1 Classroom Environment Influence on Student Self-efficacy in Mathematics

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Self-efficacy and Classroom environment

Citation preview

  • CLASSROOM ENVIRONMENT INFLUENCE ON STUDENT SELF-EFFICACY IN

    MATHEMATICS

    A Dissertation

    by

    HILLARY P. CROISSANT

    Submitted to the Office of Graduate Studies

    of Texas A&M University-Commerce

    in partial fulfillment of the requirements

    for the degree of

    DOCTOR OF EDUCATION

    May 2014

  • CLASSROOM ENVIRONMENT INFLUENCE ON STUDENT SELF-EFFICACY IN

    MATHEMATICS

    A Dissertation

    by

    HILLARY P. CROISSANT

    Approved by:

    Advisor: Gilbert Naizer

    Committee: Tami Morton

    Katy Denson

    Head of Department: Martha Foote

    Dean of the College: Gail Johnson

    Dean of Graduate Studies: Arlene Horne

  • iii

    Copyright 2014

    Hillary P. Croissant

  • iv

    ABSTRACT

    CLASSROOM ENVIRONMENT INFLUENCE ON STUDENT SELF-EFFICACY IN

    MATHEMATICS

    Hillary P. Croissant, EdD

    Texas A&M University-Commerce, 2014

    Advisor: Gilbert Naizer, PhD

    This study aimed to find the characteristics of public school math classrooms and how

    they influence self-efficacy of students. Data were collected on math students in grades 4

    through 12 in a North Texas school district. Two surveys were administered to students in the

    district. Within 10 days, the students completed a classroom environment survey, followed by a

    self-efficacy survey. Both surveys were electronic and administered during the school day.

    Student data were analyzed by conducting a simple linear regression in order to determine if a

    relationship existed between classroom environment and student self-efficacy. A multiple

    regression was used in order to determine which dimensions under classroom environment could

    predict a high or low self-efficacy. Data analysis was unable to generalize low self-efficacy in

    mathematics and classroom environment correlation due to a small effect size. High self-

    efficacy in mathematics was found to increase as cohesion and satisfaction would increase and

    high self-efficacy in mathematics would increase as friction and difficulty would decrease.

  • v

    ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

    Teaching is a work of the heart is a sign I have up in my classroom that reminds me

    that not only as a teacher can I make a difference, but I am impacted by all kinds of teachers

    throughout my life. I want to thank the many mentors and supporters from whom I have had the

    opportunity to be influenced by and taught. You have encouraged me, supported me, and given

    me the strength to complete this journey.

    I would like to thank my closest friends for supporting me throughout this journey. First,

    to my friend Wendy Ulrich for encouraging me to continue on this path while being my work

    spouse by ensuring me that I could be a teacher and student at the same time. Also, to Laura

    Ahrens for reminding me how fortunate I am to be on this adventure and keeping me passionate

    about the knowledge that I was gaining.

    My inspiration comes from my teachers from the past. This great idea started with my

    professors from Austin CollegeJane White, Julia Shahid, and Barbara Sylvesterand my goal

    to be like them someday. I appreciate my mentors and support from administration and fellow

    teachers in my school district for their extended support. A special thanks goes out to my

    advisor Dr. Gilbert Naizer who has read and reread through my work, emailed and conferenced,

    and helped me make sure that I am the best that I could be. Also thank you to Katy Denson and

    Tami Morton for being a part of my dissertation committee and supporting my statistical and

    literary efforts. Jane Braddock and Kelli Knight for bringing snacks to class and being the

    perfect support system for this doctoral stage of life.

    Lastly I would like to thank my family for their continued support. My mom and dad for

    always being my number one fan as well as my parents-in-law who support any crazy idea I

    come up with and ensure I have everything I need to be successful. Most of all I want to thank

  • vi

    my husband Eric for putting up with the late nights, study sessions, and tears that come with the

    crazy life of being a doctoral student. You are my greatest supporter and sounding board, and I

    could not have done it without your continued love and motivation. Weston better be ready for a

    wild ride as a part of this family.

    This dissertation is dedicated to students who strongly dislike math in the hope that one

    day they will be positively impacted by a classroom or a teacher who instills the love of math in

    them so that it becomes a subject to be passionate about rather than despised.

  • vii

    TABLE OF CONTENTS

    LIST OF TABLES ...........................................................................................................................x

    LIST OF FIGURES ....................................................................................................................... xi

    CHAPTER

    1. INTRODUCTION .........................................................................................................1

    Statement of the Problem .........................................................................................1

    Purpose of the Study ................................................................................................3

    Research Questions ..................................................................................................3

    Research Hypotheses ...............................................................................................4

    Theoretical Framework ............................................................................................4

    Significance of the Problem ...................................................................................10

    Method of Procedure..............................................................................................11

    Definitions of Terms ..............................................................................................13

    Limitations .............................................................................................................14

    Delimitations ..........................................................................................................15

    Assumptions ...........................................................................................................15

    Organization of the Study ......................................................................................16

    2. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE .............................................................................17

    Math Anxiety .........................................................................................................17

    Classroom Environment.........................................................................................20

    Self-Efficacy and Classroom Environment ..........................................................25

    Self-Efficacy and Math ..........................................................................................30

    Self-Efficacy and Achievement .............................................................................32

  • viii

    Student Attitudes and Achievement.......................................................................38

    Anxiety and Achievement......................................................................................39

    Teacher Attitudes ...................................................................................................42

    Conclusions ............................................................................................................46

    3. METHOD OF PROCEDURE......................................................................................48

    Research Design.....................................................................................................49

    Population and Sample ..........................................................................................50

    Instrumentation ......................................................................................................51

    Procedures ..............................................................................................................55

    Data Gathering .......................................................................................................57

    Treatment of Data ..................................................................................................58

    Summary ................................................................................................................58

    4. ANALYSIS OF DATA................................................................................................60

    Results ....................................................................................................................60

    Summary ................................................................................................................65

    5. SUMMARY OF THE STUDY AND THE FINDINGS, CONCLUSIONS,

    IMPLICATIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH ......66

    Summary of the Study ...........................................................................................66

    Summary of the Findings .......................................................................................66

    Conclusions ............................................................................................................67

    Implications............................................................................................................71

    Recommendations for Further Research ................................................................74

    Summary ................................................................................................................75

  • ix

    REFERENCES ..............................................................................................................................76

    APPENDICES .............................................................................................................................107

    Appendix

    A. My Classroom Inventory .....................................................................................108

    B. Patterns of Adaptive Learning Survey .................................................................111

    C. Parent Permission Form .......................................................................................115

    D. Child/Minor Agreement to Be in a Research Study ............................................119

    E. District Agreement ...............................................................................................122

    F. Parent and Student Recruitment Letters ..............................................................125

    G. Demographic Survey ...........................................................................................127

    H. Video Script .........................................................................................................129

    I. Spanish Translation of Parent Letter....................................................................132

    J. Spanish Translation of Parent Permission Form ..................................................134

    K. Signed Site Letter .................................................................................................138

    L. Tables 1-4.............................................................................................................141

    VITA ............................................................................................................................................144

  • x

    LIST OF TABLES

    TABLE

    1. Means, Standard Deviations and Intercorrelations for High Self-Efficacy, Cohesiveness,

    Friction, Satisfaction, Difficulty, and Competitiveness .....................................................62

    2. Multiple Regression Analysis Summary for Variables Predicting High Math Self-

    Efficacy ..............................................................................................................................63

    3. Means, Standard Deviations and Intercorrelations for Low Self-Efficacy, Cohesiveness,

    Friction, Satisfaction, Difficulty, and Competitiveness .....................................................64

    4. Multiple Regression Analysis Summary for Variables Predicting Low Math Self-Efficacy

    ............................................................................................................................................65

  • xi

    LIST OF FIGURES

    FIGURE

    1. Theoretical Framework ........................................................................................................5

  • 1

    Chapter 1

    INTRODUCTION

    Starting at a young age, people are very impressionable through interactions in their

    environment including at home, at school, and with peers. These impressions can be reinforced

    or changed throughout the students life. An impression that has been an epidemic in our society

    is the negative attitude toward mathematics. Having a negative attitude in mathematics can lead

    to lower achievement in mathematics and lack of interest in continuing to develop a knowledge

    base of this topic. This study aimed to examine existing research and add to the body of

    knowledge in order to create an environment for students that leads to an increase in

    mathematics self-efficacy and ultimately improves attitudes and achievement in mathematics.

    Statement of the Problem

    A 2005 Associated Press poll found that nearly 40% of adults strongly disliked

    mathematics in school, twice the percentage of adults who disliked other subjects (Philipp,

    2007). The way individuals see mathematics can negatively or positively impact their attitude

    toward the subject. While students learn mathematics, they acquire skills, understand maths

    value, how it is learned, who should learn it, and what is needed for engagement in mathematics

    understanding. Heller stated, Be careful how you interpret the world; it is like that (McFague,

    2001, p. 39). This implies that the way that an individual makes sense of the world, not only

    defines the person for the world, but also the world for that person.

    The importance and need for math are emphasized in many areas of the world around us

    and in our life and workplace. Math is a significant part of the scientific and technical

    community our society has become, as well as our cultural heritage (National Council of

    Teachers of Mathematics [NCTM], 2000). The increase in the complexity of our everyday life

  • 2

    has raised the importance and significance of mathematics and the role it has in our society.

    Unfortunately, the level of difficulty and abstractness of math are a large reason why people have

    developed a negative view, attitude, or affect toward mathematics (Adeyemi, 2012). This

    negative view of educators can trickle down to students and lead to unsatisfactory achievement

    and participation in mathematics (Malmivuori, 2008). Lack of intrinsic motivation can lead to

    resistance toward mathematics and the learners self-perception will decline and difficulties in

    mathematics will increase (Royer & Walles, 2007). Students who have difficulties in math often

    have lower confidence in math and lower achievement in mathematics.

    Self-efficacy is defined by Bandura (1986) as peoples judgments of their capabilities to

    organize and execute courses of action required to attain designated types of performances (p.

    391) and can be easily confused with attitudes. Attitudes toward math have been defined as a

    liking or disliking of mathematics, a tendency to engage in or avoid mathematics activities, a

    belief that one is good or bad at mathematics, and a belief that mathematics is useful or useless

    (Neale, 1969, p. 623). While both strongly reflect an individuals feelings toward an area of

    focus, in this case mathematics, they are different through the fact that self-efficacy has a greater

    emphasis on the performance that is associated with the attitude rather than just the feeling.

    It is crucial that educators create learning environments that build students into adults that

    approach challenging math and science tasks with full force. Times where students shy away

    from these tasks should be limited. Educators need to be sure that they present environments

    where students are getting a positive feeling about how they do mathematics and want to do

    more. When students feel successful in a school setting, they are more likely to want to explore

    it further into their adult life. Classroom environment is a topic that needs to be explored so we

    can not only prevent students from avoiding math, but also encourage them to take it further.

  • 3

    Purpose of the Study

    The purpose of the study was to examine how a classroom and the environment created

    by the teacher and classmates can impact how students feel about their ability to do and be

    successful in mathematics. It focused on how students perceived their classroom environment

    and measured student attitudes toward mathematics in order to determine relationships between

    the two. The relationship between how the students perceived their classroom environment and

    their attitudes toward math was analyzed.

    The quantitative data collected gave insight into the classroom environment

    characteristics that foster negative and/or positive students self-efficacy in mathematics

    classrooms. This study determined how different characteristics of a classroom correlates to

    student self-efficacy in mathematics. The researcher sought to find what characteristics in a

    classroom environment are predictors of negative and/or positive attitudes toward mathematics.

    Emphasis was placed on examining how students feel about the environment created in a

    mathematics classroom and how their feelings toward mathematics were affected. Another

    emphasis of this research was to examine which classroom environment dimensions impact

    negative and positive student self-efficacy in mathematics.

    Research Questions

    This study addressed the following questions:

    1. Which dimensions of classroom environment (cohesiveness, friction, satisfaction,

    difficulty, or competitiveness) are the best predictors of high self-efficacy for students

    in mathematics?

  • 4

    2. Which dimensions of classroom environment (cohesiveness, friction, satisfaction,

    difficulty, or competitiveness) are the best predictors of low self-efficacy for students

    in mathematics?

    Research Hypotheses

    The following null hypotheses reflect the research questions:

    1. No relationship exists among the dimensions of classroom environment

    (cohesiveness, friction, satisfaction, difficulty, or competitiveness) and students high

    self-efficacy in mathematics.

    2. No relationship exists among the dimensions of classroom environment

    (cohesiveness, friction, satisfaction, difficulty, or competitiveness) and students low

    self-efficacy in mathematics.

    Theoretical Framework

    Individuals behaviors and attitudes are caused by multiple variables including their

    environment, peer interactions, feedback from authority figures, and their personal experiences

    (Battistich, Solomon, Kim, Watson, & Schaps, 1995). In a classroom, all of these variables

    impact students and their behavior as well as self-efficacy toward the subject being taught. The

    following historical and current theories support these findings. These theories include the social

    cognitive theory, attribution theory, self-efficacy theory, person environment fit theory, and the

    expectancy-value theory.

    Each theory supports different variables in this study. Figure 1 shows a visual

    representation of how each theory is directly connected to the current study. Self-efficacy is

    connected to the social cognitive theory, attribution theory, and expectancy-value theory.

    Classroom environment is supported by the person-environment fit theory. The social cognitive

  • 5

    theory is thinking about and reflecting over your behavior. Self-efficacy is impacted by the

    social cognitive theory because thinking leads to judgments created about oneself which impact

    individual future performance. The attribution theory causes individuals to think about why they

    succeed or fail and these ideas can lead to future behaviors and performance. Expectancy-value

    theory involves individual motivation in an area based on its value according to that individual.

    This can impact future performance from that individual, as well as self-efficacy. Person-

    environment fit theory is based on how the environment impacts behavior. This directly

    connects to how a classroom is conducted, and the culture created within it can impact the

    students in it.

    Social Cognitive Theory

    Social cognitive learning theorists view human functioning as reciprocal interactions

    among behaviors of individuals, environmental variables, cognition, and personal factors

    (Bandura, 1986). When individuals perform a task, the perceived importance of the task is a

    large part of the result of the outcome expectation the individual has for the task. Bandura

    (1986) stated that beliefs determine expectations; therefore people generally value what they feel

  • 6

    capable of accomplishing and do not value the activities in which they have little confidence.

    Through self-reflection, individuals evaluate their own experiences and thought processes, which

    powerfully influences how they will behave in future tasks (Pajares, 1996).

    Banduras (1997) social cognitive theory proposed that self-efficacy is strongly affected

    by previous performance and influenced by observing others, verbal persuasion, and

    interpretation of physiological states, with possibilities that student perceptions of their learning

    environment also affect their efficacy. People are self-organizing, proactive, self-reflecting, self-

    regulating, non-reactive beings easily influenced by their environmental or inner impulses.

    People interpret their own behavior, which impacts their environment and personal impulses and

    can therefore alter their subsequent behavior. Pajares (2002) supported the idea that teachers can

    work to improve their students perception of school and students emotional state in order to

    self-correct false self-beliefs and develop habits to improve their academic skill and self-

    regulatory practices.

    Additionally, society constructs values and standards that impact the ways students view

    themselves, depending on their approach and success with given tasks in the education system

    (Hickey & Granade, 2004). The social cognitive theory is based on the idea that people

    purposefully engage in their own development and can make things happen through their actions.

    Attribution Theory

    The attribution theory emphasizes the thought that for individuals who believe success is

    due to high ability and failure is due to lack of effort, motivation will remain constant. However,

    students who believe success is luck and failure is expected are less likely to be motivated

    (Diener & Dweck, 1978). Students or others who have always failed in the past in a specific

    task, attribute that failure to themselves, especially if they see others succeeding (Weiner, 2004).

  • 7

    The attribution theory is based on causal attributions that people make about the success or

    failure of their actions that will influence how they feel and how they expect to perform on future

    tasks or activities of the same nature (Weiner, 1986). The effect of childrens own perceptions of

    their ability to achieve success has a direct impact on their personal attitude toward math.

    Attributions influence motivation and performance through the meditational role of self-efficacy

    (Bandura, 1995; Schunk, 1991).

    Self-Efficacy

    Individuals self-efficacy influences how people feel, think, motivate themselves, and

    behave. Bandura (1997) described four major processes that are impacted by self-efficacy

    including cognitive, motivational, affective, and selection processes. Major focuses of cognition

    included the impact of comparing, feedback, and amount of control over a

    situation. Additionally, individuals that believe they will perform well will tend to perform well,

    while those that feel inferior will perform poorly. Students with high self-efficacy seem to

    participate more readily, work harder, persist longer, and achieve higher results.

    Bandura (1986) defined self-efficacy as peoples judgments of their capabilities to

    organize and execute courses of action required to attain designated types of performances (p.

    391). Self-efficacy impacts almost every aspect of peoples lives and is the core of human

    motivation, well-being, and personal accomplishment. It influences individual choices, goals,

    emotional reactions, efforts, coping, and persistence (Gist, Mitchell, & Mitchell, 1992). When

    individuals are faced with adversity, self-efficacy determines their behavior (Pajares, 2002).

    Self-efficacy impacts motivation, affect, and actions based on the interaction of what the

    individual believes rather than what is actually true (Bandura, 1997).

  • 8

    Self-efficacy influences the choices that people make and how much effort they put into

    the tasks, their thought patterns, and their emotional reactions (Pajares, 2002). There are four

    different sources through which self-efficacy can be developed including mastery experience,

    vicarious experience, social persuasions, and somatic and emotional states. Mastery experience

    is the most influential source and is the act of individuals engaging in the actual task or activity

    and then interpreting the results of their actions. These interpretations are then used to develop a

    personal belief about their capability to perform the task or activity and then act in line with the

    beliefs they have created (Pajares, 2002). Researchers have shown that self-efficacy is related to

    the career path and choices made by individuals along with other decisional behaviors (Betz &

    Hackett, 1981, 1983; Lent, Brown, & Larkin, 1987). Also, self-efficacy can predict success and

    persistence in certain academic majors and is strongly related to achievement status (Multon,

    Brown, & Lent, 1991).

    Person Environment Fit Theory

    The person-environment fit theory (Lewin, 1935; Murray, 1938, 1951) emphasizes the

    idea that behavior is a function of the person and the environment. There is a mutual relationship

    between the environment and person such that the environment influences behavior. Hunt

    (1975) emphasized the need for a match between the person and the environment in the course of

    learning. Early adolescents have an increase in a need for higher quality interactions with adults,

    sense of autonomy, and a sense of belonging (Eccles, et al., 1993; Kuperminc, Leadbeater, &

    Blatt, 2001; Midgley, Feldlaufer, & Eccles, 1989; Osterman, 2000). There is dual emphasis on

    the person and the environment and behavior, attitudes, and well-being are determined by both

    the person and the environment. Within the research under person-environment fit theory, the

    feeling gained by the individual arises not from the person or environment but rather by his or

  • 9

    her fit or congruence with one another (Edwards, Caplan, & Harrison, 1998). Classroom

    environments have a culture of their own created by the people within and surrounding it. The

    environment created has an impact on the individuals that are a part of it, which include the

    students. This theory supports the concept that the environment created has an impact on the

    behavior of those that are a part of the environment, in this case, with emphasis on the students.

    Expectancy-value Theory

    The expectancy value theory emphasizes how motivation is a primary result of an

    individuals belief about the outcome of a specific activity and the importance placed on that

    outcome (Atkinson, 1957; McClelland, 1985; Rotter, 1982). Individuals will be motivated to

    participate in tasks if they find value in the outcome of that particular task and will not be

    motivated to take part in a task if they do not find value in the outcome. Researchers have

    agreed that competence in completing a task plays a crucial role if the task will be valued by the

    individual (Eccles, 1983; Wigfield & Eccles, 1992). Bandura (1986) emphasized that outcome

    expectation will have a stronger influence on the motivation and predicting behavior of the task

    performed. Bandura stated that personal judgments of the individuals competence are different

    than the individuals judgment of the likely outcome from the task. Those who expect success

    will behave in such a way in order to achieve that goal. The opposite is also true; if individuals

    expect failure, they will be more likely to fulfill that belief (Pajares, 1996).

    According to Eccles (2009), achievement related behaviors like course selection and

    occupational aspiration are most directly influenced by the individuals expectation for success.

    Research has indicated that students who are most likely to take math courses and to aspire to

    math focused careers place higher value and have greater confidence in their math abilities than

    those who do not (Eccles, 2007).

  • 10

    The expectancy-value theory also shows that the feedback students receive on their

    academic performance influences their motivational beliefs and academic choices (Eccles, 2009).

    Wang (2012) concurred; he found that students who earned higher grades in math also reported

    higher math expectancies and subjective task values, and were more likely to continue with

    course work in math and have math-related jobs in the future.

    Significance of the Problem

    Students in our colleges are straying away from majoring in mathematics intensive fields

    because of the lack of self-efficacy in this area (Committee on Science, Engineering, and Public

    Policy, 2007). This shortage of math majors and graduates has put the United States behind in

    mathematics, science, and technology development. The Industrial Revolution spawned a

    multitude of engineering endeavors that spring boarded the economy in the United States

    (Committee on Science, Engineering, and Public Policy, 2007). Many areas of our life including

    transportation, communication, agriculture, education, health, defense, and employment

    opportunities are available due to the investment in scientific research and engineering (Popper

    & Wagner, 2002). The United States has been considered a leader in science and engineering

    activities since the early 1900s with 30% of the worlds scientists and engineers as well as 17 of

    the worlds top 20 universities (Freeman, 2005). With the reputation so high in the US, other

    countries have stepped up and increased their competitiveness with the US over the past 20

    years. This changing global market requires the US to produce not only more engineers, but

    higher quality engineers that are needed to be worldwide leaders in this high-tech production

    market. High school graduates pursuing engineering degrees are declining (Noeth, Cruce, &

    Harmston, 2003), and less than half the freshmen who begin college with engineering as their

    major finishing with an engineering degree (Besterfield-Sacre, Atman, & Shuman, 1997). One

  • 11

    attempt to solve this problem is to increase the number of students choosing to study engineering

    (Fantz, Siller, & Demiranda, 2011). Mathematics is a crucial piece of many fields, including the

    engineering field. Without mathematics, problem solving, process formation, and application

    would find disconnect within this field of study.

    This research study helps to determine what characteristics of classrooms can lead to a

    low or high self-efficacy in mathematics. Using this information, educators will be able to

    determine what they can do in their classroom to encourage high mathematics self-efficacy in

    their students and eliminate characteristics that tend to form a lower self-efficacy. This will lead

    to improved math interest and achievement as well as an increase in students in mathematic

    career fields. This boost in mathematics in America could jump start the society with

    improvement in areas like Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM) fields.

    Method of Procedure

    This research study sought to determine what characteristics of the mathematic classroom

    environment could predict high or low student self-efficacy in mathematics. Two surveys were

    administered to participants in order to collect data. The data were then analyzed using multiple

    regression.

    Selection of Sample

    Participants for this study included students in fourth through 12th grade in a small North

    Texas school district. Only participants with parent permission and student assent were included

    in the data analysis. The school district superintendent gave prior permission for the researcher

    to collect the student data. Approximately 400 students participated in this study.

  • 12

    Instrumentation

    The Patterns of Adaptive Learning Scale (PALS) (Midgley et al., 2000) instrument as a

    whole is a tool used to measure a variety of learning aspects of the student. This study focused

    solely on high and low self-efficacy, therefore, only parts of the PALS instrument were used in

    data analysis to emphasize self-efficacy rather than the other student scales. The PALS

    instrument was chosen because of its validity and reliability and there was no other appropriate

    mathematics self-efficacy instrument.

    Self-handicapping is associated with maladaptive behavior which leads to low self-

    efficacy (Patrick, Kaplan, & Ryan, 2011), therefore low self-efficacy was measured using the

    statements under academic self-handicapping strategies (p. 368). Self-efficacy also has been

    found to be positively related to mastery goal structure, personal mastery goal orientation, effort,

    not cheating, satisfaction with learning, school-related effort, and achievement (Ames & Archer,

    1988; Anderman, 1999; Kaplan & Midgley, 1999; Murdock, Hale, & Weber, 2001). Therefore,

    high self-efficacy was measured by analyzing the statements that fall under mastery goal

    orientation and academic efficacy.

    Students also took the My Classroom Inventory (MCI) (Fraser, Anderson, & Walberg,

    1982) which measured students perception of the classroom environment in their mathematics

    classroom. The MCI measures five dimensions of social climate, including cohesiveness,

    friction, satisfaction, difficulty, and competitiveness.

    Collection of Data

    Students took the two separate surveys, in an electronic version, during the regular school

    day in their computer lab, the MCI. My Classroom Inventory (MCI) and the selected items from

    the Patterns of Adaptive Learning Survey (PALS) (Midgley et al., 2000) were used to collect and

  • 13

    analyze data from the students. Students entered some demographic information on both

    surveys, including their ID number in order for their surveys to be matched by a district

    employee for analysis. The ID number is a school district issued number that students are

    familiar with and use on a daily basis. Student ID numbers were removed before data were given

    to the researcher.

    Treatment of the Data

    The data were collected and analyzed using Statistical Program for Social Sciences

    (SPSS) through conducting two multiple regressions to determine which dimensions of

    classroom environment can predict a high or low math self-efficacy. Student demographics were

    reported.

    Definitions of Terms

    The following terms are used in the present study:

    Classroom environment. Classroom environment involves interpersonal relationships

    with peers, relationships between students and their teacher, the relationship between students,

    the subject studied and teaching methods, in addition to student perceptions of structural

    characteristics of the class (Fraser et al., 1982). In this study, classroom environment was

    measured by My Classroom Inventory (MCI) (Fraser et al., 1982). The five subscales under

    classroom environment are listed below:

    Cohesiveness- extent to which students, know, help and are friendly toward each other;

    Friction- amount of tension and quarrelling among students;

    Satisfaction- extent of enjoyment of class work;

    Difficulty- the extent to which students find difficulty with the work of the class; and

    Competitiveness- emphasis is placed on students competing with each other.

  • 14

    High self-efficacy. High self-efficacy is defined as mastery goal orientation and

    academic efficacy. High self-efficacy was measured by Patterns of Adaptive Learning Survey

    (PALS) (Midgley et al., 2000) using the mastery goal orientation and academic efficacy

    scales.

    Low self-efficacy. Low self-efficacy is defined as looking at student attribution through

    academic self-handicapping strategies. Low self-efficacy was measured by Patterns of

    Adaptive Learning Survey (PALS) (Midgley et al., 2000) using the academic self-handicapping

    scale.

    Mathematics self-efficacy. Self-efficacy of students specifically in the mathematics

    classroom and academic area of math (Bagaka, 2011).

    Mathematics classroom. Mathematics classrooms ranged from a self-contained

    elementary classroom to a dual credit calculus classroom.

    Self-efficacy. Self-efficacy was defined by Albert Bandura (1994) as the belief in ones

    capabilities to organize and execute the courses of action required to manage prospective

    situations (p. 72). It measures how people think, feel, and behave in certain situations and their

    personal opinion of how they can succeed in that environment.

    Students. Students are defined as children from fourth through 12th grades who were

    approximately age 9 to 19.

    Limitations

    The limitations of this study were as follows:

    1. The district selected has a small population and limited subgroups (ethnicity,

    socioeconomic status, languages spoken).

    2. The sampled participants were not an exact representation of the population of the

  • 15

    school district due to the requirements needed for students to participate.

    3. Students completed the surveys on a computer-based survey system that could cause

    students to make mistakes by incorrectly clicking an answer they do not want.

    4. Previous experiences and events that occur prior to students taking the surveys were

    not controlled by the researcher and could impact the results of the survey.

    Delimitations

    The delimitations of this study were as follows:

    1. The data were collected from one school district.

    2. The data were collected with limited student subgroups.

    3. Only the student section of the PALS survey was used to measure students

    perception of classroom environment in mathematics classrooms. No data were

    collected using the teacher portion of the instrument.

    4. The data were collected within a 10-day time period which could cause some

    difference in data collection and change in attitudes of the participants.

    5. Only grades four through 12 were analyzed.

    6. The researcher chose the order in which the students completed the surveys.

    Assumptions

    This study is based on the following assumptions:

    1. Students responded accurately and honestly.

    2. Teachers administrating the surveys did not impact student responses.

    3. Math teachers did not alter their teaching in order to gain specific results from the

    data collected.

  • 16

    4. Both instruments are valid and reliable and the two surveys did not influence each

    other.

    Organization of the Study

    This dissertation is organized into five chapters. Chapter 1 includes a statement of the

    problem, purpose of the study, research questions, research hypotheses, theoretical framework,

    significance of the study, definitions of terms, limitations, delimitations, and assumptions.

    Chapter 2 includes related professional literature regarding self-efficacy and classroom

    environment in mathematics. In Chapter 3 is a discussion of the research methodology. Chapter

    4 includes the analysis of the data; Chapter 5 includes a discussion of the findings and

    applications to education.

  • 17

    Chapter 2

    LITERATURE REVIEW

    This study aimed to determine what dimensions of classroom environment predict high

    self-efficacy and low self-efficacy in mathematics. Students in a North Texas school district

    participated in taking two surveys. One survey measured their perception of the classroom

    environment of their math class while the other measured the high and low self-efficacy in

    mathematics. Data were analyzed using a multiple regression in order to determine what

    characteristics of the math class could predict high and low self-efficacy in mathematics.

    Self-efficacy plays a major role in individuals everyday lives. Many different variables

    can impact each individuals self-efficacy, especially in the area of mathematics (Hackett &

    Betz, 1989). Students attitudes are influenced by many different things including parents, peers,

    school, teacher, and classroom environment (Klassen & Usher, 2010). This literature review

    examines the importance of a positive self-efficacy in students at all ages in the area of

    mathematics and explains why classroom environment, in regard to self-efficacy, needs to be

    studied further.

    Math Anxiety

    Mathematic anxiety is a worldwide concern. The root of the problem is in schools, where

    students are developing negative attitudes toward mathematics at a very early age (Ashcraft,

    2002). Math anxiety can be described as a feeling of tension that interferes with the

    manipulations of numbers and the solving of mathematical problems in academic and ordinary

    life situations (Sousa, 2008, p. 171). Math anxiety has been defined as the feeling of tension,

    helplessness, mental disorganization and dread when one is required to work and manipulate

    math problems (Ashcraft & Faust, 1994). Math anxiety can conjure up feelings of apprehension,

  • 18

    dislike, fear and dread (McLeod, 1994). It can prevent students from interacting with situations

    that are math intensive and these students avoid upper level math courses (Akin & Kurbanoglu,

    2011). Lazarus (1974) believed that mathematic anxiety developed in elementary and secondary

    grades. Researchers have shown that negative math experiences can start around third or fourth

    grade (Ashcraft & Ridley, 2005; Beilock, Gunderson, Ramirez, & Levine, 2010).

    Math anxiety occurs in people from all different race, gender, and age group and can be a

    product of the home, school, or society. Burns (1998) estimated that 60% of adults have a fear

    of mathematics. Students develop a fear of mathematics (math anxiety) through negative

    experiences in math classes or having a lack of self-confidence with numbers (Sousa, 2008).

    These experiences and lack of confidence usually lead to fear of calculation, failure, and

    difficulty in mathematics. The fear causes their minds to go blank and then causes frustration,

    which leads to additional amnesia. Fear and anxiety is increased when time limits are added to

    the mathematics activity. Students who have developed math anxiety need help to replace the

    memory of failure with the possibility for success (Ashcraft, 2002).

    The most obvious consequence of math anxiety is poor achievement and poor grades in

    mathematics (Sousa, 2008). Poor performance can be caused by a chemical change happening in

    the brain through the biology of the body. Any kind of anxiety causes the body to release

    cortisol into the bloodstream. Cortisol is a hormone that refocuses the brain on the anxiety to

    determine what action to take to relieve the stress. While this is happening, the frontal lobe is no

    longer interested in learning or processing the mathematical operation while the brain is dealing

    with a threat to the individuals safety. Therefore, the student cannot focus and has to cope with

    the frustration of inattention. As well as their inability to manipulate and retain numbers and

    expressions due to a disruption in the working memory (Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001).

  • 19

    Beilock, Gunderson, Ramirez, and Levine (2010) focused on how math anxiety can

    impact students, specifically girls. This study looked at how female elementary teachers math

    anxiety influences the female students achievement and how that compared to the male students.

    Students were first and second grade students who were given math assessments throughout the

    school year. Students were told two gender neutral stories about students who were good at

    math and the other was good at reading and then the students drew a picture of what each looked

    like. The pictures were coded and correlated with the math assessment finding that girls who

    had confirmed gender ability roles (boys are good at math, and girls are good at reading)

    performed worse on the math assessment than girls who did not. These girls also performed

    worse than the boys with these differences related to the anxiety that the teacher had about math.

    Harper and Daane (1998) studied the causes of math anxiety in preservice elementary

    teachers and found that the cause usually stemmed from elementary school and included fear of

    making mistakes, having the right answer, amount of time given for a task, word problems, and

    problem solving. Philippous and Christou (2003) studied preservice teachers in Greece and

    found that teachers with negative attitudes toward mathematics were slightly positively impacted

    when they understood the usefulness of the skill while the deeply rooted anxieties about

    mathematics did not seem to change.

    Ma (1999) found that there is a significant relationship between math anxiety and math

    achievement. Bretscher, Dwindell, Hey, and Higbee (1989) posited that students who learned

    math because they wanted to, had higher math achievement, therefore the motivation toward

    performing math increased the student achievement. Norwood (1994) found that the elements of

    math anxiety included a mixture of truancy, poor self-image, poor coping skills, teacher attitude,

    and the emphasis on learning math through drill practice rather than understanding. Zakaria and

  • 20

    Nordin (2008) found that students who had a high math anxiety also had a low math achievement

    as well as the students with low math anxiety had high math achievement.

    Classroom Environment

    The term classroom environment refers to the social and psychological surroundings of

    the classroom (Fraser, 1991). The teacher is a part of and contributes to the classroom

    environment which influences choices and norms of the classroom (Shuell, 1996). Research has

    shown that the quality of classroom environment is a significant determinant of student learning

    (Fraser, 1994, 1998b). Early seminal work by Lewin (1935, 1936) and Murray (1938)

    recognized that both the environment and its interaction with personal characteristics of the

    individual are determinants of the human behavior. Students learn better when they perceive the

    classroom environment positively (Dorman, 2003). Research on classroom environment has

    been diverse and varied, but began with the work of Walberg (1979) and Moos (1974), who

    spawned additional research programs all over the world. While questionnaires were used

    greatly in the beginning of the classroom environment research, both quantitative and qualitative

    methods are the more typical route of researchers. The majority of classroom environment

    research has been done in science classrooms and very few have involved mathematics

    classrooms (Spinner & Fraser, 2005).

    Classroom environment has been shown to be the most significant factor in students

    learning and attitudes in math and science (Fraser & Kahle, 2007). The classroom environment

    is a critical context for promoting the development of students educational and career interests

    (Simpkins, Davis-Kean, & Eccles, 2006). There is evidence to suggest that classroom

    environment influences how well students achieve a range of desirable outcomes (Fraser, 2007).

    Research has supported the fact that the social environment of classrooms can significantly

  • 21

    impact students motivated behavior, specifically the level of friendship students feel for each

    other measured by students getting to know each other, helping each other, and working together

    (Fraser & Fisher, 1983; Trickett & Moos, 1974). Students have been found to achieve better in

    the types of classroom environments that they prefer (Fraser & Fisher, 1983).

    Teacher techniques that include the focus on memorization rather than understanding the

    concept are among the main sources of math anxiety. Math anxiety also stems from a classroom

    culture that searches for one right answer with no recognition or appreciation for the thinking the

    student goes through or their cognitive process. Flewelling and Higginson (2001) found that

    students who have rewarding and successful learning experiences with math were able to

    overcome their math anxiety. Math classrooms and teachers who focus on making sense of that

    mathematical process and not memorizing or being correct cultivate students who avoid math

    anxiety.

    Having a positive classroom environment is a valuable goal of education (Fraser, 2001).

    Describing the class through the actual participants, students are in a good position to make

    judgments about classrooms because they have experienced many different learning

    environments and have spent enough time in the class to form accurate opinions. While teachers

    can be inconsistent in daily behavior, there is usually a consistent picture of the traditions and

    features of the classroom environment. While observation is a strategy used to collect data on

    classroom environments, it does not tell the whole story about the students perspective.

    Classroom environment includes the relationships between students, teachers, and subject

    material (Fraser et al., 1982). Five components of classroom environment will be emphasized in

    this research including cohesiveness, friction, satisfaction, difficulty, and competitiveness.

  • 22

    Sinclair and Fraser (2002) conducted research that looked into three areas of classroom

    environment. They worked on developing an instrument (Middle School Inventory of

    Classroom Environments or ICE), collecting quantitative and qualitative data on typical

    classroom environments, and used the information so teachers could positively impact their

    classroom and students. Data were collected from about 745 students on their perceived and

    preferred classroom environments, along with data collected from ten teachers on their perceived

    and preferred classroom environments. Sinclair and Fraser also took part in classroom

    observations of the participating teachers. Analysis of the data collected compared the teacher

    and student preferred and perceptions of the classroom environment. A one-way analysis of

    variance (ANOVA) was used in order to analyze the data for each scale within the instrument.

    After initial scores were collected on the teachers and students, the researchers met with the

    teachers to share the information and determine what areas that the teacher wanted to improve

    upon in order to increase student perceived classroom environment. One teacher aimed to

    improve her students perceptions of involvement and teacher empathy in her class. The teacher

    worked on including students in the science lab preparation as well as assistance with class pet

    maintenance.

    Research done on classroom social climates has shown that classrooms characterized by

    cohesiveness, satisfaction, and goal directions are preferred by students and are associated with

    positive outcomes for students (Fraser, 1991). Students sense of autonomy and participation in

    decision making has also been shown to have positive effects for children (Lewin, Lippitt, &

    White, 1939). Having a caring environment conveys a set of values such as mutual respect,

    valuing individual members contributions, and obligation of each member to meet the needs of

    the community (Battistich, Solomon, Kim, Watson, & Schaps, 1995). Fraser (1998a), with

  • 23

    support from Goh, Young, and Fraser (1995) found associations between students perception of

    the classroom environment in mathematical classes and established that students with greater

    cohesiveness were linked to higher achievement for math and teacher support: task orientation

    and equity were linked with more positive attitudes and self-esteem.

    Cooperative classroom strategies are associated with improved peer relations and

    supporting mutual respect (Anderson, 2004). Johnson and Johnson (1991) found that

    cooperative learning environments lead to productive classrooms where students exert high

    effort to achieve positive and supportive relationships and psychologically healthy and socially

    competent students. In a teacher-centered mathematics classroom that is controlled by rules,

    routines, and individual drilling, there is little room for student autonomy or social belonging

    within the mathematic learning. Student-centered classrooms with teamwork and emphasis on

    meaning making give students many opportunities to have students needs met through a variety

    of approaches (Hannula, 2006).

    The degree to which a classroom is challenging can also influence academic self-

    efficacy. Challenging is defined as an environment where students are given progressively

    difficult tasks as their proficiency increases. Some researchers have suggested that challenging

    students can lead to a stronger belief in the students personal academic abilities (Battistich et al.,

    1995; Pajares, 1996).

    One of the essential ways to improve middle grade education is to establish a safe and

    healthy school environment (Jackson & Davis, 2000). Students can be placed at academic risk

    of failure because of the quality of their school and classroom learning environment

    (Montgomery & Rossi, 1994). Ineffective and dysfunctional classrooms and instructional

    learning environments have been uncovered in multiple middle schools (Midgley, Eccles, &

  • 24

    Feldlaufer, 1991; MacIver & Epstein, 1993; Waxman, Huang, & Padron, 1995). Middle schools

    are usually structured, formal, and less personal than elementary schools and students frequently

    become bored and alienated with an increase in teacher talk and lack of student involvement

    (Waxman et al., 1995). Middle school classes tend to be more teacher-centered and discipline

    focused where teacher student relations and student decision making are not a focus (Feldlaufer,

    Midgley, & Eccles, 1988). Additionally, middle schools often do not encourage personal

    relationships even though caring and supportive environments are critical for students (Baker,

    1998; Roeser, Midgley, & Urdan, 1996). Classroom environment needs to be a focus in the

    middle grades in order to increase student cognitive and affective outcomes (Fraser, 1998;

    Haertel, Walberg, & Haertel, 1981). Researchers have shown that cohesiveness, student

    satisfaction, and teacher support are positively related to student increase in academic

    achievement (Waxman, Read, & Garcia, 2008).

    Research has been devoted to comparing the perception students have of their classroom

    as one which is performance based or encourages mastery (Patrick, Kaplan, & Ryan, 2011).

    Classrooms structured around mastery goals focus on effort put into a task as well as the intrinsic

    value of learning. This is compared to the performance-based classroom that focuses on

    competition and natural ability. Previous research has found that classrooms based around the

    mastery goal model have higher academic self-efficacy (Friedel, Cortina, Turner, & Midgley,

    2007). The degree to which students perceive their classroom as a caring environment also has

    an influence on self-efficacy. Teachers in these classrooms express personal interest in the

    students, provide emotional support, and create a comfortable atmosphere. Murdock and Miller

    (2003) suggested that students who perceive their teachers as caring are more likely to view

    themselves as more academically capable, set higher goals for themselves, and have significantly

  • 25

    higher self-efficacy. The effect of emotional support on math achievement was larger than on

    quantity of math instruction.

    Roeser et al. (1996) found that a greater sense of school belonging, along with an

    emphasis on effort, understanding, and beliefs that all students can learn, were associated with

    academic self-efficacy. Cowen, Work, Hightower, Wyman, Parker, & Lotyczewski (1991)

    found those students who perceive high levels of classroom competition, friction, and difficulty,

    felt less efficacy when approached with an academic challenge.

    McMahon, Wernsman, and Rose (2009) examined 149 fourth and fifth graders from

    diverse backgrounds in California that completed two self-reports on their perceived classroom

    environment. The MCI (My Classroom Inventory) was used to collect data from the students on

    their perceived classroom environment, school belongingness was measured using the

    Psychological Sense of School Membership Scale, and self-efficacy in language arts and math

    was also measured using a The Academic Self-Efficacy Scale. They found that satisfaction,

    cohesion, and school belonging were significantly and positively correlated along with difficulty,

    competitiveness and friction. Additionally, classroom environment and school belonging predict

    self-efficacy and lower difficulty predicted higher math and science self-efficacy. School

    belonging and satisfaction and cohesion did not significantly predict math and science self-

    efficacy.

    Self-Efficacy and Classroom Environment

    Consistent and convincing research gives evidence that the quality of the classroom

    environment is a significant determinant of student learning (Fraser, 1994). A positive learning

    environment can influence student academic achievement and attitudes (Fisher, Henderson, &

  • 26

    Fraser, 1995). Fraser (1994) indicated that student perceptions of learning environments are an

    important factor in explaining their cognitive and affective outcomes.

    In terms of self-efficacy and classroom climate, these factors play important roles in the

    learning environment (Pitkaniemi & Vanninen, 2012). Students are more likely to have greater

    expectancy values in math which can lead to students taking more math courses and pursuing a

    career in mathematics. These students can then encourage, cooperate, interact, and help their

    classmates and view the curriculum and teaching as meaningful and relevant to their lives when

    they perceive their teacher as understanding and supportive while having high expectations for

    their learning achievement (Wang, 2012). Teacher and school practices that promote students

    mathematical self-efficacy may not only promote mathematic achievements, but also could

    narrow the achievement gaps in mathematics as found by gender, socioeconomic status, and

    minority status (Bagaka, 2011).

    Self-efficacy predicts students math achievement, and there are reasons to suspect that

    the relationship between teachers classroom behavior and students academic performance are

    also positively correlated (Weinstein & McKown, 1998). Students carefully observe teachers

    verbal and nonverbal behaviors while developing self-beliefs and academic behaviors based on

    these observations (Weinstein & McKown, 1998). When educators demonstrate a direct interest

    in student care and concern, as well as respect for their thoughts, opinions, and ideas, the

    outcome supports a decrease in student depressive symptoms and an increase in self-esteem

    (Reddy, Rhones, & Mulhall, 2003). Further et al. (1998) determined that affective teacher

    behavior including listening, respect, recognition, and fair treatment significantly influenced

    young adolescent motivation. Muller, Katz, and Dance (1999) established that students 8-18

    years of age desire a personal connection with their teacher and yearn for the instructor to

  • 27

    maintain high academic expectations. Fairness is an additional characteristic that students retain

    from their educator in the classroom. Students identify with different ways teachers treat

    students associated with success and ability (Weinstein & McKown, 1998).

    The powerful relationship that grows between the teacher and student in the classroom

    plays a crucial role in developing the emotional, motivational, and academic behaviors of the

    student. Teacher support correlates directly with youth adjustment, achievement, social, and

    motivational development. While educators have a specialized focus of specific academic

    content, there needs to be an equal focus on student affect and social-emotional needs (Osterman,

    2000). Through self-recorded data, students show a decline in teacher support throughout school

    years (Reddy et al., 2003) as well as a decline in a sense of belonging over time (Anderman,

    2003). The data from the mathematics self-reports suggest that students feel less valuable and

    see a lower persistence in middle school years.

    A supportive teaching style has been positively linked to student achievement. It has

    been found that if teachers academic support (the teacher cares about their learning, tries to help

    them learn, and wants them to do their best), academic press (the teacher checks for

    understanding and engagement), and mastery goal (the teacher emphasizes learning and

    understanding, focuses on student development) are all implemented in the classroom, student

    achievement improves (Goodenow, 1993; Kaplan & Midgley, 1999; Wentzel, 1994, 1997).

    Students who perceive that their math teachers take into account student relatedness and

    competence, and enforce positive demands on students academic work show more positive

    motivational beliefs and achieve higher grades. Students who perceive their teacher as

    responsive, helpful and recognizant of good work tend to perform better than their peers whose

    teachers are perceived as less supportive (Ambrose, 2004). These results support Slovenes

  • 28

    findings of early adolescents perceptions of their teachers and motivational beliefs (self-efficacy

    and intrinsic motivation) (Puklek, 2001; Puklek, 2004).

    Self-efficacy beliefs are created through the individuals interpretation of information

    from different internal and external sources (Bandura, 1997; Pajares, 2002). An external source

    of self-efficacy beliefs is verbal judgments that others provide about their capabilities. Teachers

    are a crucial element of the classroom environment. Students perception of affective teacher

    support can influence their enjoyment in mathematics.

    Math and science self-efficacy were significantly negatively correlated with difficulty,

    and positively correlated with language arts self-efficacy (Pajares, 2002). Predictors of self-

    efficacy include satisfaction and cohesiveness; difficulty, competitiveness, and friction, and

    school belonging. In terms of math and science self-efficacy, difficulty was the sole predictor

    when a self-efficacy test was given for the second time. Other variables that can impact self-

    efficacy are parental influence, teacher-student and student-student interaction, teacher

    instructional techniques, and appropriate teacher support. Kaplan, Gheen, and Midgley (2002)

    suggested that students are more likely to have positive self-efficacy from mastering a subject

    rather than from performing a standard. This could explain the finding that perceived difficulty

    predicted math and science self-efficacy.

    When students have a perceived high academic self-efficacy, they exhibit a positive

    behavioral adjustment and social competence, greater self-concept, and stronger relationships

    with peers and parents (Kuperminc, Blatt, & Leadbeater, 1997). Student academic self-efficacy

    is a strong predictor of academic engagement, persistence, academic effort and performance

    (Linenbrink & Pintrich, 1997). School environment significantly influences a students self-

    efficacy.

  • 29

    The relationship between academic effort and academic achievement in middle school is

    important because it has been found to predict math achievement in high school, which will

    directly impact the student in college (Wang & Goldschmidt, 2003). Previous theory and

    research suggested a positive relation between academic self-efficacy beliefs and academic

    outcomes of students (Bandura, 1997; Pajares & Graham, 1999). Lorsbach and Jinks (1999)

    suggested that student perceptions of their learning environment are influenced by student

    academic self-efficacy and can lead to an appreciation of what is happening in classrooms.

    As expected, students who reported a greater sense of belonging in their mathematics

    classroom were likely to report higher academic enjoyment (Wang, 2012). Researchers did not

    find any statistical significance between academic enjoyment and academic hopelessness, or

    between academic enjoyment and academic self-efficacy. Academic enjoyment proved to be a

    powerful connection with academic effort. Students who reported higher teacher affective

    support were likely to report lower academic hopelessness, which was associated with greater

    academic self-efficacy (Pekrun, Goetz, Frenzel, Barchfeld, & Perry, 2011). Academic

    hopelessness did negatively predict academic effort through its detrimental effect on academic

    self-efficacy. Students who reported high academic hopelessness were likely to report low

    academic self-efficacy belief and related to lower academic effort. Students who report higher

    academic self-efficacy tended to report greater academic success in mathematics.

    There was a positive correlation between teacher academic press and student

    motivational beliefs; students self-efficacy and mastery goal orientation in math were positively

    related to their math grade. Results also showed that level of parental involvement would predict

    student math grades, while the math teaching measures were the most powerful predictors of

    student self-efficacy in math (Battistich et al., 1995). The students ratings of math teachers

  • 30

    academic support contributed to student mastery goal orientation and math achievement. The

    perceptions of teacher academic press predicted student self-efficacy and mastery goal

    orientation in math and their math grade (Anderson, Hamilton, & Hattie, 2004).

    Overall, it was found that classroom environment does have an impact on student

    academic self-efficacy and the many different variables that can impact these relate to students

    and their experiences (Weinstein & McKown, 1998).

    Self-Efficacy and Math

    Self-efficacy in mathematics has been studied, but not in great detail. Math self-efficacy

    is a strong predictor of math performance (Pajares & Miller 1994, 1995). The self-efficacy

    theory states that perceived self-efficacy influences and is influenced by thought patterns,

    affective arousal, and choice behavior as well as task performance (Bandura, 1977, 1986).

    According to the social learning theory, self-efficacy expectations are an important factor in

    influencing math attitudes and math anxiety (Bandura, 1977; Hackett & Betz, 1981). Bandura

    (1986), Pajares (1996), and Schunk (1991) found that self-efficacy beliefs predict student

    performance in mathematics. Self-efficacy can also influence math performance as strongly as

    general mathematics ability (Pajares & Kranzler, 1995). Across ability levels, students who have

    high self-efficacy are more accurate in their mathematical computation and are more persistent

    when faced with a challenge when compared to students with low self-efficacy (Collins, 1985).

    Lloyd, Walsh, and Yailagh (2005) analyzed fourth and seventh graders in order to

    compare their math grades, math foundation skills, performance attributions, and self-efficacy

    looking specifically at gender differences. They found that boys and girls equally attributed their

    success to effort and fourth graders were more likely to attribute their success to effort when

    compared to seventh graders. Ability was the attribution that the majority of students believed

  • 31

    lead to success. Fourth graders were also more likely than seventh graders to attribute their

    success to help from their teachers than seventh graders. Fourth graders were more efficacious

    than seventh graders and girls tended to be under-confident while boys were over-confident;

    however, girls achievement met or exceeded boys achievement.

    Akin and Kurbanoglu (2011) examined the relationship between math anxiety and self-

    efficacy. Participants included 372 university students in Turkey who took the assessment R-

    MARS (Revised Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale) to measure anxiety, Mathematics Attitudes

    Scale to measure mathematic attitudes, and Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire

    (MSLQ) to measure self-efficacy. They found that self-efficacy is a proximal determinant of

    math attitudes and self-efficacy and that math anxiety was predicted by negative self-efficacy.

    Overall, the stronger the self-efficacy, the more active are the individuals efforts and the longer

    they will persist. Additionally, self-efficacy predicted negative attitudes and positive attitudes.

    Pajares and Miller (1996) found that math self-efficacy has stronger direct effects on

    mathematics problem solving than self-concept, perceived usefulness, or prior experience.

    Judgments of individuals ability to solve math problems should be more strongly able to predict

    their ability to solve those problems than their confidence in their ability on math related tasks.

    Similarly, judgments of their ability to succeed in math related courses should predict their

    choice to enroll in math courses than should their confidence in their ability to solve specific

    problems or perform math tasks (Pajares, 1996). Schunk (1981) showed that teacher modeling

    increased persistence and accuracy on division problems by arising student self-efficacy, which

    had a direct effect on skill. Additionally, he found that student effort was attributed to feedback

    of prior performance. This behavior raised student self-efficacy expectations in elementary

  • 32

    students. Later, he also discovered that ability feedback had a stronger effect on self-efficacy

    and performance (Schunk & Gunn, 1986).

    Self-Efficacy and Achievement

    Self-efficacy contributes to personal goals individuals set for themselves, how much

    effort they will exert in order to perform a task, how long an individual will persevere when

    facing a challenge, and how resilient the individual is toward failures. Bandura (1982) found

    that self-efficacy is more strongly related to future and actual task performance than past

    performance. Self-efficacy is not concerned with specific skills an individual has but the

    judgments and self-belief of what one can do with the skills they possess (Bandura, 1982).

    Several studies have established the strong positive connection between student self-

    efficacy and their academic performance (Pajares, 1996; Pajares & Graham, 1999). Self-efficacy

    has been shown to predict achievement outcomes in a variety of content areas including

    mathematics, science, and writing (Klassen & Usher, 2010; Pajares, 1996; Pajares & Urdan,

    2006). Self-efficacy is also a powerful predictor of student achievement (Al-Harthy, Was, &

    Isaacson, 2010; Andrew, 1998; Bandura, 1993; Barkly, 2006; Paulsen & Gentry, 1995; Schunk,

    1989; Zimmerman, 2000).

    Bandura (1977, 1997) and Pajares (1996) found that higher self-efficacy scores leads to

    better performance and persistence in engineering courses. Self-efficacy is a task-specific

    capability (Gist, Mitchell, & Mitchell, 1992) and a dynamic construct. The self-efficacy

    judgment from the individual changes over time as new information and experiences are gained

    (Bandura, 1989). Personal efficacy beliefs help individuals determine how much effort people

    will spend on an activity, for how long they will persevere when faced with a challenge, and how

    resilient they are when the odds are not in their favor (Pajares, 1996). Self-efficacy also

  • 33

    influences the thought patterns and emotional reactions of individuals. Self-efficacy beliefs are

    powerful predictors of the choices that individuals make on a daily basis, the level of effort that

    they put on the task, and their persistence toward facing challenges (Multon, Brown, & Lent,

    1991). Individuals with low self-efficacy may view a challenge and think that it is more difficult

    than it really is, impacting their stress level, depression, and ability to best solve the problem. In

    contrast, high self-efficacy helps individuals create a feeling of confidence when approaching

    difficult tasks and activities (Pajares, 1996). Pekrun et al. (2011) found that focusing on

    academic enjoyment in a college classroom positively impacted self-efficacy, intrinsic and

    extrinsic motivation, academic effort, self-regulation, and academic performance.

    There are four factors that influence self-efficacy: mastery experience, vicarious

    experience, social persuasions, and somatic emotional state. Mastery experience, interpreting

    ones own performance, is the most potent source of self-efficacy (Bandura, 1986, 1997). Prior

    experience will affect students initial belief in their personal capabilities. Those who perform

    well on the activity believe they are capable of furthering their abilities in that area. Individuals

    who experience challenge and difficulties may doubt their capabilities (Schunk, 1989). Actions

    perceived by the individual as successful typically raise self-efficacy and perceived failure

    lowers it. Positive feedback can enhance self-efficacy but can be short lived if efforts following

    the feedback are poor as students are generally not motivated to behave in ways that they believe

    will result in negative outcomes (Schunk, 1989). Research shows that mastery goal orientation is

    linked to positive, adaptive pattern of attributions, whereas a performance goal orientation was

    linked to a maladaptive, helpless pattern of attributions (Ames, 1992b; Dweck & Leggett, 1988).

    Under mastery goal orientation, students are more likely to see a strong link between effort and

    outcomes and make more effort attributions for success and failure (Schunk, Meece, & Pintrich,

  • 34

    2014). Students with performance goal orientation see effort and ability as inversely related, as

    opposed to the positive relation under mastery goal (Schunk et al., 2014). Self-efficacy has been

    found to be related to goal orientation and found that people with mastery goals have higher self-

    efficacy and better task performance than people with performance goals (Locke, Frederick, Lee,

    & Bobko, 1984; Locke & Latham, 1990; Wood, Bandura, & Bailey, 1990).

    Researchers have found links between mastery goals and judgments of self-efficacy are

    generally positive (Sakiz, 2011). As mastery goals were formed, Dweck and Leggett (1988)

    performed laboratory research that showed that students oriented toward mastery and learning

    maintained positive and adaptive self-efficacy beliefs and perceptions of competence in the face

    of difficult tasks. Mastery goals related positively to self-efficacy in college students enrolled in

    statistics courses (Bandalos, Finney, & Geske, 2003). Bong (2009), Kaplan and Midgley (1997),

    Middleton and Midgley (1997), Sakiz (2011), and Thorkildsen and Nicholls (1998) have also

    shown the same general pattern.

    Vicarious experience, observing the actions of others, is also another way that individuals

    obtain information about what they can do (Bandura, 1997; Schunk, 1987). Students who

    observe similar peers perform a task may believe that they are capable as well. This source of

    self-efficacy is not as strong as mastery experience, but when individuals are uncertain of their

    abilities or have little prior experience, they become more sensitive to it (Pajares, 2002). Self-

    efficacy can also be created through the result of social persuasions received from others in their

    environment. Efficacy will increase when individuals are being told they are capable by a

    trustworthy source. This can include verbal judgments from peers or adults and play an

    important role in the development of an individuals self-beliefs. Individuals compare

    themselves to others in their environment around them and evaluate themselves with those who

  • 35

    are similar in ability (Festinger, 1954). Lastly, anxiety, stress, arousal, and mood states fall

    under that category of somatic and emotional states and can influence self-efficacy. Strong

    emotional reactions to a task can foreshadow the anticipated success or failure of the outcome

    (Pajares, 2002). Bandura (1997) found that people live in psychic environments that are of their

    own making, so therefore, individuals have the capability to alter their own thinking and feeling

    to enhance their self-efficacy beliefs. Self-efficacy can change as a result of learning,

    experience, and feedback (Gist et al., 1992).

    Self-efficacy can affect individuals psychological well-being and performance while

    exerting some influence over their lives through the environments they select and environments

    they create. Personal efficacy affects each individuals choices of activities to take part in.

    Those who believe they are not capable of a task will avoid it, but the same individual will be

    willing to take on an alternate activity they feel they are capable of completing or accomplishing

    (Wood & Bandura, 1989). Perceived self-efficacy also has an impact on the choice of the

    individuals career path with stronger self-efficacy connecting to more career options they

    consider to be possible (Betz & Hackett, 1986; Lent & Hackett, 1987). Self-efficacy also

    enhances students memory performance by enhancing persistence (Berry, 1987).

    Academic self-efficacy can be seen as a part of student motivation and is defined as

    students beliefs about their ability to learn or perform specific tasks (Bandura, 1986, 1997).

    Students with high self-efficacy attempt difficult tasks and activities regularly and tend to

    achieve higher than students with low self-efficacy (Pajares, 1996; Schunk, 1991). Students with

    low self-efficacy generally give up on a learning activity when the results of success are not as

    they preferred, which can lead to lower success, and a further reduced sense of academic self-

    efficacy. High self-efficacy has been linked to higher grade point averages, standardized test

  • 36

    scores, persistence on a challenging task, and enrollment in upper-level math courses (Pajares,

    1996; Pintrich & Schunk, 2002).

    Students with high self-efficacy have a variety of characteristics that help them increase

    their achievement and success in the classroom (Schunk, 1981). These students try harder, and

    persevere longer than their lower self-efficacy counterparts (Bandura, 1982; Bandura, 1986;

    Pajares, 2003; Pajares & Schunk, 2001) while having a strong sense of responsibility. They are

    more concerned with the subject, deeply involved in the classroom activities, and try different

    strategies when they meet difficulties, which lead to greater effort and success (Morgan & Jinks,

    1999). Students with high self-efficacy set high expectations for themselves and produce

    behaviors to perform well (Maxwell, 1998) along with being comfortable and confidently

    approaching tasks (Schunk, 1991; Bandura, 1993). When these students are faced with a

    challenge, they put forth greater effort to overcome obstacles (Bandura, 1986, 1997) and spend

    more energy when encountering difficulties (Schunk, 1990) while being more relaxed and

    efficient when faced with a challenge (Bandura, 1993; Schunk, 1991). Students with higher

    math self-efficacy persist longer on difficult tasks and are more accurate in computations

    compared to students with lower math self-efficacy (Collins, 1985; Hoffman & Schraw, 2009).

    The students with low self-efficacy in writing were easily distracted from activities,

    wandered around the room, avoided writing tasks, gave up easily, and took a lot of time to write

    (Kim & Lorsbach, 2005). Other characteristics of low self-efficacy include a lack of strong

    achievement (Schunk, 1981), giving up easily and that leads to lower success (Morgan & Jinks,

    1999). These students also may avoid specific choices (Bandura, 1982) and experience stress

    and ineffectiveness when faced with a challenge (Bandura, 1986, 1997).

  • 37

    Efficacy cues include performance outcomes where success in a task raises the self-

    efficacy and failure will lower it. Individuals can perceive their success or failure using

    attribution cues such as ability, effort, task difficulty, or luck (Frieze, 1980; Weiner, 1985).

    Bodily symptoms like sweating and trembling can symbolize physiological cues for determining

    efficacy.

    Self-efficacy can also be assumed to be a motivating factor and is correlated with

    characteristics of the learning environment such as goal orientation, high cohesion, satisfaction,

    and a low level of disorder and conflict (Anderson et al., 2004). Bandura (1997), Nichols

    (1996), and Pajares (1997) argued that student perceptions of self-efficacy have a positive impact

    on student motivation and achievement. Self-efficacy determines individuals level of

    motivation which is reflected in how much effort they will exert and how long they will

    persevere. The stronger their self-efficacy, the more persistence, effort, and accomplishment

    they have (Bandura & Cervone, 1983, 1986; Weinberg, Gould, & Jackson, 1979). Self-efficacy

    can lead to self-aiding or self-hindering thought patterns, as well as personal goal setting. The

    higher their self-efficacy, the higher goals are set and the firmer the commitments to those goals

    (Locke et al., 1984; Taylor, Locke, Lee, & Gist, 1984).

    Student perceived self-efficacy affects their academic interest and motivation as well as

    management of stress (Bassi, Steca, Fave, & Caprara, 2007) while mediating the effect of skill,

    previous experience, mental ability, or other self-beliefs on subsequent achievement (Pajares &

    Schunk, 2001). Additionally, Eccles, Midgley, Wigfield, Buchanan, Reuman, Flanagan, and

    MacIver (1993) suggested that achievement related activities selected by individuals are

    influenced by social contexts of the individual, like the classroom and family.

  • 38

    Student Attitudes and Achievement

    Student achievement in mathematics is impacted by environmental factors including the

    emotional response to math (Sousa, 2008). Math and reading have been the standard in the

    United States to determine the academic abilities of s