Components Analysis of Cognitive Strategy Instruction

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/17/2019 Components Analysis of Cognitive Strategy Instruction

    1/9

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    1989, Vol. 81 , No. 3, 353-561

    Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

    0022-O663/89/SOO.75

    Com ponents Analysis of Cognitive Strategy Instruction: Effects on

    Learning Disabled Students' Compositions and  Self-Efficacy

    Steve Graha m and Karen R. Ha rris

    Department

     of

     Special Education

    University of Maryland

    The viability of self-instructional strategy training am ong learning disabled (LD ) stude nts exhib-

    iting composition deficiencies was investigated. F urtherm ore,

     the

      theoretically proposed incre-

    mental effects of explicit self-regulation procedures were examined in terms of writing perform-

    ance measures

     at

     posttest, maintenance,

     and

     generalization,

     and in

     terms

     of

     Ss self-efficacy.

     Ss

    were 22 LD and 11 normally achieving students in the 5th and 6th grades. Results indicated that

    self-instructional strategy training produced meaningful  and lasting effects on Ss' composition

    skills and a significantly heightened sense of self-efficacy. Explic it self-regulation proce dures did

    not significantly augment strategy-instruction effects  for  either writing performance  or self-

    efficacy. Composition performance after instruction among LD Ss did

     not

     differ significantly

     in

    terms of story grammar elements from that of a contrast group of normally achieving, competent

    writers. However, normally achieving students' compositions were longer

     and

      received signifi-

    cantly higher quality ratings.

    In 1980, Hobb s, Moguin, Tyroher, and Lahey commented

    that relatively  few  cognitive-behavioral studies had  focused

    on academic skills. Since that time cognitive strategy instruc-

    tion  in academic areas has become a  major focus of educa-

    tional research with  the mildly  to  moderately handicapped

    and other inefficient learners (cf. M eichenbaum,  1983; Press-

    ley & Levin, 1986). Cognitive-behavioral theorists have pro-

    posed that effective strategy instruction involves three major

    comp onents: strategies, knowledge about the use and signifi-

    cance of those strategies (metastrategy information), and self-

    regulation of strategic performance

     (cf.

     Brown, Campione,

     &

    Day,

      1981;

     Harris & Graham, 1985).

    Multicomponent strategy instruction interventions based

    on th is theoretical view have pro ved efficacious  in improving

    performance—and frequently in o btaining generalization and

    maintenance—among young children and problem learners

    (Harris, 1986a; Reeve &  Brown, 1985). Pressley and Levin

    (1986) indicated that such interventions have frequently re-

    sulted in  performance among handicapped learners equiva-

    lent to that obtained by their nonh andicapped peers. Previous

    self-instructional strategy-training studies have incorporated

    strategy instruction (using a  self-instructional format), meta-

    strategy knowledge,  and  explicit self-regulation. This  ap-

    This study

      was

      supported

      by a

      grant from

      the

      University

      of

    Maryland Graduate Research Board

     to

     Karen

     R.

     Harris; authorship

    was determined alphabetically. Appreciation is expressed to the chil-

    dren, teachers, and principals of the P rince George's Coun ty schools

    participating  in  this project. Gratitude  is  extended  to the  strategy

    instructors, Linda Artman, Cassie Chapman, Barbara

     Danoff,

     Tricia

    Marker, Beth North,

     and

     Andrea Rothm an. Finally,

     we

     would like

    to thank Michael Pressley

     and the

      anonymous reviewers

     for

      their

    feedback on a draft of th is article.

    Correspondence concerning this article should  be  addressed  to

    Steve Graham, Department of Special Education, College of Educa-

    tion, U niversity of Maryland, College Park, M aryland 20742.

    proach has been successful in improving written language and

    mathematical problem-solving skills among learning disabled

    (LD) students (Graham  & Harris,  1989, in  press; Graham,

    Harris, & Sawyer, 1987; Harris & Grah am , 1985).

    Although multicomponent interventions are frequently  ef-

    fective, a major issue in cognitive strategy instruction research

    is determining both the relative contributions of instructional

    components and the variables responsible for  change. Cost-

    benefit relationships  of  components also need  to be  deter-

    mined (Harris,  1985;  Pressley, Forrest-Pressley,  &  Elliott-

    Faust,

      in

      press). Component analyses studies remain rare,

    however. Elliott-Faust and Pressley (1986) demo nstrated th at

    multicomponent strategy instruction, including  a  complete

    and effective strategy embedded  in a  self-instruction routine

    combined with self-monitoring of strategy use and effective-

    ness, was superior to simply teaching children the task strat-

    egy. However,  no  studies were located that investigated the

    three theoretically-based critical elements of multicomponent

    strategy instruction.

    It has  been theorized that explicit instruction in  self-regu-

    lation  of  strategic performance will result  in  incremental

    effects and is critical to effective strategy deploy men t, pro duc-

    tion of new metastrategy information, independent strategy

    use,

     and

     m aintenance

     and

     generalization

     of

      effects (O'Leary

    & Dubey,  1979; Pressley  &  Levin, 1986). Self-regulation

    procedures have also been effective  in  increasing children's

    self-efficacy (Bandura

     &

     Schunk, 1981). In addition, previous

    research has established that LD students respond enthusiast-

    ically to self-regulation procedu res and would highly recom-

    men d such procedures to other students experiencing learning

    problems (Graham & Harris, 1989; Harris, 1986b; Harris &

    Graham, 1985). It is not  known, however,  if  explicit  self-

    regulation procedures will produce incremental effects when

    combined with an intervention that provides strategy instruc-

    tion and metastrategy information. Thus, a major purpose of

    the present investigation

     was

     to investigate possible increme n-

     5

  • 8/17/2019 Components Analysis of Cognitive Strategy Instruction

    2/9

     5

    STEVE GRAHAM

      AND

     KAREN

      R.

      HARRIS

    tal effects that are due to  instruction  in  self-regulation  of

    strategic performance.

    A second major purpose of this investigation was to deter-

    min e the viability of a cognitive strategy instruction appro ach

    among  LD  students exhibiting composition deficiencies.

    Composition skills represent a  significant deficiency among

    these, students. Merely requiring students, whether

      LD or

    normally achieving, to write mo re frequently typically fails to

    result in  improved writing performance; practice alone does

    not improve writing skills (Graham, 1982; Graham, Harris,

    & Sawyer, 1988). Creative compositions written by LD stu-

    dents frequently lack even  the  most basic story elements

    (Barenbaum, Newcomer, & Nodine, 1987; Graham & Harris,

    1989;

      Nodine, Barenbaum,

      &

      Newcomer, 1985). Further-

    more, judgments concerning  the  overall quality of LD stu-

    dents' writing  are  significantly related  to the  number  and

    quality of basic story elements included in their compositions

    (MacArthur & Graham, 1987).

    Stein and G lenn (1979) noted that the gramm ar or structure

    of stories

     can be

     reduced

     to two

     major compo nents: setting

    and episode. T he setting typically incorporate s three elements:

    introduction of the main character, a description of the locale,

    and information about the time of the story. Episodes intro-

    duce and resolve the plot of  the story and  typically include

    five elemen ts: starter event (a precipitating event occurs), goal

    (the main character reacts  to the  precipitating event  and

    formulates  a  goal), action  (a  planned effort  to  achieve the

    goal),

      ending

      (the

      results

      of the

      action),

      and

      reaction

      (an

    emphatic statement or a final response of a story character to

    the consequences  of the  action). Short  and  Ryan (1984)

    reported that story gramm ar training based on these elements

    produced m eaningful gains in reading comprehension a mong

    poor readers. Short

     and

     Ryan's training emphasized compre-

    hension monitoring, through the use of five questions abo ut

    settings  and  episodes,  to  select important aspects  of  story

    information  for  further study. In the present study, the story

    grammar strategy component  of  training emphasized  the

    generation of seven questions about setting and episode; these

    seven questions include and expand upon the five questions

    used by Short and Ryan. In the present investigation, how-

    ever, these questions were used in a  self-instructional strategy-

    training intervention designed to facilitate advanced planning

    an d the development of writing c ontent.

    The Present Investigation

    Two self-instructional strategy training groups were  in-

    cluded in the present study. Both groups received the story

    gramm ar strategy instruction  and instruction  in the  signifi-

    cance of this strategy. One group also received instruction in

    explicit self-regulation. Self-regulation procedures included

    criterion setting and self-monitoring (self-assessment and self-

    recording). Instruction was conducted by preservice teachers

    in the schools.

    Multiple measures were used to assess instructional proce-

    dures and effects. The incorporation of story gramm ar sche-

    mata in LD students' com positions was assessed prior to and

    immediately following instruction. To determine  if  changes

    in schematic structure were associated with improvem ent in

    story quality, pre- and posttest compositions were also eval-

    uated  in  terms of  overall quality. Furthermore, changes in

    individual story gram mar elements were examined to deter-

    mine which aspects of  narrative structure were affected  by

    instruction. Generalization across settings  and  short-term

    maintenance were also assessed. In addition, a group of nor-

    mally achieving, com peten t writers served as a contrast gro up

    in posttest assessments, to determine if any differences in the

    educational significance of the instruction existed between the

    two groups. Treatmen t validity was determined by collecting

    evidence

     on

     students' use of the story gram mar strategy both

    during instruction  and  independently. Teacher and  student

    comments collected throughout instruction were helpful  in

    further determ ining the social validity of the two interv ention

    approaches. Finally,  LD  students' self-efficacy  for  creative

    comp osing w as assessed before and after instruction .

    In summary, the present study addressed five critical ques-

    tions: (a) Do LD students benefit from self-instructional story

    grammar strategy training (in either or both conditions)? (b)

    Do explicit self-regulation procedures meaningfully augment

    self-instructional strategy training combined with instruction

    in  the  significance  of the  strategies? (c) Can  validation  of

    treatment procedures be provided (i.e., can treatment validity

    be established)? (d) Can evidence of  educational validity be

    established? (e) Did  either  or  both  of the  intervention ap-

    proaches meaningfully affect  LD  students' perceived  self-

    efficacy?

    M e t h o d

    Subjects

    LD .

      Subjects

     were

     22

     fifth-

     and sixth-grade LD students receiving

    resource room services

     in

     three elementary schools located

     in

     subur-

    ban, middle-class neighborhoods outside of W ashington, DC. E ighty-

    six percent of the students enrolled in these three schools were B lack.

    Because of problems of validity with the LD label and the heteroge-

    neity of  school-identified  LD populations (Harris, 1986a), all of th e

    LD subjects selected

     for

     this study m et the following stepwise criteria:

    identification as LD

     by the

     school district, 1Q scores between 85

     and

    115

     on an

      individually administered intelligence test, achievement

    that was at least 2 years below grade level in one or more academic

    areas, absence of any other handicapping condition, and  interviews

    with the  resource room teachers indicating that significant composi-

    tion problems were evident. IQ scores

     for

     the

     LD

     subjects were from

    either

     the

     Wechsler Intelligence Scale

     for

     Children-Revised (WISC-

    R; Wechsler,

      1974) or the

      Slosson Intelligence Test

      (SIT,

     Slosson,

    1971). All IQ  scores were taken from school files and had  been

    administered by school psychologists for all of the subjects within the

    past  3  years. Although WISC-R scares were preferable, they were

    available

     for

     only

     6

     of the 22 L D subjects. Achievement scores

     for

     all

    LD subjects were from

     the

      Peabody Individual Achievement Test

    (Dunn  Markwardt, 1970), administered within

      the

     past year

     by

    school personnel and taken from students

    1

     files.

    Subjects were randomly identified

      and

     evaluated

     on the

     basis

     of

    these criteria until

      an

      equal number

      of

      fifth-

      and

     sixth-grade

     LD

    students were selected. Fourteen of the subjects were boys and 8 were

    girls. Seventeen of the subjects were Black, and 5 were White. Mean

    IQ score was 95 (range =  85-113); mean chronological age was 11

    years,

     8 months (range = 10 years, 2 months-13 years, 7 months).

    Further information

      on

      subject characteristics within experimental

    conditions

     is

     presented

     in

     Table

     1.

  • 8/17/2019 Components Analysis of Cognitive Strategy Instruction

    3/9

    COMPONENTS ANALYSIS  OF  COGNITIVE STRATEGY INSTRUCTION

    355

    Table

     I

    Subject

     Characteristics

     by E xperimental

     Groups

    Variable

    CA

    M

    SD

    IQ

    M

    SD

    Race

    Black («)

    White («)

    Sex

    Female (n)

    Male (n)

    Grade

    5th (n)

    6th (n)

    SIST

    142.18

    7.32

    93.45

    8.03

    9

    2

    4

    7

    5

    6

    Group

    8

    SIST+SR

    137.64

    11.82

    97.09

    6.61

    8

    3

    4

    7

    6

    5

    NA

    133.60

    6.01

    8

    3

    5

    6

    5

    6

    Note.

      SIST =  self-instructional strategy training; SIST+SR  = self-

    instructional strategy training plus self-regulation training;  NA =

    normally achieving; CA

     =

      chronological age.

    a

     n —

     11 for each group .

    LD subjects were randomly assigned to one of the two self-instruc-

    tional strategy-training conditions (self-instructional strategy training

    or self-instructional strategy training plus self-regulation training).

    Separate univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAS) were performed to

    determine if  there were any  significant differences betwen the two

    groups on important subject variables. No significant differences  were

    found on IQ , F  (1 , 20) =  1.36, p =  .26, chronological age, F (1 , 20)

    = 1.18, p =  .29, pretest self-efficacy,  F(\,  20) -

      .38,

     p =  .55, pretest

    quality ratings, F ( 1, 20) =  .02, p

     —

      .90, and pretest story grammar

    elements, F  (1 , 20) =  .16, p = .70. Procedures for  assessing self-

    efficacy, quality,

     and

      story grammar elements

     are

     described

      in the

    next section.

    Normally

     achieving.  The normative comparison group consisted

    of

      11

     randomly selected students who attended

     the

     same schools

     as

    the LD subjects. IQ testing of normally achieving students was not

    allowed

      by the

      school system. Although normal performance

      is

    commonly defined  at plus and minus one standard deviation from

    the mean (equivalent to

     a

     percentile rank range

     of 16 to

     84), we used

    a more conservative definition  of  the lower end of the range. The

    normally achieving subjects scored between the 29th and 81st per-

    centiles (M

     —

     64.0) on the Cognitive Abilities Test (Thorndike &

    Hagen, 1978) and above the 29th p ercentile on the following subtests

    of the C alifornia A chievement T ests (1979): reading vocabulary (M

    =

      59.2), reading comprehension (M = 65.6), language m echanics

     (M

    =

     76.2), and language expression

     (M

     = 67.4). These students received

    no special education services

     and

     were capable writers according

     to

    their classroom teachers. Composition  of  this contrast group was

    equivalent to that of the tw o LD training groups in terms of sex, race,

    and grade (see Table 1).

    Instruments and Materials

    Story grammar elements.

      We developed a scale for assessing the

    schematic structure of w ritten stories. The scale was designed so th at

    students' stories could  be  assessed  in  terms of the  inclusion and

    quality of the eight story gram mar elements identified by Stein and

    Glenn (1979): main character, locale, time, starter event, goal, action,

    ending,

     and

     reaction.

    For each story element, a  score of 0 was assigned if  the element

    was not present in the story, and a  score of  1 was assigned if the

    element w as included. Elements tha t w ere highly developed received

    a score of 2. For the goal story element, a score of 3 was awarded if

    tw o  or  more goals were present. Similarly,  for the  action story

    element, scores of 3 or 4  were assigned  if the actions  or  events

    happened in a  logical manner or if there was more than one well-

    defined episode (or both). Scores were determ ined for each element

    separately;  in  addition,  by  totaling  the  scores  for the  individual

    elements, a total story grammar element score was calculated (a total

    of 19 points was possible).

    All stories written by a subject were scored both by their instructor

    an d by a  second trained examiner who was naive to the student's

    group assignment and the purpose and design of  the investigation.

    For all scores (individual element scores and total score), the ratings

    of the instructor and second exam iner were averaged. Interobserver

    reliability between the instructors and the second examiner for the

    total story grammar element score was .80. Interrater agreement on

    presence of  the element for each of  the eight story elements was as

    follows: .95, for main character, .94, for locale; .96, for time; .64, for

    starter event; .95, for goal; .93, for action; .84, for ending; and .96,

    for reaction.

    Evidence on the validity of  the story grammar element scale was

    obtained  by  correlating  the  total scores of 26  normally achieving

    sixth-grade students with (a) their scores on the Thematic Maturity

    subtest of the

      Test

     of

     Written Language

     (Hammill & Larsen, 1983)

    an d (b) a  measure  of  written fluency (i.e.,  the  number  of words

    written). Students' scores on the  scale were moderately correlated

    with thematic maturity (/• =  .40) and number of words w ritten (r =

    .32). In  addition, MacArthur and Graham (1987) reported that LD

    students' total scores on  this scale were significantly correlated with

    measures of story length (« =  .8O-.85) and story quality (rs = . 7 1 -

    .86).

    Holistic

     rating

     scale.

      A holistic rating scale was used to

     assess

     the

    quality of subjects' stories following Gra ham 's (1982) recommenda-

    tions. Examiners were directed to read each story attentively but no t

    laboriously in order to obtain a  general impression of quality. Ex-

    aminers were told that aptness of word choice, grammar, organiza-

    tion, sentence structure, and  imagination should  all be  taken into

    account in forming a

     single

     judgment about the overall quality of the

    writing sample and that no one factor should receive un due weight.

    Compositions were then scored on a 7-point scale, with 1 representing

    the

     lowest quality

     of writing, and 7 representing the

     highest quality.

    Examiners were provided with  a  representative sample  of a low,

    medium, and high scoring composition to use as a guide or anchor

    point in  scoring the subjects* compositions. The high, medium, and

    low com positions were obtained from a regular sixth-grade class; all

    students in this class not receiving special services wrote a story, and

    two trained graduate students selected the best, the middle, and the

    poorest story on the basis of the scoring criteria noted above.

    Two elementary-school teachers unfamiliar with

     the

     purpose

     and

    design of the  study independently assigned holistic quality scores to

    the stories written by the L D students during the pretest and posttest

    writing probes and to the stories written by the  normally achieving

    students. Prior

      to

     scoring

     the

     students

    1

      stories, both examiners

     re-

    ceived considerable training and practice in using the holistic scoring

    procedure. Interobserver reliability between

     the two

     examiners

     was

    .76. For each story, the ratings of the two examiners were averaged.

    Self efficacy

     measure.

      An

     individually administered scale assess-

    ing self-efficacy—or judgments of one's capability to  perform given

    activities—for creative writing was developed and administered ac-

    cording to procedures detailed by Bandura and Schunk (1981). The

    efficacy scale ranged from 10 to  100 in  10-unit intervals; the higher

    the scale value, the higher th e perceived self-efficacy. Verbal descrip-

    tors occurred

     at the

     following points: 10

     {not sure),

     40

     (maybe),

     70

  • 8/17/2019 Components Analysis of Cognitive Strategy Instruction

    4/9

    356

    STEVE GRAHAM

      AND

      KAREN

      R.

      HARRIS

    {pretty sure), 100

     {real

     sure). Practice with the  efficacy assessment

    procedure was provided by having subjects judge their capability to

    jump progressively longer distances, from

     a few

      inches

      to

      several

    yards. Following this introduc tion, subjects were read  10 items prob-

    ing self-efficacy

      for

     writing

      a

     made-up story."

     The

     first

     9

     questions,

    all introduced with the phrase  Can you write a story

     that,**

     were as

    follows:

     (a)

     tells ab out

     the

     main character's feelings?

     (b)

     clearly tells

    about the setting? (c) has a good beginning? (d) tells who the main

    character

     is? (e)

     tells about several things tha t h appen

     to the

     main

    character? (f) tells when the story takes place? (g) tells where the story

    took place?

     (h)

     tells w hat

     the

     main character wants

     to do? (i) has a

    good ending? The 1 Oth question was (j) Can you w rite a

     good,

     creative

    (made-up) story? Subjects were asked

      to be

      honest

      and to

      mark

    privately the appropriate num ber on the scale or each item. Subjects

    understood these procedures

     and did not

     experience difficulty com-

    pleting their judgments. The  summed magnitude scores divided by

    the total number

     of

     questions provided

     the

      measure

     of

      strength

     of

    self-efficacy.

    Coefficient alpha

     for the

     self-efficacy scale was determ ined

     to be

    .80 among 26  normally achieving sixth-grade students prior to  this

    study.

      In

      addition,

      the

      internal-consistency reliability (coefficient

    alpha)

     of

     the self-efficacy measure among

     the

     22

     LD

     students

     in

     this

    study

     at

     pretest was .76.

    Writing

     stimuli.  Fou r black-and-white pictures were used

     as

     writ-

    ing stimuli for  four different writing probes: pretest, posttest, gener-

    alization (administered

      in the

      student's resource room following

    treatment), and maintenance (administered 2 weeks after the termi-

    nation

      of

      treatment). Pictures were selected

      to be

      interesting

      to

    elementary-school-age children, to be fairly easy to write about, and

    to  be similar in natu re (showing only one character). The four pictures

    were (a) a boy walking across a field, (b) a scuba diver in the water,

    (c)

     an

     Indian

     on a

     horse,

     and

     (d)

     a man in a

     boat. The pictures were

    randomly assigned to the pretest, posttest, generalization, and main-

    tenance probes. Pictures were administered to all subjects in the sam e

    order, because several researchers(cf. Englert & Thom as, 1987) have

    demonstrated that similar writing stimuli produce similar results

    (thus,

     randomly ordering stimuli for each subject appeared unneces-

    sary). Additional, similar black-and-white pictures served

     as

     writing

    stimuli for practice stories written du ring training.

    General Procedures

    Six senior-level undergraduate students

     who had

      extensive field

    experience and were majoring

     in

     special education served as instruc-

    tors; each instructor worked with an equivalent number of students

    from both of the instructional groups. Instructors were blind to the

    specific theoretical issues examined by the study so as not to predis-

    pose them

     to

     either approach. Th e instructors were introduced

     to all

    subjects

     as

     "special writing teach ers" who would

     be

     working

     at

     their

    school. Instructors received considerable training

     and

     practice

     in the

    application of  the instructional procedures, until they were able to

    role-play the lessons without error. Detailed lesson plans containing

    step-by-step procedures for each phase of instruction were provided

    in

     a

     notebook. Each step of the daily lesson plan was checked

     off

     as

    it was completed;

     we

     monitored these notebooks closely

     and

     talked

    with each instructor frequently throughout the study

     to

     ensure stand-

    ard training procedures across instructors.

    Instructors worked with small groups of tw o or  three students at

    their schools; instructors

     met

     with each group

     for

     approximately

     45

    min,

     2 to 3

     days

     a

     week,

     for 2 to 3

     weeks. Because instruction

     was

    criterion-based (students were required to master each lesson before

    proceeding to the next), the number of sessions and amount of time

    required varied slightly for each instructional group. The number of

    sessions required for the strategy instruction groups ranged from five

    to seven, with instructional tim e ranging from

     5 to 6 hr in

     total.

     For

    the strategy instruction plus self-regulation groups,  the  number of

    sessions ranged from five

     to

      eight; total instructional time ranged

    from 4 to 6.5 hr. Thus, the  self-regulation components were easily

    incorporated into the instructional procedures and did not necessitate

    either extra sessions or longer instructional times. All of the subjects

    mastered each

      of the

      instructional steps,

     and

      none

     of the

      subjects

    found the instruction to be particularly complicated, perhaps because

    of the criterion-based n ature

     of

     instruction.

    The two pre- and posttests (writing probe and self-efficacy measure)

    were administered individually by the writing instructors on different

    days

     (to

     avoid carryover effects)

     at the

      subject's school,

     as was the

    maintenance writing probe. The short-term maintenance probe was

    administered

     2

     weeks after

     the

     termination

     of

     instruction;

     a

      second,

    longer term maintenance probe was not  possible because  of the

    conclusion of the school year. The generalization-across-settings writ-

    ing probe was collected in the resource room by the resource room

    teacher w ithin

      1

     week after instruction.

     The

     writing probe

     for nor-

    mally achieving subjects was administered by an instructor congruent

    with the time of posttesting for LD subjects. At no time during any

    of these sessions were subjects provided feedback  on  their perform-

    ance. When

      the

      pretest, posttest, maintenance,

      and

      generalization

    writing probes were administered, the following standardized instruc-

    tions were given:

    Look at this picture and write a story to go with

     it.

     Use everything

    that

     you

     have learned about writing stories

     to

     help you. Please

    remember that  I  cannot help you write the  story. However, if

    you

     do not

     know how to spell

     a

     word,

     I

     will write

     it out for

     you.

    Instructional

     Procedures

    The self-instructional strategy training steps and c ompon ents were

    developed and validated previously (cf.  Graham & Harris, in press;

    Graha m, H arris, & Sawyer, 1987; Harris

     &

     Graha m, 1985).

     In

     both

    conditions, Instruction emphasized  the  student's role as an  active

    collaborator

      and

      emphasized interactive learning between teacher

    and students, with responsibility for recruiting an d applying strategies

    gradually placed upon

      the

     student. Principles

     of

     interactional

     scaf-

    folding  and  Socratic dialogue were incorporated; instructors were

    enthusiastic

     and

     responsive

     to

     each child

     and

      provided individually

    tailored feedback. Strategies were explicitly and  overtly modeled in

    context;

     the

     goal

     and

     significance

      of

      the strategies were also made

    clear. Finally, all  instruction  was  criterion-based rather than time-

    based, and previously taught skills/strategies were routinely reviewed.

    The strategy instruction

      and the

      strategy instruction plus

      self-

    regulation groups received  the  same instructional program except

    that

     the

      strategy instruction plus self-regulation groups received

     in-

    struction  and practice in (a)  self-monitoring their performance (re-

    cording

     and

     graphing

     the

     number

     and

     kind

      of

      story grammar

     ele-

    ments contained

      in

      their practice stories)

     and (b)

      criterion setting

    (establishing goals  for the  number  of  elements  to be  included  in

    subsequent practice stories). Instructional steps for the two groups

    were as  follows (all  steps were conducted in groups of tw o to three

    students).

    1

    Step

     1: ^retraining.  Pretrainin g focused on defining, identifying,

    and generating story grammar elements using

      a

      mnemonic; this

    1

     Five lesson plans were developed to incorporate the seven training

    steps.

     Lesson Plan 1 covered T raining Steps 1 and 2; Lesson Plan 2

    covered Training Steps 3,

     4, and 5;

     Lesson Plan

     3

     covered T raining

    Steps 5

     and

     6; Lesson Plans

     4 and

     5 covered Training Step 7. Lesson

    plans were typically completed in one or two training sessions. Copies

    of these lesson plans are available from the authors.

  • 8/17/2019 Components Analysis of Cognitive Strategy Instruction

    5/9

    COMPONENTS ANALYSIS  OF  COGNITIVE STRATEGY INSTRUCTION

    357

    mnemonic was then used

     in

     the writing strategy tau ght

     later.

     A small

    chart provided the mnem onic for the seven story gramma r questions:

    W-W-W; What = 2; How = 2. T he questions were (a) Who is the

    main character, who else is in the story? (b)  When does the story take

    place? (c) Where does the story take place? (d)

      What

     does the main

    character want to do? (e)

      What

     happens when he or she tries to do

    it? (f)

     H ow

     does the story end? and (g)

     Ho w

     does the main character

    feel? After discussing the meaning of each element, students practiced

    until they could recite the m nemonic and its meaning from m emory.

    Students then identified story grammar elements in existing stories

    and generated story elements while looking at a picture.

    Step

      2:

      Review current performance level

     and

     training ration-

    ale.

      Once students had mastered the  story grammar elements, the

    instructor  and  students discussed students' pretest performance  in

    terms of their ability to incorporate the elements into their composi-

    tions. The instructor and students also discussed the goal of instruc-

    tion (to w rite better stories), why this is important, and how inclusion

    and expansion  of the  story grammar elements improve  a  story.

    Subjects  in the  strategy instruction plus self-regulation conditio n

    received a graph depicting their performance  on the pretest, and

    graphing procedures were explained.

    Step 3: Describe th e learning

     strategy.  A small chart was used

     to

    introduce and discuss afive-step writing strategy. Th e five steps w ere

    (a) look at the picture; (b) let your mind be free; (c) write down the

    story part reminder (W-W-W, What = 2, How = 2); (d) write down

    story-part ideas for each p art; and (e) write your story: use good parts

    and m ake sense. The instructor also modeled and then discussed with

    the students three creativity self-statements helpful  in  thinking of

    good story parts (e.g., "Take my time, good parts will come to me";

    "Let my mind be free, think of new, fun ideas"; and "What ideas do

    I see in this picture?"). Students then generated tw o or three of then-

    own, preferred self-statements, recorded them o n paper, and practiced

    using these self-statements to generate story parts.

    Step 4; Model the strategy a nd

      self-instructions.  The mnemonic

    an d five-step strategy charts, the list of creativity self-statements, and

    a new stimulus picture were set out. The instructor modeled the use

    of the writing strategy by w riting a story while "thinking out loud."

    Consistent with Meichenbaum's (1977) self-instructional training

    guidelines, the  instructor modeled four additional types of self-in-

    structions while composing: problem definition (i.e., "What  is it I

    have to do?"), planning (i.e., "Now I'd better write down my  story-

    parts reminder"), self-evaluation (i.e.,  Am I  using all my parts so

    far?"),

      and self-reinforcement (i.e., "Good ,  I  like these parts "). A

    modeling script was provided in the lesson plans. After the modeling,

    instructor and students discussed the importance

     of

     what we say

     to

    ourselves while we work. Students generated and recorded their ow n

    examples of the four types of self-instructions.

    Step

      5:

     Mastery

      of

      strategy steps.

      Students were required  to

    practice the five-step writing strategy until

     it

     was memorized. Para-

    phrasing was allowed as long as meaning rem ained in tact.

    Step

      6:

     Controlled

     practice.

      The

      instructor

      and

      students

     con-

    jointly planned one story following the first four steps of the five-step

    strategy, each student then wrote the story independently. The mne-

    monic andfive-step strategy c harts, as well as t he student-generated

    self-instruction lists, were available a s prom pts. Although the instruc-

    tor directed and monitored the process, she did not write the stories.

    In both conditions, the teacher and students reviewed each student's

    story as a group; if any of the story elements were missing, the g roup

    discussed how

     and

     where they could

     be

     added.

     For

     students

     in the

    strategy instruction plus self-regulation groups,  the  students and

    instructor initially set a  goal to  include all of the  story grammar

    elements. After the stories were written, each stude nt in these groups

    and the  instructor independently counted the number of story ele-

    ments included in  the story, compared counts, graphed the number

    on the student's chart, and compared performance to the criterion.

    Step  :

      Independent

     performance.  Students independently com-

    posed two stories using the five-step strategy and self-instructional

    statements. Positive and corrective feedback was provided as needed;

    as in Step 6, the instructor and student reviewed each story (in both

    conditions) and discussed how missing elements (if any) could be

    included. Transition to  covert self-instruction was encouraged. Stu-

    dents were allowed

     to

     use the charts and self-instruction list only

     for

    the first of the two stories written. Students in the strategy instruction

    plus self-regulation group followed

      the

      same goal-setting

     and self-

    monitoring procedures described in Step 6.

    Throughout the instructional sessions, students were asked to share

    what they were learning with their teachers  and  parents. Students

    discussed with their instructors how what they were learning could

    be used in their resource and regular classrooms; spontaneous com-

    ments made by students during these discussions were recorded by

    the instructors. Both

     the

     instructors

     and the

     investigators discussed

    the interventions with the students and the resource room teachers

    and recorded their comments.

     It

     should also be noted that any paper

    that students wrote on during the writing probe sessions (pre, post,

    maintenance,

     and

     generalization) was collected, because this could

    provide evidence concerning strategy usage (i.e., Were the seven story

    grammar questions

     and

      their responses written

      out

     prior

      to

      story

    generation?).

    Results

    Story Elements: Total Score

    A 2  (instructional groups) x 4 (trials) repeated measures

    ANOVA

      design was used to examine the effects of the two

    instructional conditions (strategy instruction vs. strategy in-

    struction plus self-regulation) on LD subjects' total scores on

    the story grammar element scale and to determine if these

    scores differed significantly at pretest, posttest, generalization,

    and maintenance. The F ratio for trials was significant, ^ 3 ,

    60) =

      20.85,

     p

  • 8/17/2019 Components Analysis of Cognitive Strategy Instruction

    6/9

    358

    STEVE GRAHAM AND KAREN R. HARRIS

    Table 2

    Means and Standard Deviations of Total Story Grammar

    Elements

     Score

     as a

     Function

     of Training

     Group

     and

    Writing Probe

    Condition

    Pretest

    M

    SD

    Posttest

    M

    SD

    Generalization

    M

    SD

    Maintenance

    M

    SD

    SIST"

    6.05

    2.32

    9.82

    1.01

    9.86

    2.52

    9.05

    1.29

    Group

    SIST+SR-

    5.68

    1.95

    9.23

    1.06

    7.95

    2.39

    8.36

    2.06

    Combmed

    b

    5.86

    2.10

    9.52

    1.05

    8.91

    2.59

    8.70

    1.72

    Note.

      SIST = self-instructional strategy training; SIST+SR =  self-

    instructional strategy training plus self-regulation.

    instruction the performance of the normally achieving stu-

    dents was significantly higher than the average pretest per-

    formance of the two instructional groups of LD students (M

    = 5.86). As noted previously, the pretest performance of the

    two groups did no t differ significantly.

    Neither the LD nor the normally achieving students scored

    near the max imum possible on total story grammar elements

    (to avoid ceiling effects, the scale was designed so that 19

    points were possible only for unusual, exemplary stories).

    However, the change in the writing of the LD students was

    clearly educationally significant. Nodine et al. (1985) indi-

    cated that the minimal essential components of a creative

    story are a ma in charac ter, goal, action, and end ing. Although

    only

     36%

     of the LD su bjects' stories at pretest m et this criteria,

    91   , 73%, an d 73% of their stories met this criteria at posttest,

    maintenance, and generalization, respectively. Among the

    normally achieving subjects,  83% of stories met this criterion.

    Furthermore, none of the LD subjects evidenced any debili-

    tative effects from training. Total story grammar element

    scores improved from pre- to posttest for 20 of the 22 LD

    subjects, but scores for the other 2 subjects, who had attained

    the two highest scores on the pretest, remained unchanged

    from pre- to posttest. Finally, 86% of LD students' posttest

    stories contained seven of the eight story gram mar elements

    possible; only 36% of pretest writing probes met this criteria.

    Individual E lement Scores

    The scores for each of the separate story gramm ar elements

    were analyzed using a one-way multivariate analysis of vari-

    ance with the four trials as a factor. Because previous analyses

    indicated no significant differences between the two groups

    of LD students on total story gramm ar elemen ts score, effects

    of group mem bership were no t analyzed. R esults were eval-

    uated based on Rao's approximation of WUks's lambda. A

    mu ltivariate effect for trials was obtained, F (24, 163) = 3.05,

    p <   .01. Separate univa riate analyses revealed that significant

    trial effects were obtained for the following story grammar

    elements: time, F(3, 63) = 9.07, p

  • 8/17/2019 Components Analysis of Cognitive Strategy Instruction

    7/9

    COMPONENTS ANALYSIS

      OF

      COGNITIVE STRATEGY INSTRUCTION

    359

    Table 3

    Learning Disabled Students' Mean

     Scores

     and

     Percentage

     of Stories

     Incorporating

     Each

    Story Grammar Element

    Story grammar

    element

    Main character

    Locale

    Time

    Starter event

    Goal

    Action

    Ending

    Reaction

    Pretest

    M

    1.02

    1.02

    .64

    .27

    .73

    1.25

    .59

    .36

     

    100

    91

    73

    36

    50

    86

    64

    36

    Posttest

    M

    .95

    .95

    1.25

    .77

    1.82

    1.93

    1.02

    .77

    %

    95

    91

    95

    82

    100

    95

    91

    77

    Generalization

    M

    .93

    1.14

    1.23

    .59

    1.39

    1.77

    1.00

    .91

     

    91

    100

    91

    64

    77

    86

    91

    82

    Maintenance

    M

    .98

    1.00

    1.14

    .48

    1.64

    1.98

    .95

    .70

     

    95

    100

    91

    64

    86

    95

    86

    68

    Note. These values are based on the 22 students from the  self-instructional strategy training and self-

    instructional strategy training plu s self-regulation training groups.

    Strategy Usage

    Evidence on  strategy usage provided validation of instruc-

    tional manipulations. E xamination of the papers that students

    wrote on  during  the  writing probes revealed that  the  story

    grammar mnemonic and  responses were generated by 91%,

    91%, and  100% of the subjects during the posttest, generali-

    zation, and m aintenance probes, respectively.

    Self-Efficacy

    A 2 (instructional groups) x  2 (trials) ANOVA with repeated

    measures design was used to  examine the effects  of strategy

    instruc tion an d strategy instruc tion p lus self-regulation on LD

    students' self-efficacy scores and to  determine  if pre- and

    posttest scores differed significantly. A lthough

     the F

     ratio

     for

    trials w as significant, F{\

    t

     20) =  43.5, p 

  • 8/17/2019 Components Analysis of Cognitive Strategy Instruction

    8/9

    360

    STEVE GRAHAM  AND KAREN  R.  HARRIS

    mately half the length of  those of  their normally achieving

    peers.

      Thus, further instruction could emphasize writing

    longer stories (e.g., adding more detail) while maintaining

    schematic integrity.

    A control group was not included in  this study because it

    was not allowed by th e scho ol system for ethical reasons, and

    a wait-list control group

      was not

      possible because

      of the

    approaching end of the  school year. Practice effects might

    therefore

     be

     suggested

     as

     a counterexplanation of he improve-

    me nt in L D subjects* com positions. T his is unlikely, however.

    Significant com position deficits are well-substantiated am ong

    LD students, and  practice alone is unlikely to  result in im-

    provement (Barenbaum et aL, 1987; Graham  & Harris, in

    press;

     Nodine

     et al.,

      1985). Furthermore,

      in

      another study

    using the same pictorial writing prompts, G raham et al. (1988)

    found that  LD  subjects' writing performance actually de-

    creased slightly in a  practice-only condition. In addition, no

    significant differences existed between  the  instructional

    groups on  important subject variables such as IQ and pretest

    performance, thus strengthening experimental control.

     Fi-

    nally, evidence was  obtained that subjects used the strategy

    both during  and  after instruction, thus strengthening  the

    argument that instruction mediated performance.

    Incremental E ffects

    Interestingly,  no  incremental effects  due to  explicit  self-

    regulation of strategic performance were found  in  terms of

    the schematic structure of LD  students' compositions, the

    quality  of  stories,  the  educational significance  of  their im-

    provem ent, or th eir judg me nts (self-efficacy) concern ing th eir

    capability

     to

     write stories.

     It

     should be noted that we

     did not

    compare

     the

     presence

     of

      self-regulation with

     the

     absence

     of

    self-regulation. Rather,  the  comparison made  was between

    strategy instruction combined with instruction in and use of

    explicit  self-regulation procedures (goal setting, self-assess-

    ment, and self-recording) and  strategy instruction alone. It is

    likely, however, that th e strategy instruc tion include d implicit

    self-regulation information  and may  thus have induced less

    formal self-regulation procedures. Theorists have noted that

    a self-regulation fun ction is inhere nt in strategy interventions

    such as self-instructional training (Meichenbaum,  1983; Press-

    ley et al., in press). Indeed, it would be difficult  to conceive

    of any meaningful strategy instruction and acquisition pro-

    cedures in which self-regulation  is  neither implicit nor

    induced. It has  been argued, however, that explicit  self-

    regulation instruction will significantly augm ent strategic per-

    formance

     and is

     particularly impo rtant

     in

     obtaining general-

    ization and maintenance (cf. Brown et al.,  198 1; Pressley et

    al.,

     in press; Pressley & Levin, 1986).

    Clearly, both replication of the  results of  this study and

    further com ponents analysis research are needed. Future stud-

    ies, however, might profitably focus on the characteristics as

    well as the  components  of  instruction. Noteworthy here is

    that instruction focused  on a  meaningful academic task,

    emphasized interactive learning, was provided by preservice

    teachers  in the  students' schools,  and was  criterion-based

    rather than time-based. Any or all of these characteristics may

    be related

     to the

      lack

     of

      incremental effects

      due to

      explicit

    self-regulation  in  this study. Furthermore, future research

    should explore the possibility that explicit self-regulation en-

    hances regulation and effects  of succeeding strategies tau ght

    or acquired. Finally, the  inclusion of  explicit self-regulation

    procedures was in no way "costly," and students frequently

    mentioned "the graphs" as one of their favorite instructional

    components (consistent with

     our

     earlier work;

     cf.

     Graham

     &

    Harris, 1989). Explicit self-regulation appears to have strong

    social validity.

    Self-Efficacy

    A major issue in the pres ent investigation w as the effects of

    the two-strategy intervention conditions  on  subjects'  self-

    efficacy. Self-efficacy  is postulated to have a causal effect on

    performance  as it  mediates choice of  activities, expenditure

    of effort, and persistence in the face of difficulty. Furth erm ore,

    explicit, proxima l goal-setting procedures, su ch as those used

    in the present inv estigation, have been effective

      in

     increasing

    children's self-efficacy (Bandu ra & Sch unk, 1981). Th us, aug-

    mental effects of explicit in struction in goal setting and self-

    monitoring appeared likely. Such effects were  not realized;

    self-efficacy increased significantly among subjects  in  both

    conditions.

    Equally as notable as the lack of augmen tal effects was the

    relatively high level  of  self-efficacy among  LD  students  at

    pretest. The ability to assess on e's own capabilities, and par-

    ticularly the ability to  know that  one has a  problem, is an

    important metacognitive skill (Brown  et al., 1981;  Harris,

    Graham,  &  Freeman, 1988). A  growing body  of research

    indicates that young children and pro blem learners experience

    significant difficulties with predicting or  assessing their per-

    formance

      (cf.

     Brown

     et

     al.,  1981; Harris

     et

     al., 1988).

     In the

    present study,  LD  students consistently overestimated their

    composition abilities. Bandura and Schunk (1981) noted the

    same problem  of  overestimation among children making

    judgm ents of ma them atical self-efficacy; such o verestimation

    was attributed to misperceptions of task demands, faulty self-

    knowledge, and selective atten tion to mastered, as opposed to

    unmastered, task elements. Unreatistically high pretask ex-

    pectancies may also be due to  comprehension deficiencies,

    use of a  self-protective coping strategy, or a  developmental

    delay in the  ability  to  match task demands  to  ability level

    (Harris et al., 1988). Thus, further research on pretask expec-

    tancies may be important in understanding problem learners.

    Although

     the

     need

     for

     further research is evident,

     the

     present

    study adds to a growing body of research that indicates th at

    sound strategy instruction produces meaningful results.

    References

    Bandura, A., &  Schunk,  D.  (1981). Cultivating competence,  self-

    efficacy,  and  intrinsic interest through proximal self-motivation.

    Journal of Personality an d Social

     Psychology,

      41 ,

     586-598.

    Barenbaum,

      E.,

      Newcomer,

      P., &

     Nodine,

      B.

      (1987). Children's

    ability to write stories as a function  of variation in  task, age, and

    developmental level.

     Learning Disability

     Quarterly,

      10,

     175-188.

    Brown,

     A. L.,

     Campione

    ?

     J. C, & Day, J. D.

     (1981). Learning

     to

  • 8/17/2019 Components Analysis of Cognitive Strategy Instruction

    9/9

    COMPONENTS ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE STRATEGY INSTRUCTION

    361

    learn: On training students to learn from texts.

     Educational Re-

    searcher,

      10, 14-21.

    Th e California Achievement Tests: Technical bulletin No. 1

     (1979).

    Monterey, CA: CTB/McGraw-HUL

    Dunn, L. M., & Markwardt, F. C. (1970).

      Peabody Individual

    Achievement Test. Circle Pines, MN : American G uidance Service.

    Elliott-Faust, D . J., & Pressley, M. (1986). How to teach comparison

    processing to increase children's short- and long-term listening

    comprehension monitoring.

      Journal of Educational Psychology,

    78 ,  27-33.

    Englert, C. S., & Thomas, C. C. (1987). Sensitivity to text structure

    in reading and writing: A comparison between learning disabled

    and non-learning disabled students.

     Learning Disability

     Quarterly,

    10,

     93-105.

    Graham, S. (1982). Composition research and practice: A unified

    approach.

     Focus on

     Exceptional

     Children, 14,

     1-16.

    Gra ham , S., & Harris, K. R. (198 9). Cognitive training: Implications

    for written language. In J. Hughes & R. Hall (Eds.),

     Cognitive

    behavioral psychology

     in the schools: A

      comprehensive

     handbook

    (pp. 247-279). New York: Guilford Press.

    Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (in press). Improving learning disabled

    students' skills at composing essays: Self-instructional strategy

    training.

     Exceptional Children.

    Grah am, S., Harris, K. R., & Sawyer, R. (1987). Composition instruc-

    tion with learning disabled students: Self-instructional strategy

    training.

     Focus on Exceptional Children, 20,

      1-11.

    Gra ham , S., Harris, K. R ., & Sawyer, R. (1988). [Improving learning

    disabled students' composition skills with story grammar strategy

    training: A second comp onents analysis of self-instructional strat-

    egy

     training].

     Unpublished raw data.

    Hammill, D,, & Larsen, S. (1983).

      Test of written

     language.  Austin,

    TX : Pro-Ed.

    Harris, K. R. (1985). Conceptual, methodological, and clinical issues

    in cognitive-behavioral assessment.

      Journal of Abnormal Child

    Psychology,

      13 ,

     373-390.

    Harris, K. R. (1986a). The effects of cognitive-behavior modification

    on private speech and task performance during problem solving

    among learning disabled and normally achieving children.

     Journal

    of Abnormal Child Psychology,  14,

     63-77.

    Harris, K. R. (198 6b). Self-monitoring of attentiona l behavior versus

    self-monitoring of productivity: Effects on on-task behavior and

    academic response rate am ong learning disabled children.

     Journal

    ofApplied

     Behavior

     Analysis, 19,

     417-423.

    Harris, K. R.,

      &

      Graham, S. (1985). Improving learning disabled

    students' composition skills: Self-control strategy training.

     Learning

    Disability

     Quarterly,

     8,

     27-36.

    Harris, K. R., Graham, S.,

      &

      Freeman, S. (1988). The effects of

    strategy training and study conditions on metamemory among

    learning disabled students.

     Exceptional Children, 54,

     332-338.

    Hobbs, S. A., Moguin, L. E., Tyroler,  U.

    f

      & Lahey, B. B. (1980).

    Cognitive behavior therapy with children: Has clinical utility been

    demonstrated?

     Psychological

     Bulletin, 87, 147-165.

    MacA rthur, C , & Grah am, S. (1987). Learning disabled students'

    composing with three methods; Handwriting, dictation, and word

    processing.

     Journal of Special Education, 21(3),

     22-42.

    Meiehenbaum, D. (1977).

     Cognitive behavior modification: An inte-

    grative approach.

     New York: Plenum Press.

    Meiehenbaum , D. (1983). Teaching thinking: A cognitive-behavioral

    approach.

      Interdisciplinary  voices  in learning  disabilities and re-

    medial education

      (pp. 1-28), Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Nodine, B., Barenbaum, E., & Newcomer, P. (1985). Story compo-

    sition by learning disabled, reading disabled, and normal children.

    Learning

     Disability

     Quarterly,, 8,  167-181.

    O'Leary, S. G., & D ubey, D . R. (1979). Applications of self-control

    procedures by children: A review.

      Jo urnal of Applied Behavior

    Analysis, 12,

     449-465.

    Pressley, M., Forrest-Pressley, D., & Elliott-Faust, D. J. (in press).

    How to study strategy instructional en richment: Illustrations from

    research on children's prose memory and comprehension. In F.

    Weinert & M. Perlmutter (Eds.),

     Memory

     development:

      Universal

    changes a nd individual

     development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Pressley, M., & Levin, J. R. (1986). Elaborative learning strategies for

    the inefficient learner. In S. J. Ceci (Ed),

     H andbook of cognitive,

    social,

      an d neuropsychological aspects  of learning disabilities

      (pp.

    175-211). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum .

    Reeve, R. A., & Brown, A. L. (1985). Metacognition reconsidered:

    Implications for intervention research.

     Journa l of Abnorm al Child

    Psychology,

      13,

      343-356.

    Short, E. J., & Ryan, E. B. (1984). Metacognitive differences between

    skilled and less skilled readers: Remediating deficits through story

    grammar and attribution training.

     Journal of Educational

     Psychol-

    ogy,

     76, 225-235.

    Slosson, R. L. (1971).

     Slosson Intelligence

     Test.  East Aurora, NY:

    Slosson Educ ational Publications.

    Stein, N. L,, & Glenn, C. C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehen-

    sion in elementary school children. In R . O. Freedle (Ed.),

     Advances

    in discourse processes:

     Vol.

     2. New directions  in discourse process-

    ing.

     Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Thoradike, R., & Hagen, E. (1978).

     Cognitive Abilities

     Test.  Boston:

    Houghton Mifflin.

    Wechsler, D. (1974).

     Manua l for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for

    Children-Revised.

      New York: Psychological C orporation.

    Received September 2, 1987

    Revision received March 10, 1989

    Accepted March 20, 1989 •