Anatomy and Aging of Amygdala and Hippocampus in ASD

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/13/2019 Anatomy and Aging of Amygdala and Hippocampus in ASD

    1/10

    RESEARCH ARTICLE

    Anatomy and Aging of the Amygdala and Hippocampus in AutismSpectrum Disorder: An In Vivo Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studyof Asperger Syndrome

    Clodagh M. Murphy, Q. Deeley, E.M. Daly, C. Ecker, F.M. OBrien, B. Hallahan, E. Loth, F. Toal, S. Reed,S. Hales, D.M. Robertson, M.C. Craig, D. Mullins, G.J. Barker, T. Lavender, P. Johnston, K.C. Murphy,and D.G. Murphy

    It has been proposed that people with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have abnormal morphometry and development ofthe amygdala and hippocampus (AH). However, previous reports are inconsistent, perhaps because they included peopleof different ASD diagnoses, ages, and health. We compared, using magnetic resonance imaging, the in vivo anatomy ofthe AH in 32 healthy individuals with Asperger syndrome (1247 years) and 32 healthy controls who did not differsignificantly in age or IQ. We measured bulk (gray1white matter) volume of the AH using manual tracing (MEASURE).We first compared the volume of AH between individuals with Asperger syndrome and controls and then investigatedage-related differences. We compared differences in anatomy before, and after, correcting for whole brain size. There wasno significant between group differences in whole brain volume. However, individuals with Asperger syndrome had asignificantly larger raw bulk volume of total (Po0.01), right (Po0.01), and left amygdala (Po0.05); and when correctedfor overall brain size, total (Po0.05), and right amygdala (Po0.01). There was a significant group difference in aging of

    left amygdala; controls, but not individuals with Asperger syndrome, had a significant age-related increase in volume(r50.486, Po0.01, and r50.007, P50.97, z51.995). There were no significant group differences in volume or age-related effects in hippocampus. Individuals with Asperger syndrome have significant differences from controls in bulkvolume and aging of the amygdala. Autism Res 2011,4:xxxxxx. & 2011 International Society for Autism Research,Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

    Keywords:Asperger syndrome; autism; amygdala; hippocampus; age

    Introduction

    It has been suggested that the biological basis of autism

    spectrum disorder (ASD) may include anatomical abnorm-

    alities in the amygdala and hippocampus [Baron-Cohen,

    Ring et al., 2000; Schultz, 2005]. To date, however, researchfindings have been variable [Cody, Pelphrey et al., 2002].

    Classic lesion studies [e.g. see Kluver, 1939; Rosvold,

    Mirsky et al., 1954] and a review [Bachevalier, 1994] have

    demonstrated a crucial role of the amygdala in primate

    social and emotional behaviors. In humans, the amygda-

    la contributes to reward and motivation [Baxter &

    Murray, 2002; Gottfried, ODoherty et al., 2003; Holland

    & Gallagher, 2004], processing of faces [Haxby, Hoffman,

    et al., 2002], recognition of emotions [Adolphs, 2003],

    emotional memory [McGaugh, 2004], detecting threat

    [Amaral, 2003; Amaral & Bauman, 2003], fear and

    anxiety [Davis, Walker et al., 2003; LeDoux, 2003], and

    modulates social brain regions [such as fusiform-

    extrastriate cortices [Deeley, Daly et al., 2007]. The

    amygdala also interacts reciprocally with the hippocam-

    pus to encode emotional memories [Richardson, Strangeet al., 2004]. The hippocampus, in addition to its role in

    emotional processing and memory [Bannerman, Rawlins

    et al., 2004; Vargha-Khadem, Gadian et al., 1997], is also

    involved in spatial learning [Maguire, Gadian et al.,

    2000] and verbal novelty detection [Grunwald, Lehnertz

    et al., 1998]. Given the difficulties of individuals with

    ASD in responding to, and processing, socioemotional

    cues, it has been suggested that its biological basis

    includes abnormalities in the development and function

    of limbic structures, including the amygdala and

    INSAR Autism Research 4: 110, 2011 1

    Received October 6, 2010; accepted for publication July 20, 2011

    Published online in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com)

    DOI: 10.1002/aur.227

    &2011 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

    Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article.Clodagh M. Murphy and Q. Deeley are first authors.

    From the Kings College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Science, London, United Kingdom (C.M.M.,

    Q.D., E.M.D., C.E., B.H., E.L., F.T., S.R., S.H., D.M.R., M.C.C., D.M., T.L., P.J., D.G.M.); Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Dublin, Ireland (F.M.O.,

    K.C.M.); Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Kings College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Centre for Neuroimaging Sciences, London, United

    Kingdom (G.J.B.)

    Address for correspondence and reprints: Clodagh M. Murphy, Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Science, Institute of Psychiatry,

    De Crespigny Park, PO Box 50, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK. E-mail: [email protected]

    Grant sponsor: Medical Research Council.

    35: 312, 2012 3

    21 September 2011 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com)

    2012,5:312. 2011 International Society for Autism Research,

  • 8/13/2019 Anatomy and Aging of Amygdala and Hippocampus in ASD

    2/10

  • 8/13/2019 Anatomy and Aging of Amygdala and Hippocampus in ASD

    3/10

    Controls were recruited locally by advertisement.

    Individuals with Asperger syndrome were recruited withthe support of the Medical Research Council (MRC) UK

    Autism Imaging Multicentre Study (A.I.M.S) (MRC AIMS)

    program. Diagnosis of Asperger syndrome was made by

    two Consultant Psychiatrists (D.G.M. and D.R.) and a

    Nurse Specialist (S.R.), using ICD-10 research diagnostic

    criteria [1994]. All individuals fulfiled the diagnostic

    criteria of childhood autism, except that they did not

    have a history of language delay; they were therefore

    subtyped as having Asperger syndrome (International

    Classification of Diseases, 10th Edition ICD-10, F84.5).

    Where permission was given by individuals with Asperger

    syndrome, additional diagnostic measures were sought.

    The Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) [Lord

    et al., 1994] was completed for all 32 individuals with

    Asperger syndrome (13 young people and 19 adults).

    Thirty of the 32 reached the ADI algorithm cut-offs for

    autism in all three domains (social, communication, and

    restricted and stereotyped behaviors). One adult and one

    young person just failed to reach the ADI Communication

    algorithm cut-off for autism by one point. Six adults with

    Asperger syndrome agreed to complete the Autism

    Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) [Lord, Rutter

    et al., 1989], and were above cut-off for autism on all

    ADOS domains.

    Ethical approval was obtained from the Institute ofPsychiatry and Bethlem and Maudsley Hospitals Trust

    Research Ethics Committee. All participants gave written

    informed consent, including each childs parent (where

    relevant).

    All participants underwent a structured physical and

    psychiatric examination to exclude comorbid medical and

    psychiatric disorders, and biochemical, hematologic or

    chromosomal abnormalities (including Fragile X syn-

    drome) possibly affecting brain function. Participants were

    excluded if they had a comorbid psychiatric or medical

    disorder affecting brain development (e.g. epilepsy or

    psychosis), history of head injury, genetic disorder asso-

    ciated with autistic spectrum disorder (e.g. tuberous

    sclerosis or Fragile X syndrome), or an IQo70. Intelligence

    quotients were measured using the Wechsler Adult Intelli-

    gence ScaleRevised (WAIS-R) short form [Weschler, 1999].

    MRI Data Acquisition

    MRI data were obtained using a GE Sigma 1.5T

    Neuro-optimised MR system (General Electric, Milwaukee,

    Wisconsin). Whole head coronal three-dimensional (3D)

    Inversion recovery prepared spoiled gradient echo (IR-SPGR

    images) (repetition time513.8 msec, echo time52.8msec,

    inversion time5450 msec, 256192 acquisition matrix,

    reconstructed as a 256256 matrix, over a 220220 field of

    view, 1241.5-mm slices) were obtained from all subjects.

    ROI Approach

    Manual tracing of the bulk volume (i.e. both gray andwhite matter) of total, left, and right amygdala and

    hippocampus was carried out as previously described

    [Cutter, Daly et al., 2006] by a single rater. In brief,

    manual tracing was performed on IR-SPGR data sets,

    using both MEASURE Image Analysis software [Barta

    et al., 1997] (Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore,

    Maryland) and published anatomical definitions [van

    Amelsvoort, Daly et al., 2001]. The anatomical defini-

    tions of the regions were defined using a modified

    version of Watsons guidelines [Watson, Andermann

    et al., 1992]. Images were realigned parallel to the sylvian

    fissure. The most posterior hippocampus measurement

    started on the slice displaying the aqueduct of sylvius.

    Continuing anteriorly, the superior border of the hippo-

    campus merges with the inferior border of the amygdala

    and the regions are delineated by white matter and the

    temporal horn of the lateral ventricle. If the delineation

    is unclear, the inferior border of the posterior amygdala is

    arbitrarily marked as a horizontal line drawn medially

    from the head of the temporal stem to the medial border

    of the amygdala; the hippocampus taken to be gray

    matter inferior to the line. The anterior boundary of the

    amygdala measurement was made on the slice with

    closure of the lateral sulcus (i.e. temporal lobe is joined

    to frontal lobe by the limen insulae). Total (i.e. left plusright) and individual left and right amygdala and

    hippocampus were measured. As manually traced regio-

    nal brain volumes are affected by brain size, we also

    measured total whole brain volume (WBV). The volume

    of each region was calculated by multiplying the summed

    pixel cross-sectional areas measured by the slice thick-

    ness. The rater was blind to subject status and intrarater

    reliabilities were obtained for all regions identified. On a

    separate test data set of ten scans, the rater had achieved

    Table I. Participant Demographics

    Controls,N5 32

    (31 male, 1

    female)

    Asperger syndrome,

    N5 32 (30 male, 2

    female)

    Age in years, mean7SD (range) 23711 (1049) 23711 (1247)

    FSIQ, mean7SD 111715 108713

    Autistic Diagnostic Interview (ADI), N532 (30 male, 2 female)

    Social (mean7SD) 1976

    Communication (mean7SD) 1375

    Restricted, repetitive behavior

    (mean7SD)

    673

    Autistic Diagnostic Observation

    Schedule (ADOS), N56 (5

    male, 1 female)

    Communication 571

    Social 872

    Total (social and communication) 1373

    Stereotyped behaviors and

    restricted interests

    272

    INSAR Murphy et al./Anatomy and aging of the amygdala 35

  • 8/13/2019 Anatomy and Aging of Amygdala and Hippocampus in ASD

    4/10

    intrarater and interrater reliabilities of r40.9 for all

    regions traced [Bartko & Carpenter, 1976].

    In order to control for the relationship of brain volume

    and head size, amygdala and hippocampus volumes were

    expressed as raw (uncorrected) volumes, and when

    normalized, as a percentage of traced WBV. Statistical

    analyses were carried out on both raw and normalizedbrain volumes.

    Statistical Analysis

    SPSS 15.0 for Windows (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL) was used for

    all statistical analyses. All demographic data (age and IQ)

    and brain volume measurements were normally distributed.

    We corrected for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni). Those

    regions that did survive a corrected Bonferroni (P50.0038)

    are marked as a in Table II (Summary of results). Those

    regions that did not survive are marked as b. Level

    of statistical significance was defined as Po0.05. Results

    in the discussion are based on findings with a level ofstatistical significance ofPo0.05.

    Because regional brain volumes may be affected by

    overall brain size, and some have suggested that people

    with ASD have differences in WBV (i.e. macrocephaly);

    statistical analyses were performed on both the raw bulk

    volume of the AHC, and after being expressed as a

    percentage of WBV (% WBV).

    To test our main hypothesis, we first compared people

    with Asperger syndrome with the controls. Between-group

    differences in age, IQ, and brain volumes were calculated

    using independent-samples t-tests.

    We then investigated our subsidiary hypothesis, that

    there would be no group difference in the effect of age,

    using a correlational approach. Prior to comparing differ-

    ences in age-related correlations, the data were tested for

    (i) linearity, (ii) independence, (iii) homoscedasticity, and(iv) normality error distribution. None of these assump-

    tions was violated. Hence, we initially calculated Pearson

    productmoment correlations to investigate within-group

    age-related differences in AHC volumes. Then, in order to

    examine between-group differences in brain aging, we

    converted the relevant Pearsons r coefficient into Fishers

    Z-score to test the statistical significance of between group

    differences in correlations [Pallant, 2005].

    In addition, we further investigated the relationship

    between age and total amygdala hippocampus volume by

    calculating a partial correlation to control for the effect of

    WBV. This did not change our results and even enhancedthe effect size of our correlation coefficients.

    ResultsEffect of Group: Asperger Syndrome (N532) and Controls(N532)

    There was no significant between group difference in:

    age, Full-Scale IQ, WBV, or bulk volume of hippocampus.

    Table II. Summary of Results: Mean AH Volumes and Significant Differences

    Controls (N5 32), mean7SD Asperger syndrome (N5 32), mean7SD

    WBV

    Total WBV (ml) 1,095.837104.80 1,117.507134.12 t(62)50.720, P5 0.474

    Amygdala

    Total

    Raw (ml) 5.1670.50a 5.5870.66a t(62)52.863, P5 0.006a,b

    Corrected (%WBV) 0.4770.04a 0.5070.07a t(62)52.306, P5 0.024a,b

    Right

    Raw (ml) 2.5270.26a 2.7870.36a t(62)53.300, P5 0.002a,c

    Corrected (%WBV) 0.2370.02a 0.2570.04a t(62)52.719, P5 0.008a,b

    Left

    Raw (ml) 2.6470.30a 2.8070.34a t(62)52.053, P5 0.044a,b

    Corrected (%WBV) 0.2470.02 0.2570.03 t(62)51.516, P5 0.135

    Hippocampus

    Total

    Raw (ml) 5.6770.78 5.8070.86 t(62)50.623, P5 0.535

    Corrected (%WBV) 0.5270.06 0.5270.07 t(62)50.253, P5 0.801

    Right

    Raw (ml) 2.9470.44 3.0270.47 t(62)50.691, P5 0.492

    Corrected (%WBV) 0.2770.03 0.2770.04 t(62)50.223, P5 0.824

    Left

    Raw (ml) 2.7370.38 2.7870.43 t(62)50.456, P5 0.650

    Corrected (%WBV) 0.2570.03 0.2570.04 t(62)50.069, P5 0.946

    WBV, whole brain volume; AH, amygdala and hippocampal.aSignificant between group difference on independent samplest-test.bDid not survive Bonferroni.cSurvived Bonferroni.

    4 Murphy et al./Anatomy and aging of the amygdala INSAR6

  • 8/13/2019 Anatomy and Aging of Amygdala and Hippocampus in ASD

    5/10

    However, individuals with Asperger syndrome had a

    significantly larger uncorrected bulk volume of total

    (left plus right) (t (62)52.863, Po0.01), right

    (t (62)53.300, Po0.01), and left amygdala

    (t(62)52.053, Po0.05) and in the corrected (% WBV)

    total (t (49.335) 52.306, Po0.05) and right amygdala

    (t(50.066)52.719, Po0.01).

    Age and Volume of Amygdala and Hippocampus

    Increased corrected (% WBV) volume of total (r50.483,

    Po0.01) and left amygdala (r50.486, Po0.01) in con-

    trols, but not individuals with Asperger syndrome, was

    significantly positively correlated with increasing age.

    Further, controls had a significantly larger age-related

    increase in corrected (% WBV) volume of left amygdala

    than people with Asperger syndrome (z51.995) (z5 sig-

    nificant ifr1.96 or Z1.96).

    No significant correlations between age and volume of

    hippocampus were identified in either group.

    Increasing age in controls, but not individuals with

    Asperger syndrome, was significantly negatively corre-lated with WBV (r50.549, Po0.01). There was a trend

    toward controls having a significantly larger age-related

    decrease in WBV than people with Asperger syndrome

    (z51.953; Fig. 1) (See Supplementary data Appendix

    Table A).

    Discussion

    We compared the bulk volume and age-related differ-

    ences of the amygdala and hippocampus in physically

    healthy individuals with Asperger syndrome and

    controls, who did not differ significantly in age and IQ.

    Volume of the Amygdala and Hippocampus

    Our finding that people with Asperger syndrome have a

    significantly larger volume of total, left, and right

    amygdala and no difference in volume of hippocampus,

    is in agreement with earlier findings of amygdala

    enlargement in individuals with ASD as measured using

    VBM [Abell, Krams et al., 1999], ANALYZE [Howard,

    Cowell et al., 2000], and manual tracing [Mosconi,

    Cody-Hazlett et al., 2009]. It is also in agreement with

    previous reports of no significant difference in hippo-

    campal volume as measured using ANALYZE [Howard

    et al., 2000] and manual tracing [Piven, Bailey et al.,

    1998]. Furthermore, our finding of an enlarged amygdala

    was true for both uncorrected (total, left, and right

    amygdala) and corrected (total and right amygdala)

    values.

    Our results are, nevertheless, in contrast to others who

    found no significant difference in bulk volume of

    amygdala [Haznedar et al., 2000; Palmen et al., 2006],

    or reduced volume of amygdala [Nacewicz, Dalton et al.,

    2006] as measured using manual tracing. Palmens study

    cohort was only very slightly smaller than ours but it

    included much younger (725 years old) groups of mixed

    diagnoses (both autism and Asperger syndrome). Simi-

    larly, Nacewiczs study included a younger (825 years

    old) group of 11 males with autism and 5 with Aspergersyndrome or pervasive developmental disorder. Likewise,

    Haznedars study included ten individuals with autism

    and seven individuals with Asperger syndrome, who

    ranged in IQ from 55 to 125. In contrast, we included a

    slightly larger sample of people with Asperger syndrome

    from a wider age group, who had an IQ above 70, and

    they did not significantly differ from controls in IQ or

    age. Distinct differences between brain anatomy of

    individuals with Asperger and autism have been reported

    [McAlonan, Cheung et al., 2009; McAlonan, Suckling

    et al., 2008]. Such differences in autistic subtypes, age,

    and IQ may partially explain the differences in our

    findings.

    Development of the Amygdala and Hippocampus

    There are relatively few in vivo neuroimaging studies of

    normal amydala and hippocampal development, and

    most previous studies have been cross-sectional [Giedd

    et al., 1996]. Nevertheless, those that are available suggest

    that amygdala and hippocampus volume in typical

    human development changes with age, and that the

    Figure 1. The relationship between age and corrected bulk

    volume of the left amygdala. NB: Controls had a significantly

    larger age-related difference in corrected bulk volume of the left

    amygdala (significant between group difference: z51.995).

    r5 correlation co-efficient; correlation is significant at the

    0.01 level; strength of relationship: r50.10.295 small,

    r50.300.495medium, and r50.501.05 large; z5 signifi-

    cant if o1.96 or41.96; AS, Asperger syndrome.

    INSAR Murphy et al./Anatomy and aging of the amygdala 57

  • 8/13/2019 Anatomy and Aging of Amygdala and Hippocampus in ASD

    6/10

    developmental trajectory may differ between amygdala

    and hippocampus. For example, in boys left amygdala

    volume increases between 4 and 18 years of age [Giedd

    et al., 1996], but changes relatively little in early

    adulthood [Pruessner, Collins, et al., 2001]. In contrast,

    male hippocampal volume does not change during

    adolescence [Giedd et al., 1996; Schumann et al., 2004],

    increases in young adulthood (1921 years old, [Suzuki,

    Hagino et al., 2005] but decreases in later life and old age[Liu, Lemieux et al., 2003; Murphy, DeCarli et al., 1996;

    Pruessner et al., 2001; Raz, Lindenberger et al., 2005].

    However, rates of volume loss may be significantly less

    marked than in parietal and frontal areas [Grieve, Clark

    et al., 2005] suggesting that the timing of age-related

    changes may differ across brain regions.

    Schumanns [Schumann et al., 2004, 2009] investiga-

    tion of the amygdala and hippocampus further high-

    lights the importance of age in amygdala hippocampal

    development. Schumanns [Schumann et al., 2004] study

    suggested that differences in bulk volume of the

    amygdala and hippocampus (measured using manualtracing) between people with ASD and controls vary

    by age, diagnostic subtype, and anatomical location.

    Schumanns group first investigated their sample as one

    age group (718 years old) and found amygdala enlarge-

    ment only in young people with low-functioning autism

    and hippocampal enlargement in young people with

    both low- and high-functioning autism. No differences

    were found in young people with Asperger syndrome.

    In controls, age positively correlated with amygdala

    volume, whereas no correlation between age and volume

    was found for individuals with autism or Asperger

    syndrome. As such, Schumann et al. further investigated

    the relationship between age and amydala by dividing

    their sample into children and adolescents; amygdala

    enlargement was specific to young children (712 years

    old) with autism (both low and high functioning),

    although a trend toward enlarged right amygdala was

    found in young children with Asperger syndrome

    (N511, P50.06). No significant difference in amygdala

    volume was found between adolescents (12.7518.5 years

    old) with autism, Asperger syndrome (N513), and

    controls (N511). While this study was a very valuable

    first step, subjects were separated into categorical age

    groups (i.e. children and adolescents), rather than

    using a continuous approach. However, neuroimagingevidence suggests that postnatal phases of brain devel-

    opment do not begin, or end, at specific chronological

    ages, but extend across age ranges [Giedd et al., 1996].

    Age and Volume of the Amygdala and Hippocampus

    We therefore investigated the continuous relationship

    between age and volume of the amygdala and hippocampus

    from childhood into adulthood. We found a significantly

    stronger association of age and left amygdala volume in

    controls relative to people with Asperger syndrome. Other

    studies have previously reported the presence of amygdala

    enlargement at an early age in childhood autism [Mosconi

    et al., 2009; Schumann et al., 2004, 2009; Sparks et al., 2002]

    and in adolescence [Groen et al., 2010]. Our findings extend

    that work and suggest that amygdala enlargement in

    individuals with Asperger syndrome persists into adult-

    hood. Also, they suggest that although the amygdalavolume of people with Asperger syndrome may be

    significantly enlarged at an early age, it does not continue

    to increase in volume with age. In contrast, our finding of

    increasing amygdala volume with age in controls is in

    keeping with previous reports of typical amygdala develop-

    ment [Giedd et al., 1996; Schumann et al., 2004]. In sum,

    our findings support earlier reports of amygdala enlarge-

    ment in young children and adolescents with ASD

    [Mosconi et al., 2009; Schumann et al., 2004, 2009; Sparks

    et al., 2002] and also suggest that, in adolescent and adult

    males with Asperger syndrome, the amygdala continues to

    be significantly larger compared with age-matched controls.Furthermore, the development of the amygdala appeared to

    be different across groups. The amygdala of controls

    continued to increase with age, whereas the amygdala of

    individuals with Asperger syndrome did not. Our results

    suggest that both the volume and aging of the amygdala is

    significantly different in individuals with Asperger syn-

    drome from controls. Future longitudinal studies are

    required to help clarify the development and aging of the

    amygdala and hippocampus across the lifespan; and to

    determine if this differs across the diagnostic subtypes of

    ASD.

    Potential Causes

    The causes of the subtle differences in morphometry and

    development of the amygdala and hippocampus that we

    found are not known, but probably include a complex

    geneenvironment interaction. For example, the amyg-

    dala plays a central role in fear and anxiety [Davis et al.,

    2003; LeDoux, 2003]; people with an ASD have a

    significantly increased risk of experiencing stress and

    anxiety disorders [for review, see White, Oswald et al.,

    2009]; and stress in turn impacts on amygdala develop-

    ment [McEwen, 2007]. For instance, in animals, acute

    and chronic stress increases dendrite growth in amygdala[Vyas, Mitra et al., 2002] and it has been suggested that

    stress-related hypertrophy of amygdala may contribute to

    anxiety [Vyas, Bernal et al., 2003]. Hence, it is possible

    that increased stress in individuals with Asperger syn-

    drome may have contributed to, or result from, the

    increased volume of amygdala we found. However,

    genetic influences may also contribute to differences in

    amygdala and hippocampus volume. Preliminary support

    for this suggestion is provided by studies that reported

    6 Murphy et al./Anatomy and aging of the amygdala INSAR8

  • 8/13/2019 Anatomy and Aging of Amygdala and Hippocampus in ASD

    7/10

    differences in amygdala hippocampal volume within the

    relatives of individuals with ASD [Dalton et al., 2007;

    Rojas et al., 2004].

    The specific genes/gene systems that may underpin the

    differences we found are unknown. Nevertheless, some

    have reported that cortical gray matter overgrowth,

    including within the temporal lobes, in young people

    [Wassink, Hazlett et al., 2007] (but not adults [Raznahan,

    Pugliese et al., 2009] with autism may be associated with

    functional variation in the serotonin transporter gene.

    This suggestion is supported by other work demonstrating

    that serotonin is crucial to brain development, including

    synaptic modeling, neurogenesis, dendritic organization

    and axon myelination [Whitaker-Azmitia, 2001], and

    abnormal brain serotonin synthesis has been found in

    children with autism [Chugani, Muzik et al., 1999]; and a

    significant reduction in brain 5-HT2A receptor density

    has been reported in adults with ASD [Murphy, Daly

    et al., 2006] and their relatives [Goldberg, Anderson

    et al., 2009]. Hence, genetically determined differences in

    serotonin metabolism may contribute toward our find-ings. There are however, other potential candidate genes.

    For example, there is increasing support from our group

    and others for an association between ASD and genetic

    variation in the glutamatergic and GABAergic systems; in

    nonautistic populations glutamate and GABA modify

    neuronal growth, connectivity and function, and we have

    previously reported that physically healthy, normal IQ,

    medication nave adults with ASD have a significant

    increase in the glutamate/glutamine (Glx) concentration

    of amygdala and hippocampus complex [Page, Daly

    et al., 2006]. Future studies are required to replicate our

    work, and to investigate potential genetic and environ-

    mental factors associated with differences in morphome-

    try of the amygdala and hippocampus.

    Finally, we do not suggest that people with Asperger

    syndrome only have differences in the anatomy of the

    amygdala and hippocampus, as both metabolic [Endo,

    Shioiri et al., 2007; Otsuka, Harada et al., 1999; Page

    et al., 2006] functional [Ashwin, Baron-Cohen, et al.,

    2007; Baron-Cohen, Ring et al., 1999; Critchley, Daly

    et al., 2000; Grelotti, Klin et al., 2005; Pierce et al.,

    2001], and subtle neural network differences [Ecker,

    Rocha-Rego et al., 2010] in the amygdala and hippo-

    campus of people with ASD have also been reported.

    Hence, our findings add to the body of evidence that

    individuals with ASD have complex differences from

    controls in the structure, function, and metabolism of

    the amygdala and hippocampus. However, the relation-

    ship between these differences is unknown, and further

    research is required to clarify this.

    Limitations. This study was cross-sectional in design,and specific to individuals with Asperger syndrome.Hence, our findings describe age-related differences, not

    individual changes across time, and our findings may notgeneralize to others within the autism spectrum.Nevertheless, we were able to examine age-relateddifferences across an age-span of 39 years (1049 years ofage), which would not have been practically possible usinga longitudinal design. Also, the study was limited to apredominantly male sample and further studies are neededto ascertain whether these findings are also present infemales [Craig, Zaman et al., 2007]. ADOS assessmentswere not available for all subjects. However, ADI-Rs werecompleted on all subjects (N532), and ICD-10 researchdiagnoses were confirmed by two Consultant psychiatristsand a Nurse Specialist trained in the ADI and ADOS.

    We also carried out multiple statistical tests. However,

    we corrected (Bonferroni testing) for the increased risk of

    Type 1 errors due to multiple testing. Moreover, we used a

    relatively large sample of one clearly defined diagnostic

    group (healthy individuals with Asperger syndrome),

    who had no other medical history that may adversely

    affect brain development, and we controlled for head

    size. Finally, due to the nature of our study, we cannot

    determine if our findings of differences in volume ofamygdala are a cause or a consequence of social

    communication difficulties and their associated stress

    and anxiety. Hence, our findings must be viewed as

    preliminary.

    Conclusion

    Individuals with Asperger syndrome have significant

    differences from controls in volume and aging of the

    amygdala. The cause of these differences is unknown, and

    most likely includes a complex interaction between a

    primary difference in brain development and abnormal

    interactions between the affected individual and their

    environment. Future studies of amygdala and hippocampus

    development, combining structural and functional imaging

    with clinical measures of behavior are warranted in ASD.

    Acknowledgments

    We thank all the individuals and their families who

    participated in this study and our colleagues for their

    help in recruiting subjects. This study was supported by a

    grant from the Medical Research Council (MRC UK

    A.I.M.S research program). None of the authors reported

    any financial interests or potential conflicts of interests

    associated with this study. The authors declare no

    conflict of interest.

    References

    Abell, F., Krams, M., Ashburner, J., Passingham, R., Friston, K.,

    et al. (1999). The neuroanatomy of autism: A voxel-based

    INSAR Murphy et al./Anatomy and aging of the amygdala 79

  • 8/13/2019 Anatomy and Aging of Amygdala and Hippocampus in ASD

    8/10

  • 8/13/2019 Anatomy and Aging of Amygdala and Hippocampus in ASD

    9/10

    Grieve, S.M., Clark, C.R., Williams, L.M., Peduto, A.J., &

    Gordon, E. (2005). Preservation of limbic and paralimbic

    structures in aging. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 391401.

    Groen, W., Teluij, M., Buitelaar, J., & Tendolkar, I. (2010).

    Amygdala and hippocampus enlargement during adolescence

    in autism. Journal of American Academy of Child Adolescent

    and Psychiatry, 49, 552560.

    Grunwald, T., Lehnertz, K., Heinze, H.J., Helmstaedter, C., &

    Elger, C.E. (1998). Verbal novelty detection within thehuman hippocampus proper. Proceedings of the National

    Academy of Sciences USA, 95, 31933197.

    Haug, J.S., Goldner, C.M., Yazlovitskaya, E.M., Voziyan, P.A., &

    Melnykovych, G. (1994). Directed cell killing (apoptosis)

    in human lymphoblastoid cells incubated in the presence

    of farnesol: Effect of phosphatidylcholine. Biochimica

    Biophysica Acta, 1223, 133140.

    Haxby, J.V., Hoffman, E.A., & Gobbini, M.I. (2002). Human

    neural systems for face recognition and social communica-

    tion. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 5967.

    Haznedar, M.M., Buchsbaum, M.S., Wei, T.C., Hof, P.R.,

    Cartwright, C., et al. (2000). Limbic circuitry in patients with

    autism spectrum disorders studied with positron emissiontomography and magnetic resonance imaging. American

    Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 19942001.

    Herbert, M.R., Ziegler, D.A., Deutsch, C.K., OBrien, L.M.,

    Lange, N., et al. (2003). Dissociations of cerebral cortex,

    subcortical and cerebral white matter volumes in autistic

    boys. Brain, 126, 11821192.

    Holland, P.C., & Gallagher, M. (2004). Amygdala-frontal inter-

    actions and reward expectancy. Current Opinion in Neuro-

    biology, 14, 148155.

    Howard, M.A., Cowell, P.E., Boucher, J., Broks, P., Mayes, A., et al.

    (2000). Convergent neuroanatomical and behavioural evi-

    dence of an amygdala hypothesis of autism. Neuroreport, 11,

    29312935.

    Kluver, H.a.B.P. (1939). Preliminary analysis of functioning of

    the temporal lobes in monkeys. Archives of Neurological

    Psychiatry, 42, 9791000.

    LeDoux, J. (2003). The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala.

    Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 23, 727738.

    Liu, R.S., Lemieux, L., Bell, G.S., Sisodiya, S.M., Shorvon, S.D.,

    et al. (2003). A longitudinal study of brain morphometrics

    using quantitative magnetic resonance imaging and differ-

    ence image analysis. Neuroimage, 20, 2233.

    Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., et al.

    (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A standar-

    dized observation of communicative and social behavior.

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19,

    185212.

    Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic

    Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview

    for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive develop-

    mental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental

    Disorders, 24, 659685.

    Maguire, E.A., Gadian, D.G., Johnsrude, I.S., Good, C.D.,

    Ashburner, J., et al. (2000). Navigation-related structural

    change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of

    the National Academy of Sciences USA, 97, 43984403.

    McAlonan, G.M., Suckling, J., Wong, N., Cheung, V.,

    Lienenkaemper, N., et al. (2008). Distinct patterns of grey

    matter abnormality in high-functioning autism and Asper-

    gers syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,

    49, 12871295.

    McAlonan, G.M., Cheung, C., Cheung, V., Wong, N., Suckling, J.,

    et al. (2009). Differential effects on white-matter systems in

    high-functioning autism and Aspergers syndrome. Psycho-

    logical Medicine, 39, 18851893.McEwen, B.S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and

    adaptation: Central role of the brain. Physiological Reviews,

    87, 873904.

    McGaugh, J.L. (2004). The amygdala modulates the consolida-

    tion of memories of emotionally arousing experiences.

    Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27: 128.

    Mosconi, M.W., Cody-Hazlett, H., Poe, M.D., Gerig, G.,

    Gimpel-Smith, R., et al. (2009). Longitudinal study of amygdala

    volume and joint attention in 2- to 4-year-old children with

    autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 509516.

    Murphy, D.G., DeCarli, C., McIntosh, A.R., Daly, E., Mentis, M.J.,

    et al. (1996). Sex differences in human brain morphometry

    and metabolism: An in vivo quantitative magnetic resonance

    imaging and positron emission tomography study on the

    effect of aging. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 585594.

    Murphy, D.G., Daly, E., Schmitz, N., Toal, F., Murphy, K., et al.

    (2006). Cortical serotonin 5-HT2A receptor binding and

    social communication in adults with Aspergers syndrome:

    An in vivo SPECT study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163,

    934936.

    Nacewicz, B.M., Dalton, K.M., Johnstone, T., Long, M.T.,

    McAuliff, E.M., et al. (2006). Amygdala volume and non-

    verbal social impairment in adolescent and adult males with

    autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 14171428.

    Nicolson, R., DeVito, T.J., Vidal, C.N., Sui, Y., Hayashi, K.M.,

    et al. (2006). Detection and mapping of hippocampal

    abnormalities in autism. Psychiatry Research, 148, 1121.Otsuka, H., Harada, M., Mori, K., Hisaoka, S., & Nishitani, H.

    (1999). Brain metabolites in the hippocampus-amygdala

    region and cerebellum in autism: An 1H-MR spectroscopy

    study. Neuroradiology, 41, 517519.

    Page, L.A., Daly, E., Schmitz, N., Simmons, A., Toal, F., et al.

    (2006). In vivo 1H-magnetic resonance spectroscopy study of

    amygdala-hippocampal and parietal regions in autism.

    American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 21892192.

    Pallant, J. (2005). SPSS survival manual. A step by step guide to

    data analysis using SPSS for Windows. Buckingham, Maiden-

    head, UK: Open University Press.

    Palmen, S.J., van Engeland, H., Hof, P.R., Schmitz, C., et al. (2004).

    Neuropathological findings in autism. Brain, 127, 25722583.Palmen, S.J., Hulshoff Pol, H.E., Kemner, C., Schnack, H.G.,

    Sitskoorn, M.M., et al. (2005). Brain anatomy in non-affected

    parents of autistic probands: A MRI study. Psychological

    Medicine, 35, 14111420.

    Palmen, S.J., Durston, S., Nederveen, H., & Van Engeland, H.

    (2006). No evidence for preferential involvement of medial

    temporal lobe structures in high-functioning autism. Psycho-

    logical Medicine, 36, 827834.

    Pierce, K., Muller, R.A., Ambrose, J., Allen, G., & Courchesne, E.

    (2001). Face processing occurs outside the fusiform face

    INSAR Murphy et al./Anatomy and aging of the amygdala 911

  • 8/13/2019 Anatomy and Aging of Amygdala and Hippocampus in ASD

    10/10

    area in autism: Evidence from functional MRI. Brain, 124,

    20592073.

    Piven, J., Bailey, J., Ranson, B.J., & Arndt, S. (1998). No difference

    in hippocampus volume detected on magnetic resonance

    imaging in autistic individuals. Journal of Autism and

    Developmental Disorders, 28, 105110.

    Pruessner, J.C., Collins, D.L., Pruessner, M., & Evans, A.C. (2001).

    Age and gender predict volume decline in the anterior and

    posterior hippocampus in early adulthood. Journal of

    Neuroscience, 21, 194200.

    Raymond, G.V., Bauman, M.L., & Kemper, T.L. (1996). Hippo-

    campus in autism: A Golgi analysis. Acta Neuropathology, 91,

    117119.

    Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K.M., Kennedy, K.M.,

    Head, D., et al. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging

    healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and

    modifiers. Cereberal Cortex, 15, 16761689.

    Raznahan, A., Pugliese, L., Barker, G.J., Daly, E., Powell, J., et al.

    (2009). Serotonin transporter genotype and neuroanatomy in

    autism spectrum disorders. Psychiatric Genetics, 19, 147150.

    Richardson, M.P., Strange, B.A., Dolan, R.J. (2004). Encoding of

    emotional memories depends on amygdala and hippocampus

    and their interactions. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 278285.Rojas, D.C., Smith, J.A., Benkers, T.L., Camou, S.L., Reite, M.L.,

    et al. (2004). Hippocampus and amygdala volumes in parents

    of children with autistic disorder. American Journal of

    Psychiatry, 161, 20382044.

    Rosvold, H.E., Mirsky, A.F., & Pribram, K.H. (1954). Influence of

    amygdalectomy on social behavior in monkeys. Journal of

    Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47, 173178.

    Saitoh, O., Karns, C.M., & Courchesne, E. (2001). Development of

    the hippocampal formation from 2 to 42 years: MRI evidence

    of smaller area dentata in autism. Brain, 124, 13171324.

    Salmond, C.H., Ashburner, J., Connelly, A., Friston, K.J.,

    Gadian, D.G., et al. (2005). The role of the medial temporal

    lobe in autistic spectrum disorders. European Journal of

    Neurosciences, 22, 764772.

    Schultz, R.T. (2005). Developmental deficits in social perception

    in autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face area.

    International Journal of Developmental Neurosciences, 23,

    125141.

    Schumann, C.M., & Amaral, D.G. (2005). Stereological estima-

    tion of the number of neurons in the human amygdaloid

    complex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 491, 320329.

    Schumann, C.M., & Amaral, D.G. (2006). Stereological analysis

    of amygdala neuron number in autism. Journal of Neu-

    roscience, 26, 76747679.

    Schumann, C.M., Hamstra, J., Goodlin-Jones, B.L., Lotspeich, L.J.,

    Kwon, H., et al. (2004). The amygdala is enlarged in children

    but not adolescents with autism; the hippocampus is enlargedat all ages. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 63926401.

    Schumann, C.M., Barnes, C.C., Lord, C., & Courchesne, E.

    (2009). Amygdala enlargement in toddlers with autism

    related to severity of social and communication impairments.

    Biological Psychiatry, 66, 942949.

    Sparks, B.F., Friedman, S.D., Shaw, D.W., Aylward, E.H.,

    Echelard, D., et al. (2002). Brain structural abnormalities in

    young children with autism spectrum disorder. Neurology,

    59, 184192.

    Suzuki, M., Hagino, H., Nohara, S., Zhou, S.Y., Kawasaki, Y., et al.

    (2005). Male-specific volume expansion of the human

    hippocampus during adolescence. Cereberal Cortex, 15,

    187193.

    Sweeten, T.L., Posey, D.J., Shekhar, A., & McDougle, C.J. (2002).

    The amygdala and related structures in the pathophysiology

    of autism. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behaviour, 71,

    449455.

    The ICD10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders.

    Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. (1994).

    Geneva: World Health Organization.

    van Amelsvoort, T., Daly, E., Robertson, D., Suckling, J., Ng, V.,

    et al. (2001). Structural brain abnormalities associated with

    deletion at chromosome 22q11: Quantitative neuroimaging

    study of adults with velo-cardio-facial syndrome. British

    Journal of Psychiatry, 178: 412419.

    Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D.G., Watkins, K.E., Connelly, A.,Van Paesschen, W., et al. (1997). Differential effects of early

    hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory.

    Science, 277, 376380.

    Vyas, A., Mitra, R., Shankaranarayana Rao, B.S., & Chattarji, S.

    (2002). Chronic stress induces contrasting patterns of

    dendritic remodeling in hippocampal and amygdaloid neu-

    rons. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 68106818.

    Vyas, A., Bernal, S., & Chattarji, S. (2003). Effects of chronic

    stress on dendritic arborization in the central and extended

    amygdala. Brain Research, 965, 290294.

    Wassink, T.H., Hazlett, H.C., Epping, E.A., Arndt, S., Dager, S.R.,

    et al. (2007). Cerebral cortical gray matter overgrowth and

    functional variation of the serotonin transporter gene in

    autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 709717.

    Watson, C., Andermann, F., Gloor, P., Jones-Gotman, M.,

    Peters, T., et al. (1992). Anatomic basis of amygdaloid and

    hippocampal volume measurement by magnetic resonance

    imaging. Neurology, 42, 17431750.

    Weschler, D. (1999). Weschler abbreviated scale of intelligence.

    Psychological Corporation. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Press.

    West, M.J., Slomianka, L., & Gundersen, H.J. (1991). Unbiased

    stereological estimation of the total number of neurons in the

    subdivisions of the rat hippocampus using the optical

    fractionator. The Anatomical Record, 231, 482497.

    Whitaker-Azmitia, P.M. (2001). Serotonin and brain develop-

    ment: Role in human developmental diseases. Brain Research

    Bulletin, 56, 479485.White, S.W., Oswald, D., Ollendick, T., & Scahill, L. (2009).

    Anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spectrum

    disorders. Clinical Psychology Reviews, 29, 216229.

    10 Murphy et al./Anatomy and aging of the amygdala INSAR12