1
Cerebellar contributions to skilled operations in verbal working memory A.L. Hayter, D.W. Langdon and N. Ramnani Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway University of London, U.K. Introduction Working memory related activations in the cerebellum? In this study a demanding verbal working memory task (the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task; PASAT 6 ), was utilized to isolate activity related to skilled cognitive operations. On the basis of previous work we predicted activity localized to the human homologue of macaque prefrontal area 46 (human area 9/46 of Petrides & Pandya, 1999 7 ). Importantly, we also predicted such activity in hemispheral and vermal components Lobule VII of the cerebellar cortex. Reciprocal Cortico-Cerebellar Connections The cortico-cerebellar system is composed of multiple, parallel loops. Evidence from non-human primates shows dense projections between the cerebellum and the cortical motor system 1 , as well as additional projections between the cerebellum and the prefrontal cortex (Walker’s area 46 2 ). The latter suggests a role for this system in processing information that may not be directly related to motor control. The motor cortex (area 4) is reciprocally connected with lobules HV, HVI, HVIIB and HVIII of the cerebellar cortex. Prefrontal area 46 is reciprocally connected to vermal and hemispheral parts of lobule VII (see figure 1). In humans, evidence from Diffusion Tractography shows that prefrontal inputs from the prefrontal cortex have selectively evolved 3 . Experimental Design 1s visual instruction cue jittered over first 9 seconds 5 numbers presented through MRI compatible headphones Two conditions ADD (experimental condition): On presentation of every number (n), that number must be added to the preceding number heard in the sequence (n-1) REPEAT (control condition): Repeat last number heard In ADD and REPEAT, participants were required to speak their answer aloud. Verbal responses were recorded with an MRI compatible microphone. Sparse Sampling The requirement to produce an overt verbal response is an important feature of the PASAT that ensures the imposition of relatively high cognitive demands. We used sparse sampling in order to avoid artifacts caused by speech-related head motion (see figure 2). EPI images were acquired rapidly in the first 2 seconds of each TR. Since no data were acquired during the remaining 1s interval, participants could speak without risk of head motion-related artifact. Information processing in the cerebellar-prefrontal loop Information processing within the cerebellar-prefrontal loop is poorly understood 4 . Cerebellar theory suggests that cerebellar components of the “motor” loop acquire forward models of cerebral cortical information processing. These aid the automatic execution of processes required for motor control 4 . This information processing account can be extended to the “prefrontal loop”, such that prefrontal-projecting areas of the cerebellar cortex may acquire forward models of prefrontal information processing. These may play an important role in the rapid and efficient processing of routine cognitive operations. Such processes should therefore activate the cerebellar components of the prefrontal loop 4 , just as skilled motor control activates cerebellar components of the motor loop 5 . Analysis Methods Experimental Timings Data Acquisition 644 EPI images were acquired using a 3T Siemens Trio MRI. 27 transversal slices were collected, TE = 32 s, TR = 3 s, voxel size 3 mm³ + 1mm gap, FoV = 192 x 192, image matrix = 64 x 64 pixels. The scanning sequence lasted 32.5 minutes. T1 structurals were also acquired. The experiment consisted of two phases: Preparation phase: participants practiced a traditional version of the PASAT outside the scanner (they heard 60 numbers consecutively). They performed mental calculations in the manner specified below. Then the participants practiced our variant of the PASAT (see below) within the scanner (2 blocks of experimental and 2 blocks of control trials). Experimental phase: Participants presented with 35 experimental blocks, 35 control blocks and 10 null block, pseudorandomly intermixed. Visual instruction cue Scanner noise (volume acquisition) 9s 15s 1s 3s TR Jitter 2s 1s Instruction Cue Period Experimental Block Auditory stimuli Verbal responses 5 4 2 6 3 “9” “6” “8” “9” Figure 2 Scans were analyzed in SPM2. Preprocessing: Scans were realigned and normalized to the reference space of the MNI template brain, and then smoothed with a Gaussian kernel (8 mm). Statistical Analysis: First-level single-subject analysis: Four event types were modeled in each subject-specific GLM: 1. Instruction cue (modeled as transient HRF); 2. ADD block; 3. REPEAT block; 4. ERROR block (8 % of all ADD blocks that were excluded from 2 in which participants made errors). At the first level, events were convolved with the canonical HRF and the resulting regressors were incorporated into a subject-specific GLM. Head motion parameters estimated during realignment were incorporated as confounding covariates. The following SPM{t} contrasts were applied: Results During scanning, participants scored an average of 97% correct for ADD. There were no errors in the REPEAT condition. Conjunction: Areas commonly activated by ADD and REPEAT included ventral regions of sensorimotor cortex (BA 3, BA 4; see figure 3A) and superior temporal sulcus. These reflect the common auditory processing and speech production demands of both conditions. We additionally report common activity in cerebellar areas known to be interconnected with the primary motor cortex (figure 3B). ADD > REPEAT: Figure 4 Consistent with out prediction, activity was found in areas of the prefrontal cortex, 9/46 (figure 4, A2) and also areas of the cerebellum known to be interconnected with this area in non-human primates (lobule VII; figure 4, B3). Conclusions • Activity in the ‘motor loop’: The conjunction across conditions revealed activity related to the common motor demands of both conditions. Such activity was found in ventral regions of the precentral gyrus known to contain representations of facial musculature. This area was co-activated with cerebellar cortical lobule HVI which is known to be interconnected with the motor cortex in non-human primates. • Activity in the ‘prefrontal loop’: • Differential activity between ADD and REPEAT reflected information processing related to the maintenance and manipulation of information in verbal working memory, while controlling for non-specific sensory and motor factors common to both conditions. Such activity was found in prefrontal area 9/46 as reported in several previous studies. • Consistent with our hypothesis, such activity was also found in lobule VII of the cerebellar cortex. • The co-activation of prefrontal interconnected lobule VII of the cerebellum with these areas imply that they act as a functional network involved in abstract cognitive information processing – the ‘prefrontal’ cortico-cerebellar loop. • We propose that circuits in cerebellar cortical lobule VII acquire internal models of prefrontal information processing related to verbal working memory. These may be used in a feed-forward manner during the execution of routine and automatic cognitive operations. (see Hayter, A.L., Langdon, D.W., & Ramnani, N., “Cerebellar contributions to working memory”. NeuroImage (In press). 1. ADD > REPEAT 2. ADD only 3. REPEAT only Second-level random effects analysis: ADD>REPEAT: Group level analyses were conducted by incorporating single-subject contrast images into a one-sample t-test (FWE corrected, p<0.05) • Conjunction: a one-way ANOVA was applied to contrast images 2 and 3 determined at the first level (FWE corrected). References 1. Brodal, P., (1978), Brain: 101,251-283 5. Ito, M., (2005), Progress in Brain Research: 148, 95-109 2. Middleton, F.A. & Strick, P.L., (2001), Journal of Neuroscience: 21, 700-712 6. Gronwall, D.M., (1977), Perceptual and Motor Skills: 44, 367-373 3. Ramnani, N., et al.., (2006), Cerebral Cortex: 16, 811-818 7. Petrides, M. & Pandya, D.N., (1999), European Journal of Neuroscience: 11, 1011-1036 4. Ramnani, N., (2006), Nature Reviews Neuroscience: 7, 511-522 Figure 1 Table 1: ADD>REPEAT, 1 sample t-test, FWE Sulcaland G yralAnatom y FunctionalA rea T Z M NI C oordinates x y z Cytoarchitectonicarea (probablistic m ap percentage) F rontal lobe Prefrontal cortex M iddle frontalgyrus Prefrontalcortex 9.37 5.2 -46 32 34 BA 9/46;BA 45 (10 % ) Posteriorsuperiorfrontalgyrus Prefrontal/Premotor cortex 8.95 5.1 28 0 66 BA 6 (10 % ) Anteriorcingulate sulcus 11.33 5.6 -10 18 36 BA 32 M iddle frontalgyrus /Inferior frontalgyrus(parstriangularis) Prefrontalcortex 10.25 5.4 -42 36 24 BA 9/46, BA 44 (10 % ) M iddle frontalgyrus Prefrontalcortex 9.84 5.3 46 42 28 BA 9/46, BA 9 Inferiorfrontalgyrus(pars triangularis) PartofBroca’sarea 8.72 5 -48 32 28 BA 45 (40 % ); B A 44 (10 % ) Premotor/primary motor cortex Superiorfrontalgyrus(medial) Pre-SMA 16.97 6.5 - 4 10 56 BA 6(50 % ) Precentralgyrus Premotorcortex 11.55 5.7 -34 0 46 BA 6 (10 % ) Parietal lobe Superiorparietallobule 14.04 6.1 -44 -44 56 BA 2 (40 % ), B A 1(20 %) Intraparietalsulcus 12.02 5.8 -30 -52 46 n/a Angulargyrus 9.02 5.1 30 -58 50 BA 7 Superiorparietalgyrus 9.13 5.1 -34 -54 62 BA 2 (20 % );B A (10 % ) Superiorparietalgyrus 9.23 5.2 -2 -64 66 BA 7 Insular L obe Shortinsulargyri 14.06 6.1 38 20 -4 n/a Shortinsulargyri 10.76 5.5 -28 18 12 n/a Basal Ganglia Caudate nucleus(tail) 9.6 5.3 -18 -10 24 n/a Caudate nucleus(head) 8.99 5.1 22 16 12 n/a Brain Stem Red nucleus 9.16 5.1 - 2 -24 -10 n/a Cerebellum Cerebellarcortex (lobule VI) 8.83 5.1 0 -76 -12 n/a Cerebellarcortex (lobule VI) 10.21 5.4 6 -78 -22 n/a Cerebellarcortex (lobule VII) 9.61 5.3 6 -84 -34 n/a Cerebellarcortex (lobule VII) 9.68 5.3 - 8 -80 -22 n/a Figure 3

Cerebellar contributions to skilled operations in verbal working memory A.L. Hayter, D.W. Langdon and N. Ramnani Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway

  • View
    216

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Cerebellar contributions to skilled operations in verbal working memory A.L. Hayter, D.W. Langdon and N. Ramnani Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway

Cerebellar contributions to skilled operationsin verbal working memory

A.L. Hayter, D.W. Langdon and N. Ramnani Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway University of London, U.K.

Introduction

Working memory related activations in the cerebellum?

In this study a demanding verbal working memory task (the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task; PASAT6), was utilized to isolate activity related to skilled cognitive operations. On the basis of previous work we predicted activity localized to the human homologue of macaque prefrontal area 46 (human area 9/46 of Petrides & Pandya, 19997). Importantly, we also predicted such activity in hemispheral and vermal components Lobule VII of the cerebellar cortex.

Reciprocal Cortico-Cerebellar Connections

The cortico-cerebellar system is composed of multiple, parallel loops. Evidence from non-human primates shows dense projections between the cerebellum and the cortical motor system1, as well as additional projections between the cerebellum and the prefrontal cortex (Walker’s area 462). The latter suggests a role for this system in processing information that may not be directly related to motor control. The motor cortex (area 4) is reciprocally connected with lobules HV, HVI, HVIIB and HVIII of the cerebellar cortex. Prefrontal area 46 is reciprocally connected to vermal and hemispheral parts of lobule VII (see figure 1). In humans, evidence from Diffusion Tractography shows that prefrontal inputs from the prefrontal cortex have selectively evolved3.

Experimental Design

1s visual instruction cue jittered over first 9 seconds

5 numbers presented through MRI compatible headphones

Two conditions

ADD (experimental condition): On presentation of every number (n), that number must be added to the preceding number heard in the sequence (n-1)

REPEAT (control condition): Repeat last number heard

In ADD and REPEAT, participants were required to speak their answer aloud. Verbal responses were recorded with an MRI compatible microphone.

Sparse Sampling

The requirement to produce an overt verbal response is an important feature of the PASAT that ensures the imposition of relatively high cognitive demands. We used sparse sampling in order to avoid artifacts caused by speech-related head motion (see figure 2). EPI images were acquired rapidly in the first 2 seconds of each TR. Since no data were acquired during the remaining 1s interval, participants could speak without risk of head motion-related artifact.

Information processing in the cerebellar-prefrontal loop

Information processing within the cerebellar-prefrontal loop is poorly understood4. Cerebellar theory suggests that cerebellar components of the “motor” loop acquire forward models of cerebral cortical information processing. These aid the automatic execution of processes required for motor control4. This information processing account can be extended to the “prefrontal loop”, such that prefrontal-projecting areas of the cerebellar cortex may acquire forward models of prefrontal information processing. These may play an important role in the rapid and efficient processing of routine cognitive operations. Such processes should therefore activate the cerebellar components of the prefrontal loop4, just as skilled motor control activates cerebellar components of the motor loop5.

Analysis Methods

Experimental Timings

Data Acquisition

644 EPI images were acquired using a 3T Siemens Trio MRI. 27 transversal slices were collected, TE = 32 s, TR = 3 s, voxel size 3 mm³ + 1mm gap, FoV = 192 x 192, image matrix = 64 x 64 pixels. The scanning sequence lasted 32.5 minutes. T1 structurals were also acquired.

The experiment consisted of two phases:

Preparation phase: participants practiced a traditional version of the PASAT outside the scanner (they heard 60 numbers consecutively). They performed mental calculations in the manner specified below. Then the participants practiced our variant of the PASAT (see below) within the scanner (2 blocks of experimental and 2 blocks of control trials).

Experimental phase: Participants presented with 35 experimental blocks, 35 control blocks and 10 null block, pseudorandomly intermixed.

Visual instruction cue

Scanner noise (volume acquisition)

9s 15s

1s

3s TR

Jitter

2s

1s

Instruction Cue Period Experimental Block

Auditory stimuli

Verbal responses

5 4 2 6 3

“9” “6” “8” “9”

Figure 2

Scans were analyzed in SPM2.

Preprocessing:• Scans were realigned and normalized to the reference space of the MNI template brain, and then smoothed with a Gaussian

kernel (8 mm).

Statistical Analysis: • First-level single-subject analysis:

Four event types were modeled in each subject-specific GLM: 1. Instruction cue (modeled as transient HRF); 2. ADD block; 3. REPEAT block; 4. ERROR block (8 % of all ADD blocks that were excluded from 2 in which participants made errors). At the first level, events were convolved with the canonical HRF and the resulting regressors were incorporated into a subject-specific GLM. Head motion parameters estimated during realignment were incorporated as confounding covariates. The following SPM{t} contrasts were applied:

ResultsDuring scanning, participants scored an average of 97% correct for ADD. There were no errors in the REPEAT condition.

Conjunction: Areas commonly activated by ADD and REPEAT included ventral regions of sensorimotor cortex (BA 3, BA 4; see figure 3A) and superior temporal sulcus. These reflect the common auditory processing and speech production demands of both conditions.

We additionally report common activity in cerebellar areas known to be interconnected with the primary motor cortex (figure 3B).

ADD > REPEAT:

Figure 4

Consistent with out prediction, activity was found in areas of the prefrontal cortex, 9/46 (figure 4, A2) and also areas of the cerebellum known to be interconnected with this area in non-human primates (lobule VII; figure 4, B3).

Conclusions• Activity in the ‘motor loop’: The conjunction across conditions revealed activity related to the common motor demands of both conditions. Such activity was found in ventral regions of the precentral gyrus known to contain representations of facial musculature. This area was co-activated with cerebellar cortical lobule HVI which is known to be interconnected with the motor cortex in non-human primates.

• Activity in the ‘prefrontal loop’: • Differential activity between ADD and REPEAT reflected information processing related to the maintenance and manipulation of information in verbal working memory, while controlling for non-specific sensory and motor factors common to both conditions. Such activity was found in prefrontal area 9/46 as reported in several previous studies. • Consistent with our hypothesis, such activity was also found in lobule VII of the cerebellar cortex.• The co-activation of prefrontal interconnected lobule VII of the cerebellum with these areas imply that they act as a functional network involved in abstract cognitive information processing – the ‘prefrontal’ cortico-cerebellar loop.

• We propose that circuits in cerebellar cortical lobule VII acquire internal models of prefrontal information processing related to verbal working memory. These may be used in a feed-forward manner during the execution of routine and automatic cognitive operations.

(see Hayter, A.L., Langdon, D.W., & Ramnani, N., “Cerebellar contributions to working memory”. NeuroImage (In press).

1. ADD > REPEAT2. ADD only 3. REPEAT only

• Second-level random effects analysis: • ADD>REPEAT: Group level analyses were conducted by incorporating single-subject contrast images into a one-sample t-test (FWE corrected, p<0.05) • Conjunction: a one-way ANOVA was applied to contrast images 2 and 3 determined at the first level (FWE corrected).

References1. Brodal, P., (1978), Brain: 101,251-283 5. Ito, M., (2005), Progress in Brain Research: 148, 95-1092. Middleton, F.A. & Strick, P.L., (2001), Journal of Neuroscience: 21, 700-712 6. Gronwall, D.M., (1977), Perceptual and Motor Skills: 44, 367-373 3. Ramnani, N., et al.., (2006), Cerebral Cortex: 16, 811-818 7. Petrides, M. & Pandya, D.N., (1999), European Journal of Neuroscience: 11, 1011-1036 4. Ramnani, N., (2006), Nature Reviews Neuroscience: 7, 511-522

Figure 1

Table 1: ADD>REPEAT, 1 sample t-test, FWE

Sulcal and Gyral Anatomy Functional Area T Z MNI Coordinates x y z

Cytoarchitectonic area (probablistic map percentage)

Frontal lobe Prefrontal cortex Middle frontal gyrus Prefrontal cortex 9.37 5.2 -46 32 34 BA 9/46; BA 45 (10 %)

Posterior superior frontal gyrus Prefrontal / Premotor cortex

8.95 5.1 28 0 66 BA 6 (10 %)

Anterior cingulate sulcus 11.33 5.6 -10 18 36 BA 32 Middle frontal gyrus / Inferior frontal gyrus (pars triangularis)

Prefrontal cortex 10.25 5.4 -42 36 24 BA 9/46, BA 44 (10 %)

Middle frontal gyrus Prefrontal cortex 9.84 5.3 46 42 28 BA 9/46, BA 9 Inferior frontal gyrus (pars triangularis)

Part of Broca’s area 8.72 5 -48 32 28 BA 45 (40 %); BA 44 (10 %)

Premotor/primary motor cortex

Superior frontal gyrus (medial) Pre-SMA 16.97 6.5 - 4 10 56 BA 6(50 %) Precentral gyrus Premotor cortex 11.55 5.7 - 34 0 46 BA 6 (10 %) Parietal lobe Superior parietal lobule 14.04 6.1 -44 -44 56 BA 2 (40 %), BA 1(20

%) Intraparietal sulcus 12.02 5.8 -30 -52 46 n/a Angular gyrus 9.02 5.1 30 -58 50 BA 7 Superior parietal gyrus 9.13 5.1 -34 -54 62 BA 2 (20 %); BA (10 %) Superior parietal gyrus 9.23 5.2 - 2 -64 66 BA 7 Insular Lobe Short insular gyri 14.06 6.1 38 20 - 4 n/a Short insular gyri 10.76 5.5 -28 18 12 n/a Basal Ganglia Caudate nucleus (tail) 9.6 5.3 -18 -10 24 n/a Caudate nucleus (head) 8.99 5.1 22 16 12 n/a Brain Stem Red nucleus 9.16 5.1 - 2 -24 -10 n/a Cerebellum Cerebellar cortex (lobule VI) 8.83 5.1 0 -76 -12 n/a Cerebellar cortex (lobule VI) 10.21 5.4 6 -78 -22 n/a Cerebellar cortex (lobule VII) 9.61 5.3 6 -84 -34 n/a Cerebellar cortex (lobule VII) 9.68 5.3 - 8 -80 -22 n/a

Figure 3