11
Classically, the definitional existence of personal- ity and the conceptual foundation of personality traits in particular rested on the existence of “rela- tively enduring patterns of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors” (Roberts, 2009). Longitudinal and devel- opmental research over the last two decades has re- affirmed the existence of these relatively enduring patterns of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, while at the same time showing indisputable evidence that personality traits also change. This body of re- search has led to the necessity of changing our def- inition of personality traits. Specifically, we now de- fine personality traits as the relatively automatic thoughts, feelings, and behaviors that arise in re- sponse to environmental stimuli. These patterns of automatic responses are relatively enduring across time when people are confronted with isomorphic environmental conditions. Finally, these automatic patterns can be changed through relatively endur- ing environmental or intrapsychic presses that con- tradict given patterns of thought, feeling, and be- havior. This definition of personality traits is explicitly different than standard deviations for several rea- sons, but most importantly because of the inclusion of the idea that personality traits change. What has changed over the last few decades that would ne- cessitate such a definitional refinement? Over the last few decades, research that examines multiple indices of continuity and change has shown that personality traits are consistent over time, but they also change throughout adulthood (e.g., Mroczek & Spiro, 2003; Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006; Soto, John, Gosling, & Potter, 2011; Srivastava, John, Gosling, & Porter, 2003). We are just now integrating this empirical edifice into our theoretical and con- ceptual models. Therefore, in this paper we provide an overview of the evidence for personality trait continuity and change and some of the possible Personality Trait Development in Adulthood: Patterns and Implications 1) Brent W. ROBERTS Yusuke TAKAHASHI University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign Kyoto University Recent longitudinal and cross-sectional aging research has shown that personality traits are both consistent over time and yet change systematically in adulthood. In this paper, we review the evidence for personality trait continuity, mean-level change in personality traits, as well as individual differences in change across the life span. We then discuss several reasons for why personality would continue to change in adulthood and the im- plications of those changes for human capital and social policy. Key words: personality traits, personality development, adulthood, continuity, change © 日本パーソナリティ心理学会 2011 パーソナリティ研究 2011 20 巻 第 1 号 1–10 依頼論文 1) This research was supported by grants R01 AG21178 and R01 AG1846 from the National Institute of Aging.

Document1

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

articol psihologie

Citation preview

  • Classically, the definitional existence of personal-

    ity and the conceptual foundation of personality

    traits in particular rested on the existence of rela-

    tively enduring patterns of thoughts, feelings, and

    behaviors (Roberts, 2009). Longitudinal and devel-

    opmental research over the last two decades has re-

    affirmed the existence of these relatively enduring

    patterns of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, while

    at the same time showing indisputable evidence

    that personality traits also change. This body of re-

    search has led to the necessity of changing our def-

    inition of personality traits. Specifically, we now de-

    fine personality traits as the relatively automatic

    thoughts, feelings, and behaviors that arise in re-

    sponse to environmental stimuli. These patterns of

    automatic responses are relatively enduring across

    time when people are confronted with isomorphic

    environmental conditions. Finally, these automatic

    patterns can be changed through relatively endur-

    ing environmental or intrapsychic presses that con-

    tradict given patterns of thought, feeling, and be-

    havior.

    This definition of personality traits is explicitly

    different than standard deviations for several rea-

    sons, but most importantly because of the inclusion

    of the idea that personality traits change. What has

    changed over the last few decades that would ne-

    cessitate such a definitional refinement? Over the

    last few decades, research that examines multiple

    indices of continuity and change has shown that

    personality traits are consistent over time, but they

    also change throughout adulthood (e.g., Mroczek &

    Spiro, 2003; Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006;

    Soto, John, Gosling, & Potter, 2011; Srivastava, John,

    Gosling, & Porter, 2003). We are just now integrating

    this empirical edifice into our theoretical and con-

    ceptual models. Therefore, in this paper we provide

    an overview of the evidence for personality trait

    continuity and change and some of the possible

    Personality Trait Development in Adulthood: Patterns and Implications1)

    Brent W. ROBERTS Yusuke TAKAHASHIUniversity of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign Kyoto University

    Recent longitudinal and cross-sectional aging research has shown that personality traits are both consistent

    over time and yet change systematically in adulthood. In this paper, we review the evidence for personality trait

    continuity, mean-level change in personality traits, as well as individual differences in change across the life

    span. We then discuss several reasons for why personality would continue to change in adulthood and the im-

    plications of those changes for human capital and social policy.

    Key words: personality traits, personality development, adulthood, continuity, change

    2011

    2011 20 1110

    1) This research was supported by grants R01 AG21178

    and R01 AG1846 from the National Institute of Aging.

  • reasons for these changes. We also touch upon

    some of the practical and theoretical implications

    of these overall changes in personality traits.

    Definitions of Personality

    Continuity and Change

    One reason for the confusion over personality

    continuity and change is that researchers fail to

    clarify what they mean when they describe person-

    ality as consistent or changeable. Part of the diffi-

    culty arises from the multiple ways to track conti-

    nuity and change, such as rank-order consistency,

    mean-level change, structural consistency, and indi-

    vidual differences in change. A complete under-

    standing of personality continuity and change can

    only come from a thorough examination of multi-

    ple indices of continuity and change as they are

    often independent of one another and therefore

    provide different answers to the question, Do per-

    sonality traits change? (Roberts, Wood, & Caspi,

    2008).

    In this paper, we review rank-order consistency,

    mean-level change, and individual differences in

    change because these indices reflect the most di-

    rect indicators of continuity and change. Rank-

    order consistency refers to the maintenance of rank

    on a trait relative to others in a sample or popula-

    tion. Mean-level change refers to gains and/or

    losses in specific personality traits over a pre-speci-

    fied period of time and age for a population of in-

    dividuals. Individual differences in change reflect

    deviations from these overall patterns of mean-level

    change. That is, some people will change much

    more or less than the average patterns of increase

    or decrease.

    Rank-Order Consistency/Change

    Despite widely differing views on the meaning of

    rank-order stability, researchers have been finding

    the same two findings since the earliest review of

    rank-order consistency (i.e., Crook, 1941). Personal-

    ity traits demonstrate modest to high rank-order

    consistency (e.g, correlations between .4 and .6)

    over reasonably long periods of time (e.g., 4 to 10

    years) and the longer one tracks rank-order consis-

    tency, the lower it gets (e.g., Fraley & Roberts, 2005),

    with the evidence zeroing in on a long-term level of

    about .2 over 40 years. Five meta-analyses on the

    topic have come to similar conclusions (Ardelt,

    2000; Bazana & Stelmack, 2004; Ferguson, 2010;

    Roberts & DelVecchio, 2000; Schuerger, Zarrella, &

    Hotz, 1989).

    The findings of these meta-analyses on the rank-

    order consistency of personality traits provide a

    very clear picture. Across hundreds of studies, test-

    retest correlations over time for personality are

    moderate in magnitude, even from childhood to

    early adulthood. Furthermore, rank-order consis-

    tency increases as people age and then reaches a

    plateau around .70 between ages 50 and 70. Per-

    sonality traits demonstrate a clear pattern of in-

    creasing continuity across the life course.

    In terms of the overall levels of test-retest consis-

    tency, personality psychologists can find solace in

    the fact that the magnitude of rank-order consis-

    tency, although not high enough to argue for ab-

    solute stability, is still remarkably high. The only psy-

    chological constructs more consistent than person-

    ality traits are measures of cognitive ability (Conley,

    1984; Schuerger, Tait, & Tavernelli, 1982) and voca-

    tional interests are just about equal in their consis-

    tency (Low, Yoon, Roberts, & Rounds, 2005). Sec-

    ond, the level of continuity in childhood and ado-

    lescence is much higher than originally expected

    especially after age three. Although childhood char-

    acter is by no means fate, there are striking conti-

    2 20 1

  • nuities that point to the importance of childhood

    temperament and the effects of cumulative conti-

    nuity from childhood through adulthood (Moffitt,

    Arseneault, Belsky, Dickson, Hancox, Harrington, &

    Caspi, 2011).

    One of the most conspicuous aspects of the in-

    crease in rank-order consistency is the fact that it is

    linear. Given developmental depictions of adoles-

    cence and young adulthood, one would expect to

    see marked decreases in rank-order consistency in

    these age periods, especially if environmental

    changes result in a drastic reconstruction of ones

    personality. For example, adolescence is often char-

    acterized as a time of psychological, if not social tu-

    mult (Conger & Donnellan, 2007). If dynamic and

    difficult life experiences translate one-to-one into

    personality trait change, then we would expect a

    marked decrease in continuity during these periods

    of the life course. Despite these dramatic psycho-

    logical and demographic shifts, personality consis-

    tency marches in a linear fashion towards its peak

    from childhood through old age. This implies that

    the relationship between life experiences and per-

    sonality trait development is neither simple nor di-

    rect.

    Mean-Level Changes in

    Personality Traits

    Complementing the data showing an increase in

    continuity is the finding that personality traits show

    reliable mean-level changes well into adulthood.

    Specifically, cross-sectional research has shown that

    middle-aged individuals tend to score higher than

    young adults on agreeableness and conscientious-

    ness and lower on extraversion, neuroticism, and

    openness (Soto et al., 2011; Srivastava et al., 2003).

    Moreover, within middle age, 60-year old partici-

    pants scored higher than 40-year old participants

    on most dimensions. The fact that these findings

    continue to be replicated across several decades

    (e.g., Soto et al., 2011) provides strong evidence

    against the argument that age differences in per-

    sonality traits are due to cohort effects.

    Longitudinal studies, mostly of western cultures,

    have found strikingly similar results. Changes in

    mean-levels of personality traits were examined in a

    meta-analysis of 92 longitudinal studies covering

    the life course from age 10 to 101 (Roberts, Walton,

    et al., 2006). Like the cross-sectional studies, signifi-

    cant mean-level change in all trait domains was

    found at some point in the life course and statisti-

    cally significant change in 75% of personality traits

    in middle (age 40 to 60) and old age (age 60).

    Clearly, personality traits continue to develop in

    adulthood.

    Several important conclusions about personality

    development can be drawn from the studies of

    mean-level change in personality traits. First, most

    mean-level personality trait change occurs between

    the ages of 20 and 40. This contradicts the widely

    held perspective that the most interesting years for

    studying personality development are either early

    or late in life. In contrast, young adulthood appears

    to be the most important period for mean-level per-

    sonality trait change. We believe that this finding

    opens a new area of focus in developmental sci-

    ence as the causes and mechanisms responsible for

    personality trait change in young adulthood have

    received little empirical or theoretical attention.

    Second, personality traits continue to change,

    even in old age. One of the precepts of a life span

    orientation is that humans are open systems (e.g.,

    Baltes & Nesselroade, 1973). That is, people retain

    the capacity to change at all ages. The changes in

    personality traits in middle and old age are by no

    means dramatic, but nonetheless they show that the

    3Personality Trait Development in Adulthood

  • life span orientation applies to personality traits and

    that personality is not set like plaster at any point in

    the life course.

    Third, time has a positive effect on personality

    trait change. Studies that follow people for a longer

    period of time show larger mean-level changes

    (Roberts, Walton, et al., 2006). The positive associa-

    tion between time and mean-level change is impor-

    tant for theoretical models of human nature. A com-

    mon assumption is that personality traits act like

    metabolic set points. People may stray briefly from

    their biological propensity, but they will then tend

    to drift back to their genetically driven set point

    (see Kandler, Bleidorn, Riemann, Spinath, Thiel, &

    Angleitner, 2010 for a review). Under these types of

    models, one would expect to find a negative or null

    association between time and mean-level change

    because any change will represent short-term fluc-

    tuations that disappear as people return to their bi-

    ologically driven set point. However, time is posi-

    tively associated with personality trait change,

    which indicates that a strong set-point model does

    not apply to personality trait development. That is,

    when people change they tend to retain the

    changes in personality traits for the remainder of

    their lives.

    Fourth, the direction of change is clearly in the

    positive direction. People become more confident,

    warm, responsible, and calm with age, or what

    some have described as socially mature (Roberts &

    Wood, 2006). Social maturity is equated with the ca-

    pacity to become a productive and involved con-

    tributor to society. Accordingly, most people do be-

    come more socially mature with age, and those

    who develop the cardinal traits of psychological

    maturity earliest are more effective in their relation-

    ships and work, and lead healthier and longer lives

    (Roberts, Kuncel, Shiner, Caspi, & Goldberg, 2007).

    The recent research on mean-level personality

    trait change has mapped out the normative trends

    in personality trait development and has led to sev-

    eral surprises. Personality traits change more in

    young adulthood than another other period of the

    life course and continue to change in old age.

    Moreover, most of the change is positive. These find-

    ings motivate a new generation of questions con-

    cerning why personality traits change more in

    young adulthood than other periods of the life

    course, and what the implications of the mostly pos-

    itive trend in personality trait change might be. Of

    course, one of the realities of any sweeping gener-

    alization is that it does not apply to all people

    much of this research needs to be replicated in

    non-Western cultures before firm conclusions are

    drawn. Moreover, some people fail to conform to

    the general trends by either not changing at all,

    being more accelerated in their change patterns

    with time, or changing in ways that contradict nor-

    mative trends. These deviations are captured with

    the concept of individual differences in personality

    trait change.

    Individual Differences in Personality

    Trait Change in Adulthood

    The concept of individual differences in change,

    a major tenet of lifespan developmental theory

    (Baltes & Nesselroade, 1973), refers to the gains or

    losses (or lack of) in absolute levels of a personal-

    ity trait that each individual experiences over time.

    These are changes that deviate from the population

    mean-level pattern of change. This perspective

    holds that personality change (and stability) is an

    individual differences variable and a complete un-

    derstanding of personality development is only pos-

    sible if individual differences in personality trait

    change are examined alongside more traditional in-

    4 20 1

  • dices of development.

    The key empirical hurdle that needs to be ad-

    dressed is whether individual differences in change

    are real or simply represent error in measurement

    (Watson, 2004). This has drawn many personality

    development researchers toward techniques to

    gauge the amount and pattern of change over time,

    such as the Reliable Change Index (Roberts, Caspi,

    & Moffitt, 2001) or the variance in slopes from

    growth models (Allemand, Gomez, & Jackson,

    2010). Using these techniques, numerous studies

    have established that the Big Five traits show un-

    mistakable variability across individuals in direction

    and rate of change.

    Once the existence of reliable individual differ-

    ences in personality trait change has been estab-

    lished, the compelling question becomes why these

    changes occur. A number of studies have shown

    that life experiences are associated with changes in

    personality traits (for review see Roberts, et al.,

    2008). For example, people who experience more

    successful and satisfying careers in young adult-

    hood increase disproportionately on measures of

    emotional stability and conscientiousness (Roberts,

    Caspi, & Moffitt, 2003). Similarly, initiating and stay-

    ing in a committed relationship in young adulthood

    his associated with increases in conscientiousness

    and decreases in neuroticism (Robins, Caspi, &

    Moffitt, 2002; Lehnart, Neyer, & Eccles, 2010). Fur-

    thermore, men who get remarried in middle age

    show decreases in neuroticism (Mroczek & Spiro,

    2003).

    Not all experiences are for the better. People who

    conduct problematic, counterproductive activities

    at work, such as theft, aggression, and malingering

    are prone to decrease on measures of conscien-

    tiousness and emotional stability (Roberts, Bogg,

    Walton, & Caspi, 2006). Similarly, people who con-

    tinue to abuse drugs and alcohol tend to decrease

    in conscientiousness and neuroticism in young

    adulthood (see Littlefield, Sher, & Wood, 2010). In-

    terestingly, seeing a psychotherapist is associated

    with increases in neuroticism in college students

    whether or not the experience is a good one

    (Ldtke, Roberts, Trautwein, & Nagy, in press).

    The irony of individual differences in change is

    that many individual difference researchers fail to

    appreciate their existence. Their meaning is rather

    profound for individual development, however, as

    they indicate that many people fail to follow the

    normative path of development. Moreover, follow-

    ing a non-normative path appears to be conse-

    quential as it might result in personality changes, or

    the lack thereof, that are not optimal.

    Theoretical Implications

    The first and most significant implication of the

    fact that personality traits are both relatively endur-

    ing and changing across the life course is the need

    to revise our most prominent models of personality

    and personality traits. For a variety of reasons, most

    theoretical perspectives of personality traits fall into

    one of two extreme camps (Roberts, 2009). On one

    side is the view that personality traits are biological,

    unchanging, and causal. This perspective is mani-

    fest in some theoretical systems explicitly (McCrae

    & Costa, 2008). It is also manifest implicitly in most

    research utilizing personality traits, as most re-

    search invoking personality assumes that traits

    cause outcomes and therefore are used solely as

    predictors. Based on these assumptions, personality

    traits need not be assessed more than once be-

    cause they do not change. Alternatively, some theo-

    reticians minimize the significance and consistency

    of personality traits, typically by overemphasizing

    the state level of analysis (Mischel, 2009). Those

    5Personality Trait Development in Adulthood

  • sympathetic to this perspective often take the fact

    that personality traits change as support for a com-

    pletely contextual model of personality traits and

    personality in general.

    Of course, reality is uncomfortably different than

    either of these overly simplistic perspectives. Per-

    sonality traits are, in fact, quite consistent over time,

    but not absolutely consistent. Personality traits also

    change with time, but are not a buzz of confusion

    bouncing to and fro depending on the environ-

    mental winds of change. Rather, what emerges from

    the data is a unique perspective that combines op-

    posing views (Roberts, 2009). Personality traits are

    consistent, and consistent enough to be considered

    causal forces. Personality traits also change and

    change enough to consider change itself a phe-

    nomenon of interest and key feature of human

    functioning. Personality trait change, especially that

    which occurs in middle and old age invites some

    challenging questions. Why would human beings

    remain open systems in which modifications are ca-

    pable of being made late in life? Most developmen-

    tal models assume that childhood is the critical

    stage of personality development and that little of

    interest happens thereafter. However, the majority

    of personality trait change appears to occur in

    adulthood, which begs the question of why?

    Practical Implications

    One of the unique aspects of the study of indi-

    vidual differences in change is that personality

    traits are considered dependent variables rather

    than solely independent variables, as they are typi-

    cally viewed. For example, in studies like Roberts

    (1997), personality traits are seen as the conse-

    quence of work experiences. One reason to con-

    sider personality traits as dependent variables is

    that personality trait change may be quite conse-

    quential for people. Mroczek and Spiro (2007)

    demonstrated that long-term increases in neuroti-

    cism were predictive of mortality in an 18-year sur-

    vival analysis. Those who started high on neuroti-

    cism (above the sample median) and increased

    over 10 years had higher mortality, controlling for

    age, depression, and physical health.

    In our ongoing research, we have found similar

    patterns when linking personality to health. Taka-

    hashi, Edmonds, Jackson, and Roberts (2011) ex-

    amined whether and how initial levels and changes

    in conscientiousness are linked to initial levels and

    changes in both preventative health-related behav-

    iors and self-rated physical health, using latent

    change models (Hertzog & Nesselroade, 2003;

    McArdle & Nesselroade, 1994). Our two-wave longi-

    tudinal study extended previous research on con-

    scientiousness and physical health by exploring re-

    lations between latent variables of continuity and

    change over a 3-year period. This study provided ev-

    idence that people who increased in conscien-

    tiousness also increased in their use of preventative

    health-related behaviors, and in turn, became in-

    creasingly healthy. Thus, personality trait change

    may be an important health issue above and be-

    yond how people stand on a trait at the beginning

    of a longitudinal study.

    Can Personality Traits Be Changed?

    The fact that personality traits change and that

    the change in personality traits may bring about

    positive outcomes, such as greater success in work

    and better health and longevity, leads to the next

    question, which is can personality traits be

    changed? Surprisingly, the answer appears to be yes

    and has been known in some circles for quite some

    time. Specifically, in their profoundly important

    meta-analysis on the efficacy of psychotherapy,

    6 20 1

  • Smith, Glass, and Miller (1980) also documented

    the fact that therapy affected change in personality

    traits in addition to many other outcomes. In fact,

    cognitive-behavioral therapy changed measures of

    personality traits more than a standard deviation.

    Unfortunately, the existence of this empirical proof

    of the changeability of personality traits has gone

    largely unnoticed.

    In the interim there has been a smattering of

    studies reporting on the changeability of personal-

    ity traits through direct intervention across a num-

    ber of domains. For example, after a 20-week cog-

    nitive behavior therapy intervention aimed to treat

    depression, patients changed on a number of per-

    sonality traits, most notably in extraversion and neu-

    roticism (Clark, Vittengl, Kraft, & Jarrett, 2003). Evi-

    dence also exists that personality traits change in re-

    sponsive to a combination of therapy and medica-

    tion (Santor, Bagby, & Joffe, 1997). For example,

    De Fruyt, Van Leeuwen, Bagby, Rolland, and Rouil-

    lon (2006) found that individuals treated with a

    combination of either tianeptine or fluoxetine (the

    active drug in Prozac) and therapy showed signifi-

    cant positive increase in all Big Five personality

    traits. Similarly, a recent study also found that both

    cognitive therapy and medication (SSRIs) were as-

    sociated with changes in neuroticism and extraver-

    sion compared to a control group (Tang, DeRubeis,

    Hollon, Amsterdam, Shelton, & Schalet, 2009).

    In addition to clinical interventions, a number of

    other types of intervention studies demonstrate that

    personality traits are amenable to change. Training

    programs, where the participant learns some type

    of skill, appear to be an especially effective in

    changing personality traits. For example, a recent

    intervention trained medical students to become

    more mindful. The mindfulness intervention re-

    sulted in personality trait changes in the traits of

    conscientiousness, agreeableness, empathy, and

    emotional stability (Krasner, Epstein, Beckman,

    Suchman, Chapman, Mooney, & Quill, 2009). Simi-

    larly, a social skill training programs for recovering

    substance abusers led to increases in agreeable-

    ness, conscientiousness and emotional stability

    (Piedmont, 2001). Moreover, a cognitive training in-

    tervention for older adults was also associated with

    changes in a personality trait. Across 16-weeks elder

    adults learned inductive reasoning skills and com-

    pleted 10 hours a week of crossword and Sudoku

    puzzles. Compared to a control condition, the in-

    tervention increased participants levels of open-

    ness to experience (Jackson, Hill, Payne, Roberts, &

    Stine-Morrow, in press).

    Based on these studies it is clear that personality

    traits can be changed through a variety of interven-

    tions over a relatively short period of time. These

    findings raise several issues. First, as none of these

    studies was embedded in an ongoing longitudinal

    study, it is unclear whether the changes actually

    stick. It is quite possible that people change for the

    better in the short run only to regress back to where

    they were at earlier times in the absence of some

    active intervention. On the other hand, if personal-

    ity traits can be changed and the change remains, it

    generates some interesting questions. For example,

    most interventions focus on proximal thoughts, feel-

    ings, attitudes, and behaviors that are linked to out-

    comes deemed important by societies. Studies in-

    tervening to increase the efficacy for specific out-

    comes, such as achievement and health behaviors

    are legion. Unfortunately, specific attitudes and ex-

    pectancies tend to either not generalize or to be

    negatively related to separate domains. For exam-

    ple, in the education domain, efficacy in a topic

    such as mathematics is often negatively correlated

    with efficacy in language arts (Trautwein, Ldtke,

    7Personality Trait Development in Adulthood

  • Roberts, Schnyder, & Niggli, 2009). In the case of

    educational achievement, conscientiousness is pos-

    itively related to effort and achievement across do-

    mains (Trautwein et al., 2009). Therefore, interven-

    ing to increase conscientiousness makes more

    sense than intervening to change domain-specific

    attitudes because it could result in positive effects

    across domains and not adversely affect other im-

    portant outcomes.

    Summary

    In the first portion of this paper, focusing on per-

    sonality development in adulthood, we reviewed

    the evidence showing both the continuity and

    change in personality over time. With respect to the

    rank-order stability, as Roberts and DelVecchio

    (2000) found personality stability increases with

    age. On the other hand, (Roberts, Walton et al.

    2006) found that personality traits show mean-level

    changes. The significant point to be stressed here is

    that personality is stable and changeable at the

    same time. In the second portion of this paper, we

    placed an emphasis on discussing individual differ-

    ences in personality change, because the knowl-

    edge about personality change is relatively sparse.

    We introduced that personality changes are pre-

    dictable for individuals health and health behav-

    iors (Takahashi et al., 2011), and reviewed that per-

    sonality traits can be changed by receiving medica-

    tion, psychological therapy, and cognitive interven-

    tion. With these in mind, we need to move on the

    next steps on personality research. We believe one

    possible future direction of personality psychology

    is to explore the pragmatic questions raised by the

    existence of personality change. For example, how

    can personality trait change be used to promote

    better health and economic outcomes; are person-

    ality traits appropriate candidates as an epidemio-

    logical risk factor that predicts the development of

    physical diseases (Roberts, 2009), and how we

    should develop our personality traits to live as a

    sound members of human society (Heckman,

    2007).

    References

    Allemand, M., Gomez, V., & Jackson, J. J. (2010). Personal-

    ity trait development in midlife: Exploring the impact of

    psychological turning points. European Journal of Age-

    ing, 7, 147155.

    Ardelt, M. (2000). Still stable after all these years? Person-

    ality stability theory revisited. Social Psychology Quar-

    terly, 63, 392405.

    Baltes, P. B., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1973). The developmen-

    tal analysis of individual differences on multiple meas-

    ures. In J. R. Nesselroade & H. W. Reese (Eds.), Life-span

    developmental psychology: Methodological issues. New

    York: Academic Press. pp. 219251.

    Bazana, P. G., & Stelmack, R. M. (2004). Stability of per-

    sonality across the life span: A meta-analysis. In R. M.

    Stelmack (Ed.), On the Psychobiology of Personality. Ox-

    ford: UK: Elsevier. pp. 113144.

    Clark, L. A., Vittengl, J., Kraft, D., & Jarrett, R. J. (2003). Sep-

    arate personality traits from states to predict depression.

    Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 152172.

    Conley, J. J. (1984). Longitudinal consistency of adult per-

    sonality: Self-reported psychological characteristics

    across 45 years. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-

    chology, 47, 13251333.

    Conger, R. D., & Donnellan, M. B. (2007). An interactionist

    perspective on the socioeconomic context of human

    development. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 175

    199.

    Crook, M. N. (1941). Retest correlations in neuroticism.

    Journal of General Psychology, 24, 173182.

    De Fruyt, F., Van Leeuwen, K., Bagby, R. M., Rolland, J. P., &

    Rouillon, F. (2006). Assessing and interpreting personal-

    ity change and continuity in patients treated for major

    depression. Psychological Assessment, 1188, 7180.

    Ferguson, C. J. (2010). A meta-analysis of normal and dis-

    ordered personality across the life span. Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 659667.

    Fraley, C., & Roberts, B. W. (2005). Patterns of continuity: A

    8 20 1

  • dynamic model for conceptualizing the stability of indi-

    vidual differences in psychological constructs across

    the life course. Psychological Review, 112, 6074.

    Heckman, J. J. (2007). The economics, technology, and

    neuroscience of human capability formation. Proceed-

    ings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 13250

    13255.

    Hertzog, C., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2003). Assessing psycho-

    logical change in adulthood: An overview of method-

    ological issues. Psychology and Aging, 18, 639657.

    Jackson, J. J., Hill, P. L., Payne, B. R., Roberts, B. W., & Stine-

    Morrow, E. A. L. (in press). Can an old dog learn (and

    want to experience) new tricks? Cognitive training in-

    creases openness to experience in older adults. Psy-

    chology and Aging.

    Kandler, C., Bleidorn, W., Riemann, R., Spinath, F. M.,

    Thiel, W., & Angleitner, A. (2010). Sources of cumulative

    continuity in personality: A longitudinal multiple-rater

    twin study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-

    ogy, 98, 9951008.

    Krasner, M. S., Epstein, R. M., Beckman, H., Suchman, A.

    L., Chapman, B., Mooney, C. J., & Quill, T. E. (2009). As-

    sociation of an educational program in mindful com-

    munication with burnout, empathy, and attitudes

    among primary care physicians. Journal of the Ameri-

    can Medical Association, 302, 12841293.

    Lehnart, J., Neyer, F. J., & Eccles, J. (2010). Long-term ef-

    fects of social investment: The case of partnering in

    young adulthood. Journal of Personality, 78, 639670.

    Littlefield, A. K., Sher, K. J., & Wood, P. K. (2010). A per-

    sonality-based description of maturing out of alcohol

    problems: Extension with a Five-Factor Model and ro-

    bustness to modeling challenges. Addictive Behaviors,

    35, 948954.

    Low, D. K. S., Yoon, M., Roberts, B. W., & Rounds, J. (2005).

    The stability of interests from early adolescence to mid-

    dle adulthood: A quantitative review of longitudinal

    studies. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 713737.

    Ldtke, O., Roberts, B. W., Trautwein, U., & Nagy, G. (in

    press). A random walk down university avenue: Life

    paths, life events, and personality trait change at the

    transition to university life. Journal of Personality and

    Social Psychology.

    McArdle, J. J., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1994). Using multivari-

    ate data to structure developmental change. In H. W.

    Reese & S. H. Cohen (Eds.), Lifespan developmental psy-

    chology: Methodological contributions. Hillsdale, NJ: Erl-

    baum. pp. 223267.

    McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (2008). The five-factor the-

    ory of personality. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Per-

    vin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and re-

    search. 3rd ed. New York: Guilford. pp. 157180.

    Mischel, W. (2009). From Personality and Assessment

    (1968) to Personality Science, 2009. Journal of Re-

    search in Personality, 43, 282290.

    Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Han-

    cox, R. J., Harrington, H., & Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient

    of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and

    public safety, Proceedings of the National Academy of

    Sciences, 108, 26932698.

    Mroczek, D. K., & Spiro, A. (2003). Modeling intraindivid-

    ual change in personality traits: Findings from the Nor-

    mative Aging Study. Journals of Gerontology: Psycho-

    logical Sciences, 58B, 153165.

    Mroczek, D. K., & Spiro, A. (2007). Personality change in-

    fluences mortality in older men. Psychological Sci-

    ence, 18, 371376.

    Piedmont, R. L. (2001). Cracking the plaster cast: Big five

    personality change during intensive outpatient counsel-

    ing. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 500520.

    Roberts, B. W. (1997). Plaster or plasticity: Are work exper-

    iences associated with personality change in women?

    Journal of Personality, 65, 205232.

    Roberts, B. W. (2009). Back to the future: Personality and

    assessment and personality development. Journal of

    Research in Personality, 43, 137145.

    Roberts, B. W., Bogg, T., Walton, K., & Caspi, A. (2006). De-

    investment in Work and Non-normative Personality Trait

    Change in Young Adulthood. European Journal of Per-

    sonality, 20, 461474.

    Roberts, B. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. (2001). The kids are

    alright: Growth and stability in personality development

    from adolescence to adulthood. Journal of Personality

    and Social Psychology, 81, 670683.

    Roberts, B. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. (2003). Work experi-

    ences and personality development in young adult-

    hood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84,

    582593.

    Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order

    consistency of personality from childhood to old age: A

    quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychologi-

    cal Bulletin, 126, 325.

    9Personality Trait Development in Adulthood

  • Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N., Shiner, R. N., Caspi, A., & Gold-

    berg, L. (2007). The power of personality: A compara-

    tive analysis of the predictive validity of personality

    traits, SES, and IQ. Perspectives in Psychological Sci-

    ence, 2, 313345.

    Roberts, B. W., Walton, K., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Pat-

    terns of mean-level change in personality traits across

    the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies.

    Psychological Bulletin, 132, 125.

    Roberts, B. W., & Wood, D. (2006). Personality develop-

    ment in the context of the Neo-Socioanalytic Model of

    personality. In D. Mroczek & T. Little (Eds.), Handbook

    of personality development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrance Erl-

    baum Associates. pp. 1139.

    Roberts, B. W., Wood, D., & Caspi, A. (2008). Personality

    Development. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin

    (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research.

    3rd ed. New York, NY: Guilford. pp. 375398.

    Robins, R. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. (2002). Its not just

    who youre with, its who you are: Personality and rela-

    tionship experiences across multiple relationships.

    Journal of Personality, 70, 925964.

    Santor, D. A., Bagby, R. M., & Joffe, R. T. (1997). Evaluating

    stability and change in personality and depression.

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73,

    13541362.

    Schuerger, J. M., Tait, E., & Tavernelli, M. (1982). Temporal

    stability of personality by questionnaire. Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 176182.

    Schuerger, J. M., Zarrella, K. L., & Hotz, A. S. (1989). Fac-

    tors that influence the temporal stability of personality

    by questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-

    chology, 56, 777783.

    Smith, M. L., Glass, G. V., & Miller, T. I. (1980). The benefits

    of psychotherapy. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University

    Press.

    Soto, S. J., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2011). Age

    differences in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big five

    domains and facets in a large cross-sectional sample.

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100,

    330348.

    Srivastava, S., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2003).

    Development of personality in early and middle adult-

    hood: Set like plaster or persistent change? Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 10411053.

    Takahashi, Y., Edmonds, G. W., Jackson, J. J., & Roberts, B.

    W. (2011). Longitudinal correlated changes in consci-

    entiousness, preventative health-related behaviors, and

    physical health. Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Tang, T. Z., DeRubeis, R. J., Hollon, S. D., Amsterdam, J.,

    Shelton, R., & Schalet, B. (2009). Personality change

    during depression treatment: A placebo-controlled trial.

    Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 13221330.

    Trautwein, U., Ldtke, O., Roberts, B. W., Schnyder, I., &

    Niggli, A. (2009). Different forces, same consequence:

    Conscientiousness and competence beliefs are inde-

    pendent predictors of academic effort and achieve-

    ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97,

    11151128.

    Watson, D. (2004). Stability versus change, dependability

    versus error: Issues in the assessment of personality over

    time. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 319350.

    2011.4.282011.5.13

    10 20 1

  • Copyright of Japanese Journal of Personality is the property of Japan Society of PersonalityPsychology and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to alistserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print,download, or email articles for individual use.