82
Cultural Complexity Wolfgang Fikentscher SFI WORKING PAPER: 1998-10-087 SFI Working Papers contain accounts of scientific work of the author(s) and do not necessarily represent the views of the Santa Fe Institute. We accept papers intended for publication in peer-reviewed journals or proceedings volumes, but not papers that have already appeared in print. Except for papers by our external faculty, papers must be based on work done at SFI, inspired by an invited visit to or collaboration at SFI, or funded by an SFI grant. ©NOTICE: This working paper is included by permission of the contributing author(s) as a means to ensure timely distribution of the scholarly and technical work on a non-commercial basis. Copyright and all rights therein are maintained by the author(s). It is understood that all persons copying this information will adhere to the terms and constraints invoked by each author's copyright. These works may be reposted only with the explicit permission of the copyright holder. www.santafe.edu SANTA FE INSTITUTE

Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Cultural ComplexityWolfgang Fikentscher

SFI WORKING PAPER: 1998-10-087

SFI Working Papers contain accounts of scientific work of the author(s) and do not necessarily represent theviews of the Santa Fe Institute. We accept papers intended for publication in peer-reviewed journals or proceedings volumes, but not papers that have already appeared in print. Except for papers by our externalfaculty, papers must be based on work done at SFI, inspired by an invited visit to or collaboration at SFI, orfunded by an SFI grant.©NOTICE: This working paper is included by permission of the contributing author(s) as a means to ensuretimely distribution of the scholarly and technical work on a non-commercial basis. Copyright and all rightstherein are maintained by the author(s). It is understood that all persons copying this information willadhere to the terms and constraints invoked by each author's copyright. These works may be reposted onlywith the explicit permission of the copyright holder.www.santafe.edu

SANTA FE INSTITUTE

Page 2: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

C U L T U R A L C O M P L E X I T Y

Wolfgang Fikentscher

98-09-087

Santa Fe Institute

This research was supported in part by grants to the Santa

Fe Institute, including core funding from the John D. and

Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation; the National Science

Foundation (PHY 9021437); the U.S. Department of Energy (DE-

FG05-88ER25054); and the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft,

Bonn (Fi 89/7-1).

Page 3: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Contents

Introduction 4

I. Definitions 6

1. System and "Less Than Systems" 62. Complexity 133. Adaptation. Kinds of Systems 14

II. Culture and Cultures as Matters of Complexity 21

1. Culture, Cultures: Definitions 212. The "Point of the Ought" 223. Subdivisions of Culture 234. Modes of Thought, and the Inside-Outside Issue 265. Complexity and Development of Cultures 316. Understanding the Past 38

III. Culture(s) and Modes of Thought as Complex Adaptive Systems (CAS) 41

IV. Some Implications 43

1. The Ontology-Metaphor Problem 442. Anthropological Systems Theory 543. Artificial Culture(s)? 554. Recognition of Cultures 58

V. Summary 59

VI. 12 Theses 59

Sources for the Figures to "Cultural Complexity" 64

References 65

Page 4: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

List of Figures

1 The curve of the invisible hand 7

2 a,b,c Sketches of systems 8,9

3 Thought-modal kinds of orderings 12

4 Concepts of time 19

5 Themes of Culture 24

6 Modes of thought 28

7 Offices in the Pueblos 33

8 Ontological and metaphorical reasoning 46,47

9 Classical subdivisions of anthropology 52

10 Anthropology as an integrated system 53

Page 5: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Introduction

This paper entitled "Cultural Complexity" is divided

into four parts1.

The first section (I.) consists of definitions which

center around the concept of complexity. As a

minimum, the terms, system, complexity, and

adaptation as used in this paper need an

explanation. The second section (II.) is a sketch of

cultural complexity in the real world. I will use

Pueblo tribal laws and other ethnographic material

as examples of that complexity. This part draws on

    1 This Working Paper draws from five unprinted presentations

in the Santa Fe Institute: " Modes of Thought in Law: PuebloTribal Court Studies in Cultural Complexity", January 17,1992; "Cultural Complexity in Human Behavior", December 15,1995; "Behavior: A Taxonomic Attempt", February 7, 1996(with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for aStudy in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute ScienceSymposium, March 9, 1996; and "Cultural Complexity", March22, 1996. - I want to thank the SFI, its directors andstaff, for assisting me with a desk, and other workingfacilities twice, in 1991/92 and 1995/96, and thus enablingme to gather legal-ethnographic material on Native Americanreservations, in particular in the Rio Grande Pueblos. Thisresearch contributed to the present paper on "culturalcomplexity". - I am indebted to Kai Fikentscher for readinga draft and commenting on it. - Other articles reportingresearch results from my Pueblo studies are listed in thereferences, infra. - A terminological remark: Pueblo denotesa certain geographic place, such as Zia Pueblo, NM. A Pueblomay also be the inhabitant of a pueblo (Puebloan isobsolete). Thus, Pueblos may be the plural of anyone of thetwenty still existing Pueblos in New Mexico and Arizona, orinhabitants of them. The word Pueblo is also used forhistoric, no longer existing Pueblos such as Pecos Pueblo,Guisewa Pueblo (near Jemez Pueblo), San Lazaro Pueblo (nearSanta Fe), etc. On the other hand, pueblo denotes anySpanish or Indian settlement in the form of a little ormedium-sized town or village after the Spanish colonization.

Page 6: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

anthropological fieldwork on the customary laws of

Southwestern Indian tribes, especially the Pueblos

of New Mexico and Arizona. With intermissions, this

fieldwork took place between 1987 and 1998,

comprising periods of time that add up more than

thirteen months.

This research included the internal laws ("tribal

law") of the following nations: White Mountain

Apache, San Carlos Apache, Jicarilla Apache,

Yavapai-Apache, Pascua Yaqui, Tohono O`odham, Pima-

Maricopa, Navajo, Kaibab Paiute, Moapa Paiute, Las

Vegas Paiute, Taos Pueblo, Picuris Pueblo, San Juan

Pueblo, Santa Clara Pueblo, Pojoaque Pueblo, Nambe

Pueblo, San Ildefonso Pueblo, Tesuque Pueblo, Jemez

Pueblo, Zia Pueblo, Cochiti Pueblo, Santa Domingo

Pueblo, Santa Ana Pueblo (I and II), San Felipe

Pueblo, Sandia Pueblo, Isleta Pueblo, Laguna Pueblo,

Acoma, Zuni, and Hopi.2

The third part of the paper is (III.) what a lawyer

would call a subsumption, and a logician a minor

premise. In a brief but central remark I would like

to state that cultural variety (II.) meets the

requirements of complexity in the defined sense

(I.).

    2 R.D. Cooter and W. Fikentscher (1992), with revisions 1994,

1995, 1997 and 1998.

Page 7: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

A fourth and final section (IV.) offers some

implications of this theoretical point of departure.

The implications are, in part, of philosophical

nature, and, among other aspects, deal with the

ontological and epistemological difficulties of

drawing conclusions in complex settings (in nature

and culture). Ontology in this sense is - in short -

the philosophy of truth, and epistemology the

philosophy of discovering truth. A summary (V.) and

some theses (VI.) conclude the paper.

I. Definitions

Intuitively, cultures appear complex, and culture in

the singular ("holistic meaning" of culture) also

seems to be complex. Do the cultures, and does

culture, meet the essentials of a complex adaptive

system? This depends on the definitions of

complexity, adaptiveness, and system.

1. System and "Less Than Systems"

John Holland begins his seminal book on the "Hidden

Order" of things with the question, how in a big

city like New York everyone gets his or her

breakfast. Holland calls this a mystery, a magic,

because he sees no system that takes care of the

supply (Holland 1995, p. 1).

Page 8: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

The mystery unravels when the observer turns from

the system within the context of the natural

sciences to the system as a methodological concept

within the humanities and the social sciences. The

latter concept works, in a scientific way, with

values, and value judgments. Of course, in the first

place one has to accept that working with values in

a scientific sense is possible. David Hume denies

this, Immanuel Kant affirms it, and here I follow

Kant (for details, see Fikentscher 1995a, 39;

concisely stated by Hahlbrock 1995). The fact that

social sciences are sciences in this country, but

humanities in Europe, tells a story about the Kant

reception in the U.S.

The values that regulate the availability of

breakfast in New York are technically often called

preferences (Raiffa 1969; Tietz 1981; Fikentscher

1981). Economic value judgments within a market

defined by alternatives lead to a system of

competing offers at diminishing returns, at least in

the bulk markets (W. Brian Arthur 1994; 1995;

Waldrop 1992, 252). Such a market attracts suppliers

of breakfast wherever there is a demand, at prices

near cost, as long as there is enough competition.

Competitive markets follow what Adam Smith has

Page 9: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

called the "invisible hand". Figure 1 attempts to

make the invisible hand visible:

(Figure 1: The Curve of the Invisible Hand)

Every offeror, and every customer, finds its place

along this curve, because the curve represents a

system, the system of a market, the term market

being understood in a subjective, perceived sense

(Fikentscher 1996).

This illustrates the idea that a system is a

combination of a general principle, the so-called

genus proximus, which here is the working of the

invisible hand, and one or more applications of this

principle, the so-called differentia specifica, here

the spots where suppliers and customers find

themselves along the curve of the invisible hand.

(Figures 2 a,b, and c: Sketches of Systems)

From the differentia specifica 1 and 2 a principle

is derived (inferred, induced). The principle (= the

genus proximus) is then applied (deduced) to a new

specific 3. Example: cranes (point 1) and gulls

(point 2) are orders of birds (a class). So are

Page 10: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

storks (point 3). Systems serve to reasonably define

items in two directions, genus proximus and

differentia specifica.

The following two examples demonstrate incorrect

systematic reasoning. A system does not emerge if

the principle does not cover the third point (Figure

2b): A bat, although flying, is not a bird. Nor does

a system emerge if the principle does cover that

point, but the third point does not belong to the

same level of abstraction as points 1 and 2 (Figure

2c): Point 3a is a subspecies of gulls, Point 3b is

archaeopterix belonging to the underclass of

archaeornithes.

We learn from the New York breakfast example that

there are systems observable that may concern values

and value judgments. Moreover, we note that such

systems may be quite complex: There is a multitude

of birds. Thus, there are complex value systems, or,

to say the same in other words: There is cultural

complexity.

Through its constituent parts, that is, the

principle, and the items brought under the

principle, a system brings order into a disorderly,

motley world. However, there are many more ways to

order things besides by a system. The system is an

Page 11: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

invention of the ancient Greek mind. Herodotus

wondered why the Persians and the Egyptians did not

know the system, and accordingly distinguished the

Greek, the Persian and the Egyptian mind (logos). In

cultural anthropology, it appears that the system is

a culture-specific trait (Fikentscher 1975-77: vol.

IV (1977), 108ff.; idem 1995a, 157ff., 183, 351,

465).

Viehweg (1993, 27; Jacob 1995, 19, consenting) warns

us: "But if we succumbed to that impulse (of

systematization) we would undercut the actual

intention of the topoi ... In the end, we would be

forced to ascertain a clearly discernible rift

yawning between the system we have developed and the

world of problems, which through all these

systematizing efforts has lost none of its

problematical character. Obviously we would have

disturbed originally complex interconnections. There

seems to be an inter-relationship that cannot be

simplified to a merely logical one, so that our

deductive systematizing efforts in the end yield

only constructions that are isolated and do not

matter."

Therefore, let us look to other ways of ordering

items. Another way to order things is to list them

in form of a mere compilation - possibly even at the

edge of chaos; or assort them serially, that is,

Page 12: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

either by way of a simple series such as the letters

in an alphabet, or in an associative series as in

story-telling, or as a topical series, such as in

aspective art (Brunner-Traut 1990; Viehweg 1993;

Jacob 1995); or in a cyclical manner like the

unending cycles of life in the Karma belief of

Brahmanism. The various ways of ordering things

reflect what, in anthropology, is called the modes

of thought (Fikentscher 1995).

Hence, system is a qualified order. Compilation;

simple, associative, and topical series; and

cyclicity may suitably be identified as "less-than-

systematic" orderings. Consequently, once we will

have added complexity (2) and adaptiveness (3),

there will be complex adaptive systems, and complex

adaptive less-than-systems.

Page 13: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

A non-exclusive survey of orderings shows:

Figure 3: Thought-modal kinds of orderings

kinds examples

compilation faunal population on a new volcanic island,

result of a brainstorming session, Islamic umma

(congregation)

simple series alphabet, brotherhood in segmented society

(Evans-Pritchard), late renaissance suite

associative series biblical story-telling by word identities,

most fairy tales

topical series mnemotechnic rhyme, Bach suite,

aspective presentation (Ancient Egyptian art),

older brother - younger brother - relation in

Confucianist 5-rules ethics

cyclicity harvest cycles, powwow dance, Reigen dance,

karma belief

system Linnaeus`botanical work,

table of chemical elements,

Greek polis,

monograph by Ramee 1570, treatise, Pueblo dance,

Shakespearean plot, Latin Grammar

Page 14: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

2. Complexity

Complexity is a property of things making them fit

to be ordered, whether by self-organization or by

some outside effort. Complexity does not imply the

way in which things have to be ordered. It can be

ordered in a compilatory; simple, associative or

topical serial; cyclical; systematic; or possibly

still other way. Thus, complexity is not a direct

correlate to a certain kind of ordering, e.g., a

system. Usually there are a large number of things

to be ordered, and after having been ordered, their

disorder turns into the complexity of compilations,

serials, cycles, or systems. In the following

discussion, I want to concentrate on systems3.

It follows that reductionism and complexity are not

mutually exclusive: Ongoing systematic (and other

ordering) work, in nature or in culture, is

necessarily, of a reductionist nature. This is so

because it concerns complex details, for instance of

a system, and thus thrives on complexity (the

differentia specifica) while reducing it to

scientifically manageable chunks (the genera

    3 The interpretation of a galaxy as a vortex attributes to the

galaxy the essence of a complex system. On other ways ofordering things, Fikentscher 1995a, 175-188. On systems, seeCanaris 1969, 1983.

Page 15: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

proxima). Hence, complexity should not be invoked

against reductionism.

The opposite of complex is simple. There are simple

compilations, series, cycles, or systems. Simple

topically serial orders are, e.g., the two last emu

birds before their species became extinct, the

Confucianist older brother - younger brother

relation, or a mnemotechnic rhyme.

By referring to ongoing systematic work, the element

of time is introduced, and by adding time to system

and complexity, we approach a definition of

adaptation.

3. Adaptation. Kinds of Systems

In order to understand adaptation, two different

kinds of systems have to be distinguished, namely

closed and open ones4. A steam regulator that

throttles down the steam pressure whenever the

pressure is rising, is the classic example of a

closed system. Closed systems have been described by

Richard Wagner, Norbert Wiener, Niklas Luhmann and

    4 Canaris, 1969, 1983; Fikentscher 1975-77: vol. I (1975) 568;

vol. II (1975) 64, 68f., 252; vol. IV (1977) 115ff., 162ff., 380; both with further materials. The openness of thesystem is generated by the open extension of the genusproximus: in Figures 2 a,b,c, the upper line is open at bothends.

Page 16: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

others. Closed systems are time-neutral in that they

do not evolve over time. Closed systems do not adapt

to outside factors.

a) Most systems are open. This means they work

across time. They develop. Hence, they are able to

adapt. Moreover, they may be complex. Open systems

are, e.g., botanical systems as long as additional

species are developing or being developed (e.g., by

breeding); legal systems; or a city growing

according to city-planning.

b) But the steam regulator does not evolve, and a

symphony does not develop, or adapt. Steam

regulators and symphonies are closed systems. Take a

Beethoven symphony: If the orchestra were to leave

out the second movement, or ten measures in the

scherzo, most people in the audience would say:

"There`s something missing". This is a test that

these compositions are closed systems, because it is

to be assumed of an open system that there are still

differentia specifica missing. (What can be adapted,

though, is the systematic way in which the conductor

conducts the symphony, for example in correspondence

to the dominant pseudoromantic taste. The art of

conducting is a complex adaptive system, it is

open.)

Page 17: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Closed systems are unable to evolve or adapt. A

crystal which no longer grows and has no chance to

grow is a closed system.

c) From the foregoing it follows that the term

"dynamic system" can be used in two senses, focusing

on either open or closed systems:

Since an open system can only exist in the course of

time, "dynamic" may just mean "open". Examples are:

A recently discovered kind of bird fits under the

genus proximus "bird", and the word "recently"

demonstrates the openness over time of the class

"bird". The law of the sea may develop to be applied

by way of analogy to the law of outer space. One way

to express this is that systems develop, or evolve,

or adapt. A system can be "dynamic" in this

developing, evolving, or adapting sense. If it is

complex, it is a complex adaptive system. In this

context, "dynamic" designates nothing more than the

openness of a system.

However, if somebody says: A steam regulator is

functioning and therefore is working "dynamically",

or: this Beethoven Symphony is "dynamic", "dynamic"

is used in a different way: It assumes the sense of

Page 18: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

"functioning", or "working". No development takes

place.

d) Focusing on open systems, social sciences combine

system and time in the sense of the Great Antinomy

as defined by the economist Walter Eucken: In the

social sciences, every issue can only meaningfully

be raised, and answered, both in terms of system and

time ,5 (and by the way, only if two more categories

are added, evaluation, and method (Fikentscher 1975-

77: vol. IV 1977, 357).

Open systems are more frequent than closed systems.

Open systems involve the factor of time. There are

numerous concepts of time. In Western culture

(Fikentscher 1995, 355 ff.), open systems are able

to imply only one understanding of time, namely,

"time as a straight line".

For "Westerners", open systems and time as a

straight line constitute one another (Fikentscher

1975-77: vol. IV 1977, 19 ff.).

Time as a straight line is the understanding of time

used, in Western culture, to interpret evolution.

    5 Eucken 1950, 34; see also de Saussure 1916; Fikentscher

1960, where the distinction between synchrony (system, notime) and diachrony (time, no system) in various socialsciences is discussed.

Page 19: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Therefore, to speak of a complex adaptive system, in

the sense as it is developed here, is just another

way of saying that there are systemic rules in

evolution. It is promising to catch evolutionary

systemic rules under the conceptual net of complex

adaptive systems because, in Western thought, this

helps to understand evolution as a process.

Note, however, that most non-Western cultures do not

systematize.

This is a sweeping statement, and it requires

substantiation. This, in turn, requires a definition

of the term "system". For this definition, reference

may be made to the preceding text under I.1., supra

(a system is a combination of a general principle

and one or more applications of this principle to

specifics, applications that deserve, under

ontologies and epistemologies still to be chosen,

the judgment "true"). Thus, a system always defines

a specific from at least to points of view: the

principle and the comparison with other specifics.

Graphically, a system needs at least two lines, and

connecting lines between those two lines (see

Figures 2 a,b,c). Hence, systematic thinking is

perspective (Fikentscher 1975 - 1977, IV, 61-107).

Perspective thinking is an attribute of Greek

(today: Western) thinking (Hamburger 1967; Richter

Page 20: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

1970; Brunner-Traut 1990; Fikentscher 1975-1977, I

170-234, 398 f., 414 ff; IV 116; V 20-27; 1995, 183,

292, 349 ff., 379 ff., 393, 397-401, 431 ff., 460

ff.). Non-"Greek" modes of thought, and the cultures

influenced by them, often prefer compilatory, serial

(especially topical, Viehweg 1993; Jacob 1995; see

I. 1., supra), and cyclical orderings of things (a

summary: Otte 1970).

It follows from the foregoing that, empirically, one

can check this non-systematic ordering in non-

Western cultures by investigating the time concepts

of these cultures. They correlate to culturally

specific ways of ordering things:

(Figure 4.: Concepts of Time)

It is "time as a straight line" that is at the

bottom of the openness of a system (see Figures 2 a,

b, c). Other concepts of time may but do not

necessarily conflict with systematic thinking.

E.g., the "perspective antiquarianism" of the

humanists (" ad fontes " - and nothing between those

"fontes" and yourself), or the "aspective use of

historical development for the explanation of the

present" ("pure Roman law" is to be preferred over

Page 21: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

epigonic developments) tend to permit only closed

systems. This is important, for example, for the

understanding of texts of legal history.6

The "understandings of time" in Figure 4 are not the

only possible ones. Empirical observation of the

real world may produce more (Kirste 1998;

Fikentscher 1975-1977, IV, 76 ff., 637, 650; 1995,

181 ff., 238, 289 ff., 334, 376, 394, 419 ff., 452,

473), and computational theory seems to permit new

concepts of time for chosen purposes, unknown to the

real world.

The evolutionary tendency which is part and parcel

of complex adaptive systems gives these systems the

inherent quality of a pluralist "ought": They are on

the way to developing into subsequent evolutiony

states. The "ought" implied in this development is

open and indeterministic. Consequently, it is a

noticeable limitation to the argument of the

naturalistic fallacy. The implications for natural

law theories, and for Kant`s categorical imperative

cannot be discussed here (see, for the time being,

Fikentscher 1997, 43 f.).

    6 A similar statement is made by Dieter Nörr (1972).

Page 22: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

II. Culture and Cultures as Matters of Complexity

1. Culture, Cultures: Definitions .

The word culture may be used with regard to a

specific culture (a) ("Inuit culture") or as an

abstract term that denotes "culture as such" (in the

"holistic" sense) (b).

a) Edward Burnett Tylor`s definition of culture in

the specific sense, posited in 1881 (1881,1), is

still applied by most culture comparatists and

anthropologists:

"Culture is that complex whole which includes

knowledge, belief, art, moral, law, custom, and any

other capabilities and habits acquired by man as a

member of society."

With respect to our subject of discussion,

complexity, a more modern form of this definition

may be the following:

"Culture is the complex attribute of a society that

refers to the patterns of conduct of its

participants - traditional but open to change - to

societal situations concerning knowledge, beliefs,

Page 23: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

arts, morals, law, custom, or other mentally

reflected themes" (cf.Fikentscher 1995a, 23).

Thus, a culture (in the specific sense) is not

identical with a society, a concept that focuses on

the relations among individuals and between

individuals and groups; and hence anthropology, the

empirical science of humans as a cultural beings is

not identical with sociology.

b) Culture, without an s (= culture in the

"holistic" sense), is an abstraction of the various

cultures. Thus, it can be said that humankind is

defined by having "culture".

In the following text, both meanings are used, the

specific and the holistic. The context indicates

what is being discussed.

2. The "Point of the Ought" .

Historically, culture (in general) occured when, in

the animal-human-transition field, beings started to

critically relate to their natural (physical and

biological) environment and abilities. This enabled

these beings, from now on entitled to be called

humans, to constrain incest and power behavior

Page 24: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

normatively, and to reflect these normative

constraints back upon nature (hereby forming a

belief system).7

It follows that there is no human society without

law, law forming an essential part of culture. And

once culture had occured, said reflection about

culture and its relation to physical and biological

environment began. Historically, this is the

shaman's task, so that there is no human culture

without a belief system either.8

Let`s call this point of reflection about culture

and nature the "point of the ought". Margaret Mead

called it the point where nurture and nature split.

Murray Gell-Mann defines it as the point from which

on human ideas cause complexity to become

"potential", by which he means that complexity,

having entered the realm of culture, becomes much

more variform and greatly expands.1

    7 See for the pro and con of this normative theory of the

definition of humanity Fikentscher (1975-77), vol. I (1975),71-79; cf., Johanson & Edey, 418 ff.

    8 See the discussion in Fikentscher (ibid.), 79-104.

    9 Gell-Mann (1994) 68 f., 70, 294 ff.; the center of this lineof thought is on p. 70 (potential complexity).

Page 25: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

The reverse conclusion is permitted: Empiry

demonstrates that there is no human society without

a shaman (defined as the functionary of bridging

world and other-world). Therefore, there is no human

society without the shaman`s task to reflect on the

"point of the ought", on the breaking away from

nature by introducing human constraints and

liberations. Thus, even for those who stipulate a

continuum from nature to culture by imperceptible

degrees, a change of quality from not-reflected to

reflected constraints and liberations is inescapable

(Fikentscher and McGuire 1994).

3. Subdivisions of Culture

Culture, thus defined and explained, is subdivided

in various ways, according to different standards of

categorization, e.g., formal (a) and substantive

(b). (This is the material of cultural complexity.)

a) A formal subdivision is the following. It

subdivides a culture by its themes. A culture is

composed of its themes. The term "theme" serves as a

genus proximus and is composed of, but not limited

to, its observable differentia specifica. The

subdivision of a culture according to themes listed

here is, with minor variations (e.g. Pospisil 1986,

Page 26: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

117; Fikentscher 1995, 24), in general use for

teaching purposes with minor variations.

(Figure 5: Themes of Culture)

To illustrate:

- "traits" are the most commonly used "atoms" of a

culture. Pueblo reclusiveness is a cultural trait. A

complex is a set of traits ( e.g. "art").

- Ruth Benedict introduced the term "pattern", as

an English translation of "Gestalt", taken from

German "Gestaltpsychologie". It describes coherent

aggregates of cultural traits such as the proverbial

"peacefulness" of the Hopi, or the - equally

proverbial - nomadic way of life of the Roma and

Sinti (the so-called "gypsies").

- A cultural "purpose" may be the "happiness" of

the people in a socialist culture. It need not be

reached.

- A cultural behavioral "function" may be hegemony,

or - less than that - sovereignty. U.S. Justice

Oliver Wendell Holmes, Jr., defined sovereignty as

Page 27: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

the ability and willingness of a nation, through its

army, to tolerate other nations to exist.10

Covert themes need full familiarity with the

language, and two to three years of living in the

culture observed (communication Pospísil, personal

communication). - The other items of Figure 5 are

self-explanatory.

b) Substantively, there are thousands of cultures in

this world, more than 3000 alone south of the

Sahara. Some say that there are about 10 000

cultures in this world, in history and present. The

Human Relations Area Files (HRAF), George Peter

Murdock`s and others` work at Yale, Pittsburgh etc.,

cover 300 "important" cultures, compiling 700 traits

for each of them, in total 210,000 traits. The

private laws of the twenty Pueblos, collected by

Robert D. Cooter and myself between 1986 and 1996,

consist of overt, ideationally valued, patterns and

traits.11

    10 Holmes-Pollock Letters, The Correspondence of Mr.

Justice Holmes and Sir Frederick Pollock, ed. by Mark deWolfe Howe, Cambridge, Mass., 1961, vol. II, 27.

        11 See footnote 1, supra.

Page 28: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

4. The Modes of Thought, and the Inside-Outside

Issue

Having subdivided cultures to show their complexity,

the question is permitted whether there is a more

"reductionist" unit behind the cultures. It is the

same kind of question that seeks subunits of atoms,

particles of atoms, and as of now leads to the

quarks.

There is indeed a unit behind the cultures. Since

E.B. Tylor, the various cultures were the basic

unit. However, the cultures build upon what may be

called the "modes of thought"(see, e.g. Horton &

Finnegan 1973). For example, the Hinayana-Buddhist

mode of thought is the foundation of Thai culture . I

have attempted to describe this relationship between

the modes of thought and the cultures in my book on

the "Modes of Thought" (1995) and will no longer

dwell on this issue here (cf. Figure 6).

(Figure 6: Modes of Thought)

Page 29: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

However, the modes of thought may be helpful in

solving a well-known problem in anthropology, the

inside-outside issue.12

A culture may be viewed from the outside. This

approach is taken where, e.g., an anthropologist

asks a pueblo judge: "Does the law of your tribe

know intestate succession?" (= inheritance without a

will). The anthropologist who is asking in this

manner applies Western analytical concepts (the

distinction between intestate and willed

inheritance) to tribal law. He or she works

"etically", as the term goes (from phon-etic).

The anthropologist should not ask in this manner,

because tribal inheritance law may be conceptually

different from Western law. For example, tribal law

may know the transfer of property at the time of the

agreement among the heirs apparent on the one side,

and the owner still living and decedent-to-be on the

other. This agreement - a kind of stipulated "civil

death" for the purpose of succession - is a transfer

of an estate by a will inter vivos. To my knowledge,

Western law does not know this kind of transfer.

        12 Pike 1954; Headland, Pike, and Harris 1990.

Page 30: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Thus, the anthropologist should ask conceptually in

terms of the law she or he is going to investigate.

But this law is still unknown to the anthropologist

who sets out to investigate it.

Logically, this impasse is insurmountable. Of

course, an anthropologist knows this seemingly

unsolvable problem, and will carry on the

investigation in the "emic" way (from phon-emic),

once she or he has gained insight of the "inside" of

the culture to be studied.

From this current anthropological practice draws the

conclusion that "emic" is the "folk-way", and "etic"

is the scientific analysis of this "folk-way".

However, this is a rather conceited, ethnocentric

position which strides against the anthropological

principle of the equality of cultures. Because upon

closer examination, the analytic, "etic",

interpretation of culture is nothing but the emic

view of the Western culture;13 so that a seemingly

"etical" analysis of another culture is an

ethnocentric emical effort. Instead of the purported

"etical" objective analysis, nothing other than emic

    13 Pospísil 1978/85, 3 ff.

Page 31: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

is opposed to emic, if in a concealed way, and no

understanding of the other appears to be possible.

The way out to resolve this dilemma of cultural

complexity is shown by the theory of the modes of

thought. A mode of thought is a mind-set that

connects human data perception with mentally

reflected behavior in a culture-shaping way that is

predominantly covert.14 Modes of thought induce a

conceptual and value-related meta-thinking. Thus,

they enable the observer to compare, on a meta-

level, all "emically" analyzed cultures, including

the culture of Western (historically: Greek)

thinking.15

5. Complexity and Development of Cultures

Cultural complexity is to some degree structured

differently from physical and biological complexity.

a) In physics and biology, as a rule evolution

promotes complexity.16 I refer to well-known

phenomena such as the Cambrian explosion, the

    14 Fikentscher (1995a), 21.

    15 Details in Fikentscher (1995a), 116 ff.

 

~  16 Cf. Gell-Mann (1994) 244, 329 ff.

Page 32: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

mutation-selection process, or the niche theory -

developments that underline increasing complexity

over time as developed by Murray Gell-Mann, 1994.

Once in a while, though, events usually called

catastrophes occur, such as the relatively sudden

disappearance of the dinosaurs. Gumerman may have in

mind this slow-rise-sudden-fall pattern when he

speaks of a pattern to the evolution of human

culture, of poorly understood and poorly articulated

laws of social evolution which, of course, do not

exclude the uniqueness of historic details.17

b) In culture, however, complexity and evolution

rather tend to go separate ways. Often, complexity

evolves (for example: Feeley and Lester 1994), but

not always. The following examples from my own

research are intended to illustrate this latter

point.

aa) The Hopi society, to cite one example, is based

on the following highly complex system of cultural

traits (= a complex, see Figure 5, supra):

(1) exogamous households; (2) the central clan

households; (3) exogamous lineages; (4) totemically

    17 I am quoting from a draft note for our Working Group on

human behavior; cf. footnote 1.

Page 33: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

named exogamous clans; (5) phratries, as ceremonial

clan combinations; (6) villages; and (7) nation.18

This system is much more complex than the nuclear

family, county, state and nation system of white

society. I do not see the Western society evolving

towards the more complex Hopi system.

bb) The US have a tripartite government

(legislature, administration, judiciary; following

Montesquieu`s "De l`esprit des lois"). Germany has

four separate powers, the three from Montesquieu`s

repertoire, plus the Bundesbank (the United Kingdom

followed this pattern in 1997, and the European

Union has promised to follow it for the European

currency, the Euro, administered by the European

Central Bank); Taiwan R.O.C. has five separate

powers of government, the Keresan pueblos have

eight, and the Tewa pueblos nine, everyone in

control of all the others. Again, I do not sense any

development from lower to higher complexity.

cc) For the pueblo communities, tradition often

means retaining traditional older customs while

accepting and adding cultural developments. For

example, the Pueblo of Taos long ago added the

    18 See Sekaquaptewa`s (1995) precise analysis.

Page 34: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

buffalo dance, a traditional dance common to many

Plains Indians, to the standard repertoire of the

Taos dances.

Historically, offices in the pueblos were piled on

top of the other, until the octopartite system of

governmental powers emerged which is presently in

use among the Keresan speaking Pueblos. As

mentioned, the Tewa speaking Pueblos have nine

governmental powers since there are two moiety heads

that take turns in guiding the pueblo (Fikentscher

1995 b: 8 ff., 17 ff.); further details in this

respect were discussed in my SFI lecture of Jan. 17,

1992 (Fikentscher 1992).

Figure 7: Offices in the Pueblos (subject tosubstantial variations)

From Pre-Columbian Time:moiety headstribal council (principales, elders. etc.)society headswar captains

Under Spanish Influence:governorlieutenant governoralguacil (sheriff)fiscales (church officials)

Under US-American Influence:modern court

Page 35: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

The cultural complexity of the office and power

situation in the Pueblos is apparent. Here, a trend

towards increased complexity is noticeable, but this

complex system will hardly diffuse to other

cultures. In most other cultures, traditions may

change. But they are not compiled and added up layer

by layer.

dd) Many tribes (including Hopi) have a religious

system that consists of at least four religious

types: Totemism, animism in the narrow sense, magic

and ancestor worship.19 Compared with this elaborate

religious system, Islamic, Judaic and Christian

monotheism, even in the more elaborate Judaic and

Christian versions, is quite simple and not at all

complex (for details see Fikentscher 1995a, chapters

6, 7, 9 and 10). Is there an evolution from less to

more complex? In religious culture, the opposite

seems to prevail. One of the most simplifying events

in the development of the modes of thought and the

cultures was the axial age (700 - 450 B.C.E.). It

was the period during which many religious types of

animism (in a wider sense) became reduced to total

religious (in Pospísil`s terms).

    19 A description of religious types in Fikentscher (1995a) 170-

294.

Page 36: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

c) Maybe it is an anthropological inclination to get

excited when really complex data are being

discovered. Often cultural complexity and its study

seems to come down to a school of circumspection,

and of tolerance. The Chief Judge in one of the

major tribes in the North American Southwest asked

me in 1996: "What do you think tribes are good for?"

Thinking about this question (after having answered

to the Chief Judge in a very unsatisfactory way), I

gathered some possible answers: Let me submit them

to you for critical review:

aa) It has often been stated that animism (in the

narrow sense of a belief in animated nature, not in

the wider sense of pre-axial age belief system) is

the feeling of being caught between culture and

nature, nature in its double role of a feeder and

destroyer. Indian fairy-tales tell about hunters who

got punished by nature because they hunted more than

was necessary for the sustenance of family and

tribe. Indians tend to minimize the impact on

nature. Hence, one may conclude: Tribes are good in

order to understand the need for environmental

protection, for sustaining the complexity of our

environment, and of our cultures.

Page 37: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

bb) A person who studies the "multiplicity of

cultures" (cf. Pospísil 1971; Fikentscher 1995a: 21

ff. following Pospisil) will try to refrain from

ethnocentric judgments, and thus tend to honor

complexity as a value. Gell-Mann's two last chapters

in "The Quark and the Jaguar" (1994) give eloquent

witness of environmental complexity as a value. The

same holds true for cultural complexity. There is

tolerance and justice due to every single culture,

as long as it is willing to be tolerant towards

others.20 Someone who grows up in a totally red

environment does not know other colors, but she or

he does not know what red is either (Pospísil`s

example, in 1982, 13).

cc) Since cultures can be traced back to their

underlying modes of thought (see II. 4., supra), not

only cultures but also the modes of thought are

complex. If the other culture belongs to another

mode of thought, concepts such as space, distance,

time, causality, risk, social and societal ordering,

aesthetic judgments, and the respective use of

adjectives or process descriptions for

characterizations may be different (Fikentscher

1995, 181 ff.). Islam, for instance, knows no

causality in the Western sense, Confucianism uses a

    20 This is the meaning of the term "cultural justice", as it is

intended in Fikentscher (1991 and 1992a).

Page 38: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

concept of holistic causality different from the

Western concept (which may be called a rather

ecclectic approach to causal phenomena), and the

"activism" of the early Greek mind in Homer's Iliad

causes the shield of Achilles to be described not by

its appearance but by how it is made.

It follows that a culture which uses, e.g., non-

Western concepts of causality and risk is exempt

from Robert Axelrod`s "Evolution of Cooperation"

(Axelrod 1984). Axelrod`s cultural convergence and

polarization model does not exactly take up the

cooperation issue again, rather, it underlines the

improbability of cooperation under a number of given

cultural conditions (Axelrod 1995). In a similar

vein, testing the iterated Prisoner's Dilemma game

for analyzing the evolution of island societies,

Gumerman (1986, at 49) holds that modeling the

complexities of human behavior on the basis of the

abstract formulation of a computer simulation game

masks many potentially important variables, such as

the relationship between hierarchical social oders

and competition and cooperation.

John Holland`s "Hidden Order" starts on p. 3 with

the Platonic notion (transmitted to us by Aristotle)

that "the whole is more than the sum of its parts",

a theory that does not function in Confucianism, nor

Page 39: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

in Islam.1e     1 Cf. Nader (for a Shia Moslem village);

Fikentscher 1995 (307 ff., 349 ff.) (for Confucianism).

Holland continues on p. 4 with the Western concept

of an invidual agent, a notion unfit for Hinayana

Buddhism and many pre-axial-age ethnia. He also

mentions on p. 7 the stimulus-response theorem which

in this broadness is inapplicable in Islam, however.

Thus, these non-Western modes of thought, and the

cultures building upon them, emically do not know

what in Western thinking is meant by cooperation, or

"hidden orders" (which Holland on p. 4 defines as

generalizations of complex adaptive systems - our

subject of discussion). And if we were to say: Well,

that's our scientific analysis, we are thrown back

on the emic/etic issue, realizing that etic means

"just western" (see II. 4., supra).

It follows that the scientific observer here comes

to terms with the fact that in order to cope with

cultural complexity one has to re-think the analytic

premises of physical and biological complexity. This

may imply that the concept of complex adaptive

system begins to oscillate.

I think culture-comparative observation can put the

concept of complex adaptive system back on its feet,

though. We only have to take cultural complexity

Page 40: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

seriously, and familiarize ourselves with its

epistemology. The epistemology of cultural

complexity is the key to culture-independent

research and understanding. This topic will be

discussed in Parts III. and IV. below.

6. Understanding the Past

Cultures as substance matter of complexity include

past cultures. It is only in passing that I can

mention here the studies of archeologists and

archeological anthropologists (Gumerman (1986, 1992,

1994); Gumerman and Dean (1994); Gumerman and Gell-

Mann (1994a, 1994b); Gumerman and Kohler (1996);

Carla van West (1994); Jeffrey Dean, A.J. Lindsay

and W.J. Robinson (1978); Jeffrey Dean, William

Doelle and Janet Orcutt (1994); Emma Brunner-Traut

(1990) and others). They provide us with ample

materials of cultural complexity in the past, often

linked with the present, as is the case of the

Pueblos.

To mention just some examples: Room functions may

indicate historical data referring to demography,

social organization, and subsistence activities

(Gumerman 1992, 86 f.). In 1990 an SFI Working Group

examined how increases or decreases in the

complexity of regional systems of exchange and

Page 41: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

sociopolitical alliances might be modeled using John

Holland's Genetic Algorithm (Holland 1995), as

modified by workshop participant Robert Reynolds

from Wayne State University. Using surrogate

measures to monitor dimensions of demographic,

economic, social, and political behavior, the

working group investigated the potential of a model

generated from the Genetic Algorithm to reproduce

systems of comparable complexity to those known from

archeology. Incongruencies between the results of

the model and the actual archeological findings may

contribute to the assumption or - most probably -

exclusion of factors for explaning hitherto

inexplicable historic events: "efforts such as this

one will eventually allow us to specify the ways in

which systems of human behavior mirror or depart

from the behavior of other complex adaptive systems"

(George J. Gumerman, Murray Gell-Mann, and Linda

Cordell in their introduction to Gumerman and Gell-

Mann 1994b, 11).

If the modeling does not confirm the archeological

findings, and vice versa, other unnoticed factors

may be of importance, inviting to be researched and

eventually included.

If cultural complexity is subdivided by themes as is

proposed here, and aspects of the modes of thought,

Page 42: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

and the apparent disconnectedness of complexity and

evolution are taken into consideration, new

discoveries may lie ahead. One, among many possible,

may be that the Anasazi lived under a rather

different risk concept, compared with our modern

Western. This could perhaps explain abandonment

better, and implement artificial Anasazi simulation.

III. Culture(s) and Modes of Thought as Complex

Adaptive Systems (CAS )

In Part I., the CAS was defined.

In Part II., cultures (and modes of thought, shaping

cultures) were described as complex.

Inspite of their complexity, cultures each represent

a genus proximus of their themes, as their

differentia specifica (II. 3., supra). Therefore, a

culture may be regarded as a system.

The themes can be empirically observed. New themes

may be discovered. This means that culture is an

open system.

Page 43: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Thus, a culture is able to develop across time, it

is adaptive. Hence, culture change and culture

transfer are established subfields of anthropology.

Culture as such (in the "holistic" sense, see II.

1.b), supra) is an abstraction from cultures. The

modes of thought are substantively qualifying

factors of cultures. Hence, culture as such, as well

as the modes of thought, share the quality of a

culture of representing a genus proximus for

cultural themes.

One can now draw the conclusion that the several

cultures, present and past, and culture as such (in

the "holistic" sense), fit the concepts of system,

complexity, and adaptiveness . Cultures and culture

are open systems across time). The same holds true

for the concept of the mode of thought, as a

culture-embracing and culture-backing concept.

This is a matter of mere syllogisms. Hence, cultures

as well as culture, and the modes of thought behind

the cultures, are CAS.

Page 44: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

IV. Selected Implications

If cultural complexity is subjected to the theory of

complex adaptive systems, important consequences may

be drawn, and several possible advantages may be

identified. For example (1.), a new view may be

gained on the ontology-metaphor problem in

computational theory and beyond; and (2.) a new

theoretical field of anthropology may be

indentified: anthropological systems theory. (3.)

Moreover, a broader approach may be opened to

simulate cultures and societies ("artificial

culture(s)"), for the patterning, among others, of

multicultural societies; and (4.) the legitimation

of independent cultures may be facilitated; for

there is the "political" advantage of granting

cultures a right to exist, because adaptiveness

implies evolution, and evolution involves a "yes" to

existence.

For reasons of space and time, I confine myself to

the discussion of these four possible advantages. Of

course, there are further advantages, such as (5.) a

better understanding of the various concepts of time

and history, and (6.) a more accentuated survey on

the many cultures of this world. This survey should

be perceived as a plea for cultural multiplicity,

Page 45: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

and against the proverbial melting pot.21 However,

these and other implications of the treatment of

culture and cultures as complex adaptive systems

cannot be discussed within the framework of the

present paper.

1. The Ontology-Metaphor Problem

Since complex adaptive systems (CAS) offer

scientific processing opportunities, the question

arises as to the way in which cultures can be

processed as CAS. The technicalities may not be

simple. I am not equipped to speculate or comment in

this regard. However, the processing element touches

upon the representational side of the issues at

stake, and representation, in this context, involves

the confrontation, in computational theroy, of what

there is "in reality", with means how to express it.

I will call this issue the ontology-metaphor

problem.

a) In computer science, there is frequent talk of

viruses, neurons, learning, hardwired, a forest,

niches, noise, fitness, landscapes, nets and

networks, basin, spin glass, etc. The

    21 This amounts to a restatement of Gell-Mann's (1994, 294

ff) argument.

Page 46: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

representational or ontology-metaphor issue includes

the question of whether the results obtained from

using these terms are true in the sense of formal

logic; or just made plausible by (1) the induction -

deduction inference called analogy; or by (2) the

use of a model; or (3) simulation; or (4) metaphor;

or (5) by the use of a comparison.

These are just some possibilities for giving an

answer to the ontology-metaphor problem; there may

be more. Cowan, Pines and Meltzer (1994) discuss a

great number in this respect. If these possibilities

are brought into an order, in the form of a series

(not a system!), the result may look like this:

formal logic; analogy; model; simulation; metaphor,

and comparison. You will notice a decreasing scale

of plausibility, from the strongly convincing force

of formal logic belonging to the "ontology" of

things, down to a "mere comparison"; and even to

what David Sherrington called "caricatures of

biology" in his lecture on Feb. 23, 1996, at SFI.

Another warning comes from John Searle, who is

quoted by Alice Boatwright as having said in an

interview: "It`s very important to distinguish

between simulated thinking and the real thing"

(Boatwright 1997, p. 2).

Page 47: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Figure 8 shows a number of reasonings achieved by

way of making use of biological or other natural-

science inferences:

b) Let me draw your attention to the fact that so

far the ontology-metaphor problem has mainly been

discussed in relation to the borderline between

physics and mathematics on the one side, and biology

on the other. For example, ideas and methods from

complex systems` research, including adaptive

computation, are used to solve problems in

immunology. Or - reversely - neurons of the brain

are taken as models for "neurons" in the computer

(Sante Fe Institute 1995, C-4 through C 7, offering

numerous examples of conclusions drawn from

complexity research).

However, if the borderline between physics,

mathematics, and biology on the one side and

cultures on the other comes into focus, the

ontology-metaphor problem (for example when Anasazi

pueblo abandonment is contemplated) poses no

less,even additional (Gell-Mann (1994) 70:

"potential") difficulties.

Page 48: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Figure 8: Ontological and metaphorical reasoning

A. Ontological conclusions

1.) Mathematical proof2.) "Direct reasoning from structure" (Doran & Nigel, in

Nigel and Doran, 5); also called the "analyticalapproach" (Doran & Nigel, in Nigel and Doran, 2, 4, 5)

3.) Conclusions by formal logic, "first-order logic"(Doran & Nigel, in Nigel and Doran, 10)

B. Metaphorical conclusions (target (or goal) metaphorbehavior, Doran & Nigel, in Nigel and Doran, 12, 14); Cowanand Pines; untechnical language: "expressing", reproducing"(Doran & Nigel, in Nigel and Doran, 12, 14)

4.) analogy, using an induction - deduction inference (M.Mitchell 1993)

5.) modeling (Mithen, in Nigel and Doran, 191; M. Mitchell(1996 b); Reynolds, in Nigel and Doran 243; Doran &Nigel, in Nigel and Doran, 9)

a) fully numerical differential & recurrence equations,incl. non-linear models of chaos theory(Doran & Nigel, in Nigel and Doran, 9, also for thefollowing:)

b) stochastic and statistical modelsc) models of traditional operations researchd) ai (artificial intelligence) models ("usually

symbolic")aa) symbolic modelsbb) CAS, or "agent", models

6.) simulation (= "running a model", Doran & Nigel, inNigel and Doran, 9; Reynolds, in Nigel and Doran, 223ff.; Mithen, in Nigel and Doran, 179)

7.) comparison (Cowan and Pines) 8.) approximation (Doran & Nigel, in Nigel and Doran, 14) 9.) biological metaphor (Haasdijk/Barrow/Gerrets;

Helmreich 335)10.) other metaphorical conclusions (e.g., a loose use of

the term "metaphor", Holland 68)11.) pejoratively: "caricatures from biology"

(Sherrington, see supra , bevor Figure 8)

Page 49: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

The solution of the ontology - methaphor problem

depends on improved insights into the nature of

mathematical and scientific cognition and

conclusion. This by far surpasses the limits of my

presentation. Here follow just some more or less

general remarks:

First, a survey on the logically possible inferences

should be obtained. This refers, in a very general

sense, to the work of Frege (1879) and Boole (1847).

Potentially admissable inferences include

mathematical proof, logical formulization,

symbolization, analogy (about which Melanie Mitchell

(1996) has written a seminal book), and the

metaphor.

Secondly, for a solution of the ontology-metaphor

problem much depends on the philosophy chosen.

Because the philosophical view determines what is

meant by "ontology". Upon this - it is generally

claimed - depends the determination of the

epistemological approach to this ontology, including

the definition of "metaphor".

2) This leads to the philosophical issues of how the

"truth" can be defined, and how, by using human

thinking capacities, the ways of "knowing the truth"

can meaningfully be related to those definitions of

Page 50: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

the truth. The first issue may be called the

"ontological" one ("what is there""), the second the

"epistemological" ("how do I know what is there?");

see the Introduction, supra, before I.

Of course, these basic questions of philosophy

cannot be dealt with here at some length. Moreover,

I am not a philosopher. In view of these

limitations, I have to take refuge to a generally

accepted students book that tries to identify the

various ways of ontological and epistemological

theorizing.

In his famous introduction to the kinds of

philosophizing, Stephen C. Pepper (1942)

distinguishes, four major ontologies in Western

philosophy , and consequently four appertaining

epistemologies as the pertinent kinds of knowing the

truth. It is permissible to use Pepper's

classifications for explaining the ontology-metaphor

issue because they are both simple and circumspect:

One of these four ontologies/epistemologies is

pragmatism in the form that was developed by Charles

Peirce (1931 - 1958) and William James (1907), of

which John Holland says that it is at the

philosophical base of the genetic algorithm used by

himself (communication). Pragmatism unites "is" and

Page 51: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

"as if" to the extent they have the same practical

outcome ("the same" being pragmatically interpreted

as identity of event, Pepper 232). Hans Vaihinger

calls pragmatism "the philosophy of the as-if"

(1911). In Pepper`s book pragmatism is called

contextualism. Contextually, 2 x 2 is 4.

According to Pepper, for the "formist" view of Plato

and Aristotle, ontologies in mathematics, biology,

social sciences, etc. would be distinct from one

another, with the consequence that only analogous

conclusions are permitted. The root metaphor of

formism is similarity, Pepper 151). Thus, 2 x 2 is

not 4, but equals 4.

Pepper`s third, so-called "mechanistic" ontology (of

Isaac Newton, e.g.) looks for truth by symbols and

inferences (Pepper 221). It may produce22 "hidden

orders" (Holland), "inherent orders" (St.

Kauffmann), and "original conditions" (M. Gell-

Mann), and therefore the term "metaphor" is

particularly appropriate for similarities produced

by such concealed regularities. By inference, 2 x 2

corresponds to 4.

1     22 my examples, not Pepper`s; also the 2 x 2 "mathematics" are

mine, with a posthumous apology to Pepper.

Page 52: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

In Pepper's system, the fourth Western ontology,

"organicism", deduces from higher principles and

would have to solve the ontology-metaphor problem by

thinking in terms of emanations, unfoldings, or

derivatives (Pepper`s example: Hegel, at 293). From

2 x 2, 4 emanates as if from a higher principle.

In addition, and in passing, Pepper lists a few non-

Western ways of philosophizing, such as animism.

Pepper discards them for his purposes. I would like

to include them and think it worthwile to consider

some more non-Western "modes of thought" except

animism, such as East and South Asian detachment,

Islam, and secular totalitarianism (for details, see

Fikentscher 1995). It is evident that each

philosophic way of reasoning (Pepper) or "mode of

thought" (from an anthropological point of view)

offers a different answer to the ontology-metaphor

problem.

However, almost never, to my - rather limited -

observation, is an inference used for tackling the

ontology-metaphor issue which Horgan (1995) refers

to in the center of his criticism, namely the

syllogism. The syllogism is the inference which in

logical science is technically called the Modus

Barbara II. It is, e.g., the classical way of a

conclusion in law. In Figure 8, the syllogism

Page 53: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

belongs to the ontological conclusions by formal

logic (A.3.).

Melanie Mitchell (1996 b) correctly rejects Horgan`s

syllogistic requirement, and instead prefers the

"modeling" metaphor:

"He (scil.: Horgan) points out that 'numerical

models of natural systems' cannot be absolutely

verified, that 'the only propositions that can be

verified - that is, proved true - are those

concerning `closed systems`, based on pure

mathematics and logic.' Somehow, this non-

controversial point is used to imply that 'the

entire field of complexity' is based on a false

'syllogism'. However, Horgan neglects to point out

that the same argument can be applied to all of

natural science - no model of nature can ever be

proven correct. Scientists are well aware of this,

and, unlike mathematicians, they are not in the

business of proving their theories correct, but

rather in the business of coming up with and

supporting - or falsifying - explanations of the

mechanisms of nature. The criteria for the success

of a theory or model are not absolute verification

and proof - rather they are predictive power, new

insight, contribution to new technologies, and so

on."

Page 54: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

d) Let me conclude this sketch of the ontology-

metaphor problem and its elusive bunch of solutions

by trying to demonstrate where and how the

anthropologist is confronted with the ontology-

metaphor issue.

The classical subdivisions of anthropology are

ordered as follows

(Figure 9: Classical Subdivisions of Anthropology)

However, it makes much more sense to relate physical

(=biological) anthropology to all subfields of

cultural anthropology ( Figure 10 ):

Relating physical (=biological) anthropology to

corresponding fields of cultural anthropology

produces, in Figure 10, a vertical line in the

center of the chart. It is apparent that the

vertical middle line represents one of an

anthropologist`s several possible views on the

ontology-metaphor issue: e.g., the legal culture of

contracting may be related to reciprocity as an

ethological universal; or; "cognition" can be

culture- or brain-related.

Page 55: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Of course, these anthropological statements

concerning the relation of cultural and physical

aspects of the conditio humana are of statements of

truth. (Culture is not an analogy to nature, nor

nature to culture.) But they are statements of

similarity, of correspondence, and of mutual

confirmation. Almost every anthropological statement

can be made in terms of culture and in terms of

behavior. Comparison as one form of metaphorical

conclusion might be the fitting epistemological tool

in this context.

2. Anthropological Systems Theory

The subjection of the study of cultural complexity

to the theory of complex adaptive systems renders

another considerable advantage. It is indirectly

connected to the ontology-metaphor problem. The

ontology-metaphor problem concerns the

epistemological foundations of reasoning, in

science, in the humanities, and also in the social

sciences. If one looks at the substance matter of

natural sciences, the humanities and the social

sciences as contrasted to their epistemologies, a

general theory of systems surfaces, that concerns

the systematic arrangements of those sciences,

humanities, and social sciences. There has long

Page 56: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

since been talk of a general systems theory (e.g.,

Canaris - see footnote 3, supra; Kambartel 1969).

What will happen if this general systems theory is

applied to a specific natural science, or "art"

science, or social science, let us say to the social

science of anthropology? Anthropology is the social

science of culture comparison, of the comparison

between exactly those cultures that furnished the

complexity we are being called to study.

The result is a new field of anthropology that may

be called " anthropological systems theory ".

Anthropological systems theory concerns the rules of

systematic (and other ordering of) thinking, of

complexities, and of adaption in anthropology . This

appears to be a promising field of anthropological

theory. The emic/etic issue23, for example, seems to

belong to this new field, as well as the study of

concepts such as correct "reasoning", concept-

forming, "concluding" between cultural and physical

anthropology by going from one side to the other of

the vertical line in Figure 10, and thus

understanding and "interpreting" cultures and

culture, as well as human behavior.

    23 Meta-reasoning in culture comparison is one of several

possible reductions of cultural complexity, Fikentscher1995, 140; idem 1992a.

Page 57: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

3. Artificial Culture(s) ?

As has already been mentioned, another benefit of

applying complexity research to the study of

cultures and culture may consist in the improved

possibility of drafting artificial culture(s).

a) In the book on the "Modes of Thought" (1995a) one

of the main interests was to break down cultures to

particles. I may again refer to Figure 5, supra. I

assumed that then three operations would be

possible:

- explaining extant or extinct cultures by referring

to their

components,

- asking for patterns behind the cultures in order

to find the "missing links" (for example: projecting

and predicting religious sects), similar to the use

of the chemical table following Mendeleyev's

discovery, and

- inventing cultures from the set of available

components (Fikentscher 1995, 172 f.). It became

possible to envisage artificial cultures. In writing

Page 58: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

the "Modes of Thought", I was not aware how close I

came to what in compu

-ter science is called artificial life. Hence I

called my artificial, constructable cultures

"culture chemistry". I could have called them

"artificial culture", or "simulated societies" as

well (in 1995, 173, I called them "artificial" modes

of thought). Thus, there is a close relationship

between modes of thought research and the study of

artificial societies.

b) However, it may not always be necessary to expand

the scope of analogous, modelled, simulated,

metaphoric, or compared culture so broadly. Rather

than addressing cultures, or culture as such, it

might often be more appropriate to begin with

smaller objects of study, for instance with certain,

representative cultural traits.

For example, as a cultural trait fit for being

subjected to a test in "culture chemistry", or

artificial culture, or artificial mode of thought, a

certain kind of behavior may be selected (e.g.

aggression against foreigners). Behavior often is

culture-specific, and it may reach both into

physical=biological, and cultural, anthropology.

Page 59: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Hence, according to the results reached above,

culture-analogous, model-induced, computationally-

simulated, metaphoric, or culturally-compared

behaviors become conceivable. Whether they may be

called "simulated behavior", "ethological

simulation", "computational ethology", or

"artificial behavior" is inconsequential (Nigel and

Doran). Nigel and Doran have collected a series of

essays on the simulation of societal and cultural

phenomena (by Doran, Nigel, Séror, Nowak, Latané,

Drogoul, Ferber, Mithen, Palmer, Mellars, R.G.

Reynolds and others). W. Brian Arthur (1993, 1994,

1996) employs similar simulations in economics.

George Gumerman's and others' work in simulating

past culture have already been mentioned (II.6.).

Society, economy, and objects of archeology are

themes of cultural anthropology, at least in part.

Therefore, what remains to be claimed is a general

access of artificial life to culture.

To make such a cultural study operational, it often

will be advisable to concentrate on specific kinds

of human cultural behavior. Two promising candidates

among others seem to be risk-related behavior (risk

transfer, etc.) and the subject-object relation and

its behavioral-cultural expressions. Because, how a

subject faces an object within a given culture, is

Page 60: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

indicative of a lot of other cultural traits. Thus,

both risk- and subject-object-related behavior are

interesting objects for cross-cultural investigation

in simulated, that is, artificial cultures.

4. Recognition of Cultures

For lack of space and time, only brief mention can

be made of the "natural law issue" as to whether

existing cultures should enjoy a right to exist and

to be protected (for a more detailed discussion, see

Fikentscher 1992a).

It is generally accepted that drawing a conclusion

from what is to what should be amounts to a

naturalistic fallacy. Repetition of facts does not

justify an ought (David Hume, 1740 and 1777).

Immanuel Kant (1785) tried to help out with the

categorical imperative, in order to render the ought

reasonable.

It touches upon these fundamental issues of Western

philosophy to discuss the question whether one may

possibly draw the conclusion that, due to the

obviously and empirically observable evolution of

nature and cultures, nature and cultures

fs28 deserve recognition and protection.

Page 61: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Still, it seems plausible that an ought is contained

in evolution. Here is not the place to decide this

issue of natural law. Suffice to say that

anthropological systems theory, i.e., the

application of the theory of complex adaptive

systems (and less-than-systems) to the several

cultures, raises the is-and-ought issue anew, and

from a hitherto less conspicuous angle. A

modification of the doctrine of naturalistic fallacy

may be one of the most far-reaching consequences of

regarding cultures as complex adaptive systems.

V. Summary

Cultural complexity fits the concept and the

processing-possibilities of complex adaptive

systems, shares the ontology-methaphor problem, and

is therefore open to artificiality.

The following 12 theses serve as my conclusion.

VI. 12 Theses

1) A system is only one of a number of possibilities

to order non-complex (=simple) or complex things.

Page 62: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Other possibilities are topical, serial, cyclical,

and compilatory orderings (may be that edge-of-chaos

ordering may also fit here).

2) A system is defined by at least one upper

proposition, the genus proximus, and (usually) more

subordinate propositions, the differentia specifica.

A system can be closed or open.

3) Complexity is the material, the substance, being

ordered, whether by self-organization, or by some

outside effort. Complexity is therefore nothing more

and nothing less than the acknowledg-ment that

something can be ordered.

4) Thus, complexity invites a comparison of the ways

and means in which the multitude of perceivable

things can be ordered in a systematic, topical,

serial, cyclical, compilatory order or otherwise:

There may be talk of a study of "comparative

orderings". It could also be called the study of

possible orders.

5) In essence, this study of possible orders is the

study of complexity. One example is the development

of computer immune systems from biological immune

systems; another insight gained from such

Page 63: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

comparative order is the interpretation of the

galaxy as a vortex (whirlpool).

6) Orderings, including systems, are open to

adaptation. Examples are open systems, series which

get longer, and compilations that grow bigger or

begin structuring themselves. Any adaptation

introduces the concept of time. To speak of an

complex adaptitive system is but another way of

saying that there is a structure in evolution, a

structure that permits generalizations and

particularizations (because this dichotomy is the

essence of a system).

7) Human evolution includes physical and biological

complexities, forming the field of physical or

biological anthropology. It also includes a point

from which the human mind reflects on these physical

and biological complexities. This reflection is the

source of ought-behavior ("the point of the ought").

From then on human complexities greatly expand

(Gell-Mann: potential complexity). This kind of

expansion is called culture.

8) Hence, culture is susceptible to order, including

systematic order. This is the field of culture

comparison which studies cultural complexity in a

comparative way.

Page 64: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

9) Cultural complexity has substantive and

methological aspects.

a) In substantive regards, culture comparison views

the complexities of

- conceptional subdivisions of culture such as

overt or covert traits; or the emic/etic

distinction;

- the various cultures whereby early (esp.

animist) cultures are often more complex than

cultures of longer development;

- the modes of thought lying at the foundation of

the various cultures; and

- culture as an abstract from the various

cultures (as opposed to civilization, society, etc.)

b) In methodological regards, and using a meta-

language, culture comparison studies the

complexities of

- the cultures as complex adaptive systems

- culture itself as a complex adaptive system

- the "modes of thought", and

- artificial cultures (in order to trace, to

quote just one example, the reasons for the

abandonment of Anasazi pueblo before the entrada).

Page 65: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

c) Note that 9a) represents (complexity-centered)

cultural anthropology, and 9b) what within the

present context has been called "anthropological

systems theory" as a new field of anthropological

research.

10) There is evidence to assume that the studies of

physical and biological complexity, or cultural

complexity, can learn from one another, and even use

common tools and methods such as simulations, or the

emic/etic dichotomy (the "vertical line" in Figure

10).

11) However, in experimenting with such comparisons,

common concepts and methods, consideration must be

given to

a) the expanding nature of the potential complexity

once "the point of the ought" has been passed,

b) the ontology-metaphor problem, and

c) the possibilities of artificial cultures and

culture-traits.

d) the political question whether cultures should be

protected because they exist (to a certain extent,

Page 66: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

evolution may be able to bridge the is-and-ought

dichotomy), thus correcting to some

extent the naturalistic fallacy.

12) Contrary to other opinions, the ontology-

metaphor issue cannot be answered in a simple yes-

or-no manner. There is a descending scale from

ontological , non-metaphorical use of biological

discoveries for explanations in the cultural field

and vice-versa, to an increasingly metaphorical or

simply comparative use. This scale of imperceptible

degrees is more than a linguistic trick. It might

lead to hidden orders, hidden behind the metaphors,

and thus to a new concept of a "scientific law",

which most probably will be no "syllogism".

Moreover, the answer depends on the kind of the

ontology/epistemology chosen.

In sum, one should not hesitate to embark on the

study of cultural complexity . In archeology,

sociology, and economics, research in cultural

complexity has already commenced. We may continue to

explore these fields and others under the wider

concept of cultural complexity .

Page 67: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Sources for the Figures to "Cultural Complexity"

Figure 1: Fikentscher, Wolfgang, Die umweltsoziale

Marktwirtschaft - als Rechtsproblem -, Juristische

Studiengesellschaft Karlsruhe No. 197, Heidelberg:

C.F. Mueller, 1991. p.7.

English version: idem, Free Trade and Protection of

Environment as an Integrated Economic Value System:

Outline of an Environment-Conscious Social Market

Economy - A Lawyer`s View. Cassel Lecture 1991.

University of Stockholm School of Law. Stockholm

1992.

Figure 2 a,b,c: Fikentscher, Wolfgang, Methoden des

Rechts, vol. 4: Dogmatischer Teil, Tuebingen: Mohr,

1977, chapter 32.

Figure 3: new for this Working Paper

Figure 4: Fikentscher, Wolfgang, Modes of Thought: A

Study

in the Anthropology of Law and Religion, Tuebingen:

Mohr, 1995, p. 182.

Figure 5: same as Figure 4, p. 24.

Figure 6: same as Figure 4, p. 160.

Figure 7: SFI lecture of Jan. 17, 1992, unpublished

Figure 8: new for this Working Paper

Figure 9: same as Figure 4, p. 92

Figure 10: same as Figure 4, p. 96.

Page 68: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

References

Alker, Hayward R. Rediscoveries and Reformulations:

Humanistic Methodologies for International Studies .

Cambridge, UK 1996: Cambridge Univ. Press.

Arthur, W. Brian. "Why Things Become More Complex".

Scientific American , May 1993, 144.

--. Increasing Returns and Path Dependence in the Economy .

Ann Arbor 1994: Univ. of Michigan Press.

--. "Complexity in Economic and Financial Markets".

Complexity , No. 1, 1995, 20-25.

--. "Increasing Returns and the New Worlds of Business".

Harvard Business Review (1996), 100-109.

Axelrod, Robert. The Evolution of Cooperation , Basic Book:

New York 1984.

--. The Convergence and Stability of Cultures: Local

Convergence and Global Polarization . Santa Fe Institute.

Working Paper 95-03-028, = Institute of Public Policy

Studies, Discussion Paper No. 375, View of Michigan, Ann

Arbor MI 48109.

Bäck, Thomas. Evolutionary Algorithm in Theory and

Practice: Evolutionary Strategies, Evolutionary

Programming, Genetic Algorithms , Oxford Univ. Press: New

York and Oxford 1996.

Benedict, Ruth. Patterns of Culture . New York 1934:

Houghton, Mifflin & Co.

Page 69: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Binnig, Gerd. " The Fractal Structure of Evolution ", in:

Essays in Honour of Benoit B. Mandelbrot, Fractals in

Physics, A. Aharony and J. Feder (eds.), North-Holland:

Amsterdam. Physica D 38 (1989) 32-36.

Boatwright, Alice. "I Compute, Therefore I Am - Or Am I?"

25 Berkeleyan No. 25 of Feb. 26 - March 4, 1997, p. 1f.

Boole, George. The Mathematical Analysis of Logic . New

York 1965: Barnes & Noble (orig. 1847).

--. An Investigation of the Laws of Thought . New York 1951:

Dover Publ.

Brunner-Traut, Emma. Fr ü hformen des Erkennens am Beispiel

Ä gyptens . Darmstadt 1990: Wiss. Buchgesellschaft.

Canaris, Claus-Wilhelm. Systemdenken und Systembegriff in

der Jurisprudenz , 3rd ed. Berlin 1983: Duncker & Humblot

(1st ed. 1969, 2nd ed. 1978).

Cooter, Robert D., and Fikentscher, Wolfgang. Is There

Indian Common Law? The Role of Custom in American Indian

Tribal Courts . John M. Olin Working Paper Series, Olin

Working Paper No. 92-3, University of California at

Berkeley, December 1992. Revised versions 1994, 1995,

1997and 1998, to appear in The American Journal of

Comparative Law (1998).

Cowan, George A., and Pines, David. "From Metaphors to

Reality?", in: Cowan, George A., Pines, David, and Meltzer,

David (eds.), Complexity: Metaphors, Models, and Reality ,

Reading, Mass., 1994, Addison-Wesley Publ. Co., 709-718.

Page 70: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Darwin, Charles. The Expressions of the Emotions in Man

and Animals , 1872.

Dawkins, Richard. The Selfish Gene . London 1978: Paladin

Books; Oxford 1989: Oxford Univ. Press (orig. 1976).

Dean, J.S., Lindsay, A.J., and Robinson, W.J.,

"Prehistoric Settlement in Longhouse Valley, Northeastern

Arizona". In: Euler, R.C. and Gumerman, G.J. (eds.),

Investigations of the Southwestern Anthropological Research

Group: An Experiment in Anthropological Cooperation. The

Proceeding of the 1976 Conference . Flagstaff 1978: Museum

of Northern Arizona, Bulletin 50.

Dean, Jeffrey S., Doelle, William H., and Orcutt, Janet

D. " Adaptive Stress, Environment and Demography ". In:

Gumerman, George J. (ed.). Themes in Southwest Prehistory.

Santa Fe NM 1994: School of American Research Press, 53-86.

Dennett, Daniel C., " Memes and the Exploitation of

Imagination ". 48 Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism,

No. 2, Spring 1990, 127-135.

Donald, Merlin. Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages

in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition . Cambridge/ Mass.

1991: Harvard Univ. Press.

Doxtater, Dennis. " Reflections on the Anasazi Cosmos ". In:

Gron, Ole, Engelstad, Ericka, and Lindblom, Inge (eds.).

Social Space, Human Spatial Behavior in Dwellings and

Settlements, Proceedings of an International Conference.

Odense 1991, Odense Univ. Press: 155-184.

Eucken, Walter. Die Grundlagen der National ö konomie . 6th

ed. Godesberg 1950.

Page 71: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Evans-Pritchard, E.E. The Nuer. A Description of the

Modes of the Livelihood and Political Institutions of a

Nilotic People . Oxford 1940: Clarendon (reprints 1950 and

1968).

Feeley, Malcolm, and Charles Lester. Legal Complexity and

the Transformation of the Criminal Process, in: Couron

André, Laurant Mayali, Schioppa Antonio Padoa, and Simon

Dieter (eds.). Subjektivierung des justiziellen

Beweisverfahrens, Beitr ä ge zum Zeugenbeweis in Europa und

USA (18.-20. Jahrhundert) . Frankfurt/M. 1994: Klostermann,

337-375.

Fikentscher, Wolfgang. "Les rapport du droit privé et de

son histoire". Studi in Memoria di Lorenzo Mossa , vol. II,

Padua 1961: CEDAM, 181-187; published also as monograph,

Padua 1960: CEDAM; reprinted in Fikentscher 1975-77, vol.

IV (1977), 685-692.

--. Methoden des Rechts , 5 vol., Mohr-Siebeck, Tübingen,

1975-77.

--. "Zum Stand der Werte-Diskussion in der heutigen

deutschen Jurisprudenz und eine juristische Theorie der

Meta-Werte". In: Tietz, Reinhard (ed.). Wert- und

Pr ä ferenzprobleme in den Sozialwissenschaften . Schriften

des Vereins für Socialpolitik, New Series, No. 122. Berlin

1981: Duncker & Humblot, 43 - 78.

--. Free Trade and Protection of the Environment as an

Integrated Value System: Outline of an Environment-

Conscious Social Market Economy - A Lawyer's View , The 1991

Cassel Lecture, Stockholm 1991: Juridiska Fakulteten i

Stockholm, Skriftserien 34.

Page 72: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

--. "The Sense of Justice and the Concept of Cultural

Justice: Views from Law and Anthropology." 34 American

Behavioral Scientist 314-334 (1991); an enlarged version

in: Roger D. Masters and Margaret Gruter (eds.), The Sense

of Justice: Biological Foundations of Law. Newbury Park

etc. 1992: Sage, 106-127 (1992a).

--. Modes of Thought in Law: Pueblo Tribal Court Studies in

Cultural Complexity , Santa Fe Institute Colloquium Series,

January 17, 1992 (unpublished) (1992b).

--. "Die Erforschung des lebenden Rechts in einer

multikulturellen Gesellschaft: Karl N. Llewellyns Cheyenne-

und Pueblo-Studien." In: U. Drobnig and M. Rehbinder

(eds.). Rechtsrealismus, multikulturelle Gesellschaft und

Handelsrecht: Karl N. Llewellyn und seine Bedeutung heute .

Berlin 1994: Duncker & Humblot, 45-70.

--. Modes of Thought: A Study in the Anthropology of Law

and Religion , Mohr: Tübingen 1995 (1995a).

--. Zur Anthropology der K ö rperschaft - Polis,

Genossenschaft, Tewa Pueblo - (ein Feldforschungsbericht) ,

Bayerische Akademie der Wissenschaften, Phil.-Hist.Klasse,

Sitzungsberichte 1995/2, München 1995 (1995b).

--. " Mehrzielige Marktwirtschaft subjektiver M ä rkte: Wider

das Europa- und das Weltmarktargument ." In: Festschrift

E.J. Mestmäcker, Baden-Baden 1996: Nomos, 567-578.

--. "Domestic Violence under Indian Pueblo Law". In:

Margaret Gruter and Manfred Rehbinder (eds.). Gewalt in der

Kleingruppe und das Recht. Festschrift f ü r Martin Usteri .

Berne 1997: Stämpfli, 45-73 (1997a).

Page 73: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

--. Wirtschaftliche Gerechtigkeit und kulturelle

Gerechtigkeit. Vom Sinn der Kulturvergleichung in

Anthropologie , Wirtschaft und Recht . Heidelberg 1997: C.F.

Müller (1997b).

--. "Rechtsanthropologie - am Beispiel des Rechts gegen

häusliche Gewalt bei Pueblo-Indianern". JURA 1998, 182-189.

Fikentscher, Wolfgang, and McGuire, Michael T. "A Four-

Function Theory of Biology for Law". 25 Rechtstheorie 1-20

(1994).

Fikentscher, Wolfgang. See also Cooter, Robert.

Fischmann, Joshua. " New Clues Surface About the Making of

the Mind ". 262 Science, 3 Dec. 1993, 1517.

Fish, Paul R., Fish, Susanne K., Gumerman, George J., and

Reid, J. Jefferson. " Toward an Explanation for Southwestern

`Abandonments' ". In: Gumerman, George J. (ed.). Themes in

Southwest Prehistory. Santa Fe NM 1994: School of American

Research Press, 135-163.

Freeman, Walter J. Societies of Brains: A Study in the

Neuroscience of Love and Hate. The Spinoza Lectures .

Hillsdale, New Jersey and Hove, UK, 1995: Lawrence Erlbaum

Ass. Publ.

Frege, F.L. Gottlob. Begriffsschrift . 1879 (2nd ed. 1964)

Fridlund, Alan J. Human Facial Expressions: An Evolutiony

View. San Diego 1994: Academic Press.

Page 74: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Gell-Mann, Murray. The Quark and the Jaguar: Adventures in

the Simple and in the Complex . Freeman: New York, 1994.

--. "What is Complexity?". 1 Complexity , No. 1, 1995, 16-

19.

--. "Nature Conformable to Herself ". 1 Complexity No. 1,

1995-96, 9-12.

Gell-Mann, Murray, and Lloyd, Seth. " Information

Measures, Effective Complexity and Total Information "

(Paper), Santa Fe Institute (mgm@ santafe.edu).

Gilbert, Nigel, and Doran, Jim (eds.) Simulating

Societies: The Computer Simulation of Social Phenomena ,

London 1994: UCL Press.

Goldberg, D.E. Genetic Algorithms in Search, Optimization

and Machine Learning , 1989: Addison-Wesley: Reading, Mass.

etc.

Gumerman, George J. "The Role of Competition and

Cooperation in the Evolution of Island Societies." In:

Kirch, Patrick V. (ed). Island Societies: Archeological

Approaches to Evolution and Transformation . Cambridge 1986:

Cambridge V. Press, 42-49.

--. A View from Black Mesa: The Changing Face of

Archeology. Tucson & London 1992: V of Arizona Press.

--. Themes in Southwest Prehistory . Santa Fe NM 1994:

School of American Research Press.

Gumerman, George J., and Dean, Jeffrey S. "Prehistoric

Cooperation and Competition in the Western Anasazi Area."

Page 75: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

In: Gumerman, George J. (ed.). Themes in Southwestern

Prehistory . Santa Fe NM 1994: School of American Research

Press, 99-148.

Gumerman, George J. and Gell-Mann, Murray. Cultural

Evolution in the Prehistoric Southwest. In: Gumerman,

George J. (ed.). Themes in Southwestern Prehistory . Santa

Fe NM 1994: School of American Research Press, 11-31

(1994a).

--. and --. (ed). Understanding Complexity in the

Prehistoric Southwest . Proceedings Vol. XV. Reading, Mass.

1994: Addison-Wesley (1994b).

Gumerman, George J. and Kohler, Timothy A. Creating

Alternative Cultural Histories in the Prehistoric

Southwest: Agent-based Modeling in Archeology . Santa Fe

Institute Working Paper 96-03-007. Santa Fe NM 1996.

Haasdyk, Evert W., Barrow, David, and Gerrets, Monique

C. Genetic Algorithms in Business . Paper (evert@

capints.uucp, or CompuServe (100315.1117).

Hahlbrock, Klaus. "Thematische Innovation - eingeschränkt

durch

ein Vorurteil über `soft science`", MPG-Spiegel 3/1995, 2-

3.

Hamburger, Ludwig. Fragmentierte Gesellschaft: Die

Struktur der Thai-Familie. 17 K ö lner Zeitschrift f ü r

Soziologie und Sozialpsychologie , No. 1, 49 (1965).

--. Fragmented Society: The Structure of Thai Music.

Soziologus 1967, 54.

Page 76: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Headland, Thomas N., Pike, Kenneth Lee, and Harris,

Marvin (eds.). Emics and Etics. The Insider/Outsider

Debate. Newbury Park 1990: Sage.

Helmreich, Stefan Gordon. Anthropology Inside and Outside

the Looking-Glass World of Artificial Life , 2 vol. Diss.

Stanford, Dept. of Anthropology. August 1995.

Herodotos . Historiae . Felix Jacoby (ed.), in Pauly-Wissowa

Real-Encyclopaedie, suppl. 2, 205-520, 1913.

Hoebel, E. Adamson. "The Character of Keresan Pueblo Law".

112 Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 127-

130 (1968).

Holland, John H. Hidden Order: How Adaptation Builds

Complexity , Helix Books Reading, Mass. etc. 1995: Addison-

Wesley Publ. Co.

Holton, Gerald. "The Controversy over the End of Science".

Scientific American 191 (Oct. 1995).

Horgan, John. "From Complexity to Perplexity", Scientific

American 104-109 (Oct. 1995).

--. "The New Social Darwinists", Scientific American , 174-

181 (Oct. 1995).

Horton, Robin G., and Finnegan, Ruth (eds.). Modes of

Thought. Essays on Thinking in Western and Non-western

Societies . London 1973: Faber & Faber.

Hume, David. An Inquiry Concerning Human Understanding.

1740.

Page 77: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

--. A Treatise of Human Natur. 1777.

Johanson, Donald, and Edey, Maitland. Lucy: Die Anf ä nge

der Menschheit . München & Zürich 1982: Piper (orig. Lucy:

The Beginnings of Humankind . New York 1981: Simon &

Schuster).

Jacob, Bernard E., Viehweg: The Phenomenology of the

Topics. Paper. Hempstead 1995.

James, William. Pragmatism , 1907.

Johnson, George. Fire in the Mind: Science, Faith, and

Search for Order . New York 1996: Knopf.

Johnson, Jeffrey. A Language of Structure in the Science

of Complexity. 1 Complexity , 22-29 (1995).

Kambartel, Friedrich. "`System` und `Begründung` als

wissenschaftliche und philosophische Ordnungsbegriffe bei

und vor Kant", in: Blühdorn and J. Ritter (eds.).

Philosophie und Rechtswissenschaft . Frankfurt/Main 1969,

99.

Kant, Immanuel. Grundlegung zur Metaphysik der Sitten. Riga

1785.

Kauffman, Stuart. At Home in the Universe: The Search for

Laws of Self-Organization and Complexity . New York/Oxford

1995: Oxford Univ. Press.

Keitt, Timothy H., and Marquet, Pablo A. The Introduced

Hawaiian Avifauna Reconsidered: Evidence for Self-Organized

Criticality ?, Santa Fe Institute Working Paper 95-10-088.

Page 78: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Kirste, Stephan. Die Zeitlichkeit des positiven Rechts und

die Geschichtlichkeit des Rechtsbewu ß tseins. Momente der

Ideengeschichte und Grundz ü ge einer systematischen

Begr ü ndung . Berlin 1998: Duncker & Humblot.

Kopp, Stephan, Reidys, Christian, and Schuster, Peter.

Exploration of Artificial Landscapes Built on Random

Grayphs , IMB-Jena, Beutenberger Str. 11, PO-Box 100 813, D-

07708 Jena, Germany (paper).

Luhmann, Niklas. Soziale Systeme. Frankfurt/M. 1984:

Suhrkamp

Mendeleyev. See Posin.

Mitchell, Melanie. Analogy-Making as Perception: A

Computer Model . Cambridge, Mass., and London 1993: MIT

Press.

--. An Introduction to Genetic Algorithms . Cambridge,

Mass., and London 1996: MIT Press (1996 a).

--. Letter to the Editors, of Febr. 20, 1996, Scientific

American (1996) (1996b).

Montesquieu, Charles Louis de Secondat. De l'esprit des

loi. 1748.

Moritz, Elan. "Memetic Science: I-General Introduction". 1

Journal of Ideas , No. 1, Sept. 1990, 3-23.

Nader, Laura. "Dispute Settlement and Community

Organization: Shia Moslem and Mexican Zapotec". In: Black,

Donald and Mileski, Maureen (eds.). The Social Organization

Page 79: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

of Law . New York etc. 1973: Seminar Press (Harcourt, Brace,

Jovanovich), 198-205.

Nigel, Gilbert, and Doran, Jim. Simulating Societies. The

Computer Simulation of Social Phenomena . London 1994: UCL

Press.

Nörr, Dieter. Divisio und partitio . Berlin 1972: J.

Schweitzer.

Oppenländer, Karl Heinrich. "Ansatzpunkte einer

innovationsorientierten Wachstumspolitik". In: Harabi,

Najib. Kreativit ä t-Wirtschaft-Recht . Zürich 1996:

Hochschulverlag AG an der ETH.

Otte, Gerhard. "20 Jahre Topik-Diskussion". Rechtstheorie

1970, 381.

Peirce, Charles S. Collected Papers. 8 vol. ed. by Charles

Hartshorne & Paul Weiss. Cambridge, Mass. 1931-1958.

Pepper, Stephen C. World Hypotheses. Prolegomena to

Systematic Philosophy and a Complete Survey on Metaphysics .

Berkeley CA 1942: U. of California Press.

Pike, Kenneth Lee. Language in Relation to a Unified Theory

of the Structure of Human Behavior . Prelim.ed. Glendale,

Calif., Summer Institute of Linguistics, 1954.

Posin, D. Q. Mendeleyev. Story of a Great Scientist . 1948

Pospísil, Leopold. Anthropology of Law: A Comparative

Theory . New York 1971: Harper & Row. 2nd ed. New Haven

1974: HRAF Press. Reprints 1987 and 1994.

Page 80: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Otte, Gerhard. "20 Jahre Topik-Diskussion". Rechtstheorie

1970, 381.

--. The Ethnology of Law . 2nd ed. Menlo Park CA 1978, 3rd

ed. 1985: Cummings Publ.

--. Anthropologie des Rechts. Recht und Gesellschaft in

archaischen und modernen Kulturen . Munich 1982: C.H. Beck

(transl. from the 2nd ed. 1974 by Chr. Schaefer & E. Blenk-

Knokke).

--. The Structure of Culture (manuscript). Yale Univ. New

Haven 1986.

Raiffa, H. Preferences for Multi-Attributed Alternatives .

Santa Monica CA. April 1969: The Rand Corp.

Richter, Gisela M.A. Perspective in Greek and Roman Art .

London & New York 1970.

Rosenberg, Erika L. "Facing the Facts". 373 Nature , of 16

Feb. 1995, 569.

Ruhl, J.B. "Complexity Theory as a Paradigm for the

Dynamical Law-and-Society System: A Wake-up Call for Legal

Reductionism and the Modern Administrative State". 45 Duke

Law Journal 849-928 (1996).

Ruse, Michael, and Wilson, Edward O. "Moral Philosphy as

Applied Science", 61 Philosophy , 173-192 (1986).

Santa Fe Institute. Results from a Decade of Research

(1985-1995). Paper. Santa Fe NM 1995 (WWW.santafe.edu).

Page 81: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

Saussure, F. de. Cours de linguistique g é n é rale . Genève

1916.

Sekaquaptewa, Pat. " Liberalism and the Hopi Way ", Seminar

paper of 12/1/95. Anthropology of Law Class (Professor

Robert D. Cooter): Spring Semester, UC Berkeley CA, 1995.

Sheets-Johnstone, Maxime. The Roots of Thinking .

Philadelphia 1990: Temple Univ.

Simmons, Jr., L. Michael. "Complexity Reconsidered"

(letter to the editor), Scientific American , October 1995,

10.

Sober, Elliott. "Models of Cultural Evolution". In:

Griffiths, P. (ed.), Trees of Life: Essays in the

Philosophy of Biology (Australian Studies in the History

and Philosophy of Science), Kluwer 199, 17-38.

Svozil, K. "How Real are Virtual Realities, How Virtual is

Reality? - Constructive Re-interpretation of Physical

Undecidability". 1 Complexity 43-54 (1995-96).

Tietz, Reinhard (ed.). Wert- und Pr ä ferenzprobleme in den

Sozialwissenschaften . Schriften des Vereins für

Sozialpolitik NF 122, Berlin 1981: Duncker & Humblot.

Tylor, Edward B. Primitive Culture . London 1871. 2nd ed.

London 1873: John Murray.

--. Anthropology. An Introduction to the Study of Man and

Civilization. London 1881: Macmillan & Co.

Vaihinger, Hans. Die Philosophie des Als-Ob . 1911.

Page 82: Cultural Complexity - sfi-edu.s3.amazonaws.com · (with Dan McShea); "Pueblo Law Research - An Example for a Study in Cultural Complexity", Santa Fe Institute Science ... This illustrates

van West, Carla. Modeling Prehistoric Agricultural

Productivity in Southwestern Colorado: A GIS Approach.

Reports of Investigations 67. Dept. of Anthropology,

Washington State Univ., Pullman, 1994.

Vicenti, Carey N. (Chief Judge of the Jicarilla Apache

Tribe), Of Primitive Man and the Modern World. Paper (no

year; about 1995).

Viehweg, Theodor. Topics and Law (W. Cole Durham trans.).

Berne etc. 1993: Peter Lang (German orig. Topik und

Jurisprudence. Munich (1953).

Wagner, Richard. Ü ber Regulationen im lebenden Organismus .

München 1950: C.H. Beck (Comm.)

--. Probleme und Beispiele biologischer Regelung . Stuttgart

1954: Thieme.

Waldrop, M. Mitchell. Complexity . New York etc. 1992:

Simon & Schuster.

Wiener, Norbert. Cybernetics: Or Control and Communication

in the Animal and in the Machine. Cambridge, Mass. 1948:

Technol. Press.

Wilson, David S., and Sober, Elliott. Re-Introducing Group

Selection to the Human Behavioral Sciences . Cambridge UK

1994: Cambridge Univ. Press.

Wilcox, David R., and Haas, Jonathan. " The Scream of the Butterfly:

Competition and Conflict in the Prehistoric Southwest ". In: Gumerman,

George J. (ed.). Themes in Southwest Prehistory. Santa Fe NM 1994:

School of American Research Press, 211-238.