29
1 Wagering in Australia: A retrospective behavioural analysis of betting patterns based on player account data Sally Gainsbury (a), Saalem Sadeque (b), Dick Mizerski (b), & Alex Blaszczynski (c) (a) Centre for Gambling Education & Research, Southern Cross University PO Box 157, Lismore NSW 2480 Australia (b) Business School, University of Western Australia (M263) 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley WA 6009 Australia (c) School of Psychology, The University of Sydney Brennan MacCallum Building (A19), University of Sydney NSW 2006 Australia This manuscript has been published in the Journal of Gambling Business & Economics. Citation: Gainsbury, S., Sadeque, S., Mizerski, R., & Blaszczynski, A. (2012). Wagering in Australia: A retrospective behavioural analysis of betting patterns based on player account data. Journal of Gambling Business and Economics, 6(2), 50-68 The authors would like to acknowledge the European Association for the Study of Gambling who provided funding for this research in the form of a Young Investigator Grant for the first author. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Sally Gainsbury, Centre for Gambling Education & Research, Southern Cross University, PO Box 157, Lismore NSW 2480, Australia. Email: [email protected].

Wagering in Australia: A retrospective behavioural analysis of betting patterns based on player account data

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

1

Wagering in Australia: A retrospective behavioural analysis of

betting patterns based on player account data

Sally Gainsbury (a), Saalem Sadeque (b), Dick Mizerski (b), & Alex Blaszczynski (c)

(a) Centre for Gambling Education & Research, Southern Cross University

PO Box 157, Lismore NSW 2480 Australia

(b) Business School, University of Western Australia (M263)

35 Stirling Highway, Crawley WA 6009 Australia

(c) School of Psychology, The University of Sydney

Brennan MacCallum Building (A19), University of Sydney NSW 2006 Australia

This manuscript has been published in the Journal of Gambling Business & Economics.

Citation: Gainsbury, S., Sadeque, S., Mizerski, R., & Blaszczynski, A. (2012). Wagering in

Australia: A retrospective behavioural analysis of betting patterns based on player account

data. Journal of Gambling Business and Economics, 6(2), 50-68

The authors would like to acknowledge the European Association for the Study of Gambling who

provided funding for this research in the form of a Young Investigator Grant for the first author. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Sally Gainsbury, Centre for Gambling Education & Research, Southern Cross University, PO Box

157, Lismore NSW 2480, Australia. Email: [email protected].

2

Wagering in Australia: A retrospective behavioural analysis of

betting patterns based on player account data

Abstract

Gambling research often relies on self-report and cross-sectional data which is limited by inaccuracies

in recall. Analysis of behavioural data is necessary to advance conceptual understandings of gambling.

This paper analysed player account data of 11,394 customers of a large Australian wagering operator

over a ten-year period to investigate characteristics and betting patterns of account holders.

Comparisons were made between players based on the total number of bets placed. More frequent

bettors (those with greater total bet frequency counts), made smaller bets, but bet greater total amounts

and lost smaller proportions as compared to less frequent bettors. Less frequent bettors bet larger

single bets and lost a greater proportion of their total amounts bet. A minority of bettors accounted for

a disproportionately high number of bets but lost the lowest proportion of these. The results indicate

that players exhibit differential patterns of betting and subgroups of gamblers can be identified and

appropriately targeted with player education and responsible gambling strategies.

Keywords: gambling, betting, wagering, sports, behavioural analysis

3

Wagering in Australia: A retrospective behavioural analysis of

betting patterns based on player account data

Globally, sports and races fill an important role socially, economically and as a form of entertainment.

Betting or wagering is complementary to sporting events and a popular form of entertainment. For

example, on an annual basis 34% of adults in the UK bet on horse or dog race or sporting events

(Wardle et al., 2011), which does not include those who bet on non-sporting events and private

betting, which would increase participation rates, and similar rates are reported for Australia

(Productivity Commission, 2010). However, relatively little research has focused on this form of

gambling resulting in a limited understanding of consumer’s wagering behaviour. This paper aims to

identify patterns of wagering that differentiate subgroups of customers by analysing actual behavioural

data derived from a large sample of gamblers. Understanding common patterns of play will further the

understanding of how gambling products are typically used, which will aid the development of

appropriate consumer protection and customer interaction strategies..

Although race wagering has remained fairly stable over the past ten years in terms of participation and

expenditure, sports betting has increased driven by cable and digital television channels dedicated to a

wide range of events, increased Internet access enabling customers to bet online and follow statistics

and, liberalised regulation of wagering (Church-Sanders, 2011). Wagering, which includes betting on

both races and sports, is thought to be leading the global online gambling market, accounting for an

estimated 39% of online gambling, despite representing only 5% of total global gross gambling win

(ie, stakes less prizes) (Henwood, 2011). Increased marketing through sports sponsorship has

increased publicity and awareness of betting, and particularly Internet betting, in society (Lamont,

Hing, & Gainsbury, 2011) and anecdotal reports suggests that there has been an increase in young

males presenting for gambling treatment in relation to sports betting (University of Sydney, 2010).

Given the increased participation in online wagering (Gainsbury et al., 2012; Wardle et al., 2011),

4

consumers that engage in wagering represent an important target for research to further the

understanding of this gambling activity and the potential impact on individuals.

Methodological considerations in gambling research

A significant limitation of gambling research historically is its reliance on self-report measures of

gambling involvement (Blaszczynski, Ladouceur, Goulet, & Savard, 2006; Gainsbury, 2011;

Gainsbury & Blaszczynski, 2011; Hodgins & Makarchuk, 2003; McMillen & Wenzel, 2006; Walker,

2008; Williams & Volberg, 2009). Self-reported behaviours may be inaccurate as self-perception and

demand characteristics introduce response biases and reduce reliability and generalisability of findings

(Dickerson & Baron, 2000; Productivity Commission, 2010). Furthermore, self-reports of intended

behaviours by participants are often discordant with their actual behaviour (Baumeister, Vohs, &

Funder 2007). Most studies tend to view gamblers as a homogenous group and dismiss potential

implications of varied forms of gambling on etiological, personality or motivational factors

(Gainsbury, 2011; Holtgrave, 2009). This limits the extent to which research can contribute to an

understanding of gamblers based on different patterns of betting and consequentially devise

appropriate strategies to communicate with players and ensure play remains within appropriate limits.

The current study advances the field by comparing gamblers based on their betting frequency, which

will enable a greater understanding of different types of gamblers, driving further research.

Cross-sectional research designs are often used for cost and convenience purposes; however these do

not enable directions of causality to be determined. Longitudinal studies are costly, difficult to

implement, and often limited by attrition. Therefore, longitudinal studies that incorporate actual

behavioural and/or expenditure data based on player accounts would allow for more accurate

investigations of betting patterns and their variability over specified timeframes (Gainsbury, 2011;

Shaffer, Peller, LaPlante, Nelson, & LaBrie 2010). There is increasing evidence that suggests

electronically-recorded behavioural data (frequency, type, electronic interactions with operators)

obtained from gambling account holders can be used to effectively predict future behaviour and to

differentiate subgroups of gamblers (Braverman & Shaffer, 2010; Dragicevic, Tsogas, & Kudic, 2011;

5

Haefeli, Lischer, & Schwarx, 2011; Jolley, Mizerski & Olaru, 2006; Lam, 2006; LaBrie, LaPlante,

Nelson, Schumann, & Shaffer, 2007; LaBrie & Shaffer, 2010; Mizerski, Miller, Mizerski & Lam,

2004; Mizerski & Mizerski, 2001). These studies are based on recognition of the heterogeneity of

gamblers and their participation in gambling and the importance of understanding specific

involvement and risky behaviours to guide prevention and treatment initiatives. Analysis of

behavioural data is limited in that contextual variables, such as thoughts and emotions, are not

typically measured. However, it is argued that behavioural analysis has the advantage of overcoming

some limitations of self-report and cross-sectional studies and combined with multi-modal research

can be triangulated to evaluate conceptual models and theoretical postulates (Fantino, 2008;

Gainsbury, 2011).

Race wagering & Australian gambling

Australia represents a small but strong market for gambling and wagering. Australia had the highest

global adult per capita gambling expenditure; US$1,300 for every resident aged 17 and over as

compared to the US and Britain who spend less than US$400 per person on gambling (H2 Gambling

Capital as reported in The Economist). Betting on sports and races accounts for approximately 15% of

gambling losses (Australian Gaming Statistics as cited by Church-Sanders, 2011) and sports betting is

by far the fastest growing gambling segment, with a compound annual growth of 14.7% over the past

five years (IbisWorld, 2012).Bets may be placed in person with bookies at race tracks, at stand-alone

betting offices or agencies located in neighbourhood stores, hotels and clubs, or through telephone and

online accounts.

Wagering behaviour

Little research has been conducted examining behavioural characteristics and psychological profiles

associated with wagering. Woolley (2003) surveyed 2,945 account holders from two Australian

online wagering operators. The majority bet on races online at least once a week, although

participation in sports betting online appeared to be less frequent. There also appeared to be different

patterns of participation or subgroups of bettors, which is consistent with subsequent research (Lloyd

6

et al., 2010). Although this study provided useful insight into betting behaviour, the self-selected

sample may not be representative of all bettors and as the surveys were conducted almost ten years

ago, the results may not represent the current behaviour of online bettors. Furthermore, more recent

Australian research (Gainsbury et al., 2012) indicates that there may be differences between those who

gamble online and at land-based facilities, indicating that studies of online only betting may not

provide a comprehensive exploration of betting patterns.

Nonetheless, the majority of player behavioural data comes from online operators. In an examination

of more than 40,000 European online sports betting subscribers who opened accounts in 2005, LaBrie

et al. (2007) found players typically placed few small bets once a week and that only a small

percentage deviated from this pattern. Among those deviating was a subgroup displaying a pattern of

more frequent bets than the majority, but sustaining lower percentage losses. Subsequent studies with

the same cohort suggested there appeared to be a subgroup of gamblers characterised by potentially

less control over their play and more likely to repeatedly exceed pre-set individual limits on play

(Broda et al., 2008; LaPlante, Schumann, LaBrie, & Shaffer, 2008). Although these studies represent a

significant contribution to the field as they provide data on actual Internet gambling behaviour, they

are limited as they only provide information about a specific sample of bettors who opened accounts in

a defined period in 2005. The results do not provide information about players who have opened

accounts more recently and, as it is widely accepted that new customers to most businesses behave

significantly differently than already existing customers (Griffiths & Auer, 2011), this further limits

the extent to which results can be generalised to other populations.

In a Canadian population survey, Holtgrave (2009) found that gamblers who bet online and those who

bet on sports or horse races were distinctly different from gamblers who bet on other activities. These

types of gambling activities were associated with a greater likelihood that participants gambled

frequently, had greater gambling expenditure, and were more likely to have gambling-related

problems. Frequency of play has been found to be associated with and predictive of problem gambling

(Currie et al., 2006; Griffiths, Wardle, Orford, Sproston, & Erens, 2009; Hopley & Nicki, 2010; Lam

7

& Mizerski, 2009). An analysis of behavioural patterns related to potential gambling-related problems

indicated that players characterised by both high frequency of gambling and variability of bet sizes

during their first month of gambling were at higher risk of closing accounts due to gambling problems

(Braverman & Shaffer, 2010). However, although customers often closed accounts due to gambling-

related problems, no measures were used to verify that these individuals had gambling problems at a

clinical level. These results suggest that frequency of play is an important variable that may separate

subgroups of gamblers.

The current study

This study aimed to analyse comprehensive account-based data to investigate the characteristics,

betting patterns and behaviours of Australian wagerers. The objective was to investigate subgroups of

gamblers based on wagering frequency and to identify key betting characteristics and common

patterns of wagering. The purpose of the study was to undertake a comparative analysis of gambling

behaviours between more and less frequent bettors, determine the correlation between gambling

behaviours based on level of betting frequency, and identify factors distinguishing more and less

frequent bettors.

METHOD

Sample

An Australian wagering operator provided a database of de-identified account information for 22,849

individuals, with 11,394 customers making at least one bet. The percentage of opened accounts with

absent bets is similar to that reported in an online electronic gaming machine (EGM) study (Jolley et

al., 2006), and anecdotally with another online wagering provider (personal communication, requested

anonymity, 2011). The operator provides wagering services on thoroughbred, harness, and greyhounds

races, and sports events (including overseas events). Pari-mutuel betting (all bets accumulated in a

pool, operators subtract commissions and distribute balance to winning bettors in proportion to

wagered amounts) and fixed-odds betting products (where the odds are known at the time of the

8

placement of the wager) are offered with bets placed variably through; telephone, Internet, on-course

and networks of hotel, club and retail outlets. The website is one of the top ten Internet betting sites in

Australia (Alexa, cited by Church-Sanders, 2010).

The data period ranged from July 1, 2001 to November 24, 2010 and accounted for all bets placed (ie,

bets placed through all channels). The dataset included information on number of betting days, bets

placed, minimum and maximum bet value per single bet, total bet value for the specified timeframe,

and total dividend (ie, total amount returned to the player). All variables except for the first two were

expressed in Australian dollars (AUD$). The dataset also included the dates of the first bet and the last

bet made by each player.

Measures

Several measures were developed to enable data analysis; These include betting duration (difference

between first and last betting date), average dollar per bet (total bet amount divided by number of

bets), average number of bets per day (number of bets divided by number of betting days), frequency

of betting sessions (number of betting days divided by betting duration and expressed as a percentage),

net loss in dollars (difference between total amount bet and total dividends paid) and net loss

percentage (net loss divided by total amount bets).

Analysis

The dataset was divided into two groups using the median split based on the total number of bets

(frequency) made. Groups were labelled as more frequent (top 50% of the sample) and less frequent

bettors (lower 50% of the sample). Median value split has been widely used in academic literature (eg,

Bearden, Rose, & Teel, 1994; Gauri, Sudhir, & Talukdar, 2008; Greene & Greene, 2008; Hong &

Sternthal, 2010; Kilbourne & LaForge, 2010; Reinartz & Kumar, 2000; Schmittlein, Cooper, &

Morrison, 1993). The median split is particularly useful on strongly skewed distributions such as those

found in gambling (Jolley et al., 2006). The data exhibited the skewed Negative Binomial

Distributions (NBD) for variables under consideration. The frequency groups were created with the

9

aim of understanding the role of gambling variables for an actual gambling operator. In addition, a

discriminant analysis (DA) was conducted to identify the variables most useful in distinguishing

between the two frequency groups.

RESULTS

Demographics

Limited demographics information was provided for the account holders. Of the 10,655 customers that

provided personal details, 78% were male with a median age of 49 years (SD = 17.9 years). This data

was missing for 739 customers as it was not initially required to open an account in 2001; this has

become a requirement in subsequent years.

Player wagering behaviour

The average (mean) length of time that players actively placed wagers (duration) was 2,117.66 days

(SD = 1,304.34), which was highly variable over the ten year period under study. For the period in

which players had active accounts, they bet an average of 45.6 days and made an average of 717.6

bets. The mean percent betting days within the betting interval (frequency of betting sessions) was

6.93%, although the median was substantially lower at 1.56% with considerable variation in this

activity (SD = 16.64%).

As seen in Figure 1, the number of bets placed for the entire sample was highly skewed in the

direction of fewer bets. A majority of the players bet only a few times, and a small minority of bettors

accounted for the majority of bets. This is evidenced by the large standard deviation relative to means

and differences between mean and median values. This distribution reflects a pattern of behaviour that

fits the Negative Binomial Distribution (NBD) found in most frequently purchased products and

gambling (Ehrenberg, Uncles, & Goodhardt, 2004; Lam & Mizerski, 2009). Because of this type of

skewed distribution, player groups were created based on the median value for the total number of bets

(114). These groups were labelled as less frequent bettors (n=5,684) and more frequent bettors

(n=5,696). Table 1 provides the descriptive statistics for these two groups.

10

Insert Figure 1 & Table 1 about here

Analyses indicated that 91.78% of players lost money, on average players lost 34.07% of the total

amount wagered (SD = -29.94%, n = 11,069). Less and more frequent bettors had similar average

frequency of betting sessions during the period in which they actively placed bets with the operator

(see Table 1). However, less frequent bettors were active for a shorter duration but had a greater

average minimum bet value ($6.92 vs. $1.51), average dollars per bet ($18.82 vs. $14.05) and

percentage net loss (43.76% vs. 24.86%) than more frequent bettors.

To identify statistical differences between the two groups of involvement, Mann-Whitney U tests were

conducted on all the comparisons. The Mann-Whitney U test is preferable when assumptions of

normality cannot be met, as with the NBD (Allen & Bennett, 2010). In this test, the median values of

the variables are compared (Pallant, 2005). The analysis showed that the median values of all the

variables (see Table 1) between the more and less frequent bettors were significantly different

(P<.001). The more and less frequent bettor groups significantly differ in their patterns of gambling.

Those in the top 1% of the entire sample (n=113) based on total number of bets were classified as the

most frequent bettors. As shown in Table 1, the most frequent bettors had a lower average minimum

bet value, lower average dollars per bet, and lower percentage losses compared to the remainder of the

sample. The most frequent bettors accounted for 16.88% of the total bet value of the entire sample and

received 19.28% of total dividends paid to the entire sample. In comparison, the remainder of the

sample (n=11,267) accounted for 83.12% of the total bet value and received 80.72% of the

total dividends. The most frequent were combined with the more frequent bettors for further analysis.

Relationship between other gambling variables and number of bets

Correlation analyses were conducted to investigate specific associations between variables. As the

variables are skewed, Kendall’s tau-b (represented as ‘τ’) was used to examine the interrelationships

11

among the variables. Kendall’s tau-b is a suitable alternative when the assumptions of normality

cannot be met (Allen & Bennett, 2010). Kendall’s tends to provide better estimate of true correlation

in the population compared to Spearman’s Rho (Allen & Bennett, 2010; Malhotra, 2010), and

provides protection against Type I errors (Arndt, Turvey, & Andreasen., 1999). Moreover, Kendall’s

tau-b can be interpreted in the same way as Spearman’s Rho (Table 2).

Insert Table 2 about here

A number of significant relationships between variables were found, many of which make intuitive

sense given that groups were created based on betting frequency. As would be expected, the

correlation between number of betting days and number of bets was positive, and similar for both

groups. Similarly, the correlation between the total bet value and the total dividend was very high for

both groups. Of interest, the frequency of betting sessions was negatively correlated with duration

(how long the bettor has been with the provider) for both the groups. This effect was larger for less

frequent bettors (τ = -.48) than the more frequent bettors (τ = -.31).

Total bet value was positively correlated with percentage net loss. This effect was larger for the less (τ

= .27) as compared to the more frequent bettors (τ = .12) and again reflected the finding that less

frequent bettors tend to lose proportionally more. The number of betting days and number of bets was

also positively correlated with percentage net loss. This was larger for the less frequent bettors than

the more frequent bettors, indicating that the less frequent bettors lost a greater proportional amount of

bets. All correlations were significant at the 0.01 level, as indicated in Table 2.

Determining user group membership

The variables provided were analysed to try to determine group membership using multivariate

discriminant analysis (MDA). Only the original variables (number of betting days, duration, maximum

bet value, minimum bet value, and total bet value) were included as independent variables. Because

the new variables were a combination of the original variables, omitting them avoids the problem of

singularity which can severely affect the results of an MDA (Hair, Black, Babin, & Anderson, 2010;

Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). The number of bets variable was also omitted because the groups were

12

created based on this variable. The total dividend was also omitted as this does not accurately measure

gambling behaviour, but it is based on external factors including other bettors. Table 1 shows that the

original variables are skewed, which is a normal occurrence in datasets such as this where a large

proportion of the sample wagers infrequently (LaBrie et al., 2007; LaBrie & Shaffer, 2010).

Accordingly, these variables were normalized through natural log transformation

An MDA requires a holdout sample for validating the discriminant function. Because of the large

number of cases (n = 11,380), the dataset was randomly divided into two groups. This resulted in

5,690 cases for the analysis sample which was used to generate the discriminant function. The other

group (holdout sample) was used to assess the internal validity of the discriminant function for

predicting the group membership. Since there is no prior theory to determine which of the five

variables considered in this analysis will be most useful in distinguishing between the user groups, a

step-wise MDA was employed. In a step-wise estimation method, independent variables are entered

into the discriminant function one at a time based on their discriminating power. Mahalanobis D2 was

employed to determine overall significance (Hair et al., 2010; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007).

The MDA found a significant discriminant function (Wilks’ λ = 0.352; χ2 = 5936.73; df = 5, p<0.001)

in the model building sample. The canonical correlation was 0.81 which implied that the function

could explain 65.6% of the variance in the two user groups. The function could correctly classify

93.6% of analysis sample and this result was identical in the holdout sample. Table 3 shows the hit

ratios for the two user groups for both the samples. These hit ratios were compared against the

proportional chance (50%) and maximum chance criteria (50%). Hair et al. (2010) recommend that for

practical significance, the achieved classification accuracy must exceed the selected comparison

standard by at least 25 percent. The hit ratios for both the analysis and holdout samples exceeded this

accuracy (62.5%).

Insert Table 3 about here

13

The resultant MDA identified all five variables under consideration as being important in

distinguishing between the two player groups. Table 4 reports the mean values of log transformed

variables along with the discriminant loadings. Discriminant loadings are considered more useful for

interpretive purposes compared to discriminant coefficients (Hair et al., 2010; Tabachnick and Fidell

2007). Tabachnick and Fidell (2007) suggest that loadings over 0.33 can be considered eligible for

inclusion in the model. As can be seen from Table 4, only three (number of betting days, total bet

value and minimum bet value) out of the five variables qualify under this rule. An F-test of the

equality of group means supported these results (p<0.001). The results indicate that a greater number

of betting days and total bet value was predictive of being a more frequent bettor. A greater minimum

bet value was predictive of being a less frequent bettor.

Insert Table 4 about here

DISCUSSION

The large skew found in the gambling behaviour in the current betting sample is similar to the

observed and self-reported behaviour of gamblers in other studies (Jolley et al., 2006; LaBrie et al.,

2007; Lam & Mizerski, 2009; Mizerski et al., 2004; Shaffer et al. 2010) and shows that there are

distinct differences in how players engage with gambling activities. As betting frequency was seen to

differ greatly between players and has been used in previous research to group gamblers (Broda et al.,

2008; LaPlante et al., 2008), this variable was selected to create player groups for comparison. Less

frequent bettors typically bet higher minimum amounts and more dollars per bet than more frequent

bettors. However, the total amount bet and average number of bets was lower for less frequent than for

the more frequent bettors, indicating that over time, more frequent bettors tended to wager more

money. This may also indicate different betting strategies, whereby more frequent bettors spread their

investment over a larger number of bets to reduce risks, while less frequent bettors may be less

familiar with different types of bets or events to wager on.

14

As more frequent bettors lost a lower proportion of their wagers, this may indicate that this group are

more savvy bettors, picking odds and making bets in a more successful manner than less frequent

bettors, who may be less concerned with the outcome and spend less time conducting research to

inform their bets. These results are similar to those reported by Broda et al. (2008) and LaBrie et al.

(2007). This consistency between Australian and European bettors indicates that the difference

between bettors based on bet frequency may be widespread, although more research is needed to

confirm this.

Analysis of the most frequent bettors (top 1%) found that this group typically bet smaller amounts per

wager. Although they lost greater amounts overall, this was related to the greater total amounts bet and

closer analysis reveals that they lost a lower proportion of bets than the vast majority of players. This

indicates that although there is great variation amongst these players, many were very successful in

their betting. This again replicates the results found by La Brie et al. (2007) and indicates that there are

negative relationships between percent loss and aggregated total of money wagered and frequency of

betting. This group may represent semi-professional or professional wagerers who are knowledgeable

about betting and spend time researching the optimal bets to place and are willing to stake large sums

on the outcomes. As high frequency gamblers have also been targeted as a group of potential problem

gamblers (Broda et al., 2008; LaPlante et al., 2008), this subgroup of players clearly requires further

research to ensure that appropriate customer interaction and responsible gambling initiatives are

implemented with these players.

Across the whole sample, customers with shorter account duration (time over which the account was

active) tended to make bets less frequently than those with a longer active betting history. This was

particularly pronounced for the least frequent bettors, suggesting that customers may have been trying

out or testing the operator to see if they enjoyed the activity, but did not overly enjoy betting or betting

with the operator. Given the large number of operators that offer gambling and the relative ease of

opening an account it is not surprising that some customer may test out wagering with an operator and

15

fail to continue. These customers may be of particular interest to online operators as a target for

retention strategies.

The discriminant analysis results showed that the key variables that distinguish between the less and

more frequent betting groups are the number of betting days, total bet value, and minimum bet value.

Specifically, players that place a greater total number of bets appear to place bets on a greater number

of days and account for large dollar amounts. In contrast, bettors who placed a lower total number of

bets participate in wagering less often but on average made higher bets. Future research may

investigate how wagering providers cater to different customers and whether they tend to may make

greater profits from low frequency, but higher bet average customers, who may lose a greater

proportion of their wagers. it is also important to investigate the extent to which these players also bet

on other gambling activities as negative financial consequences of gambling may place them at risk of

gambling-related harms.

The current study found that although more frequent betting was associated with large losses, the

proportion of losses to amounts wagered was lower for more frequent bettors than for less frequent

bettors. This questions the use of the variable of betting frequency to classify bettors as potential

problem gamblers, although the current study did not include any contextual variables such as

discretionary disposable income, psychological wellbeing and impact of gambling. Therefore,

although frequency of betting involvement may be a useful variable to classify and analyse bettors,

more research is needed to investigate whether this is an appropriate variable for the identification of

problem gamblers.

Although the implications of frequency of betting are not clearly defined this is an important variable

to use for the classification of gamblers. Based on an analysis of survey data of Canadian gamblers,

Humphreys et al. (2011) argued that frequency of participation is a better measure of the intensity of

consumer participation in gambling than reported expenditure. Self-reported expenditure data is often

inaccurate due to respondent’s varied understanding of expenditure in addition to recall bias

16

(Gainsbury et al., 2012; Wood & Williams, 2007). Self-report of gambling frequency may be more

accurate as this is not as complex to calculate as expenditure and therefore, this variable may be more

accurate in predicting future gambling behaviour. Despite the limitations of self-report data, this

method has unique advantages as it is able to examine more personal and subjective variables, such as

psychological characteristics. Therefore, future research may benefit by taking a multimodal approach

and considering both behavioural and self-report data to further the understanding of consumer

gambling behaviour.

As with most gambling research, this study is not without its limitations, some of which have already

been mentioned. Similar to other behavioural analyses of Internet gamblers, this study is limited to the

betting behaviour of customers to one unique gambling operator and may not be representative of

customers of other gambling operators. However, a key strength is the inclusion of customers that bet

via multiple channels, as opposed to only online gamblers. Few contextual variables were available

and no cognitive or emotive variables were measured, making it impossible to identify problem

gamblers; to do so require validation using clinical interviews or problem gambling measures. Future

research should attempt to overcome these limitations by combining player account behavioural

analysis with self-report data and scales. Comparisons should also be made between player account

data provided by various wagering operators to investigate if there are different betting behavioural

patterns found between gambling providers. In the current study, bet frequency was used for analytical

comparison given its identification being associated with problem gambling in previous studies

(Currie et al., 2006; Griffiths, Wardle, Orford, Sproston, & Erens, 2009; Hopley & Nicki, 2010;

LaBrie & Shaffer, 2011; Lam & Mizerski, 2009). The number of bets placed does not necessarily

reflect a high degree of riskiness or involvement given the importance of factors such as the odds of

winning. Future research should explore betting behaviour in an exploratory manner to investigate

what other variables may separate groups of bettors. Furthermore, ongoing research should consider

other variables relevant to betting involvement in examining subgroups of bettors, including

expenditure, bet size and the number of betting days.

17

Despite these limitations, this study has many important strengths; this is the first behavioural analysis

of actual wagering behaviour in Australia and provides insight into how customers place bets over a

ten year period. Previous studies have used a more limited time period and ongoing research will

further examine this longitudinal dataset. The finding of distinctive characteristics in betting patterns

based on gambling frequency has important implications for public policy, industry, research and

treatment providers. Recently, a federal parliamentary committee in Australia recommended the

introduction of ‘low intensity’ poker machines with a maximum of $1 bet per game (Productivity

Commission, 2010). Although our results are in the context of wagering, less frequent bettors

accounted for higher average bet value per bet than the more frequent bettors. Many of these less

frequent bettors could potentially be light and occasional (ie, Melbourne Cup Horse race, Football

Finals) recreational gamblers. This may suggest that the proposed change in maximum bet value may

affect the less frequent bettors rather than more frequent bettors, which may not be an optimal

responsible gambling strategy given previous research indicating that more frequent gamblers are at

greater risk of harm (Currie et al., 2006; Griffiths, Wardle, Orford, Sproston, & Erens, 2009; Hopley

& Nicki, 2010; Lam & Mizerski, 2009).

This study indicates that there are significant differences between groups of bettors based on their

betting patterns. Bettors that placed a greater number of bets lost more money overall, but were more

successful than less frequent bettors in terms of the proportion of wagers lost. This may indicate that

more frequent bettors are more skilled, or put more time and effort into placing smaller, but more

frequent bets to minimise losses as compared to less frequent bettors who place larger single bets and

tend to be less successful. These results have implications for betting operators in terms of their

approach to customers based on betting behaviour. Policy makers may consider initiatives to target

less frequent, recreational players, to ensure that these individuals spend within their means and ensure

the activity does not cause problems, as well as more frequent bettors who still lose greater overall

amounts. Future research should analyse behavioural data to further explore the development of

patterns of play and potentially identify players that may be at risk of experiencing gambling-related

harms.

18

19

REFERENCES

P Allen and K Bennett PASW statistics by SPSS: A practical guide, version 18.0 (South

Melbourne, Victoria, Cengage Learning Australia Pty Limited, 1st edition, 2010).

S Arndt, C Turvey and NC Andreasen. ‘Correlating and predicting psychiatric symptom

ratings: Spearman’s r versus Kendall’s tau correlation’ (1999) Journal of Psychiatric

Research, 33, 97.

RF Baumeister, KD Vohs and DC Funder ‘Psychology as the science of self-reports and

finger movements: Whatever happened to actual behavior?’ (2007) Perspectives on

Psychological Science, 2 , 396.

W Bearden, RL Rose and JE Teel ‘Correlates of conformity in the consumption of illicit

drugs and alcohol’ (1994) Journal of Business Research, 30, 25.

A Blaszczynski, R Ladouceur, A Goulet and C Savard ‘How much do you spend gambling?

Ambiguities in questionnaire items assessing expenditure’ (2006) International

Gambling Studies, 6, 123.

J Braverman and HJ Shaffer ‘How do gamblers start gambling: Identifying behavioural

markers for high-risk internet gambling.’ (2010). European Journal of Public Health.

Advance online publication. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckp232

A Broda, D LaPlante, SE Nelson, RA LaBrie, LB Bosworth and HJ Shaffer ‘Virtual harm

reduction efforts for Internet gambling: Effects of deposit limits on actual Internet

sports gambling behavior’ (2008) Harm Reduction Journal, 5, 27.

R Church-Sanders The global business of online casinos: Outlook, forecasts and analysis

(London, iGaming Business , 2010).

R Church-Sanders Online sports betting: A market assessment and outlook (London:,

iGaming Business, 2011).

20

SR Currie, DC Hodgins, J Wang, N el-Guebaly, H Wynne and S Chen ‘Risk of harm among

gamblers in the general population as a function of level or participation in gambling

activities’ (2006) Addiction, 101, 570.

M Dickerson and E Baron ‘Contemporary issues and future directions for research into

pathological gambling’ (2000) Addiction, 95, 1145.

S Dragicevic, G Tsogas and A Kudic ‘Analysis of casino online gambling data in relation to

behavioural risk markers for high-risk gambling and player protection’ (2011)

International Gambling Studies, 11, 377.

SC Ehrenberg, M Uncles and G Goodhardt ‘Understanding brand performance measures:

Using Dirichlet benchmarks’ (2004) Journal of Business Research, 57, 1307.

E Fantino ‘Behavior analysis: Thriving, but how about its future?’ (2008) Journal of

Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 125.

S Gainsbury ‘Player Account-based Gambling: Potentials for Behaviour-based Research

Methodologies’ (2011) International Gambling Studies, 11, 153.

S Gainsbury and A Blaszczynski ‘A systematic review of Internet-based therapy for the

treatment of addictions’ (2011) Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 490.

S Gainsbury, R Wood, A Russell, N Hing and A Blaszczynski. ‘A digital revolution:

Comparison of demographic profiles, attitudes and gambling behaviour of Internet and

non-Internet gamblers’ (2012) Computers in Human Behaviour, 28(4), 1388.

DK Gauri, K Sudhir and D Talukdar ‘The temporal and spatial dimensions of price search:

Insights from matching household survey and purchase data’ (2008) Journal of

Marketing Research, 45, 226.

H Greene and S Greene ‘Enhancing segmentation systems’ (2008) Journal of Targeting,

Measurement, and Analysis for Marketing, 16, 298.

21

M Griffiths and M Auer ‘Approaches to understanding online versus offline gambling

impacts’ (2011) Casino & Gaming International, 3, 45.

M Griffiths, H Wardle, J Orford, K Sproston and B Erens ‘Internet gambling, health,

smoking and alcohol use: Findings from the 2007 British Gambling Prevalence

Survey’ (2009) International Journal of Mental Health and Addictions, 9, 1.

J Haefeli, S Lischer and J Schwarx ‘Early detection items and responsible gambling features

for online gambling’ (2011) International Gambling Studies, 11, 273.

JF Hair, WC Black, BJ Babin and RE Anderson Multivariate data analysis: A global

perspective . (New Jersey, Pearson Prentice Hall, ).. 7th

edition, 2010).

D Henwood ‘Sports betting: The big picture’ (2011) Presentation at EGR Live, Business

Design Centre, London. May 3-4, 2011. Retrieved from:

http://www.h2gc.com/news.php?article=H2+Gambling+Capital+Presentations+-

+EGR+Live+May+2011

DC Hodgins and K Makarchuk ‘Trusting problem gamblers: Reliability and validity of self-

reported gambling behavior’ (2003) Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 17, 244.

T Holtgraves ‘Gambling, gambling activities, and problem gambling’ (2009) Psychology of

Addictive Behavior, 23, 295.

J Hong and B Sternthal ‘The effects of consumer prior knowledge and processing strategies

on judgments’ (2010) Journal of Marketing Research, 47, 301.

AAB Hopley and RM Nicki ‘Predictive factors of excessive online poker playing’ (2010)

Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 13, 379.

BR Humphreys, YS Lee and BP Soebbing ‘Modelling consumers’ participation in gambling

markets and frequency of gambling’ (2011) Journal of Gambling Business and

Economics, 5, 1.

22

IBISWorld ‘Footy season kicks off the annual serge in sports betting’ (2012, March 21)

IBISWorld, Melbourne.

B Jolley, R Mizerski and D Olaru ‘How habit and satisfaction affects player retention for

online gambling’ (2006) Journal of Business Research, 59, 770.

WE Kilbourne and MC LaForge ‘Materialism and its relationship to individual values’ (2010)

Psychology and Marketing, 27, 780.

RA LaBrie, DA LaPlante, SE Nelson, A Schumann and HJ Shaffer ‘Assessing the playing

field: A prospective longitudinal study of Internet sports gambling behavior’ (2007)

Journal of Gambling Studies, 23, 347.

R LaBrie and HJ Shaffer ‘Identifying behavioural markers of disordered Internet sports

gambling’ (2010) Addiction Research & Theory, 19, 56.

D Lam ‘Applicability of the duplication of purchase law to gaming’ (2006) UNLV Gaming

Research & Review Journal, 10, 55.

D Lam, D and R Mizerski ‘An investigation into gambling purchases using the NBD and

NBD-Dirichlet models’ (2009) Marketing Letters, 20, 263.

M Lamont, N Hing and S Gainsbury ‘Gambling on sport sponsorship: A conceptual

framework for research and regulatory review’ (2011) Sport Management Review, 14,

246.

DA LaPlante, A Schumann, RA LaBrieand HJ Shaffer ‘Population trends in Internet sports

gambling’ (2008) Computers in Human Behavior, 24, 2399.

J Lloyd, H Doll, K Hawton, WH Dutton, JR Geddes, GM Goodwin and RD Rogers ‘Internet

gamblers: a latent class analysis of their behaviours and health experiences’ (2010)

Journal of Gambling Studies 26, 387.

NK Malhotra Marketing research: An applied orientation (New Jersey, Prentice-Hall, 6th

edition, 2010).

23

J McMillen and M Wenzel ‘Measuring problem gambling: Assessment of three prevalence

screens’ (2006) International Gambling Studies, 6, 147.

D Mizerski, R Miller, K Mizerski and D Lam The stochastic nature of purchasing a state’s

lottery products’ (2004) Australasian Marketing Journal, 12, 56.

D Mizerski K Mizerski ‘The effect and implications for a stochastic pattern of lotto game

play’ (2001) International Gambling Studies, 1, 132.

J Pallant SPSS survival manual (Berkshire, United Kingdom, Open University Press, 2nd

edition, 2005).

Productivity Commission Gambling (Canberra, Report no. 50, 2010).

WJ Reinartz and V Kumar ‘On profitability of long-life customers in a noncontractual setting:

An empirical investigation and implications for marketing’ (2000) Journal of

Marketing, 64, 17.

Roy Morgan Research Roy Morgan Gambling Monitor (Sydney, Roy Morgan Research,

2011).

D Schmittlein, LG Cooper and D Morrison ‘Truth in the concentration in the land of (80/20)

laws’ (1993) Marketing Science, 12, 167.

HJ Shaffer, AJ Peller, DA LaPlante, SE Nelson and RA LaBrie ‘Toward a paradigm shift in

internet gambling research: From opinion and self-report to actual behaviour’ (2010)

Addiction Research and Theory, 18, 270.

BG Tabachnick and LS Fidell Using multivariate statistics (New Jersey, Pearson Education,

5th

edition, 2007).

The Economist online ‘The biggest losers’ (2011, May 16) The Economist Online. Retrieved

from http://www.economist.com/blogs/dailychart/2011/05/gambling

University of Sydney ‘Internet adds to gambling woes’ (2011, Mar 14) Science Alert.

Retrieved from: http://www.sciencealert.com.au/news/20111503-21947.html

24

DM Walker Challenges that confront researchers on estimating the social costs of gambling

(Washington, DC, American Gaming Association, 2008).

H Wardle, A Moody, S Spence, J Orford, R Volberg, D Jotangia, M Griffiths, D Hussey and

F Dobbie ‘British gambling prevalence survey 2010’ (London, National Centre for

Social Research, 2011).

R Williams and RA Volberg ‘Impact of survey description, administration format, and

exclusionary criteria on population prevalence rates of problem gambling’ (2009)

International Gambling Studies, 9, 101.

R Woolley ‘Mapping internet gambling: Emerging modes of online participation in wagering

and sports betting’ (2003) International Gambling Studies, 3, 3.

R Wood and R Williams ‘How much money do you spend on gambling? The comparative

validity of questions wordings used to assess gambling expenditure’ (2007)

International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 10, 63.

25

Table 1: Descriptive statistics of less frequent bettors vs. more frequent bettors vs. most

frequent bettors group (frequency = number of bets)

Less Frequent Bettors

(n=5,684) Bottom 50% of the full

sample

More Frequent Bettors

(n=5,696) Top 50% of the full

sample

Most Frequent Bettors (n=

113) Top 1% of the full sample

Variable Mean (SD) Median Mean(SD) Median Mean(SD) Median No. of betting

days* 9.29 (10.18) 6 81.94 (72.71) 57 252.32

(78.76) 263

No. of bets

(frequency)* 36.78

(32.08) 27 1,398.73

(2,968.31) 487.5 17,273.48

(8,765.96) 14,079

Minimum bet

value ($) 6.918 (24.61) 3 1.51 (2.26) 1 .57 (.21) .50

Maximum bet

value ($) 65.12

(459.19) 20 133.60

(551.43) 45 374.22

(1,749.63) 43

Total bet value

($)* 619.86

(2,145.83) 206 15,322.84

(84,831.17) 2,748.07 135,632.35

(370,000) 48,123

Total Dividend

($)* 370.47

(1,118.05) 99.50 11,973.89

(71,709.48) 2,748.08 119,340.44

(354,000) 37,846.61

Duration (days)** 1,915.75

(1,327.19) 2,234.5 2,319.63

(1,249.04) 3,073.5 2,519.03

(1,191.91) 3,336

Average number

of bets (based on

total betting days)

5.46 (5.99) 3.64 14.89 (17.06) 10.27 77.64

(53.48) 62.38

Average dollar per

bet ($) 18.82

(63.98) 8.46 14.05

(41.30) 6.69 7.54 (20.80) 3.15

Frequency of

betting sessions

(% days during

active

membership)

6.91 (20.90) .44 6.96 (10.81) 3.15 17.60

(19.16) 9.86

Net Loss ($)* 190.62

(825.57) 76.33 3,182.72

(26,685.11) 833.22 15,571.46

(69,496.64) 7,419.90

Net Loss (%)* 43.76 (75.63) 48.24 24.86 (21.66) 23.48 19.56 (9.14) 20.71

*Totals are for the entire period between 01.07.01 and 24.11.10; ** Number of days between

individuals’ first and last bets (within the period from 01.07.01 and 24.11.10)

26

Table 2: Correlations among gambling behaviour variables for less frequent bettors (above

diagonal) and more frequent bettors (below diagonal)

Variable

No. of

betting days

No.

of bets

Minimu

m bet value ($)

Maximu

m bet value ($)

Total bet

value

($)

Total

Dividend ($)

Duratio

n (days)

Average

number

of bets

Average dollar

per bet

($)

Frequenc

y of visit (%)

Net

Loss ($)

Net

Loss (%)

No. of betting

days -

.6

0 -.13 .24 .54 .48 .15 -.09 .08 .39 -.32 .25

No. of

bets .56 - -.29 .20 .59 .55 .09 .34 -.03 .28 -.36 .28

Minimu

m bet

value ($) -.07

-

.2

6 - .22

.02

* .01

ns .02* -.32 .49 -.08

.00n

s -

.02ns

Maximum bet

value ($) .08

.0

7 .12 - .56 .42 .03 .00

ns .65 .13 -.41 .11

Total bet

value ($) .43 .5

2 -.01

ns .47 - .71 .08 .17 .38 .25 -.54 .27

Total

Dividend ($)

.43 .5

2 -.01

ns .43 .88 - .08 .17 .29 .23 -.23 .56

Duration (days) .24

.1

5 .04 .01

ns .12 .13 - -.06 .02* -.48 -.03 .06

Average

number

of bets

-

.01ns

.4

3 -.33 -.00

ns .25 .26 -.05 - -.17 .00

ns -.12 .10

Average dollar per

bet ($) -.04

-

.1

2 .31 .62 .36 .33 -.01

ns -.16 - .04 -.28 .06

Frequenc

y of betting

sessions

(%)

.46 .3

3 -.1 .03 .24 .24 -.31 .07 -.06 - -.17 .11

Net Loss

($) -.35 -

.4

3 .07 -.38 -.65 -.53 -.09 -.23 -.26 -.20 - .19

Net Loss (%) .09

.0

9 .07 .03 .12 .24 .04 .06 .04 .05 .23 -

Note: Nonparametric Kendall’s tau-b; all correlation is significant at 0.01 level unless marked

with * and ns

.

*Correlation is significant at 0.05 level

ns

Correlation is non-significant

27

Table 3: Classification results for both analysis and holdout samples

Predicted group membership (%) Sample size (n)

Actual group (%) Less

frequent bettors

More frequent bettors

Analysis sample

Less frequent bettors

91.7 8.3 2,859

More frequent bettors

4.6 95.4 2,831

Holdout sample

Less frequent bettors

92.1 7.9 2,825

More frequent bettors

4.9 95.1 2,865

Note: 93.6 percentage of analysis and holdout sample correctly classified.

Table 4: Summary of discriminant analysis

Analysis sample (n=5,690) Holdout sample (n=5,690)

Variables Log Mean Discriminant loading

Log Mean Discriminant loading

Less

frequent bettors (n=2,859)

More

frequent bettors (n=2,831)

Less

frequent bettors (n=2,825)

More

frequent bettors (n=2,865)

Number of betting days

1.69 4.05 0.87 1.69 4.02 0.88

Total bet value ($)

.10 .13 0.76 .10 .13 0.79

Minimum bet

value ($) .06 .05 -0.41 .06 .05 -0.38

Maximum bet

value ($) .08 .08 0.20 .08 .08 0.22

Duration (days) 6.74 7.43 0.16 6.76 7.39 0.15

Note: Group centroids (analysis sample): Less frequent bettors = -1.35; More frequent bettors = 1.36

Group centroids (holdout sample): Less frequent bettors = -1.35; More frequent bettors = 1.33

28

Figure 1 Distribution of frequency of bets

29