8
6. The First and Second Sound Shift: the separation of the Germanic language grouping from the Indo-European language family the first non-trivial systematic sound shift to be discovered in the Germanic lingui its formulation was a turning point in the development of linguistics, enabling the of a rigorous methodology to historical linguistic research. The correspondence between Latin p and Germanic f was first noted by Friedrich von Schlegel in 1!" 11 #asmus $hristian #as% elaborated the set of correspondences to include other &n 'uropean languages such as Sans%rit and Gree%, and the full range of consonants involved &n 1(( )acob Grimm* Deutsche Grammatik , formulated the law as a general rule +and e tended it include standard German The e ceptions that he noticed were over-viewed for several decades but eventually receive e planation from the anish linguist /arl 0erner in his law that %ept his name* 0erner s Grimm2s law consists of three parts which form consecutive phases in the sense of a chain shift . The phases are usually constructed as follows* 1. 3roto-&ndo-'uropean voiceless stops change into voiceless fricatives. (. 3roto-&ndo-'uropean voiced stops become voiceless stops. 4. 3roto-&ndo-'uropean voiced aspirated stops become voiced stops or fricatives +as allophones . This chain shift can be abstractly represented as* bʰ 5b5p5ɸ dʰ 5d5t56 gʰ 5g5%5

6.1. First Sound Shift

  • Upload
    oanabun

  • View
    214

  • Download
    2

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

6

Citation preview

6. The First and Second Sound Shift: the separation of the Germanic language grouping from the Indo-European language family the first non-trivial systematic sound shift to be discovered in the Germanic linguistics

its formulation was a turning point in the development of linguistics, enabling the introduction of a rigorous methodology to historical linguistic research.

The correspondence between Latin p and Germanic f was first noted by Friedrich von Schlegel in 1806

1818 Rasmus Christian Rask elaborated the set of correspondences to include other Indo-European languages such as

Sanskrit and

Greek,

and the full range of consonants involved

In 1822 Jacob Grimm: Deutsche Grammatik, formulated the law as a general rule (and extended it to include standard German)

The exceptions that he noticed were over-viewed for several decades but eventually received explanation from the Danish linguist Karl Verner in his law that kept his name: Verners Law

Grimm's law consists of three parts which form consecutive phases in the sense of a chain shift. The phases are usually constructed as follows:

1. Proto-Indo-European voiceless stops change into voiceless fricatives.

2. Proto-Indo-European voiced stops become voiceless stops.

3. Proto-Indo-European voiced aspirated stops become voiced stops or fricatives (as allophones).

This chain shift can be abstractly represented as:

b>b>p> d>d>t> g>g>k>x g>g>k>xHere each sound moves one position to the right to take on its new sound value. Within Proto-Germanic, the sounds denoted by

b, d, g and gw were stops in some environments and fricatives in others, so b > b should be understood here as b > b/, and likewise for the others. The voiceless fricatives are customarily spelled f, , h and hw in the context of GermanicThe exact details of the shift are unknown, and it may have progressed in a variety of ways before arriving at the final situation. The three stages listed above show the progression of a "pull chain", in which each change leaves a "gap" in the phonological system that "pulls" other phonemes into it to fill the gap. But it is also conceivable that the shift happened as a push chain, where the changes happened in reverse order, with each change "pushing" the next forward to avoid merging the phonemes.

The steps could also have occurred somewhat differently. Another possible sequence of events could have been:

1. Voiceless stops are allophonically aspirated under most conditions.

2. Voiced stops become voiceless stops.

3. Aspirated stops become fricativesThis sequence would lead to the same end result. This variety of Grimm's Law is often suggested in the context of the glottalic theory of Proto-Indo-European, which is followed by a minority of linguists. This theoretical framework assumes that "voiced stops" in PIE were actually voiceless to begin with, so that the second phase did not actually exist as such, or was not actually devoicing but a loss of some other articulatory feature such as glottalization. This alternative sequence also accounts for the phonetics of Verners Law which is easier to explain within the glottalic theory framework when Grimm's Law is formulated in this manner.

Once Grimm's Law had taken place, there was only one type of voiced consonant, with no distinction between voiced stops = voiced fricatives>stops at the beginning of a word

,

The voiced aspirated stops may have > voiced fricatives. (they may also have become stops at first, softening to fricatives in most positions later) the voicing of the voiceless fricatives For example:

Proto-Indo-European*brhtr ("brother") > Proto-Germanic *brr (Old English broor, Old High German bruothar/bruodar)

Proto-Indo-European*phtr ("father") > Proto-Germanic *fadr (Old English fder, Old High German fatar)

Here, the same sound *t appears as * in one word (following Grimm's law), but as

*d in another (apparently violating Grimm's law) The early Germanic *gw (from Proto-Indo-European *g (and from *k through Verner's law) underwent further changes of various sorts:

After *n it was preserved as a labiovelar stop *gw, but later changed to a plain velar *g in West Germanic Following vowels, it seems to have become*w, presumably through a fricative stage*.

Word-initially, the most plausible reflex is a labiovelar stop *g at first, but the further development is unclear. In that position, it became either *w, *g or *b during late Proto-Germanic.

The regular reflex next to *u would likely have been *g, due to loss of the labial element before a labial vowel.

Further changes following Grimm's law as well as sound changes in other Indo-European languages, can sometimes obscure its effects. The most illustrative examples are used here.

MeaningNon-Germanic (unshifted) cognatesChangeGermanic (shifted) examples

footAncient Greek: , (pos, pods), Latin: ps, pedis, Sanskrit: pda, Russian: (pod) "under; floor", Lithuanian:pda, Latvianpda*p > f []English: foot, West Frisian: foet, German: Fu, Gothic: ftus, Icelandic, Faroese: ftur, Danish: fod, Norwegian, Swedish:fot

thirdAncient Greek: (tritos), Latin: tertius, Welsh: trydydd, Sanskrit: treta, Russian: (tretij), Lithuanian: treias, Albanian:tret*t > []English: third, Old Frisian: thredda, Old Saxon: thriddio, Gothic: ridja, Icelandic: riji, Danish: tredje

dogAncient Greek: (kn), Latin: canis, Welsh: ci (pl.cwn)*k > h[x]English: hound, Dutch: hond, German: Hund, Gothic: hunds, Icelandic, Faroese: hundur, Danish, Norwegian, Swedish:hund

whatLatin: quod, Irish cad, Sanskrit: kd, Russian: - (ko-), Lithuanian: kas*k> hw[x]English: what, Gothic: a ("hwa"), Icelandic: hva, Faroese:hvat, Danish: hvad, Norwegian: hva

deepLithuanian: dubs*b > p [p]English: deep, West Frisian: djip, Dutch: diep, Icelandic:djpur, Swedish: djup, Gothic diups

tenLatin: decem, Greek: (dka), Irish: deich, Sanskrit: daan, Russian: (desyat'), Lithuanian: deimt*d > t [t]English: ten, Dutch: tien, Gothic: tahun, Icelandic: tu, Faroese: tggju, Danish, Norwegian: ti, Swedish: tio

coldLatin: gel, Greek: (gelandrs), Lithuanian: gelmenis, gelum*g > k [k]English: cold, West Frisian: kld, Dutch: koud, German: kalt, Icelandic, Faroese: kaldur, Danish: kold, Norwegian: kald, Swedish: kall

aliveLithuanian: gyvas*g > kw [k]English: quick, West Frisian: kwik, kwyk, Dutch: kwiek, German: keck, Gothic: qius, Icelandic, Faroese: kvikur, Danish: kvik, Swedish: kvick, Norwegian kvikk

brotherSanskrit: bhrt, Ancient Greek: (phrtr) ("member of a brotherhood")*b> b[b]/[]English: brother, West Frisian, Dutch: broeder, German:Bruder, Gothic: broar, Icelandic, Faroese: brir, Danish, Swedish, Norwegian: broder

honeySanskrit: mdhu, Homeric Greek: (methu)*d > d[d]/[]English: mead, East Frisian: meede, Dutch: mede, Danish/Norwegian: mjd, Icelandic: mjur , Swedish: mjd

walk, stepSanskrit: stighnoti, Ancient Greek: (stekhein)*g > g[]/[]Old English: stgan, Dutch: stijgen, German: steigen, Icelandic, Faroese: stga, Danish, Norwegian: stige, Gothicsteigan (all meaning "ascend, climb")

singHomeric Greek: (ephth) "sang, sounded", (omph) "voice"*g> gw[](after n)English: sing, West Frisian: sjonge, Dutch: zingen, German:singen, Gothic: siggwan, Old Icelandic: syngva, syngja, Icelandic, Faroese: syngja, Swedish: sjunga, Danish:synge/sjunge

warmSanskrit:gharm-, Avestan:garm, Old Prussian:gorme*g > gw > {b, g w}(otherwise merged with existing gand w)English: warm, West Frisian: waarm, Dutch, German: warm, Swedish: varm, Icelandic: varmur

This is strikingly regular. Each phase involves one single change which applies equally to the labials (p, b, b, f) and their equivalent dentals (t, d, d, ), velars (k, g, g, h) and rounded velars (k, g, g, h). The first phase left the phoneme repertoire of the language without voiceless stops, the second phase filled this gap, but created a new one, and so on until the chain had run its course.

Consonant clusters

When two obstruents occurred in a pair, the first underwent Grimm's law if possible, while the second did not. If either of the two was voiceless, the whole cluster was devoiced, and the first obstruent also lost its labialization, if it was present.

Most examples of this occurred with obstruents preceded by *s (resulting in *sp, *st, *sk, *sk), or obstruents followed by *t (giving *ft, *ss, *ht, *ht) or *s (giving *fs, *ss, *hs, *hs). The latter change was frequent in suffixes, and became a phonotactic restriction known as the Germanic spirant Law. This rule remained productive throughout the Proto-Germanic period. The cluster *tt became *ss, but this was often restored to *st later on.

Examples with preceding *s:

Non-Germanic examplesChangeGermanic examples

Latin: spuere, Lithuanian: spjuti*spEnglish: spew, West Frisian: spije, Dutch: spuwen, German: speien,Danish, Norwegian, Swedish: spy, Icelandic: spja, Faroese: spggja, Gothic: speiwan

Latin: stre, Irish: stad, Sanskrit: sta, Russian: (stat'), Lithuanian: stoti, Persian: (istdan)*stEnglish: stand, Icelandic, Faroese, Norwegian: standa, Gothic: standan; West Frisian: stean, Dutch: staan, German: stehen, Danish, Swedish: st

Lithuanian: skurdus*skEnglish: short, Old High German: scurz, Icelandic: skorta

Irish: scal*skEnglish: scold, Icelandic: skld, Norwegian: skald; West Frisian: skelle, Dutch: schelden, German: schelten

English and German later underwent a change/sk/>//. German also changed /sp/ and /st/ to /p/ and /t/.

Diutch has /sk/ > /sx/, though this is a separate development that occurred in Middle English.

Examples with following *t:

Non-Germanic examplesChangeGermanic examples

Ancient Greek: (klepts), Old Prussian: au-klipts "hidden"*ptftGothic: hliftus "thief"

Latin: atta, Greek: (tta)*ttttOld High German: atto, Gothic: atta "father"

Ancient Greek: (okt), Irish: ocht, Latin: oct*kthtEnglish: eight, West Frisian, Dutch, German:acht, Gothic: ahtu, Icelandic: tta(pronounced [auhta])

Irish: anocht, Latin: nox, noct-, Greek: , - (nks, nukt-),Sanskrit: (naktam), Lithuanian: naktis, Hittite (genitive): nekuz (pronounced/nekts/)*kthtEnglish: night, West Frisian, Dutch, German:nacht, Gothic: nahts, Icelandic: ntt (pronounced [nouht]

Icelandic ntt /nouht/ comes from Old Norse ntt, ntt, from Proto-Germanic *naht-. The Germanic *ht regularly becomes *tt in Old Norse, and this then becomes preaspirated in Icelandic. Thus, the [h] of the modern Icelandic form is not a direct descendant of the Germanic /h/]The same ancestry holds for the /tt/ of Icelandic tta as well

after a nasal consonants

fricatives elsewhere