152
The Pennsylvania State University The Graduate School College of Health and Human Development A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL ACTIVITY A Dissertation in Kinesiology by Chih-Hsiang Yang © 2017 Chih-Hsiang Yang Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy August 2017

A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

The Pennsylvania State University

The Graduate School

College of Health and Human Development

A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL ACTIVITY

A Dissertation in

Kinesiology

by

Chih-Hsiang Yang

© 2017 Chih-Hsiang Yang

Submitted in Partial Fulfillment

of the Requirements

for the Degree of

Doctor of Philosophy

August 2017

Page 2: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

ii

The dissertation of Chih-Hsiang Yang was reviewed and approved* by the following faculties:

David E. Conroy

Professor of Kinesiology and Human Development & Family Studies

Dissertation Advisor

Chair of Committee

Steriani Elavsky

Associate Professor of Kinesiology

Michael Blair Evans

Assistant Professor of Kinesiology

Lesley A. Ross

Associate Professor of Human Development and Family Studies

Stephen J. Piazza

Professor of Kinesiology and Physiology

Graduate Program Director of Kinesiology

*Signatures are on file in the graduate school

Page 3: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

iii

Abstract

Physical activity and mindfulness training can each be health enhancing but limited

efforts have been made to integrate them in research or clinical practice. Combining physical

activity with mindfulness as a single intervention have potential to escalate the health effects.

Practicing mindfulness during physical activity interventions may also help people to maintain

their increased physical activity level, leading to a more sustainable intervention. The

disproportional attention on trait mindfulness but limited focus on state mindfulness in previous

studies signifies a main barrier that restricted the application for implementing mindfulness in

physical activity interventions. To overcome this main obstacle, three studies in this dissertation

were designed to address the barrier and advance understanding of the relation between physical

activity and mindfulness.

The first study involved a systematic review of peer-reviewed research articles in major

academic database and Google Scholar to synthesize and identify gaps in the mindfulness-

physical activity literature. Available evidence showed that physical activity and trait

mindfulness were positively related, but little has been known regarding the association between

physical activity and state mindfulness. Based on the lack of state mindfulness studies in the

current literature, a three-level mindfulness framework (global, contextual, and situational level)

was proposed to organize current research studies and available mindfulness self-report measures

within each mindfulness level. This hierarchical framework that differentiated the contextual and

temporal specificity of mindfulness is needed to bridge the gap in the literature.

The second study applied experience sampling methods to investigate the relations

between momentary time allocation in different daily activities/postures and well-being among

college students (N=158). Results from multilevel analysis suggested that college students’

Page 4: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

iv

negative affect was associated with their daily activities/postures and mindfulness in the within-

person level. When students moved more or stood more compared to sitting in their daily life,

they reported lower levels of negative affect. Further, students’ situational mindfulness seemed

to moderate this within-person association between momentary activities/postures and negative

affect. When students were more mindful while moving, they experienced greater degrees of

reduction in negative affect momentarily.

The final study designed and evaluated an outdoor mindful walking program to determine

its feasibility, acceptability, sustainability and preliminary efficacy in reducing negative affect

among older adults. Community-dwelling older adults (N=27) attended a one-month, eight

session mindful walking program and completed multiple self-report measures before, during,

and after the program. Overall, participants perceived this outdoor mindful walking program to

be acceptable, engaging and appealing. After the program, most of them were still willing to

maintain and actually continued practicing mindful walking in their leisure time. Preliminary

efficacy of the program was also identified. After the mindful walking sessions, reductions in

negative affect were associated with increases in situational mindfulness among older adults.

Based on the promising results, future randomized controlled trials are warranted to test the

efficacy of mindful walking program among older adults.

Collectively, these studies highlight the value of studying mindfulness as a time-varying

construct in physical activity studies. Differentiating global mindfulness from context- or

situation-specific mindfulness not only broadens our scope in understanding the relation between

physical activity and mindfulness, but also provides a new avenue for future research. Each study

in the current dissertation made unique contributions to the physical activity and mindfulness

literature. First, this dissertation synthesized research evidence on the physical activity-

Page 5: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

v

mindfulness association, identified the literature gap, and provided a useful framework for

further studies to close the gap. Second, this dissertation was the first experience sampling study

that identified the time-varying association between momentary physical activity, mindfulness,

and negative affect in daily life. Lastly, this dissertation designed an innovative and feasible

mindful walking program which may inform future dissemination for reducing negative affect in

older adults. In general, the within-person associations between physical activity and

mindfulness established in this dissertation provided valuable implications in facilitating and

developing effective behavioral intervention strategies to promote health and well-being in

different populations.

Page 6: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

vi

Table of Contents

List of Tables ................................................................................................................................ vii

List of Figures .............................................................................................................................. viii

Acknowledgements ........................................................................................................................ ix

Chapter 1: Introduction ................................................................................................................... 1

References ................................................................................................................................... 8

Chapter 2: Mindfulness and Physical Activity: A Systematic Review and Hierarchical Model of

Mindfulness................................................................................................................................... 17

Methods ..................................................................................................................................... 21

Results ....................................................................................................................................... 21

Discussion ................................................................................................................................. 26

References ................................................................................................................................. 37

Chapter 3: Momentary Negative Affect is Lower During Mindful Movement than While Sitting:

An Experience Sampling Study .................................................................................................... 55

Abstract ..................................................................................................................................... 56

Methods ..................................................................................................................................... 62

Results ....................................................................................................................................... 68

Discussion ................................................................................................................................. 70

References ................................................................................................................................. 77

Chapter 4: Feasibility of an Outdoor Mindful Walking Program for Reducing Negative Affect in

Older Adults .................................................................................................................................. 94

Abstract ..................................................................................................................................... 95

Methods ................................................................................................................................... 100

Results ..................................................................................................................................... 106

Discussion ............................................................................................................................... 109

References ............................................................................................................................... 115

Chapter 5: Conclusions ............................................................................................................... 129

References ............................................................................................................................... 138

Page 7: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

vii

List of Tables

Table 2.1. Description of the reviewed studies on mindfulness and physical activity….…….…46

Table 2.2. Current mindfulness questionnaires organized by their level in the proposed

hierarchical model……………...….…………………………………………………...…….….50

Table 3.1. Descriptive statistics, correlations, and intraclass correlation coefficients of subjective experiences, physical activity, and other variables of interest………….................………….....91 Table 3.2. Multilevel model coefficients for model predicting negative affect....………………92 Table 4.1. Overview of the eight walking sessions in the mindful walking program...………..124

Table 4.2. Contexts of the mindful walking sessions.............…………………...……………..125

Table 4.3. Correlation coefficient matrix of the pre-post walking session variables.……...…..126

Table 4.4. Result of multiple regression model examining associations between mindfulness

level and negative affective reactivity…………………………..……………….……..………127

Page 8: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

viii

List of Figures

Figure 1.1. Conceptual framework of the proposed studies designed in the dissertation ...………15

Figure 2.1. The PRISMA flow chart for literature research ...…………………………………....53

Figure 3.1. Frequency of experience sampling responses at each time during the study period …89

Figure 3.2. Associations of momentary mindfulness and moving on negative affect …………....90

Figure 4.1. CONSORT flow diagram of mindful walking program………………...…………...123

Page 9: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

ix

Acknowledgements

In my journey of pursuing the doctoral degree in Kinesiology, I have received tremendous support

from multiple individuals. My success is the product of their support and encouragement.

First and foremost, I would like to express my sincere gratitude to my advisor David E. Conroy for

providing me his excellent guidance and support throughout my graduate training over the past four years.

His mentorship is always thoughtful and patient. I am very fortunate to be his student and work with him.

His expertise and constructive feedback have inspired my work and prepared me to become a better

researcher. My deepest appreciation also goes out to Dr. Steriani Elavsky, Dr. Blair Evans, and Dr. Lesley

Ross for their kindness and precious time in serving on my dissertation committee. With their valuable

insights and feedback, the quality of my dissertation has elevated in all aspects.

I am forever grateful to my family, including my parents and my sisters Peggy and Carol, for their

consistent, unconditional support and love; and to my dear friends and colleagues both in Taiwan and

Penn State for their support and positive thoughts. I am also grateful to Jessie Fu for believing in me and

willing to walk with me in reaching the finish line of this marathon. Her warm consideration and

understanding has undoubtedly strengthened my ability to complete this challenging process.

Special thanks to my bachelor and master mentor Dr. Zhong, Sheng-Xiao, Dr. Sheue-Mei Lu, and

Principle Ching-Yuan Huang at Ta-Yeh Elementary School for their encouragement, ongoing support,

and valuable experiences they have shared with me. Thanks to all the great people in Happy Valley, to

those who were so gracious with their time and resources in helping with my study recruitment, and to

those who expressed their genuine attitude in participating my study. Without their commitment, there

would not be my dissertation.

Finally, I would also like to acknowledge the funding agents - the Studying Abroad Scholarship

from the Ministry of Education in Taiwan for supporting my graduate training, and the Dissertation

Research Endowment Funds from College of Health and Human Development at Penn State for

supporting my dissertation work. Without the financial resources, my work would not be possible.

Page 10: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

1

Chapter 1: Introduction

Page 11: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

2

The mind-body practice of mindful movement has become increasingly popular as a non-

pharmacological therapy to promoting health and well-being. This approach can be simply

described as combining features from both mindfulness meditation and bodily-based activities

into a particular form of practice (La Forge, 2016; Tang, Jiang, & Tang, 2017). Accumulating

research evidence has revealed the salutary impacts of mindful movement approach on a variety

of physical and mental health outcomes in both the clinical and general populations (Appling,

Scarvalone, MacDonald, McBeth, & Helzlsouer, 2012; Lee, Mancuso, & Charlson, 2004; Paul et

al., 2013; Schure, Christopher, & Christopher, 2008; Tang et al., 2017; Wells, Phillips, &

McCarthy, 2011). Despite the promising outcomes of mindful movement interventions, the

association between mindfulness and physical activity has not been fully studied.

Mindfulness is the ability to regulate one’s attention and awareness towards the present

moment experiences, and adopting an accepting, open-minded, and curious orientation towards

these experiences (Kabat-Zinn, 2011; Langer, 1992). Previous research has made a distinction

between trait and state modes of mindfulness to characterize their between-person and within-

person individual difference, respectively (Brown & Ryan, 2003; Thompson & Waltz, 2015).

Trait and state mindfulness are closely associated, but they are considered different constructs

theoretically and operationally (Bishop et al., 2004). The literature points to a profound effect of

mindfulness-based interventions on physical and mental health. These interventions are generally

designed to target people’s trait mindfulness, so the beneficial effects can be observed as

participants’ trait mindfulness increased (Greeson, 2009). Mindfulness-based interventions can

positively impact health behaviors including eating, sleeping, substance use, and cardiovascular

health (Gilbert & Waltz, 2010; Loucks et al., 2015). They have also consistently exerted

substantial efficacy in reducing negative affective states including stress, anxiety, and depression

Page 12: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

3

(Davis & Hayes, 2011; Gotink et al., 2015; Grossman, Niemann, Schmidt, & Walach, 2004;

Keng, Smoski, & Robins, 2011; Sharma & Rush, 2014). These results have mostly focused

health behavior and well-being differences between more and less mindful people. Only a few

studies thus far explored the time-varying association between mindfulness and negative affect

(Snippe et al., 2016; Snippe, Nyklíček, Schroevers, & Bos, 2015).

Physical activity refers to any bodily movement that increases energy expenditure from

the contraction of skeletal muscle (Caspersen, Powell, & Christenson, 1985). Engaging in

moderate-to-vigorous physical activity can lower the risk for early death, heart disease, stroke,

type-2 diabetes, depression, and some cancers (Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory

Committee, 2008). Regular physical activity participation is also associated with better mental

health and less psychological illness in different populations (Biddle & Asare, 2011; Mammen &

Faulkner, 2013; Rebar et al., 2015; Rosenbaum, Tiedemann, Sherrington, Curtis, & Ward, 2014).

For improving well-being, recent reviews and meta-analyses have reported that physical activity

engagement, especially under moderate and vigorous intensities, may reduce different aspects of

negative affect including stress, anxiety, and depression. (Cooney, Dwan, & Mead, 2014; Ensari,

Greenlee, Motl, & Petruzzello, 2015; Schuch, Rocha, & Cadore, 2015; Stults-Kolehmainen &

Sinha, 2013). As with mindfulness research, the majority of these findings are based on between-

person differences, with fewer studies focusing on within-person associations between physical

activity and negative affective states (Dunton et al., 2014; Hyde, Conroy, Pincus, & Ram, 2011;

Liao, Shonkoff, & Dunton, 2015). Based on the well-established positive findings in both

physical activity and mindfulness interventions in improving health and well-being, it would be

interesting to investigate how mindfulness is related to physical activity, and whether there

Page 13: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

4

would be potential synergistic effects from integrating these two factors in promoting health and

well-being.

In mindfulness-based interventions, the training on attention-regulation and the adoption

of an open, accepting orientation to mental and bodily experience are considered central

elements across different schools of thought (Hart, Ivtzan, & Hart, 2013). Considering the

critical role of self-regulation in sustaining people’s physical activity, training the regulation of

attention and awareness via mindfulness interventions may also play beneficial roles in

maintaining individual’s physical activity (Bandura, 2005; Buckley, Cohen, Kramer, McAuley,

& Mullen, 2014; de Bruin et al., 2012). In fact, one cross-sectional study has already identified

that people’s trait mindfulness level may amplify relations between intentions and physical

activity (Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2007). Thus, prescribing mindfulness practice in physical

activity programs may provide additional intervention effect in increasing and sustaining

people’s physical activity level.

On the other hand, regular physical activity participation can also impact people’s

cognitive ability including attention and executive function (Best, Nagamatsu, & Liu-Ambrose,

2014; Kamijo & Takeda, 2010; Sofi et al., 2011). The improved attention and self-control from

physical activity are common outcomes from mindfulness practices, suggesting that physical

activity engagement may also facilitate people to be more mindful (Dickenson, Berkman, Arch,

& Lieberman, 2012; Jha, Krompinger, & Baime, 2007; Tang et al., 2007). Preliminary research

evidence supported this notion by showing that people who exercise more regularly and who can

meet their exercise goals tend to report higher levels of mindfulness (Ulmer, Stetson, & Salmon,

2010).

Page 14: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

5

Although there are some studies focusing on how physical activity and mindfulness could

facilitate each other, these studies focus on linking trait mindfulness with physical activity, and

few studies have examined associations between state mindfulness and physical activity. Thus,

little is known about whether state mindfulness, or the mindfulness people experience at a given

moment in time, may also relate to physical activity or interact with physical activity

momentarily in predicting health outcomes. Based on the conceptualization that mindfulness is

more of a state than a trait mental construct, researchers should study mindfulness as a time-

varying state to unpack its relation with physical activity, health, and well-being (Bishop et al.,

2004).

To overcome this limitation in current literature, this dissertation comprises three studies

that advance understanding of how mindfulness and physical activity combine at the momentary

level to enhance health and well-being. Negative affect regulation was used as an indicator of

well-being for this work because negative affective experiences are prevalent in people’s daily

life across different populations (American Psychological Association, 2015). Excessive daily

negative affective experiences (i.e., feelings of stress, anxiety, depression) have been

documented as precursors to more severe, clinical-level mental disorders (Cohen, Janicki-

Deverts, & Miller, 2007; Watson, Clark, & Carey, 1988). Tackling these negative affective states

may prevent the onset of mental illness, and in turn, improve people’s health and well-being.

The conceptual model in Figure 1.1 illustrates the overall association of the main study

constructs. The solid arrows represent the established between-person association among the

constructs based on the literature. These associations are served as the background information

and are not the focus of the current dissertation. For example, intention could predict physical

activity and physical activity could further impact negative affect. However, negative affect

Page 15: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

6

could in turn moderate intention-behavior relations. The dashed arrows represent the gaps in the

literature that have not been explored fully, especially from the within-person perspective. For

example, the within-person association between physical activity and mindfulness, or their

potential interaction in predicting negative affect, has not been addressed in depth thus far. Using

this model as a guide, three studies were conducted to address key gaps in the literature.

Chapter 2 reports a systematic literature review of evidence regarding the association

between trait or state mindfulness and physical activity. This review (a) synthesized current

strength of research evidence between both forms of mindfulness and physical activity, (b)

identified key gaps in the literature, and (c) proposed a hierarchical framework to organize

mindfulness at different levels of analysis to improve conceptual clarity and precision.

Chapter 3 reports results from a 14-day experience sampling study conducted to evaluate

among daily activities/postures, mindfulness and negative affect among college students. This

study (a) separated the time-varying and time-invariant components of daily activities/postures

and mindfulness that regulate negative affect in college students, and (b) established the potential

interaction effect of daily activities/postures and momentary mindfulness in moderating negative

affect. These findings provide a foundation for the development of mindful movement

interventions to improve well-being.

Chapter 4 presents the results from a study investigating the feasibility of an outdoor

mindfulness-based walking program implemented with healthy, community-dwelling older

adults. This study (a) evaluated whether an outdoor mindful walking program was feasible,

acceptable, and sustainable to older adults in the community, and (b) examined whether this

intervention showed promise for reducing negative affect among older adults.

Page 16: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

7

These three studies fill important gaps in the literature on mindfulness and physical

activity. They address the need to disambiguate forms of state mindfulness in physical activity

research. They provide an example of how mindful movement can improve one indicator of

well-being. These results provide critical insights and generate new hypotheses to inform efforts

to integrate mindfulness and physical activity in ways that improve health and well-being.

Page 17: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

8

References

American Psychological Association. (2015). Stress in America. Retrieved May 2, 2016,

from http://www.apa.org/news/press/releases/stress/2014/highlights.aspx

Appling, S. E., Scarvalone, S., MacDonald, R., McBeth, M., & Helzlsouer, K. J. (2012).

Fatigue in breast cancer survivors: The impact of a mind-body medicine intervention. Oncology

Nursing Forum, 39(3), 278–286. https://doi.org/10.1188/12.ONF.278-286

Bandura, A. (2005). The primacy of self-regulation in health promotion. Applied

Psychology, 54(2), 245–254. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2005.00208.x

Best, J. R., Nagamatsu, L. S., & Liu-Ambrose, T. (2014). Improvements to executive

function during exercise training predict maintenance of physical activity over the following

year. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00353

Biddle, S. J. H., & Asare, M. (2011). Physical activity and mental health in children and

adolescents: A review of reviews. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 45(11), 886–895.

https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2011-090185

Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., …

Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science

and Practice, 11(3), 230–241. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bph077

Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its

role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

Buckley, J., Cohen, J. D., Kramer, A. F., McAuley, E., & Mullen, S. P. (2014). Cognitive

control in the self-regulation of physical activity and sedentary behavior. Frontiers in Human

Neuroscience, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00747

Page 18: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

9

Caspersen, C. J., Powell, K. E., & Christenson, G. M. (1985). Physical activity, exercise,

and physical fitness: Definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public Health

Reports, 100(2), 126–131.

Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Hagger, M. S. (2007). Mindfulness and the intention-behavior

relationship within the Theory of Planned Behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,

33(5), 663-676. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206297401

Cohen, S., Janicki-Deverts, D., & Miller, G. E. (2007). Psychological stress and disease.

Journal of the American Medical Association, 298(14), 1685–1687.

https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.298.14.1685

Cooney, G., Dwan, K., & Mead, G. (2014). Exercise for depression. JAMA, 311(23),

2432–2433. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2014.4930

Davis, D. M., & Hayes, J. A. (2011). What are the benefits of mindfulness? A practice

review of psychotherapy-related research. Psychotherapy, 48(2), 198–208.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022062

de Bruin, M., Sheeran, P., Kok, G., Hiemstra, A., Prins, J. M., Hospers, H. J., & P, J.

(2012). Self-regulatory processes mediate the intention-behavior relation for adherence and

exercise behaviors. Health Psychology, 31(6), 695–703. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027425

Dickenson, J., Berkman, E. T., Arch, J., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). Neural correlates of

focused attention during a brief mindfulness induction. Social Cognitive and Affective

Neuroscience, 8(1), 40. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss030

Dunton, G. F., Huh, J., Leventhal, A., Riggs, N., Spruijt-Metz, D., Pentz, M. A., &

Hedeker, D. (2014). Momentary assessment of affect, physical feeling states, and physical

activity in children. Health Psychology, 33(3), 255–263. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032640

Page 19: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

10

Ensari, I., Greenlee, T. A., Motl, R. W., & Petruzzello, S. J. (2015). Meta-analysis of

acute exercise effects on state anxiety: An update of randomized controlled trials over the past 25

years. Depression and Anxiety, 32(8), 624–634. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22370

Gilbert, D., & Waltz, J. (2010). Mindfulness and health behaviors. Mindfulness, 1(4),

227–234. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-010-0032-3

Goodwin, R. D. (2003). Association between physical activity and mental disorders

among adults in the United States. Preventive Medicine, 36(6), 698–703.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0091-7435(03)00042-2

Gotink, R. A., Chu, P., Busschbach, J. J. V., Benson, H., Fricchione, G. L., & Hunink, M.

G. M. (2015). Standardised mindfulness-based interventions in healthcare: An overview of

systematic reviews and meta-analyses of RCTs. PLOS ONE, 10(4), e0124344.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124344

Greeson, J. M. (2009). Mindfulness research update: 2008. Complementary Health

Practice Review, 14(1), 10–18. https://doi.org/10.1177/1533210108329862

Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H. (2004). Mindfulness-based stress

reduction and health benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 57(1), 35–

43. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00573-7

Hart, R., Ivtzan, I., & Hart, D. (2013). Mind the gap in mindfulness research: A

comparative account of the leading schools of thought. Review of General Psychology, 17(4),

453–466. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035212

Hyde, A. L., Conroy, D. E., Pincus, A. L., & Ram, N. (2011). Unpacking the feel good

effect of free-time physical activity: between- and within-person associations with pleasant-

activated feeling states. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 33(6), 884–902.

Page 20: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

11

Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies

subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2), 109–119.

https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.7.2.109

Kabat-Zinn, J. (2011). Mindfulness for beginners: Reclaiming the present moment–and

your life. Sounds True.

Kamijo, K., & Takeda, Y. (2010). Regular physical activity improves executive function

during task switching in young adults. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 75(3), 304–

311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.01.002

Keng, S.-L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of mindfulness on

psychological health: A review of empirical studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(6), 1041–

1056. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006

La Forge, R. (2016). Mind-body (mindful) exercise in practice. ACSM’s Health & Fitness

Journal, 20(4), 6–8.

Langer, E. J. (1992). Matters of mind: Mindfulness/mindlessness in perspective.

Consciousness and Cognition, 1(3), 289–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/1053-8100(92)90066-J

Lee, S. W., Mancuso, C. A., & Charlson, M. E. (2004). Prospective study of new

participants in a community-based mind-body training program. Journal of General Internal

Medicine, 19(7), 760–765. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1525-1497.2004.30011.x

Liao, Y., Shonkoff, E. T., & Dunton, G. F. (2015). The acute relationships between

affect, physical feeling states, and physical activity in daily life: A review of current evidence.

Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01975

Loucks, E. B., Schuman-Olivier, Z., Britton, W. B., Fresco, D. M., Desbordes, G.,

Brewer, J. A., & Fulwiler, C. (2015). Mindfulness and cardiovascular disease risk: State of the

Page 21: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

12

evidence, plausible mechanisms, and theoretical framework. Current Cardiology Reports,

17(12). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11886-015-0668-7

Mammen, G., & Faulkner, G. (2013). Physical activity and the prevention of depression:

A systematic review of prospective studies. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 45(5),

649–657. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2013.08.001

Paul, A., Cramer, H., Lauche, R., Altner, N., Langhorst, J., & Dobos, G. J. (2013). An

oncology mind-body medicine day care clinic: concept and case presentation. Integrative Cancer

Therapies, 12(6), 503–507. https://doi.org/10.1177/1534735412473639

Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee. (2008). Physical activity guidelines

advisory committee report. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services,

2008, A1-H14.

Rebar, A. L., Stanton, R., Geard, D., Short, C., Duncan, M. J., & Vandelanotte, C.

(2015). A meta-meta-analysis of the effect of physical activity on depression and anxiety in non-

clinical adult populations. Health Psychology Review, 9(3), 366–378.

https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2015.1022901

Rosenbaum, S., Tiedemann, A., Sherrington, C., Curtis, J., & Ward, P. B. (2014).

Physical activity interventions for people with mental illness: a systematic review and meta-

analysis. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 75(9), 964–974.

https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.13r08765

Schuch, F. B., Rocha, N., & Cadore, E. L. (2015). Progress in Physical activity and

Exercise and Affective and Anxiety Disorders: Translational Studies, Perspectives and Future

Directions. Frontiers Media SA.

Page 22: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

13

Schure, M. B., Christopher, J., & Christopher, S. (2008). Mind-body medicine and the art

of self-care: Teaching mindfulness to counseling students through yoga, meditation, and qigong.

Journal of Counseling and Development : JCD; Alexandria, 86(1), 47–56.

Sharma, M., & Rush, S. E. (2014). Mindfulness-based stress reduction as a stress

management intervention for healthy individuals: A systematic review. Journal of Evidence-

Based Complementary & Alternative Medicine, 19(4), 271–286.

https://doi.org/10.1177/2156587214543143

Snippe, E., Nyklíček, I., Schroevers, M. J., & Bos, E. H. (2015). The temporal order of

change in daily mindfulness and affect during mindfulness-based stress reduction. Journal of

Counseling Psychology, 62(2), 106–114. https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000057

Snippe, E., Simons, C. J. P., Hartmann, J. A., Menne-Lothmann, C., Kramer, I., Booij, S.

H., … Wichers, M. (2016). Change in daily life behaviors and depression: Within-person and

between-person associations. Health Psychology, 35(5), 433–441.

https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000312

Sofi, F., Valecchi, D., Bacci, D., Abbate, R., Gensini, G. F., Casini, A., & Macchi, C.

(2011). Physical activity and risk of cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies.

Journal of Internal Medicine, 269(1), 107–117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-

2796.2010.02281.x

Stults-Kolehmainen, M. A., & Sinha, R. (2013). The effects of stress on physical activity

and exercise. Sports Medicine, 44(1), 81–121. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-013-0090-5

Tang, Y.-Y., Jiang, C., & Tang, R. (2017). How mind-body practice works—integration

or separation? Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00866

Page 23: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

14

Tang, Y.-Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., … Posner, M. I. (2007). Short-

term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National

Academy of Sciences, 104(43), 17152–17156. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0707678104

Thompson, B. L., & Waltz, J. (2015). Everyday mindfulness and mindfulness meditation:

Overlapping constructs or not? Personality and Individual Differences, 43(7), 1875–1885.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.06.017

Ulmer, C. S., Stetson, B. A., & Salmon, P. G. (2010). Mindfulness and acceptance are

associated with exercise maintenance in YMCA exercisers. Behaviour Research and Therapy,

48(8), 805–809. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.04.009

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G. (1988). Positive and negative affectivity and their

relation to anxiety and depressive disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97(3), 346–353.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.97.3.346

Wells, R. E., Phillips, R. S., & McCarthy, E. P. (2011). Patterns of mind-body therapies

in adults with common neurological conditions. Neuroepidemiology, 36(1), 46–51.

https://doi.org/10.1159/000322949

Page 24: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

15

Figure Caption

Figure 1.1. Conceptual framework of the proposed studies designed in the dissertation.

Page 25: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

16

Page 26: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

17

Chapter 2: Mindfulness and Physical Activity: A Systematic Review and Hierarchical

Model of Mindfulness

Page 27: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

18

Abstract

Mindfulness has attracted recent interest for its potential to promote and sustain health

behaviors such as physical activity. It is conceptualized as an individual characteristic or mental

skill reflecting differences between people (trait) as well as differences within people across

contexts and time (state). Emerging empirical studies have investigated the association between

mindfulness and physical activity, but most have narrowly focused on trait mindfulness and not

examined state mindfulness as a time-varying construct. This paper reports a systematic review

of research on the association between mindfulness and physical activity. The available evidence

supported a positive association between trait mindfulness and physical activity and confirmed a

large gap in research on state mindfulness and physical activity. A three-level, hierarchical

framework – including global, contextual, and situational mindfulness – was proposed to

stimulate research on state mindfulness and physical activity by distinguishing between

momentary and context-specific levels of mindfulness. Available mindfulness measures

corresponding to each level of analysis were identified and reviewed to recommend

measurement options at each level. Finally, future research priorities were proposed based on the

three-level framework of mindfulness. These priorities include developing level-specific

mindfulness measurements based on a unifying conceptual model, conducting more longitudinal

and experimental studies that evaluate the dynamics of mindfulness, and translating emerging

evidence into interventions to promote physical activity and public health.

Page 28: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

19

Mindfulness and Physical Activity: A Systematic Review and Hierarchical Model of

Mindfulness

Physical inactivity is the fourth leading risk factor for global mortality in both men and

women (Bull & Bauman, 2011; Lee et al., 2011). The annual worldwide health care burden of

physical-inactivity has been conservatively estimated to exceed $500 billion (Ding et al., 2016).

Despite the well-known benefits of physical activity, a substantial proportion of the population

fails to meet physical activity guidelines (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2017).

Many who are motivated also fail to enact their intentions for physical activity (Rhodes & de

Bruijn, 2013). This inactivity problem involves environmental, policy, social, and intrapersonal

factors (Seefeldt, Malina, & Clark, 2012; Trost, Owen, Bauman, Sallis, & Brown, 2002).

Mindfulness, a form of heightened awareness and acceptance, is a modifiable intrapersonal

factor that has demonstrated a salutary influence on a variety of health behaviors including

eating, sleeping, and substance use (Greeson, 2009; Jordan, Wang, Donatoni, & Meier, 2014).

Mindfulness meditation has also been shown to improve immune function and lower

inflammatory disease risk (Creswell et al., 2016.; Davidson et al., 2003). Emerging evidence

suggests a positive association between mindfulness and overall cardiovascular health (Kennedy

& Resnick, 2015; Loucks, Britton, Howe, Eaton, & Buka, 2014). This systematic review

summarizes the available evidence on associations between mindfulness and physical activity,

identifies critical gaps in the literature, and proposes a framework to guide future research.

Mindfulness is conceived as modifiable because it can be trained and cultivated by

paying attention in a sustained and particular way (Kabat-Zinn, 2011). The two leading

contemporary schools of thought about mindfulness were advanced by Langer and Kabat-Zinn

(Hart, Ivtzan, & Hart, 2013). Langer considered mindfulness as both a state of conscious

Page 29: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

20

awareness and a state of openness to novelty (Langer, 1992). Kabat-Zinn, on the other hand,

described mindfulness as a process of bringing a certain quality of attention to one’s moment-by-

moment experience. This process requires paying attention in a very particular way: on purpose,

in the present moment, and nonjudgmentally (Kabat-Zinn, 1990, 1994). Bishop et al. (2004) later

integrated these definitions and conceived mindfulness as both a state and a mental skill, as well

as a constellation of personality traits that contribute to mindful states. The trait and state

versions of mindfulness have historically been conceptualized as separate constructs theoretically

(Davidson, 2010; Thompson & Waltz, 2015).

Individuals are assumed to have a dispositional capacity (i.e., trait) for mindfulness

across contexts. Momentary levels (i.e., state) of mindfulness vary over time during specific

activities and situations, regardless of one’s level of trait mindfulness (Brown & Ryan, 2003).

The established trait and state mindfulness model corresponds to the contrast involving between-

and within-person variation (Curran & Bauer, 2011), respectively. It is important to consider

both sources of variation to understand fully how mindfulness is associated with physical

activity.

The present paper reports a systematic review of literature on mindfulness and physical

activity. First, the available evidence linking physical activity with both trait and state

mindfulness is summarized. Second, key gaps in that literature are identified. Third, a

hierarchical model is proposed to improve conceptual clarity and precision when defining and

measuring mindfulness in physical activity studies. Measures at each level of analysis are

reviewed to develop recommendations for harmonizing assessments. Finally, priorities for

further research are proposed to improve our understanding for how mindfulness can be

implemented as a strategy to promote physical activity.

Page 30: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

21

Methods

Using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses

(PRISMA; Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altman, & Group, 2009) as a guide, we searched the

PubMed, PsycINFO, and Web of Science (Science Citation Index and Social Science Citation

Index) databases and Google Scholar (limited to the first 1000 articles) to identify relevant

research on mindfulness and physical activity. Inclusion criteria for the targeted studies included:

(1) peer-reviewed publication, (2) full-text of article is available, (3) published in English, (4)

original research, (5) human subject research, and (6) quantitative data for both mindfulness and

physical activity. Title and abstract field search was conducted in April 2017 using the following

search terms: (exercise OR sport OR sports OR “leisure activity” OR “physical activities” OR

“leisure activities” OR “physical activity” OR “health behavior” OR “health behaviour” OR

“health behaviors” OR “health behaviours”) AND (mindful* OR mindfulness-based OR

meditation OR mind-body OR "acceptance-based" OR self-compassion OR yoga). Key words

such as leisure activity, health behavior, acceptance-based, self-compassion, or yoga were

included in the search term because of their relevance to physical activity or mindfulness.

Articles were excluded out if they did not explicitly assess both mindfulness and physical

activity levels. No limitations were specified for the starting date in the search, so the earliest

articles were identified automatically by each database or search engine.

Results

As seen in Figure 1.1, a total of 1515 articles were first generated from four sources

based on our search term and inclusion criteria. After removing duplicates and screening for

eligibility, 13 articles were included in the current review (Butryn, Forman, Hoffman, Shaw, &

Page 31: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

22

Juarascio, 2011; Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2007; Gilbert & Waltz, 2010; Kangasniemi,

Lappalainen, Kankaanpää, & Tammelin, 2014; Loucks et al., 2014; Mothes, Klaperski, Seelig,

Schmidt, & Fuchs, 2014; Murphy, Mermelstein, Edwards, & Gidycz, 2012; Roberts & Danoff-

Burg, 2010; Ruffault, Bernier, Juge, & Fournier, 2015; Salmoirago-Blotcher, Hunsinger,

Morgan, Fischer, & Carmody, 2013; Sheeran, 2002; Tsafou, De Ridder, van Ee, & Lacroix,

2015; Tsafou, Lacroix, Ee, Vinkers, & Ridder, 2016; Ulmer, Stetson, & Salmon, 2010). Table

2.1 summarizes key characteristics of these articles.

Samples. Across the 13 articles, a total of 3891 participants were included, with one article

including two studies with separate samples (Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2007). The majority of

the studies included both female and male participants, except two studies that recruited only

female students (Butryn et al., 2011; Murphy et al., 2012) and one study that included only

young male adults (Mothes et al., 2014). All studies included participants who were age 18 or

older, but most of studies involved only undergraduates or middle-aged adults. Only three

studies included participants with a relatively wider age range (over 18 or between 18 to 65), but

the majority of the participants in these studies were middle-aged adults (Tsafou et al., 2015,

2016; Ulmer et al., 2010).

Study design. Nine of the 13 studies utilized cross-sectional study designs. One longitudinal

study observed undergraduate students’ levels of mindfulness and physical activity at the

beginning and the end of a 10-week semester (Murphy et al., 2012). Two randomized controlled

trials were conducted to identify whether exercise training could enhance mindfulness in middle-

age male adults (Mothes et al., 2014), and whether an Acceptance and Commitment Therapy

(ACT) with mindfulness components could increase physical activity in young female adults

(Butryn et al., 2011). Finally, one intervention study with a single group pre-post design was

Page 32: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

23

conducted to test if an 8-week mindfulness-based stress reduction program (MBSR) increased

physical activity (Salmoirago-Blotcher, Hunsinger, Morgan, Fischer, & Carmody, 2013).

Mindfulness measures. Self-report measures were used to assess mindfulness in all studies. Six

different mindfulness scales were identified: the Five-Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire

(FFMQ; Baer et al., 2008), Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003),

Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale (PHLMS; Cardaciotto, Herbert, Forman, Moitra, & Farrow,

2008), Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS; Baer, Smith, & Allen, 2004), Frieberg

Mindfulness Inventory (FMI; Walach, Buchheld, Buttenmüller, Kleinknecht, & Schmidt, 2006),

and Mindfulness in Physical Activity Questionnaire (MFPA; Tsafou et al., 2015). The MAAS

(used in eight studies including two with Dutch versions) and FFMQ (used in four studies) were

the most frequently used instruments to assess trait mindfulness. Three studies used more than

one mindfulness scale to assess both state and trait mindfulness. In one study, the MAAS and

FMI were used to assess the trait and the state mindfulness, respectively (Ulmer et al., 2010). In

the other two studies, MAAS and FFMQ were used to assess trait mindfulness, and the MFPA

was designed to measure mindfulness in the context of physical activities (Tsafou et al., 2015,

2016).

Physical activity measures. Measures for physical activity were more heterogeneous but 12 of

them used self-report measures. The International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ; Craig

et al., 2003) was the most common measure being applied in six studies. Other self-report

measures included the Godin Leisure-Time Exercise Questionnaire (GLTEQ; Godin &

Shephard, 1985) and the Physical Activity, Exercise, and Sport Questionnaire (Fuchs, Klaperski,

Gerber, & Seelig, 2015). Researchers used selected items from Rapid Assessment of Physical

Activity questionnaire (RAPA; Topolski et al., 2006), Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance System,

Page 33: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

24

and the Weight and Lifestyle Inventory (WALI; Wadden & Foster, 2006) to assess weekly, daily

physical activity, and daily lifestyle activity. One study used the number of athletic center visits

as proxy for PA frequency (Butryn et al., 2011). Only one study utilized accelerometers to

measure physical activity (Kangasniemi et al., 2014). In addition to assessing physical activity

level, several studies also assessed PA habit strength, exercise maintenance, and satisfaction with

physical activity via self-report questionnaires (Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2007; Tsafou et al.,

2015, 2016; Ulmer et al., 2010).

Trait mindfulness and physical activity

Cross-sectional studies largely indicated a positive association between trait mindfulness

and physical activity. Trait mindfulness was positively associated with higher frequency and

total volume of physical activity among college students (Gilbert & Waltz, 2010; Roberts &

Danoff-Burg, 2010; Tsafou et al., 2015, 2016). Trait mindfulness was also positively associated

with physical activity maintenance, achievement of exercise goals, physical activity enjoyment,

and satisfaction with physical activity (Loucks et al., 2014; Roberts & Danoff-Burg, 2010;

Tsafou et al., 2015; Ulmer et al., 2010). A positive association between mindfulness and device-

measured physical activity was observed in one study, suggesting that active people are more

mindful than less active people (Kangasniemi et al., 2014). One longitudinal study did not find a

significant association between trait mindfulness and frequency of leisure-time exercise at either

the beginning or end of a 10-week study period (Murphy et al., 2012).

A single-group intervention study revealed that the proportion of inactive participants

decreased after an eight-week MBSR program, and participants increased their overall strength

or flexibility (Salmoirago-Blotcher et al., 2013). The correlation between changes in trait

mindfulness scores was not significantly related to changes in physical activity in that study. The

Page 34: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

25

strongest evidence for a positive association between trait mindfulness and physical activity

came from two randomized controlled trials. In the first trial, sedentary male employees were

randomly assigned to either 12-week aerobic exercise group, relaxation training group, or a

waitlist control group (Mothes et al., 2014). The aerobic exercise group increased trait

mindfulness, but the relaxation training and control groups did not increase in trait mindfulness.

In the second study, female students who completed Acceptance-Commitment Therapy sessions

in groups visited the athletic center more frequently over a five-week period than their peers in a

physical activity education group (Butryn et al., 2011). Associations were not reported between

changes in trait mindfulness and the frequency of athletic center visits from baseline through the

end of intervention and the one-month follow-up.

Trait mindfulness may also moderate relations between motivation and action. In one

cross-sectional study, intentions positively predicted physical activity for students who were

more, but not less, trait mindful. Additionally, those who acted more mindfully and less

habitually were more likely to sustain their physical activity intentions than those who acted less

mindfully and more habitually (Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2007). Tsafou et al. (2015) later

replicated this finding by showing that trait mindfulness was only associated with physical

activity among people with weak physical activity habits. Another cross-sectional study

indicated that trait mindfulness moderated relations between intrinsic motivation and physical

activity. Compared to medium or lower levels of trait mindfulness, people with higher levels of

trait mindfulness had a stronger association between their intrinsic motivation and physical

activity (Ruffault et al., 2015).

State mindfulness and physical activity association

Page 35: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

26

Two cross-sectional studies examined physical activity -specific state mindfulness and its

association with physical activity. In both, increased state mindfulness in physical activity

settings was associated with greater physical activity and this association was mediated by

satisfaction towards physical activity (Tsafou et al., 2015, 2016). The Tsafou (2016) study

further showed that both state mindfulness and satisfaction with physical activity mediated the

positive association between trait mindfulness and physical activity. Another cross-sectional

study revealed that YMCA exercisers who reported higher levels of state mindfulness had

stronger beliefs that they could successfully meet their exercise goals (Ulmer et al., 2010). On

the other hand, exercisers who reported missing one week of exercise sessions in the past year

had lower state mindfulness; however, the association between state mindfulness (as measured

by Frieberg Mindfulness Inventory) and physical activity was not clear. This result may be

partially explained by the fact that items from the Frieberg Mindfulness Inventory ask about

more advanced mindfulness experiences, and are considered inappropriate for people who are

not familiar with or have no experience with mindfulness practices (Bergomi, Tschacher, &

Kupper, 2012b). No study assessed momentary mindfulness level and its relation to physical

activity based on the current review.

Discussion

This review examined research on mindfulness and physical activity. Findings revealed

that samples have largely been limited to young and middle-aged adults. No studies to date

sampled younger (younger than 18 years) or older adult (older than 65 years) populations. Study

designs were typically cross-sectional with self-report measures of both mindfulness and

physical activity. Researchers used a variety of measures to assess mindfulness and physical

Page 36: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

27

activity, with the MAAS and the IPAQ being the most popular option for mindfulness and

physical activity, respectively.

Previous research has examined both the direct association between mindfulness and

physical activity, and the role of mindfulness in moderating relations between motivational

processes and physical activity. All 13 studies included in this review assessed trait or

dispositional mindfulness, and 10 of them found significant positive associations between trait

mindfulness and physical activity. In contrast, only three studies assessed state mindfulness in

specific exercise or physical activity contexts, and two of them found that higher state

mindfulness could predict physical activity level (Tsafou et al., 2015, 2016). Collectively, the

evidence suggests that both trait and state mindfulness are associated with greater physical

activity. Also, trait mindfulness may help to bridge the intention-behavior gap for physical

activity, so interventions that cultivate mindfulness may be important component of programs to

address this persistent barrier to behavior change (Rhodes & Dickau, 2012; Sheeran, 2002).

In terms of the strength of the evidence, 11 out of the 13 studies used observational or

cross-sectional designs, so it is not possible to make strong statements about the existence or

direction of causal relations between mindfulness and physical activity in most of the reviewed

studies. Two studies applied experimental methods to test whether mindfulness impacted

physical activity or vice versa, but neither study included mixed gender samples which limited

their generalizability. It should also be noted that one of the randomized controlled trials

operationalized physical activity as the frequency of athletic center visits (Butryn et al., 2011).

This measure may not be sensitive to variability in the duration or intensity of physical activity in

that context, or to the frequency, duration or intensity of physical activity in other contexts.

Overall, based on the prevalence of cross-sectional designs and the limitations of the few

Page 37: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

28

randomized controlled trials identified, it is not possible to draw strong causal inferences about

the direction of relations between mindfulness and physical activity.

Gap in state mindfulness and physical activity research

With respect to state mindfulness, no research was identified on relations between

momentary mindfulness levels and physical activity. There is a clear gap in our understanding of

how state mindfulness influences thoughts, feelings and actions associated with physical activity.

Generalizing findings about research on trait mindfulness to predict how mindfulness in a

particular context or at a particular moment will impact people’s behavior is risky. People

typically do not act, think or feel the same way in all contexts or even on all occasions within a

context (Fleeson, 2001). Likewise, mindfulness in a particular context may differ from

mindfulness in other contexts or even across occasions within that original context (Brown &

Ryan, 2003). State mindfulness experienced during physical activity may be similar to or

different from state mindfulness during activities that do not involve physical activity (or even

from one physical activity episode to another). In addition to context-specific mindfulness,

people can also experience different levels of state mindfulness from moment to moment in their

everyday life (Thompson & Waltz, 2015). Without disentangling contextual and temporal

variability in state mindfulness, it is not possible to draw clear conclusions about context- and

occasion-specific mindfulness experiences. This confound could present a barrier to

understanding relations between mindfulness and physical activity. As research on state

mindfulness and physical activity develops, it will be important to differentiate between context-

specific states and momentary states with a more refined framework.

The three-level hierarchical mindfulness framework

Page 38: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

29

A hierarchical, three-level model can be applied to differentiate between forms of state

mindfulness based on the contextual- and temporal-specificity of the phenomenon. A similar

approach was applied profitably to advance research on intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in

sport and exercise (Vallerand, 1997, 2007a, 2007b). This hierarchical framework differentiates

three levels of mindfulness: global, contextual, and situational.

Global mindfulness represents a general disposition towards mindfulness across the

varied contexts and moments of daily life. In that sense, it represents a person’s default level of

mindfulness (all else being equal). It is similar to trait mindfulness in the two-component (trait-

state) model because it is relatively stable across time and circumstances. Contextual mindfulness

represents a person’s typical level of mindfulness within a specific life context (e.g., while

exercising). It represents a person’s default level of mindfulness at any point of time in a

particular context but may vary from one context to another. Situational mindfulness represents

the level of mindfulness that an individual experiences during a specific activity at a specific

moment. That is, situational mindfulness varies as a function of both context and time. These

three levels of mindfulness should be differentiated to capture the whole spectrum of

mindfulness as it relates to physical activity.

The major difference between the existing two-component model and the proposed

hierarchical framework of mindfulness is that the latter decomposes state mindfulness into

contextual and momentary components. This distinction reduces conceptual fuzziness and should

lead to improved assessments, less ambiguous evidence, and accelerated theory development. A

context-specific mindfulness construct is needed because some life contexts or activities may

elicit more mindfulness than others. For instance, people who feel more involved in the natural

environment have reported higher levels of trait mindfulness and well-being (Howell, Dopko,

Page 39: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

30

Passmore, & Buro, 2011; Wolsko & Lindberg, 2013) Likewise, activities that direct people to the

internal incorporation of regulated breathing and focused attention, such as yoga, are believed to

evoke more mindfulness than other physical activities (La Forge, 1997). These observations

become specific, testable questions if contextual mindfulness is differentiated and assessed.

The hierarchical framework also postulates bidirectional relations across the three levels

(Vallerand, 1997, 2007a, 2007b). Top-down effects describe how mindfulness at more abstract

levels in the hierarchy can influence mindfulness at more specific levels (e.g., global mindfulness

influencing contextual mindfulness, or contextual mindfulness influencing situational

mindfulness). Bottom-up effects describe how mindfulness at more specific levels can

accumulate to influence more abstract levels over time (e.g., increases in situational mindfulness

within a context accumulating to increase corresponding contextual mindfulness). The Top-down

principle is consistent with Brown and Ryan’s (2003) study showing that people who had higher

global mindfulness were more likely to express higher situational mindfulness, when assessed by

both the trait and state versions of Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale. The Bottom-up

principle has also been partially supported in a longitudinal study showing that over time,

increases in global mindfulness level were positively predicted by their rates of change in

contextual mindfulness during meditation practice (Kiken, Garland, Bluth, Palsson, & Gaylord,

2015).

Organizing mindfulness measures within the three-level hierarchical framework

To realize the potential of this hierarchical framework, researchers need instruments to

measure mindfulness validly at each level. The majority of existing scales have been used to

assess individual differences in global mindfulness (Bergomi et al., 2012b; Sauer et al., 2012).

Only a few scales are available for measuring either contextual or situational mindfulness. Table

Page 40: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

31

2.2 summaries the major measures used in contemporary research on mindfulness (including

research on mindfulness and physical activity) according to the three levels in the proposed

hierarchical framework of mindfulness.

The eight available global mindfulness measures in Table 2.2 generally demonstrate

acceptable validity and reliability (Baer, 2011). They differ in the presumed latent structure of

mindfulness, the target populations for which the measures were developed, and the scope of

meditation experience required to respond to the items (Sauer et al., 2012). These differences

present a challenge when comparing and replicating research findings (Bergomi, Tschacher, &

Kupper, 2012a; Bergomi et al., 2012b). The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale has been used

most frequently in research on physical activity, followed by the Five Facet Mindfulness

Questionnaire. The main difference between the two measures is that the former assesses a

unidimensional mindfulness structure, whereas the latter measures a multidimensional

mindfulness structure and thus requires more items and time to complete.

Measures of contextual mindfulness are more limited with only three options specific for

sport or physical activity contexts. Scores from the Mindfulness Inventory for Sport have

psychometric support from factor analysis and evidence of convergent validity with related

constructs (Thienot et al., 2014). The State Mindfulness Scale for physical activity is an

adaptation of the State Mindfulness Scale (Cox, Ullrich-French, & French, 2016; Tanay &

Bernstein, 2013). Scores from State Mindfulness Scale measure have demonstrated convergent

validity with scores from the Toronto Mindfulness Scale and showed mean score differences

between groups with and without yoga experience. To the best of our knowledge, the six-item

Mindfulness in Physical Activity scale (Tsafou, De Ridder, van Ee, & Lacroix, 2015) is the only

contextual mindfulness scale that has been used in more than one empirical study to measure

Page 41: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

32

mindfulness in the physical activity context. Neither the validity nor the reliability evidence of

scores from this questionnaire were reported in those studies (Tsafou et al., 2015, 2016).

Assessment options for situational mindfulness are also limited. None of the measures in

Table 2.2 have been used to assess situational mindfulness during physical activity or even while

contemplating physical activity. The Toronto Mindfulness Scale was designed to assess

mindfulness as a state that can vary across short periods of time; however, in practice, it has been

used exclusively to measure an individual's level of mindfulness during a formal meditation

session which requires participants to first sit quietly and pay attention to their breath for a short

period of time before responding. This scale has not been widely applied to assess situational

mindfulness in different settings (Feldman et al., 2006). The State-Mindful Attention and

Awareness Scale is a five-item scale derived from Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale which

assesses an individual’s mindfulness over a short period of time (Brown & Ryan, 2003). The

main limitation of this short scale is that it mainly samples individuals’ conscious awareness or

attention of mindfulness and lacks items for assessing physical and mental experiences that are

also central components of mindfulness. For example, it does not involve items measuring the

awareness of one’s emotional states, physical sensations, and passing thoughts (Tanay &

Bernstein, 2013). As a result, construct validity remains a concern when using the State-Mindful

Attention and Awareness Scale to assess situational mindfulness. Lastly, the State Mindfulness

Scale measures mindfulness during a specific time period (e.g., the preceding 15 minutes) and in

a particular context (e.g., meditation or other types of activities). Strong evidence exists for the

construct and incremental predictive validity of scores from this scale. It has been rigorously

tested in its developmental phase and corresponds well with the concept of situational

mindfulness (Tanay & Bernstein, 2013). Despite the superiority of this tool relative to other

Page 42: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

33

options, it has not yet been used widely in research on mindfulness generally or in research on

mindfulness and physical activity specifically.

In sum, the majority of mindfulness measures were designed to assess global

mindfulness, and approximately half of these measures have been applied in research on physical

activity. Fewer scales with known psychometric properties are available for assessing situational

or contextual mindfulness, and these measures have rarely been used in physical activity

research. Based on the brief summary of the mindfulness measures, the best available choices for

measuring global, contextual and situational mindfulness in physical activity research (at the

present time) are likely to be the Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale, State Mindfulness

Scale-PA, and State Mindfulness Scale, respectively. The Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale

is by far the most frequently used measure in physical activity research as well as the most

accepted and broadly-applied global mindfulness measure (Sauer et al., 2012). Further, State

Mindfulness Scale-PA is derived from the State Mindfulness Scale and shares a common

underlying conceptual model. The shared conceptual model underlying these measures will

facilitate meaningful inferences about mindfulness and physical activity relations between the

contextual and situational levels. Scores from these three instruments are all well-validated and

compatible with unidimensional definitions of mindfulness.

Harmonizing future measures based on conceptual and structural consistency would

facilitate comparisons across levels of analysis in research on physical activity. No single

measure is capable of capturing all three levels of mindfulness. The three-level framework

described above can be applied to develop precise hypotheses, select measures, and refine

theories about the role of mindfulness in physical activity promotion.

Future directions

Page 43: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

34

This review suggested several priorities for future research on mindfulness and physical

activity. First, the bulk of this evidence derives from research on global mindfulness and uses

cross-sectional research designs. Future work should emphasize the contextual and situational

levels of mindfulness to fill an identified gap and determine whether conclusions about a positive

association between global (trait) mindfulness and physical activity generalize to the contextual

and situational levels. Future work should also investigate whether contextual and situational

mindfulness can bridge the intention-behavior gap that has also been revealed as a within-person

phenomenon (Conroy, Elavsky, Doerksen, & Maher, 2013; Conroy, Elavsky, Hyde, & Doerksen,

2011; Maher, Dzubur, Huh, Intille, & Dunton, 2016). To conduct this work, it would be valuable

to develop parallel measures for assessing mindfulness at each level of analysis. This work will

also benefit from study designs and measures that are capable of capturing both the time-varying

nature of situational mindfulness and the context-dependent nature of contextual mindfulness.

Ecological momentary assessment is an optimal approach that has been suggested to obtain

mindfulness or physical activity in daily settings (Davidson & Kaszniak, 2015; Robbins, Kubiak,

& Mostofsky, 2014), and should be applied more in future research to test the real-time

association between mindfulness and physical activity. Sampling mindfulness during a variety of

activities or settings will also be useful for addressing the confounding of mindfulness practices

with a specific type of activity (i.e., yoga, walking).

Experimental evidence is needed to draw causal inferences about mindfulness and

physical activity. The associations between mindfulness and physical activity reported here were

based on college student and mid-age adult populations, and predominately use self-report

measurements. Future experimental studies should target understudied populations (e.g.,

adolescents, older adults), and include objective measures of mindfulness (i.e., cognitive and

Page 44: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

35

attentional tasks) and device-based measures of physical activity (i.e., accelerometers). As this

literature develops, researchers will need to investigate questions about dose-response

association, the best modes of mindfulness practice, and the potential mechanism and moderators

in mindfulness and physical activity.

The population-level inactivity problem also involves prolonged sedentary behavior (i.e.,

excessive sitting time). Relations between mindfulness and sedentary behavior have not been

investigated to date. Sedentary behavior has a habitual component but it also varies within

people over time and is negatively associated with fluctuations in intentions to limit sedentary

time (Conroy, Maher, Elavsky, Hyde, & Doerksen, 2013). The ability to be more aware of

thoughts, emotions and physical sensations in a given moment, which is a key characteristic of

mindfulness, may be a potential strategy for helping people to disengage from habitual behavior

patterns in different contexts and situations.

Conclusion

This review synthesized current evidence addressing the positive association between

mindfulness and physical activity, and proposed a hierarchical model to disambiguate mindful

states in future research. A similar hierarchical framework has been applied profitably to

organize research on intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (Vallerand, 2007a). Existing measures

corresponding to each level of mindfulness were summarized, and best-in-class measures of

mindfulness at each level were identified. Harmonizing measures in future research would

facilitate comparisons of research findings and accelerate understanding of relations between

mindfulness and physical activity.

The organizing hierarchical framework outlined in this review will also generate new

hypotheses that can inform intervention development. Of particular interest is the potential for

Page 45: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

36

contextual and situational mindfulness to serve as self-regulatory tools for shielding motivation

and reducing interference from factors that often derail physical activity. If this potential is borne

out, mindfulness training may be a powerful tool for promoting physical activity and improving

public health.

Page 46: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

37

References

Baer, R. A. (2011). Measuring mindfulness. Contemporary Buddhism, 12(1), 241–261.

https://doi.org/10.1080/14639947.2011.564842

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-

report The Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills. Assessment, 11(3), 191–206.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191104268029

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E., Button, D., Krietemeyer, J., Sauer, S., … Williams,

J. M. G. (2008). Construct validity of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire in meditating

and nonmeditating samples. Assessment, 15(3), 329–342.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191107313003

Bergomi, C., Tschacher, W., & Kupper, Z. (2012a). Measuring mindfulness: First steps

towards the development of a comprehensive mindfulness scale. Mindfulness, 4(1), 18–32.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0102-9

Bergomi, C., Tschacher, W., & Kupper, Z. (2012b). The assessment of mindfulness with

self-report measures: Existing scales and open issues. Mindfulness, 4(3), 191–202.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0110-9

Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., …

Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science

and Practice, 11(3), 230–241. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bph077

Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its

role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

Page 47: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

38

Bull, F. C., & Bauman, A. E. (2011). Physical inactivity: the “Cinderella” risk factor for

noncommunicable disease prevention. Journal of Health Communication, 16 Suppl 2, 13–26.

https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2011.601226

Butryn, M. L., Forman, E., Hoffman, K., Shaw, J., & Juarascio, A. (2011). A pilot study

of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy for promotion of physical activity. Journal of Physical

Activity and Health, 8(4), 516–522. https://doi.org/10.1123/jpah.8.4.516

Cardaciotto, L., Herbert, J. D., Forman, E. M., Moitra, E., & Farrow, V. (2008). The

assessment of present-moment awareness and acceptance The Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale.

Assessment, 15(2), 204–223. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191107311467

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2017). Facts about Physical Activity.

Retrieved May 16, 2017, from https://www.cdc.gov/physicalactivity/data/facts.htm

Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Hagger, M. S. (2007). Mindfulness and the intention-behavior

relationship within the Theory of Planned Behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206297401

Conroy, D. E., Elavsky, S., Doerksen, S. E., & Maher, J. P. (2013). A daily process

analysis of intentions and physical activity in college students. Journal of Sport & Exercise

Psychology, 35(5), 493–502.

Conroy, D. E., Elavsky, S., Hyde, A. L., & Doerksen, S. E. (2011). The dynamic nature

of physical activity intentions: A within-person perspective on intention-behavior coupling.

Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 33(6), 807–827. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.33.6.807

Conroy, D. E., Maher, J. P., Elavsky, S., Hyde, A. L., & Doerksen, S. E. (2013).

Sedentary behavior as a daily process regulated by habits and intentions. Health Psychology,

32(11), 1149–1157. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031629

Page 48: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

39

Cox, A. E., Ullrich-French, S., & French, B. F. (2016). Validity evidence for the State

Mindfulness Scale for Physical Activity. Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise

Science, 20(1), 38–49. https://doi.org/10.1080/1091367X.2015.1089404

Craig, C. L., Marshall, A. L., Sjostrom, M., Bauman, A. E., Booth, M. L., Ainsworth, B.

E., … Oja, P. (2003). International Physical Activity Questionnaire: 12-country reliability and

validity. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 35(8), 1381–1395.

https://doi.org/10.1249/01.MSS.0000078924.61453.FB

Creswell, J. D., Taren, A. A., Lindsay, E. K., Greco, C. M., Gianaros, P. J., Fairgrieve,

A., … Ferris, J. L. (2016). Alterations in resting state functional connectivity link mindfulness

meditation with reduced interleukin-6: a randomized controlled trial. Biological Psychiatry.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.01.008

Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2011). The disaggregation of within-person and between-

person effects in longitudinal models of change. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 583–619.

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100356

Davidson, R. J. (2010). Empirical explorations of mindfulness: Conceptual and

methodological conundrums. Emotion, 10(1), 8–11. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018480

Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli,

S. F., … Sheridan, J. F. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune function produced by

mindfulness meditation: Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(4), 564–570.

https://doi.org/10.1097/01.PSY.0000077505.67574.E3

Davidson, R. J., & Kaszniak, A. W. (2015). Conceptual and methodological issues in

research on mindfulness and meditation. American Psychologist, 70(7), 581–592.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039512

Page 49: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

40

Ding, D., Lawson, K. D., Kolbe-Alexander, T. L., Finkelstein, E. A., Katzmarzyk, P. T.,

van Mechelen, W., & Pratt, M. (2016). The economic burden of physical inactivity: a global

analysis of major non-communicable diseases. The Lancet. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-

6736(16)30383-X

Feldman, G., Hayes, A., Kumar, S., Greeson, J., & Laurenceau, J.-P. (2006). Mindfulness

and emotion regulation: The development and initial validation of the Cognitive and Affective

Mindfulness Scale-Revised (CAMS-R). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,

29(3), 177–190. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-006-9035-8

Fleeson. (2001). Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: traits as

density distribution of states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(6), 1011–1027.

Fuchs, R., Klaperski, S., Gerber, M., & Seelig, H. (2015). Messung der bewegungs- und

sportaktivität mit dem BSA-Fragebogen: Eine methodische zwischenbilanz. Zeitschrift für

Gesundheitspsychologie, 23(2), 60–76. https://doi.org/10.1026/0943-8149/a000137

Gilbert, D., & Waltz, J. (2010). Mindfulness and health behaviors. Mindfulness, 1(4),

227–234. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-010-0032-3

Godin, G., & Shephard, R. J. (1985). A simple method to assess exercise behavior in the

community. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sciences. Journal Canadien Des Sciences

Appliquées Au Sport, 10(3), 141–146.

Greeson, J. M. (2009). Mindfulness research update: 2008. Complementary Health

Practice Review, 14(1), 10–18. https://doi.org/10.1177/1533210108329862

Hart, R., Ivtzan, I., & Hart, D. (2013). Mind the gap in mindfulness research: A

comparative account of the leading schools of thought. Review of General Psychology, 17(4),

453–466. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035212

Page 50: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

41

Hewett, Z. L., Ransdell, L. B., Gao, Y., Petlichkoff, L. M., & Lucas, S. (2011). An

examination of the effectiveness of an 8-week Bikram yoga program on mindfulness, perceived

Stress, and physical fitness. Journal of Exercise Science & Fitness, 9(2), 87–92.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S1728-869X(12)60003-3

Howell, A. J., Dopko, R. L., Passmore, H.-A., & Buro, K. (2011). Nature connectedness:

Associations with well-being and mindfulness. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(2),

166–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.037

Jordan, C. H., Wang, W., Donatoni, L., & Meier, B. P. (2014). Mindful eating: Trait and

state mindfulness predict healthier eating behavior. Personality and Individual Differences,

68(1), 107–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.04.013

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full Catastrophe Living: Using the Wisdom of Your Body and

Mind to Face Stress, Pain, and Illness (15 Abr Anv edition). New York: Bantam Doubleday Dell

Publishing.

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever You Go, There You Are: Mindfulness Meditation in

Everyday Life. New York: Hyperion.

Kabat-Zinn, J. (2011). Mindfulness for beginners: Reclaiming the present moment–and

your life. Boulder: Sounds True.

Kangasniemi, A., Lappalainen, R., Kankaanpää, A., & Tammelin, T. (2014). Mindfulness

skills, psychological flexibility, and psychological symptoms among physically less active and

active adults. Mental Health and Physical Activity, 7(3), 121–127.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2014.06.005

Kennedy, A. B., & Resnick, P. B. (2015). Mindfulness and physical activity. American

Journal of Lifestyle Medicine, 9(3), 221–223. https://doi.org/10.1177/1559827614564546

Page 51: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

42

Kiken, L. G., Garland, E. L., Bluth, K., Palsson, O. S., & Gaylord, S. A. (2015). From a

state to a trait: Trajectories of state mindfulness in meditation during intervention predict

changes in trait mindfulness. Personality and Individual Differences.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.044

La Forge, R. (1997). Mind-body fitness: Encouraging prospects for primary and

secondary prevention. The Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing, 11(3), 53–65.

https://doi.org/10.1097/00005082-199704000-00006

Langer, E. J. (1992). Matters of mind: Mindfulness/mindlessness in perspective.

Consciousness and Cognition, 1(3), 289–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/1053-8100(92)90066-J

Lee, D.-C., Sui, X., Ortega, F. B., Kim, Y.-S., Church, T. S., Winett, R. A., … Blair, S.

N. (2011). Comparisons of leisure-time physical activity and cardiorespiratory fitness as

predictors of all-cause mortality in men and women. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 45(6),

504–510. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2009.066209

Loucks, E. B., Britton, W. B., Howe, C. J., Eaton, C. B., & Buka, S. L. (2014). Positive

associations of dispositional mindfulness with cardiovascular health: the New England Family

Study. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-014-

9448-9

Maher, J. P., Dzubur, E., Huh, J., Intille, S., & Dunton, G. F. (2016). Within-day time-

varying associations between behavioral cognitions and physical activity in adults. Journal of

Sport and Exercise Psychology, 38(4), 423–434. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2016-0058

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., & Group, T. P. (2009). Preferred

reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLOS

Medicine, 6(7), e1000097. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

Page 52: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

43

Mothes, H., Klaperski, S., Seelig, H., Schmidt, S., & Fuchs, R. (2014). Regular aerobic

exercise increases dispositional mindfulness in men: A randomized controlled trial. Mental

Health and Physical Activity, 7(2), 111–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2014.02.003

Murphy, M. J., Mermelstein, L. C., Edwards, K. M., & Gidycz, C. A. (2012). The

benefits of dispositional mindfulness in physical health: A longitudinal study of female college

students. Journal of American College Health, 60(5), 341–348.

https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2011.629260

Patrick, D. L., Williams, B., Walwick, J., & Patrick, M. B. (2006). The rapid assessment

of physical activity (RAPA) among older adults. Retrieved from

https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/49e8/6eb094fdda1c12d9ff5a03e70880ac26f4fc.pdf

Rhodes, R. E., & de Bruijn, G.-J. (2013). What predicts intention-behavior discordance?

A review of the action control framework. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 41(4), 201–207.

Rhodes, R. E., & Dickau, L. (2012). Experimental evidence for the intention–behavior

relationship in the physical activity domain: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 31(6), 724–

727. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027290

Robbins, M. L., Kubiak, T., & Mostofsky, D. I. (2014). Ecological monetary assessment

in behavioral medicine: research and practice. The Handbook of Behavioral Medicine, 46.

Retrieved from http://observelab.ucr.edu/wp-content/uploads/2014/09/Robbins-Kubiak-2014-

EMA-chapter.pdf

Roberts, & Danoff-Burg. (2010). Mindfulness and health behaviors: Is paying attention

good for you? Journal of American College Health, 59(3), 165–173.

https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2010.484452

Page 53: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

44

Ruffault, A., Bernier, M., Juge, N., & Fournier, J. F. (2015). Mindfulness may moderate

the relationship between intrinsic motivation and physical activity: A cross-sectional study.

Mindfulness, 7(2), 445–452. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-015-0467-7

Salmoirago-Blotcher, E., Hunsinger, M., Morgan, L., Fischer, D., & Carmody, J. (2013).

Mindfulness-based stress reduction and change in health-related behaviors. Journal of Evidence-

Based Complementary & Alternative Medicine, 2156587213488600.

https://doi.org/10.1177/2156587213488600

Sauer, S., Walach, H., Schmidt, S., Hinterberger, T., Lynch, S., Büssing, A., & Kohls, N.

(2012). Assessment of mindfulness: Review on state of the art. Mindfulness, 4(1), 3–17.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0122-5

Seefeldt, V., Malina, R. M., & Clark, M. A. (2012). Factors affecting levels of physical

activity in adults. Sports Medicine, 32(3), 143–168. https://doi.org/10.2165/00007256-

200232030-00001

Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention-behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review.

European Review of Social Psychology, 12(1), 1–36.

https://doi.org/10.1080/14792772143000003

Tanay, G., & Bernstein, A. (2013). State Mindfulness Scale (SMS): Development and

initial validation. Psychological Assessment, 25(4), 1286–1299.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034044

Thienot, E., Jackson, B., Dimmock, J., Grove, J. R., Bernier, M., & Fournier, J. F. (2014).

Development and preliminary validation of the Mindfulness Inventory for Sport. Psychology of

Sport and Exercise, 15(1), 72–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.10.003

Page 54: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

45

Thompson, B. L., & Waltz, J. (2015). Everyday mindfulness and mindfulness meditation:

Overlapping constructs or not? Personality and Individual Differences, 43(7), 1875–1885.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.06.017

Trost, S. G., Owen, N., Bauman, A. E., Sallis, J. F., & Brown, W. (2002). Correlates of

adults’ participation in physical activity: review and update. Medicine and Science in Sports and

Exercise, 34(12), 1996–2001. https://doi.org/10.1249/01.MSS.0000038974.76900.92

Tsafou, K.-E., De Ridder, D. T., van Ee, R., & Lacroix, J. P. (2015). Mindfulness and

satisfaction in physical activity: A cross-sectional study in the Dutch population. Journal of

Health Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105314567207

Tsafou, K.-E., Lacroix, J. P., Ee, R. van, Vinkers, C. D., & Ridder, D. T. D. (2016). The

relation of trait and state mindfulness with satisfaction and physical activity: A cross-sectional

study in 305 Dutch participants. Journal of Health Psychology, 1359105315624748.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105315624748

Ulmer, C. S., Stetson, B. A., & Salmon, P. G. (2010). Mindfulness and acceptance are

associated with exercise maintenance in YMCA exercisers. Behaviour Research and Therapy,

48(8), 805–809. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.04.009

Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic

motivation. In Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 29 (pp. 271–360). San Diego,

CA, US: Academic Press.

Vallerand, R. J. (2007a). A hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation for

sport and physical activity. In M. S. Hagger & N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic

motivation and self-determination in exercise and sport (pp. 255–363). Champaign, IL, US:

Human Kinetics.

Page 55: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

46

Vallerand, R. J. (2007b). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in sport and physical activity:

A review and a look at the future. In G. Tenenbaum & R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of Sport

Psychology (pp. 59–83). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Retrieved from

http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118270011.ch3/summary

Wadden, T. A., & Foster, G. D. (2006). Weight and Lifestyle Inventory (WALI). Obesity,

14(S3), 99S–118S. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2006.289

Walach, H., Buchheld, N., Buttenmüller, V., Kleinknecht, N., & Schmidt, S. (2006).

Measuring mindfulness—the Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI). Personality and Individual

Differences, 40(8), 1543–1555. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.025

Wolsko, C., & Lindberg, K. (2013). Experiencing connection with nature: The matrix of

psychological well-being, mindfulness, and outdoor recreation. Ecopsychology, 5(2), 80–91.

https://doi.org/10.1089/eco.2013.0008

Page 56: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

47

Table 2.1. Description of the reviewed studies on mindfulness and physical activity

Article Participants

and design

Measure of

mindfulness

Measure of physical

activity or exercise

Main findings in mindfulness and

physical activity

Roberts, K. C., &

Danoff-Burg, S. (2010).

553 undergraduate

students;

Cross-sectional

The Five-Factor

Mindfulness

Questionnaire

(FFMQ)

Selected items from

Youth Risk Behavior

Surveillance System

and

Weight and Lifestyle

Inventory

Mindfulness was positively associated

with:

1. Perceived daily physical activity

level

2. Enjoyment of physical activity

3. Number of days reported to be

physically active in the past week

Gilbert, D., & Waltz, J.

(2010).

269 undergraduate

students;

Cross-sectional

FFMQ International

Physical Activity

Questionnaire

(IPAQ)

1. The Observe subscale of FFMQ

predicted MPA and VPA in male.

2. The Describe subscale predicted

MPA, and the Act with Awareness

subscale predicted VPA in female.

Loucks, E. B., Britton,

W. B., Howe, C. J.,

Eaton, C. B., & Buka, S.

L. (2014).

382

adults with mean

age 47; Cross-

sectional

Mindful Attention

and Awareness

Scale (MAAS)

IPAQ short form High levels of mindfulness related to

high levels of physical activity level.

Murphy, M. J.,

Mermelstein, L. C.,

Edwards, K. M., &

Gidycz, C. A. (2012).

441

Female college

students;

Cross-sectional

MAAS

Godin leisure-time

exercise

questionnaire

(GLTEQ)

1. Higher mindfulness level related to

healthier eating practices, better

quality of sleep, and better physical

health.

2. Time1 Frequency of exercise was

not significantly related to

mindfulness in Time 1 and Time 2.

Page 57: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

48

Chatzisarantis, N. L., &

Hagger, M. S. (2007).

226

undergraduate

students;

Cross-sectional

MAAS

GLTEQ

Self-Report Habit

Index (SRHI):

1. Mindfulness moderated the

intention-behavior relationship for

physical activity.

2. Intentions predicted physical

activity for more mindful

participants only.

3. More mindful and not habitual

participants are more likely to act on

their intentions compared to habitual

and not mindful participants.

Ruffault, A., Bernier, M.,

Juge, N., & Fournier, J.

F. (2016).

280 adults age

between

18 and 37;

Cross-sectional

MAAS

IPAQ

Higher mindfulness level was related to

improved intrinsic motivation and

physical activity level.

Mothes, H., Klaperski,

S., Seelig, H., Schmidt,

S., & Fuchs, R. (2014).

143 sedentary

male adults;

Cross-sectional

MAAS

Physical Activity,

Exercise, and Sport

Questionnaire

Mindfulness increases in the aerobic

exercise group but not in the relaxation

or the control group.

Butryn, M. L., Forman,

E., Hoffman, K., Shaw,

J., & Juarascio, A.

(2011).

54 female college

students:

RCT:

Education group

(n = 19)

Acceptance and

Commitment

Therapy (ACT)

group (n = 35)

The Philadelphia

Mindfulness

Scale (PHLMS)

Athletic Center

Visits

ACT group significantly increased

physical activity level compared to the

education participants.

Page 58: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

49

Salmoirago-Blotcher, E.,

Hunsinger, M., Morgan,

L., Fischer, D., &

Carmody, J. (2013).

174 adults with

mean age 47, and

psychological

symptom; Single

group pre–post

intervention

FFMQ Health Behaviors

Questionnaire:

selected items from

the Rapid

Assessment of

Physical Activity

questionnaire

(RAPA)

After completing the mindfulness based

stress reduction program, participants

showed:

1. Significant improvements in dietary

behaviors and sleep quality

2. Partial improvements in inactivity

level

Kangasniemi, A.,

Lappalainen, R.,

Kankaanpää, A., &

Tammelin, T. (2014).

108

Finnish adults. (58

physically less

active and 50

physically active);

Cross-sectional

Kentucky

Inventory of

Mindfulness Skills

(KIMS)

ActiGraph-GT1M

accelerometer

1. Mindfulness was positively

associated with MVPA time.

2. Compared to less active

participants, active individuals had

higher mindfulness level and less

psychological symptoms.

Ulmer, C. S., Stetson, B.

A., & Salmon, P. G.

(2010).

266

YMCA exercisers

with age 18 or

older; Cross-

sectional

Frieberg

Mindfulness

Inventory (FMI):

used to measure

state mindfulness

in this study

MAAS

Exercise

Maintenance

Questionnaire

IPAQ

Trait mindfulness:

1. Higher levels of mindfulness and

acceptance, and lower levels of

suppression were found in

participants who were more

successful at maintaining exercise,

who exercising more regularly, and

who were more successful in

meeting their exercise goals.

2. Lower levels of acceptance and

mindfulness were found in

participants who missed two

consecutive weeks of exercise over

the previous year.

State mindfulness:

1. Higher levels of acceptance and

mindfulness were found in

Page 59: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

50

participants who were more

successful in meeting their exercise

goals

2. Higher levels of acceptance and

lower levels of mindfulness were

found in participants who missed 1

week of exercise over the previous

year.

Tsafou, K. E., De Ridder,

D. T., van Ee, R., &

Lacroix, J. P. (2015).

398

Dutch speaking

adults age between

18 to 65; Cross-

sectional

The Dutch version

MAAS

Mindfulness in

Physical Activity

(MFPA)

IPAQ short form

SRHI

Satisfaction with

physical activity

questionnaire

1. Both satisfaction with physical

activity and mindfulness predicted

physical activity level.

2. Satisfaction with physical activity

significant mediate the relationship

between mindfulness and physical

activity.

3. Mindfulness affects physical

activity only when habitual physical

activity is weak.

4. Mindfulness and habit, and their

interaction were significant in

predicting satisfaction with physical

activity.

Tsafou, K.-E., Lacroix, J.

P., Ee, R. van, Vinkers,

C. D., & Ridder, D. T. D.

(2016).

305 Dutch

speaking adults

age between 18 to

65; Cross-sectional

The Dutch version

MAAS

MFPA

FFMQ

IPAQ short form

Satisfaction with

physical activity

questionnaire

1. Satisfaction with physical activity

mediated the positive association

between mindfulness in physical

activity and physical activity level.

2. Trait mindfulness level was

positively associated with physical

activity level through physical

activity mindfulness and satisfaction

with physical activity.

Page 60: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

51

Table 2. Current mindfulness questionnaires organized by their level in the proposed hierarchical framework

Measurements in each level Scores and structure Total

items

Used in

PA

researchc

Global Mindfulness Measures

Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI; Walach, Buchheld, Buttenmüller,

Kleinknecht, & Schmidt, 2006)

Unidimensional 30/14a Yes

Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003) Unidimensional 15 Yes

Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised

(CAMS-R; Feldman et al., 2006)

Unidimensional 12 No

Southampton Mindfulness Questionnaire (SMQ; Chadwick et al., 2008) Unidimensional 16 No

Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Scale

(KIMS; Baer, Smith, & Allen, 2004)

Four factors:

(1) Observing

(2) Describing

(3) Acting with awareness

(4) Accepting without judgment

39 Yes

Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer et al., 2008) Five factors:

(1) Nonreact

(2) Observe

(3) Act aware

(4) Describe

(5) Nonjudge

39 Yes

Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale (PHLMS; Cardaciotto, Herbert, Forman,

Moitra, & Farrow, 2008)

Two factors:

(1) Present-moment

(2) Awareness and acceptance

20 Yes

Page 61: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

52

Trait Toronto Mindfulness Scale (Trait-TMS; Davis, Lau, & Cairns, 2009) Two factors:

(1) Decentering

(2) Curiosity

13 No

Contextual Mindfulness Measures

Mindfulness in Physical Activity Questionnaire

(MFPA; Tsafou et al., 2015)

Unidimensional 6 Yes

Mindfulness Inventory for Sport (MIS; Thienot et al., 2014) Three factors:

(1) Awareness

(2) Nonjudgmental attitude

(3) Refocusing

15 No

State Mindfulness Scale for Physical Activity (SMS-PA; Cox et al., 2016) Unidimensional mindfulness with

two main dimensions:

(1) state mind

(2) state body

12 No

Situational Mindfulness Measures

State Mindful Attention Awareness Scale

(State-MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003)

Unidimensional 5b No

Toronto Mindfulness Scale (TMS; Lau et al., 2006) Two factors:

(1) Decentering

(2) Curiosity

13 No

State Mindfulness Scale (SMS; Tanay & Bernstein, 2013) Unidimensional mindfulness with

two main dimensions:

(1) mindfulness of mind

(2) mindfulness of body

21 No

Note: a Long and short forms of this measure exist. b Adapted from the MAAS. C Not including studies aim at validating the measure.

Page 62: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

53

Figure Caption

Figure 2.1. The PRISMA flow chart for literature research on mindfulness and physical activity.

Page 63: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

54

Data Base Searching in PubMed,

PsycINFO and Web of Science

(n = 1310)

Data Base Searching in PubMed,

PsycINFO and Web of Science

(n = 1310)

Scr

eenin

g f

or

Eli

gib

ilit

y

Incl

uded

L

iter

ature

Iden

tifi

cati

on

Manual Searching in Google

Scholar search engine

(n = 205)

Manual Searching in Google Scholar

search engine

(n = 205)

All records after duplicates removed

(n = 632)

All records after duplicates removed

(n = 632)

Title and abstract screening

(n = 225)

Title and abstract screening

(n = 225)

Excluded articles that

did not explicitly

include both physical

activity and

mindfulness

component

(n = 407)

Excluded articles that

did not explicitly

include both physical

activity and

mindfulness

component

(n = 407)

Full-text screening

(n = 16)

Full-text screening

(n = 16)

Excluded articles that

did not provide

quantitative data for

both physical activity

and mindfulness

(n = 209)

Excluded articles that

did not provide

qualitative data for

both physical activity

and mindfulness

(n = 209)

Final articles included in qualitative

synthesis (n =13)

Final articles included in qualitative

synthesis (n =13)

Excluded articles

(n = 678)

Excluded articles

(n = 678)

Excluded articles that

did not test

association between

physical activity and

mindfulness

(n = 3)

Excluded articles that

did not test

association between

physical activity and

mindfulness

(n = 3)

Page 64: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

55

Chapter 3: Momentary Negative Affect is Lower During Mindful Movement than While

Sitting: An Experience Sampling Study

Page 65: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

56

Abstract

Physical activity and mindfulness can decrease many aspects of negative affect (e.g.,

stress, anxiety, depression). It is unclear whether mindfulness during various activity behaviors

or postures (e.g., standing, moving, sitting) influences the associations between those

behaviors/postures and negative affect. This study tested whether contextual or situational

mindfulness moderated associations between (1) usual behavioral/postural patterns and negative

affect, or (2) momentary behaviors/postures and negative affect. A 14-day experience sampling

study was conducted to assess college students’ momentary behaviors/postures and subjective

experiences using a smartphone application. When students moved (versus sitting), their

momentary negative affect was lower if they were more mindful than usual (controlling for the

perceived autonomy of their behavior and other temporal variables at that moment). These

results extend prior work showing that negative affective states can be reduced by mindfulness

and physical activity to the momentary level. Integrating mindfulness practices with physical

activity may lead to greater mental health benefits and this hypothesis should be tested in

experimental research.

Page 66: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

57

Momentary Negative Affect is Lower During Mindful Movement than While Sitting:

An Experience Sampling Study

Regular physical activity participation has been associated with better mental health and

less intense psychological symptoms in both clinical and non-clinical populations (Biddle &

Asare, 2011; Mammen & Faulkner, 2013; Rebar et al., 2015; Rosenbaum, Tiedemann,

Sherrington, Curtis, & Ward, 2014). Similarly, mindfulness-based interventions that enhance

people’s awareness and acceptance of their bodily and emotional responses have consistently

reduced negative affect (Davis & Hayes, 2011; Gotink et al., 2015; Grossman, Niemann,

Schmidt, & Walach, 2004; Keng, Smoski, & Robins, 2011; Sharma & Rush, 2014). Little is

known about whether people’s mindfulness during everyday activity behaviors or postures

influences the intensity of negative affective experiences. This study investigated whether usual

or momentary mindfulness levels moderated associations between activity behaviors/postures

and negative affect.

Negative affect refers to individuals’ subjective experiences of displeasure (Ekkekakis &

Petruzzello, 2002). Examples of negative affective states include feeling sad, anxious, stressed,

and depressed (Harmon-Jones, Harmon-Jones, Amodio, & Gable, 2011; Posner, Russell, &

Peterson, 2005). Young adults, especially the college student population, consistently experience

negative affect in their daily life. For instance, young adults’ stress level was well above the

average stress level among all generations of US adults (APA, 2015). The American College

Health Association (ACHA) similarly revealed that more than half of the college student

population experienced negative affective states including overwhelming anxiety (57.7%),

sadness (63.5%), or mental exhaustion (81.6%) at least once a year. College students further

reported that stress and anxiety were the top two factors impacting their academic performance

Page 67: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

58

(ACHA, 2015), and these negative states have impaired academic performance, self-esteem and

overall health status (Fogle & Pettijohn, 2013; Hudd et al., 2000; Zajacova, Lynch, &

Espenshade, 2005). Negative affect is consistently associated with lower levels of physical

activity and mindfulness; however, most of this work has focused on characterizing differences

between people (Conn, 2010; Davis & Hayes, 2011; Ensari, Greenlee, Motl, & Petruzzello,

2015; Keng et al., 2011; Rebar et al., 2015; Sharma & Rush, 2014; Stathopoulou, Powers, Berry,

Smits, & Otto, 2006). Less is known about the role of daily activity behaviors and postures or

how mindfulness interacts with those behaviors/postures in regulating momentary affective

states.

Physical activity refers to any bodily movement that increases energy expenditure above

a basal level due to the contraction of skeletal muscle (Caspersen, Powell, & Christenson, 1985).

Physical activity, especially under moderate and vigorous intensities, can reduce negative

affective states, including stress, anxiety, and depression. This negative association has been

found in research with different designs such as cross-sectional studies, prospective studies, and

randomized controlled trials (Cooney, Dwan, & Mead, 2014; Ensari et al., 2015; Schuch, Rocha,

& Cadore, 2015; Stults-Kolehmainen & Sinha, 2013). Less is known about how bodily postures

that do not involve locomotion (i.e., sitting, standing) or light intensity physical activities can

impact negative affect and the available results have been quite heterogeneous. One review

focusing on the proximal association of daily physical activity with subsequent affective

responses produced mixed and inconclusive findings. Some studies found negative or non-

significant associations between daily physical activity and subsequent negative affective

responses (Liao, Shonkoff, & Dunton, 2015). Some of this heterogeneity may be due to variation

in people’s mental states during their daily activities, such as their mindfulness levels.

Page 68: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

59

Mindfulness is a form of heightened attention and awareness (Kabat-Zinn, 1994). It has

been identified as either a trait that varies between people or a state that changes within people

over time and contexts (Davidson, 2010; Thompson & Waltz, 2015). Based on different temporal

specificities, trait mindfulness can be conceptualized as the global level of mindfulness that is

relatively stable across time, whereas state mindfulness can be conceptualized as either the

contextual or the situational mindfulness that varies within different contexts and situations

(Bishop et al., 2004). This three level framework can enhance conceptual clarity and precision in

studying mindfulness, and is similar to what has been used to organize research on intrinsic and

extrinsic motivation (Vallerand, 2007; see Chapter 2).

In the past few decades, mindfulness practice has become a popular strategy widely

implemented in stress management interventions (Kabat-Zinn & Hanh, 2009; Praissman, 2008).

College students who participated in mindfulness-based stress reduction programs have

consistently decreased negative affect as indicated by lower levels of stress, anxiety, and

depression (Beddoe & Murphy, 2004; Jain et al., 2007; Moscaritolo, 2009; Schure, Christopher,

& Christopher, 2008; Shapiro, Oman, Thoresen, Plante, & Flinders, 2008). Aside from the

majority of the intervention studies that aim to enhance global mindfulness, simply elevating an

individual’s situational mindfulness at a particular time may improve affective valence. Bishop

et al. (2004) describe the state (situational) form of mindfulness as an active self-regulation of

one’s awareness and attention to the experience in the present moment, which indicates that

people could be relatively more mindful in any given moment without extra training in

mindfulness or meditation. Previous experience sampling research showed college students’

situational mindfulness level was negatively associated with momentary negative affect in terms

of both intensity and frequency over a few weeks (Brown & Ryan, 2003). The present study was

Page 69: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

60

designed to investigate whether situational mindfulness can moderate associations between a

person’s activity or posture and their negative affective state.

The interaction hypothesis between mindfulness and physical activity in reducing

negative affect has not been explored in depth, and the available findings particularly came from

studies comparing between-person differences. In one systematic review including only

randomized controlled trials, researchers seek to compare the relative efficacy of reducing

depression between mindfulness-based exercise including yoga, tai chi, and qigong training and

other exercise interventions. They concluded that both interventions were effective in reducing

depression symptoms or levels in the short term (Tsang, Chan, & Cheung, 2008). Findings about

differences between mindful and non-mindful exercises were inconclusive due to methodological

limitations and insufficient sample sizes in most of the reviewed studies. Older breast cancer

survivors who participated in mindful dancing increased their quality of life through reduction of

recurrence fear and improvement of global mindfulness attitude compared to cancer survivors in

the control group (Crane-Okada et al., 2012). In another study, psychologically-distressed

patients who engaged in a mindful walking programs significantly decreased their stress

symptoms and increased quality of life compared to the waitlist control group (Teut et al., 2013).

To the best of our knowledge, only one study used experience sampling methods to study the

differences in momentary negative affect between more and less mindful activity. In this study,

mindful walking was carried out by adults with mindfulness training experiences (most of whom

had history of depression). Mindful walking significantly reduced negative affect compared to

the same duration of daily normal activities (Gotink et al., 2016).

These studies provide preliminary evidence for integrating mindfulness and physical

activity to reduce negative affect, but none explicitly examined the interaction between

Page 70: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

61

momentary mindfulness and activity behaviors or postures. These studies also targeted patients

in clinical settings exclusively and no studies focused on college students population that

experience high levels of negative affect.

The Present Study

The present study was designed to evaluate whether momentary mindfulness levels

moderated associations between physical activity and negative affect. A second goal of this study

was to deepen understanding of the specific activity behaviors or postures that are most

associated with negative affect so we assessed a variety of activity intensities and postures that

are common across contexts of daily life (i.e., sitting, standing, moving). Based on the premise

that affect, daily activity behaviors/posture, mindfulness, and negative affect are all time-varying

phenomena, experience sampling methods were used to distinguish variance that differentiated

between people from variance that differentiated between moments nested within people

(Dunton, 2017). The primary within-person hypotheses in the current study were that (1)

momentary activity-negative affect relations would be stronger when participants were more

mindful than usual, and (2) negative affect would be lower when participants were moving

(relative to sitting). The primary between-person hypothesis in the current study was that once

the within-person associations were controlled, there would be no significant associations

between students’ usual daily activity, usual (global) mindfulness and negative affect.

Given the observational nature of these data, statistical controls were included for age,

gender, response time, day of the week, time in study, perceived autonomy, and lagged negative

affective state. These covariates were selected because they can each impact negative affect. For

example, stress perception or reactivity differ by age, gender, and the temporal contexts within a

day or week (APA, 2011; Jorm et al., 2005; Ragsdale, Beehr, Grebner, & Han, 2011; Rudolph &

Page 71: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

62

Hammen, 1999). As the time in study increases, or as the prompts accumulated throughout the

day, participants may feel irritated and become more anxious as they receive frequent

unpredictable prompts (Beute, de Kort, & IJsselsteijn, 2016). Additionally, many college

students’ activities or postures are obligatory (e.g., due to norms for sitting in classrooms), so

perceived autonomy was assessed to control its influence on stress, anxiety or mental health

(Deci & Ryan, 2008; Ng et al., 2012). A single-lagged negative affect term also included to

adjust for any carry-over influence (Towbes & Cohen, 1996).

Methods

Participants and Procedures

University students enrolled in an undergraduate course (N= 161) participated in this

study as part of the course requirements. Three participants were excluded from analysis because

their data was too sparse (M = 13 responses out of 112 prompts). The remaining 158 participants

were predominantly female (58%), not Hispanic (91%) and White (82%) with a mean age of

20.0 years (SD = 1.26).

The institutional review board approved the study protocol and participants provided

informed consent at an initial lab session. Small groups of up to five participants met with the

researcher to download and install the Personal Analytics Companion Application (Paco;

https://www.pacoapp.com/) onto their smartphones (16% iOS, 84% Android). Following

installation, the researcher helped each participant complete a practice experience sampling

survey on Paco, and set a personalized 12-hour window for experience sampling prompts. The

default time block for receiving the notifications was within 8AM to 8PM, but participants could

adjust this time window based on their schedule.

Page 72: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

63

Participants then completed an online questionnaire about their demographic

characteristics (e.g., age, gender). For the next 14 days, participants completed up to eight

randomly-delivered questionnaires from Paco each day during their waking time. There were a

minimum one-hour interval between prompts and a five-minute time window for responding.

Following each prompt, students reported their activity behavior/posture and rated their negative

affect and mindfulness at the moment they were prompted. Participants on average spent less

than one minute to response to each prompt. The researcher deactivated all participants’ accounts

from receiving the daily prompts from Paco after the two-week study period, and participants

were free to uninstall the app from their smartphones. All data were collected between late

October and mid-November 2015.

Measures

Demographics. Age, gender and ethnicity were self-reported.

Daily activity / postures. Momentary activity behaviors/postures were assessed using a

single item, “What were you doing when the signal went off?” Response options included sitting,

standing, moving, or sleeping. Sleep was rarely reported (5%) so these entries were excluded

from the data analysis. Dummy variables were created for standing and moving (0 = not selected,

1 = selected) with sitting serving as the reference category.

Negative affect. Three items were used to measure perceived stress (“During this

activity, I felt stressed”), anxiety (“During this activity, I felt anxious”), and affective valence

(“Please rate how you felt in general during this activity”). Participants responded to the stress

and anxiety items on a scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely) and to the negative

valence item on a reverse-coded scale ranging from 0 (very bad) to 4 (very good). These items

were slightly modified from the “Emotional Distress - Anxiety”, “Distress - Depression”, and

Page 73: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

64

“Positive Affect” short form measures in the Patient Reported Outcomes Measurement

Information System (PROMIS; Pilkonis et al., 2011; Schalet et al., 2016) to accommodate the

frequent momentary assessments required in an experience sampling design. Responses between

the stress, anxiety and the valence items were moderately to highly correlated (r ranged from

0.35 to 0.74 across observations and people and from 0.25 to 0.50 across participants), and the

three items were internally consistent as a whole (coefficient was 0.64 across observations and

0.76 across participants). Initial analyses were run on responses to each item individually and

results were consistent, so the three items were aggregated into a composite index of negative

affect for the analyses reported in the current study.

Mindfulness. Momentary mindfulness was assessed using two items from the State

Mindfulness Scale (SMS; Tanay & Bernstein, 2013). This measure was developed to assess

situational mindfulness (i.e., mindfulness during a specific time period and particular context,

such as during academic or other activities). Items were selected based on their factor loadings to

include the items whose responses were most closely associated with latent scores for

mindfulness of mind (“During this activity, I noticed pleasant and unpleasant emotions”) and

body (“During this activity, I noticed physical sensations come and go”). Participants indicated

their perceived situational mindfulness level on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very

well). Based on the strong correlation (Spearman-Brown ρ=0.55 across observations and people;

ρ = 0.76 across people), the two items were combined to represent situational mindfulness.

Autonomy. One item modified from the Situational Motivation Scale (SIMS; Guay,

Vallerand, & Blanchard, 2000), was used to assess participant’s momentary perceptions of

autonomy: “I was engaging in this activity because I want to, not because I have to.” Participants

rated their momentary perceived autonomy on a scale ranging from 0 (do not agree at all) to 4

Page 74: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

65

(strongly agree). Higher scores indicated that the person experienced greater relative autonomy

for the activity.

Temporal context. The temporal context of responses was recorded automatically by the

Paco app in the form of time and date stamps for each experience sample. To accommodate

college students’ schedules which can run past midnight, the time of day was expressed as

minutes since 4am (as opposed to using midnight; see Rives, 2007). Dummy variables were

created to represent the day of week and Saturday served as the reference category because it

coincided with the lowest mean negative affect scores.

Data Preparation and Analysis

Momentary variables were transformed to separate variance at the between- and within-

person levels (Bolger, Davis, & Rafaeli, 2003; Schwartz & Stone, 1998). First, each person’s

usual level of a variable was calculated as their intraindividual mean (imean) score. This data

preparation approach was equivalent to cluster-mean centering method described by Enders and

Tofighi (2007) for multilevel model analysis. Next, each momentary response was person mean

centered by subtracting each individual score from the corresponding person-level mean score.

The resulting difference score represented the deviation of a person’s activities or experiences

around their mean levels at a given time. For instance, participant i’s usual level of mindfulness

(Usual Mindfulnessi) was calculated as the imean of his/her momentary responses to mindfulness

items across the 2 weeks, and each participant’s momentary levels of mindfulness (Momentary

Mindfulnessit) were calculated by subtracting the scores at each occasion t from his or her imean.

Usual Mindfulnessi scores across the 2 weeks differentiate the more or less mindful people in

general (between-person differences), whereas Momentary Mindfulnessit scores represent

momentary deviations from usual mindfulness levels (within-person differences).

Page 75: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

66

Negative affect scores were not over dispersed (kurtosis = 0.21)) but negatively skewed

(skewness = 0.93), so scores were transformed using the Box-Cox method (optimal λ = 0.39;

transformed skew = -0.57; Box & Cox, 1964; Osborne, 2010). This transformed score was used

in all analyses. The distributions of momentary mindfulness (skewness = 0.08; kurtosis = -0.75)

and perceived autonomy (skewness = -0.61; kurtosis = -0.78) ratings were not over skewed or

dispersed so the original score metric was retained.

Due to the nested structure of the data (moments nested within people), multilevel models

(MLM) were used to examine the between- and within-person associations between their

momentary activities and negative affect. Models were estimated using R 3.2.2 (R Core Team,

2013) with the nlme package (Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy, Sarkar, & R Core Team, 2016). Missing

values (6%) were excluded from the analysis. Predictors in the multilevel models were

categorized into three different sources: the between-person factors, the within-person factors,

and the interactions between within-person factors. The within-person interactions were selected

by all the combinations between mindfulness and the activities (standing and moving).

Consistent with standard multilevel modeling practice, the intraclass correlation

coefficients (ICC) and the pseudo-R2 were calculated. The former index estimates the proportion

of total variance (between- and within-person) that can be attributed to between-person

differences (Kreft, Kreft, & Leeuw, 1998). The latter index represents the proportion of total

variability explained by a set of predictors. The pseudo-R2 is also considered as an effect size

estimation of the model (Nakagawa & Schielzeth, 2013).

Model specification. The final multilevel model in predicting negative affect is presented

below:

Page 76: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

67

Level-1: Negative affectit = β0i + β1i (Response timeit) + β2i (Day in studyit) + β3i

(Mondayit) + β4i (Tuesdayit) + β5i (Wednesdayit) + β6i (Thursdayit) + β7i

(Fridayit) + β8i (Sundayit) + β9i (Deviations from usual standingit) + β10i

(Deviations from usual movingit) + β11i (Momentary autonomyit) + β12i

(Momentary mindfulnessit) + β13i (Prior negative affectit) +

β14i (Deviations from usual standingit x Momentary mindfulnessit) +

β15i (Deviations from usual movingit x Momentary mindfulnessit) + eit

(1)

Level-2: β0i = γ00 + γ01 (Agei) + γ02 (Genderi) + γ03 (Usual standingi) +

γ04 (Usual movingi) + γ05 (Usual autonomyi) +

γ06 (Usual mindfulnessi) + u0i

(2)

β(1 -14)i = γ(1-14)0 (3)

where γ00 represented the intercept for momentary negative affect, γ01 to γ06 represented the

between-person associations between negative affect and age (γ01), gender (γ02), usual standing

(γ03), usual moving (γ04), usual mindfulness level (γ05), and usual autonomy level (γ06); γ10 to γ70

represented the association between negative affect and response time (γ10) and day of the week

(i.e., γ20 for Monday, γ30 for Tuesday, etc.). Saturday had the lowest negative affect so was used

as the reference day. The parameters γ80 to γ110 represented associations between negative affect

and individual’s usual level of standing and moving (γ80 and γ90, both relative to sitting), usual

mindfulness level (γ100), and usual autonomy level (γ110), respectively; γ120 was the association

with lagged negative affect; γ130 and γ140 represented the association between negative affect and

interactions between momentary activity and momentary level of mindfulness. All predictors

were treated as fixed effects to reduce model complexity and accommodate the available sample

Page 77: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

68

size. The coefficient u0i is the person-specific residual deviations that is uncorrelated with the

momentary-level residual eit.

Results

Participants provided a total of 14,591 responses (80.9% response rate) and an average of

92 responses per person (SD = 24.76) during the two-week study period. The frequency plot in

Figure 3.1 shows that almost all the responses (99%) were provided between 8AM and 12PM

(midnight), indicating a good coverage of typical waking hours.

As seen in Table 3.1, on average, participants reported low negative affect (M = 3.15 on a

0-12 scale), moderate mindfulness (M = 3.49 on a 0-8 scale) and moderate perceived autonomy

(M = 2.62 on a 0-4 scale). Sitting, moving, and standing were reported during 62%, 22%, and

16% of the valid experience samples, respectively. Table 3.1 also reports between- and within-

person correlation coefficients above and below the diagonal, respectively. Similar patterns were

observed in the between- and within-person correlations. For example, negative affect had a

moderate negative association with autonomy (between-person r = -0.48; within-person r = -

0.28), and weak negative associations with standing (between-person r = -0.07; within-person r

= -0.08), and moving (between-person r = -0.08; within-person r = -0.10). The ICC for negative

affect indicated that approximately 37% of the variance in scores was due to differences between

people with the remaining variance due to within-person fluctuations or measurement error. This

estimate justified testing both between- and within-person hypotheses in a multilevel modeling

framework.

Table 3.2 summarizes coefficients from the multilevel model that regressed negative

affect on a variety of predictors after controlling for other variables. The outcome variable in this

Page 78: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

69

model is a composite negative affect score but, as a reminder, conclusions were identical when

individual models were tested for stress, anxiety and affective valence.

The first primary hypothesis was supported: People reported less negative affect while

moving (γ90 = -0.70, p < .01) or standing (γ100 = -0.51, p < .01) than sitting. At moments when

people were more active than their usual, they experienced lower levels of negative affect.

The second primary hypothesis was also supported: At moments when individuals were

more mindful than usual, the within-person associations between negative affect and moving

(γ150 = -0.10, p < .01) differed from when participants were sitting. A significant interaction

effect was not observed in the within-person interaction between standing and momentary

mindfulness (p > .05). People’s momentary negative affect and standing did not vary as a

function of their momentary mindfulness. Figure 3.2 presents this interaction in which

associations between momentary negative affect and moving varied as a function of their

momentary mindfulness. People reported significant less negative affect while moving mindfully

compared to sitting.

The hypothesis regarding the primary between-person association was supported: After

accounting for the within-person variations in people’s daily activity and mindfulness, the

differences between people’s usual activity level as well as their usual mindfulness level did not

predict negative affect (p > .05). People’s negative affect did not differ for people who more

frequently reported moving or standing relative to sitting, nor for people who reported being

mindful more frequently.

Both usual and momentary autonomy were negatively associated with negative affect.

Individuals with greater autonomy were less likely to experience higher negative affect (γ06 = -

0.93, p < .01). In moments when people were more autonomous than usual, they tended to

Page 79: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

70

experience less negative affect (γ120 = -0.57, p < .01). The model also indicated that college

student’s negative affect levels differed by the day of the week. Compared to Saturdays, students

reported higher negative affect on all other days except Fridays. The model also showed that

students’ previous negative affective state carried over with time and positively predicted their

subsequent levels of negative affect (γ130 = 0.35, p < .01). The pseudo R2 for this model indicated

that the fixed effects of all the predictors in the model accounted for approximately 38% of the

variance in momentary negative affect.

Discussion

The current study applied experience sampling methods to investigate associations

between daily activity behaviors/postures, mindfulness and negative affect in college students’

daily lives. The key finding was that college students who reported being more mindful while

moving in everyday life reported lower levels of negative affect than they did when they were

less mindful or sitting. This finding was consistent with previous studies showing that engaging

in mindful physical activity is associated with lower perceived negative affect (Gotink et al.,

2016; Robert-McComb et al., 2015; Teut et al., 2013; Tsang et al., 2008). These results provide

the first evidence of mindful movement being associated with negative affect in a variety of life

contexts, without participating in a structured mindful moving program.

Both engaging in physical activity and mindfulness practice have been shown to reduce

negative affect in the college student population (Regehr, Glancy, & Pitts, 2013; Stults-

Kolehmainen & Sinha, 2013). The present study extended previous research by explicitly testing

the interaction between these two strategies. A potential synergetic effect emerged between

physical activity behavior/posture and mindfulness that dampened negative affect even more

Page 80: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

71

than behavior/posture or mindfulness by itself. Higher levels of momentary mindfulness

strengthened the inverse association between physical activity and negative affect. This finding

implies that implementing mindfulness practices into everyday activity behaviors/postures may

lower students’ negative affective experiences to a greater degree. With the inclusion of usual

and momentary mindfulness levels, we also found that only students’ momentary activity

behavior/posture, but not their usual level of activity behavior/posture, was associated with

negative affect.

The finding that deviations from college students’ usual activity level, but not their usual

activity level, was significantly associated with negative affect was consistent with one prior

study (Haas, Schmid, Stadler, Reuter, & Gawrilow, 2017), but contradicted with another (Hyde,

Conroy, Pincus, & Ram, 2011). Study from Haas et al. (2017) found that when young adults

(mostly college students) engaged in more daily moderate to vigorous physical activity than their

usual level, they reported lower levels of negative affect in the same evening. The between-

person differences in daily physical activity level in this study did not yield the same association

with negative affect. On the other hand, study from Hyde et al. (2011) revealed that both college

students’ overall and daily free-time physical activity were not associated with daily unpleasant-

activated (nervous, embarrassed, upset, stress, and tense) or unpleasant-deactivated (sluggish,

sad, bored, depressed, and disappointed) affective responses. These mixed results indicate a need

for future studies to investigate whether people’s mindfulness level or the activation (arousal)

level of affect are determinants of the between- and within-person association between daily

physical activity and negative affect (Dunton et al., 2014; Kanning & Schlicht, 2010; Reed &

Ones, 2006).

Page 81: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

72

Further, rather than focusing on moderate-to-vigorous physical activity level in daily life,

the present study showed that daily activity behaviors and postures (standing and moving relative

to sitting) also yield an inverse association with negative affect. When people moved or stood

(relative to sitting), they experienced lower levels of negative affect. This finding may also

inform future studies to investigate the role of everyday light activities or postures in improving

people’s mental health, which will provide tremendous public health implications.

The present study relied on self-reports and did not incorporate devices (e.g.,

accelerometers) to capture daily movements and postures. Those devices are often considered

gold standards for investigating daily variations in physical activity and affective states

(Kanning, Ebner-Priemer, & Schlicht, 2013). The self-reported daily activity patterns reported

here corresponded with population-based estimates of time spent in different activity behaviors

and postures from devices (Hansen, Kolle, Dyrstad, Holme, & Anderssen, 2012). For example,

the proportion of sedentary time reported in both studies was identical (both 62%). This

concordance is likely a product of intensive within-person assessments that averaged 909

responses per hour across a broad swatch of waking hours. Future studies should investigate

whether ecological momentary assessment approaches with frequent responses of physical

activity could be a valid proxy for the total amount of physical activity measured by devices.

The intensive longitudinal assessments in the current study extended prior mindfulness

work by demonstrating affective differences as a function of situational mindfulness levels in

college student’s everyday life. To reduce negative affect in college students, prior mindfulness-

based programs usually focused on affective differences between more and less mindful

students, and changes in global mindfulness level before and after the program (Chiesa &

Serretti, 2009; Eberth & Sedlmeier, 2012; Shapiro et al., 2008). Those studies usually included

Page 82: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

73

limited samplings of mindfulness and negative affect, and thus could not disaggregate the

between-person and within-person variation in these time varying constructs (Beddoe & Murphy,

2004; Jain et al., 2007; Moscaritolo, 2009; Schure et al., 2008; Shapiro et al., 2008). The present

finding of a within-person association between mindfulness and negative affect has implications

for future intervention designs to examine the associations between people’s global, situational

mindfulness and negative affect. An experimental study with ecological momentary assessment

of mindfulness and negative affect is needed to establish causality between people’s increases in

global and/or momentary mindfulness and reductions in negative affect.

Perceived autonomy is the only predictor in our study that was significant in predicting

negative affect at both the between- and the within-person levels. People with higher levels of

perceived autonomy or who perceived greater autonomy at a given moment often experience

more enjoyment and pleasure while engaging in physical activity (Teixeira, Carraça, Markland,

Silva, & Ryan, 2012). It was not surprising to see that higher levels of usual and momentary

autonomy were related to lower levels of negative affect in the present study. This finding

reinforces the importance of cultivating a sense of autonomy, a key factor depicted in self-

determination theory, to support intrinsic motivation for physical activity and promote health and

well-being (Deci & Ryan, 2008).

To account for the potential threat of affective reactivity due to intensive measures, time

in study and response time variables were included in our analysis to control covariation with

negative affect. The model did not indicate any signs of elevated negative affect increased with

time, but suggested a significant decrease in negative affect as the numbers of days in the study

increased. Thus, concerns about experience sampling-induced reactivity should be minimal.

Moreover, with ecological momentary assessment over a course of two weeks, we were able to

Page 83: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

74

test the differences between weekday and weekend differences in negative affect in college

students. After controlling for other variables, we identified that college students experienced

higher negative affect in most weekdays compared to Saturday. Such cyclicity of weekly

negative affective level has also been identified in previous study (Ragsdale et al., 2011). The

prevalence of experiencing greater negative affect in school days highlights the need for

effective interventions to promote well-being in college students’ daily schedule.

Several limitations in the current study should be noted. The age range of the sample was

narrow so conclusions may be age-specific and may not generalize to other populations in

different age groups. For instance, younger children usually do their daily activity with less

conscious awareness due to the lack of autonomy and agency, since their daily schedule has

mostly been instructed and planned out (Fodor & Hooker, 2008). On the other hand, with the

perception of limited future life time, older adults tend to pay more attention to their emotional

well-being and physical conditions in the present moment of everyday life (Scheibe &

Carstensen, 2010). Thus, it is possible that the same study in children or older could lead to

different results.

For physical activity measurement, the self-reported momentary activity in our study was

not sensitive to differences in the intensity, duration or mode of physical activity. Future work

with devices (i.e., accelerometers) that are capable of activity classification would help to extend

understanding of the processes of daily activities in predicting negative affect. Also, a number of

contextual factors that could influence negative affect were not assessed in the present study. For

example, the social context, the built environment, and the weather condition have been shown

to impact people’s daily affective responses (Galea, Ahern, Rudenstine, Wallace, & Vlahov,

Page 84: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

75

2005; Kööts, Realo, & Allik, 2011; Society, 2004). These factors should be considered as

covariates in future research.

To minimize participant burden from responding to the frequent experience sampling

prompts, only a few items were selected to measure subjective experiences including negative

affect (assessed by three items), mindfulness (two items), and autonomy (one item). Using fewer

items to measure psychological constructs is common to reduce burden and fatigue in experience

sampling studies, but this approach may be more vulnerable to random measurement errors or

unidentified biases in interpretation of the question (Hoeppner, Kelly, Urbanoski, & Slaymaker,

2011). We sought to minimize measurement errors and sustain face validity by selecting and

modifying our experience sampling questions from available scales, but results may have

differed if other types of measures or more items were used to capture each construct in our

study. Finally, the research design was non-experimental, so causal inferences are not possible.

In sum, this study was the first to apply experience sampling methods to investigate the

interaction between momentary mindfulness, physical activity, and negative affect. Results

revealed a within-person association between daily activity and situational mindfulness in

predicting negative affect. Simply moving more or standing more rather than sitting in everyday

life may reduce negative affect but this conclusion requires replication using experimental

methods. Situational mindfulness may moderate the association between daily activity

behaviors/postures and negative affect. These findings suggest new possibilities for using

ecological momentary interventions to promote situational mindfulness during upright postures

and while moving in everyday life to amplify the effects of physical activity. Instructions on how

to carry out mindful movement or incorporate mindfulness techniques with movement-based

Page 85: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

76

activities in college setting may be promising strategies for improving mental health among

college students.

Page 86: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

77

References

American College Health Association. (2015). American College Health Association National

College Health Assessment II: Undergraduate Students Reference Group Data Report

Fall 2015. Linthicum.

American Psychological Association. (2015). Stress in America. Retrieved May 2, 2016, from

http://www.apa.org/news/press/releases/stress/2014/highlights.aspx

American Psychological Association (APA). (2011). Stress and Gender: 2011. Retrieved January

16, 2017, from http://www.apa.org/news/press/releases/stress/2011/gender.aspx

Beddoe, A. E., & Murphy, S. O. (2004). Does mindfulness decrease stress and foster empathy

among nursing students? Journal of Nursing Education, 43(7), 305–312.

https://doi.org/10.3928/01484834-20040701-07

Beute, F., de Kort, Y., & IJsselsteijn, W. (2016). Restoration in its natural context: How

ecological momentary assessment can advance restoration research. International

Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 13(4), 420.

https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph13040420

Biddle, S. J. H., & Asare, M. (2011). Physical activity and mental health in children and

adolescents: a review of reviews. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 45(11), 886-895.

https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2011-090185

Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., … Devins, G.

(2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science

and Practice, 11(3), 230–241. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bph077

Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: Capturing life as it is lived. Annual

Review of Psychology, 54(1), 579–616.

Page 87: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

78

Box, G. E. P., & Cox, D. R. (1964). An analysis of transformations. Journal of the Royal

Statistical Society, 26(2), 211–252.

Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in

psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

Caspersen, C. J., Powell, K. E., & Christenson, G. M. (1985). Physical activity, exercise, and

physical fitness: definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public Health

Reports, 100(2), 126–131.

Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2009). Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction for Stress Management in

Healthy People: A Review and Meta-Analysis. The Journal of Alternative and

Complementary Medicine, 15(5), 593–600. https://doi.org/10.1089/acm.2008.0495

Conn, V. S. (2010). Anxiety outcomes after physical activity interventions: Meta-analysis

findings. Nursing Research, 59(3), 224–231.

https://doi.org/10.1097/NNR.0b013e3181dbb2f8

Cooney, G., Dwan, K., & Mead, G. (2014). Exercise for depression. JAMA, 311(23), 2432–2433.

https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2014.4930

Crane-Okada, R., Kiger, H., Sugerman, F., Uman, G. C., Shapiro, S. L., Wyman-McGinty, W.,

& Anderson, N. L. R. (2012). Mindful movement program for older breast cancer

survivors: A pilot study. Cancer Nursing, 35(4), E1–E13.

https://doi.org/10.1097/NCC.0b013e3182280f73

Davidson, R. J. (2010). Empirical explorations of mindfulness: Conceptual and methodological

conundrums. Emotion, 10(1), 8–11. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018480

Page 88: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

79

Davis, D. M., & Hayes, J. A. (2011). What are the benefits of mindfulness? A practice review of

psychotherapy-related research. Psychotherapy, 48(2), 198–208.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022062

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Self-Determination Theory: A macrotheory of human

motivation, development, and health. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne,

49(3), 182–185. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012801

Dunton, G. F. (2017). Ecological momentary assessment in physical activity research. Exercise

and Sport Sciences Reviews, 45(1), 48–54.

https://doi.org/10.1249/JES.0000000000000092

Dunton, G. F., Huh, J., Leventhal, A., Riggs, N., Spruijt-Metz, D., Pentz, M. A., & Hedeker, D.

(2014). Momentary assessment of affect, physical feeling states, and physical activity in

children. Health Psychology, 33(3), 255–263. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032640

Eberth, J., & Sedlmeier, P. (2012). The Effects of Mindfulness Meditation: A Meta-Analysis.

Mindfulness, 3(3), 174–189. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0101-x

Ekkekakis, P., & Petruzzello, S. J. (2002). Analysis of the affect measurement conundrum in

exercise psychology: IV. A conceptual case for the affect circumplex. Psychology of

Sport and Exercise, 3(1), 35–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1469-0292(01)00028-0

Enders, C. K., & Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel

models: A new look at an old issue. Psychological Methods, 12(2), 121–138.

https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121

Ensari, I., Greenlee, T. A., Motl, R. W., & Petruzzello, S. J. (2015). Meta-analysis of acute

exercise effects on state anxiety: An update of randomized controlled trials over the past

25 years. Depression and Anxiety, 32(8), 624–634. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22370

Page 89: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

80

Fodor, I. E., & Hooker, K. E. (2008). Teaching mindfulness to children. Gestalt Review, 12(1),

75–91.

Fogle, G. E., & Pettijohn, T. F. (2013). Stress and health habits in college students. Open Journal

of Medical Psychology, 2(2), 61–68. https://doi.org/10.4236/ojmp.2013.22010

Galea, S., Ahern, J., Rudenstine, S., Wallace, Z., & Vlahov, D. (2005). Urban built environment

and depression: a multilevel analysis. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health,

59(10), 822–827. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech.2005.033084

Goodwin, R. D. (2003). Association between physical activity and mental disorders among

adults in the United States. Preventive Medicine, 36(6), 698–703.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0091-7435(03)00042-2

Gotink, R. A., Chu, P., Busschbach, J. J. V., Benson, H., Fricchione, G. L., & Hunink, M. G. M.

(2015). Standardised mindfulness-based interventions in healthcare: An overview of

systematic reviews and meta-analyses of RCTs. PLOS ONE, 10(4), e0124344.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124344

Gotink, R. A., Hermans, K. S. F. M., Geschwind, N., Nooij, R. D., Groot, W. T. D., & Speckens,

A. E. M. (2016). Mindfulness and mood stimulate each other in an upward spiral: a

mindful walking intervention using experience sampling. Mindfulness, 7(5), 1114–1122.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-016-0550-8

Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H. (2004). Mindfulness-based stress

reduction and health benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,

57(1), 35–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00573-7

Page 90: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

81

Guay, F., Vallerand, R. J., & Blanchard, C. (2000). On the assessment of situational intrinsic and

extrinsic motivation: The Situational Motivation Scale (SIMS). Motivation and Emotion,

24(3), 175–213. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005614228250

Haas, P., Schmid, J., Stadler, G., Reuter, M., & Gawrilow, C. (2017). Zooming into daily life:

within-person associations between physical activity and affect in young adults.

Psychology & Health, 32(5), 588–604. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2017.1291943

Hansen, B. H., Kolle, E., Dyrstad, S. M., Holme, I., & Anderssen, S. A. (2012). Accelerometer-

determined physical activity in adults and older people. Medicine & Science in Sports &

Exercise, 44(2), 266–272. https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e31822cb354

Harmon-Jones, E., Harmon-Jones, C., Amodio, D. M., & Gable, P. A. (2011). Attitudes toward

emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(6), 1332–1350.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024951

Hoeppner, B. B., Kelly, J. F., Urbanoski, K. A., & Slaymaker, V. (2011). Comparative utility of

a single-item vs. multiple-item measure of self-efficacy in predicting relapse among

young adults. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 41(3), 305–312.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsat.2011.04.005

Hudd, S. S., Dumlao, J., Erdmann-Sager, D., Murray, D., Phan, E., Soukas, N., & Yokozuka, N.

(2000). Stress at college: effects on health habits, health status and self-esteem. College

Student Journal, 34(2), 217–228.

Hyde, A. L., Conroy, D. E., Pincus, A. L., & Ram, N. (2011). Unpacking the feel good effect of

free-time physical activity: between- and within-person associations with pleasant-

activated feeling states. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 33(6), 884–902.

Page 91: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

82

Jain, S., Shapiro, S. L., Swanick, S., Roesch, S. C., Mills, P. J., Bell, I., & Schwartz, G. E. R.

(2007). A randomized controlled trial of mindfulness meditation versus relaxation

training: Effects on distress, positive states of mind, rumination, and distraction. Annals

of Behavioral Medicine, 33(1), 11–21. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324796abm3301_2

Jorm, A. F., Windsor, T. D., Dear, K. B. G., Anstey, K. J., Christensen, H., & Rodgers, B.

(2005). Age group differences in psychological distress: the role of psychosocial risk

factors that vary with age. Psychological Medicine, 35(9), 1253–1263.

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291705004976

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever You Go, There You Are: Mindfulness Meditation in Everyday

Life. New York: Hyperion.

Kabat-Zinn, J., & Hanh, T. N. (2009). Full Catastrophe Living: Using the Wisdom of Your Body

and Mind to Face Stress, Pain, and Illness. New York: Random House.

Kanning, Ebner-Priemer, & Schlicht. (2013). How to investigate within-subject associations

between physical activity and momentary affective states in everyday life: A position

statement based on a literature overview. Frontiers in Psychology, 4,187.

https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00187

Kanning, & Schlicht. (2010). Be active and become happy: an ecological momentary assessment

of physical activity and mood. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 32(2), 253–

261.

Keng, S.-L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of mindfulness on psychological

health: A review of empirical studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(6), 1041–1056.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006

Page 92: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

83

Kööts, L., Realo, A., & Allik, J. (2011). The influence of the weather on affective experience.

Journal of Individual Differences, 32(2), 74–84. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-

0001/a000037

Kreft, I. G. G., Kreft, I., & Leeuw, J. de. (1998). Introducing Multilevel Modeling. Thousand

Oaks: SAGE.

Liao, Y., Shonkoff, E. T., & Dunton, G. F. (2015). The acute relationships between affect,

physical feeling states, and physical activity in daily life: A review of current evidence.

Frontiers in Psychology, 6,1975. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01975

Mammen, G., & Faulkner, G. (2013). Physical activity and the prevention of depression: A

systematic review of prospective studies. American Journal of Preventive Medicine,

45(5), 649–657. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2013.08.001

Moscaritolo, L. M. (2009). Interventional strategies to decrease nursing student anxiety in the

clinical learning environment. Journal of Nursing Education, 48(1), 17–23.

https://doi.org/10.3928/01484834-20090101-08

Nakagawa, S., & Schielzeth, H. (2013). A general and simple method for obtaining R 2 from

generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 4(2), 133–

142. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-210x.2012.00261.x

Ng, J. Y. Y., Ntoumanis, N., Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Duda, J. L., &

Williams, G. C. (2012). Self-determination theory applied to health contexts: A meta-

analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(4), 325–340.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612447309

Osborne, J. W. (2010). Improving your data transformations: Applying the Box-Cox

transformation. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 15(12), 1–9.

Page 93: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

84

Pilkonis, P. A., Choi, S. W., Reise, S. P., Stover, A. M., Riley, W. T., & Cella, D. (2011). Item

banks for measuring emotional distress from the Patient-Reported Outcomes

Measurement Information System (PROMIS® ): Depression, anxiety, and anger.

Assessment, 18(3), 263–283. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191111411667

Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy, Sarkar, & R Core Team. (2016). nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed

Effects Models, R package version 3.1-128. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-

project.org/package=nlme

Posner, J., Russell, J. A., & Peterson, B. S. (2005). The circumplex model of affect: An

integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and

psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 17(3).

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050340

Praissman, S. (2008). Mindfulness-based stress reduction: A literature review and clinician’s

guide. Journal of the American Academy of Nurse Practitioners, 20(4), 212–216.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-7599.2008.00306.x

R Core Team. (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation

for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from

http://www.eea.europa.eu/data-and-maps/indicators/oxygen-consuming-substances-in-

rivers/r-development-core-team-2006

Ragsdale, J. M., Beehr, T. A., Grebner, S., & Han, K. (2011). An integrated model of weekday

stress and weekend recovery of students. International Journal of Stress Management,

18(2), 153–180. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023190

Rebar, A. L., Stanton, R., Geard, D., Short, C., Duncan, M. J., & Vandelanotte, C. (2015). A

meta-meta-analysis of the effect of physical activity on depression and anxiety in non-

Page 94: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

85

clinical adult populations. Health Psychology Review, 9(3), 366–378.

https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2015.1022901

Reed, J., & Ones, D. S. (2006). The effect of acute aerobic exercise on positive activated affect:

A meta-analysis. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7(5), 477–514.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2005.11.003

Regehr, C., Glancy, D., & Pitts, A. (2013). Interventions to reduce stress in university students:

A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 148(1), 1–11.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2012.11.026

Rives. (2007). Transcript of “The 4 a.m. mystery.” Retrieved February 15, 2016, from

https://www.ted.com/talks/rives_on_4_a_m/transcript

Robert-McComb, J. J., Cisneros, A., Tacón, A., Panike, R., Norman, R., Qian, X.-P., &

McGlone, J. (2015). The effects of mindfulness-based movement on parameters of stress.

International Journal of Yoga Therapy, 25(1), 79–88. https://doi.org/10.17761/1531-

2054-25.1.79

Rosenbaum, S., Tiedemann, A., Sherrington, C., Curtis, J., & Ward, P. B. (2014). Physical

activity interventions for people with mental illness: a systematic review and meta-

analysis. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 75(9), 964–974.

https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.13r08765

Rosenzweig, S., Reibel, D. K., Greeson, J. M., Brainard, G. C., & Hojat, M. (2003).

Mindfulness-based stress reduction lowers psychological distress in medical students.

Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 15(2), 88–92.

https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328015TLM1502_03

Page 95: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

86

Rudolph, K. D., & Hammen, C. (1999). Age and gender as determinants of stress exposure,

generation, and reactions in youngsters: a transactional perspective. Child Development,

70(3), 660–677.

Schalet, B. D., Pilkonis, P. A., Yu, L., Dodds, N., Johnston, K. L., Yount, S., … Cella, D. (2016).

Clinical validity of PROMIS depression, anxiety, and anger across diverse clinical

samples. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 73, 119–127.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2015.08.036

Scheibe, S., & Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Emotional aging: Recent findings and future trends. The

Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 65B(2),

135–144. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbp132

Schuch, F. B., Rocha, N., & Cadore, E. L. (2015). Progress in Physical activity and Exercise and

Affective and Anxiety Disorders: Translational Studies, Perspectives and Future

Directions. Frontiers Media SA.

Schure, M. B., Christopher, J., & Christopher, S. (2008). Mind-body medicine and the art of self-

care: Teaching mindfulness to counseling students through yoga, meditation, and qigong.

Journal of Counseling and Development : JCD; Alexandria, 86(1), 47–56.

Schwartz, J. E., & Stone, A. A. (1998). Strategies for analyzing ecological momentary

assessment data. Health Psychology, 17(1), 6–16.

Shapiro, S. L., Oman, D., Thoresen, C. E., Plante, T. G., & Flinders, T. (2008). Cultivating

mindfulness: effects on well-being. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64(7), 840–862.

https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20491

Page 96: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

87

Shapiro, S. L., Schwartz, G. E., & Bonner, G. (1998). Effects of mindfulness-based stress

reduction on medical and premedical students. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 21(6),

581–599. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018700829825

Sharma, M., & Rush, S. E. (2014). Mindfulness-based stress reduction as a stress management

intervention for healthy individuals: A systematic review. Journal of Evidence-Based

Complementary & Alternative Medicine, 19(4), 271–286.

https://doi.org/10.1177/2156587214543143

Society, T. R. (2004). The social context of well–being. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal

Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 359(1449), 1435–1446.

https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2004.1522

Stathopoulou, G., Powers, M. B., Berry, A. C., Smits, J. A. J., & Otto, M. W. (2006). Exercise

interventions for mental health: A quantitative and qualitative review. Clinical

Psychology: Science and Practice, 13(2), 179–193. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-

2850.2006.00021.x

Stults-Kolehmainen, M. A., & Sinha, R. (2013). The effects of stress on physical activity and

exercise. Sports Medicine, 44(1), 81–121. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-013-0090-5

Tanay, G., & Bernstein, A. (2013). State Mindfulness Scale (SMS): Development and initial

validation. Psychological Assessment, 25(4), 1286–1299.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034044

Teixeira, P. J., Carraça, E. V., Markland, D., Silva, M. N., & Ryan, R. M. (2012). Exercise,

physical activity, and self-determination theory: A systematic review. International

Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 9, 78.

https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-9-78

Page 97: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

88

Teut, M., Roesner, E. J., Ortiz, M., Reese, F., Binting, S., Roll, S., … Brinkhaus, B. (2013).

Mindful walking in psychologically distressed individuals: A randomized controlled trial.

Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, 2013, e489856.

https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/489856

Thompson, B. L., & Waltz, J. (2015). Everyday mindfulness and mindfulness meditation:

Overlapping constructs or not? Personality and Individual Differences, 43(7), 1875–

1885. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.06.017

Towbes, L. C., & Cohen, L. H. (1996). Chronic stress in the lives of college students: Scale

development and prospective prediction of distress. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,

25(2), 199–217. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01537344

Tsang, H. W. H., Chan, E. P., & Cheung, W. M. (2008). Effects of mindful and non-mindful

exercises on people with depression: A systematic review. British Journal of Clinical

Psychology, 47(3), 303–322. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466508X279260

Vallerand, R. J. (2007). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in sport and physical activity: A review

and a look at the future. In G. Tenenbaum & R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of Sport

Psychology (pp. 59–83). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Retrieved from

http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118270011.ch3/summary

Zajacova, A., Lynch, S. M., & Espenshade, T. J. (2005). Self-Efficacy, stress, and academic

success in college. Research in Higher Education, 46(6), 677–706.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11162-004-4139-z

Page 98: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

89

Figure Captions

Figure 3.1. Frequency of experience sampling responses at each time during the study period.

Figure 3.2. Associations of momentary mindfulness and moving on negative affect.

Page 99: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

90

Page 100: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

91

Page 101: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

92

Table 3.1. Descriptive statistics, correlations, and intraclass correlation coefficients of subjective experiences, physical activity, and

other variables of interest

M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

1. Negative Affect 3.15 2.78 (.37) .05** -.48** -.07** -.08** -.14** -.08** .09**

2. Mindfulness 3.49 2.26 .09** (.59) .02* -.06** .22** -.07** .02** .10**

3. Autonomy 2.62 1.33 -.28** .08** (.17) .06** .09** .19** .23** -.06**

4. Standing (%) 0.16 0.07 -.08** -.01 .07** 1.00 -.22** -.08** .06** .15**

5. Moving (%) 0.22 0.09 -.10** .13** .09** -.23** 1.00 -.01 .13** .10**

6. Response Time (hour) 12.05 3.89 -.05** .04** .10** .03** -.05** 1.00 -.01 -.03**

7. Age 19.99 1.27 -.08** .01 .04** .01 .01 -.01 1.00 -.36**

8. Gender (0=male; 1=female)

0.59 0.49 .09** .03** .03** .01 -.01 -.03** -.36** 1.00

N=158, *p<.05, **p<.01. Intraclass correlations (ICC) were calculated in the diagonal parentheses of the matrix.

Page 102: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

93

Table 3.2. Multilevel model coefficients for model predicting negative affect

Model Coefficient Std. Error

Fixed Effect

Intercept, 𝛾00 2.88 1.51

Age, 𝛾01 .08 .08

Gender, 𝛾02 .35 .19

Response Time, 𝛾10 .01 .01

Day in the Study, 𝛾20 -.01** <.01

Monday, 𝛾30 .30** .07

Tuesday, 𝛾40 .33** .07

Wednesday, 𝛾50 .38** .07

Thursday, 𝛾60 .15** .07

Friday, 𝛾70 <.01 .07

Sunday, 𝛾80 .34** .07

Usual standing, 𝛾03 -1.86 1.20

Usual moving, 𝛾04 -.95 1.03

Usual mindfulness, 𝛾05 .08 .05

Usual autonomy, 𝛾06 -.93** .16

Deviations from usual standing, 𝛾90 -.51** .05

Deviations from usual moving, 𝛾100 -.70** .05

Momentary mindfulness, 𝛾110 .14** .01

Momentary autonomy, 𝛾120 -.57** .01

Previous negative affective state, 𝛾130 .35** .01

Deviations from usual standing x Momentary mindfulness 𝛾140 -.02 .03

Deviations from usual moving x Momentary mindfulness 𝛾150 -.10** .03

Random Effect

Variance Intercept, 𝜎2𝑢0

1.03

Residual Variance, 𝜎𝑒2 3.34

*p<.05, **p<.01, N of group=150.

Page 103: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

94

Chapter 4: Feasibility of an Outdoor Mindful Walking Program for Reducing Negative

Affect in Older Adults

Page 104: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

95

Abstract

Mindful walking has emerged as a potential intervention strategy to improve mental

health and promote well-being in adult and clinical populations. This strategy has not been

implemented with older adults to date. This study evaluated the feasibility, acceptability,

sustainability, and preliminary efficacy of a mindful walking program for reducing negative

affect in older adults. Community-dwelling older adults (n = 29) completed a one-month,

outdoor mindful walking program distributed across eight 30-minute sessions. Overall, 93% of

the recruited participants (n = 27) completed all walking sessions and assessments. Responses

from the post-program and the follow-up questionnaires revealed that mindful walking was well-

accepted, highly-valued, and maintained after the program ended. Open-ended feedback

reinforced these findings. Analysis from the pre-walk and post-walk surveys also suggested the

preliminary efficacy of mindful walking program for reducing negative affect. In general, the

mindful walking program in the current study can be implemented as intended (i.e., feasible) and

should be considered acceptable and sustainable by older adults. Positive results identified in the

current feasibility study indicate readiness for randomized controlled trials to further examine the

efficacy and effectiveness of a mindful walking intervention for promoting health and well-being

in older populations.

Page 105: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

96

Feasibility of an Outdoor Mindful Walking Program for Reducing Negative Affect in Older

Adults

The older adult population is growing rapidly. By 2050, over 20% of the population –

approximately two billion of the population globally – will be 60 years of age or older (WHO,

2017). To help this rapidly growing older adult population live longer, healthier and happier, the

promotion of healthy aging has become a priority to the society (McLaughlin, Connell, Heeringa,

Li, & Roberts, 2010). Approximately 10% of older adults report 14 or more days of poor mental

health including stress, depression, or other negative emotional symptoms within a month (CDC,

2008; Gerteis et al., 2014). Around 15% to 52% of community older adults also reported anxiety

symptoms based on a literature review (Bryant, Jackson, & Ames, 2008). The prevalence of poor

mental health among the elderly foreshadow significant health care challenges. People are now

living longer but have lower psychological well-being that may lead to poor health. National data

recently revealed that only 1% of the health cost for older Americans have been spent in health

prevention work (National Council on Aging, 2015). Therefore, the design and evaluation of

cost-effective, innovative, and scalable intervention strategies to improve well-being in older

adults is a pressing manner from a public health standpoint.

Engaging in regular physical activity and mindfulness practice have both been introduced

as promising strategies to promote successful aging (Harmell, Jeste, & Depp, 2014), but

strategies that integrate mindfulness practice and a single form of physical activity have not been

explored in depth, especially in older adults. It remains unclear whether a mindful walking

program would be feasible and acceptable for older adults or if it could potentially improve their

well-being by reducing negative affect. Given this, the present study was conducted to evaluate

Page 106: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

97

the feasibility, acceptability, sustainability, and preliminary efficacy of an outdoor mindful

walking intervention for community-dwelling older adults.

Although the rates of psychological symptoms tend to decline with age, older adults

experience several aging-related stressors such as decreased physical and mental abilities,

physical pain and discomfort, insufficient time spend with family and friends, and concern about

their own death during everyday life (Stokes & Gordon, 2003). These stressors exact a toll on

older adults’ overall health and well-being, and could impair their quality of life. Poor well-being

is closely associated with worse cognitive functioning and physical health in older adults

(Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics, 2016). Better well-being is positively

associated with longevity and serves a protective role in maintaining physical health while

people get older, regardless of baseline mental/physical health and demographic backgrounds

(Steptoe, Deaton, & Stone, 2015). On the other hand, health risk behaviors, such as smoking,

inactivity, and poor diet, were more common among distressed older adults (CDC, 2008).

Epidemiologic data from older adults indicates that faster cognitive decline is associated with

increasing levels of perceived stress after adjusting for various biophysiological factors and

demographics (Aggarwal et al., 2014). Collectively, these findings point to the need for

prevention work for older adults to alleviate negative affect, improve well-being, and in turn,

sustain better mental and physical health.

Exercise, or moderate-to-vigorous physical activity, is widely recognized for its effects

on well-being by reducing negative affective states (Rebar et al., 2015; Rosenbaum, Tiedemann,

Sherrington, Curtis, & Ward, 2014; Stathopoulou, Powers, Berry, Smits, & Otto, 2006; Stults-

Kolehmainen & Sinha, 2013). Older adults engage in very limited amounts of exercise and

recent work has examined whether light physical activity, such as walking, can elicit similar

Page 107: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

98

benefits for them. An observational study conducted in two major metropolitan regions in the

United States revealed a positive association between light intensity physical activity and well-

being (i.e., lower stress and depressed feelings, higher life satisfaction) among older adults

(Buman et al., 2010). Another large observational study indicated that older adults who

participated in self-selected light intensity for at least five times per week, or leisure walking for

at least an hour per week had higher levels of subjective well-being compared to their peers

(Black et al., 2015). Meta-analysis has reported that for improving older adults’ psychological

well-being, the effect size was modest for moderate exercise (d = 0.34) and was small for light-

intensity exercise (d =0.14), respectively (Netz, Wu, Becker, & Tenenbaum, 2005).

Integrating other interventions that promote well-being may increase the effects of light

physical activity on well-being. Mindfulness is both a trait and a state form of mental experience

that is characterized by heightened attention, awareness, and acceptance of emotional and

physical sensations (Brown & Ryan, 2003). Trait and state mindfulness can also be

conceptualized via the three-level hierarchical framework (see Chapter 2). In this model, trait

mindfulness is synonymous with global mindfulness and is relatively time-invariant. State

mindfulness can be either contextually- or temporally-specific. Contextual mindfulness refers to

a person’s typical level of mindfulness in a particular activity or context, whereas situational

mindfulness refers to their momentary experience of mindfulness. Research on the mindfulness-

based stress reduction program (MBSR; Kabat-Zinn & Borysenko, 1996; Kabat-Zinn & Hanh,

2009) and the mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT; Morgan, 2003) indicated a negative

association between global mindfulness and negative affect (Fjorback, Arendt, Ø rnbøl, Fink, &

Walach, 2011; Gotink et al., 2015; Gu, Strauss, Bond, & Cavanagh, 2015). Older adults who

completed an eight-week MBSR or MBCT significantly decreased their reported depression and

Page 108: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

99

anxiety symptoms, and improved their emotional well-being (Foulk, Ingersoll-Dayton,

Kavanagh, Robinson, & Kales, 2014; Young & Baime, 2010; Splevins, Smith, & Simpson,

2009). One review concluded that the average MBSR effect sizes on negative affect in distressed

older adults were moderate (d = 0.50), despite the large heterogeneity and publication bias in the

mindfulness literature (Young & Baime, 2010). Another meta-analysis focused on the effect of

MCBT in older adults, and suggested similar effect sizes for decreasing depressive (g = 0.55) and

anxious (g = 0.58) symptoms (Kishita, Takei, & Stewart, 2016).

Interventions have attempted to integrate physical activity and mindfulness to improve

people’s mental health (Russell, 2011; Tsang, Chan, & Cheung, 2008). Mindful movement can

be simply described as mindful components being implemented concurrently with physical

activity, although light physical activity is considered to be optimal for cultivating mindfulness

(La Forge, 2016). For older adults, practicing mindfulness with a slow walking pace may be

desirable because light physical activity is both safe and health-enhancing (Loprinzi, Lee, &

Cardinal, 2015; Nelson et al., 2007). Only a handful of studies have emphasized mindful

walking as primary intervention strategy to enhance people’s well-being or reduce negative

affect. Two randomized controlled trials have shown that mindful walking or walking meditation

can be effective for decreasing negative affect in adults with moderate to high levels of distress

or depressive symptoms (Prakhinkit, Suppapitiporn, Tanaka, & Suksom, 2013; Teut et al., 2013).

This literature has important limitations. For example, none of the available trials recruited

relatively healthy older adults from the community or engaged participants in walking

individually (versus in groups). Only one study provided follow-up information regarding the

maintenance rate after the end of the supervised program so it is not clear whether mindful

walking is potentially sustainable after an initial supervised period (Teut et al., 2013). Finally,

Page 109: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

100

the mechanism for mindful walking benefits on negative affect likely involves momentary

changes mindfulness while walking but no studies have assessed those momentary changes.

The primary objective of the current study was to evaluate the feasibility of an outdoor

mindful walking program for community-dwelling older adults prior to designing a randomized

controlled trial. Eight 30-minute mindful walking sessions were delivered at an public arboretum

that has flat and wide walking trails. Participants were taught (in stages) to use basic and

common practices in traditional mindfulness training by focusing on their breath, steps,

emotional and bodily sensations while walking at a slow, fixed pace (Kabat-Zinn, 1994; Kabat-

Zinn & Hanh, 2009). Participants completed measures at the beginning and end of the program

as well as before and after each session to evaluate the feasibility of monitoring changes in

mindfulness and negative affect during the program. It was hypothesized that older adults in this

study would perceive this program to be acceptable, feasible and sustainable. Acceptability and

feasibility would be indicated by receiving an average score of six or above (out of a seven-point

Likert scale) on each of the questions regarding program feasibility and acceptability; acceptable

program sustainability would be indicated by obtaining an over 60% maintenance rate of mindful

walking at one month follow-up. Exploratory analyses were conducted to evaluate the benefit of

increasing mindfulness while walking. It was hypothesized that negative affect would decrease

most for older adults who increased their mindfulness most during the slow walks.

Methods

Participants

Recruitment was conducted over 6 weeks using fliers posted in a retirement village, a

senior learning institute and a variety of community locations (e.g., churches, restaurants). The

Page 110: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

101

fliers invited older adults to participate in an outdoor slow walking program at a local arboretum.

Fliers did not mention the word “mindfulness” to minimize expectations and biases that could

influence the psychological outcomes (Davidson & Kaszniak, 2015). Instead, the project was

advertised as a “slow walking” study. Figure 4.1 presents the CONSORT diagram for enrollment

and follow-up. Participants were eligible if they were age 65 years or older, could walk without

assistance, spoke and read English fluently, and were not allergic to plants and flowers. Older

adults were excluded if they had any mobility limitations or contraindications for regular light

physical activity.

Over six weeks of advertising, volunteers (n = 29) were enrolled using advertisements in

the retirement village (n = 2), community locations (n = 13), and a senior learning institute (n =

14). The mean age of participants was 73.2 years (range = 66 to 89, SD = 6.47). Most

participants were female (83%), Caucasian (79%), and highly educated (62% of them earned at

least a bachelor’s degree). Three participants (10%) had up to several hours of

mindfulness/meditation experiences before but this experience was more than 18 months prior

(range = 1.5 to 20.0 years).

Procedure

In an initial lab visit, each participant completed a baseline assessment of demographic

characteristics. The researcher then scheduled the participant’s eight walking sessions. Prior to

the first walking session, the researcher sent each participant a reminder email. Walking sessions

involved 30 minutes of individual, slow walking along a flat, designated route in an arboretum.

Prior to each walking session, the researcher greeted each participant at the arboretum to provide

instructions and a small reminder sheet detailing the mindfulness tasks for that day’s walk.

Participants completed a brief survey at the beginning and end of each walking session to report

Page 111: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

102

their negative affect and situational mindfulness. After the final walking session, participants

completed a second lab visit. In this visit, they rated the acceptability of the slow walking

program. One month after the second lab visit, a web-based follow-up questionnaire was emailed

to participants to inquire whether they continued to participate in the slow walking activity

following the end of the supervised program. Participants who completed the eight walking

sessions and two lab visits were enrolled in a raffle to win one of the nine $25 gift cards. The

study protocol and all materials were approved by the institutional review board.

Mindful Walking Intervention

The mindful walking program was designed to incorporate basic elements of mindfulness

practice or walking meditation in a way that would be accessible for the general older population

without extensive experience in meditation or mindfulness training. These elements involved

being attentive to the rhythm of their breathing and the movement of each step, and scanning the

body to identify sensations that arise (Kabat-Zinn, 1994; Kabat-Zinn & Borysenko, 1996; Kabat-

Zinn & Hanh, 2009). Table 4.1 summarizes the timing and content of slow walking and

mindfulness training in each of the eight sessions. The first session familiarized participants with

the walking route at the arboretum and provided an opportunity to practice walking at a slower

pace than normal. Older adults can lose their balance more easily when walking at a much

slower speed so participants were asked to select their most relaxing and comfortable way of

walking slowly (Morone & Greco, 2014). Paying attention to one’s breath is considered the most

fundamental mindful practice so it is incorporated beginning in session 2 (Kabat-Zinn, 1994).

Two other mindfulness tasks – namely, focusing on the movement and scanning the body – were

introduced in sessions four and six. The duration of mindfulness training during each session

increased from five minutes to a maximum of 30 minutes.

Page 112: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

103

Measures

First lab visit. A demographic survey was administered to assess participants’ age,

gender, race, highest level of educational attainment, prior experiences in mindfulness

(meditation) training or practice, and divided attention. The 4-item Divided Attention subscale

from the Everyday Cognition Scale for older adults was applied to characterize participants’

baseline attentional ability (Farias et al., 2008).

Pre-walk survey. Prior to each walking session, participants rated their negative affect,

situational mindfulness, and the quality of their previous night’s sleep. Negative affect was

assessed using four items that sampled perceived stress (“I feel stressed now”), anxiety (“I feel

anxious now”), depression (“I feel depressed now”), and displeasure (“I feel unpleasant now”).

Participants responded to each item on a scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly

agree) before and after each walking session. Items were modified from items in the anxiety,

stress, and affect pools of the Patient Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System

(PROMIS; Ader, 2007; Pilkonis et al., 2011). The four negative affect items were moderately to

highly correlated before the walking sessions (r ranged from 0.30 to 0.85) across participants, so

the responses were summed to create a single score representing negative affect. The internal

consistency (α) of the four pre-walk items across all occasions was 0.81.

Situational mindfulness was assessed using two items that measured perceived awareness

of mental states (“I notice pleasant or unpleasant emotions now”) and physical states (“I notice

bodily sensations come and go now”). These items were selected from the State Mindfulness

Scale (SMS) because they sampled the two key components (mindful or physical sensation and

mental events) of state mindfulness (Tanay & Bernstein, 2013). Participants rated their perceived

mindfulness level on a scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The

Page 113: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

104

internal consistency (Spearman-Brown ρ) of the two items in pre-walk surveys was 0.45

(Eisinga, Grotenhuis, & Pelzer, 2013). Additional analyses showed that the results did not differ

when the mindful-emotion or the mindful-body scores were separated in different models. Thus,

two situational mindfulness items were combine to represent a unidimensional construct of

mindfulness state (Tanay & Bernstein, 2013).

Participants reported their perceived past-night sleep quality on a 7-point bipolar scale

ranging from 1 (very bad) to 7 (very good). The researcher recorded the date, day of the week,

number of the session, and the time of both the pre- and post-surveys. Additionally, the daily

average temperature and weather conditions during each walking session was collected via the

https://weather.com website.

Post walk. Immediately after completing each walk, participants rated their negative

affect and situational mindfulness with the same items used prior to the walk. Correlations

between the negative affect items ranged from 0.40 to 0.76 and the internal consistency (α) of the

four post-walk items across all occasions was 0.80. The responses from the four items were

again summed to create a single score representing negative affect. Likewise, the two post-walk

situational mindfulness items were aggregated to represent a unified mindfulness state

(Spearman-Brown ρ = .38), consistent with the pre-walk measure.

Second lab visit. The feasibility and acceptability of the program were assessed using

items that sampled perceptions of satisfaction (“I feel satisfied with this program”), usefulness

(“I think this program is useful to me”), interest (“I am interested in participating in a similar

program in the future”), and overall quality (“I would recommend others to join this program”).

Participants rated the four items on the same scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7

(strongly agree) after they completed the program, and the responses of the four items were

Page 114: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

105

moderately to highly correlated (r ranged from 0.42 to 0.84). An open-ended question was used

to elicit any additional feedback, comments or concerns participants had after completing the

program.

One-month follow-up questionnaire. Maintenance was assessed with one binary item

(“After you finished the program, have you ever done the slow walking in the past month?”). If a

participant responded “yes,” additional questions were asked about the frequency and duration of

their slow walking. The frequency item was, “After you finished the program, how often did you

engage in slow walking for the past month?” and response options included “1 to 3 times”, “4 to

6 times”, “7 to 9 times”, and “more than 9 times.” The duration item was, “After you finished

the program, how long did you usually engage in slow walking each time for the past month?”

and response options included “less than 30 minutes”, “30 minutes to less than 1 hour”, “1 hour

to less than 2 hours”, and “more than 2 hours.” Finally, participants were asked to indicate their

continued use of the specific mindfulness training techniques while slow walking on a scale

ranging from 1 (almost never) to 4 (almost always).

Data Analysis

Descriptive statistics were calculated to characterize the feasibility, acceptability,

sustainability, and preliminary efficacy of the program. Single-group repeated measure t-tests

were used to determine the within-group differences before and after mindful walking sessions.

Standardized mean differences were calculated to represent paired-sample effect sizes. Finally,

to account for the inter-dependency of the nested data structure in the current study (the walking

session data nested within each participant), a multilevel model was utilized to analyze the

association between changes in mindfulness scores and negative affect after mindful walking

sessions (Steenbergen & Jones, 2002).

Page 115: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

106

Results

Twenty-seven participants (93%) completed all eight slow walking sessions and the pre-

post program measurements, and 26 participants (90%) completed the one-month follow-up

questionnaire, indicating high adherence rates. One participant dropped out after her first slow

walking session because of difficulties navigating the path in arboretum; another participant

dropped out after finishing his fifth slow walking session due to an unexpected painful groin pull

during his leisure activity (unrelated to the study). With respect to the measurement completion

rate (95%), participants provided data on 525 out of the 551 possible measurement occasions (19

possible occasions/participant: two lab visits, eight pre-walk assessments, eight post-walk

assessments, one follow-up).

Table 4.2 summarizes the temporal and environmental context of the slow walking

sessions. The majority of the slow walking sessions were held between 10am and 3pm (90%) on

weekdays (86% from Monday to Friday) in November (71%). The average daily temperature of

all slow walking sessions was 40.6°F (range: 9°F to 72°F), and 18% of the sessions had an

average daily temperature at or below 32°F (the freezing point). Adverse weather conditions

(e.g., rain, hail, snow) occurred during approximately 10% of the slow walking sessions. Among

the 222 scheduled sessions, 17 advance cancellation notices (8% of all scheduled sessions) were

received from participants due to adverse weather conditions (i.e., snow shower, n = 6) or

holiday traveling (i.e., Thanksgiving break, n = 11). None of the participants missed their

walking session without informing the researcher beforehand. Overall, participants completed

92% of their scheduled sessions.

At the end of the slow walking program, 96% of participants indicated that they would

recommend this program to others. Participants also reported high levels of satisfaction (M =

Page 116: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

107

6.18, SD =0.78), usefulness (M = 6.15, SD = 0.87), and interest (M = 6.19, SD = 1.08). In the

follow-up questionnaire, 92% of participants indicated that they were still interested in slow

walking, and 65% reported that they continued to practice slow walking after program

completion. For those who maintained their slow walking practice, more than half (59%)

practiced more than 4 times within the past month, and the majority (65%) practiced mindful

strategies frequently during their slow walking. All participants who maintained their slow

walking practice reported that they practiced slow walking for more than 30 minutes each time.

Table 4.3 summarizes descriptive statistics and correlations for key study variables.

There was no temporal trend indicating systematic changes in negative affect or mindfulness as a

function of experience with the slow walking (number of attended walking sessions, p > .05).

Consequently, scores from each participant were averaged for comparisons of pre-post walk on

negative affect and mindfulness. Single-group repeated-measure t-tests revealed significant

differences between participants’ average pre-and post-walk negative affect (t[27] = -6.81, p

< .01) and situational mindfulness scores (t[27] = 4.50, p < .01). Participants reported

significantly less negative affect (average d = -0.61, range = -0.60 to -0.77) and greater

situational mindfulness (average d = 0.55, range = 0.37 to 0.79) following the walking sessions.

Of note, 92% of the participants reported lower or similar levels of negative affect (Mdiff = -2.45,

SD = 3.52), and 87% reported higher or similar levels of mindfulness in the post-walking survey

across all mindful walking sessions (Mdiff = 1.06, SD = 2.03).

Post-walk negative affect was then regressed on pre-walk negative affect and changes in

situational mindfulness, F (9,178) = 15.57, p < .01. As seen in Table 4.4, after controlling for the

pre-walk negative affect scores and other temporal and contextual factors, residualized changes

in negative affect were associated with increases in situational mindfulness scores after the

Page 117: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

108

walking sessions (β = -0.25, p < .01). That is, older adults who increased their situational

mindfulness more than their usual level during the walks also decreased their negative affect

most. Overall, based on the conditional pseudo R2, the fixed factors in this model explained

approximately 50% of the variance in post-walk negative affect (Nakagawa & Schielzeth, 2013).

Open-ended feedback was very positive and there were no adverse events reported by

older adults related to participation. Participants mentioned that the program was interesting

(“Great experience! It was interesting overall”), beneficial (“There were days when I was upset

and could feel that my blood pressure was elevated, but I no longer felt that way after my walk”),

enjoyable (“I enjoyed it so much, even [though] sometimes very cold”), sustainable (“I liked the

environment and plan to return to the garden to walk after the program ends”), and fulfilled their

needs to reduce stress (“The program was very calming and very beneficial for me to relieve

stress”). Although mindfulness training was not explicitly addressed during the whole program,

several participants described feelings similar to what they had experienced during prior

meditation or yoga practice (“I found that slowing down my steps allowed relaxation, it’s almost

a meditation feeling”; “Helped me be more aware of myself and my surroundings, invigorating

somewhat like yoga”). Interestingly, some participants also mentioned that walking at the

arboretum helped them sustain physical activity or increase their intention for walking (“This

program increased my interest in walking in the arboretum periodically”). Some participants

raised concerns about the unpleasant weather while partaking this outdoor program in winter.

One participant reported not being able to concentrate on the mindfulness task and another

questioned whether the response scale on the survey could capture their mental experience.

Page 118: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

109

Discussion

This study evaluated the feasibility, acceptability, and sustainability of an outdoor

mindful walking program for reducing negative affect among older adults from the community.

Prior work suggested that the combination of walking and mindfulness could be a promising

mind-body intervention for reducing negative affect in a few clinical and adult populations, but

this combination had not previously been implemented with non-clinical samples of older adults.

This population could benefit from the intervention because negative affective responses such as

depressive symptoms are critical predictors of overall health and well-being among the elderly.

Experiencing higher levels of depressive symptoms are related to higher rates of dementia,

physical illness, functional disability, and greater expenditure of health cost and resources in the

aging population (Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics, 2016). Consistent

with hypotheses, results indicated that (1) it was feasible to implement an outdoor mindful

walking program with older adults, (2) the target population found the program to be acceptable,

and (3) even a brief supervised program could lead to sustained behavior change one-month after

the end of the program.

The mindful walking program implemented in this study differed from previous mindful

walking interventions in important ways. First, previous studies have implemented mindful

walking in group settings where participants had opportunities to converse or interact with each

other while walking (Gotink et al., 2016; Prakhinkit et al., 2013; Shin et al., 2013; Teut et al.,

2013). This group approach provides social distractions and may interfere with mindfulness

practices that require heightened attention. In contrast, the slow walking program implemented

here minimized social interaction and emphasized individual walking to facilitate focus during

Page 119: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

110

the mindfulness practices. Second, participants in previous outdoor mindful walking studies

typically walked in unfamiliar environments (e.g., forest, riverside), whereas the present study

used a consistent spatial context (Gotink et al., 2016; Shin et al., 2013). This consistency may

have facilitated situational mindfulness by reducing the attentional resources required while

walking. Third, previous interventions sometimes contained extra mindfulness or physical

activity components such as sitting meditation or gymnastic exercises (Gotink et al., 2016; Teut

et al., 2013). In contrast, the mindful walking program implemented here only included the

essential mindfulness practices to simplify the intervention design.

The high enrollment rate (85%) indicated that a slow walking program was appealing to

older adults and it is feasible to enroll older adults from the community. This rate may have been

impacted by the timing of recruitment to coincide with the approach of winter. Even better

recruitment rates might have been obtained if the program was advertised during a warmer

season. Despite the relatively cold weather, participants completed a large proportion of the

possible measures (95%), and 93% of the participants completed all study measures, lab visits,

and eight walking sessions as scheduled. The enrollment rate in our study was higher than two

previous mindful walking studies also reported the screening process. Studies from Teut et al.

(2013) and Gotink et al. (2016) had a 44% and 65% enrollment rate, respectively. Both studies

had similar sample sizes in mindful walking group (36 and 32 participants) as the current study.

The program completion rate in the current study was similar to previous mindful walking

studies which ranged from 85% to 92%. This consistent finding suggested that most participants

in mindful walking programs are able to comply and finish all study protocols, despite the

different designs and target populations. The high measurement completion and attendance rates

in this study further indicated that older adults in the community were committed and dedicated

Page 120: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

111

to the program. The current mindful walking program can be delivered as intended to older

population.

Based on the post-program questionnaire, participants evaluated the program positively.

The majority of them rated this program as highly satisfactory, useful, and interesting. Most

participants also indicated that they would recommend the program to others. The written

feedback from the participants were also largely positive. Some participants reflected that this

program met their needs for relieving stress, and others mentioned that the slow walking

practices in the current study were easy to perform and were similar to yoga or meditation

training. These comments were noteworthy because mindfulness was not mentioned when

advertising the current study. A few participants commented that the program motivated them to

sustain their physical activity level and walking habits. Future studies should investigate whether

a mindful walking program can support intentions to be active or help older adults to develop

walking habits. Feedback also indicated that the arboretum was an optimal and safe place to

administer the walking program. Several participants raised concerns about doing mindful

walking in adverse weather conditions, but ultimately no study-related adverse events were

reported in the current study. Two prior studies had participants doing mindful walking on

indoor oval tracks, however, there were no feedback reported from both studies, so we are not

aware of whether participants have similar perceptions while walking indoors versus outdoors

(Prakhinkit et al., 2013; Shin et al., 2013).

Participants reported a moderate-to-high level of maintenance one-month after

completing their last supervised mindful walking session. This rate greatly exceeded the prior

two-month estimate of 14% from Teut et al. (2013). This difference may reflect differences in

either the follow-up intervals, the populations sampled, or the program components. For

Page 121: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

112

example, Teut et al. (2013) recruited participants age between 18 to 65 who usually had not yet

retired, whereas participants in this study were all retired older adults. Time constraints related to

daily work schedules may be a barrier to maintaining mindful walking. Moreover, study from

Teut et al. (2013) also required participants to complete gymnastic exercises before and after the

mindful walking, whereas our program only required participants to walk slower while

practicing mindfulness. The extra component and burden required in that mindful walking

program may be another determinant of maintenance.

The study was not designed (or powered) to evaluate efficacy but preliminary analyses

indicated that participants who increased their situational mindfulness during walks also

decreased their negative affect. This result was consistent with previous mindfulness-based

walking programs showing significant reductions of negative affect after the walking sessions

among more distressed participants (Gotink et al., 2016; McCaffrey & Liehr, 2015; Prakhinkit et

al., 2013; Shin et al., 2013; Teut et al., 2013). It was also consistent with a previous mindfulness

study targeting older adults (Splevins et al., 2009). The present study concluded that an outdoor

mindful walking program was feasible, acceptable and sustainable. Due to the lack of a

comparison group in the present study, it was not possible to rule out factors from the program or

the environment that could account for the favorable changes (Duvall, 2011; Sternberg, 2010;

TKF Foundation, 2012). Future research should incorporate carefully-selected control groups

and assign participants to different conditions randomly to strengthen causal inferences about

intervention effects.

In contrast to prior studies, participants in this sample were not selected for high levels of

distress or negative affect. Participants in this study were mostly white, female, well-educated,

healthy, high-functioning, and had middle-to-high social economic status from a restricted

Page 122: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

113

geographic area. It is not clear whether participants from other sub-populations would accept,

complete, or sustain the mindful walking program implemented in this study as well. Future

studies should recruit more diverse samples in broader geographical regions. On a related note,

arthritis symptoms are common among older adults (Hootman, Helmick, Barbour, Theis, &

Boring, 2016). These symptoms are closely related to changes in barometric pressure and

ambient temperature (McAlindon, Formica, Schmid, & Fletcher, 2007). Walking outdoors

during winter may intensify painful feelings from arthritis, and impact participants’ affective

responses after walking. If future studies are conducted in similar conditions, it would be useful

to collect information on arthritis symptoms, subjective pain, and real-feel temperature during

each session.

The intervention dose in this study was informed by previous mindful walking programs

(Prakhinkit et al., 2013; Teut et al., 2013). There are no consensus guidelines in terms of the

frequency, duration, or other essential elements of an outdoor mindful walking program.

Findings may differ with different doses of mindful walking. We included only three basic

mindfulness techniques (focusing on breaths, steps and body scan) that were easy to learn if

participants were naïve in mindfulness practices. More advanced strategies could be

implemented in a mindful walking program and the results may differ. For instance, some

mindful walking practices encourage participants to direct their attention to the environment

(e.g., the smell, sounds, breeze, sun light), use a phrase with the appropriate number of syllables

to pace their breathing or stepping, modify inspiratory and expiratory rhythm as a function of

step length, or pay attention on multiple objects simultaneously (Hanh, 1985, 2011; Insight

Meditation Center, 2003). The program implemented in this study involved focusing narrowly on

one object at a time and did not challenge participants to broaden their attention field. The

Page 123: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

114

majority of the participants were female older adults and previous work has shown stronger

associations between light physical activity and well-being for women than men (Conroy, Wolin,

Blair, & Demark-Wahnefried, in press). It will be important to test potential sex differences in

intervention responses in future research. Finally, it was clear whether participants engaged in

extra mindful walking practice in their leisure time. Any differential changes could be due to any

additional practice they completed. Future studies should monitor activities outside of the

supervised sessions.

Conclusion

In sum, this study indicated that participants accepted and complied with the outdoor

mindful walking program. Many also sustained their engagement in mindful walking one-month

after the supervised sessions ended. This program was implemented as planned with older adults

and shows promise for helping them to regulate negative affect. The design of this study

precludes causal inferences about intervention effects but, based on the evidence for feasibility, it

would be appropriate to design and deliver a randomized controlled trial to evaluate efficacy.

Page 124: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

115

References

Ader, D. N. (2007). Developing the patient-reported outcomes measurement information system

(PROMIS): deression, anxiety, and anger. Medical Care, 45(Suppl 1), S1–S2.

https://doi.org/10.1097/01.mlr.0000260537.45076.74

Aggarwal, N. T., Wilson, R. S., Beck, T. L., Rajan, K. B., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Evans, D. A.,

& Everson-Rose, S. A. (2014). Perceived stress and change in cognitive function among

adults aged 65 and older. Psychosomatic Medicine, 76(1), 80–85.

https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000016

Black, S. V., Cooper, R., Martin, K. R., Brage, S., Kuh, D., & Stafford, M. (2015). Physical

activity and mental well-being in a cohort aged 60–64 years. American Journal of

Preventive Medicine, 49(2), 172–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2015.03.009

Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in

psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

Bryant, C., Jackson, H., & Ames, D. (2008). The prevalence of anxiety in older adults:

Methodological issues and a review of the literature. Journal of Affective Disorders,

109(3), 233–250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2007.11.008

Buman, M. P., Hekler, E. B., Haskell, W. L., Pruitt, L., Conway, T. L., Cain, K. L., … King, A.

C. (2010). Objective light-intensity physical activity associations with rated health in

older adults. American Journal of Epidemiology, 172(10), 1155–1165.

https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwq249

Page 125: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

116

CDC. (2008). The State of Mental Health and Aging in America Issue Brief 1: What Do the Data

Tell Us? National Association of Chronic Disease Directors. Atlanta, GA. Retrieved from

https://www.cdc.gov/aging/pdf/mental_health.pdf

Conroy, D. E., Wolin K.Y., Blair C.K., & Demark-Wahnefried W. (in press). Gender-varying

associations between physical activity intensity and mental quality of life in older cancer

survivors. Supportive Care in Cancer.

Davidson, R. J., & Kaszniak, A. W. (2015). Conceptual and methodological issues in research on

mindfulness and meditation. American Psychologist, 70(7), 581–592.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039512

Duvall, J. (2011). Enhancing the benefits of outdoor walking with cognitive engagement

strategies. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 31(1), 27–35.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.09.003

Eisinga, R., Grotenhuis, M. te, & Pelzer, B. (2013). The reliability of a two-item scale: Pearson,

Cronbach, or Spearman-Brown? International Journal of Public Health, 58(4), 637–642.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00038-012-0416-3

Farias, S. T., Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., Cahn-Weiner, D., Jagust, W., Baynes, K., & DeCarli, C.

(2008). The measurement of everyday cognition (ECog): Scale development and

psychometric properties. Neuropsychology, 22(4), 531–544.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.22.4.531

Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics. (2016). Older Americans 2016: Key

indicators of well-being. The Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics.

Fjorback, L. O., Arendt, M., Ø rnbøl, E., Fink, P., & Walach, H. (2011). Mindfulness-Based

Stress Reduction and Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy – a systematic review of

Page 126: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

117

randomized controlled trials. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 124(2), 102–119.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01704.x

Foulk, M. A., Ingersoll-Dayton, B., Kavanagh, J., Robinson, E., & Kales, H. C. (2014).

Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy with older adults: An exploratory study. Journal

of Gerontological Social Work, 57(5), 498–520.

https://doi.org/10.1080/01634372.2013.869787

Gerteis J. Izrael D. Deitz D. LeRoy L. Ricciardi R. Miller T. & Basu J. (2014). Multiple Chronic

Conditions Chartbook. Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality.

Retrieved from

https://www.ahrq.gov/sites/default/files/wysiwyg/professionals/prevention-chronic-

care/decision/mcc/mccchartbook.pdf

Gotink, R. A., Chu, P., Busschbach, J. J. V., Benson, H., Fricchione, G. L., & Hunink, M. G. M.

(2015). Standardised mindfulness-based interventions in healthcare: An overview of

systematic reviews and meta-analyses of RCTs. PLOS ONE, 10(4), e0124344.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124344

Gotink, R. A., Hermans, K. S. F. M., Geschwind, N., Nooij, R. D., Groot, W. T. D., & Speckens,

A. E. M. (2016). Mindfulness and mood stimulate each other in an upward spiral: A

mindful walking intervention using experience sampling. Mindfulness, 7(5), 1114–1122.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-016-0550-8

Gu, J., Strauss, C., Bond, R., & Cavanagh, K. (2015). How do mindfulness-based cognitive

therapy and mindfulness-based stress reduction improve mental health and wellbeing? A

systematic review and meta-analysis of mediation studies. Clinical Psychology Review,

37, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2015.01.006

Page 127: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

118

Hanh, T. N. (1985). A Guide to Walking Meditation. Nyack, NY: Fellowship Publications.

Hanh, T. N. (2011). The Long Road Turns to Joy: A Guide to Walking Meditation. Berkeley:

Parallax Press.

Harmell, A. L., Jeste, D., & Depp, C. (2014). Strategies for successful aging: A research update.

Current Psychiatry Reports, 16(10), 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-014-0476-6

Hootman, J. M., Helmick, C. G., Barbour, K. E., Theis, K. A., & Boring, M. A. (2016). Updated

projected prevalence of self-reported doctor-diagnosed arthritis and arthritis-attributable

activity limitation among US adults, 2015-2040. Arthritis & Rheumatology, 68(7), 1582–

1587. https://doi.org/10.1002/art.39692

Insight Meditation Center. (2003). Instructions for Walking Meditation. Retrieved from

http://www.insightmeditationcenter.org/books-articles/articles/instructions-for-walking-

meditation/

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever You Go, There You Are: Mindfulness Meditation in Everyday

Life. New York: Hyperion.

Kabat-Zinn, J., & Borysenko, J. Z. (1996). Full Catastrophe Living: How to Cope with Stress,

Pain and Illness Using Mindfulness Meditation. London: Piatkus Books.

Kabat-Zinn, J., & Hanh, T. N. (2009). Full Catastrophe Living: Using the Wisdom of Your Body

and Mind to Face Stress, Pain, and Illness. New York: Random House.

Kishita, N., Takei, Y., & Stewart, I. (2016). A meta-analysis of third wave mindfulness-based

cognitive behavioral therapies for older people. International Journal of Geriatric

Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.4621

La Forge, R. (2016). Mind-body (mindful) exercise in practice. ACSM’s Health & Fitness

Journal, 20(4), 6–8.

Page 128: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

119

Loprinzi, P. D., Lee, H., & Cardinal, B. J. (2015). Evidence to support including lifestyle light-

intensity recommendations in Physical Activity Guidelines for Older Adults. American

Journal of Health Promotion, 29(5), 277–284. https://doi.org/10.4278/ajhp.130709-

QUAN-354

McAlindon, T., Formica, M., Schmid, C. H., & Fletcher, J. (2007). Changes in barometric

pressure and ambient temperature influence osteoarthritis pain. The American Journal of

Medicine, 120(5), 429–434. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amjmed.2006.07.036

McCaffrey, R., & Liehr, P. (2015). The effect of reflective garden walking on adults with

increased levels of psychological stress. Journal of Holistic Nursing, 898010115594934.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0898010115594934

McLaughlin, S. J., Connell, C. M., Heeringa, S. G., Li, L. W., & Roberts, J. S. (2010).

Successful aging in the United States: Prevalence estimates from a national sample of

older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 65B(2), 216–226.

https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbp101

Morgan, D. (2003). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: A new approach to

preventing relapse. Psychotherapy Research, 13(1), 123–125.

https://doi.org/10.1080/713869628

Morone, N. E., & Greco, C. M. (2014). Adapting mindfulness meditation for the older adult.

Mindfulness, 5(5), 610–612. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-014-0297-z

Nakagawa, S., & Schielzeth, H. (2013). A general and simple method for obtaining R 2 from

generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 4(2), 133–

142. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-210x.2012.00261.x

Page 129: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

120

National Council on Aging. (2015). Facts About Chronic Disease Management. Retrieved May

23, 2017, from https://www.ncoa.org/news/resources-for-reporters/get-the-facts/chronic-

disease-facts/

Nelson, M. E., Rejeski, W. J., Blair, S. N., Duncan, P. W., Judge, J. O., King, A. C., …

Castaneda-Sceppa, C. (2007). Physical activity and public health in older adults.

Recommendation from the American College of Sports Medicine and the American

Heart Association. Circulation.

https://doi.org/10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.107.185650

Netz, Y., Wu, M.-J., Becker, B. J., & Tenenbaum, G. (2005). Physical Activity and

psychological well-being in advanced age: A meta-analysis of intervention studies.

Psychology and Aging, 20(2), 272–284. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.2.272

Pilkonis, P. A., Choi, S. W., Reise, S. P., Stover, A. M., Riley, W. T., & Cella, D. (2011). Item

banks for measuring emotional distress from the Patient-Reported Outcomes

Measurement Information System (PROMIS® ): Depression, anxiety, and anger.

Assessment, 18(3), 263–283. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191111411667

Prakhinkit, S., Suppapitiporn, S., Tanaka, H., & Suksom, D. (2013). Effects of Buddhism

walking meditation on depression, functional fitness, and endothelium-dependent

vasodilation in depressed elderly. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary

Medicine, 20(5), 411–416. https://doi.org/10.1089/acm.2013.0205

Rebar, A. L., Stanton, R., Geard, D., Short, C., Duncan, M. J., & Vandelanotte, C. (2015). A

meta-meta-analysis of the effect of physical activity on depression and anxiety in non-

clinical adult populations. Health Psychology Review, 9(3), 366–378.

https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2015.1022901

Page 130: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

121

Rosenbaum, S., Tiedemann, A., Sherrington, C., Curtis, J., & Ward, P. B. (2014). Physical

activity interventions for people with mental illness: a systematic review and meta-

analysis. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 75(9), 964–974.

https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.13r08765

Russell, T. (2011). Body in mind training: mindful movement for severe and enduring mental

illness. Br. J. Wellbeing, 2(3), 13–16.

Shin, Y.-K., Kim, D. J., Jung-Choi, K., Son, Y., Koo, J.-W., Min, J.-A., & Chae, J.-H. (2013).

Differences of psychological effects between meditative and athletic walking in a forest

and gymnasium. Scandinavian Journal of Forest Research, 28(1), 64–72.

https://doi.org/10.1080/02827581.2012.706634

Splevins, K., Smith, A., & Simpson, J. (2009). Do improvements in emotional distress correlate

with becoming more mindful? A study of older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 13(3),

328–335. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607860802459807

Stathopoulou, G., Powers, M. B., Berry, A. C., Smits, J. A. J., & Otto, M. W. (2006). Exercise

interventions for mental health: A quantitative and qualitative review. Clinical

Psychology: Science and Practice, 13(2), 179–193. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-

2850.2006.00021.x

Steenbergen, M. R., & Jones, B. S. (2002). Modeling Multilevel Data Structures. American

Journal of Political Science, 46(1), 218–237. https://doi.org/10.2307/3088424

Steptoe, A., Deaton, A., & Stone, A. A. (2015). Psychological wellbeing, health and ageing.

Lancet, 385(9968), 640–648. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61489-0

Sternberg, E. M. (2010). Healing Spaces: The Science of Place and Well-Being. Cambridge,

MA; London: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

Page 131: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

122

Stokes, S. A., & Gordon, S. E. (2003). Common stressors experienced by the well elderly:

Clinical implications. Journal of Gerontological Nursing; Thorofare, 29(5), 38–46.

Stults-Kolehmainen, M. A., & Sinha, R. (2013). The effects of stress on physical activity and

exercise. Sports Medicine, 44(1), 81–121. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-013-0090-5

Tanay, G., & Bernstein, A. (2013). State Mindfulness Scale (SMS): Development and initial

validation. Psychological Assessment, 25(4), 1286–1299.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034044

Teut, M., Roesner, E. J., Ortiz, M., Reese, F., Binting, S., Roll, S., … Brinkhaus, B. (2013).

Mindful walking in psychologically distressed individuals: A randomized controlled trial.

Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, 2013, e489856.

https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/489856

TKF Foundation. (2012). Research on the beneficial aspects of the experience of nature in cities:

A literature review. Retrieved from http://your.kingcounty.gov/dnrp/library/water-and-

land/land-conservation/meeting-09-29-2016/benefits-of-nature-in-cities-lit-review-02-

12.pdf

Tsang, H. W. H., Chan, E. P., & Cheung, W. M. (2008). Effects of mindful and non-mindful

exercises on people with depression: A systematic review. British Journal of Clinical

Psychology, 47(3), 303–322. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466508X279260

WHO. (2017). Mental health and older adults. Retrieved from

http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs381/en/

Young, L. A., & Baime, M. J. (2010). Mindfulness-based stress reduction: Effect on emotional

distress in older adults. Complementary Health Practice Review, 15(2), 59–64.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1533210110387687

Page 132: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

123

Figure Caption

Figure 4.1. CONSORT flow diagram of mindful walking program.

Page 133: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

124

Assessed for eligibility (n= 34)

Assessed for eligibility (n= 34)

Excluded (n= 5)

Not meeting inclusion criteria (n=1)

Declined to participate (n=3)

Other reason – Injured before the initial

lab visit (n=1)

Date Analysed (n= 27)

Attending initial lab visit (n= 29)

Completed baseline assessment (n= 29)

Started walking program (n=29)

Attending initial lab visit (n= 29)

Completed baseline assessment (n= 29)

Started walking program (n=29)

Completed program and post assessment

(n= 27)

Completed program and post assessment

(n= 27)

Intervention

Analysis

Analysis

End of Program

End of Program

Enrollment

Enrollment

Dropped out (n= 2)

Injured from leisure activity

participation (n=1)

Incapability of self-navigating (n=1)

Lost to follow-up (n=1)

Lost contact

Follow-Up

Follow-Up

Follow-up Date Analysed (n= 26)

)

Completed one-month follow-up (n= 26)

Completed one-month follow-up (n= 26)

Analysis

Analysis

Page 134: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

125

Table 4.1. Overview of the eight walking sessions in the mindful walking program

Session Mindful

walking

Slow

walking

Mindfulness task and duration

1 0 minutes 30 minutes No mindfulness task. Participants familiarized

themselves with the arboretum trail and practice slow

walking individually.

2 5 minutes 25 minutes Walking slowly while sustaining their attention on the

rhythm of their every breath.

3 10 minutes 20 minutes Walking slowly while sustaining their attention on the

rhythm of their every breath.

4 10 minutes 20 minutes Same as session 3

5 20 minutes 10 minutes First 10 minutes: walking slowly while sustaining their

attention on the rhythm of their every breath.

Second 10 minutes: awareness of the gentle heel-to-toe

rhythm for each step they make.

6 20 minutes 10 minutes Same as session 5

7 30 minutes 0 minutes First 10 minutes: walking slowly while sustaining their

attention on the rhythm of their every breath.

Second 10 minutes: awareness of the gentle heel-to-toe

rhythm for each step they make.

Last 10 minutes: full body scan to expand their attention

to any bodily sensation or emotional feelings that may

arise.

8 30 minutes 0 minutes Same as session 7

Page 135: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

126

Table 4.2. Contexts of the mindful walking sessions

N (%)

Month

October 4 (2%)

November 158 (71%)

December 60 (27%)

Day of the week

Monday 40 (18%)

Tuesday 51 (23%)

Wednesday 40 (18%)

Thursday 21 (10%)

Friday 39 (17%)

Saturday 18 (8%)

Sunday 13 (6%)

Time of the day

9am-11am 67 (30%)

11am-1pm 106 (48%)

1pm-3pm 31 (14%)

3pm-5pm 18 (8%)

Daily average temperature

15-32°F 41 (18%)

32-45°F 97 (44%)

46-60°F 84 (38%)

Prevailing Weather Condition

Sunny 56 (25%)

Fair, partly or mostly cloudy 82 (37%)

Cloudy and wind 62 (28%)

Rain shower or rain snow 11 (5%)

Snow or hailing 11 (5%)

Page 136: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

127

Table 4.3. Correlation coefficient matrix of the pre-post walking session variables. M

(SD)

1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12.

1. Age 73.2

(6.47) 1

2. Gender - -0.04 1

3. Sequence of

walking session - -0.01 -0.03 1

4. Day of the week - 0.02 0.02 0.04 1

5. Average

Temperature (°F)

40.6

(8.36) 0.10 0.07 -0.36** 0.01 1

6. Survey time

(hour)

11.78

(1.57) 0.03 0.01 -0.05 -0.28** 0.08 1

7. Sleep quality 5.40

(1.38) -0.11 0.05 0.04 0.08 -0.02 -0.06 1

8. Baseline

Attention

4.98

(0.78) 0.11 -0.06 -0.01 0.02 -0.01 0.16* 0.19** 1

9. Pre-session

negative affect

8.22

(4.49) 0.05 -0.13* 0.05 -0.01 -0.07 -0.06 -0.17* -0.29** 1

10. Post-session

negative affect

5.78

(3.29) 0.04 0.01 0.01 0.01 -0.09 -0.04 -0.17* -0.33** 0.60** 1

11. Pre-session

mindful level

9.46

(2.23) 0.03 -0.21** 0.19** -0.03 0.01 0.06 0.02 0.10 0.13 0.02 1

12. Post-session

mindful level

10.52

(2.08) -0.09 -0.17** 0.14* -0.03 -0.01 -0.08 0.17* 0.01 0.01 -0.17* 0.54** 1

Note: *p< .05; **p<.01; N=222.

Page 137: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

128

Table 4.4. Result of multilevel model examining associations between mindfulness level and

negative affective reactivity

Model Coefficient SE

Fixed Effect

(Intercept) 6.69 4.39

Age -0.02 0.04

Gender

0.55 0.72

Number of walking session 0.08 0.09

Day of the week -0.01 0.09

Average temperature -0.03 0.02

Baseline attention -0.60 0.38

Survey time 0.07 0.12

Previous day sleep quality -0.20 0.14

Average pre-walking negative affect 0.55** 0.09

Deviations from average negative affect 0.22** 0.05

Average mindfulness change (pre-walk to post-walk) -0.31 0.24

Deviations from average mindfulness change

(pre-walk to post-walk)

-0.27** 0.09

Random Effect

Variance Intercept 1.35

Residual Variance 4.41

Note: Analysis was based on 7 walking sessions with mindfulness practice incorporated.

*p < .05; **p < .01.

Page 138: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

129

Chapter 5: Conclusions

Page 139: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

130

This dissertation investigated relations between physical activity and mindfulness to

understand how their interaction influenced well-being in college students and older adults.

Findings from this dissertation can be applied to inform future research on mindfulness and

physical activity, as well as to guide the development of mind-body approaches to improve

health and well-being. Specific contributions of this work are reviewed below.

This dissertation provided the first systematic review on the association between physical

activity and mindfulness. Previous reviews have briefly synthesized the salutary impact of

mindfulness on cardiovascular disease risk, physical and mental health and provided several

supporting studies (Gilbert & Waltz, 2010; Kennedy & Resnick, 2015; Loucks et al., 2015).

These reviews provided evidence on the positive relation between physical activity and

mindfulness but did not investigate the strength of the relation based on all available quantitative

studies. This dissertation presented current evidence of physical activity-mindfulness association

through a systematic literature search of quantitative studies in major academic databases. After

screening over 1500 articles, 13 studies generally supported the conclusion that physical activity

level is positively related to people’s mindfulness level, but there was a lack of evidence from

experimental studies. Future research should be devoted to conduct more quantitative studies

including randomized controlled trials to provide stronger evidence on how mindfulness may

impact individual’s physical activity level.

This review also identified critical gaps in the literature that may hinder the advance of

studying the role of mindfulness in physical activity. Mindfulness is a mental construct that

involves both between-person and within-person variation (Bishop et al., 2004; Brown & Ryan,

2003). Current studies exclusively focused on people’s trait mindfulness level and failed to

consider the variability of people’s mindfulness levels that are context- and situational-

Page 140: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

131

dependent. Without highlighting the trait and state differences, we are not able to obtain accurate

and precise understanding of the association between physical activity and mindfulness. The

proposed three-level hierarchical framework is a specific contribution of this work. This model

differentiates between global, contextual, and situational forms of mindfulness based on the

contextual and temporal specificity of the phenomenon. To enhance communication and advance

knowledge in the field, future studies should study the association between physical activity and

the contextual or situational mindfulness. Measures are needed to assess contextual and

situational levels of mindfulness. These developments will facilitate research on within-person

associations between mindfulness, physical activity, and health and well-being outcomes.

This dissertation also revealed the within-person association between daily activity

(posture) and mindfulness in predicting negative affect among college students. Previous

experience sampling studies reported mixed findings regarding the impact of daily physical

activity level in predicting subsequent negative affect. Some studies suggested an inverse

association whereas others found no significant results between the two variables (Liao,

Shonkoff, & Dunton, 2015). This dissertation expended previous work by exploring the role of

mindfulness during physical activity and its association with negative affect. A clear inverse

relation between daily activity and negative affect was observed and different levels of

situational mindfulness may moderate momentary activity (posture) in predicting negative affect

in college students. Students with higher situational mindfulness while moving or standing

(compared to sitting) experienced greater reductions in negative affect in their daily life. This

finding indicated that interventions may lead to greater mental health benefits by integrating

mindfulness practices with physical activity.

Page 141: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

132

Additionally, the research in this dissertation established a negative association between

daily postures and negative affective states. Relative to being sedentary, when college students

move or stand more in their daily life, they reported lower levels of negative affect. The physical

activity literature thus far has documented the effect of moderate to vigorous physical activity on

well-being and mental health (Ensari, Greenlee, Motl, & Petruzzello, 2015; Penedo & Dahn,

2005; Rebar et al., 2015). The preliminary effect of low-intensity physical activity on physical

health has also been investigated in recent years (Bann et al., 2015; Carson et al., 2013; Herzig et

al., 2014). This dissertation provided extra contribution to the physical activity literature by

showing that low-intensity daily activities were positively related to college student’s

psychological well-being. Most importantly, a useful public health message can be implied from

this result was that simply by moving or standing more and not sitting in everyday life may

reduce negative affect and thus improve well-being in the college population.

The final study in this dissertation evaluated the feasibility of implementing a mind-body

intervention program based on the premise that combining physical activity and situational

mindfulness may decrease negative affect among older adults. In this mindful walking program,

an integration of mindfulness practice with walking activity was delivered to older adults to

evaluate its feasibility, acceptability, and sustainability in reducing negative affect. This

feasibility study was initiated because none of the mindful walking programs to date have

targeted psychological well-being in the non-clinical older adult populations. The major

hypothesis that an outdoor mindful walking program was well-accepted, engaged, and

maintained were later supported by both the self-report ratings and the written feedback from

older adults. The secondary hypothesis that mindful walking program could show preliminary

Page 142: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

133

efficacy in decreasing older adults’ negative affect was also suggested by the analysis from the

repeated measures across mindful walking sessions.

Finally, this dissertation demonstrated that this mind-body intervention with eight 30-

minutes mindful walking within one month is feasible to deliver to healthy, community-dwelling

older adults. Based on the positive results of program feasibility, future experimental studies

should be developed and executed to test the effect of mindful walking in improving well-being

and healthy aging in the older population. Randomized controlled trials with different control

arms are also needed to disentangle the relative effectiveness of mindful walking intervention

with other individual or environmental factors. For example, outdoor physical activities have

been reported to impact negative affective response greater than indoor physical activities

(Dunton, Liao, Intille, Huh, & Leventhal, 2015). Different control arms such as sitting

meditation, indoor mindful walking, and walking with cognitive training may help to further

elaborate whether different types of mindful activity, or different mindful walking environments,

or different forms of mental task engaged during walking may play relatively critical roles in

explaining the beneficial psychological effect.

Limitations, Future Directions, and Conclusion

Although this dissertation achieved its scientific objectives, several limitations should be

noted to guide future research. First, the associations between mindfulness, physical activity, and

negative affect identified in this dissertation were based on self-report measures exclusively. The

systematic review reported in Chapter 2 did not identify any studies that applied objective

measures of mindfulness in relation to physical activity. In the experience sampling study

reported in Chapter 3, two items from a validated scale were used to assess situational

mindfulness. This ecological momentary assessment of mindfulness was recommended by

Page 143: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

134

Davidson and Kaszniak (2015) to minimize retrospective bias from self-reporting mindfulness.

In the feasibility study reported in Chapter 4, the same two mindfulness items were included in

the pre-walk and post-walk surveys. Mindfulness has been conceptualized as both a

unidimensional and multidimensional phenomenon and there is no consensus on the necessary

components or the number of facets that should be included in a mindfulness scale. The validity

of assessing subjective mindfulness experience with self-report questionnaires is also under

discussion (Baer, 2016; Bergomi, Tschacher, & Kupper, 2012; Davidson, 2010; Goldberg et al.,

2015; Sauer et al., 2012). Physical activity researchers should strive to include objective

measures of mindfulness based on cognitive and attentional tasks, neuroimaging or

neuropsychological measures to strengthen conclusions about the association between physical

activity and mindfulness.

Similarly, physical activity measures have largely relied on self-report methods in this

literature. Only one study in our literature review provided accelerometry-based data on

participants’ physical activity level (Kangasniemi, Lappalainen, Kankaanpää, & Tammelin,

2014). Although our ecological momentary assessment on physical activity and postural patterns

exhibited similar results as population-based studies, future studies should consider device-based

measures of physical activity when studying the within-person relation between physical activity

and subjective experiences (Kanning, Ebner-Priemer, & Schlicht, 2013).

The subjective negative affect assessed in the studies reported in Chapters 3 and 4

involved stress, anxiety, and depression. The common component of this selective set of negative

affective states involves a generalized sense of distress (Akin, 2007). There are in fact other

common aspects of negative affect (e.g., anger, fatigue, upset, disappointed) which have been

assessed in other studies (Kuppens, Van Mechelen, Nezlek, Dossche, & Timmermans, 2007;

Page 144: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

135

Watson & Clark, 1999). Future studies should include different negative affective states in

mindfulness and physical activity research to investigate whether the results may differ when

different aspects of negative affect are assessed.

Second, the participants were relatively homogeneous in each of the empirical studies

reported here. The younger sample in Chapter 3 was mostly White, not Hispanic, and educated.

Findings may not generalize to college student samples from other geographical locations, to

people from more diverse racial and ethnic backgrounds, or to young adults who do not attend

college or university. Young adults without college experience likely encounter different stressors

in daily life and may have different affective patterns. Likewise, the older sample in Chapter 4

was mostly White, healthy, and affluent. Results may differ if the program is launched in an

urban area with a more diverse population, or in older adults with clinical issues. For example,

urban environments pose an extra threat on people’s well-being (Lederbogen et al., 2011;

Matheson et al., 2006). The limited green space and the crowded environment in the city also

may limit opportunities to implement this mindful walking program in urban environments.

Future research should target more diverse samples and test whether the beneficial outcomes

identified in this dissertation may vary as a function of demographic factors.

Third, this dissertation mainly focused on intrapersonal factors and did not assess many

other external factors that may also regulate negative affect. Data analyses adjusted for variation

associated with the temporal factors that may impact negative affect in college students, but did

not involve other interpersonal factors such as the presence of others (classmates, friends, or

family members), or contextual factors such as the location (classroom, dorm, or home) at the

time of daily prompts. These factors may influence or interact with students’ mindfulness and

physical activity in predicting negative affect. Temporal factors and environmental conditions

Page 145: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

136

were also modeled in the feasibility study, however, unexpected life events can also influence

older adult’s affective states. For example, one participant acknowledged in her written feedback

that her mood seemed to be dampened by a major political event – the US presidential election –

that occurred during the study. Thus, the adoption of a more complete social-ecological

perspective in future studies could advance understanding of the interpersonal, intrapersonal and

external factors that regulate individual’s affective states (Evans, 2003; Lakhan & Ekúndayò,

2013).

Finally, the mindful walking program designed in this dissertation cultivated participants’

attention, awareness, acceptance, and non-judgmental attitudes to their mental or physical

experiences. These techniques are basic and fundamental mindfulness practices (Hart, Ivtzan, &

Hart, 2013), however, other mindfulness components such as curiosity, empathy, and compassion

have also been applied in mindfulness programs and were not included in this study (Birnie,

Speca, & Carlson, 2010; Greeson, 2009; Langer, 1992). Mindfulness interventions emphasizing

self-compassion have previously shown promise for regulating negative affect (Van Dam,

Sheppard, Forsyth, & Earleywine, 2011). It would be interesting to examine whether mindful

walking programs consisting of different mindfulness components yield similar findings in

future studies. Experimental studies can also can be conducted to explore the relative impact of

different mindfulness components in reducing negative affect.

People face a variety of stressors across adulthood and stress can detract from health and

well-being. There is great interest in mind-body approaches to promote health and well-being

and both physical activity and mindfulness represent promising strategies. This dissertation

enriched understanding of how physical activity and mindfulness can be used to promote well-

being. The proposed three-level framework of mindfulness can be used to direct future studies

Page 146: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

137

and advance understanding of relations between mindfulness and physical activity. This

dissertation further investigated the interplay between physical activity and mindfulness in

reducing negative affect. Knowing that negative affect may decrease to a greater degree by being

more active and mindful momentarily provides a foundation for designing just-in-time

interventions to improve well-being in our everyday life. Furthermore, the potential impact of

non-exercise-related bodily postures on negative affect converges with assertions that, “some

activity is better than none” (Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee, 2008). Finally,

this dissertation demonstrated that an outdoor mindful walking program can be delivered to

community-dwelling older adults. The ease of combining mindful practice with daily walks, and

the minimal fitness level required for mindful walking makes it a viable preventative strategy for

improving well-being and promoting healthy aging in older adults. The efficacy of this program

for reducing negative affect and improving other indicators of health and well-being remains to

be determined and should be a logical next step in this rapidly-developing literature on mind-

body approaches to improve health and well-being.

Page 147: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

138

References

Akin, A. (2007). The Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS): The study of validity and

reliability.

Baer, R. (2016). Assessment of mindfulness and closely related constructs: Introduction to the

special issue. Psychological Assessment, 28(7), 787–790.

https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000309

Bann, D., Hire, D., Manini, T., Cooper, R., Botoseneanu, A., McDermott, M. M., … for the LIFE

Study Group. (2015). Light intensity physical activity and sedentary behavior in relation

to body mass index and grip strength in older adults: Cross-sectional findings from the

Lifestyle Interventions and Independence for Elders (LIFE) study. PLoS ONE, 10(2),

e0116058. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0116058

Bergomi, C., Tschacher, W., & Kupper, Z. (2012). The assessment of mindfulness with self-

report measures: Existing scales and open issues. Mindfulness, 4(3), 191–202.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0110-9

Birnie, K., Speca, M., & Carlson, L. E. (2010). Exploring self-compassion and empathy in the

context of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR). Stress and Health, 26(5), 359–

371. https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.1305

Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., … Devins, G.

(2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science

and Practice, 11(3), 230–241. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bph077

Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in

psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

Page 148: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

139

Carson, V., Ridgers, N. D., Howard, B. J., Winkler, E. A. H., Healy, G. N., Owen, N., … Salmon,

J. (2013). Light-intensity physical activity and cardiometabolic biomarkers in US

adolescents. PLoS ONE, 8(8), e71417. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071417

Davidson, R. J. (2010). Empirical explorations of mindfulness: Conceptual and methodological

conundrums. Emotion, 10(1), 8–11. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018480

Davidson, R. J., & Kaszniak, A. W. (2015). Conceptual and methodological issues in research on

mindfulness and meditation. American Psychologist, 70(7), 581–592.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039512

Dunton, G. F., Liao, Y., Intille, S., Huh, J., & Leventhal, A. (2015). Momentary assessment of

contextual influences on affective response during physical activity. Health Psychology:

Official Journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological

Association, 34(12), 1145–1153. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000223

Ensari, I., Greenlee, T. A., Motl, R. W., & Petruzzello, S. J. (2015). Meta-analysis of acute

exercise effects on state anxiety: An update of randomized controlled trials over the past

25 years. Depression and Anxiety, 32(8), 624–634. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22370

Evans, G. (2003). The built environment and mental health. Journal of Urban Health, 80(4),

536–555. https://doi.org/10.1093/jurban/jtg063

Gilbert, D., & Waltz, J. (2010). Mindfulness and health behaviors. Mindfulness, 1(4), 227–234.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-010-0032-3

Goldberg, S. B., Wielgosz, J., Dahl, C., Schuyler, B., MacCoon, D. S., Rosenkranz, M., …

Davidson, R. J. (2015). Does the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire measure what we

think it does? Construct validity evidence from an active controlled randomized clinical

trial. Psychological Assessment, 28(8), 1009-1014. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000233

Page 149: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

140

Greeson, J. M. (2009). Mindfulness research update: 2008. Complementary Health Practice

Review, 14(1), 10–18. https://doi.org/10.1177/1533210108329862

Hart, R., Ivtzan, I., & Hart, D. (2013). Mind the gap in mindfulness research: A comparative

account of the leading schools of thought. Review of General Psychology, 17(4), 453–

466. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035212

Herzig, K.-H., Ahola, R., Leppäluoto, J., Jokelainen, J., Jämsä, T., & Keinänen-Kiukaanniemi, S.

(2014). Light physical activity determined by a motion sensor decreases insulin

resistance, improves lipid homeostasis and reduces visceral fat in high-risk subjects:

PreDiabEx study RCT. International Journal of Obesity, 38(8), 1089–1096.

https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2013.224

Kangasniemi, A., Lappalainen, R., Kankaanpää, A., & Tammelin, T. (2014). Mindfulness skills,

psychological flexibility, and psychological symptoms among physically less active and

active adults. Mental Health and Physical Activity, 7(3), 121–127.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2014.06.005

Kanning, M. K., Ebner-Priemer, U. W., & Schlicht, W. M. (2013). How to investigate within-

subject associations between physical activity and momentary affective states in everyday

life: A position statement based on a literature overview. Frontiers in Psychology, 4.

https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00187

Kennedy, A. B., & Resnick, P. B. (2015). Mindfulness and physical activity. American Journal of

Lifestyle Medicine, 9(3), 221–223. https://doi.org/10.1177/1559827614564546

Kuppens, P., Van Mechelen, I., Nezlek, J. B., Dossche, D., & Timmermans, T. (2007). Individual

differences in core affect variability and their relationship to personality and

Page 150: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

141

psychological adjustment. Emotion, 7(2), 262–274. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-

3542.7.2.262

Lakhan, R., & Ekúndayò, O. T. (2013). Application of the ecological framework in depression:

An approach whose time has come. AP J Psychol Med, 14, 103–9.

Langer, E. J. (1992). Matters of mind: Mindfulness/mindlessness in perspective. Consciousness

and Cognition, 1(3), 289–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/1053-8100(92)90066-J

Lederbogen, F., Kirsch, P., Haddad, L., Streit, F., Tost, H., Schuch, P., … Meyer-Lindenberg, A.

(2011). City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans.

Nature, 474(7352), 498–501. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10190

Liao, Y., Shonkoff, E. T., & Dunton, G. F. (2015). The acute relationships between affect,

physical feeling states, and physical activity in daily life: A review of current evidence.

Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01975

Loucks, E. B., Schuman-Olivier, Z., Britton, W. B., Fresco, D. M., Desbordes, G., Brewer, J. A.,

& Fulwiler, C. (2015). Mindfulness and cardiovascular disease risk: State of the

evidence, plausible mechanisms, and theoretical framework. Current Cardiology Reports,

17(12). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11886-015-0668-7

Matheson, F. I., Moineddin, R., Dunn, J. R., Creatore, M. I., Gozdyra, P., & Glazier, R. H.

(2006). Urban neighborhoods, chronic stress, gender and depression. Social Science &

Medicine, 63(10), 2604–2616. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.07.001

Penedo, F. J., & Dahn, J. R. (2005). Exercise and well-being: a review of mental and physical

health benefits associated with physical activity. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 18(2),

189–193.

Page 151: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

142

Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee. (2008). Physical activity guidelines advisory

committee report, 2008. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human

Services, 2008, A1-H14.

Rebar, A. L., Stanton, R., Geard, D., Short, C., Duncan, M. J., & Vandelanotte, C. (2015). A

meta-meta-analysis of the effect of physical activity on depression and anxiety in non-

clinical adult populations. Health Psychology Review, 9(3), 366–378.

https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2015.1022901

Sauer, S., Walach, H., Schmidt, S., Hinterberger, T., Lynch, S., Büssing, A., & Kohls, N. (2012).

Assessment of mindfulness: Review on state of the art. Mindfulness, 4(1), 3–17.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0122-5

Van Dam, N. T., Sheppard, S. C., Forsyth, J. P., & Earleywine, M. (2011). Self-compassion is a

better predictor than mindfulness of symptom severity and quality of life in mixed

anxiety and depression. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25(1), 123–130.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.08.011

Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1999). The PANAS-X: Manual for the positive and negative affect

schedule-expanded form. Retrieved from

http://ir.uiowa.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1011&context=psychology_pubs

Page 152: A TIME-VARYING PERSPECTIVE ON MINDFULNESS AND PHYSICAL

143

Curriculum Vitae Chih-Hsiang Yang

CONTACT INFORMATION

17 Rec Hall (Motivation Lab)

Department of Kinesiology

The Pennsylvania State University

University Park, PA 16803 USA

Office: 814-654-7935

Cell phone: 814-777-6976

Email: [email protected]

EDUCATION

PhD (candidate), Department of Kinesiology, The Pennsylvania State University,

University Park, PA, USA.

Specialization: Psychology of Physical Activity

Minor Program: Human Development and Family Studies

Advisor: David E. Conroy, Ph.D.

2013-

2017

Master of Education, National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan.

Major: Educational Psychology and Counseling

Advisor: Sheue-Mei Lu, Ph.D.

Bachelor of Education, National Taipei University of Education, Taipei, Taiwan.

Major: Psychology and Counseling

2010

2005

AWARDS AND FUNDINGS

Student Mentoring and Travel Grant, The 5th International Conference on

Ambulatory Monitoring of Physical Activity and Movement.

The National Science Foundation’s Smart and Connected Health Program.

2017

Highly-Cited Paper, Essential Science Indicators/Web of Science, based on

Conroy, Yang & Maher (2014) receiving enough citations by June 2017 to

place it in the top 1% of the academic field of Social Sciences.

2017

RESEARCH

1. Yang, C. H., Maher, J. P., & Conroy, D. E. (2015). Acceptability of mobile health

interventions to reduce inactivity-related health risk in central Pennsylvania adults.

Preventive Medicine Reports, 2, 669-672.

2. Yang, C. H., Maher, J. P., & Conroy, D. E. (2015). Implementation of behavior change

techniques in mobile apps for physical activity. American Journal of Preventive

Medicine. 48(4), 452-55.

3. Conroy, D.E., Yang, C. H., & Maher J.P. (2014). Behavior change techniques in top-ranked

mobile apps for physical activity. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 46(6), 649-

52.

4. Ashour, M., Bekiroglu, K., Yang, C. H., Lagoa, C., Conroy, D., Smyth, J., & Lanza, S. T..

(2016, September). On the mathematical modeling of human physical activity.

Proceedings of the IEEE Multi-conference on Systems and Control (pp. 1084-1091),

Buenos Aires, Argentina. http://ieeexplore.ieee.org/abstract/document/7587951/