18
Decision-making From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia See also: decision theory Sample flowchart representing the decision process to add a new article to Wikipedia. Decision-making can be regarded as the cognitive process resulting in the selection of a belief or a course of action among several alternative possibilities. Every decision-making process produces a final choice [1] that may or may not prompt action. Decision-making is the study of identifying and choosing alternatives based on the values and preferences of the decision maker. Decision-making is one of the central activities of management and is a huge part of any process of implementation. Overview[edit ] Human performance with regard to decisions has been the subject of active research from several perspectives: Psychological : examining individual decisions in the context of a set of needs, preferences and values the individual has or seeks. Cognitive : the decision-making process regarded as a continuous process integrated in the interaction with the environment. Normative : the analysis of individual decisions concerned with the logic of decision-making and rationality and the invariant choice it leads to. [2]

Decision Making

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Decision Making

Decision-makingFrom Wikipedia, the free encyclopediaSee also: decision theory

Sample flowchart representing the decision process to add a new article to Wikipedia.

Decision-making can be regarded as the cognitive process resulting in the selection of a belief or a course of action among several alternative possibilities. Every decision-making process produces a final choice [1]  that may or may not prompt action. Decision-making is the study of identifying and choosing alternatives based on the values and preferences of the decision maker. Decision-making is one of the central activities of management and is a huge part of any process of implementation.

Overview[edit]

Human performance with regard to decisions has been the subject of active research from several

perspectives:

Psychological : examining individual decisions in the context of a set of needs, preferences

and values the individual has or seeks.

Cognitive : the decision-making process regarded as a continuous process integrated in the

interaction with the environment.

Normative : the analysis of individual decisions concerned with the logic of decision-

making and rationality and the invariant choice it leads to.[2]

Decision-making can also be regarded as a problem-solving activity terminated by a solution

deemed to be satisfactory. It is, therefore, a reasoning or emotional process which can

be rational or irrational and can be based on explicit assumptions or tacit assumptions. Rational

choice theory encompasses the notion that people try to maximize benefits while minimizing costs.[3]

Page 2: Decision Making

Some have argued that most decisions are made unconsciously. Jim Nightingale states that "we

simply decide without thinking much about the decision process."[4] In a controlled environment, such

as a classroom, instructors might try to encourage students to weigh pros and cons before making a

decision. This strategy is known as Franklin's rule. However, because such a rule requires time,

cognitive resources and full access to relevant information about the decision, this rule may not best

describe how people make decisions.[citation needed]

Logical decision-making is an important part of all science-based professions, where specialists

apply their knowledge in a given area to make informed decisions. For example, medical decision-

making often involves a diagnosis and the selection of appropriate treatment. Some[which?] research

using naturalistic methods shows, however, that in situations with higher time pressure, higher

stakes, or increased ambiguities, experts use intuitive decision-making rather than structured

approaches – following a recognition primed decision that fits the their experience – and arrive at a

course of action without weighing alternatives. Recent robust decision research has formally

integrated uncertainty into its decision-making model.[citation needed] Decision analysis recognized and

included uncertainties in its theorizing since its conception in 1964.[citation needed]

A major part of decision-making involves the analysis of a finite set of alternatives described in terms

of evaluative criteria. Information overload occurs when there is a substantial gap between the

capacity of information and the ways in which people may or can adapt. The overload of information

can be related to problem≠ processing and tasking, which effects decision-making.[5] These criteria

may be benefit or cost in nature. Then the problem might be to rank these alternatives in terms of

how attractive they are to the decision-maker(s) when all the criteria are considered simultaneously.

Another goal might be to just find the best alternative or to determine the relative total priority of each

alternative (for instance, if alternatives represent projects competing for funds) when all the criteria

are considered simultaneously. Solving such problems is the focus of multi-criteria decision

analysis (MCDA), also known as multi-criteria decision-making (MCDM). This area of decision-

making, although very old, has attracted the interest of many researchers and practitioners and is

still highly debated as there are many MCDA/MCDM methods which may yield very different results

when they are applied on exactly the same data.[6] This leads to the formulation of a decision-making

paradox.

In regards to management and decision-making, each level of management is responsible for

different things. Top level managers look at and create strategic plans where the organization's

vision, goals, and values are taken into account to create a plan that is cohesive with the mission

statement. For mid-level managers, tactical plans are created with specific steps with actions that

need to be executed to meet the strategic objective. Finally, the front-line managers are responsible

for creating and executing operational plans. These plans include the policies, processes, and

procedures of the organization. Each must take into account the overall goals and processes of the

organization.

Page 3: Decision Making

Rational and irrational decision-making[edit]

In economics, it is thought that if humans are rational and free to make their own decisions, then

they would behave according to rational choice theory.[7] This theory states that people make

decisions by determining the likelihood of a potential outcome, the value of the outcome and then

multiplying the two. For example, with a 50% chance of winning $20 or a 100% chance of winning

$10, people more likely to choose the first option.[7]

In reality, however, there are some factors that affect decision-making abilities and cause people to

make irrational decisions, one of them being availability bias. Availability bias is the tendency for

some items that are more readily available in memory to be judged as more frequently occurring.[7] For example, someone who watches a lot of movies about terrorist attacks may think the

frequency of terrorism to be higher than it actually is.

Information overload[edit]

Information overload is "a gap between the volume of information and the tools we need to

assimilate it."[8] It is proven in some studies[which?] that the more information overload, the worse the

quality of decisions made. There are five factors:

Personal Information Factors: personal qualifications, experiences, attitudes etc.

Information Characteristics: information quality, quantity and frequency

Tasks and Process: standardized procedures or methods

Organizational Design: organizations' cooperation, processing capacity and organization

relationship

Information Technology: IT management, and general technology

Hall, Ariss & Todorov with an assistant Rashar phinyor (2007) described an illusion of knowledge,

meaning that as individuals encounter too much knowledge it actually interferes with their ability to

make rational decisions.[9]

Problem analysis vs. decision-making[edit]

It is important to differentiate between problem analysis and decision-making. The concepts are

completely separate from one another. Traditionally, it is argued that problem analysis must be done

first, so that the information gathered in that process may be used towards decision-making.[10]

Problem analysis

Analyze performance, what should the results be against what they actually are

Page 4: Decision Making

Problems are merely deviations from performance standards

Problem must be precisely identified and described

Problems are caused by a change from a distinctive feature

Something can always be used to distinguish between what has and hasn't been affected by

a cause

Causes to problems can be deducted from relevant changes found in analyzing the problem

Most likely cause to a problem is the one that exactly explains all the facts

Decision-making

Objectives must first be established

Objectives must be classified and placed in order of importance

Alternative actions must be developed

The alternative must be evaluated against all the objectives

The alternative that is able to achieve all the objectives is the tentative decision

The tentative decision is evaluated for more possible consequences

The decisive actions are taken, and additional actions are taken to prevent any adverse

consequences from becoming problems and starting both systems (problem analysis and

decision-making) all over again

There are steps that are generally followed that result in a decision model that can be used

to determine an optimal production plan.[11]

In a situation featuring conflict, role-playing may be helpful for predicting decisions to be

made by involved parties.[12]

Decision planning[edit]

Making a decision without planning is fairly common, but does not often end well. Planning allows for

decisions to be made comfortably and in a smart way. Planning makes decision-making a lot more

simple than it is.

Decision will get four benefits out of planning: 1. Planning give chance to the establishment of

independent goals. It is a conscious and directed series of choices. 2. Planning provides a standard

of measurement. It is a measurement of whether you are going towards or further away from your

Page 5: Decision Making

goal. 3. Planning converts values to action. You think twice about the plan and decide what will help

advance your plan best. 4. Planning allows for limited resources to be committed in an orderly way.

Always govern the use of what is limited to you. (e.g. money, time, etc.)[13]

Analysis paralysis[edit]

Analysis paralysis is the state of over-analyzing (or over-thinking) a situation, or citing sources, so

that a decision or action is never taken, in effect paralyzing the outcome.

Everyday techniques[edit]

Decision-making techniques can be separated into two broad categories: Group decision-

making and individual decision-making techniques.

Group decision-making techniques[edit]

Consensus decision-making  tries to avoid "winners" and "losers". Consensus requires that a

majority approve a given course of action, but that the minority agree to go along with the course

of action. In other words, if the minority opposes the course of action, consensus requires that

the course of action be modified to remove objectionable features.

Voting-based methods .

Range voting  lets each member score one or more of the available options. The option with the highest average is chosen. This method has experimentally been shown to produce the lowest Bayesian regret among common voting methods, even when voters are strategic.[citation needed]

Majority  requires support from more than 50% of the members of the group. Thus, the bar for action is lower than with unanimity and a group of "losers" is implicit to this rule.[citation needed]

Plurality , where the largest block in a group decides, even if it falls short of a majority.

Delphi method  is structured communication technique for groups, originally developed for

collaborative forecasting but has also been used for policy making.

Dotmocracy  is a facilitation method that relies on the use of special forms called Dotmocracy

Sheets to allow large groups to collectively brainstorm and recognize agreement on an unlimited

number of ideas they have authored.

Individual decision-making techniques[edit]

Pros and cons: listing the advantages and disadvantages of each option, popularized

by Plato and Benjamin Franklin.[14][15] Contrast the costs and benefits of all alternatives. Also

called "rational decision-making".

Page 6: Decision Making

Simple prioritization : choosing the alternative with the highest probability-weighted utility for

each alternative (see Decision analysis).

Satisficing : examining alternatives only until an acceptable one is found.

Elimination by aspects: choosing between alternatives using Mathematical psychology [16]  The

technique was introduced by Amos Tversky in 1972. It is a covert elimination process that

involves comparing all available alternatives by aspects. The decision-maker chooses an

aspect; any alternatives without that aspect are then eliminated. The decision-maker repeats this

process with as many aspects as needed until there remains only one alternative[17]

Preference trees: In 1979, Tversky and Shmuel Sattach updated the elimination by aspects

technique by presenting a more ordered and structured way of comparing the available

alternatives. This technique compared the alternatives by presenting the aspects in a decided

and sequential order. It became a more hierarchical system in which the aspects are ordered

from general to specific [18]

Acquiesce to a person in authority or an "expert"; "just following orders".

Flipism : flipping a coin, cutting a deck of playing cards, and other random or coincidence

methods[19]

Prayer , tarot cards, astrology, augurs, revelation, or other forms of divination.

Taking the most opposite action compared to the advice of mistrusted authorities (parents,

police officers, partners...)

Opportunity cost : calculating the opportunity cost of each options and decide the decision.

Bureaucratic: set up criteria for automated decisions.

Political: negotiate choices among interest groups.

Participative decision-making  (PDM): a methodology in which a single decision-maker, in

order to take advantage of additional input, opens up the decision-making process to a group for

a collaborative effort.

Use of a structured decision-making method.[20]

Individual decision-making techniques can often be applied by a group as part of a group decision-

making technique.

Page 7: Decision Making

A need to use software for a decision-making process is emerging for individuals and businesses.

This is due to increasing decision complexity and an increase in the need to consider additional

stakeholders, categories, elements or other factors that effect decisions.

Stages of group decision-making[edit]

According to B. Aubrey Fisher, there are four stages or phases that should be involved in all group

decision-making:[21]

Orientation. Members meet for the first time and start to get to know each other.

Conflict. Once group members become familiar with each other, disputes, little fights and

arguments occur. Group members eventually work it out.

Emergence. The group begins to clear up vague opinions by talking about them.

Reinforcement. Members finally make a decision and provide justification for it.

It is said that critical norms in a group improves the quality of decisions, while the majority of

opinions (called consensus norms) do not. This is due to collaboration between one another, and

when group members get used to, and familiar with, each other, they will tend to argue and create

more of a dispute to agree upon one decision. This does not mean that all group members fully

agree; they may not want argue further just to be liked by other group members or to "fit in".[22]

Decision-making steps[edit]

Each step in the decision-making process may include social, cognitive and cultural obstacles to

successfully negotiating dilemmas. It has been suggested that becoming more aware of these

obstacles allows one to better anticipate and overcome them.[23] The Arkansas program presents

eight stages of moral decision-making based on the work ofJames Rest:

1. Establishing community: creating and nurturing the relationships, norms, and procedures

that will influence how problems are understood and communicated. This stage takes place

prior to and during a moral dilemma.

2. Perception: recognizing that a problem exists.

3. Interpretation: identifying competing explanations for the problem, and evaluating the drivers

behind those interpretations.

4. Judgment: sifting through various possible actions or responses and determining which is

more justifiable.

Page 8: Decision Making

5. Motivation: examining the competing commitments which may distract from a more moral

course of action and then prioritizing and committing to moral values over other personal,

institutional or social values.

6. Action: following through with action that supports the more justified decision. Integrity is

supported by the ability to overcome distractions and obstacles, developing implementing

skills, and ego strength.

7. Reflection in action.

8. Reflection on action.

Other decision-making processes have also been proposed. One such process, proposed by Pam

Brown of Singleton Hospital in Swansea, Wales, breaks decision-making down into seven steps:[24]

1. Outline your goal and outcome.

2. Gather data.

3. Develop alternatives (i.e., brainstorming)

4. List pros and cons of each alternative.

5. Make the decision.

6. Immediately take action to implement it.

7. Learn from and reflect on the decision.

Cognitive and personal biases[edit]

Biases usually creep into decision-making processes. Many different people have made a decision

about the same question (e.g. "Should I have a doctor look at this troubling breast cancer symptom

I've discovered?" "Why did I ignore the evidence that the project was going over budget?") and then

craft potential cognitive interventions aimed at improving the outcome of decision-making.

Here is a list of commonly debated biases in judgment and decision-making.

Selective search for evidence (aka confirmation bias; Scott Plous, 1993). People tend to be

willing to gather facts that support certain conclusions but disregard other facts that support

different conclusions. Individuals who are highly defensive in this manner show significantly

greater left prefrontal cortex activity as measured by EEG than do less defensive individuals.[25]

Premature termination of search for evidence. People tend to accept the first alternative that

looks like it might work.

Page 9: Decision Making

Cognitive inertia . Unwillingness to change existing thought patterns in the face of new

circumstances.

Selective perception. We actively screen out information that we do not think is important

(see also prejudice). In one demonstration of this effect, discounting of arguments with which

one disagrees (by judging them as untrue or irrelevant) was decreased by selective activation of

right prefrontal cortex.[26]

Wishful thinking . A tendency to want to see things in a certain – usually positive – light, which

can distort perception and thinking.[27]

Choice-supportive bias  occurs when people distort their memories of chosen and rejected

options to make the chosen options seem more attractive.

Recency. People tend to place more attention on more recent information and either ignore

or forget more distant information (see semantic priming). The opposite effect in the first set of

data or other information is termed primacy effect.[28]

Repetition bias. A willingness to believe what one has been told most often and by the

greatest number of different sources.

Anchoring and adjustment . Decisions are unduly influenced by initial information that shapes

our view of subsequent information.

Group think . Peer pressure to conform to the opinions held by the group.

Source credibility bias. A tendency to reject a person's statement on the basis of a bias

against the person, organization, or group to which the person belongs. People preferentially

accept statement by others that they like (see prejudice).

Incremental decision-making and escalating commitment. We look at a decision as a small

step in a process and this tends to perpetuate a series of similar decisions. This can be

contrasted with "zero-based decision-making" (see slippery slope).

Attribution asymmetry . People tend to attribute their own success to internal factors,

including abilities and talents, but explain their failures in terms of external factors such as bad

luck. The reverse bias is shown when people explain others' success or failure.

Role fulfillment. A tendency to conform to others' decision-making expectations.

Underestimating uncertainty and the illusion of control. People tend to underestimate future

uncertainty because of a tendency to believe they have more control over events than they

really do.

Page 10: Decision Making

Framing bias . This is best avoided by using numeracy with absolute measures of efficacy.[29]

Sunk-cost fallacy . A specific type of framing effect that affects decision-making. It involves an individual making a decision about a current situation based on what they have previously invested in the situation.[30] A possible example to this would be an individual that is refraining from dropping a class that that they are most likely to fail, due to the fact that they feel as though they have done so much work in the course thus far.

Prospect theory . Involves the idea that when faced with a decision-making event, an

individual is more likely to take on a risk when evaluating potential losses, and are more likely to

avoid risks when evaluating potential gains. This can influence one's decision-making depending

if the situation entails a threat, or opportunity.[31]

Reference class forecasting was developed to eliminate or reduce cognitive biases in decision-

making.

Post-decision analysis[edit]

Evaluation and analysis of past decisions is complementary to decision-making; see also mental

accounting and postmortem documentation.

Cognitive styles[edit]

Influence of Myers-Briggs type[edit]

According to behavioralist Isabel Briggs Myers, a person's decision-making process depends to a

significant degree on their cognitive style.[32] Myers developed a set of four bi-polar dimensions,

called the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI). The terminal points on these dimensions

are: thinking and feeling; extroversion and introversion; judgment andperception;

and sensing and intuition. She claimed that a person's decision-making style correlates well with

how they score on these four dimensions. For example, someone who scored near the thinking,

extroversion, sensing, and judgment ends of the dimensions would tend to have a logical, analytical,

objective, critical, and empirical decision-making style. However, some[who?] psychologists say that

the MBTI lacks reliability and validity and is poorly constructed.

Other studies suggest that these national or cross-cultural differences exist across entire societies.

For example, Maris Martinsons has found that American, Japanese and Chinese business leaders

each exhibit a distinctive national style of decision-making.[33]

Optimizing vs. satisficing[edit]

Herbert A. Simon coined the phrase "bounded rationality" to express the idea that human decision-

making is limited by available information, available time and the mind's information-processing

ability. Simon also defined two cognitive styles: maximizers try to make an optimal decision,

whereas satisficers simply try to find a solution that is "good enough". Maximizers tend to take longer

Page 11: Decision Making

making decisions due to the need to maximize performance across all variables and make tradeoffs

carefully; they also tend to more often regret their decisions (perhaps because they are more able

than satisficers to recognise that a decision turned out to be sub-optimal).[34]

Combinatorial vs. positional[edit]

Styles and methods of decision-making were elaborated by Aron Katsenelinboigen, the founder

of predispositioning theory. In his analysis on styles and methods, Katsenelinboigen referred to the

game of chess, saying that “chess does disclose various methods of operation, notably the creation

of predisposition – methods which may be applicable to other, more complex systems.”[35]

In his book, Katsenelinboigen states that apart from the methods (reactive and selective) and sub-

methods (randomization, predispositioning, programming), there are two major styles: positional and

combinational. Both styles are utilized in the game of chess. According to Katsenelinboigen, the two

styles reflect two basic approaches to the uncertainty: deterministic (combinational style) and

indeterministic (positional style). Katsenelinboigen’s definition of the two styles are the

following.

This article contains embedded lists that may be poorly

defined, unverified or indiscriminate. Please help to clean it up to meet

Wikipedia's quality standards. Where appropriate, incorporate items into

the main body of the article. (February 2008)

The combinational style is characterized by:

a very narrow, clearly defined, primarily material goal; and

a program that links the initial position with the final outcome.

In defining the combinational style in chess, Katsenelinboigen writes:

The combinational style features a clearly formulated limited objective, namely the capture of

material (the main constituent element of a chess position). The objective is implemented via a well-

defined, and in some cases, unique sequence of moves aimed at reaching the set goal. As a rule,

this sequence leaves no options for the opponent. Finding a combinational objective allows the

player to focus all his energies on efficient execution, that is, the player’s analysis may be limited to

the pieces directly partaking in the combination. This approach is the crux of the combination and

the combinational style of play.[35]

The positional style is distinguished by:

a positional goal; and

a formation of semi-complete linkages between the initial step and final outcome.

Page 12: Decision Making

“Unlike the combinational player, the positional player is occupied, first and foremost, with the

elaboration of the position that will allow him to develop in the unknown future. In playing the

positional style, the player must evaluate relational and material parameters as independent

variables. ... The positional style gives the player the opportunity to develop a position until it

becomes pregnant with a combination. However, the combination is not the final goal of the

positional player—it helps him to achieve the desirable, keeping in mind a predisposition for the

future development. The pyrrhic victory is the best example of one’s inability to think positionally."[36]

The positional style serves to:

create a predisposition to the future development of the position;

induce the environment in a certain way;

absorb an unexpected outcome in one’s favor;

avoid the negative aspects of unexpected outcomes.

Katsenelinboigen writes:

"As the game progressed and defense became more sophisticated the combinational style of play declined. ... The positional style of chess does not eliminate the combinational one with its attempt to see the entire program of action in advance. The positional style merely prepares the transformation to a combination when the latter becomes feasible.”[37]

Neuroscience[edit]

Decision-making is a region of intense study in the fields of systems neuroscience, and cognitive

neuroscience. Several brain structures, including the anterior cingulate

cortex(ACC), orbitofrontal cortex and the overlapping ventromedial prefrontal cortex are believed

to be involved in decision-making processes. A recent neuroimaging study[38] found distinctive

patterns of neural activation in these regions depending on whether decisions were made on the

basis of perceived personal volition or following directions from someone else. Patients with

damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex have difficulty making advantageous decisions.[39]

A common laboratory paradigm for studying neural decision-making is the two-alternative forced

choice task (TAFC), in which a subject has to choose between two alternatives within a certain

time. A study of a two-alternative forced choice task involving rhesus monkeys found that

neurons in the parietal cortex not only represent the formation of a decision but also signal the

degree of certainty (or "confidence") associated with the decision.[40] Another recent study found

that lesions to the ACC in the macaque resulted in impaired decision-making in the long run of

reinforcement guided tasks suggesting that the ACC may be involved in evaluating past

reinforcement information and guiding future action.[41] A 2012 study found that rats and humans

can optimally accumulate incoming sensory evidence, to make statistically optimal decisions.[42]

Page 13: Decision Making

Emotion appears able to aid the decision-making process. Decision-making often occurs in the

face of uncertainty about whether one's choices will lead to benefit or harm (see also risk).

The somatic-marker hypothesis is a neurobiological theory of how decisions are made in the

face of uncertain outcome. This theory holds that such decisions are aided by emotions, in the

form of bodily states, that are elicited during the deliberation of future consequences and that

mark different options for behavior as being advantageous or disadvantageous. This process

involves an interplay between neural systems that elicit emotional/bodily states and neural

systems that map these emotional/bodily states.[43]

Although it is unclear whether the studies generalize to all processing, subconscious processes

have been implicated in the initiation of conscious volitional movements. See theNeuroscience

of free will.

Decision-making in adolescents vs. adults[edit]

This section needs additional citations for verification. Please

help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources.

Unsourced material may be challenged and removed. (May 2014)

During their adolescent years, teens are known for their high-risk behaviors and rash decisions.

There has not, however, been that much research in this area. Recent research[citation needed] has

shown, though, that there are some differences in cognitive processes between adolescents and

adults during decision-making. Researchers have concluded that differences in decision-making

are not due to a lack of logic or reasoning, but more due to the immaturity

of psychosocial capacities, capacities that influence decision-making. Examples would be

impulse control, emotion regulation, delayed gratification and resistance to peer pressure. In the

past, researchers have thought that adolescent behavior was simply due to incompetency

regarding decision-making. Currently, researchers have concluded that adults and adolescents

are both competent decision-makers, not just adults. However, adolescents’ competent

decision-making skills decrease when psychosocial capacities become present.

Recent research[citation needed] has shown that risk-taking behaviors in adolescents may be the

product of interactions between the socioemotional brain network and its cognitive-control

network. The socioemotional part of the brain processes social and emotional stimuli and has

been shown to be important in reward processing. The cognitive-control network assists in

planning and self-regulation. Both of these sections of the brain change over the course

of puberty. However, the socioemotional network changes quickly and abruptly, while the

cognitive-control network changes more gradually. Because of this difference in change the

cognitive-control network, which usually regulates the socioemotional network, [the adolescent?]

Page 14: Decision Making

struggles to control the socioemotional network when psychosocial capacities are present.[clarification needed]

When adolescents are exposed to social and emotional stimuli, their socioemotional network is

activated as well as areas of the brain involved in reward processing. Because teens often gain

a sense of reward from risk-taking behaviors, their repetition becomes ever more probable due

to the reward experienced. In this, the process mirrors addiction. Teens can become addicted to

risky behavior because they are in a high state of arousal and are rewarded for it not only by

their own internal functions but also by their peers around them.

This is why adults are generally better able to control their risk-taking because their cognitive-

control system has matured enough to the point where it can control the socioemotional

network, even in the context of high arousal or when psychosocial capacities are present. Also,

adults are less likely to find themselves in situations that push them to do risky things. For

example, teens are more likely to be around peers who peer pressure them into doing things,

while adults are not as exposed to this sort of social setting.[44][45]