13
‘I Get by with a Little Help from my Friends’: Adults with Intellectual Disability Discuss Loneliness 1 Keith R. McVilly*, Roger J. Stancliffe*, Trevor R. Parmenter* and Rosanne M. Burton-Smith  *Centre for Developmental Disability Studies, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia,  School of Psychology, University of Tasmania, Hobart, Australia Accepted for publication 10 May 2005 Background This study explored ‘loneliness’ as experi- enced by adults with intellectual disability, with ‘inter- mittent’ to ‘limited’ support needs. Method A measure of loneliness was piloted, and qualit- ative techniques used to develop a greater understand- ing of the participants’ experience. Results The Loneliness Scale proved valid and reliable and the participants reported loneliness in ways com- parable with the general population. Conclusions The findings demonstrate the effectiveness of combining quantitative and qualitative techniques to enhance understanding of people’s perspective when developing support systems to promote their quality of life. Based on participant perspectives, rec- ommendations are made concerning the issues and types of support families and professionals could consi- der when seeking to assist people with intellectual dis- ability address loneliness. Further investigation of the effects of differing educational and vocational oppor- tunities on people’s post-school social networks appears warranted. Keywords: friendship, intellectual disability, loneliness, qualitative analysis, quality of life, relationships Introduction Stable and rewarding interpersonal relationships are arguably the single most important factor influencing a person’s quality of life (Kennedy & Itkonen 1996). In support of this, the literature documents how relation- ships provide practical aid, emotional support, informa- tion, assistance with decision making, and opportunities to broaden existing support networks (Hughes 1999), as well as important safeguards against stress and psycho- logical illness (Duck 1991). The literature further docu- ments specific mental and physical health benefits associated with the development of interpersonal net- works, and the adverse consequences associated with having poor social networks and the experience of lone- liness (Lynch 1977; Bloom et al. 1978; Flynn 1989; Stan- feld 1999). Service providers and policy makers need to be con- scious that, for many people with disability, significant interpersonal relationships are rare or non-existent (Fleming & Stenfert-Kroese 1990; Blum et al. 1991; Rapley & Beyer 1996; Amado 2004; Duvdevany & Arar 2004). Yet, opportunities for people with disability to develop and maintain personal relationships are impor- tant to the people themselves (Halpern et al. 1986; McVilly 1995; Froese et al. 1999; Johnson 2000; Knox & Hickson 2001; Read 2002), as well as to their families and advocates (Strully & Strully 1985). Furthermore, loneliness and the lack of or disruption to people’s net- works could be significant factors contributing to psy- chopathology (e.g. depression and anxiety), manifest as challenging behaviour (Lunsky & Havercamp 1999; McVilly 2002). To put the experiences of people with intellectual dis- ability into perspective, and to highlight the potential for loneliness to be a significant factor influencing their quality of life, Katz & Yekutiel (1974) reported that 61% of graduates from an adult training programme had no 1 This paper is based on the Doctoral Research of Keith R. McV- illy, and partly funded by an Australian Postgraduate Award, in the Faculty of Medicine, University of Sydney. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 2006, 19, 191–203 Ó 2006 BILD Publications 10.1111/j.1468-3148.2005.00261.x

Adults With Intelectual Disability Discuss Loneliness

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • I Get by with a Little Help from my Friends:Adults with Intellectual Disability DiscussLoneliness1

    Keith R. McVilly*, Roger J. Stancliffe*, Trevor R. Parmenter* and Rosanne M. Burton-Smith

    *Centre for Developmental Disability Studies, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia, School of Psychology, University of

    Tasmania, Hobart, Australia

    Accepted for publication 10 May 2005

    Background This study explored loneliness as experi-

    enced by adults with intellectual disability, with inter-

    mittent to limited support needs.

    Method A measure of loneliness was piloted, and qualit-

    ative techniques used to develop a greater understand-

    ing of the participants experience.

    Results The Loneliness Scale proved valid and reliable

    and the participants reported loneliness in ways com-

    parable with the general population.

    Conclusions The findings demonstrate the effectiveness

    of combining quantitative and qualitative techniques

    to enhance understanding of peoples perspective

    when developing support systems to promote their

    quality of life. Based on participant perspectives, rec-

    ommendations are made concerning the issues and

    types of support families and professionals could consi-

    der when seeking to assist people with intellectual dis-

    ability address loneliness. Further investigation of the

    effects of differing educational and vocational oppor-

    tunities on peoples post-school social networks

    appears warranted.

    Keywords: friendship, intellectual disability, loneliness,

    qualitative analysis, quality of life, relationships

    Introduction

    Stable and rewarding interpersonal relationships are

    arguably the single most important factor influencing a

    persons quality of life (Kennedy & Itkonen 1996). In

    support of this, the literature documents how relation-

    ships provide practical aid, emotional support, informa-

    tion, assistance with decision making, and opportunities

    to broaden existing support networks (Hughes 1999), as

    well as important safeguards against stress and psycho-

    logical illness (Duck 1991). The literature further docu-

    ments specific mental and physical health benefits

    associated with the development of interpersonal net-

    works, and the adverse consequences associated with

    having poor social networks and the experience of lone-

    liness (Lynch 1977; Bloom et al. 1978; Flynn 1989; Stan-

    feld 1999).

    Service providers and policy makers need to be con-

    scious that, for many people with disability, significant

    interpersonal relationships are rare or non-existent

    (Fleming & Stenfert-Kroese 1990; Blum et al. 1991;

    Rapley & Beyer 1996; Amado 2004; Duvdevany & Arar

    2004). Yet, opportunities for people with disability to

    develop and maintain personal relationships are impor-

    tant to the people themselves (Halpern et al. 1986;

    McVilly 1995; Froese et al. 1999; Johnson 2000; Knox &

    Hickson 2001; Read 2002), as well as to their families

    and advocates (Strully & Strully 1985). Furthermore,

    loneliness and the lack of or disruption to peoples net-

    works could be significant factors contributing to psy-

    chopathology (e.g. depression and anxiety), manifest as

    challenging behaviour (Lunsky & Havercamp 1999;

    McVilly 2002).

    To put the experiences of people with intellectual dis-

    ability into perspective, and to highlight the potential

    for loneliness to be a significant factor influencing their

    quality of life, Katz & Yekutiel (1974) reported that 61%

    of graduates from an adult training programme had no

    1This paper is based on the Doctoral Research of Keith R. McV-

    illy, and partly funded by an Australian Postgraduate Award,

    in the Faculty of Medicine, University of Sydney.

    Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 2006, 19, 191203

    2006 BILD Publications 10.1111/j.1468-3148.2005.00261.x

  • friends. Hill et al. (1984) reported a special relationship

    to be evident for only 36% of people in an institutional

    facility and for only 58% of those in community resi-

    dences. Barber & Hupp (1993) reported people living in

    smaller groups to have larger social networks and con-

    cluded that the size of the social networks of those liv-

    ing in smaller groups was comparable with the size of

    social networks in the general population. However,

    Krauss et al. (1992) reported that among adults with

    intellectual disability living at home, 42% of participants

    had no friends outside of their immediate family (here,

    although the absence of non-familial relationships need

    not automatically imply loneliness). Similarly, Petrovski

    & Gleeson (1997) reported that while 73% of participants

    in competitive employment indicated that they had

    friends at work, 97% of these indicated that they did not

    see any of their work-friends outside of the work place.

    Furthermore, of the participants who indicated that they

    had friends outside of work, the majority of people

    reported seeing their non-work friends only infre-

    quently. These findings are in contrast to the experience

    of people in the general population where, in excess of

    70% of time spent socializing is reported to be with

    friends, rather than with family or extended family

    members [Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS) 1997].

    Furthermore, Emerson & McVilly (2004) reported find-

    ings from a large population-based study which indi-

    cated overall low levels of friendship activities for

    adults with intellectual disability (e.g. having a friend

    visit their house or visiting a friends house) and that

    most activities with friends occurred in public places,

    with little or no opportunity for privacy. These findings

    also suggested that the settings in which participants

    lived were more significant determinants of the form

    and content of friendship activities, than the partici-

    pants personal characteristics (i.e. skills and behav-

    iours).

    It is therefore evident that, although many people

    with developmental disability are at least more physic-

    ally integrated and have the opportunity for activities in

    their neighbourhoods and towns, most are still not

    really part of their communities. The majority of these

    people have very few, if any friends (Amado 1993, pp.

    279280). Do these individuals experience social isola-

    tion and, if so, does this give rise to an experience of

    loneliness as it is understood in the general population?

    If loneliness is a consequence, how do people with intel-

    lectual disability conceptualize this and what would

    they like to do about it?

    Peplau & Perlman (1982) have defined loneliness in

    terms of an unpleasant experience arising from insuffi-

    cient social interaction. They further assert that it is

    essentially a personal (subjective) experience, indepen-

    dent of either the quantity or duration of social interac-

    tion. For this reason, loneliness can be understood to

    reflect a discrepancy between an individuals expecta-

    tions concerning relationships and his or her interpret-

    ation of their own social experience. Andersson et al.

    (1987), consistent with Weiss (1973) and Russell et al.

    (1984), have defined loneliness as two separate, but

    inter-related dimensions. The first is the social dimen-

    sion, arising as a result of the persons experience of

    deficiencies in his/her social network and his/her lack

    of social integration (i.e. peer group acceptance and

    involvement in collective activity). The second is the

    emotional dimension, arising from the persons per-

    ceived lack of meaningful and rewarding socio-emo-

    tional bonding with others, on a more intimate level.

    Weiss (1973) questioned if the experience of loneliness

    was a meaningful and measurable construct for those at

    the earlier stages of socio-emotional development. How-

    ever, a number of studies have since established it to be

    a construct that can be measured reliably in children as

    young as 812 years of age (Asher et al. 1984; Asher &

    Wheeler 1985; Marcoen & Brumagne 1985). Furthermore,

    Cassidy & Asher (1992) reported in a study of children

    aged 57 years, 93% of participants were able to des-

    cribe feelings of both aloneness and sadness in

    response to the question Do you know what being

    lonely means?, with the remaining 7% providing a sat-

    isfactory description of aloneness, but not sadness.

    Williams & Asher (1992) reported success in measur-

    ing loneliness in a study involving 62 matched pairs of

    students with and without intellectual disability, 8

    12 years of age. So too, Chadsey-Rusch et al. (1992)

    reported that it is possible to obtain a reliable measure

    of loneliness for adults with mild to moderate intellec-

    tual disability. Similarly, Petrovski & Gleeson (1997),

    using standardized measures, reported that adults with

    mild intellectual disability did not appear to experience

    difficulties relating their experiences of loneliness. They

    found a moderate negative correlation between self-

    reported measures of loneliness and job satisfaction.

    Heiman & Margalit (1998) reported students aged 11

    16 years with mild intellectual disability to self-report

    higher levels of loneliness than their peers without dis-

    ability. Heiman (2001) further reported an association

    between self-reported ratings of loneliness and self-

    reported ratings of depression.

    Although it appears possible to use standardized

    instruments to measure loneliness in people with intel-

    lectual disability, such an approach is limited in that it

    192 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

    2006 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 19, 191203

  • superimposes on the assessment process the resear-

    chers preconceived ideas of how the participants will

    conceptualize and describe their experience. This quanti-

    tative approach is useful for testing established theory.

    However, standardized questionnaires alone are insuffi-

    cient when seeking to explore and explain the lived

    experience of participants (Barnes 1992; Schwandt 1994;

    Rice & Ezzy 1999). Conversely, open-ended or semi--

    structured interviews alone, although having the poten-

    tial to generate rich narrative data, can be more

    confusing than clarifying, especially where respondents

    have limited expressive communication skills (Biklen &

    Moseley 1988). Therefore, the integration of quantitative

    and qualitative techniques, as is commonly the practice

    in a clinical setting, is proposed as an appropriate

    research methodology (ODay & Killeen 2002). Further-

    more, harnessing qualitative techniques is in keeping

    with the growing recognition of the importance of lis-

    tening to people with disability relate their own experi-

    ences, as a means of developing an understanding of

    those experiences in order to better live and effectively

    work with them (Lowe 1992; McVilly 1995; Goodley

    1996; Azmi et al. 1997; Kitchin 2000; Knox et al. 2000;

    Knox & Hickson 2001; Heenan 2002; Pearson et al. 2002;

    Brantlinger et al. 2005).

    This study, part of a wider investigation on the

    friendship aspirations and experiences of people with

    intellectual disability (McVilly 2004), explored the con-

    struct of loneliness as experienced by participants in

    post-secondary school education and employment. It

    involved both piloting a Loneliness Scale and harnessing

    qualitative techniques to identify factors that could

    influence or explain the participants experience of lone-

    liness. It was proposed that assessing and developing an

    understanding of the participants experience would

    provide insight and guidance for family members and

    service providers, assisting them to develop more

    appropriate and effective strategies to promote a quality

    life for people with intellectual disability, including

    meaningful and rewarding relationships.

    Method

    Participants

    Participants consisted of a purposive sample of 22 males

    and 29 females, aged 1652 years (mean (M) 25 years,2 months). All had intellectual disability with limited

    to intermittent support needs; i.e. life-long, requiring

    episodic to regular planned support with the execution

    of identified conceptual, social or practical skills in

    specific life domains (Luckasson et al., 2002). This was

    established with reference to a combination of individual

    records and by assessment using the Scales of Inde-

    pendent BehaviourRevised (Bruininks et al. 1996). All

    were verbal and none had a physical or sensory disabil-

    ity. Thirty-nine lived with their parents, six in supported

    accommodation and six in their own home, although in

    receipt of regular support. Forty-two attended Technical

    And Further Education (TAFE) college programmes

    designed for adults with intellectual disability and nine

    worked in supported employment. All volunteered in

    response to an invitation to participate via their class

    teacher or work supervisor. There were no volunteers

    who required exclusion from the current study (e.g.

    because of their inability to participate in the assess-

    ment).

    Instrumentation

    Participant data were collected in two ways: a Loneli-

    ness Scale was administered to all 51 participants; and a

    sub-set of participants also undertook a semi-structured

    interview, developed as part of the wider study of

    friendship issues. Moreover, proxy data were gathered

    as part of the wider study, using a background ques-

    tionnaire for parents, teachers and work supervisors.

    The Loneliness Scale

    The original version of the Loneliness Scale was devel-

    oped for use with children (Asher & Wheeler 1985). The

    current adaptation used the 15 core loneliness questions.

    However, items were modified to read as first person

    statements and to better reflect the adult status of the

    respondents. For example, Do you have friends at

    school? was changed to read I have friends. The

    response scale was modified, extending the original

    three-point scale (Yes, No or Sometimes) to a five-point

    scale, (Never, Rarely, Sometimes, Usually and Always).

    In addition, the response format was modified from a

    verbal report, to incorporate a card-sort exercise and a

    visual analogue scale. These modifications were made to

    enhance reliability, by focusing respondent attention on

    single items and by making the decisionresponse task

    more tangible (Dattilo et al. 1996).

    The card-sort exercise incorporated a visual analogue

    scale based on five categories, with each of the descrip-

    tions augmented by ticks (Never no tick toAlways XXXX). At each of the five points, the A4landscape sheet was perforated by a metal file fastener,

    providing a spike on which response cards (hole-

    Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 193

    2006 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 19, 191203

  • punched at one end) could be placed. The presentation of

    the loneliness items on individual cards was preceded by

    seven training cards. The training items were designed to

    both teach the respondents how to use the scale and to

    identify any participants whose scale-based responses

    could not be considered reliable. Each category was

    explained to the respondent: e.g. this means what I read

    NEVER sounds like you. Then, for each item, therespondent was asked How often does this sound like

    you?. The card was then read aloud and handed to therespondent. Respondents were cued with a consistent

    moving hand gesture to select one of the response categ-

    ories. Where respondents exhibited difficulty, two forms

    of assistance were offered; first, verbatim repetition of the

    question; and second, where they still exhibited difficulty,

    a standard rephrasing, How much does this sound like

    you; how many ticks would you give it: ? (Sigelmanet al. 1983). All participants demonstrated competence in

    the use of the scale, consistently providing appropriate

    responses to the training items.

    Loneliness items were scored on a scale of 0 (Never)

    to 4 (Always). Consistent with the original scale, five

    items were phrased such that higher ratings were indic-

    ative of loneliness (e.g. I feel all alone), and 10 items

    were phrased such that higher ratings were interpreted

    to be more positive, i.e. indicative of less loneliness (e.g.

    It is easy to make new friends). Prior to conducting the

    analysis, the 10 positive items were reverse-scored. Sub-

    sequently, possible total scores ranged between 0 (never

    lonely) and 60 (always lonely).

    Semi-structured interviews

    The semi-structured interview was developed as part of

    the wider study, based on issues drawn from both the

    literature concerning friendship for adults in the general

    population and the findings of a series of focus groups

    consisting of adults with intellectual disability (McVilly

    2004). There were seven key areas of questioning: perso-

    nal profile and demographics (confirmed by parents,

    teachers and work supervisors); daily activities and

    occupation; personal networks; contact with people con-

    sidered a friend; background to individual friendships;

    description of a best friend; reflections on friendship

    experiences overall, including loss and/or absence of

    friendships.

    Procedures

    Approval was provided by both the University of Sydney

    Human Research Ethics Committee and the University of

    Tasmania Human Research Ethics Committee. Partici-

    pants at TAFE colleges were contacted via the Teacher

    Consultant in disability at each of five colleges, and parti-

    cipated as volunteers during class-time. Participants in

    supported employment were approached via their

    employer and given an opportunity to volunteer during

    work time.

    All 51 participants completed the Modified Loneliness

    Scale. Of these, 16 (31%) participants volunteered to

    repeat the questionnaire, approximately 2 weeks later.

    None reported any significant adverse life events in the

    intervening period. All respondents were then invited to

    participate in an interview about friendship issues. Sub-

    sequently, 32 (63%) volunteered for the in-depth inter-

    view. For each participant, interviews were conducted

    at a location with which they were already familiar (e.g.

    TAFE college or workplace), over two to three weekly

    sessions of approximately 1 h each. Significant others

    (parents, teachers and work supervisors) were also

    asked if, in their opinion, the participant was lonely

    and/or wanted to have more friends. Their responses

    were recorded on an informant questionnaire, conduc-

    ted as part of the wider study of friendship issues.

    Data were analysed using both deductive and induct-

    ive techniques. First, participants Loneliness Scale data

    were analysed using Statistical Package for the Social

    Sciences (SPSS 2003). Second, criterion sampling, based

    on the quantitative analysis, was then used to identify

    two sub-groups of participants (most lonely and least

    lonely). Interview data from the two sub-groups were

    compared and analysed using the Constant Comparat-

    ive Method (Lincoln & Guba 1985). The themes were

    then used to develop both a descriptive and an explana-

    tory narrative about the phenomenon of loneliness

    from the participants perspective. Direct quotations

    were used to elucidate the themes, with minimal para-

    phrasing used only in the interests of clarity. The the-

    matic interpretations were in turn validated with

    reference to a focus group of adults with intellectual dis-

    ability (McVilly 2004).

    Results

    The Modified Loneliness Scale

    Psychometric properties of the scale

    Participants scores on the Modified Loneliness Scale

    ranged between 0 and 47 (M 18.04; SD 10.41). Reli-ability of the scale total score was first explored using

    split-half (odd/even) reliability analysis, a measure of

    194 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

    2006 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 19, 191203

  • internal consistency. The Guttman split-half coefficient

    (an estimate of reliability that does not assume the two

    parts are of equal length or of equal variance) indicated

    a to be 0.78 (P < 0.05). Reliability of the scale wasfurther explored by applying a test/re-test model for a

    sub-set of 16 total scores, from those participants who

    had undergone the repeated administration. A paired-

    samples t-test indicated that there was no significant dif-

    ference between participants total scores on the first

    (M 17.69; SD 8.43) and those on the second admin-istration [M 18.69; SD 10.13; t(15) )0.87, P 0.40;Pearsons r-value was 0.89, P < 0.01]. Finally, where

    proxy reports were available (i.e. from parents, teachers

    and work supervisors), there was a significant difference

    in the Loneliness Scale scores for participants identified

    by proxies as lonely (M 26.71; SD 11.03) and thoseidentified as not lonely (M 16.86; SD 5.95,t(19) )2.69, P 0.01). Additional confirmation of theecological validity of the scale was evident in the analy-

    sis of the semi-structured interviews, discussed later.

    Relationship between ratings of loneliness and participant

    characteristics

    Self-reported ratings of loneliness were investigated in

    relation to the participants characteristics, using mul-

    tiple regression, with the Loneliness Scale score as the

    dependent variable. Independent variables were partici-

    pant gender (male or female), age (in months), living

    arrangements (with family or independently), school

    history (special education or mainstream), primary day

    activity (TAFE or employment), community access

    arrangements (supported or independent), and relation-

    ship status (single or couple). The analysis indicated no

    problems with collinerarity among any of the independ-

    ent variables. Correlations (Pearsons r) between the var-

    iables are given in Table 1.

    Following the application of stepwise regression

    (backward selection), only two factors remained in the

    equation, explaining 19% of the variance [R2 0.19,F(2,48) 5.37, P < 0.01; school history (beta )0.29,P < 0.05) and primary day activity (beta )0.32,P < 0.05)]. Participants who had attended a special

    school reported higher levels of loneliness (M 22.44;SD 10.11) than those who had attended a mainstreamschool (M 16.03; SD 10.06). Participants whose pri-mary day activity was TAFE reported higher levels of

    loneliness (M 20.93; SD 11.08) than those whoseprimary day activity was employment (M 14.23;SD 8.22). There were no statistically significant rela-tionships evident between reported levels of loneliness T

    able1

    Co

    rrel

    atio

    ns

    (Pea

    rso

    ns

    r)b

    etw

    een

    the

    char

    acte

    rist

    ics

    of

    par

    tici

    pan

    ts(n

    51)

    and

    thei

    rra

    tin

    gs

    of

    lon

    elin

    ess

    (an

    das

    soci

    ated

    P-v

    alu

    es)

    Sel

    f-re

    port

    ed

    rati

    ng

    of

    lon

    elin

    ess

    Gen

    der

    (mal

    e/fe

    mal

    e)

    Age

    (mon

    ths)

    Liv

    ing

    arra

    nge

    men

    ts

    (fam

    ily/

    inde

    pen

    den

    t)

    Sch

    ool

    hist

    ory

    (spe

    cial

    ed./

    mai

    nst

    ream

    )

    Pri

    mar

    yda

    y

    acti

    vity

    (TA

    FE

    /em

    ploy

    ed)

    Com

    mu

    nit

    yac

    cess

    (su

    ppor

    ted/

    inde

    pen

    den

    t)

    Rel

    atio

    nsh

    ip

    stat

    us

    (sin

    gle/

    cou

    ple)

    Sel

    f-re

    po

    rted

    rati

    ng

    of

    lon

    elin

    ess

    Gen

    der

    (mal

    e/fe

    mal

    e)0.

    11(0

    .23)

    Ag

    e(m

    on

    ths)

    )0.

    15(0

    .16)

    0.05

    (0.3

    8)

    Liv

    ing

    arra

    ng

    emen

    ts(f

    amil

    y/

    ind

    epen

    den

    tly

    )

    )0.

    22(0

    .07)

    0.05

    (0.3

    7)0.

    65(0

    .00)

    Sch

    oo

    lh

    isto

    ry(s

    pec

    ial

    ed./

    mai

    nst

    ream

    )

    )0.

    29(0

    .02)

    0.13

    (0.1

    9))

    0.12

    (0.2

    0)0.

    06(0

    .34)

    Pri

    mar

    yd

    ayac

    tiv

    ity

    (TA

    FE

    /em

    plo

    yed

    )

    )0.

    33(0

    .01)

    )0.

    42(0

    .00)

    0.23

    (0.0

    6)0.

    30(0

    .02)

    0.02

    (0.4

    6)

    Co

    mm

    un

    ity

    acce

    ss

    (su

    pp

    ort

    ed/

    ind

    epen

    den

    t)

    )0.

    02(0

    .44)

    0.02

    (0.4

    5))

    0.03

    (0.4

    3)0.

    08(0

    .29)

    0.09

    (0.2

    7)0.

    02(0

    .46)

    Rel

    atio

    nsh

    ipst

    atu

    s

    (sin

    gle

    /co

    up

    le)

    )0.

    14(0

    .17)

    )0.

    03(0

    .43)

    0.17

    (0.1

    2)0.

    20(0

    .08)

    )0.

    04(0

    .39)

    0.07

    (0.3

    3)0.

    13(0

    .18)

    Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 195

    2006 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 19, 191203

  • and participant gender, age, living arrangements, ability

    to access the community independent of support, or

    interestingly, relationship status. Finally, the correlations

    in Table 1 indicated that older participants more com-

    monly lived independent of their family, and those liv-

    ing with their family more commonly attended TAFE

    rather than being employed.

    Given the comparatively small initial sample size, the

    number of individual characteristics to be considered

    and the additional effect of missing data on sample size,

    additional analyses were limited to bivariate correlations

    (Pearsons r). Correlations between loneliness scores and

    various social factors are presented in Table 2. As this

    table shows, most correlations did not attain statistical

    significance. However, there was a significant, negative

    correlation between participant ratings of loneliness and

    their reported duration of contact with nominated

    friends; i.e. ratings of loneliness decreased as reported

    duration of contact increased (r )0.44, P < 0.05). Inaddition to the relationship between participant charac-

    teristics and ratings of loneliness, there was a significant

    positive correlation between the number of people

    nominated by participants as friends and the number of

    leisure activities they identified (r 0.35, P < 0.05).Moreover, there was a significant negative correlation

    between the participants reported frequency of social

    activity and their age, with older people reporting lower

    frequency of social activity (r )0.40, P < 0.05).

    The semi-structured interviews

    The semi-structured interviews of participants whose

    Loneliness Scale rating was >1 SD below or above the

    sample mean were then analysed. Consequently two

    groups, each consisting of six participants, were identi-

    fied. A summary of participant profiles for the most

    lonely and least lonely groups are provided in Table 3.

    Not surprisingly, the mean self-reported rating of loneli-

    ness for the most lonely group (M 34.50; SD 8.09)was significantly greater than that for the least lonely

    group (M 2.17; SD 2.40, t(10) )9.38, P < 0.001).Transcripts of the selected interviews were each read

    and explanatory quotes extracted and collated according

    to the major domains of the original interview protocol:

    (a) participant networks and contacts with friends; (b)

    background to and descriptions of best friendships, and

    reflections on friendship experiences.

    Participant networks and contacts with friends

    Each participant identified as most lonely was able to

    nominate a person they would call a friend. They gen-

    erally reported contact with their best friend to be less

    than once a week and mostly less than once a month.

    Five of the six most lonely participants indicated a dis-

    crepancy between how frequently they currently saw

    their nominated friend and how frequently they would

    like to see them. The one person who reported being

    happy with their current frequency of contact, several

    times a week, described his relationship thus: Hes a

    nice person; hes got a good attitude; his name is A;

    because Ive known him the longest; but he doesnt take

    it [our friendship] seriously. One of the most lonely

    participants could not identify a current friend. For the

    purpose of the interview, they chose to talk about a per-

    son who used to be a friend, and who they no longer

    saw: I had a boyfriend; she (the person who used to be

    her friend) kissed him; we stopped being friends; C and

    my boyfriend.

    Among the six most lonely participants, four spoke

    of having or having had significant relationships with

    other people with disability: we both have a disability;

    we both know what it is like to have a disability; she

    finds it hard to get friends; hes just like me, has a dis-

    ability; but different, Im goofy and hes quiet. These

    individuals suggested that after leaving school, making

    friends was difficult, but easier where the other person

    had a disability: at school you get left out (reporting

    4 years experience of an integrated high-school); at

    TAFE you are all on the same level; we have all been

    through the same things (attending TAFE classes

    designed to meet the needs of students with intellectual

    disability). The two participants in the most lonely

    group who identified a person without disability as a

    friend both spoke of their friendships having been

    Table 2 Correlations (Pearsons r) between occupational and

    social factors reported by participants (n 51) and their self-reported ratings of loneliness

    Day-time occupation

    Study days per week 0.12

    Working days per week )0.25Social network

    No. of people identified )0.21Mean frequency of contact )0.15Mean duration of contact )0.44*No. of activities identified 0.21

    Mean frequency of activities 0.00

    Mean duration of activities 0.13

    Positive correlations are associated with participant ratings

    indicating HIGHER levels of loneliness; *P < 0.05.

    196 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

    2006 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 19, 191203

  • forged in an integrated school setting. However, they

    reported their current contact to be less than once a

    month, and in both instances indicated that this was

    because she is studying her HSC. Here it was possible

    that, while the integrated school experience had fostered

    friendships, these relationships had not been sustained

    beyond the school environment in such a way as to pre-

    vent or at least temper the experience of loneliness.

    The least lonely participants reported the frequency

    of contact with their friend to be in keeping with their

    aspirations (e.g. at least several times per week): I see

    them Saturday, Sunday and Tuesday, just when ever I

    can; keep it just as it is. Furthermore, for the least

    lonely group, multiple relationships were consistently

    reported. The one participant who was living with his

    spouse spoke positively of his marriage, but for the

    focus of the interview opted to describe a relationship

    with his friend T: we stuck by one another; we kept

    contact after he moved to Queensland and when he

    came back; he is the only one from school who kept

    contact; we always ring one another up. Four of the six

    participants in the least lonely group spoke of the dis-

    tinct, yet complementary role of boyfriend/girlfriend

    and other best friends: I only see him a few times a

    week; I have time for other friends, to hang out, do

    stuff, go to the movies. Those in the least lonely group

    consistently volunteered information not only about

    their nominated friend, but also about their connections

    with others that were linked to their relationship with

    their best friend: we play cricket with our mates. One

    participant explicitly stated the value of social networks

    to promoting friendship: friends help you make

    friends.

    Background to and descriptions of best friendships, and

    reflections on friendship

    The most lonely participants described friendship as

    involving trust, a person with whom they could talk

    openly and honestly, who was a loyal confidante and

    Table 3 Summary of participant profiles

    for the most lonely and least lonely

    groups

    Group 1 most lonely Group 2 least lonely

    Gender

    One male Five males

    Five Females One female

    Age

    1731 years (M 21 years 10 months) 1751 years (M 27 years 5 months)

    SIB-R scores

    Broad Independence 13 to 54

    (M 45.75)Broad Independence 38 to 58

    (M 48.60)Support Level Score 76 to 82

    (M 79.25)Support Level Score 57 to 82

    (M 71.00)Support Category Intermittent Support Category Intermittent

    Communication skills

    All could speak in sentences

    None used Augmentative & Alternative

    Communication (AAC) techniques

    All could speak in sentences

    None used Augmentative & Alternative

    Communication (AAC) techniques

    Living arrangements

    All six living with parents Four living with parents

    One living with partner

    One living in group home

    Day activities

    One was employed Five were employed

    Five attending TAFE One attending TAFE

    Social circumstances

    All were single Five were single

    Two to seven significant others

    (M 4.33)One with a partner

    Three to seven social activities

    (M 5.33)Two to 15 significant others (M 6.67)

    Three to six social activities (M 4.83)

    Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 197

    2006 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 19, 191203

  • who could provide social and moral support: A friend

    is truthful and open; I hate secrets; I hate bitching; A

    person you can trust to keep secrets; they have to be

    honest, someone who knows how you feel; someone

    who cares about you, no matter who you are; someone

    you can trust, who cares for you; who stands up for

    you. A friend was also described as a person with

    whom to share activities of mutual interest: You can go

    out and do nice things; go out and have fun; go out

    with them on the weekends; going to parties, dance

    parties parties without drugs. However, for this

    group, activity seemed to be of secondary importance to

    the opportunity to exchange thoughts and ideas: It

    doesnt matter where you are, so long as you can talk.

    The one most lonely participant who chose to talk

    about a past friendship recalled how When I was upset

    she would cuddle me and if she was upset, I would

    cuddle her; she would listen to my problems and I

    would listen to hers; we went out to night clubs to 4am;

    I went to her place and she went to mine; I cooked for

    her and she cooked for me only once or twice. This

    same participant recalled I had a man friend, he used

    to live next door; he was trustworthy; he used to get

    drunk he was an idiot drunk; then I hated him; I dont

    want to talk about that. For those who were lonely, a

    common experience was that of recurrent betrayal of

    trust in their relationships.

    Another of the most lonely participants started by

    saying I dont really have a friend; they all turn out to

    be back stabbers. She later went on to describe having a

    boyfriend: Ive known him a long time 3 years; we

    have been dating for 1 year; he works in a factory; I

    dont know what he does; he doesnt backstab. How-

    ever, this exclusive and relatively long-standing relation-

    ship did not seem satisfying. She stated: I want more

    friends; the more you find the more you feel happy.

    However, later her comments suggested that it was the

    opportunity for communication within a relationship

    that was at the heart of her current experience of loneli-

    ness: You can go out with your boyfriend, but you can

    speak with your friends, more than your boyfriend; boy-

    friends get jealous and angry, friends dont.

    Three of the participants identified as most lonely

    spoke of wanting a relationship in the context of having

    a boyfriend or girlfriend. Each of these individuals

    spoke of having experienced a breakdown in such a

    relationship. Two participants spoke of how a third-

    party friend had become involved with their boyfriend/

    girlfriend and how subsequently they had lost both

    friendships. A third participant spoke of how the

    demands of their relationship had exceeded what they

    felt they were able to do and how they believed these

    demands fell outside the scope of how they defined

    their friendship: T, he had a disability; I couldnt take

    his responsibilities, to look after him; I dont want to be

    his mum as well as his girlfriend.

    The most lonely participants spoke of how difficult

    it was to make and keep relationships: You worry you

    will lose them. They suggested that its hard to open

    up if they are shy; because Im shy I dont go up to

    people, I expect them to come to me; its best when

    you are introduced to someone else; introduced to

    someone who might suit you; everything is hard; just

    getting along with people. Three of the six most lonely

    participants identified disability as an issue affecting

    how they got along with people and an impediment to

    the formation of satisfying relationships: its hard to

    break in on the group; Ive been picked on because of

    my disability; they ignore you; they didnt like me

    because I have disability, they saw me black out; the

    best place is on the Internet, where they cant see you;

    cant see what you look like.

    The least lonely participants described their friend-

    ships in more dynamic terms than did the lonely partici-

    pants: she does anything I want; she comes over; we go

    out together; takes me places; calms me down; helps

    me when (I) go to him; lets me sleep at his house; you

    get invites to parties.

    A further characteristic common to participants in the

    least lonely group was the apparent connection they

    had with the family of their nominated best friend: I

    get on with his family; his brothers are like brothers to

    me; his family are good too; I keep my troubles to

    myself (i.e. not relying on the nominated best friend

    for personal or practical assistance), but his family

    would help me; I met her at KFC (open employment);

    we talk to each other; were getting to know each

    others families.

    Four of the participants in the least lonely group

    identified their best friend to be a person with disabil-

    ity. In two instances these were people described in

    terms of being an old school friend, one was a current

    work mate (in supported employment) and one was a

    boyfriend, known through family connections: his

    mum is from my mums village (in Italy); I love him

    since I was seven (aged 17 at the time of interview). For

    the other two people, one nominated their best friend

    to be a youth worker in a local church group who takes

    me places; calms me down if Im in a bad mood; helps

    me solve my problems; I count on him to make me

    happy. However, although he was able to identify other

    youth leaders at the group, he was unable to identify

    198 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

    2006 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 19, 191203

  • any other peers in the group. The sixth participant nom-

    inated his sister who looks after me; looks after my doc-

    tors bill; she calls me Stalky. This participant,

    though, was unable to name any other person, also

    identified The Captain and his wife at the local church

    as being among the most important people in his life.

    Attending a group associated with the local church was

    also an important feature in his social network: you can

    make friends everywhere; at church; at Cross Roads;

    nowhere else really. For this person having a girlfriend

    (who happened to have a disability) was also important,

    but apart from naming her, he declined to comment fur-

    ther.

    Discussion

    This study explored loneliness, as experienced by

    adults with intellectual disability, and intermittent to

    limited support needs (Luckasson et al., 2002). It inclu-

    ded piloting a scale to assess loneliness and utilized

    qualitative techniques to develop an enhanced under-

    standing of the participants experience. It was proposed

    an enhanced understanding of the participants perspec-

    tive was important to inform support strategies in keep-

    ing with their priorities.

    The modified Loneliness Scale piloted in the current

    study was found to be reliable for use by people with

    intellectual disability and intermittent to limited sup-

    port needs, in terms of both its internal consistency and

    test/re-test reliability. It was also demonstrated to be a

    valid measure of loneliness, with moderate correlations

    between participants scale scores and observer ratings

    of loneliness made by significant others (parents, teach-

    ers and work supervisors). Furthermore, when inter-

    views were sorted according to the participants

    Loneliness Scale scores (most lonely/least lonely), dis-

    parate themes were evident which distinguished

    between the two groups, in the expected direction. With

    some further development (e.g. further validation and

    the development of norms), at an individual level, this

    scale could be used to assess and monitor loneliness

    experienced by adults with disability in receipt of ther-

    apy or other services. Furthermore, at a group level, it

    could be used to evaluate the effectiveness of pro-

    grammes implemented to enhance quality of life

    through improving peoples social skills and social net-

    works.

    For the quantitative analysis, generally demographic

    factors (e.g. gender, age, living arrangements, commu-

    nity access support needs and, interestingly, relationship

    status) were insufficient to explain the participants

    Loneliness Scale scores. However, there was some evi-

    dence to suggest that those who had previously atten-

    ded mainstream schools were less lonely than those

    who had previously attended special schools. Similarly,

    participants who were employed were less lonely than

    those whose primary day activity was TAFE. Future

    studies could explore these findings, and in particular

    the effect of different educational and vocational options

    on peoples post-school/adult social networks and their

    subsequent relationships (Riches & Green 2003; Hall

    et al. 2005).

    Social factors also did not satisfactorily explain the

    participants ratings of loneliness. For example, although

    there was a significant negative correlation between par-

    ticipant ratings of loneliness and the length of time they

    reported spending with their friends (i.e. lower ratings

    of loneliness were associated with reports of longer time

    spent with friends), the analysis did not support a signi-

    ficant relationship between ratings of loneliness and

    either the mean number of people nominated as part of

    the participants social networks, nor the average fre-

    quency of contact with those network members. These

    results affirm the importance of evaluating peoples

    qualitative experience when seeking to understand

    friendship and loneliness, and not relying solely on a

    quantitative analysis of their social network.

    The qualitative analysis added to the quantitative

    findings, effectively conveying the participants experi-

    ence. The data suggested distinct differences between

    the perspectives and experiences of participants whose

    Loneliness Scale ratings classified them as most lonely

    and those who were classified least lonely. These dif-

    ferences included how the two groups described their

    friendships and the expectations they had of their

    friends, as well as their personal experiences establish-

    ing, negotiating and maintaining friendships, and the

    subsequent impact of these events on their experience of

    loneliness.

    While the quantitative analysis did not indicate any

    significant association between gender and ratings of

    loneliness, this was suggested by the qualitative analy-

    sis. All but one of the six loneliest participants were

    women. This group described their ideal friend in terms

    of a loyal confidant, whom they could trust and with

    whom they could talk openly and honestly about prob-

    lems. Conversely, all but one of the least lonely partici-

    pants were men, who generally described their

    friendships in terms of shared activities and practical

    support. These descriptions were consistent with gender

    stereotypes of friendship identified in the general popu-

    lation (Phillips 1981; Cohen & Haberman 1982).

    Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 199

    2006 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 19, 191203

  • All, but one of the most lonely group could identify

    a person they would currently call a friend, though

    generally contact with their friend was infrequent (e.g.

    less than once a month). However, even where contact

    was more frequent, they were of the view that their

    friend either did not take the relationship seriously or,

    while the friendship was important (e.g. a boyfriend or

    girlfriend), it failed to meet their full range of socio-

    emotional needs. This was in contrast to the least

    lonely group, who reported higher frequency of contact

    with their friend (e.g. several times a week) and gener-

    ally described how their best friend connected them to

    others, especially members of their friends family, who

    were reported to be an important source of practical

    support.

    The qualitative analysis suggested that connection (or

    lack of connection) with a social network, which could

    meet a diversity of emotional and practicals needs, was

    a critical factor linked to the participants experience of

    loneliness. This is consistent with the assertion that rela-

    tionships serve a variety of purposes and therefore, by

    necessity, involve a variety of persons (Bayley 1997).

    Those who were least lonely described the value of

    their network, while the most lonely participants

    expressed a longing for these connections, the absence

    of which seemed to contribute to their experience of

    loneliness. Similarly, the lonely participants related stor-

    ies describing a betrayal of trust and/or a breakdown of

    honesty in their friendship, which appeared to contrib-

    ute to their experience of loneliness. These were indicat-

    ive of participants sensitivity to both the social and

    emotional dimensions of loneliness, as described for

    the general population by Weiss (1973); Russell et al.

    (1984) and Andersson et al. (1987).

    Consistent with Peplau & Perlman (1982), the qualita-

    tive analysis further suggested that those who were

    identified as most lonely appeared concerned about an

    insufficient level of social interaction or a discrepancy

    between the quantity of their social interaction and their

    personal expectations. This concern of the participants

    was not evident in the quantitative analysis.

    The participants in the current study valued both the

    social and emotional dimensions of friendship; people

    with whom to do things and people with whom they

    could be themselves. Where discrepancies between

    their expectations and their experiences emerged, they

    reported loneliness. It was evident that in addressing

    loneliness, it is important to consider fostering connec-

    tions that provide for both of these needs. Furthermore,

    it was evident that where these needs were effectively

    met, participants did not rely on a single significant

    other (i.e. a best friend or best buddy). Rather, loneli-

    ness was least evident among those who perceived

    themselves to be part of a network of people, each

    member of which performed a specific function or ful-

    filled a specific need; the combined effect of which was

    to provide a safeguard against loneliness. This finding is

    consistent with the assertions of Pescosolido (2001), who

    emphasized the centrality of social networks in the lives

    of people with disability.

    Based on the comments of the current participants,

    personal networks are most effective in meeting peo-

    ples social and emotional needs if they include oppor-

    tunities for relationships that involve people with and

    without intellectual disability. For the current partici-

    pants, relationships with family members and profes-

    sionals could address some important needs (e.g.

    practical support and assistance to solve some prob-

    lems). However, relationships with those who had

    shared life experiences associated with or linked to their

    intellectual disability were also very important. Here,

    many of the lonely participants identified difficulties

    maintaining such relationships. For this reason, family

    members and professionals could provide vital assist-

    ance in fostering the continuation of connections

    between people with intellectual disability at key transi-

    tions in their life: the completion of high school or

    TAFE; when leaving a place of employment; or moving

    house. Finding their friends telephone numbers, having

    support to exchange greetings cards at birthdays and

    Christmas, to initiate contact and to extend invitations

    to visit or to go out together, and help to identify the

    bus or train routes to their friends houses were all high-

    lighted by participants as areas where they wanted prac-

    tical assistance.

    The sample for the current study was purposive, in

    so far as adults with intellectual disability were

    approached to participate at their place of work or post-

    school education; there was no attempt at establishing a

    stratified random sample. However all participants met

    inclusion criteria, which included a screening protocol

    to assess competency to use the Loneliness Scale. Given

    the size and structure of the sample caution is warran-

    ted in generalizing the quantitative findings to the wider

    population of people with disability. This should not be

    a major concern with respect to the qualitative analysis

    (Guba & Lincoln 1994). However, given that for the qua-

    litative analysis the researcher was the instrument of

    analysis (Lincoln & Guba 1985), consideration should be

    given to the possibility that knowledge that the partici-

    pants in the two sub-groups were already classified as

    most lonely or least lonely could have influenced the

    200 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

    2006 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 19, 191203

  • interpretation of the interviews. Future studies could

    address this possibility through the use of third-party

    coders, blind to any additional information concerning

    participant profiles.

    Personal relationships are one of the key areas requi-

    ring attention if people with disability are to experience

    a quality life as valued members of the community

    (McVilly & Rawlinson 1998; Wolfensberger 2000; Rein-

    ders 2002). Furthermore, the mission statements of sup-

    port organizations commonly suggest that enhancing

    the quality of life of people with disability is a priority.

    However, in promoting and supporting personal rela-

    tionships, and in particular friendships, considerable

    work is yet to be done (McLeod et al. 2002; Read 2002).

    This work could be more effective if family members

    and support professionals had a greater understanding

    of the experiences and aspirations of people with intel-

    lectual disability. To this end, further research utilizing

    a clinical methodology incorporating both quantitative

    and qualitative techniques, as used in the current study,

    could prove to be of considerable utility.

    Correspondence

    Any correspondence should be directed to Keith R.

    McVilly, CDDS, PO Box 6 Ryde, New South Wales, Aus-

    tralia (e-mail: [email protected]).

    References

    Amado A. (ed.) (1993) Friendships and Community Connections

    between People with and without Developmental Disability. Paul

    H. Brookes, Baltimore, MD.

    Amado A. (2004) Lessons learned about promoting friendship.

    Connections 30, 812.

    Andersson L., Mullins L. & Johnson D. (1987) Parental intrusion

    versus social isolation: a dichotomous view of the sources of

    loneliness. Journal of Social Behaviour and Personality 2, 125134.

    Asher S. & Wheeler V. (1985) Childrens loneliness: a compar-

    ison of neglected and rejected peer status. Journal of Consult-

    ing and Clinical Psychology 53, 500505.

    Asher S., Hymel S. & Renshaw P. (1984) Loneliness in children.

    Child Development 55, 14561464.

    Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS) (1997) How Australians Use

    their Time. ABS, Canberra.

    Azmi S., Hatton C., Emerson E. & Caine A. (1997) Listening to

    adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities from

    South Asian communities. Journal of Applied Research in Intel-

    lectual Disabilities 10, 250263.

    Bayley M. (1997) What Price Friendship: Encouraging the Relation-

    ships of People with Learning Difficulties. Hexagon Publishing,

    Wootton Courtenay.

    Barber D. & Hupp S. (1993) A comparison of friendship pattens

    of individuals with developmental disability. Education and

    Training in Mental Retardation 28, 1322.

    Barnes C. (1992) Qualitative research: valuable or irrelevant?

    Disability, Handicap and Society 7, 115124.

    Biklen S. & Moseley C. (1988) Are you retarded? No, Im Cath-

    olic: qualitative methods in the study of people with severe

    handicaps. Journal of the Association of Severely Handicapped 13,

    155162.

    Bloom B., Asher S. & White S. (1978) Marital disruption as a

    stressor. Psychological Bulletin 85, 867894.

    Blum R., Resnick M., Nelson R. & St Germaine A. (1991) Family

    and peer issues among adolescents with spina bifida and cer-

    ebral palsy. Pediatrics 88, 280285.

    Brantlinger E., Klingner J. & Richardson V. (2005) Importance

    of experimental as well as empirical qualitative studies in

    special education. Mental Retardation 43, 92119.

    Bruininks R., Woodcock R., Weatherman R. & Hill B. (1996)

    Scales of Independent BehaviourRevised. Riverside Publishing,

    Chicago, IL.

    Cassidy J. & Asher S. (1992) Loneliness and peer relations in

    young children. Child Development 63, 350365.

    Chadsey-Rusch J., DeStefano L., OReilly M., Gonzalez P. &

    Collet-Klingengerg L. (1992) Assessing the loneliness of

    workers with mental retardation. Mental Retardation 30, 85

    92.

    Cohen S. & Haberman H. (1982) Positive Events and Social Sup-

    ports as Buffers on Life Change Stress: Maximising the Prediction

    of Health Outcome. University of Oregon Press, Corvallis, OR.

    Dattilo J., Hoge G. & Malley S. (1996) Interviewing people with

    mental retardation: validity and reliability strategies. Thera-

    peutic Recreation Journal 30, 163178.

    Duck S. (1991) Understanding Relationships. The Guilford Press,

    New York.

    Duvdevany I. & Arar E. (2004) Leisure activities, friendships

    and quality of life of persons with intellectual disability: fos-

    ter homes vs community residential settings. International

    Journal of Rehabilitation Research 27, 289296.

    Emerson E. & McVilly K. (2004) Friendship activities of adults

    with learning disabilities in supported accommodation. Jour-

    nal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 17, 17.

    Fleming I. & Stenfert-Kroese B. (1990) Evaluation of a commu-

    nity care project for people with learning difficulties. Journal

    of Mental Deficiency Research 34, 451464.

    Flynn M. (1989) The social environment. In: Making Connections

    (eds A. Brechin & J. Walmsley). Hodder & Stoughton,

    London.

    Froese P., Richardson M., Romer L. & Swank M. (1999) Com-

    paring opinions of people with developmental disabilities

    and significant persons in their lives using the Individual

    Supports Identification System (ISIS). Disability and Society 14,

    831843.

    Goodley D. (1996) Tales of hidden lives: a critical examination

    of life history research with people who have learning diffi-

    culties. Disability and Society 3, 333348.

    Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 201

    2006 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 19, 191203

  • Guba E. & Lincoln Y. (1994) Competing paradigms in qualita-

    tive methods. In: Handbook of Qualitative Research (eds N. Den-

    zin & Y. Lincoln), pp. 105117. Sage Publications, Thousand

    Oaks, CA.

    Hall I., Strydom A., Richards M., Hardy R., Bernal J. & Wads-

    worth M. (2005) Social outcomes in adulthood of children

    with intellectual impairment: evidence from a birth cohort.

    Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 49, 171182.

    Halpern A., Close D. & Nelson D. (1986) On My Own: The

    Impact of Semi-independent Living Programmes for Adults with

    Mental Retardation. Paul H. Brookes, Baltimore, MD.

    Heenan D. (2002) It wont change the world, but it turned my

    life around: participants views on the Personal Advisor

    scheme in the New Deal for Disabled People. Disability and

    Society 17, 383401.

    Heiman T. (2001) Depressive mood in students with mild intel-

    lectual disability: students reports and teachers evaluations.

    Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 45, 526534.

    Heiman T. & Margalit M. (1998) Loneliness, depression and

    social skills among students with mild mental retardation in

    different educational settings. Journal of Special Education 32,

    154163.

    Hill B., Rotegard L. & Bruininks R. (1984) The quality of life of

    mentally retarded people in residential care. Social Work 29,

    275281.

    Hughes A. (1999) Befriending: a note of caution. British Journal

    of Learning Disabilities 27, 8892.

    Johnson J. (2000) The Aspirations of People with a Disability within

    an Inclusive Victorian Community. A Report Prepared by Mill-

    ward Brown & Associates for the Department of Health Ser-

    vices (DisAbility Services Programme). DisAbility Services,

    Melbourne.

    Katz S. & Yekutiel E. (1974) Leisure time problems of mentally

    retarded graduates of training programmes. Mental Retarda-

    tion 2, 5457.

    Kennedy C. & Itkonen T. (1996) Social relationships, influential

    variables and chance across the lifespan. In: Positive Beha-

    vioural Support: Including People with Difficult Behaviour in

    the Community (eds L. Koegel, R. Koegel & G. Dunlap),

    Section iii. Paul H. Brookes, Baltimore, MD.

    Kitchin R. (2000) The researched opinions of research: dis-

    abled people and disability research. Disability and Society

    15, 2547.

    Knox M. & Hickson F. (2001) The meaning of close friendships:

    the views of four people with intellectual disabilities. Journal

    of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 14, 276291.

    Knox M., Monk M. & Parmenter T. (2000) Working with the

    experts: collaborative research with people with an intellec-

    tual disability. Disability and Society 15, 4961.

    Krauss M., Seltzer M. & Goodman S. (1992) Social support net-

    works of adults with mental retardation who live at home.

    American Journal on Mental Retardation 96, 432441.

    Lincoln Y. & Guba E. (1985) Naturalistic Inquiry. Sage Publica-

    tions, Newbury Park, CA.

    Lowe K. (1992) Community based service: what consumers

    think. British Journal of Mental Subnormality 38, 614.

    Luckasson R., Borthwick-Duffy S., Buntinx W., Craig E., Coulter

    D., Schalock R., Snell M., Spitalnick D., Reeve A., Spreat S. &

    Tasse M. (eds) (2002) Mental Retardation: Definition, Classifica-

    tion and Systems of Support, 10th edn. American Association

    on Mental Retardation (AAMR), Washington, DC.

    Lunsky Y. & Havercamp S. (1999) Distinguishing low levels of

    social support and social strain: implications for dual diagno-

    sis. Mental Retardation 104, 200204.

    Lynch (1977) The Broken Heart: Medical Consequences of Loneli-

    ness. Basic Books, New York.

    Marcoen A. & Brumagne M. (1985) Loneliness among children

    and young adolescents. Developmental Psychology 21, 1025

    1031.

    McLeod J., Stewart G. & Robertson M. (2002) Community Inclu-

    sion Enhancing Friendship Networks among People with a Cog-

    nitive Impairment. A Report Commissioned by the

    Department of Health Services, Victoria. DisAbility Services,

    Melbourne.

    McVilly K. (1995) Interviewing people with a learning disability

    about their residential service. British Journal of Learning Dis-

    abilities 23, 138142.

    McVilly K. (2002) Positive Behaviour Support for People with Intel-

    lectual Disability: Evidence-based Practice Promoting Quality of

    Life. Australian Society for the Study of Intellectual Disability,

    Sydney.

    McVilly K. (2004) I Get by with a Little Help from my Friends.

    Doctoral Dissertation, University of Sydney, Sydney.

    McVilly K. & Rawlinson R. (1998) Quality of life: issues in the

    development and evaluation of services for people with intel-

    lectual disability. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Dis-

    ability 23, 199218.

    ODay B. & Killeen M. (2002) Research on the lives of persons with

    disabilities: the emerging importance of qualitative research

    methodologies. Journal of Disability Policy Studies 13, 915.

    Pearson V., Wong Y. & Pierini J. (2002) The structure and con-

    tent of social inclusion: voices of young adults with learning

    difficulties in Guangzhou. Disability and Society 17, 365382.

    Peplau L. & Perlman D. (1982) Perspectives on loneliness. In:

    Loneliness: A Source Book of Current Theory, Research and Ther-

    apy (eds L. Peplau & D. Perlman), pp. 118. Wiley, New

    York.

    Pescosolido B. (2001) The role of social networks in the lives of

    persons with disabilities. In: Handbook of Disability Studies

    (eds G. Albrecht, K. Seelman & M. Bury) pp. 468489. Sage

    Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA.

    Petrovski P. & Gleeson G. (1997) The relationship between job

    satisfaction and psychological health in people with an intel-

    lectual disability in competitive employment. Journal of Intel-

    lectual and Developmental Disability 22, 199211.

    Phillips S. (1981) Network characteristics related to the wellbe-

    ing of normals: a comparative base. Schizophrenia Bulletin 7,

    117123.

    202 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

    2006 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 19, 191203

  • Rapley M. & Beyer S. (1996) Daily activity, community partici-

    pation and quality of life in an ordinary housing network.

    Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 9, 3139.

    Read G. (2002) Issues Arising from 43 Quality Network Reviews.

    The Quality Network. The British Institute of Learning Dis-

    abilities (BILD), Kidderminister.

    Reinders J. (2002) The good life for citizens with intellectual

    disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 46, 15.

    Rice P. & Ezzy D. (1999) Qualitative Research Methods: A Health

    Focus. Oxford University Press, Melbourne.

    Riches V. & Green V. (2003) Social integration in the workplace

    for people with disabilities: and Australian perspective. Jour-

    nal of Vocational Rehabilitation 19, 127142.

    Russell D., Cutrona C., Rose J. & Yurko K. (1984) Social and

    emotional loneliness: an examination of Weiss typology of

    loneliness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46, 1313

    1321.

    Schwandt T. (1994) Constructivist, interpretivist approaches to

    human enquiry. In: Handbook of Qualitative Research, Part 2

    (eds N. Denzin & Y. Lincoln), Part 2. Sage Publications,

    Thousand Oaks, CA.

    Sigelman C., Schoenrock C., Budd E., Winer J., Spanhel C., Mar-

    tin P., Hromas S. & Bensberg G. (1983) Communicating with

    Mentally Retarded Persons: Asking Questions and Getting

    Answers. Research and Training Centre in Mental Retarda-

    tion, Texas Technical University, Lubbock, TX.

    SPSS (2003) Statistical Package for Social Sciences, Version 12

    [Computer software]. SPSS, Chicago, IL.

    Stanfeld S. (1999) Social support and social cohesion. In: Social

    Determinants of Health (eds M. Marmot & R. Wilkinson),

    pp. 155178. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Strully J. & Strully C. (1985) Teach your children. The Canadian

    Journal on Mental Retardation 35, 311.

    Weiss R. (1973) Loneliness: The Experience of Emotional and Social

    Isolation. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Williams G. & Asher S. (1992) Assessment of loneliness at

    school among children with mild mental retardation. Ameri-

    can Journal on Mental Retardation 96, 373385.

    Wolfensberger W. (2000) A brief overview of Social Role Valori-

    zation. Mental Retardation 38, 105123.

    Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 203

    2006 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 19, 191203