Debit Card - Spending Propensity

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/2/2019 Debit Card - Spending Propensity

    1/9

    ORIGINAL PAPER

    Time to Cut Up Those Debit Cards? Effect of Payment

    Mode on Willingness to Spend

    Amy Moore & Michael Taylor

    Received: 17 May 2011 /Accepted: 22 August 2011 /

    Published online: 13 September 2011# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC. 2011

    Abstract Financial industry data indicate that consumers increasingly prefer debit cards over

    credit cards, especially as a means of enforcing financial self-discipline. Given prior research

    suggesting that credit cards act as spending facilitating stimuli, this move toward reduced credit

    card use would appear to be in the right direction. Ironically, however, the same logos that were

    implicated in facilitating spending with credit cards are the logos that appear on debit cards. In

    what should serve as an eye-opener to consumers, it is found that exposure to debit card logos

    does result in an increased willingness to spend, similar to credit cards.

    Keywords Debit cards . Credit cards . Willingness to spend . Consumer behavior. Consumer

    policy

    For consumers reeling from a series of economic body blows, debit cards are

    increasingly becoming the plastic of choice. Some use the cards, which pull money

    directly from a bank or other account, as a budgeting tool to limit spending.

    Palmeri (2008).

    Consumers are turning from one form of plastic to another....Credit cards are falling

    out of favor as cardholders become more cautious and look for more conservative

    payment methods.

    James Van Dyke, President, Javelin financial services research firm, quoted in

    Leondis (2010).

    As the above quotes indicate, debit cards are increasingly preferred by consumers

    over credit cards as a way to enforce financial self-discipline. In sheer number, debit

    J Consum Policy (2011) 34:415422DOI 10.1007/s10603-011-9172-7

    A. Moore (*)

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/2/2019 Debit Card - Spending Propensity

    2/9

    card transactions in the USA surpassed credit card transactions in 2006, with 25.3

    billion debit transactions against 21.7 billion credit transactions (US Census Bureau

    2009). The Survey of Consumer Payment Choice conducted by the Boston Federal

    Reserve reported that in 2008, more consumers had debit cards than credit cards (Foster

    et al. 2010).Consistent with this overall increase in debit card usage was the announcement by Visa

    that spending on Visa debit cards had surpassed credit spending in 2008 for the first time in

    the company's history (Visa Inc 2009). Visa Inc.'s President for North America, Bill Sheedy,

    highlighted in the announcement that....debit is an ideal way for consumers tofocus on

    smarter spending, budgeting and control.

    The use of debit cards for financial control is emphasized by other financial industry

    experts. Edgar Dunn & Company (2004) described consumers' preference to pay with debit

    cards as a fundamental shifttowardgreater fiscal discipline and added that the

    increased use of debit cards compared to credit cards is tied to consumers increasing

    commitment to greater personal financial discipline.

    In light of prior academic research demonstrating the credit card effect, this move

    toward reduced credit card use would seem to be in the right direction. The credit card

    effect (Feinberg 1986) refers to the finding that credit cards act as spending facilitating

    stimuli and people spend more when they are exposed to credit card logos. In his

    experiments, Feinberg (1986) demonstrated that exposure to credit card logos increased

    participants' willingness to spend.

    Ironically, consumers looking to enforce financial self-control with debit cards are

    exposed to the very same logos when they use their debit cards. It is thus interesting

    to study whether a similar increase in willingness to spend occurs in response todebit cards. In our experiment, we found that debit cards with logos did indeed elicit

    an increased willingness to spend compared to cash. In fact, logoed debit cards

    elicited a willingness to spend comparable to credit cards, suggesting that debit cards

    with logos may actually work to hinder a consumer's goal of financial self-discipline.

    In the absence of a logo, however, debit cards elicited no difference in spending

    compared to cash.

    We present a brief review of related literature in the next section followed by a

    description of the experimental setup. We conclude with the findings and a discussion

    of the results.

    Related Research

    Prior research has documented the tendency of consumers to spend more with credit cards

    compared to cash. Feinberg (1986) demonstrated this effect in a restaurant tipping study

    where a random sample of patrons who paid with credit cards left larger tips compared with

    patrons who paid with cash. Prelec and Duncan (2001) found that willingness to pay was

    increased in high-value buying transactions when customers were instructed to pay with a

    credit card rather than cash.Further, research has revealed that merely being exposed to credit card insignia

    results in a greater willingness to spend. The mere presence of credit card

    416 A. Moore, M. Taylor

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/2/2019 Debit Card - Spending Propensity

    3/9

    Additionally, in the presence of credit card logos, subjects estimated that they would

    donate more to charity; actual donations were also higher in the presence of credit card

    insignia.

    Raghubir and Srivastava (2008) replicated this effect of credit card logos when they

    found that participants were willing to pay more for menu items in a hypothetical restaurantwhen a credit card logo was present versus absent. McCall and Belmont (1996) found a

    similar effect in two different restaurants when diners receiving tip trays with credit card

    logos left higher tips compared to diners who received blank tip trays.

    Prior research thus suggests that merely being exposed to credit card logos1 increases

    consumers' propensity to spend. Debit cards carry the same logos as credit cards and at

    cursory glance are almost indistinguishable from credit cards. There is a dearth of research on

    the influence of debit card logos. However, there is a large body of research on the mere

    exposure effect that we can draw on. The mere exposure effect (Zajonc 1968) refers to the

    finding that frequent exposure to a stimulus results in increased liking and a positive attitude

    toward the stimulus. Over 200 experiments investigating the mere exposure effect have beenpublished (for a review, see Bornstein 1989). The effect has been demonstrated to be robust

    across cultures, species, in different stimulus domains, and even prenatally (Zajonc 2001). As

    in the original studies (Zajonc 1968), subsequent studies have found that the more often

    people are exposed to a stimulus, the more positive are their attitudes toward the stimulus.

    We therefore hypothesize that the mere exposure to logos on debit cards will result in an

    increased willingness to spend, similar to credit cards. Although debit cards have received

    some attention in academic studies (e.g., Soman 2001 includes debit cards as one method of

    payment in studying the influence of past payments on future expenditure), we are not

    aware of any research that compares debit cards with other modes of payment in terms ofconsumers' willingness to spend. If the logos on debit cards facilitate a similar willingness

    to spend as credit card logos, debit cards may well defeat consumers' intended goals of

    financial self-control.

    To assess this effect experimentally, we used four modes of payment: cash, credit, debit-logo,

    and debit-statement. Cash and credit card are self-explanatory and were included to facilitate

    comparison with prior research. Further, we used two distinct debit card conditions, one with

    logos and one without, in order to assess the mere exposure effect of logos. The debit-logo

    condition exposed subjects to credit card logos in the debit payment mode. The debit-statement

    condition, on the other hand, merely employed a statement describing the debit mode of

    payment, and subjects were not exposed to logos; it was included to serve as a control so that themere exposure effect could be explored. Our expectation based on prior research was that debit

    cards with logos would elicit an increased willingness to pay, comparable to credit cards. In

    addition, if the mere exposure explanation is viable, we expected that the debit-without-logo

    condition would not elicit a similar effect. Both these expectations were borne out by our

    experimental results, as described more fully in the following sections.

    Method

    To study the effect of payment mode on willingness to spend, we used a paper-and-pen

    survey in which participants indicated the price they would be willing to pay for six

    Time to Cut Up Those Debit Cards? 417

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/2/2019 Debit Card - Spending Propensity

    4/9

    common gift items.2 The four payment modes we used were cash, credit card, debit card

    with logo, and debit card statement. Mode of payment was between-subjects, with each

    participant being randomly assigned to one of the four payment conditions.

    We thus used a 4 (payment) 6 (items) mixed-model experimental design where

    payment was the between-subjects factor and the six items served as the within-subjectsfactor. Level of spending, the dependent variable, was measured as the willingness to pay

    for the different items. Subjects were asked to imagine that they were shopping for holiday

    gifts and that the items pictured were on their list of things to buy. They were asked to

    indicate the amount of money they would be willing to pay for the pictured item. The

    photos of the items were picked from a Google image search and had a caption such as

    Leather gloves: willing to pay $___ with space for the respondent to enter the amount. To

    ensure that responses were not influenced by any imagined budget constraints, subjects

    were told that there was an adequate budget set aside to buy all the gifts.

    To manipulate method of payment, the instructions specified that there was a store that sold

    all the gift items they wanted to buy. In each treatment condition, participants were told that thestore only accepted that particular mode of payment. In addition, to serve as a reminder, the

    payment mode was included prominently near each photo such that the respondent saw it every

    time they filled in the amount they were willing to pay for an item. For the cash condition, this

    reminder was in the form ofcash only in uppercase letters. For the credit card condition, the

    reminder used credit cards only in uppercase letters along with Visa, MasterCard and

    Discover logos. The reminder for the debit card logo condition was uppercase letters debit

    cards only along with Visa and MasterCard logos, and for the debit card statement condition

    the reminder consisted of justdebit only in uppercase letters.

    The photographs of the items along with the reminder text/logos were presented in thesame order in all four treatment conditions. After subjects had indicated their willingness to

    pay for the different items, they completed four Likert-type statements to capture attitudes

    toward spending and price awareness3 and demographic questions.

    One hundred and ten individuals from the general population of a mid-sized university town

    in Western USA participated in the study (61 males, 46 females, 3 did not specify gender).

    Young adults 18 to 23 years of age made up 54% of the sample, 24% indicated they were

    between 24 and 28 years of age, and 20% older than 29. Participants wrote in their occupation in

    response to an open-ended question; students constituted the largest occupation category (40%),

    with the rest belonging to a range of occupations from accountant to venture analyst.

    Analysis and Findings

    We first checked that participants in the four experimental conditions did not vary

    systematically in terms of their demographic or attitudinal characteristics. Chi-square tests

    on the demographic variables confirmed that the distribution of subjects' age (2(12)=

    14.55, p=.27) and gender (2(3)=3.33, p=.34) was not different across the four payment

    2

    The six items were selected on the basis of a pre-test. The pre-test consisted of an unaided recall task inwhich each of the 15 individuals made a list of as many gift items as he/she could remember that they hadpurchased recently along with the price (or price range) they had paid. Based on the responses from the pre-

    418 A. Moore, M. Taylor

  • 8/2/2019 Debit Card - Spending Propensity

    5/9

    conditions. Similarly, analysis of variance with the scores on the Likert-type attitudinal

    statements as the dependent variable and payment as the independent variable confirmedthat subjects' attitudes toward spending did not vary systematically across the treatment

    conditions (all p>.05).

    We could therefore be confident that our subsequent analysis revealed effects directly

    related to our experimental manipulation. Since willingness to pay for the items was elicited

    from each participant, we analyzed the data using a one-way repeated-measures analysis of

    variance with payment mode as the independent variable and repeated measures on the

    items. The dependent variable in the ANOVA was the item price each respondent was

    willing to pay. The repeated-measures ANOVA showed a significant effect of method of

    payment on participants' willingness to pay (F(3,104)=19.4, p

  • 8/2/2019 Debit Card - Spending Propensity

    6/9

    condition for all six items (leather gloves: Mdebit logo

    =30.92, Mdebit statement

    =16.07, t(33.1)=

    3.14, p=.004; kids' bicycle: Mdebit logo=87.73, Mdebit statement=51.25, t(36.39)=3.61, p=.001;

    handbag: Mdebit logo=75.73, Mdebit statement=37.96, t(44.81)=2.97, p=.005; perfume: Mdebit

    logo=62.04, Mdebit statement=16.96, t(28.85)=6.48, p=.0001; necklace: Mdebit logo=27, Mdebit

    statement=13.18, t(34.55)=2.85, p=.007; silverware: Mdebit logo=33.85, Mdebit statement=20.29, t

    (40.56)=3.15, p=.003). Table 1 summarizes the means under the four payment conditions

    and also indicates which means were significantly different from each other.

    Discussion and Concluding Comments

    The statistical analysis reported above reveals that participants were willing to spend

    significantly more in the debit-logo condition compared to cash for all six items. The

    willingness to spend in the debit-logo condition was as high as the stated willingness to spend in

    the credit card condition. Further, confirming the mere exposure effect of logos, we found that it

    was only in the presence of logos that participants were willing to spend more using debit cards;

    stated spending was lower when debit cards were not accompanied by the logos.

    The results thus support our hypothesis that exposure to logos on debit cards will

    increase participants' level of spending just like credit cards. The level of spending with

    debit cards was, in fact, statistically no different than spending in the credit card condition

    and was significantly higher than in the cash condition. Of particular interest is the result

    that when logos were present in the debit condition, the willingness to spend was

    significantly higher compared to the debit condition in which participants were not exposed

    to any logos. This pattern of results strongly suggests that debit card logos spur consumers'

    willingness to spend more.

    To be sure, these are results from a one-shot experimental study, which makes them

    susceptible to the caveats surrounding all experimental results. While we would love to

    validate these results through real-world transaction data, we are hampered by our lack of

    access to suitable purchase data of requisite granularity. Another limitation, derived from

    recent research conducted in New Zealand (Lie et al. 2010), is that our results areidiosyncratic to the USA.5 The results do suggest though that given current American

    attitudes toward credit and debit use a consumer may well fall prey to the logos on her

    Table 1 Mean willingness to pay by payment condition

    Item Cash Credit Debit logo Debit statement F (all p

  • 8/2/2019 Debit Card - Spending Propensity

    7/9

    from the cycle of credit card debt, the logos on debit cards can become the source of

    financial harm through overspending.

    We would like to point out here that overspending due to the debit card logo effect is

    not necessarily a given. Even in the case of credit cards, research suggests that positive

    experiences with credit card use are required for an amplification of the willingness tospend (e.g., Feinberg and Meoli 1987). In fact, Feinberg himself (Feinberg 1990), in a

    comment on Hunt et al. (1990), pointed out that historical context and positive attitudes

    toward credit cards could be necessary conditions for the manifestation of the credit card

    effect. Thus, by analogy, overspending with debit cards too may require that consumers

    have largely positive associations with debit card use.

    In other words, if consumers are subjected to numerous aversive experiences with

    the use of debit cards, they may, over time, develop negative associations with debit

    cards leading to a dampening of the overspending effect. Such dampening would have

    been more likely before recent changes in US banking regulations, because prior to July

    1, 2010, consumers were subject to prohibitive and insidious overdraft fees on debitcards. With recent Federal Reserve rules restricting banks from charging such hidden

    overdraft fees on debit card transactions, debit card use has become relatively more

    positive for consumers. Ironically, however, such positive changes to protect consumers

    from debit card fees may well mean that consumers remain susceptible to the

    overspending effect with the use of debit cards. In any event, with the current trend

    toward increased debit card use, the results from our study could serve as an eye-opener

    to consumers and policy-makers alike.

    References

    Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 19681987.Psychological Bulletin, 106(2), 265289.

    Edgar Dunn & Company. (2004, January 27). Consumers choose debit cards as most preferred card in theirwallets [Press Release]. Retrieved from http://www.directiveanalytics.com/pdf/Consumers%20Choose%20Debit%20Cards%20As%20Most%20Preferred%20Card%20in%20The..pdf

    Hunt, J. M., Florscheim, R., Chatterjee, A., & Kernan, J. (1990). Credit cards as spending facilitating stimuli:A test and extension of Feinberg's conditioning hypothesis. Psychological Reports, 67(1), 323330.

    Feinberg, R. A. (1986). Credit cards as spending facilitating stimuli: A conditioning interpretation. Journal of

    Consumer Research, 13(3), 348356.Feinberg, R. A. (1990). The social nature of classical conditioning phenomena in people: A comment on

    Hunt, Florscheim, Chatterjee, and Kernan. Psychological Reports, 67(1), 331334.Feinberg, R. A., & Meoli, J. (1987). The classical conditioning of credit card cues. Proceedings of the Division of

    Consumer Psychology, American Psychological Association 1986 Annual Conference, 138140.Foster, K., Meijer, E., Schuh, S., & Zabek, M. A. (2010). The 2008 survey of consumer payment choice.

    Public Policy Discussion Papers, Federal Reserve Bank of Boston, April 2010.Leondis, A. (2010). Cardholders prefer debit as credit-card use declines. Bloomberg Businessweek, Sept. 08,

    2010. Retrieved from http://www.businessweek.com/news/2010-09-08/cardholders-prefer-debit-as-credit-card-use-declines.html

    Lie, C., Hunt, M., Peters, H., Veliu, B., & Harper, D. (2010). The negative credit card effect: Credit cards asspending-limiting stimuli in New Zealand. The Psychological Record, 60(3), 399412.

    McCall, M., & Belmont, H. J. (1996). Credit card insignia and restaurant tipping: Evidence for an associativelink. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81(5), 609613.

    Palmeri, C. (2008). The steady ascent of the debit card. Bloomberg Business Week, Nov. 10, 2008. Retrieved

    Time to Cut Up Those Debit Cards? 421

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://www.directiveanalytics.com/pdf/Consumers%20Choose%20Debit%20Cards%20As%20Most%20Preferred%20Card%20in%20The..pdfhttp://www.directiveanalytics.com/pdf/Consumers%20Choose%20Debit%20Cards%20As%20Most%20Preferred%20Card%20in%20The..pdfhttp://www.businessweek.com/news/2010-09-08/cardholders-prefer-debit-as-credit-card-use-declines.htmlhttp://www.businessweek.com/news/2010-09-08/cardholders-prefer-debit-as-credit-card-use-declines.htmlhttp://www.businessweek.com/news/2010-09-08/cardholders-prefer-debit-as-credit-card-use-declines.htmlhttp://www.businessweek.com/news/2010-09-08/cardholders-prefer-debit-as-credit-card-use-declines.htmlhttp://www.directiveanalytics.com/pdf/Consumers%20Choose%20Debit%20Cards%20As%20Most%20Preferred%20Card%20in%20The..pdfhttp://www.directiveanalytics.com/pdf/Consumers%20Choose%20Debit%20Cards%20As%20Most%20Preferred%20Card%20in%20The..pdfhttp://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/2/2019 Debit Card - Spending Propensity

    8/9

    Shimp, T. A., & Moody, M. (2000). In search of a theoretical explanation for the credit card effect. Journal ofBusiness Research, 48(1), 1723.

    Soman, D. (2001). Effects of Payment Mechanism on Spending Behavior: The Role of Rehearsal andImmediacy of Payments. Journal of Consumer Research, 27(4), 460474.

    Census Bureau, U. S. (2009). The 2010 Statistical Abstract: Banking, Finance, and Insurance. Washington,

    DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.Visa Inc. (2009). Total U.S. Visa debit volume surpasses credit for the first time [Press Release, May 4].Retrieved from http://investor.visa.com/phoenix.zhtml?c=215693&p=irol-newsArticle&ID=1283874&highlight=

    Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social PsychologyMonographs, 9(2), 127.

    Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Current Directions in PsychologicalScience, 10(6), 224228.

    422 A. Moore, M. Taylor

    http://investor.visa.com/phoenix.zhtml?c=215693&p=irol-newsArticle&ID=1283874&highlighthttp://investor.visa.com/phoenix.zhtml?c=215693&p=irol-newsArticle&ID=1283874&highlighthttp://investor.visa.com/phoenix.zhtml?c=215693&p=irol-newsArticle&ID=1283874&highlighthttp://investor.visa.com/phoenix.zhtml?c=215693&p=irol-newsArticle&ID=1283874&highlight
  • 8/2/2019 Debit Card - Spending Propensity

    9/9

    Copyright of Journal of Consumer Policy is the property of Springer Science & Business Media B.V. and its

    content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's

    express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.