Documentcl

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • American Journal of Primatology 73:11891198 (2011)

    REVIEW ARTICLE

    Influence of Climatic Variables, Forest Type, and Condition on Activity Patternsof Geoffroyis Spider Monkeys Throughout Mesoamerica

    ARTURO GONZALEZ-ZAMORA1, VICTOR ARROYO-RODRIGUEZ2, OSCAR M. CHAVES2, SONIA SANCHEZ-LOPEZ3,FILIPPO AURELI4,5, AND KATHRYN E. STONER21Division de Posgrado, Instituto de Ecologa, Xalapa, Veracruz, Mexico2Centro de Investigaciones en Ecosistemas, Universidad Nacional Autonoma de Mexico (UNAM), Morelia, Michoacan, Mexico3Centre Especial de Recerca en Primats, Universitat de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain4Research Center in Evolutionary Anthropology and Palaeoecology, School of Natural Sciences and Psychology, Liverpool JohnMoores University, James Parsons Building, Byrom Street, Liverpool, United Kingdom5Instituto de Neuroetologa, Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, Mexico

    Understanding how species cope with variations in climatic conditions, forest types and habitat amountis a fundamental challenge for ecologists and conservation biologists. We used data from 18communities of Mesoamerican spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) throughout their range to determinewhether their activity patterns are affected by climatic variables (temperature and rainfall), foresttypes (seasonal and nonseasonal forests), and forest condition (continuous and fragmented). Data werederived from 15 published and unpublished studies carried out in four countries (Mexico, El Salvador,Costa Rica, and Panama), cumulatively representing more than 18 years (221 months, 43,645 hr) ofbehavioral observations. Overall, A. geoffroyi spent most of their time feeding (38.4714.0%, mean7SD)and resting (36.6712.8%) and less time traveling (19.8711.3%). Resting and feeding were mainlyaffected by rainfall: resting time increased with decreasing rainfall, whereas feeding time increasedwith rainfall. Traveling time was negatively related to both rainfall and maximum temperature.In addition, both resting and traveling time were higher in seasonal forests (tropical dry forest andtropical moist forest) than in nonseasonal forests (tropical wet forest), but feeding time followed theopposite pattern. Furthermore, spider monkeys spent more time feeding and less time resting (i.e.,higher feeding effort) in forest fragments than in continuous forest. These findings suggest that globalclimate changes and habitat deforestation and fragmentation in Mesoamerica will threaten the survivalof spider monkeys and reduce the distributional range of the species in the coming decades. Am. J.Primatol. 73:11891198, 2011. r 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

    Key words: activity patterns; Ateles geoffroyi; climate change; fragmentation; rainfall; seasonality;temperature

    INTRODUCTION

    Activity patterns (time spent feeding, traveling,resting, and socializing) provide information on howanimals interact with the environment and invest timeand energy for survival and reproduction [Dunbar,1988]. Studying activity patterns is crucial for betterunderstanding why a given taxon can survive in somehabitats but not in others [Dunbar et al., 2009], and forindentifying the areas that are suitable for a specieswithin their geographical range [Korstjens et al., 2006].As a consequence of accelerating global climate changeas well as unprecedented deforestation rates in tropicalregions during the last decades [Anderson et al., 2008;FAO, 2011], it is fundamental to understand the abilityof species to develop behavioral modifications in theiractivity patterns to cope with both climatic change andhabitat deforestation [Dunbar et al., 2009].

    In many primate groups, activity patterns maybe affected by climatic variables [Dunbar, 1998;

    Dunbar et al., 2009; Korstjens et al., 2006;Wiederholt & Post, 2010], seasonality [Doran, 1997;Korstjens et al., 2006], habitat characteristics [Isbellet al., 1998], or some combination of these factors.Ambient temperature and rainfall, and the season-ality of these characteristics, are the main climaticvariables driving activity patterns in many primates[e.g., Ateles: Korstjens et al., 2006; Gorilla and

    Published online 6 September 2011 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com).

    DOI 10.1002/ajp.20989

    Received 18 November 2010; revised 19 July 2011; revisionaccepted 22 July 2011

    Contract grant sponsor: Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecno-loga (CONACYT), Mexico; Contract grant numbers: 2005-51043; 2006-56799.

    Correspondence to: Arturo Gonzalez-Zamora, Division dePosgrado, Instituto de Ecologa A.C. km 2.5 Antigua Carreteraa Coatepec No. 351. 91070 Xalapa, Veracruz, Mexico.E-mail: [email protected]

    rr 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

  • Colobus: Korstjens & Dunbar, 2007; Korstjens et al.,2010; Pan: Lehmann et al., 2007; Theropithecus:Dunbar, 1998]. For instance, to deal with climaticstresses, such as high temperatures and/ordrought, primates frequently reduce traveling timebecause this is an energetically costly activity,allowing them to reduce energy expenditures[Dunbar et al., 2009; Korstjens et al., 2006] andto increase their resting time to avoid overheating[e.g., Theropithecus gelada: Dunbar, 1988, 1992a;Papio hamadryas: Stelzner, 1988; Pan troglodytes:Kosheleff & Anderson, 2009].

    Rainfall can also affect activity patterns ofprimates because of the positive relationshipbetween rainfall and resource availability [Bronikowski& Altmann, 1996; Deshmukh, 1984], particularly ofripe fleshy fruits [i.e., the main food source offrugivorous primates, such as spider monkeys;Di Fiore et al., 2008; Gonzalez-Zamora et al.,2009; Stevenson & Link, 2010]. Primates needto adjust their diet and activity patterns to copewith resource scarcity in habitats and/or duringperiods with lower rainfall regimes [e.g., Kaplinet al., 1998; McConkey et al., 2002; Stevenson et al.,2000].

    Resource availability also can affect amongforests with different climatic seasonality becausevegetation structure and composition differ amongthem [Gentry, 1982]. In contrast to tropical wet forests(TWF) that show little seasonality, fleshy fruit avail-ability may be limited in more seasonal forests [e.g.,tropical moist forest (TMF), tropical dry forest (TDF)],particularly during the dry season [Bunker & Carson,2005; Murphy & Lugo, 1995]. Anthropogenic activ-ities, such as deforestation, forest fragmentation andlogging, also can affect vegetation structure andcomposition, modifying resource availability for pri-mates [e.g., Arroyo-Rodrguez & Mandujano, 2006;Chapman et al., 2000]. The density of larger treesis typically lower in smaller forest fragments,particularly of those species that are used as foodresources by frugivorous primates [Arroyo-Rodrguez& Mandujano, 2006; Chaves et al., 2011a; Dunn et al.,2009]. Therefore, because larger trees produce higheramounts of fruits [Chapman et al., 1992], smallerfragments contain a lower food supply for frugivorousprimates than larger fragments [Arroyo-Rodrguez &Mandujano, 2006; Chapman et al., 1992; Dunn et al.,2009]. These changes in food availability ultimatelycan affect activity patterns. For example, time spentfeeding by gelada baboons (T. gelada) in Ethiopia isinversely related to spatial and temporal food avail-ability in three different habitats [i.e., two plateaus ofopen grassland mosaics at different altitudes and withdistinct plant compositions and one African savannah;Iwamoto & Dunbar, 1983]. In summary, activitypatterns seem to be a function of vegetation andclimatic characteristics [e.g., Caraco, 1979; Dunbar,1992a,b; Dunbar et al., 2009].

    Activity patterns also depend on the socialsystem in which the species live [Dunbar et al.,2009; Korstjens et al., 2006; Wrangham et al., 1993].For instance, the ecological constraints model pre-dicts that larger groups deplete food patches fasterthan smaller groups, and hence individuals in largergroups have to visit more patches each day, increaseday ranges, spend more time traveling, and in somecases, expand their home ranges [Chapman, 1990;Janson & Goldsmith, 1995; Milton, 1984; Wranghamet al., 1993]. Primates that live in social systems witha high degree of fissionfusion dynamics, such asspider monkeys (Ateles spp.), adjust their subgroupsize to local food availability [Aureli et al., 2008;Kummer, 1971] and annual rainfall (an indirectmeasure of habitat quality) [Korstjens et al., 2006].In these systems, individuals that belong to the samecommunity are rarely together, but they fission andfuse in smaller subgroups of variable composition.These fissionfusion dynamics are effective in flex-ibly managing activity patterns as larger subgroupsdo not experience greater ranging costs than smallersubgroups [Asensio et al., 2009]. Although Korstjenset al. [2006] suggest that community size is astronger determinant of time budgets than subgroupsize, they actually find that the percentages of timespent feeding and moving do not correlate withcommunity size or subgroup size using a large dataset on Ateles spp. Thus, unlike group size inrelatively cohesive groups, community size andsubgroup size are unlikely to play a major role inthe activity patterns in species characterized by ahigh degree of fissionfusion dynamics.

    Korstjens et al. [2006] investigated factorsaffecting activity patterns of the entire genus Ateles;however, research focusing on single species isnecessary to have a more detailed understanding oftheir ecology in changing habitats. Although somepublished studies document activity patterns ofAteles geoffroyi in continuous forests of Mesoamerica[e.g., Chapman, 1987a,b; Richard, 1970], no studiesto date have assessed the impact that climaticvariables, forest types, and forest condition mayhave on activity patterns through the distributionalrange. This information has critical evolutionary andconservation implications. From an evolutionaryperspective, throughout its distributional range,spider monkeys have adapted to certain areas orcomfort zones [sensu Anderson et al., 2008]characterized by climatic niches to which the speciesis tolerant [Korstjens et al., 2006]. These niches arecharacterized by relatively little variation in regionalclimatic conditions which allowed spider monkeys toadapt to their activity patterns, diet, and socialbehavior to the specific resource availability andenvironmental characteristics found in differentforest types [Anderson et al., 2008; Dunbar et al.,2009]. Unfortunately, predictive models about thepotential impact of climate change in Mesoamerica

    Am. J. Primatol.

    1190 / Gonzalez-Zamora et al.

  • indicate that temperature will increase and precipi-tation will decrease in the next 2050 years[Anderson et al., 2008]. These climatic alterationscould promote drastic changes in vegetation compo-sition and structure as well as changes in phenolo-gical cycles, with important implications for animalbehavior, population sizes, and distribution [Hannahet al., 2001; Parmesan, 2006]. Understanding howspecies cope with variations in climatic conditions,forest types, and conditions across their distribu-tional range is, therefore, critical for assessing theirability to cope with upcoming climatic alterationsand unprecedented anthropogenic disturbances.

    In this study, we gathered data from a numberof studies and analyzed them to determine how theactivity patterns of A. geoffroyi are influenced byambient temperature, rainfall, forest type (i.e.,seasonal and nonseasonal forests), and forest condi-tion (i.e., continuous and fragmented). According tosocioecological models, we hypothesized that spidermonkeys can adjust their activity patterns in order todeal with variation in climatic variables, forest types,and forest conditions [e.g., Dunbar et al., 2009;Korstjens et al., 2006; Wiederholt & Post, 2010].Because resting is an energy-saving activity, wepredicted resting would be positively related toambient temperature and negatively related torainfall because of the inferred direct relationshipbetween rainfall and food availability [e.g.,Bronikowski & Altmann, 1996; Deshmukh, 1984].Similarly, owing to differential food availability, weexpected spider monkeys to spend more time restingin seasonal forests than in nonseasonal forests andin forest fragments than in continuous forests.Traveling was expected to follow the oppositepattern of resting to reduce travel-related energyexpenditures. Finally, as spider monkeys oftenincrease the time devoted to consumption of fibrousand hard-to-digest plant items (e.g., leaves) whenavailability and/or diversity of fleshy ripe fruits isscarce [e.g., in forest fragments and during the dryseason: Chaves et al., 2011a,b; Gonzalez-Zamoraet al., 2009], we predicted time spent feeding wouldbe negatively related to rainfall and higher inseasonal and fragmented forests to compensate forfood scarcity under these situations.

    METHODS

    Literature Review

    We reviewed available studies (published arti-cles, book chapters, and unpublished theses) onactivity patterns of A. geoffroyi up to April 2011.Initially, we used internet databases (SCOPUS, ISIWeb of Science, PrimateLit, Google scholar) to locaterelevant published work. We then identified addi-tional studies cited within this primary literaturethat had not been published. We attempted to obtainas much of these unpublished data in the form of

    theses or gray literature through university librariesand by directly contacting the authors. Our data baseincluded 15 studies from 18 communities ofA. geoffroyi in four countriesMexico, El Salvador,Costa Rica, and Panamarepresenting more than18 years (221 months, 43,645 hr) of behavioralobservations (Table I). From all studies included inthe data base, we obtained the percent of time (orscans) primates spent on feeding, resting, andtraveling (see the sampling method of each study inTable I). We also calculated the percent of timedevoted to other activities (including social activities)(Table I). However, we did not include data from thiscategory in our analyses because different authorsused different definitions for this category. Whenavailable, we also obtained the size of the studycommunities (Table I), but this factor was notincluded in our analyses because, as expected, itwas not related to time resting (R25 0.02, P5 0.66),feeding (R25 0.00, P5 0.95), or traveling (R25 0.00,P5 0.90). All data were obtained with noninvasivemethods, complying with the American Society ofPrimatologists principles for the ethical study andtreatment of nonhuman primates.

    Climatic Variables, Forest Type, and ForestConditions

    Following Gentry [1982], we grouped foresttypes into seasonal (TMF: 2,0002,800 mm rain/yearand TDF: o2,000 mm rain/year) and nonseasonalforest (TWF: 42,800 mm rain/year). Based on thesize of the forest, we identified two forest conditions:continuous and fragmented. All forest fragmentswere very small (o31 ha), whereas continuousforests were greater than 1,500 ha [Gonzalez-Zamoraet al., 2009; Table I]. Additionally, we considered thefollowing three climatic variables that were availablefor the time period of each study, using data from themeteorological station located closest to each studysite: average monthly rainfall (PMEAN), maximumtemperature (TMAX), and minimum temperature(TMIN). We selected these climatic variables becauseboth rainfall and temperature are important compo-nents of primate activity patterns models [e.g.,Dunbar, 1992a; Dunbar et al., 2009; Korstjens &Dunbar, 2007; Korstjens et al., 2006; Lehmann et al.,2007]. We could not consider the effect of variation inrainfall and temperature across the year because wedid not find relevant data to calculate an appropriatemeasure for all the studies.

    Data Analyses

    Percent data were transformed to proportionsand proportion data were arcsine transformed. Then,we tested if they were normally distributed (ShapiroWilk test: P40.11 for all data). To test the effectof local climatic variables on activity patterns,we performed three separate multiple regression

    Am. J. Primatol.

    Activity Patterns of Spider Monkeys / 1191

  • TA

    BL

    EI.

    Desc

    rip

    tio

    no

    fS

    tud

    yS

    ites

    an

    dA

    cti

    vit

    yP

    att

    ern

    so

    fAtelesgeoffroyi

    inE

    ach

    Sa

    mp

    le

    Fore

    stty

    pe

    Sea

    son

    Rec

    ord

    ing

    Cli

    mati

    cvari

    ab

    les

    Fore

    stco

    nd

    itio

    nA

    ctiv

    ity

    patt

    ern

    (%)

    Com

    mu

    nit

    y

    Ref

    .S

    tud

    ysi

    tea

    (rain

    fall

    ,m

    m)b

    (stu

    dy

    len

    gth

    )cm

    eth

    od

    dP

    ME

    AN

    TM

    AX

    TM

    IN(s

    ize,

    ha)e

    Res

    tF

    eed

    Tra

    vel

    Oth

    ersi

    ze

    1L

    T,

    Mex

    ico

    TW

    F(4

    4,0

    00

    )D

    &W

    (12

    ,)

    FA

    S2

    91

    .12

    7.0

    22

    .1F

    F(2

    5)

    20

    .23

    6.7

    41

    .12

    2

    LT

    ,M

    exic

    oT

    WF

    (44

    ,00

    0)

    D&

    W(1

    1,

    66

    0)

    FA

    S4

    84

    .22

    8.0

    22

    .5F

    F(2

    5)

    16

    .06

    7.0

    17

    .00

    3

    LT

    ,M

    exic

    oT

    WF

    (44

    ,00

    0)

    D&

    W(1

    1,

    )

    FA

    S3

    96

    .12

    8.0

    21

    .3F

    F(2

    5)

    20

    .75

    42

    5.3

    01

    94

    LT

    ,M

    exic

    oT

    WF

    (44

    ,00

    0)

    Dry

    (6,

    16

    4)

    FA

    S3

    66

    .82

    5.6

    20

    .2F

    F(2

    5)

    49

    .04

    0.0

    11

    .00

    25

    5L

    T,

    Mex

    ico

    TW

    F(4

    4,0

    00

    )D

    ry(6

    ,9

    6)

    FA

    S3

    05

    .72

    9.3

    22

    .4F

    F(2

    5)

    34

    .03

    8.0

    27

    .01

    30

    6A

    MA

    ,M

    exic

    oT

    WF

    (2,8

    80

    )D

    &W

    (15

    ,1

    88

    )F

    AS

    27

    7.6

    35

    .71

    2.9

    CF

    (33

    1,2

    00

    )2

    4.3

    49

    .41

    3.9

    12

    .42

    16

    BM

    A,

    Mex

    ico

    TW

    F(2

    ,88

    0)

    D&

    W(1

    5,

    14

    6)

    FA

    S2

    77

    .63

    5.7

    12

    .9C

    F(3

    31

    ,20

    0)

    43

    .12

    3.2

    7.8

    25

    .91

    96

    CM

    A,

    Mex

    ico

    TW

    F(2

    ,88

    0)

    D&

    W(1

    5,

    16

    2)

    FA

    S2

    77

    .63

    5.7

    12

    .9C

    F(3

    31

    ,20

    0)

    50

    .93

    4.7

    5.4

    9.0

    23

    6D

    RA

    ,M

    exic

    oT

    WF

    (2,8

    80

    )D

    &W

    (15

    ,1

    26

    )F

    AS

    27

    7.6

    35

    .71

    2.9

    CF

    (1,4

    50

    )3

    5.7

    38

    .16

    .91

    9.3

    26

    6F

    1Z

    PO

    ,M

    exic

    oT

    WF

    (2,8

    80

    )D

    &W

    (15

    ,2

    36

    )F

    AS

    27

    7.6

    35

    .71

    2.9

    FF

    (31

    )2

    8.3

    26

    0.2

    4.5

    6.9

    82

    86

    F2

    ZP

    O,

    Mex

    ico

    TW

    F(2

    ,88

    0)

    D&

    W(1

    5,

    15

    2)

    FA

    S2

    77

    .63

    5.7

    12

    .9F

    F(1

    4)

    26

    .76

    4.8

    4.9

    3.6

    27

    7N

    OR

    ,E

    lS

    alv

    ad

    or

    TW

    F(4

    3,0

    00

    )W

    et(4

    ,1

    80

    )F

    AS

    &IS

    S2

    84

    .03

    2.5

    22

    .6C

    F(4

    30

    )f5

    2.0

    16

    .03

    2.0

    02

    88

    BC

    I,P

    an

    am

    aT

    MF

    (2,6

    00

    )yD

    &W

    (12

    ,)

    FA

    S

    CF

    (41

    ,50

    0)

    61

    .01

    1.0

    28

    .00

    9

    BC

    I,P

    an

    am

    aT

    MF

    (2,6

    00

    )yD

    &W

    (12

    ,)

    FA

    S&

    ISS

    22

    3.8

    31

    .12

    3.2

    CF

    (41

    ,50

    0)

    50

    .12

    5.6

    18

    .72

    1

    0B

    CI,

    Pan

    am

    aT

    MF

    (2,6

    00

    )yD

    &W

    (13

    ,1

    ,20

    0)

    ISS

    17

    8.3

    31

    .32

    4.2

    CF

    (41

    ,50

    0)

    51

    .73

    9.3

    31

    .13

    4.2

    21

    11

    EP

    L,

    Mex

    ico

    TD

    F(1

    ,36

    5)y

    D&

    W(4

    7,

    )

    ISS

    11

    3.0

    33

    .51

    4.1

    CF

    (ca.

    3,7

    00

    )4

    1.0

    36

    .02

    3.0

    01

    61

    1W

    PL

    ,M

    exic

    oT

    DF

    (1,3

    65

    )yD

    &W

    (47

    ,)

    ISS

    11

    3.0

    33

    .51

    4.1

    CF

    (ca.

    3,7

    00

    )4

    5.0

    35

    .02

    0.0

    04

    11

    2S

    R,

    Cost

    aR

    ica

    TD

    F(1

    ,53

    1)y

    D&

    W(3

    3,

    33

    5)

    FA

    S9

    2.4

    31

    .92

    2.1

    CF

    (41

    0,8

    00

    )2

    4.1

    33

    .53

    2.6

    9.8

    42

    13

    HL

    I,C

    ost

    aR

    ica

    TM

    F(o

    2,5

    00

    )D

    &W

    (11

    ,)

    ISS

    F

    F(1

    70

    )3

    12

    54

    13

    24

    14

    AZ

    BF

    E,

    Cost

    aR

    ica

    TW

    F(4

    4,0

    00

    )D

    &W

    (16

    ,)

    FA

    S2

    16

    .43

    2.8

    18

    .5C

    F(7

    00

    )4

    12

    41

    22

    3

    14

    BZ

    BF

    E,

    Cost

    aR

    ica

    TW

    F(4

    4,0

    00

    )D

    &W

    (16

    ,)

    FA

    S2

    16

    .43

    2.8

    18

    .5F

    F(3

    00

    )2

    14

    81

    61

    5

    15

    CN

    P,

    Cost

    aR

    ica

    TW

    F(4

    4,0

    00

    )D

    &W

    (12

    ,)

    FA

    S4

    02

    .71

    CF

    (41

    78

    8)

    38

    .54

    51

    5.8

    0.7

    6

    Ref

    eren

    ces:

    1.

    Ben

    tez

    -Rod

    rgu

    ez[1

    98

    9];

    2.

    Jim

    enez

    -Hu

    erta

    [19

    92

    ];3

    .G

    arc

    a-O

    rdu

    na

    [20

    02

    ];4

    .G

    on

    zale

    z-Z

    am

    ora

    [20

    03

    ];5

    .S

    an

    chez

    -Lop

    ez[2

    00

    8];

    6.

    Ch

    aves

    etal.

    [20

    11

    b];

    7.

    Arg

    uet

    a-R

    ivas

    an

    dR

    iver

    a-

    Her

    nan

    dez

    [20

    04

    ];8

    .R

    ich

    ard

    [19

    70

    ];9

    .M

    ilto

    n[1

    99

    3];

    10

    .C

    am

    pb

    ell

    [20

    00

    ];1

    1.

    Ram

    os-

    Fer

    nan

    dez

    &A

    yala

    -Oro

    zco

    [20

    03

    ];1

    2.

    Ch

    ap

    man

    etal.

    [19

    89

    ];1

    3.

    McD

    an

    iel

    [19

    94

    ];1

    4.

    Lin

    dsh

    ield

    [20

    06

    ];1

    5.W

    egh

    ors

    t[2

    00

    7].

    Ref

    eren

    ces

    15

    stu

    die

    dth

    esa

    me

    com

    mu

    nit

    yb

    ut

    ind

    iffe

    ren

    tti

    me

    per

    iod

    s;R

    efer

    ence

    6st

    ud

    ied

    thre

    ed

    iffe

    ren

    tgro

    up

    sin

    the

    Mon

    tes

    Azu

    les

    Bio

    sph

    ere

    Res

    erve

    (A5

    Sta

    tion

    site

    ,B5

    Las

    Cei

    bas

    site

    ,C5

    7si

    te,

    an

    dD5

    Ref

    orm

    aA

    gra

    ria)

    an

    din

    two

    small

    fragm

    ents

    (F15

    Batr

    izal

    pri

    vate

    rese

    rve

    an

    dF

    25

    Los

    Nid

    os

    pri

    vate

    rese

    rve)

    ;R

    efer

    ence

    11

    stu

    die

    dtw

    od

    iffe

    ren

    tgro

    up

    s(E5

    East

    an

    dW5

    Wes

    t).

    Ref

    eren

    ce1

    4(A5

    un

    dis

    turb

    edfo

    rest

    ,B5

    dis

    turb

    edfo

    rest

    ).aS

    tud

    ysi

    tes:

    LT

    ,L

    os

    Tu

    xtl

    as;

    MA

    ,M

    on

    tes

    Azu

    les;

    RA

    ,R

    eform

    aA

    gra

    ria;

    ZP

    O,

    Zam

    ora

    Pic

    od

    eO

    ro;

    NO

    R,

    Norm

    an

    da

    ,B

    CI,

    Barr

    oC

    olo

    rad

    oIs

    lan

    d;

    PL

    ,P

    un

    taL

    agu

    na;

    SR

    ,S

    an

    taR

    osa

    ;H

    LI,

    Haci

    end

    aL

    os

    Inoce

    nte

    s;Z

    BF

    E,

    La

    Zota

    Bio

    logic

    al

    Fie

    dS

    tati

    on

    .bF

    ore

    stty

    pe:

    TW

    F,

    trop

    ical

    wet

    fore

    st;

    TM

    F,

    trop

    ical

    mois

    tfo

    rest

    ;T

    DF

    ,tr

    op

    ical

    dry

    fore

    st.

    Sam

    ple

    sw

    ith

    (y)

    rep

    rese

    nt

    seaso

    nal

    fore

    sts,

    wh

    erea

    sth

    ose

    wit

    hou

    t(y

    )re

    pre

    sen

    tn

    on

    seaso

    nal

    fore

    sts.

    cS

    easo

    n:

    D&

    W,

    dry

    an

    dw

    et;

    Stu

    dy

    len

    gth

    :m

    on

    ths

    an

    dh

    ou

    rs.

    Sam

    ple

    sw

    ith

    ast

    eris

    k(

    )w

    ere

    not

    incl

    ud

    edin

    the

    an

    aly

    ses.

    dR

    ecord

    ing

    met

    hod

    :F

    AS

    ,C

    on

    tin

    uou

    sF

    oca

    lA

    nim

    al

    Sam

    pli

    ng;

    ISS

    ,In

    stan

    tan

    eou

    sS

    can

    Sam

    pli

    ng.

    eF

    ore

    stco

    nd

    itio

    n:

    FF

    ,fo

    rest

    fragm

    ent;

    CF

    ,co

    nti

    nu

    ou

    sfo

    rest

    .f T

    his

    was

    con

    sid

    ered

    con

    tin

    uou

    sb

    ecau

    seth

    eveg

    etati

    on

    issi

    mil

    ar

    toth

    eori

    gin

    al

    con

    tin

    uou

    sfo

    rest

    ,it

    isth

    ela

    rges

    tfr

    agm

    ent

    inth

    ere

    gio

    n,

    an

    dh

    as

    bee

    np

    rote

    cted

    for

    10

    yea

    rs[s

    eeA

    rgu

    eta-R

    ivas

    &R

    iver

    a-

    Her

    nan

    dez

    ,2

    00

    4].

    ()

    data

    un

    avail

    ab

    le.

    Am. J. Primatol.

    1192 / Gonzalez-Zamora et al.

  • analyses. In each analysis the dependent variable wasthe activity (resting, feeding, or traveling) and theindependent variables were PMEAN, TMAX, and TMIN.Variance inflation factor (VIF) values above fourindicate possible multicollinearity between indepen-dent variables [Chatterjee et al., 2000]. In our models,VIF never reached this value. In addition to thecomplete models (i.e., including all three climaticvariables), we used backward stepwise regressionanalyses and the Akaikes Information Criterion toselect the best model for each dependent variable[Motulsky & Christopoulos, 2003].

    We used students t-tests to assess possibleeffects of forest type and forest condition on eachactivity. Considering continuous forests, we testeddifferences among seasonal (N5 7 data points) andnonseasonal forests (N5 12). To test the effect offorest condition, we compared continuous forest(N5 6) and fragments (N5 6) of TWF, the onlyforest type for which we had data for forestfragments (Table I). We considered three samplesfrom the same group in the same fragment of LosTuxtlas (studies 1, 2, and 3 in Table I) as indepen-dent, because they were separated in time between3 and 12 years, and both community sizes andclimatic conditions were notably different across thethree samples (see Table I). Although we used thesame independent variables for multiple tests, we didnot apply Bonferroni corrections to our resultsbecause we only made three different tests (one peractivity), so we did not increase so much theprobability of the statistical error type I (rejects atrue Ho) by chance. Also, by applying Bonferronicorrections, we could increase the probability of thestatistical error type II (do not reject Ho when isfalse) [see Nakagawa, 2004].

    Only data covering the entire year (i.e., annualsamples) were used in the statistical analyses. Whendata were available only for the dry or wet season(N5 3; see Table I), the study was only used forproviding some general descriptive statistics. Alltests were performed with JMP (version 7.0, SASInstitute Inc.) and STATISTICA (version 5.5, Stat-Soft) software.

    RESULTS

    Across sites and groups, spider monkeys spentmost of their time feeding (38.4714.0%, mean7SD)and resting (36.6712.8%) and less time traveling(19.8711.3%) and in other activities (7.6710.0%)(N5 22 data points from different groups or from thesame group but during different sampling periods;Table I). However, there was large variation in timespent in each activity among groups and sites(Table I). Particularly, feeding time ranged from11% in a continuous TMF (BCI, Panama) to 67% in aTWF fragment (Los Tuxtlas, Mexico); resting rangedfrom 16% (Los Tuxtlas, Mexico) to 61% (BCI,

    Panama); and traveling ranged from 4.5 to 41.1%in two TWF fragments in Mexico (Table I).

    The best regression model indicated that restingwas mainly affected by PMEAN; when rainfalldecreased across studies monkeys spent more timeresting (Table II). Feeding time followed the oppositepattern: the best model indicated that feeding waspositively associated with PMEAN (Table II). Finally,traveling time was negatively related to both PMEANand TMAX (Table II). Importantly, the amount ofvariance explained by these climatic factors (R2)differed greatly among the models: it was only 20%for resting and 26% for feeding, but 86% for traveling(Table II).

    The percentage of time spent resting differedbetween forest types, being significantly higher inseasonal forests than in nonseasonal forests(t52.30, df5 17, P5 0.03; Fig. 1A). Similarly,traveling time was significantly higher in seasonalforests than in nonseasonal forests (t53.10,df5 17, P5 0.006; Fig. 1A). Feeding time followedthe opposite pattern, being significantly higher innonseasonal forests than in seasonal forests (t5 2.57,df5 17, P5 0.02; Fig. 1A).

    Activity patterns differed between continuousforest and forest fragments (Fig. 1B). Although thepercent of traveling time did not differ significantlybetween forest conditions (t5 1.15, df5 10,P5 0.28), monkeys spent more time feeding(t5 3.00, df5 10, P5 0.01) and less time resting(t54.19, df5 10, P5 0.002) in forest fragmentsthan in continuous forest (Fig. 1B).

    DISCUSSION

    Overall, our results demonstrate that there is alarge variation in time spent in each main activityacross communities of spider monkeys (A. geoffroyi)throughout Mesoamerica, and that a large propor-tion of this variation can be explained by annualrainfall, ambient temperature, forest types (seasonaland nonseasonal), and conditions (continuous andfragmented). These results are consistent withrecent socioecological studies on primates, andsupport the idea that temperature, rainfall, anddegree of seasonality are important climatic vari-ables driving the activity patterns of Neotropicalprimates [Chaves et al., 2011b; Dunbar et al., 2009;Korstjens et al., 2006; Wiederholt & Post, 2010]and Paleotropical primates [Bronikowski &Altmann, 1996; Dunbar, 1992b; Dunbar et al., 2009].Furthermore, consistent with Chaves et al. [2011b],our findings also indicate that spider monkeys canchange their activity patterns in forest fragments.

    In agreement with our predictions, we foundthat, in habitats with lower rainfall, spider monkeysspend more time resting and that resting time washigher in seasonal forests. Similarly, as predicted,traveling time was strongly and negatively related to

    Am. J. Primatol.

    Activity Patterns of Spider Monkeys / 1193

  • ambient temperature. Food availability for primates(including spider monkeys) increases with rainfall[Di Fiore et al., 2008; Kaplin et al., 1998; Stevenson,2001; Stevenson et al., 2000], and those living inseasonal forests are exposed to more stressfulconditions [e.g., temporal food scarcity, drought,high ambient temperature; Murphy & Lugo, 1995;Stoner & Timm, 2004]. Therefore, it is reasonable toexpect that, just like we found, monkeys devote moretime resting in seasonal forest and where rainfall islower. This could be an energy-saving strategy tocope with resource limitations during the driestperiods and/or in the driest forests [Chaves et al.,2011b]. In addition, as traveling and feeding repre-sent energetically costly activities [Dunbar, 1988,1992a; Dunn et al., 2009; Korstjens et al., 2006],reducing travel time when ambient temperature ishigher could allow primates to minimize energy costsand feeding effort owing to overheating [e.g., Dunbaret al., 2009].

    Interestingly, feeding time was positively relatedto rainfall but not significantly to temperature,suggesting that this activity could be more con-strained by differences in food availability. Similarly,feeding time was significantly higher in nonseasonalforests (TWF). Spider monkeys have been reportedto spend more time feeding during the rainy season[Chaves et al., 2011b; Felton et al., 2009], possiblybecause feeding is an energetically costly activitythat needs to be minimized during the dry season

    TABLE II. Influence of Climatic Factors on Activity Patterns of Spider Monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) Found inMesoamerica (We Indicate Both the Complete Models and the Most Parsimonious (i.e. Best Modelsa)).

    Activity R2adj AIC Factorsb Parameterc Fratio P

    Complete modelsResting 0.20 69.6 Intercept 0.328 0.38 0.713

    PMEAN 0.0004 1.24 0.239TMAX 0.025 1.28 0.224TMIN 0.010 0.84 0.416

    Feeding 0.15 67.6 Intercept 0.691 0.75 0.469PMEAN 0.001 1.99 0.070TMAX 0.004 0.17 0.871TMIN 0.005 0.37 0.717

    Traveling 0.85 91.2 Intercept 2.114 4.77 0.001PMEAN 0.001 5.01 o0.001TMAX 0.046 4.59 o0.001TMIN 0.002 0.31 0.763

    Best modelsResting 0.20 71.1 Intercept 0.720 10.3 o0.001

    PMEAN 0.001 4.70 0.048Feeding 0.26 71.3 Intercept 0.490 7.05 0.010

    PMEAN 0.001 6.13 0.027Traveling 0.86 93.1 Intercept 1.988 12.2 o0.001

    PMEAN 0.001 30.0 o0.001TMAX 0.043 89.3 o0.001

    aThe best models were selected through backward stepwise regression analyses. We indicated the adjusted R2 and Akaikes Information Criterion for eachregression model.bClimatic factors: PMEAN, average monthly rainfall; TMAX, maximum temperature; TMIN, minimum temperature.cThe sign of each parameter indicates the relationship (positive or negative) between each continuous factor and the response variable.

    * *

    **

    0

    10

    20

    30

    40

    50

    60

    Act

    ivity

    pa

    ttern

    (%

    )

    Non-seasonal Seasonal

    **

    *

    0

    10

    20

    30

    40

    50

    60

    70

    Rest Feed Travel

    Act

    ivity

    pa

    ttern

    (%

    )

    Continuous forests Forest fragments

    A

    B

    Rest Feed Travel

    Fig. 1. Activity patterns (average percentage of time7SE) ofAteles geoffroyi according to forest type (seasonal and nonseaso-nal) (A) and forest condition (continuous and fragmented) (B).Significant differences are indicated with asterisks (Po0.05;Po0.01).

    Am. J. Primatol.

    1194 / Gonzalez-Zamora et al.

  • [Dunbar et al., 2009; Korstjens et al., 2010]. Feltonet al. [2009] and Chaves et al. [2011b] also interpretgreater feeding time in the rainy season as a strategyfor spider monkeys to take advantage of peakseasonal foods allowing them to ingest surplusenergy and store it as fat in preparation for theimpending period of food scarcity. Other primatespecies accumulate fat during peaks of fruit abun-dance [Pongo pygmaeus: Knott, 1998; Lemur cattaand Eulemur sp.: Simmen et al., 2010; Lagothrixlagotricha cana: Peres, 1994; Ateles paniscus: Milton,1998; A. chamek: Wallace, 2005], which is a logicalstrategy for animals experiencing fluctuating foodsupply [Felton et al., 2009]. Further information onnutritional ecology and energetic strategies ofA. geoffroyi is needed to test the fat accumulationhypothesis.

    In contrast with our prediction, traveling timewas negatively related to rainfall. We expected apositive relationship between the two variables,as low rainfall is usually associated with lowfruit availability [Bronikowski & Altmann, 1996;Deshmukh, 1984]. Under these circumstances, areduction in traveling could be an energy-savingstrategy. The negative relation is likely owing to thefact that, when more fruit sources are available,the monkeys spend less time traveling because thedistance among food patches decreases. This idea issupported by Chaves et al. [2011b], as they also findthat traveling time tended to be higher during thedry season. However, Asensio et al. [2009] do not findthat traveling time changed with season, and arguedthat monkeys adjust their subgroup sizes (i.e.,smaller subgroups are found during the dry season)to avoid travel costs associated with larger subgroups.Nevertheless, because most of the studies analyzed inour study did not provide data on average subgroupsizes, we cannot determine the relative influence ofsubgroup size on activity patterns.

    In TWF (i.e., the only forest type for which wehad data for forest fragments), forest conditionaffected the activity patterns of spider monkeys.Specifically, monkeys spent more time feeding andless time resting in forest fragments than incontinuous forest, and hence the feeding effort[sensu Cavigelli, 1999] of monkeys was higher infragments. Similar differences in activity patternshave been reported for Chiropotes satanas in Brazil[Boyle & Smith, 2010; Boyle et al., 2009] andAlouatta palliata in Mexico [Dunn et al., 2009].Dunn et al. [2009] relate the variations in activitypatterns (and feeding effort) to the differences in dietbetween groups: leaf consumption is higher in thegroup inhabiting the smallest fragment, and thisgroup visits more food sources resulting in moretraveling, more feeding, and less resting. They arguethat this could be explained by the lower amount ofenergy these primates probably gain per food patch,as well as the need to diversify food sources when

    consuming leaves (i.e., to obtain the best complementof nutrients and avoid an overload of particulartoxins or digestibility-reducing compounds). Ourfindings are consistent with this idea, as we foundin a previous study (using a data base similar tothat in this article) that spider monkeys have amore folivorous diet in fragments [Gonzalez-Zamoraet al., 2009], which could force them to diversify theirdiet [see Chaves et al., 2011a; Felton et al., 2009;Milton, 1981, 1998] and spend more time feeding andless time resting. However, it is important to gatherdata on activity patterns in fragmented seasonalforests (TMF and TDF), to assess whether it ispossible to generalize this finding to other foresttypes.

    Conclusions and Recommendations forConservation

    Our findings indicate that spider monkeys inMesoamerica can adjust their activity patterns inorder to cope with changes in temperature, rainfall,forest type (seasonal and nonseasonal), and condition(continuous and fragmented). However, it is stillunclear whether these behavioral adjustments willinsure their health and survival in the long term,especially if we consider the current high rates ofdeforestation and the impact that climate change willhave on Mesoamerican ecosystems in the nextfew decades [Anderson et al., 2008; FAO, 2011;Wiederholt & Post, 2010]. Studies evaluating therelationships between changes in activity patternsassociated with climatic variables, forest type andcondition, and the demographic consequences (e.g.,birth and death rates) in spider monkeys populationsare urgent for their conservation.

    Based on our results, if ambient temperaturesincrease and annual rainfall decrease approximatingthe figures predicted by the climate change modelsfor Mesoamerica [Anderson et al., 2008], spidermonkeys will probably increase the enforced restingtime [i.e., resting needed for digestive and/or ther-moregulatory purposes; Korstjens et al., 2010] limit-ing the time monkeys can devote to other criticalactivities, such as feeding, traveling, and socialbehavior. This could reduce their population sizes,limit the distributional range of the species, and eventhreaten their long-term survival in the comingdecades.

    It is alarming that A. geoffroyi has been recentlylisted as Endangered by the IUCN, because habitatloss and fragmentation across its range has beensevere and the species has been estimated to havedeclined by as much as 50% over the past 45 years[Cuaron et al., 2008]. In addition, it has beensuggested that A. geoffroyi could be one of the firstMesoamerican primate species to become locallyextinct in human-modified landscapes [Garberet al., 2006]. The remaining populations are isolated

    Am. J. Primatol.

    Activity Patterns of Spider Monkeys / 1195

  • in few reserves and different-sized forest fragments[Cuaron et al., 2008]. Our results demonstrate thatfeeding effort is higher in forest fragments than incontinuous forests, and studies on other primatesindicate that an increase in feeding effort maynegatively affect fitness by increasing stress levels[Papio anubis: Sapolsky, 1986; L. catta: Cavigelli,1999; A. palliata: Jake Dunn, unpublished data].Thus, our results support the idea that monkeyscould experience higher stress levels in fragments, asit has been recently demonstrated by Rangel-Negrnet al. [2009] by analyzing fecal cortisol levels ofspider monkeys living in conserved forests, fragmen-ted forests, and captive conditions in the YucatanPeninsula, Mexico.

    Considering the potential negative effects thatclimate change would have for spider monkeys,particularly in fragmented landscapes in whichinterfragment movements is highly limited [Arroyo-Rodrguez & Mandujano, 2009], conservation effortsshould be focused on the increase of landscapeconnectivity. Landscape corridors are required tofacilitate species and population movements betweenhabitats in response to climate change [Ewers &Didham, 2006]. In this sense, the MesoamericanBiological Corridora system of land planningcoordinated by the governments of the countrieslocated in the Mesoamerican regionwould connectthe largest reserves of Mesoamerica and could have acrucial positive impact on the conservationof primates and other plant and animal species[Anderson & Jenkins, 2006; Miller et al., 2001].Furthermore, increasing fragment size is critical toreduce primates foraging effort and maintain larger(and viable) primate populations.

    It is important to recognize that our results arelimited by variation in data collection length,methodology, and interobserver experience acrossstudies. To address this shortcoming, we recommendthat, although research designs need to be appro-priate to the research questions and characteristicsof the study population and field site, we need tostandardize methodologies as much as possible. Morecoordination among scientists working on the samespecies and/or similar topics is needed in order tobuild the data bases required to address importantissues. These data bases are fundamental in allowingscientists carrying out more accurate comparisonsamong studies in order to reach critical general-izations and build theories that can be put to use forprimate conservation.

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    This study satisfied the legal requirements ofMexican Secretary of Environment and NaturalResources (SEMARNAT) and meets all the Mexicananimal care policies and the Principles of theAmerican Society of Primatologists for the Ethical

    Treatment of Nonhuman Primates. This work wassupported by grants from the Consejo Nacional deCiencia y Tecnologa (CONACYT), Mexico (2005-51043, 2006-56799). We thank ML Chavarra and RBlanco from Santa Rosa National Park, the person-nel of the Smithsonian Tropical Research InstituteEnvironmental Science Program, Biological Stationof Los Tuxtlas, Biology Institute of the UniversidadNacional Autonoma de Mexico, Coba ClimatologicStation of the Comision Nacional del AguaMexico (CNA, Mexico), and the Lacandona ClimaticStation of the Comision Federal de Electricidad(CFE, Mexico) for providing climatic information.We thank C.J. Campbell, P.S. McDaniel,S.M. Lindshield, J.A. Weghorst, F. Garca,J. Jimenez, N.A. Argueta, and G.M. Rivera forallowing us to use some data of their unpublishedThesis. We also thank A. Di Fiore and two anon-ymous referees for useful comments. SSL thanks thetechnical support provided by the Instituto deNeuroetologa (UV) and Universitat de Barcelona.VAR thanks the financial support provided by theDireccion General de Asuntos del Personal Acade-mico (DGAPA-UNAM). VAR, KES, and OCBacknowledge the technical assistance provided byJ.M. Lobato-Garca, H. Ferreira, and A. Valencia-Garca. The Instituto de Ecologa A.C and Centrode Investigaciones en Ecosistemas (UniversidadNacional Autonoma de Mexico) provided logisticalsupport.

    FA is grateful to the financial support from theNorth of England Zoological Society. The Institutode Ecologa A.C and Centro de Investigaciones enEcosistemas (Universidad Nacional Autonoma deMexico) provided logistical support.

    REFERENCES

    Anderson AB, Jenkins CN. 2006. Applying natures design:corridors as a strategy for biodiversity conservation. New York:Columbia University Press.

    Anderson ER, Cherrogton EA, Flores AI, Perez JB, Carrillo R,Sempris E. 2008. Potential impacts of climate change onbiodiversity in Central America, Mexico, and the DominicanRepublic. Panama City: CATHALAC/USAID.

    Argueta-Rivas NA, Rivera-Hernandez GM. 2004. Uso dehabitats del mono arana (Ateles geoffroyi) en el AreaNatural Protegida Normanda, Usultan, El Salvador, BSThesis, Universidad de El Salvador, El Salvador.

    Arroyo-Rodrguez V, Mandujano S. 2006. Forest fragmenta-tion modifies habitat quality for Alouatta palliata. Interna-tional Journal of Primatology 27:10791096.

    Arroyo-Rodrguez V, Mandujano S. 2009. Conceptualizationand measurement of rainforest fragmentation from theprimates perspective. International Journal of Primatology30:497514.

    Asensio N, Korstjens AH, Aureli F. 2009. Fissioning minimizesranging costs in spider monkeys: a multiple-level approach.Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63:649659.

    Aureli F, Schaffner CM, Boesch C, Bearder SK, Call J,Chapman CA, Connor R, Di Fiore A, Dunbar RIM,Henzi PS, Holekamp K, Korstjens AH, Layton R, Lee P,Lehmann J, Manson JH, Ramos-Fernandez G, Strier KB,

    Am. J. Primatol.

    1196 / Gonzalez-Zamora et al.

  • van Schaik CP. 2008. Fissionfusion dynamics: newresearch frameworks. Current Anthropology 49:627654.

    Bentez-Rodrguez J. 1989. Perfil de actividades del monoarana mexicano, Ateles geoffroyi vellerosus (Kellog yGoldman, 1944) en la Sierra de Santa Marta (Veracruz,Mexico), B.Sc. Thesis, Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa.

    Boyle SA, Smith AT. 2010. Behavioral modificationsin northern bearded saki monkeys (Chiropotes satanaschiropotes) in forest fragments of central Amazonia.Primates 51:4351.

    Boyle SA, Lourenco WC, da Silva LR, Smith AT. 2009. Traveland spatial patterns change when Chiropotes satanaschiropotes inhabit forest fragments. International Journalof Primatology 30:515531.

    Bronikowski AM, Altmann J. 1996. Foraging in a variableenvironment, weather patterns and the behavioral ecologyof baboons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 39:1125.

    Bunker DE, Carson WP. 2005. Drought stress and tropicalforest woody seedlings: effect on community structure andcomposition. Journal of Ecology 93:794806.

    Campbell CJ. 2000. The reproductive biology of black-handedspider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi): integrating behaviorand endocrinology, PhD Thesis, University of California,Berkeley.

    Caraco T. 1979. Time budgeting and group-size. TheoryEcology 60:611617.

    Cavigelli SA. 1999. Behavioural patterns associated with faecalcortisol levels in free ranging female ring-tailed lemurs,Lemur catta. Animal Behaviour 57:935944.

    Chapman CA. 1987a. Flexibility in diets of three species ofCosta Rican primates. Folia Primatologica 49:90105.

    Chapman CA. 1987b. Patch use and patch depletion by thespider and howling monkeys of Santa Rosa National Park,Costa Rica. Behaviour 105:99116.

    Chapman CA. 1990. Ecological constraints on group size inthree species of Neotropical primates. Folia Primatologica55:19.

    Chapman CA, Chapman L, McLaughlin RL. 1989. Multiplecentral place foraging by spider monkeys: travel conse-quences of using many sleeping sites. Oecologia 79:506511.

    Chapman CA, Chapman LJ, Wrangham R, Hunt K, Gebo D,Gardner L. 1992. Estimators of fruit abundance of tropicaltrees. Biotropica 24:527531.

    Chapman CA, Balcomb SR, Gillespie T, Skorupa J,Struhsaker TT. 2000. Long-term effects of logging onAfrican primate communities: a 28 year comparison fromKibale National Park, Uganda. Conservation Biology14:207217.

    Chatterjee S, Hadi AS, Price B. 2000. Regression analysis byexample. New York: Wiley.

    Chaves OM, Stoner KE, Arroyo-Rodrguez V. 2011a. Differ-ences in diet between spider monkey groups living in forestfragments and continuous forest in Lacandona, Mexico.Biotropica (DOI: 10.1111/j.1744-7429.2011.00766.x).

    Chaves OM, Stoner KE, Arroyo-Rodriguez V. 2011b. Seasonaldifferences in activity patterns of spider monkeys living incontinuous and fragmented forests in Southern Mexico.International Journal of Primatology 32:960973.

    Cuaron AD, Morales A, Shedden A, Rodriguez-Luna E, deGrammont PC. 2008. Ateles geoffroyi. In: IUCN 2010. IUCNRed List of Threatened Species. owww.iucnredlist.org4.Downloaded on April 29, 2011.

    Di Fiore A, Link A, Dew JL. 2008. Diets of wild spidermonkeys. In: Campbell CJ, editor. Spider monkeys: beha-vior, ecology and evolution of the genus Ateles. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press. p 81137.

    Deshmukh IK. 1984. A common relationship between pre-cipitation and grassland peak biomass for east and southernAfrica. African Journal of Ecology 22:181186.

    Doran D. 1997. Influence of seasonality on activity patterns,feeding behavior, ranging, and grouping patterns in Tai

    chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology18:183206.

    Dunbar RIM. 1988. Primate social systems. Ithaca: CornellUniversity Press.

    Dunbar RIM. 1992a. Time: a hidden constraint on thebehavioural ecology of baboons. Behavioral Ecology andSociobiology 31:3549.

    Dunbar RIM. 1992b. A model of the gelada socioecologicalsystem. Primates 33:6983.

    Dunbar RIM. 1998. Impact of global warming on thedistribution and survival of the gelada baboon: a modellingapproach. Global Change Biology 4:293304.

    Dunbar RIM, Korstjens AH, Lehmann J. 2009. Time as anecological constraint. Biological Reviews 84:413429.

    Dunn JC, Cristobal-Azkarate C, Vea J. 2009. Differences indiet and activity pattern between two groups of Alouattapalliata associated with the availability of big trees and fruitof top food taxa. American Journal of Primatology71:654662.

    Ewers RM, Didham RK. 2006. Confounding factors in thedetection of species responses to habitat fragmentation.Biological Reviews 81:117142.

    FAO. 2011. State of the worlds forest 2011. Rome: Food andAgriculture Organization of the United Nations.

    Felton AM, Felton A, Wood JF, Foley WJ, Raubenheimer D,Wallis IR, Lindenmayer DB. 2009. Nutritional ecology ofAteles chamek in lowland Bolivia: how macronutrientbalancing influences food choices. International Journal ofPrimatology 30:675696.

    Garber PA, Estrada A, Pavelka MSM. 2006. New perspectivein the study of Mesoamerican Primates: concludingcomments and conservation priorities. In: Estrada A,Garber PA, Pavelka MSM, Luecke M, editors. Newperspective in the study of Mesoamerican primates: dis-tribution, ecology, behaviour and conservation. New York:Springer. p 489511.

    Garca-Orduna F. 2002. Comparacion de las estrategias deforrajeo de Ateles geoffroyi vellerosus y Alouatta palliataMexicana, en un fragmento de selva de la Sierra de SantaMarta, Veracruz, M.Sc. Thesis, Universidad Veracruzana,Xalapa.

    Gentry AH. 1982. Patterns of Neotropical plant speciesdiversity. Evolution Biology 15:185.

    Gonzalez-Zamora A. 2003. Patrones de utilizacion de frag-mentos de selva por el mono arana (Ateles geoffroyivellerosus) en el sur de Veracruz, M.Sc. Thesis, Institutode Ecologa, A.C., Xalapa.

    Gonzalez-Zamora A, Arroyo-Rodrguez V, Chaves OM,Sanchez-Lopez S, Stoner KE, Riba-Hernandez P. 2009. Dietof spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) in Mesoamerica: currentknowledge and future directions. American Journal ofPrimatology 71:820.

    Hannah L, Midgley GG, Lovejoy T, Bond WJ, Bush M,Lovett JC, Scott D, Woodwards FI. 2001. Conservation ofbiodiversity in a changing climate. Conservation Biology16:264268.

    Isbell LA, Pruetz JD, Young TP. 1998. Movements of vervets(Cercopithecus aethiops) and patas monkeys (Erythrocebuspatas) as estimators of food resource size, density, anddistribution. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 42:123133.

    Iwamoto T, Dunbar RIM. 1983. Thermoregulation, habitatquality and the behavioural ecology in gelada baboons.Journal of Animal Ecology 53:357366.

    Janson CH, Goldsmith. 1995. Predicting group size inprimates: foraging costs and predation risks. BehavioralEcology 6:326336.

    Jimenez-Huerta J. 1992. Distribucion y abundancia delrecurso alimenticio en un fragmento de selva alta perenni-folia y su uso por Ateles y Alouatta en el Ejido Magallanes(Municipio de Soteapan, Veracruz), B.Sc. Thesis, Universi-dad Veracruzana, Xalapa.

    Am. J. Primatol.

    Activity Patterns of Spider Monkeys / 1197

  • Kaplin BA, Munyaligoga V, Moermond TC. 1998. Theinfluence of temporal changes in fruit availability on dietcomposition and seed handling in blue monkeys (Cerco-pithecus mitis doggetti). Biotropica 30:5671.

    Knott CF. 1998. Changes in orangutan caloric intake, energybalance, and ketones in response to fluctuating fruit avail-ability. International Journal of Primatology 19:10611079.

    Korstjens AH, Dunbar RIM. 2007. Time constraints limitgroup sizes and distribution in red and black-and-whitecolobus. International Journal of Primatology 28:551575.

    Korstjens AH, Verhoeckx I, Dunbar RIM. 2006. Time as aconstraint on group size in spider monkeys. BehavioralEcology and Sociobiology 60:683694.

    Korstjens AH, Lehmann L, Dunbar RIM. 2010. Resting time asan ecological constraint on primate biogeography. AnimalBehaviour 79:361374.

    Kosheleff VP, Anderson CNK. 2009. Temperatures influenceon the activity budget, terrestriality, and sun exposure ofchimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. AmericanJournal of Physical Antropology 139:172181.

    Kummer H. 1971. Primate societies. Group techniques ofecological adaptation. Chicago, IL: Aldine Atherton.

    Lehmann L, Korstjens AH, Dunbar RIM. 2007. Fissionfusionsocial systems as a strategy for coping with ecologicalconstraints: a primate case. Evolutionary Ecology21:613634.

    Lindshield SMMA. 2006. The density and distribution of Atelesgeoffroyi in a mosaic landscape at El Zota Biological FieldStation. Costa Rica, M.Sc. Thesis, Iowa State University,Iowa.

    McConkey KR, Aldy F, Ario A, Chivers DJ. 2002. Selection offruit by gibbons (Hylobates muellerix agilis) in the rainforests of central Borneo. International Journal of Prima-tology 23:123145.

    McDaniel PS. 1994. The social behavior and ecology ofthe black-handed spider monkey (Ateles geoffroyi), Ph.D.Thesis, Saint Louis University, Saint Louis.

    Miller K, Chang E, Johnson N. 2001. Defining common groundfor the Mesoamerican biological corridor. Washington, DC:World Resources Institute.

    Milton K. 1981. Food choice and digestive strategies of twosympatric primate species. American Naturalist117:476495.

    Milton K. 1984. Habitat, diet, and activity patterns of free-ranging woolly spider monkeys (Brachyteles arachnoidesE. Geoffroy 1806). International Journal of Primatology5:491514.

    Milton K. 1993. Diet and social organization of a free-rangingspider monkey population: the development of species-typical behavior in the absence of adults. In: Pereira M,Fairbanks LA, editors. Juvenile primates: life history,development and behavior. Oxford: Oxford university press.

    Milton K. 1998. Physiological ecology of howlers (Alouatta):energetic and digestive considerations and comparisonwith the Colobinae. International Journal of Primatology19:513548.

    Motulsky HJ, Christopoulos A. 2003. Fitting models tobiological data using linear and nonlinear regression. Apractical guide to curve fitting. San Diego: GraphPadSoftware Inc. www.graphpad.com. Downloaded on July 20,2005.

    Murphy P, Lugo AE. 1995. Dry forests of Central America andthe Caribbean. In: Bullock SH, Mooney HM, Medina E,editors. Seasonally dry tropical forests. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press. p 146194.

    Nakagawa S. 2004. A farewell to Bonferroni: the problems oflow statistical power and publication bias. BehavioralEcology 6:10441045.

    Parmesan C. 2006. Ecological and evolutionary responses torecent climate change. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolutionand Systematics 37:637669.

    Peres CA. 1994. Diet and feeding ecology of gray woollymonkeys (Lagothrix lagotricha cana) in Central Amazonia:comparisons with other atelines. International Journal ofPrimatology 15:333372.

    Ramos-Fernandez G, Ayala-Orozco B. 2003. Populationsize and habitat use of spider monkeys in Punta Laguna,Mexico. In: Marsh LK, editor. Primates in fragments:ecology and conservation. New York: Kluwer/Plenum Press.p 191209.

    Rangel-Negrn A, Alfaro JL, Valdez RA, Romano MC,Serio-Silva JC. 2009. Stress in Yucatan spider monkeys:effects of environmental conditions on fecal cortisol levels inwild and captive populations. Animal Conservation12:496502.

    Richard AA. 1970. A comparative study of the activity patternsand behavior of Alouatta villosa and Ateles geoffroyi. FoliaPrimatologica 12:241263.

    Sanchez-Lopez S. 2008. Programa de rehabilitacionconductual del mono arana cautivo (Ateles geoffroyivellerosus): estudio longitudinal de indicadores socialesy troficos. Ph.D. Thesis. Barcelona: Universitat deBarcelona.

    Sapolsky RM. 1986. Endocrine and behavioral correlates ifdrought in the wild baboons. American Journal of Prima-tology 11:217226.

    Simmen B, Bayart F, Rasamimanana H, Zahariev A, Blanc S,Pasquet P. 2010. Total energy expenditure and bodycomposition in two free-living sympatric lemurs. PLoSONE 5:e9860.

    Stelzner JK. 1988. Thermal effects on movement patterns ofyellow baboons. Primates 29:91105.

    Stevenson PR. 2001. The relationship between fruitproduction and primate abundance in Neotropicalcommunities. Biological Journal of Linnean Society 72:161178.

    Stevenson PR, Link A. 2010. Fruit preferences of Atelesbelzebuth in Tinigua Park, Northwertern Amazonia. Inter-national Journal of Primatology 31:393407.

    Stevenson PR, Quinones MJ, Ahumada JA. 2000. Influenceof fruit availability on ecological overlap among fourNeotropical primates at Tinigua National Park, Colombia.Biotropica 32:533544.

    Stoner KE, Timm RM. 2004. Tropical dry forest mammals:conservation priorities in a changing landscape. In:Frankie GW, Mata A, Vinson SB, editors. Biodiversityconservation in Costa Rica: learning the lessons in aseasonal dry forest. California: University of CaliforniaPress. p 4866.

    Wallace RB. 2005. Seasonal variations in diet andforaging behavior of Ateles chamek in a southern Amazo-nian tropical forest. International Journal of Primatology26:10531075.

    Weghorst JA. 2007. Behavioral ecology and fissionfusiondynamics of spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) in lowland,wet forest. PhD Thesis. Saint Louis: Washington UniversitySt. Louis.

    Wiederholt R, Post E. 2010. Tropical warming and thedynamics of endangered primates. Biological Letters6:257260.

    Wrangham RW, Gittleman JL, Chapman CA. 1993. Con-straints on group size in primates and carnivores: popula-tion density and day-range as assays of exploitationcompetition. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology32:199209.

    Am. J. Primatol.

    1198 / Gonzalez-Zamora et al.