4 Mental Determ

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/13/2019 4 Mental Determ

    1/13

    3

    Early determinants of mental health

    Katri Rikknen, Ph.D., Professor a,*,Anu-Katriina Pesonen, Ph.D., Senior Lecturer a,

    Tessa J. Roseboom, Ph.D., Associate Professorb

    ,Johan G. Eriksson, M.D., Ph.D., Professor, Chief Physician c,d,e,f,g

    a Department of Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlandsb Institute of Behavioral Sciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki 00014, Finlandc Department of General Practice and Primary Health Care, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finlandd Helsinki University Central Hospital, Unit of General Practice, Helsinki, Finlande Department of Chronic Disease Prevention, National Institute for Health and Welfare, Helsinki, FinlandfFolkhlsan Research Center, Helsinki, Finlandg Vaasa Central Hospital, Vaasa, Finland

    Keywords:

    behavior

    cognition

    emotion

    lifespan

    mental

    postnatal

    prenatal

    programming

    Environmental adversities in pre- and early postnatal life may havelife-long consequences. Based upon a series of epidemiological and

    clinical studies and natural experiments, this review describes how

    the early life environment may affect psychological functions and

    mental disorders later in life. We focus on studies that have exam-

    ined the associations of small body size at birth and prematurity as

    proxies of prenatal environmental adversity. We also review liter-

    ature on materno-fetal malnutrition, maternal prenatal glycyrrhizin

    in licorice consumption and hypertension-spectrum pregnancy

    disorders as factors that may compromise the fetal developmental

    milieu and hence provide insight into some of the mechanisms that

    may underlie prenatal programming. While effects of programming

    mostly take place during the rst 1000 days after conception, wenally present evidence from prospective studies suggesting that

    programming can occur also during later critical periods of devel-

    opment or windows of plasticity. The studies may bear relevance

    for future prevention and intervention programs targeting the

    potentially modiable environmental factors that will aid at

    promoting mental well-being and health of an individual.

    2012 Published by Elsevier Ltd.

    * Corresponding author. Institute of Behavioral Sciences, University of Helsinki, P.O. Box 9 (Siltavuorenpenger 1a), 00014

    Helsinki, Finland.E-mail address: katri.raikkonen@helsinki. (K. Rikknen).

    Contents lists available atSciVerse ScienceDirect

    Best Practice & Research Clinical

    Endocrinology & Metabolismj o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / lo c a t e / b e e m

    1521-690X/$ see front matter 2012 Published by Elsevier Ltd.

    doi:10.1016/j.beem.2012.03.001

    Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 26 (2012) 599611

    mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/1521690Xhttp://www.elsevier.com/locate/beemhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beem.2012.03.001http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beem.2012.03.001http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beem.2012.03.001http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beem.2012.03.001http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beem.2012.03.001http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beem.2012.03.001http://www.elsevier.com/locate/beemhttp://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/1521690Xmailto:[email protected]
  • 8/13/2019 4 Mental Determ

    2/13

    Introduction

    Early life environmental adversities may exert consequences upon the developing cells, tissues,

    organs, their structure and function. The long-term consequences of these early adversities are thought

    to be mediated by epigenetic mechanisms, resulting in phenotypic differences between individuals.

    These phenotypic differences may persist throughout the lifespan. This phenomenon related to earlylife developmental plasticity has been labeled programming.13

    It appears axiomatic that during pre- and early postnatal periods the brain is particularly suscep-

    tible to environmental adversities. This presumably reects the sensitivity of the brain during periods

    when it undergoes major developmental changes. The sensitive periods of the brain begin during the

    embryonic state, and continue throughout the prenatal period.4 These reach a peak in the rst years of

    life, and continue throughout childhood and adolescence, such that the brain does not reach its adult

    state until the end of the second or the beginning of the third decade of life.4

    Environmental adversities may, however, lead to different outcomes depending on the timing of

    exposure. According to the life cycle model of stress adversities occurring at different stages in life

    depend on areas of the brain that are undergoing change either developing/maturing or declining at

    the time of exposure.4 The stress physiological systems, including the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis, autonomic nervous system (ANS), and immune-inammatory system,

    that are all implicated in mental health, may be particularly susceptible to prenatal environmental

    adversity.5 This is because the hippocampus, the frontal cortex and the amygdala, which are important

    in regulation of the physiological stress response, are under rapid development during this period. 4

    Postnatal adversities may result in more widespread effects.4 Yet, some areas of the brain may be

    more sensitive during specic periods: in infancy, the hippocampus is developing most rapidly, from

    childhood to adolescence the amygdala undergoes rapid development, and from adolescence onwards

    the frontal volume is increasing signicantly. Thus, different brain areas might be more vulnerable to

    environmental adversities at different time periods, and therefore an adversity might result in different

    long-term outcomes. While the timing of exposure to environmental adversity is emphasized in the life

    cycle model of stress, the characteristics of the environmental adversity appears to be important aswell.4 For instance, the HPA axis seems to be hyper-responsive in individuals exposed to separation

    from both biological parents in early childhood,6 while severe abuse may result in HPA axis hypo-

    responsiveness.7 Thus, the physiological stress systems may differentiate as a function of the type of

    early environmental adversity. It is also possible that adversity at a specic sensitive period may result

    in different effects during the life cycle. For instance, low birth weight, a proxy of prenatal environ-

    mental adversity, is according to some studies associated with a hyper-responsive HPA axis in chil-

    dren,8 and a hypo-responsive HPA axis in older adults.9

    This review describes evidence for the effects of early environmental adversity on mental health

    using data from human studies. Mental health is considered broadly not restricted to describing

    mental disorders. We use the concept of mental health to cover a full range of different human

    functions ranging from cognition, behaviour, temperament and personality to a range of mentaldisorders differing in symptomatology and severity. The rationale behind this is that there is evidence

    that poorer cognitive function, behavioural problems, negative affectivity-related temperament and

    personality characteristics may compromise mental well-being and health, and co-occur with and

    increase vulnerability of mental disorders. These endophenotypesof mental disorders have also been

    repeatedly associated with altered HPA axis and ANS function.10

    First, in this review, we describe the work conducted on pre- and postnatal growth and prema-

    turity, considered as crude proxies of prenatal and early postnatal environmental adversity. Second,

    we focus on three prenatal environmental adversities that may compromise the fetal developmental

    milieu and, thus, underlie the associations of fetal growth and prematurity with mental health, namely

    materno-fetal malnutrition, overexposure to glucocorticoids by maternal prenatal consumption of

    glycyrrhizin in licorice confectionery, and hypertension-spectrum pregnancy disorders. We will notpresent evidence on prenatal stress, as this work has been recently thoroughly described in reviews by

    Glover,11 Van Den Bergh,12 and Rikknen.13 Finally, we describe the work from prospective studies

    that have tested the long-term mental health consequences of exposure to severe early life stress

    (ELS).

    K. Rikknen et al. / Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 26 (2012) 599611600

  • 8/13/2019 4 Mental Determ

    3/13

    Pre- and postnatal growth and mental health

    Findings in epidemiological studies from different parts of the world, including the Helsinki Birth

    Cohort Study (HBCS),1416 suggest that slower growth during fetal life and in early childhood appear to

    extend widespread consequences on mental health throughout the lifespan.

    Cognitive function

    Smaller weight, length, thinness and/or head circumference at birth, adjusted for length of gesta-

    tion, have been shown to be associated with poorer cognitive performance in childhood, adolescence

    and young adulthood.17,18 While there are studies suggesting that the effects of birth size do not persist

    into old age19,20 or beyond the age of 40 years,21 recent data derived from the HBCS suggest that

    smaller body size at birth extends effects to old age and may predict a more rapid age-related cognitive

    decline.22 The associations with poorer cognitive performance are not restricted to the prenatal period,

    but evidence suggests that those with poorer performance continue to grow slower also during

    childhood and adolescence.14,23 The slower postnatal growth may, however, pertain specically to

    height.14

    Temperament and personality

    Evidence also suggests that smaller body size at birth, adjusted for length of gestation, is related to

    infant and child temperamental characteristics. At the age of 6 months infants with a mild growth

    restriction (dened as a birth weight below 10th percentile for gestational age) were perceived by their

    biological parents as more fearful and negatively reactive.24 Those born smaller continued to be

    perceived by their mothers as more negatively affective, and showing more anger, discomfort, and

    sadness-proneness at the age of 5 years.25 Similar ndings have also been reported elsewhere.26

    Findings from the Generation R study, however, disagree with these ndings by showing that the

    associations between birth weight and temperament were attenuated to non-signicance when

    adjusting for maternal height, age, educational level, and national origin.27 While the sample size of the

    Generation R study outweighs that of the other previous studies, the sample is highly heterogeneous innational/cultural origin, and therefore the ndings may not directly compare with those of the earlier

    reports. Some evidence also suggests that birth weight may extend effects on temperament that

    persists over decades.28 Weight and length at birth had curvilinear, reverse J-shaped effects on harm

    avoidance temperament which was self-rated at the age of 63 years. Yet, the highest harm avoidance

    scores were most characteristic of those born the smallest.

    In addition to temperament, smaller body size at birth has also been linked with adult personality.

    Lower weight, thinness and smaller head circumference were associated with higher trait anxiety and

    hostility characteristics at the age of 63 years.29,30 Individuals with higher trait anxiety and hostility

    scores continued to be lighter and thinner throughout infancy, and from thereon put on weight more

    rapidly.

    Mental disorders

    In a series of studies focusing on sub-clinical symptoms of mental disorders, lower birth weight,

    shorter birth length, thinness and smaller head circumference at birth were associated with higher

    parent-rated ADHD symptomatology of the child at 56 months of age. Children with higher ADHD

    symptoms continued being thinner and had a smaller head circumference up to 56 months and gained

    less weight in relation to height between 20 and 56 months. Thinness and smaller head circumference

    at birth have predicted higher hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms of ADHD in another study of 5-

    year-old children.31 Lower birth weight has also been related to higher externalizing and internal-

    izing behaviour problems in 79-year-old children.32

    Of the sub-clinical symptoms of mental disorders in adults, depressive symptoms have gained most

    research attention. The existing studies have consistently demonstrated that lower birth weight isassociated with higher frequency and severity of experiencing depressive symptoms at different stages

    of adult life.3335 Though, some studies suggest that this effect may be conned to those who were born

    with a low birth weight (

  • 8/13/2019 4 Mental Determ

    4/13

    traits, such as anhedonia and aberrant perceptions in women at the age of 33 years. 36 Thendings on

    depressive symptoms and schizotypal traits suggest that prenatal growth may extend effects on mental

    health also at stages that may precede and precipitate disorders.

    A recent large register-based study using two inter-country population cohorts from Sweden and

    Denmark showed that low birth weight (

  • 8/13/2019 4 Mental Determ

    5/13

    depressive symptoms, use antidepressant medication more frequently, report a physician-diagnosed

    depressive episode more frequently,50 report more ADHD symptoms and more problems in

    attention.51

    Stress sensitivity

    We are aware of only one study that has compared HPA axis responses in subjects born pretermand/or VLBW with those born at term: a study in 812-year-olds with all degrees of prematurity found

    no differences in HPA axis responses.

    Prenatal exposure to famine and mental health

    Studies of populations born around the time of acute periods of famine have been used as a semi-

    experimental design to investigate the effects of prenatal undernutrition on mental health. Such

    studies started with the landmark studies of Stein and Susser performed in the early seventies

    following the increasing awareness in the late 1960s that early nutritional deprivation might cause

    irreversible damage to the brain.52 They studied the consequences of prenatal exposure to the Dutch

    famine of 19441945 which was a 5 month period of acute starvation that struck the Dutch pop-ulation in the West of the Netherlands during the last months of the World War II using records of

    military conscripts who had been born around the time of the Dutch famine. Since these early studies,

    there has been an increasing body of evidence of studies in different populations using various study

    designs that prenatal exposure to famine affects later mental health, specically the risk of schizo-

    phrenia, depression, addiction, stress-responsivity and possibly some aspects of cognitive function in

    later life. This suggests that undernutrition during critical periods of early brain development pro-

    grammes mental health.

    Although many characteristics of such studies indeed resemble experimental designs, an important

    limitation of such studies is that the effects of undernutrition and stress are hard to disentangle.

    However, most studies found no differences in mental health, cognition or stress responsiveness

    among people who were born before the famine and those conceived after the famine, whereas onewould expect large differences in stress levels experienced by these mothers, suggesting that lack of

    nutrients is an important factor in the early life programming of mental health.

    Cognitive function

    In their study of 19-year-old military conscripts Stein and Susser found no association between

    famine exposure and intelligence in young adulthood.52 But more recently, two studies again

    investigated effects of prenatal famine exposure on cognition, now in late adulthood. One study found

    no associations between famine exposure and later cognitive performance,53 but, in the other study,

    there were indications that maternal malnutrition during fetal life may negatively inuence some

    aspects of cognitive function in later life.54 Men and women who had been exposed to famine during

    early gestation performed worse on a selective attention task. In line with the other Dutch faminestudies, this study found no effects of prenatal famine exposure on general intelligence, nor on

    memory or perceptual motor learning. The decreased selective attention in those exposed to famine

    in early gestation might be an early manifestation of an accelerated aging process, since recent studies

    have shown that poor performance on such tasks is a strong predictor for conversion to Alzheimers

    disease even before memory decits are present.55 Also, the lower scores t with ndings of lower

    Stroop scores achieved by people suffering from schizophrenia which have been suggested to be

    caused by a dysfunction of the anterior cingulated gyrus, which may play an important role in

    selective attention56,57 White-matter lesions have also been associated with reduced Stroop task

    performance58,59 and increase with age, supporting the above-mentioned suggestion that the

    decreased Stroop performance in those exposed to famine in early gestation is because of an accel-

    erated aging process.An alternative explanation for the association between prenatal famine exposure and poorer

    selective attention may be vascular-related rather than neurodegenerative, which would t with the

    increased rates of coronary heart disease found in the cohort6062 Prenatal undernutrition may

    possibly lead to poorer selective attention performance via increased vascular damage. The reported

    K. Rikknen et al. / Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 26 (2012) 599611 603

  • 8/13/2019 4 Mental Determ

    6/13

    increase in white-matter lesions among those prenatally exposed to famine also t this explanation, as

    white-matter lesions are strongly related to cardiovascular risk factors and disease.63,64

    In addition to effects of prenatal famine exposure on selective attention, those exposed to famine

    prenatally had smaller head circumferences at the ages of 5659 years, which was related to decreased

    cognitive abilities.54 Whether this is associated with actual smaller brain size is currently being

    investigated.

    Stress sensitivity and personality

    People exposed to the Dutch famine in early gestation have been shown to have increased blood

    pressure responses to a psychosocial stress protocol, which suggests that stress appraisal may be

    higher in those prenatally exposed to famine,65 there was no evidence, however, that this was

    mediated by alterations in the HPA axis.39,66 The blood pressure response to stress, however, was found

    to be strongly linked to methylation patterns of the glucocorticoid receptor, suggesting epigenetic

    processes might play a role.67 There were subtle differences in personality according to timing of

    famine exposure in utero, that appeared to be sex-specic.68 Men exposed to famine during early

    gestation scored a little lower on conscientiousness and women exposed to famine during early

    gestation scored a little higher on agreeableness, but these differences did not survive a correction formultiple testing and otherwise there were no differences between those exposed and those unexposed

    to famine in utero in personality traits.68

    Mental disorders

    Stein and Susser initially reported an excess of congenital nervous system anomalies in individuals

    who had been conceived towards the end of the Dutch famine52 suggesting direct harmful effects of

    undernutrition on the developing nervous system. However, they found no evidence of effects on mental

    health (severe or mild mental retardation) nor on cognition in a large-scale study of military recruits.

    Later, it became clear that the congenital anomalies were mainly neural tube defects, and it seems likely

    that this was due to periconceptional folate deciency. In this context, Ezra Susser, Stein and Sussers son

    reanalyzed the original data and investigated whether there was also an excess of schizophrenia whichmany consider to be a neurodevelopmental disorder. He found that men and women who had been

    conceived during the Dutch famine had a twofold increased risk of schizophrenia.69 Subsequently, he

    found an excess of schizophrenia spectrum personality disorders among those conceived during

    famine.70 A small study suggested that the effects of famine exposure on later schizophrenia risk was

    associated with changes in brain morphology.Schizophrenic patients whohad beenexposed to faminein

    early gestation had decreased intracranial volume and increased white-matter hyperintensities

    compared to schizophrenic patients who had not been exposed to famine prenatally.63 These ndings of

    lasting effects of prenatal famineon later mental health are supported by large scalestudies performed in

    China investigating effects of prenatal exposure to the Great Leap Forward famine that followed Maos

    ambition to rapidly transform China from an agrarian economy into a modern communist society. The

    Great Leap Forward had devastating consequences for food availability, causing widespread famine andtens of millions of deaths due to undernutrition. People conceived around the time of the 19591961

    Great Leap Forward famine were also found to have doubled rates of schizophrenia71 The consistent

    results of these studies in different settings and populations strongly suggests that early prenatal famine

    is linked to an increase in the risk of schizophrenia in later life.

    Prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine has also been related to other psychiatric disorders in

    adulthood, including antisocial personality disorders,72 affective psychoses (many of which would

    today be classied as mood disorders but not psychoses),73 and depression, though not all studies

    replicated thisnding.67,73 Also, a casecontrol study found evidence that exposure to famine in early

    gestation was associated with an increased risk of addictive disorders in adult life,74 especially among

    men.

    Prenatal exposure to glycyrrhizin in licorice and mental health

    Glycyrrhizin (3b-D-diglucuronyl-18b-glycyrrhetinic or 3b-D-diglucuronyl-18b-glycyrrhizic acid) is

    a natural constituent of licorice. Its synthetic analogue, carbenoxolone, frequently used in animal

    K. Rikknen et al. / Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 26 (2012) 599611604

  • 8/13/2019 4 Mental Determ

    7/13

    models,75 is a well-known inhibitor of the placental 11-HSD2 function. Evidence from experimental

    studies on human placentas suggests that glycyrrhetinic acid inhibits 11-HSD2 function allowing

    a direct placental by-pass for maternal circulating cortisol to the fetus. 76 Evidence on the long-term

    effects of prenatal exposure to glycyrrhizin on the offspring mental health is, however, scarce. This

    is somewhat surprising, given that glycyrrhizin is found also in other foodstuffs apart from licorice

    confectionery.The existing evidence on the longer-term effects of prenatal glycyrrhizin exposures has shown that

    at the average age of eight years, those children who were exposed to higher levels glycyrrhizin as

    a consequence of maternal intake of licorice confectionery, dened as 500 mg/week representing

    the upper 10th percentile of the samples of 1049 pregnant women, performed poorer in cognitive

    tests measuring verbal and visuospatial reasoning and narrative memory than those children who

    were prenatally exposed to no or 299 mg/week.77 In addition, children in the high-exposure group

    had signicant increases in externalizing symptoms, attention, rule breaking, and aggression prob-

    lems.77 The effects on cognitive performance and externalizing symptoms were not restricted to

    children exposed to doses 500 mg/week, but the effects appeared dose-related. None of the asso-

    ciations were affected by birth weight or duration of gestation and other pre- and perinatal, maternal,

    and child characteristics implicated as risks for pregnancy and/or cognitive and psychiatricoutcomes.77

    The HPA axis activity of these children was also affected. Children of mothers consuming high levels

    of glycyrrhizin had a higher salivary cortisol peak and area under the curve upon awakening and higher

    overall salivary cortisol throughout the Trier Social Stress Test for Children.78 These ndings, thus,

    suggest that high maternal licorice consumption during pregnancy may exert deleterious effects upon

    cognitive and psychiatric outcomes and HPA axis functioning in children. The ndings do counsel

    caution in using glycyrrhizin containing foodstuffs during pregnancy.

    Hypertension-spectrum pregnancy disorders and mental health

    Hypertensive disorders in pregnancy, including chronic hypertension, gestational hypertension,and (pre)eclampsia, complicate approximately 1016% of all pregnancies.79,80 These disorders

    threaten the health and well-being of both the mother and the fetus, and are among the key

    underlying causes of prematurity and intrauterine growth restriction (IUGR). 81,82 Hypertension in

    pregnancy may induce fetal malnutrion and oxidative distress83,84 and activate similar sympathetic

    nervous system responses in the mother that are often seen under psychosocial stress. 85,86 Some

    evidence also suggests that placental 11-HSD2 deciency may be characteristic to pregnancies

    complicated by pre-eclampsia,87,88 and infants born with intrauterine growth restriction/small-for-

    gestational age.89 Thus, by compromising the prenatal developmental environment, hypertensive

    disorders in pregnancy may provide another experimental platform to study the mechanisms

    underlying prenatal programming.

    Studies that have tested associations between hypertension-spectrum pregnancy disorders andmental health outcomes of the offspring are few and the ndings are inconsistent. With regard to

    cognitive abilities, the existing evidence shows that hypertensive pregnancy disorders are not asso-

    ciated with cognitive abilities of the offspring,9093 some studies suggest that they are associated with

    poorer94101 and some that they are associated with even better cognitive abilities of the offspring.102

    One recent study demonstrated that the association between hypertensive pregnancy disorders and

    poorer cognitive abilities in young adulthood in male offspring was particularly characteristics to those

    born after primiparous pregnancies and, contrary to expected, to those with a high childhood socio-

    economic background.103

    The associations of hypertension-spectrum pregnancy disorders with mental disorders do not form

    a consistent picture. Gestational/chronic hypertension has been associated with higher risk of inter-

    nalizing behaviour problems in adolescents,

    104

    and with higher risk of any mental disorder and ofmood disorder in adulthood.104 Pre-eclampsia has, in turn, been associated with higher levels of self-

    reported depressive symptoms in the offspring in their late adulthood,105 but also with lower risk of

    internalizing behaviour problems in children,104 and with lower risk of any mental disorder and of

    substance use disorder specically in men in adulthood.106

    K. Rikknen et al. / Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 26 (2012) 599611 605

  • 8/13/2019 4 Mental Determ

    8/13

    Early life stress and mental health

    Although programming effects are mostly taken place during the rst 1000 days after conception,

    programming can occur also during later critical periods of development or windows of plasticity. A

    series of studies have provided evidence that exposure to severe early life stress (ELS) exerts long-term

    mental health consequences. However, very few studies have been able to provide evidence that isbased on objectively documented exposure to ELS. Below we describe prospective evidence from one

    such study which provides objectively documented evidence on temporary separations in early

    childhood from both biological parents due to child evacuations to foster care abroad during World

    War II in conjunction with longitudinal data on mental health outcomes across lifespan.

    Cognitive function

    In this study, we measured cognitive abilities of former child evacuees in their young adulthood as

    military conscripts. In comparison to those who were not separated from their parents, those who were

    separated performed poorer in tests measuring verbal, arithmetic and visuospatial cognitive abilities at

    the age of 20.107 The poorer cognitive abilities were shown to translate into socioeconomic disad-

    vantage in adult life. In comparison to men and women who were not separated from their biologicalparents in early childhood, those who were separated had attained a lower lifetime socioeconomic

    position, as indicated by their own highest attained occupational status, educational level and taxable

    incomes across years from young to late adulthood.108

    Mental disorders

    Severe ELS has also been associated with sub-clinical symptoms of mental disorders. One study

    focusing upon child evacuations during WWII has demonstrated that in comparison to the non-

    separated children, those who were separated display over 20% higher levels of self-reported

    depressive symptoms in late adulthood.109 They also appear to suffer more frequently from any

    mental disorder, substance use disorders and personality disorders requiring hospitalization or being

    the primary, intermediate or contributing cause of death in adulthood. However, the risk of seriousmental disorders seems to be characteristic to those separated individuals who had a better childhood

    socioeconomic status.110 In those from lower childhood socioeconomic background, the temporary

    separations did not add to the increased risk of mental disorders already associated with the lower

    socioeconomic status.110

    Stress sensitivity

    The separated men and women have also been shown to display an altered pattern of HPA axis

    activity in response to TSST stress in late adulthood. In comparison to their non-separated peers, the

    separated men and women have been shown to display 2025% higher overall salivary cortisol and

    plasma ACTH levels, and their salivary cortisol levels have been shown to increase more in response to

    the TSST stress.111

    Conclusions

    Environmental adversities in early pre- and postnatal life seem to have widespread consequences

    on mental health outcomes later in life. The existing literature reviewed here is not entirely consistent

    and suggests relatively non-specic effects on mental health outcomes. The non-specicity of the

    effects may relate to programming of the physiological stress systems. Altered HPA axis responsiveness

    has been repeatedly implicated as a factor that co-occurs with poorer cognitive and affective function,

    with temperament and personality traits acting as vulnerability factors for mental disorders, and with

    mental disorders differing in symptomatology and severity.

    While exposures to speci

    c pre- and postnatal adverse environments may offer insight into theunderlying mechanisms above and beyond the information provided by body size at birth and

    prematurity, the exact underlying mechanisms remain unknown. Epigenomic mechanisms inuencing

    gene expression at the level of the placenta and the offspring may play key roles. Indeed, evidence has

    started to accumulate showing alterations in methylation sites of various candidate and/or imprinted

    K. Rikknen et al. / Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 26 (2012) 599611606

  • 8/13/2019 4 Mental Determ

    9/13

    genes and/or epigenome-wide in the offspring at the age of 60 years in relation prenatal famine, 112 in

    the placental tissue in relation to IUGR/small-for-gestational age,113 in brain tissue of adult suicide

    victims exposed to abuse in childhood,114 and in buccal cells of adolescents exposed to parental stress

    in infancy and/or early childhood.115 One recent study has demonstrated that higher level of maternal

    anxiety a day before an elective caesarean section was associated with lower post-partum placental

    11-HSD2 mRNA level; no association was found with the 11-HSD2 enzyme activity, probablyreecting the small sample size in these analyses.116 Further studies along these lines will provide

    insight into the mechanisms underlying early life programming of mental health. These studies will

    also provide information for future prevention and intervention programs targeting the potentially

    modiable environmental factors that will aid at promoting mental well-being and health of an

    individual.

    Acknowledgements

    The study was sponsored by the European Science Foundation (EuroSTRESS), the Finnish Academy,

    and the Netherlands Organization for Scientic Research.

    References

    *1. Barker DJ, Gluckman PD, Godfrey KM, Harding JE et al. Fetal nutrition and cardiovascular disease in adult life. Lancet1993;341: 938941.

    2. Barker DJ, Osmond C, Forsen TJ, Kajantie E et al. Trajectories of growth among children who have coronary events asadults. New England Journal of Medicine 2005; 353:18021809.

    *3. Seckl JR. Physiologic programming of the fetus.Clinical Perinatology1998; 25: 939962. vii.

    4. Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR & Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour andcognition.Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2009; 10: 434445.

    *5. Kajantie E & Rikknen K. Early life predictors of the physiological stress response later in life. Neuroscience andBiobehavioral Reviews 2009; 35: 2332.

    *6. Pesonen A-K, Rikknen K, Feldt K et al. Childhood traumatic separation experience predicts hormonal response at age60 to 70: a natural experiment in World War II. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010; 35: 758767.

    Main clinical practice points

    Mental health cognitive, behavioral and emotional functioning and mental disorders, mayhave origins in prenatal and early postnatal adverse environments.

    The early environmental adversities seem to have non-specic and widespread effects on

    various psychological functions and mental disorders of different severity and

    symptomatology.

    Many of the environmental factors are potentially modiable.

    By targeting the potentially modiable environmental factors future prevention and inter-

    vention programs utilizing this information will benet health and well-being of an

    individual.

    Additional research needed

    Additional research is needed in humans that focuses on the mechanisms that underlie the

    associations of early environmental adversities and mental health.

    Human studies focusing on epigenomic mechanisms inuencing gene expression at the level

    of the placenta and the offspring may provide insight into the underlying mechanisms.

    K. Rikknen et al. / Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 26 (2012) 599611 607

  • 8/13/2019 4 Mental Determ

    10/13

    7. Miller GE, Chen E & Zhou ES. If it goes up, must it come down? Chronic stress and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis in humans. Psychological Bulletin 2007; 133: 2545.

    8. Wust S, Entringer S, Federenko IS et al. Birth weight is associated with salivary cortisol responses to psychosocial stressin adult life. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2005; 30: 591598.

    9. Kajantie E, Feldt K, Rikknen K et al. Body size at birth predicts hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis response topsychosocial stress at age 60 to 70 years. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 2007; 92: 40944100.

    10. Chida Y & Hamer M. Chronic psychosocial factors and acute physiological responses to laboratory-induced stress inhealthy populations: a quantitative review of 30 years of investigations. Psychological Bulletin 2008; 134:829885.11. Glover V. Annual research review: prenatal stress and the origins of psychopathology: an evolutionary perspective.

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry2011; 52: 356367.12. Van den Bergh BR, Mulder EJ, Mennes M et al. Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and the neurobehavioural

    development of the fetus and child: links and possible mechanisms. A review. Neuroscience Biobehaviour Review 2005;29:237258.

    *13. Rikknen K, Seckl JR, Pesonen AK et al. Stress, glucocorticoids and liquorice in human pregnancy: programmers of theoffspring brain. Stress 2011; 14: 590603.

    14. Rikknen K, Forsn T, Henriksson M et al. Growth trajectories and intellectual abilities in young adulthood: TheHelsinki Birth Cohort study. American Journal of Epidemiology2009; 170: 447455.

    15. Rikknen K, Pesonen AK, Kajantie E et al. Length of gestation and depressive symptoms at age 60 years.British Journalof Psychiatry2007; 190: 469474.

    16. Abel KM, Wicks S, Susser ES et al. Birth weight, schizophrenia, and adult mental disorder: is risk conned to thesmallest babies?Archives in General Psychiatry 2010; 67:923930.

    17. Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH et al. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were bornpreterm: a meta-analysis. JAMA 2002; 288: 728737.

    18. Shenkin SD, Starr JM & Deary IJ. Birth weight and cognitive ability in childhood: a systematic review. PsychologicalBulletin 2004; 130: 9891013.

    19. Martyn CN, Gale CR, Sayer AA et al. Growth in utero and cognitive function in adult life: follow-up study of people bornbetween 19201943.BMJ1996; 312: 13931396.

    20. Zhang ZX, Plassman BL & Xu Qea. Lifespan inuences on mide- to late-life cognitive function in a Chinese birth cohort.Neurology2009; 73: 186194.

    21. Richards M, Hardy R, Kuh D et al. Birthweight, postnatal growth and cognitive function in a national UK birth cohort.International Journal of Epidemiology 2002; 31: 342348.

    22. Rikknen K, Kajantie E, Pesonen AK, et al. Early life origins of resilience to cognitive decline in old age: The HelsinkiBirth Cohort Study; submitted for publication.

    23. Heinonen K, Rikknen K, Pesonen A-K et al. Prenatal and postnatal growth and cognitive abilities at 56 months of age:a longitudinal study of infants born at term. Pediatrics 2008; 121: e1325e1333.

    24. Pesonen AK, Rikknen K, Strandberg T et al. Do gestational age and weight for gestational age predict concordance inparental perceptions of infant temperament? Journal of Pediatric Psychology 2006; 31: 331336.

    25. Pesonen AK, Rikknen K, Kajantie E et al. Fetal programming of temperamental negative affectivity among childrenborn healthy at term.Developmental Psychobiology 2006; 48: 633643.

    26. Pluess M & Belsky J. Prenatal programming of postnatal plasticity?Developmental Psychopathology 2011; 23: 2938.27. Roza SJ, van Lier PA, Jaddoe VW et al. Intrauterine growth and infant temperamental difculties: the Generation R

    Study.Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2008; 47:264272.28. Lahti J, Rikknen K, Heinonen K et al. Body size at birth and socio-economic status in childhood: implications for

    Cloningers psychobiological model of temperament at age 60. Psychiatry Research2008; 160:167174.29. Lahti J, Rikknen K, Pesonen AK et al. Prenatal growth, postnatal growth and trait anxiety in late adulthood - the

    Helsinki Birth Cohort Study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 2009; 121:227235.30. Rikknen K, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K et al. Infant growth and hostility in adult life.Psychosometric Medicine2008;70:

    306313.31. Heinonen K, Rikknen K, Pesonen AK et al. Trajectories of growth and symptoms of attention-decit/hyperactivity

    disorder in children: a longitudinal study. BMC Pediatrics 2011; 11:84.

    32. Schlotz W, Jones A, Godfrey KM et al. Effortful control mediates associations of fetal growth with hyperactivity andbehavioural problems in 7- to 9-year-old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2008; 49: 12281233.33. Gale CR & Martyn CN. Birth weight and later risk of depression in a national birth cohort.British Journal of Psychiatry

    2004; 184: 2833.34. Cheung YB, Khoo KS & Karlberg Jea. Association between psychological symptoms in adults and growth in early life:

    longitudinal follow up study. BMJ2002; 325: 749.35. Thompson C, Syddall H & Rodin Iea. Birth weight and the risk of depressive disorder late life.British Journal of Psychiatry

    2001;179: 450455.36. Lahti J, Rikknen K, Sovio U et al. Early-life origins of schizotypal traits in adulthood.British Journal of Psychiatry2009;

    195: 132137.37. Lahti M, Rikknen K, Wahlbeck K et al. Prenatal origins of hospitalization for personality disorders: the Helsinki birth

    cohort study. Psychiatry Research2010; 179: 226230.38. Jones A, Beda A, Ward AM et al. Size at birth and autonomic function during psychological stress.Hypertension 2007;

    49: 548555.39. De Rooij SR, Painter RC, Phillips DI et al. Cortisol responses to psychological stress in adults after prenatal exposure to

    the Dutch famine. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2006; 31: 12571265.40. Entringer S, Kumsta R, Hellhammer DH et al. Prenatal exposure to maternal psychosocial stress and HPA axis regulation

    in young adults. Hormones and Behaviour2009; 55: 292298.*41. Phillips DI & Jones A. Fetal programming of autonomic and HPA function: do people who were small babies have

    enhanced stress responses? Journal of Physiology 2006; 572:4550.

    K. Rikknen et al. / Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 26 (2012) 599611608

  • 8/13/2019 4 Mental Determ

    11/13

    42. Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB et al. Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in verypreterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics 2009; 124:717728.

    43. Bergvall N, Iliadou A, Tuvemo T et al. Birth characteristics and risk of low intellectual performance in earlyadulthood: are the associations confounded by socioeconomic factors in adolescence or familial effects? Pediatrics2006; 117: 714721.

    *44. Pyhl R, Lahti J, Heinonen K et al. Neurocognitive abilities in young adults with very low birth weight.Neurology2011;

    77: 2052

    2060.45. Lhaugen GC, Gramstad A, Evensen KA et al. Cognitive prole in young adults born preterm at very low birthweight.Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 2011; 52: 11331138.

    46. Pyhl R, Rikknen K, Pesonen A-K et al. Temperament in young adults with very low birth weight -Helsinki Study ofVery Low Birth Weight Adults. Personality and Individual Differences 2009; 46: 106110.

    47. Pesonen AK, Rikknen K, Heinonen K et al. Personality of young adults born prematurely: the Helsinki study of verylow birth weight adults. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2008; 49: 609617.

    48. Kajantie E, Hovi P, Rikknen K et al. Young adults with very low birth weight: leaving the parental home and sexualrelationshipsHelsinki study of very low birth weight adults. Pediatrics 2008; 122: e62e72.

    49. Hille ET, den Ouden AL, Saigal S et al. Behavioural problems in children who weigh 1000 g or less at birth in fourcountries.Lancet2001; 357:16411643.

    *50. Rikknen K, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K et al. Depression in young adults with very low birth weight: the Helsinki studyof very low-birth-weight adults. Archives in General Psychiatry 2008; 65: 290296.

    51. Strang-Karlsson S, Rikknen K, Pesonen AK et al. Very low birth weight and behavioral symptoms of attention decithyperactivity disorder in young adulthood: the Helsinki study of very-low-birth-weight adults. American Journal of

    Psychiatry2008; 165: 13451353.52. Stein Z, Susser M, Saenger G et al. Nutrition and mental performance.Science 1972; 178: 708713.53. de Groot RH, Stein AD, Jolles J et al. Prenatal famine exposure and cognition at age 59 years. International Journal of

    Epidemiology2011; 40: 327337.*54. de Rooij SR, Wouters H, Yonker JE et al. Prenatal undernutrition and cognitive function in late adulthood.PNAS2010;

    107:16,88116,886.55. Balota DA, Tse CS, Hutchison KA et al. Predicting conversion to dementia of the Alzheimers type in a healthy control

    sample: the power of errors in Stroop color naming. Psychology and Aging2010: 208218.56. Henik A & Salo R. Schizophrenia and the stroop effect.Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews 2004; 3: 4259.57. Carter CS, Mintun M, Nichols T et al. Anterior cingulate gyrus dysfunction and selective attention decits in

    schizophrenia: [15O]H2O PET study during single-trial Stroop task performance. American Journal of Psychiatry 1997;154: 16701675.

    58. van Swieten JC, Staal S, Kappelle LJ et al. Are white matter lesions directly associated with cognitive impairment inpatients with lacunar infarcts. Journal of Neurology 1996; 243:196200.

    59. Fukui T, Sugita K, Sato Y et al. Cognitive functions in subjects with incidental cerebral hyperintensities. EuropeanNeurology1994; 34.

    60. Roseboom TJ, van der Meulen JHP, Osmond C et al. Coronary heart disease after prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine194445.Heart2000; 84: 595598.

    61. Painter RC, de Rooij SR, Bossuyt PM et al. Early onset of coronary artery disease after prenatal exposure to the Dutchfamine. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 2006; 84: 322327.

    62. van Abeelen AF, Elias SG, Bossuyt PM et al. Cardiovascular consequences of famine in the young.European Heart Journal2011;Aug 25.

    63. Hulshoff Pol HE, Hoek HW, Susser E et al. Prenatal exposure to famine and brain morphology in schizophrenia.American Journal of Psychiatry2000; 157:11701172.

    64. Breteler MM, van Swieten JC, Bolts ML et al. Cerebral white matter lesions, vascular risk factors, and cognitive functionin a population-based study: the Rotterdam study. Neurology 1994: 12461252.

    65. Painter RC, de Rooij SR, Bossuyt PM et al. Blood pressure response to psychological stressors in adults after prenatalexposure to the Dutch famine. Journal of Hypertension 2006; 24: 17711778.

    *66. De Rooij SR, Painter RC, Phillips DI et al. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity in adults who were prenatally

    exposed to the Dutch famine. European Journal of Endocrinology 2006; 155:153

    160.67. deRooij SR, Painter RC, Phillips DI et al. Self-reported depression and anxiety after prenatal famine exposure: mediationby cardio-metabolic pathway? Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease 2011; 2: 136143.

    68. De Rooij SR, Veenendaal MV, Rikknen K et al. Personality and stress appraisal in adults prenatally exposed to theDutch famine. Early Human Development2011.

    69. Susser E, Neugebauer R, Hoek HW et al. Schizophrenia after prenatal famine. Further evidence. Archives in GeneralPsychiatry1996; 53: 2531.

    70. Hoek HW, Susser E, Buck KA et al. Schizoid personality disorder after prenatal exposure to famine.American Journal ofPsychiatry1996; 153: 16371639.

    71. Xu MQ, Sun WS, Liu BX et al. Prenatal malnutrition and adult schizophrenia: further evidence from the 1959-1961Chinesefamine. Schizophrenia Bulletin 2009; 35: 568576.

    72. Neugebauer R, Hoek HW & Susser E. Prenatal exposure to wartime famine and development of antisocial personalitydisorder in early adulthood. JAMA 1999; 282:455462.

    73. Brown AS, van Os J, Driessens C et al. Further evidence of relation between prenatal famine and major affectivedisorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 2000; 157:190195.

    74. Franzek EJ, Sprangers N, Janssens AC et al. Prenatal exposure to the 194445 Dutch hunger winterand addiction laterin life. Addicition 2008; 103: 433438.

    75. Welberg LA, Seckl JR & Holmes MC. Inhibition of 11beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase, the foeto-placental barrier tomaternal glucocorticoids, permanently programs amygdala GR mRNA expression and anxiety-like behaviour in theoffspring.European Journal of Neurosciences 2000; 12: 10471054.

    K. Rikknen et al. / Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 26 (2012) 599611 609

  • 8/13/2019 4 Mental Determ

    12/13

    76. Benediktsson R, Calder AA, Edwards CR et al. Placental 11 beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase: a key regulator of fetalglucocorticoid exposure. Clinical Endocrinology (Oxford) 1997; 46: 161166.

    77. Rikknen K, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K et al. Maternal licorice consumption and detrimental cognitive and psychiatricoutcomes in children. American Journal of Epidemiology 2009; 170:11371146.

    78. Rikknen K, Seckl JR, Heinonen K et al. Maternal prenatal licorice consumption alters hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis function in children. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010; 35: 15871593.

    79. Allen VM, Joseph K, Murphy KE et al. The effect of hypertensive disorders in pregnancy on small for gestational age andstillbirth: a population based study. BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth 2004; 4: 17.80. Roberts CL, Algert CS, Morris JM et al. Hypertensive disorders in pregnancy: a population-based study.Medical Journal

    of Australia2005; 182: 332335.81. Gray PH, OCallaghan MJ, Harvey JM et al. Placental pathology and neurodevelopment of the infant with intrauterine

    growth restriction. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 1999; 41: 1620.82. Himmelmann K, Himmelmann A, Niklasson A et al. Hypertension in pregnancy and size at birth.Blood Pressure1996;5:

    278284.83. Roberts JM & Redman CW. Pre-eclampsia: more than pregnancy-induced hypertension.Lancet1993; 341:14471451.84. Sibai B, Dekker G & Kupferminc M. Pre-eclampsia.Lancet2005; 365:785799.85. Rang S, Wolf H, Montfrans GA et al. Non-invasive assessment of autonomic cardiovascular control in normal

    human pregnancy and pregnancy-associated hypertensive disorders: a review. Journal of Hypertension 2002; 20:21112119.

    86. Schobel HP & Grassi G. Hypertensive disorders of pregnancy: a dysregulation of the sympathetic nervous system?Journal of Hypertension1998; 16:569570.

    87. McCalla CO, Nacharaju VL, Muneyyirci-Delale O et al. Placental 11 beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase activity innormotensive and pre-eclamptic pregnancies. Steroids 1998; 63: 511515.

    88. Schoof E, Girstl M, Frobenius W et al. Decreased gene expression of 11beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2 and15-hydroxyprostaglandin dehydrogenase in human placenta of patients with preeclampsia. Journal of Clinical Endo-crinology and Metabolism 2001; 86: 13131317.

    89. Kajantie E, Dunkel L, Turpeinen U et al. Placental 11 beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase-2 and fetal cortisol/cortisoneshuttlein small preterm infants. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 2003; 88: 493500.

    90. Ehrenstein V, Rothman KJ, Pedersen L et al. Pregnancy-associated hypertensive disorders and adult cognitive functionamong Danish conscripts. American Journal of Epidemiology 2009; 170: 10251031.

    91. Gray PH, OCallaghan MJ, Mohay HA et al. Maternal hypertension and neurodevelopmental outcome in very preterminfants. Archives of Disease in Childhood Fetal and Neonatal Edition 1998; 79: F88F93.

    92. Kronenberg ME, Raz S & Sander CJ. Neurodevelopmental outcome in children born to mothers with hypertension inpregnancy: the signicance of suboptimal intrauterine growth. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology2006;48:200206.

    93. Many A, Fattal-Valevski A & Leitner Y. Neurodevelopmental and cognitive assessment of 6-year-old children borngrowth restricted. International Journal of Gynaecology and Obstetrics 2005; 89: 5556.

    94. Many A, Fattal A, Leitner Y et al. Neurodevelopmental and cognitive assessment of children born growth restricted tomothers with and without preeclampsia. Hypertension Pregnancy2003; 22: 2529.

    95. Ounsted MK, Moar VA, Good FJ et al. Hypertension during pregnancy with and without specic treatment; thedevelopment of the children at the age of four years. British Journal of Obstetric Gynaecology 1980; 87: 1924.

    96. Ounsted MK, Moar VA & Scott A. Children of deviant birthweight at the age of seven years: health, handicap, size anddevelopmental status. Early Human Development1984; 9: 323340.

    97. Ounsted MK, Moar VA & Scott WA. Growth and proportionality in early childhood. II. Some population differences.Early Human Development1986; 14: 4960.

    98. Seidman DS, Laor A, Gale R et al. Pre-eclampsia and offsprings blood pressure, cognitive ability and physical devel-opment at 17-years-of-age. British Journal of Obstetric Gynaecology 1991; 98: 10091014.

    99. Spinillo A, Iasci A, Capuzzo E et al. Two-year infant neurodevelopmental outcome after expectant management andindicated preterm delivery in hypertensive pregnancies.Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica 1994; 73: 625629.

    100. Spinillo A, Montanari L, Gardella B et al. Infant sex, obstetric risk factors, and 2-year neurodevelopmental outcome

    among preterm infants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 2009; 51:518

    525.101. Szymonowicz W & Yu VY. Severe pre-eclampsia and infants of very low birth weight.Archives of Disease in Childhood1987;62: 712716.

    102. Ounsted M, Moar VA, Cockburn J et al. Factors associated with the intellectual ability of children born to women withhigh risk pregnancies. British Medical Journal (Clinical Research Education) 1984; 288: 10381041.

    103. Tuovinen S, Rikknen K, Kajantie E et al. Hypertensive disorders in pregnancy and intellectual abilities in the offspringin young adulthood: The Helsinki Birth Cohort Study. Annals of Medicine 2011.

    104. Robinson M, Mattes E, Oddy WH et al. Hypertensive diseases of pregnancy and the development of behavioralproblems in childhood and adolescence: the Western Australian Pregnancy Cohort Study. Journal of Pediatrics 2009;154: 218224.

    105. Tuovinen S, Rikknen K, Kajantie E et al. Depressive symptoms in adulthood and intrauterine exposure to pre-eclampsia: the Helsinki Birth Cohort Study. BJOG 2010; 117: 12361242.

    106. Tuovinen S, Rikknen K, Pesonen AK et al. Hypertensive disorders in pregnancy and risk of severe mental disorders inthe offspring in adulthood: The Helsinki Birth Cohort Study. Journal of Psychiatric Research 2012; 46: 303310.

    107. Pesonen AK, Rikknen K, Kajantie E et al. Intellectual ability in young men separated temporarily from their parents in

    childhood.Intelligence 2011; 39.108. Pesonen AK, Rikknen K, Kajantie E et al. Inter-generational social mobility following early life stress. Annals of

    Medicine2011; 43: 320328.109. Pesonen AK, Rikknen K, Heinonen K et al. Depressive symptoms in adults separated from their parents as children:

    a natural experiment during World War II. American Journal of Epidemiology 2007; 166: 11261133.

    K. Rikknen et al. / Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 26 (2012) 599611610

  • 8/13/2019 4 Mental Determ

    13/13

    110. Rikknen K, Lahti M, Heinonen K et al. Risk of severe mental disorders in adults separated temporarily from theirparents in childhood: the Helsinki birth cohort study. Journal of Psychiatric Research 2010; 45: 332338.

    111. Pesonen AK, Raikkonen K, Feldt K et al. Childhood separation experience predicts HPA axis hormonal responses in lateadulthood: a natural experiment of World War II. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009.

    112. Heijmans BT, Tobi EW, Stein AD et al. Persistent epigenetic differences associated with prenatal exposure to famine inhumans. Proceedings of Natural Academy of Sciences, USA 2008; 105: 17,04617,049.

    113. Banister CE, Koestler DC, Maccani MA et al. Infant growth restriction is associated with distinct patterns of DNAmethylation in human placentas. Epigenetics 2011; 6: 920927.114. McGowan PO, Sasaki A, D Alessio AC et al. Epigenetic regulation of the glucocorticoid receptor in human brain asso-

    ciates with childhood abuse. Nature Neuroscience 2009; 12: 342348.115. Oberlander TF, Weinberg J, Papsdorf M, Grunau R et al. Prenatal exposure to maternal depression, neonatal methylation

    of human glucocorticoid receptor gene (NR3C1) and infant cortisol stress responses. Epigenetics 2008; 3: 97106.116. ODonnell KJ, Bugge Jensen A, Freeman L et al. Maternal prenatal anxiety and downregulation of placental 11beta-HSD2.

    Psychoneuroendocrinology2011.

    K. Rikknen et al. / Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 26 (2012) 599611 611