  

This paper proposes to explore intercultural transfer through the lens of emotional

intelligence as it is understood and explained by Trait EI theory developed by Dr. K.

V. Petrides at the London Psychometric Laboratory at UCL. The difference between

Trait EI theory and other measures of emotional intelligence is that Trait EI theory

doesn’t view emotional intelligence as a skill, but rather as a personality makeup,

which it tests through self-report measures. These correlate significantly with

personality and “non-significantly with cognitive ability” (Siegling, Nielsen, &

Petrides, 2014, p. 65), whereas maximum-performance measures show the opposite

trend. (Qualter, Gardner, Pope, Hutchinson, & Whiteley, 2012) The trend to describe

EI as a skill or a tool is clearly seen in the flood of pop psychology that has penetrated

the overall discussion on knowledge and skills. A recent article in the online

Telegraph (The Telegraph) reports on a study by the education provider Kaplan,

where emotional intelligence is placed on “the full list of competencies employers

sought in graduates.” (Kaplan, 2014, p. 15) Nonetheless, emotional intelligence as a

personality makeup can be a prerequisite for acquiring the soft skills that tend to be

valued greatly in the labor market nowadays, although we need to be careful not to

mix them up with the phenomenon of “cultural fit”, as the study on hiring and cultural

matching by Lauren A. Riviera clearly shows:

Drawing from 120 interviews with employers as well as participant observation of a hiring committee,

I argue that hiring is more than just a process of skills sorting; it is also a process of cultural matching

between candidates, evaluators, and firms. Employers sought candidates who were not only competent

  9  

but also culturally similar to themselves in terms of leisure pursuits, experiences, and self-presentation

styles.” (Rivera, 2012, p. 999)

But does that not make emotional intelligence as a personality makeup all the more

relevant, especially in terms of intercultural transfer? Studies show that culture shapes

our sense of self and that socialization influences our cognition, emotion and

behaviors (Markus & Kitayama, 1991), meaning that being exposed to unfamiliar

cultures will sensitize us and expose us to “alternative ways of thinking and acting.”

(Chao, Kung, & Yao, 2015, p. 81) This sort of exposure might result in effective

intercultural transfer, thus leading to new and quantifiable knowledge, as well as

newly acquired soft skills or useful ways of existing and communicating within and

among different cultural schemas, or it might result in drawback. (Buchtel, 2014) This

paper proposes to explore the various factors that might influence effective

intercultural transfer and to better understand the dynamics of its process.

Psychometric  Testing  of  “Intercultural  Transfer”    

Intercultural transfer can best be understood in light of psychological theories that

comprise emotional and general intelligence, meaning that psychometric testing will

need to be employed to explore the nature of intercultural transfer within the

examined cohort of the proposed case study. Empirical studies show that

psychometric tests, such as the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ),

are generally “a good predictor of socio-psychological adaptation” (Arasaratnam,

2013, p. 682) and that certain dimensions as measured and understood by certain

psychometric tests “are correlated with foreign language mastery” (ib.) or with

  10  

“international students’ adjustment.” (ib.) Psychometric testing with the Trait

Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue), “which was developed to measure

the construct of trait EI comprehensively and has been shown to possess superior

psychometric properties over other measures” (Siegling, Nielsen, & Petrides, 2014, p.

66) has revealed correlation between high trait EI scores and leadership skills.

Statistical and empirical evidence gained so far would justify the use of such

psychometric tests as relevant measures of emotional intelligence as the latent

variable that we construct and aim to observe.

Aims  and  Research  Questions  

The aim of the proposed research is to test and to examine the dynamics of

intercultural transfer within an international university cohort at the Department of

International and Comparative Education at Stockholm University.

The research questions can be categorized as follows:

• What are the dynamics of Intercultural Transfer (ICT)?

o What are the factors of ICT?

o How do the various factors of ICT correlate?

o When is ICT most effective (checking for personality traits, cognitive

ability, motivation, expectations, etc.)?

• How is Trait Emotional Intelligence correlated with ICT?

o How are the global trait and the sub-measures of Trait EI Theory

correlated with ICT?

  11  

o How are Trait EI theory and The Big 5 Personality test correlated as

markers of ICT?

o How can an improved understanding of EI and ICT contribute to the

educational process?

All of the above questions could be expanded upon and broken down into fragments

of minute psychological variables, but for the sake of brevity and clarity I shall not

attempt to present all of the sub-questions that every one of them entails. The nature

of the field is such that we are trying to measure concepts that are just that – concepts

based upon theory and prediction, meaning that they consist of many moving parts, or

in statistical language, they are latent variables that we attempt to observe by

measuring their so-called manifest variables. The first issue therefore is one of

construction. The second relevant issue is the question of causality: what factors

precede and cause what manifest variables? The third issue is the difficulty of

measurement: do the psychometric tests we apply really measure what we’re

attempting to measure? Considering these basic tenets the proposed research could be

placed somewhere between critical realism and interpretivism on the epistemological

scale and firmly in the constructionist tradition on the ontological scale. The latter is

easier to ascertain because constructionism suggests that the categories “do not have

built-in essences; instead, their meaning is constructed in and through interaction.”

(Bryman, 2012, p. 34) This is clearly what the categories of emotional intelligence or

personality are. The epistemological nature of the proposed research is more complex

because critical realists “are perfectly content to admit into their explanations

theoretical terms that are not directly amenable to observation” (ib., p. 29) and

“acknowledge and accept that the categories they employ to understand reality are

  12  

likely to be provisional,” (ib.), but the study will not employ retroductive reasoning or

aim to introduce “changes that can transform the status quo.” (ib.) The question of

causality offers great potential for the field of differential psychology, where

personality traits are generally taken to be source variables, despite theories of

personality dynamics indicating that traits should also be treated as outcome variables

(Petrides, p. 15): this research will therefore attempt to also investigate whether there

will be any change in personality traits after an intensive exposure to a multicultural

setting. Although the temptation to make causal inferences can be great, the research

will not deviate from its case study limits and will remain wary of the danger of such

conclusions. It will therefore simply observe the various variables at play and

examine their correlations under the umbrella notion of emotional intelligence. It will

therefore assume an interpretivist stance, very much in the tradition of Weber’s

Verstehen, and will employ middle-range theoretical concepts that are “much more

likely to be the focus of empirical enquiry.” (Bryman, 2012, p. 22). ICT can thus be

understood as an abstract concept, intended to help us conduct research, similar to

Goffman’s “stigma” or Hochschild’s “emotional labor.” (ib., p. 23) The approach will

therefore be deductive, but with inductive feedback to continuously reexamine the

methodology and the theory and to inform the various sub-questions that the study

will attempt to answer.

The quantitative part of the research will aim to test the following hypotheses:

H1: There will be greater intercultural transfer in students with higher Trait EI.

H1a: There will be a correlation in intercultural transfer between global Trait EI score

or certain Trait EI characteristics and certain factors of the Big 5 personality traits.

  13  

H2: Intercultural transfer will also depend on students’ expectations, motivation,

cognitive ability and academic achievement.

In proposing these hypotheses I am aware of the fact that Intercultural Transfer will

be quantitatively measured with factual information tests, which will rely on cognitive

ability and may thus not be a completely reliable marker of ICT as it is understood

and explained in the above theory, but the qualitative part of the research will aim to

overcome this delimitation by closely examining the respondents’ experience of ICT.

Given the fact that the TEIQue test, which has been empirically proven to be a

reliable measure of Trait Emotional Intelligence, is a self-report measure,

unstructured and semi-structured interviews may also be taken as a reliable measure

of information to which only the respondent has full access. (Petrides, p. 11)

Limitations  and  Delimitations  of  the  Research  

There are two kinds of limitations and delimitations that come with any research

project: the first kind has to do with time and financial constraints, or practical

constraints. The other kind has to do with the limitations that are part and parcel of the

chosen methodology and research design. In this case the research design will be a

case study, more precisely the critical case and the representative case. (Bryman,

2012, p. 70) The critical case because it will allow us “a better understanding of the

circumstances in which the hypothesis will and will not hold.” (ib.) And the

representative case because it will capture the conditions of an everyday situation

(namely a more or less typical international university cohort), although the

  14  

conditions of that everyday situation will be defined by a setting that is not

necessarily replicable or generalizable outside the university gates (namely a group of

equal individuals with no objectively hierarchical relations among them). The biggest

limitation of the proposed research is thus the common flaw of all case studies: it will

not be generalizable to the population, although its procedures will be replicable.

An overview of other concerns or possible limitations:

• Measurement validity: all the psychometric tests used are statistically reliable

measures. The factual information tests used to measure ICT and other

surveys used might be questionable and would need to be checked with a

statistical sample for reliability and validity.

• Internal validity: this is difficult to assess without conducting cross-sectional

design with more case studies, possibly at an early stage on the Bennett Scale.

Due to these question marks it would be dangerous to make any statistical inferences

from the research findings, although the ecological validity of the study should be

very strong indeed as there will be no intervention in the setting, which is a fairly

common university setting observed all over the world.

Significance  of  the  Research  

The first and foremost contribution that the research will attempt to make is the

introduction of an original concept to the theory of intercultural communication and

emotional intelligence, the notion of intercultural transfer or ICT, which it will aim to

  15  

measure and examine. In doing so it will employ several measures of psychometric

testing, which may reveal interesting and new correlations between factors such as

emotional intelligence, cognitive ability, expectations and motivation within

intercultural communication. By juxtaposing several different measures of various

concepts it will also add to the already existing literature on these existing measures

and concepts. This could prove to be a significant contribution to the theory and

praxis of multiculturalism, but also for educational studies to better understand

motivation and expectations, both important factors of educational attainment. Last

but not least, the research could also benefit the department where the case study

would be conducted: it could prove to have a relevant pedagogical impact on1st year

Master’s students who could thus be a part of research approaches and methodologies

that they are studying. In this way they might be inspired to learn more or experience

theory through a more hands-on approach. The findings could also benefit the

department by supplementing the official statistics already conducted with more in-

depth data on intercultural transfer, an important and yet vague part of learning in a

multicultural setting, and thus gain a better understanding of the internal dynamics of

the 1st year Master’s program cohort of 2015/2016. These possible gains are in line

with the recommendation of Noah & Eckstein to move more from the macro to the

micro investigation (Noah & Eckstein, 1998, p. 53) as that will make International

and Comparative Education more useful to policy-makers and administrators.

  16  

Ethical  considerations  

The research would assume a universalist stance on ethics, meaning that ethical

precepts, including harm to participants, lack of informed consent, invasion of privacy

and deception should never be broken. (Bryman, 2012, p. 133) At this point it is

relevant to note that the research proposal has not been discussed with any of the

actors whose consent and help would be necessary to conduct the research: the

proposal is therefore, at this stage, simply an exercise in theory and methodology.

Should the research proposal progress to the praxis stage, several steps would be

taken to ensure that the universalist stance on ethics is observed and maintained

throughout the research:

• Harm to participants: the nature of the study, combined with the measures

taken to ensure privacy and informed consent, ensures there would be no harm

to participants in any form or fashion. The case study would in no way

interfere with the environment observed, meaning that there would be no

“physical harm; harm to participants’ development; loss of self-esteem; stress;

and ‘inducing subjects to perform reprehensible acts’” (Bryman, 2012, p. 135)

• Informed consent: the research in itself is subject to the department’s and

students’ permission and consent to conduct psychometric testing. Students

would be fully informed of the aims and objectives of the study and would be

presented with its methodology at a brief presentation at the start of the school

year. They would then be given a short brochure to take home and study the

nature and the purpose of the study in peace. The brochure would involve a

  17  

statement of consent that they would have to sign and hand back within a

given timeframe if they would agree to participate in the study.

• Privacy: test scores, questionnaires and background information would be

carefully guarded and kept private. Should the qualitative part of the research

demand a certain exposure of such information to gain narrative quality or

greater insight, a statement of consent would again have to be signed by the

participants should they decide to do so.

• Deception: due to the nature of the study deception in this research is virtually

impossible. Full cooperation of the department and the respondents is needed,

meaning that all communication would have to be open and immediate.

Review  of  Relevant  Concepts  

Several of the relevant concepts and theories have already been touched upon in the

introductory pages, but I shall now expand on the most relevant of them by detailing

their application within the proposed research.

Multiculturalism  &  ICT  

The paper has defined ICT or intercultural transfer as the “transfer of cultural and

personal knowledge”. This distinction is somewhat similar to the distinction between

factual knowledge and soft skills, but the former division is more in line with the

newest empirical findings of differential psychology which tell us that soft skills (a

  18  

term that derives from pop psychology), such as emotional intelligence, should be

viewed as personality traits rather than as skills or tools, and that these personality

traits are very much amenable to cognitive and situational influences (Diener, 1996):

“Personality questionnaires, then, should not be viewed as proxy indices of vague underlying causal

influences, but as important variables in their own right. Emotions are but a single, albeit fundamental,

domain of personality.” (Petrides, p. 15)

This means that multicultural environments will influence our sense of self and our

attitudes. Studies report that individuals who were born and raised in multicultural

environments report lower levels of ethnocentrism than those who grew up in

homogenous environments (Harrison, 2012), and that low levels of ethnocentrism are

supposed to be associated with interculturally competent communicators.

(Arasaratnam & Doerfel, 2005) Although “there is a discernible pattern in these

findings” (Arasaratnam L. A., 2013, p. 7) they need to be “systematically tested

through empirical means before conclusions can be drawn.” (ib.) This research will

aim to conduct such empirical investigations with the help of psychometric testing.

Psychometric  testing:  TEIQue,  Big  5  Personality  Test,  Stanford-­‐Binet  IQ  

Test,  Raven’s  Matrices  Test  

TEIQue is the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire that is underpinned by the

Trait Emotional Intelligence Theory, developed by Dr. Petrides at the London

Psychometric Laboratory at UCL. The theory, to put it simply, views “faux

intelligences as personality traits, rather than as cognitive abilities,” (Petrides, p. 1)

  19  

and it uses the TEIQue to test for those personality traits. The questionnaire has

shown statistical reliability and stability, while its correlations are “substantial and

similar to those observed for the Big Five,” (ib.), another prominent test of personality

traits or “emotional intelligence”.

The Big 5 Personality Test is aimed at examining the Big Five Personality

Traits: openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness and neuroticism.

These factors are further subdivided into correlated and more specific factors. The test

has been empirically shown to have good internal validity and reliability and is

frequently used in psychometric testing and psychological research. In this research it

shall be used as a means of comparison with the TEIQue test and will thus

complement the “emotional intelligence” side of the equation.

The other side will be the cognitive aspect of our investigation, which will be

examined with two common tests, the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales and the

Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test. Both tests are standard cognitive ability measures

and will be conducted online. The latter has been chosen specifically because of its

nonverbal nature, which will enable an equal playing field among the international

respondents.

Research  Methodology  

As described and explained in detail above, this will be a case study of an

international university cohort, which will aim to examine the nature of Intercultural

Transfer (ICT) by testing for personality traits (“emotional intelligence”), cognitive

ability, truancy, expectations and motivation. An international university cohort

  20  

seems to be the perfect setting for such an investigation because “academia provides a

fruitful domain for investigating individuals interacting and working in a multi-

cultural context.” (Burke, Watkins, & Guzman, 2009, p. 2)

The research will begin with a thorough quantitative investigation and will end

with a qualitative examination of the statistical outliers that the quantitative part shall

provide. Researchers have lauded the potential of such outliers (Noah & Eckstein,

1998, pp. 55-56) and one of the objectives of this study is to try and heed their advice

to catch “the fattest Loose-Fish in our ocean of exploration.” (ib., p. 56)

Quantitative  Research  

The research will aim to conduct the following psychometric measures:

• Trait EI

• Factual knowledge

• Expectations and motivation

• Cognitive ability (Raven’s Matrices)

I have already expounded on why we would measure Trait Emotional Intelligence,

but now for a word or two about the other measures.

Expectations and motivation are to be measured because research shows that

“expectations are the best predictor of intercultural success.” (Tucker & Benson,

1979) Cognitive ability is to be measured for several reasons: the most important of

them being the reported finding that IQ does not drive intercultural adjustment.

  21  

(Morris, Savani, Mor, & Cho, 2014, p. 196) But this is contrary to the traditional

belief that learning anything is mainly driven by cognitive ability: therefore the

present study would like to explore the correlation between ICT and IQ, which will

then be augmented with Trait EI scores to test whether it is true that “other aptitudes

more relevant to procedural knowledge and social information processing predict who

learns in the laboratory or adjusts in the field.” (Morris, Savani, Mor, & Cho, 2014, p.

210)

Factual knowledge tests will examine the actual Intercultural Transfer: how

much have the students learned about the cultures, habits or personalities of other

students since the beginning of the program. Closed questions and Likert scale

statements will be used, whereas the entry and exit surveys will be based on semantic

differential scales.

Quantitative measures used for psychometric testing will be the following:

• TEIQue test (paper form)

• The Big 5 Personality Test (online)

• Stanford-Binet Intelligence Test (online)

• IQ Raven’s Matrices test (online)

• Entry and Exit Survey on Expectations, Motivation and Experiences (paper

form)

• Factual Information Questionnaire (paper form)

These measures will be supplemented with official documents and statistics

conducted and gathered by the department (academic achievement, course papers,

  22  

course evaluation statistics, truancy) to gauge two additional quantitative factors:

truancy and academic achievement.

Quantitative  Data  Analysis  

Quantitative data analysis will be performed using SPSS. The following correlations

will be examined:

• Pearson’s & Spearman’s Correlation (IQ & Trait EI; Trait EI & Big 5; IQ &

grades)

• Independent T-Test (gender & Trait EI; gender & grades)

• Dependent T-test (Trait EI test and Big 5 Personality Test re-administered 5

months later and compared with first test scores)

• One-Way ANOVA (age groups & Trait EI; grades groups & Trait EI)

Qualitative  Research  

As already stated, quantitative analysis will be followed with qualitative research that

will aim to examine either the statistical outliers produced by the first part of the

investigation or a random sample from the cohort. This will be done in order to

correlate the findings of both approaches: it may prove beneficial to supplement all

quantitative research of social concepts – which are latent variables, meaning that

there is no way to observe them directly – with qualitative investigations to ensure the

deficiencies of statistical explanatory models, such as structural equation models, do

  23  

not deceive us into making grand (and false) inferences. The paper’s stance to follow

Noah & Eckstein’s advice to conduct more micro investigations (Noah & Eckstein,

1998) can only be genuinely realized by probing where statistical analysis can’t reach:

“Since all people have the innate ability to narrate in their own ways, in-depth interviews provide an

opportunity for participants to tell their stories that help them to illustrate and shape their

understandings of a phenomenon.” (Dalib, Harun, & Yusoff, 2014, p. 132)

The qualitative part of the investigation will entail:

• A thematic analysis of course papers to recognize underlying themes, concepts

and constructs and to look for correlations with expectations, motivation and

Trait EI;

• Unstructured or semi-structured interviews with the outliers or a random

sample from the cohort.

The study will adopt Seidman’s interview structure (Seidman, 2006). The respondents

will therefore be guided to:

1. Reflect on their past experiences in light of ICT;

2. Provide details of their present experience of ICT;

3. Reflect on the meaning of that experience.

  24  

Qualitative  Analysis  

The approach to qualitative analysis will be one of grounded theory. To refer back to

the Aims and Research Questions section, where I state that the approach will be

“deductive, but with inductive feedback to continuously reexamine the methodology

and the theory and to inform the various sub-questions that the research will attempt

to answer,” one could say that the quantitative part of the research will be deductive

and the qualitative part of the study will provide the inductive feedback. Theoretical

or purposive sampling, a key step in grounded theory research, will be conducted

(interviews with statistical outliers), and followed up with coding until theoretical

saturation is reached. (Bryman, 2012, p. 568) Coding will be selective: a core

category, namely ICT, will be chosen as the central focus, around which other

categories will be planted. NVivo software will be used to aid with the analysis.

Conclusion  &  Research  Schedule  

The paper began by positing a distinction between the terms “multicultural” and

“intercultural”, where the former is a state of a heterogeneous cultural environment

and the latter is the process or a set of processes that occur, more or less, within such

an environment. This distinction was then grounded in intercultural communication

theory and a new, original term was proposed to examine the transfer of factual

knowledge and soft skills within a multicultural setting: intercultural transfer or ICT.

The paper then proposed to examine this notion of ICT against the backdrop of

emotional intelligence because empirical studies in differential psychology have

  25  

shown that EI should not be viewed as a soft skill (as is it understood by pop

psychology), but rather as a set of personality traits that can be both source and

outcome variables. The paper therefore posed the following questions: How does

Trait EI affect ICT and how does ICT affect EI? What other factors combine in

effective ICT? In examining these questions with psychometric testing the paper aims

to make an original contribution to educational studies and to further investigate the

empirically under-tested notion of multiculturalism. The research proposed would

employ a mixed-methods methodology, based on a detailed case study of an

international university cohort, with the quantitative part of the research providing a

deductive examination of hypotheses and the qualitative investigation of statistical

outliers feeding the researcher’s interpretivist and critical realist stance with in-depth

inductive feedback. The study would aim to be conducted under the following

timeframe:

• September 2015: Quantitative Data Collection: Entry Survey, TEIQue, Big 5

Personality Test, Stanford-Binet Test, Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test

• October 2015: Quantitative Data Analysis: SPSS analysis of available

correlations

• November 2015 – December 2015: Literature Review and Methodology

Refinement

• January 2016: 2nd batch of Quantitative Data Collection & Data Analysis:

Exit Survey

• February 2016: Qualitative Data Collection: Interviews

• March 2016: Qualitative Data Analysis: Coding of interviews and thematic

analysis of course papers

• April 2016: Writing the Findings

  26  

References    

Albiero, P., & Matricardi, G. (2013). Empathy Towards People of Different Race and

Ethnicity: Further empirical evidence for the Scale of Ethnocultural Empathy .

International Journal of Intercultural Relations (37), 648-655.

Arasaratnam, L. A. (2013). A Review of Articles on Multiculturalism in 35 Years of

IJIR . International Journal of Intercultural Relations (37), 676-685.

Arasaratnam, L. A., & Doerfel, M. L. (2005). Intercultural Communication

Competence: Identifying key components from multicultural perspectives.

International Journal of Intercultural Relations (29), 137-163.

Bryman, A. (2012). Social Research Methods. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Buchtel, E. E. (2014). Cultural Sensitivity or Cultural Stereotyping? Positive and

negative effects of a cultural psychology class. International Journal of Intercultural

Relations (39), 40-52.

Burke, J. M., Watkins, B. M., & Guzman, E. (2009). Performing in a Multi-Cultural

Context: The Role of Personality. International Journal of Intercultural Relations

(33), 475-485.

  27  

Chao, M. M., Kung, F. Y., & Yao, D. J. (2015). Understanding the Divergent Effects

of Multicultural Exposure. International Journal of Intercultural Relations (47), 78-

88.

Chiper, S. (2013). Teaching Intercultural Communication: ICT Resources and Best

Practices. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences (93), 1641–1645.

Dalib, S., Harun, M., & Yusoff, N. (2014). Reconceptualizing Intercultural

Competence: A Phenomenological Investigation of Students’ Intercultural

Experiences. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences (155), 130-135.

Dandy, J., & Pepua, R. (2010). Attitudes to Multiculturalism, Immigration and

Cultural Diversity: Comparsion of Dominant and Non-Dominant Groups in Three

Australian States. International Journal of Intercultural Relations (34), 34-46.

Diener, E. (1996). Traits Can Be Powerful, But Are Not Enough: Lessons from

subjective wellbeing. Journal of Research in Personality (30), 389-399.

Dolce, C. J. (1973). Multicultural Education - some issues. Journal of Teacher

Education (24), 282-284.

Harrison, N. (2012). Investigating the Impact of Personality and Early Life

Experiences on Intercultural Interactions in Internationalized Universities.

International Journal of Intercultural Relations (36), 224-237.

  28  

Howell, W. (1979). Theoretical Directions for Intercultural Communication. In M. K.

Asante, & E. Newmark, Handbook of Intercultural Communication (pp. 23-41).

Beverly Hills: Sage.

Kaplan. (2014). Graduate Recruitment Report: Employer Perspectives. Kaplan.

Kim, Y. Y. (2008). Intercultural personhood: Globalization and a way of being.

International Journal of Intercultural Relations (32), 359-368.

Liu, S. (2007). Living With Others: Mapping the Routes to Acculturation in a

Multicultural Society. International Journal of Intercultural Relations (31), 761-778.

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the Self: Implications for

Cognition, Emotion and Motivation. Psychological Review (98), 224-253.

McLuhan, M. (1962). The Guttenberg Galaxy. New York: New American Library.

Morris, M. W., Savani, K., Mor, S., & Cho, J. (2014). When in Rome: Intercultural

learning and implications for training. Research in Organizational Behavior (34),

189-215.

Noah, H. J., & Eckstein, M. A. (1998). Doing Comparative Education: Three

Decades of Collaboration. Hong Kong: The University of Hong Kong.

  29  

Petrides, K. V. (n.d.). London Psychometric Laboratory at UCL. Retrieved May 11,

2015, from Psychometric Properties of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire

(TEIQue):

http://www.psychometriclab.com/admins/files/TEIQue%20psychometric%20properti

es%20chapter.PDF

Qualter, P., Gardner, K. J., Pope, D. J., Hutchinson, J. M., & Whiteley, H. E. (2012).

Ability Emotional Intelligence, Trait Emotional Intelligence, and Academic Success

in British Secondary Schools: A 5 year longitudinal study. Learning and Individual

Differences , 83-91.

Rivera, L. A. (2012). Hiring as Cultural Matching: The Case of Elite Professional

Service Firms. American Sociological Review (77(6)), 999-1022.

Seidman, I. (2006). Interviewing as Qualitative Research: A Guide for Researchers in

Education and the Social Sciences. New York: Teachers College, Columbia

University.

Siegling, A. B., Nielsen, C., & Petrides, K. V. (2014). Trait Emotional Intelligence

and Leadership in a European Multinational Company. Personality and Individual

Differences (65), 65-68.

The Telegraph. (n.d.). University Education. Retrieved May 8, 2015, from Soft skills

valued over technical knowledge in graduates:

  30  

http://www.telegraph.co.uk/education/universityeducation/10999619/Soft-skills-

valued-over-technical-knowledge-in-graduates.html

Tucker, M., & Benson, P. (1979). The Prediction of Intercultural Adjustment: A

longitudinal validation. Interethnic and Intercultural Competence: Theoretical Issues,

Methodological Concerns and Applied Research. Society for Intercultural Education,

Training and Research.

University, S. (n.d.). Institute of International Education. Retrieved May 10, 2015,

from Department of Education: http://www.edu.su.se/english/institute-of-

international-education

Vijver, V. d., R., F. J., Breaugelmans, Schalk-Soekar, & G., R. S. (2008).

Multiculturalism: Construct Validity and Stability. International Journal of

Intercultural Relations (32), 93-104.