38
Different or alike? Female rainbow kribs choose males of similar consistency and dissimilar level of boldness Article (Accepted Version) http://sro.sussex.ac.uk Scherer, Ulrike, Kuhnhardt, Mira and Schuett, Wiebke (2017) Different or alike? Female rainbow kribs choose males of similar consistency and dissimilar level of boldness. Animal Behaviour, 128. pp. 117-124. ISSN 0003-3472 This version is available from Sussex Research Online: http://sro.sussex.ac.uk/id/eprint/78355/ This document is made available in accordance with publisher policies and may differ from the published version or from the version of record. If you wish to cite this item you are advised to consult the publisher’s version. Please see the URL above for details on accessing the published version. Copyright and reuse: Sussex Research Online is a digital repository of the research output of the University. Copyright and all moral rights to the version of the paper presented here belong to the individual author(s) and/or other copyright owners. To the extent reasonable and practicable, the material made available in SRO has been checked for eligibility before being made available. Copies of full text items generally can be reproduced, displayed or performed and given to third parties in any format or medium for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge, provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way.

Different or alike? Female rainbow kribs choose males of ...sro.sussex.ac.uk/id/eprint/78355/2/Scherer-et-al_2017...38 choice, Pelvicachromis pulcher, personality, risk‐taking, sexual

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • Different or alike? Female rainbow kribs choose males of similar consistency and dissimilar level of boldness

    Article (Accepted Version)

    http://sro.sussex.ac.uk

    Scherer, Ulrike, Kuhnhardt, Mira and Schuett, Wiebke (2017) Different or alike? Female rainbow kribs choose males of similar consistency and dissimilar level of boldness. Animal Behaviour, 128. pp. 117-124. ISSN 0003-3472

    This version is available from Sussex Research Online: http://sro.sussex.ac.uk/id/eprint/78355/

    This document is made available in accordance with publisher policies and may differ from the published version or from the version of record. If you wish to cite this item you are advised to consult the publisher’s version. Please see the URL above for details on accessing the published version.

    Copyright and reuse: Sussex Research Online is a digital repository of the research output of the University.

    Copyright and all moral rights to the version of the paper presented here belong to the individual author(s) and/or other copyright owners. To the extent reasonable and practicable, the material made available in SRO has been checked for eligibility before being made available.

    Copies of full text items generally can be reproduced, displayed or performed and given to third parties in any format or medium for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge, provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way.

    http://sro.sussex.ac.uk/

  • 1

    Differentoralike?Femalerainbowkribschoosemalesofsimilarconsistency1

    anddis‐similarlevelofboldness23

    U.Scherer1,M.Kuhnhardt1andW.Schuett14561ZoologicalInstitute,BiocentreGrindel,UniversityofHamburg,Martin‐Luther‐KingPlatz3,720146Hamburg,Germany8910Correspondence:11UlrikeScherer,ZoologicalInstitute,BiocentreGrindel,UniversityofHamburg,Martin‐12Luther‐KingPlatz3,20146Hamburg,Germany.13E‐Mail:[email protected]:+494042838–789415 16

  • 2

    17Althoughtheexistenceofconsistentbetween‐individualdifferencesinbehaviour18("personalitydifferences")hasbeenwelldocumentedduringthelastdecade,theadaptive19valueofsuchbehaviourallimitationsstillremainsanopenfieldforresearchersofanimal20behaviour.Personalitiesclearlyrestrictindividualsintheirabilitytoadjusttheirbehaviour21todifferentconditions.However,sheercostsofflexibilitycannotexplainthepolymorphism22createdbypersonalityvariation.Inacorrelativeapproach,weheretestedwhethermate23choicemightactasamajordrivingforcemaintainingpersonalityvariationinthe24monogamous,biparentalrainbowkrib,Pelvicachromispulcher.Wepersonality‐typedall25malesandfemalesfortheirboldness(activityundersimulatedpredationrisk)andallowed26femalestochoosebetweentwomalesthatdifferedintheirboldness(behaviouralleveland27consistency).Priortothechoice,femaleswereallowedtoobservebothmales,expressing28theirnaturalboldnesstowardsavideoanimatednaturalpredator.Bothsexesshowed29personalitydifferencesinboldnessovertheshort‐andlong‐term.Furthermore,when30removingside‐biasedfemales,wefoundadis‐assortativematingpreferenceforthe31behaviourallevelandanassortativepreferenceforbehaviouralconsistencyinboldness.32Suchpreferencepatternsmightfacilitateeffectiveparentalroleallocationduringoffspring33careand/orprovidegeneticbenefits.Ourresultssuggestthatsexualselectionplaysan34importantroleintheevolutionofpersonalitydifferences.3536Keywords:anti‐predatorbehaviour,assortative,behaviouralcompatibility,cichlid,mate37choice,Pelvicachromispulcher,personality,risk‐taking,sexualselection,sidebias38 39

  • 3

    Individualshavetocopewithawidearrayofenvironmentalchallenges.Therefore,40flexibilityintheexpressionofbehaviouralresponsestowardsdifferentandchanging41conditionsshouldbefavouredbyselection(Sihetal.,2004).Yet,individualsoftenshow42considerableconsistentbetween‐individualdifferencesinbehaviourovertimeand/or43contexts(Boissy,1995).Suchpersonalitydifferencesarecommonthroughouttheanimal44kingdom(reviewedinGosling,2001;Kralj‐Fišeretal.,2014)andhavebeenshownfor45variousbehaviouraltraits,suchasactivitypattern,aggressiveness,exploratorytendencies,46boldnessandfearfulness(reviewedinDalletal.,2004;Gosling,2001;Sihetal.,2004).47Personalitytraitsaremoderatelyheritable(Ariyomo,Carter,etal.,2013;Patricketal.,482013;Reifetal.,2003;vanOersetal.,2005)andhavefitnessconsequences(e.g.Ariyomoet49al.,2012;Dingemanseetal.,2005;Smithetal.,2008),suggestingtheyarenotmerelynon‐50adaptivenoisethatsurroundsanadaptiveoptimum(Wilson,1998).Nevertheless,51underlyingmechanismsthatgenerateandmaintainbehaviouralpolymorphismarelargely52unclearandmanyaspectsofthegrowingbodyoftheoreticalframeworksstillremaintobe53empiricallytested(reviewedine.g.Schuettetal.,2010;Wolfetal.,2010).5455Recently,Schuettetal.(2010)pointedoutthatsexualselectionmaybeimportantin56generatingandmaintainingpersonalityvariationthoughthispossibilityhasrarelybeen57tested(butseee.g.Montiglioetal.,2016;Schuettetal.,2011).Accordingtotheproposed58framework(Schuettetal.,2010),personalitiesareexpectedtoplayanimportantrolein59matechoicewhenapotentialmate'sbehaviouralphenotypeiseitherassociatedwith60good/compatiblegenesthatincreaseoffspringfitness(Dingemanseetal.,2004;Ihleetal.,612015;Maysetal.,2004)orprovidesnon‐geneticbenefitsincreasingthereproductive62

  • 4

    successthroughparentalabilityand/orbehaviouralcompatibilitybetweenmates.While63matechoiceforgeneticqualityandparentalabilityshouldfavourinter‐individual64agreementinthepreferenceforabehaviouraltrait,matechoiceforgeneticorbehavioural65compatibilityshoulddependonaninteractionbetweenmaleandfemale(geno‐or)66phenotype(Schuettetal.,2010).Thus,matechoiceforcompatibilitywouldleadtointer‐67individualdifferencesinmatingpreferences,creatingeitheranassortativeordis‐68assortativematingpattern(Schuettetal.,2010).6970Notmanystudiestodatehaveinvestigatedtheeffectofpersonalitytraitsonmatechoice71(reviewedinSchuettetal.,2010)andsomehaveonlyassessedthebehaviourofthechosen72butnotthechoosingsex(Godinetal.,1996;Ophiretal.,2003).Thefewstudiesconsidering73apotentialinterplaybetweenmaleandfemalepersonalityduringmatechoicehaveoften74foundassortativematechoiceforvariousbehaviouraltraits,incorrelative(Gonzagaetal.,752010;Kralj‐Fišeretal.,2013;Mascie‐Tayloretal.,1988;Montiglioetal.,2016)or76experimentalsettings(Schuettetal.,2011)andanincreasedreproductivesuccessof77assortativepairs(e.g.Ariyomo&Watt,2013;Schuettetal.,2011).However,instudiesthat78foundincreasedsuccessofassortativepairs,personalitydataareoftenobtainedpost79pairing(Bothetal.,2005;Harrisetal.,2014;Laubuetal.,2016)notallowingtoteaseapart80whethermatechoicewasaffectedbyindividualpersonalitiesorwhetherbehavioural81similaritywasachievedpost‐pairinginhighlysuccessfulpairs(Laubuetal.,2016).Indirect82evidencethatdis‐assortmentforpersonalitycansometimesbebeneficialisprovidedby83vanOersetal.(2008),whofoundassortativepairsofgreattits,Parusmajor,toshowhigher84ratesofextra‐pairpaternity.Generally,positiveassortmentforgenotypicorphenotypic85

  • 5

    traitsisbyfarmoreprominentintheanimalkingdomthanevidencefordis‐assortment86(reviewedinJiangetal.,2013).8788Personalitytraitsconsistoftwomeasures:thebehaviourallevelandthedegreeof89behaviouralconsistency.Althoughthereisconsiderablevariationinwithin‐individual90behaviouralconsistency(Dingemanseetal.,2009)theeffectofsuchindividualdifferences91inconsistencyonmatechoicehasrarelybeenconsidered(butseeSchuettetal.,2011).92Behaviouralconsistencymightbesexuallyselectedforifitreflectsindividualquality(i.e.93consistencyiscostlyunderchangingconditions)orifchoosingapredictable(i.e.consistent)94mateprovidesreliableinformationaboutfutureparentalcarebehaviourpriortomating95(Dalletal.,2004;Royleetal.,2010;Schuettetal.,2010).Forexample,afemalemightbe96abletopredictamale'sabilitytoprotectprospectiveoffspringfromtheconsistencyin97boldnessexpressedpriortomatechoice.9899Inthepresentstudy,weinvestigatedtheinfluenceofmaleandfemaleboldness(propensity100toengageinriskybehaviour;Wilsonetal.,1994)onfemalematepreferenceinasocially101monogamous,biparentalcichlidfromWestAfrica,therainbowkrib,Pelvicachromispulcher.102Inthisspecies,pairsarehighlyterritorial:theydefendterritoriesandoffspringaggressively103againstcon‐andheterospecifics.Therefore,weassumedindividualboldnesstobeatrait104thatislikelyconsideredduringmatechoice.Furthermore,boldnesshasbeenshownto105affectforagingsuccess(Dyeretal.,2008),eggfertilizationrates(Ariyomoetal.,2012),106dominance(Dahlbometal.,2011),survivorship(Smithetal.,2010),andparentalcareeffort107(Budaevetal.,1999)inotherfishspecies.Wemeasuredmaleandfemaleboldness(activity108

  • 6

    undersimulatedpredationrisk)repeatedlytotestforpersonalitydifferences.Duringmate109choiceexperiments,femaleswerefirstallowedtoobserveabolderandashyermale110expressingtheirnaturalboldnesstowardsapredatoranimation.Subsequentfemalemating111preferenceforthetwomaleswasassessedinastandardmatechoicescenario.We112consideredbothaspectsofmaleandfemalepersonality:thebehaviouralleveland113behaviouralconsistencyofeachindividual. 114115Weexpectedfemalepreferencestodependonboth,thebehaviourallevelandbehavioural116consistency,withourpredictionsbeingguidedbySchuettetal.(2010).Forthebehavioural117level,weexpected,thatifmatechoiceisbasedonmale(parentalorgenetic)quality,118femalesshouldshowageneralpreferenceforeitherboldorshymales(e.g.Godinetal.,1191996;Kortetetal.,2012).Alternatively,ifmatecompatibilityismoreimportantduring120matechoice,femalesshouldnotshowanoverallagreementbutalsoconsidertheirown121personalityduringtheirchoice.Becausebothrainbowkribparentsprovideoffspringcare122weconsideredthesecondpossibility,i.e.matecompatibility,tobemoreimportantformate123choicebasedonboldness.Inspecieswithbiparentalcare,anassortativematingpreference124forcertainbehaviouraltraitscouldreducesexualconflictoverparentalinvestment(Royle125etal.,2010)andfacilitateoffspringcarecoordinationthroughabettersynchronisationof126parentalactivities(Schuettetal.,2011).Dependingontheenvironmentalconditionsorthe127biologyofthespecies,alsodis‐assortativematingmightsometimeshaveadvantages128(Schuettetal.,2010).Forinstance,speciesthatperformseveralparentalactivitiesmight129alsobenefitfromexpressingadis‐assortativematingpreference,facilitatingroleallocation130andspecialisationduringoffspringcare.Often,asexualdimorphisminrolespecialisation131

  • 7

    canbeobservedwiththefemaleprovidingmoredirectoffspringcareandthemale132defendingtheterritory(e.g.Guerraetal.,1995;Itzkowitz,1984;Neil,1984;Richteretal.,1332010;Solomon,1993).Nevertheless,inmanyspeciesbothpartnerscanordoperformthe134samebehaviours(seeRoyleetal.,2014forareviewontheflexibilityofparentalcare135behaviour),andatleastpartlycompensatefortheirmates’tasksifneeded(Itzkowitz,1984;136Laveryetal.,2010;Sasvari,1986;Storeyetal.,1994)indicatingthatsexrolesmightbeless137fixed.Forthebehaviouralconsistency,wefolloweduptwopossiblematechoicescenarios:138ageneralpreferenceforconsistentoverinconsistentmales,whichmightindicate139predictabilityoflaterparentalperformance,and/orindividualquality(Royleetal.,2010;140Schuettetal.,2010)ormatechoiceforcompatibilityleadingtoapositiveassortative141preference(Schuettetal.,2011;Schuettetal.,2010).142143144METHODS145146EthicalNote147Inconsiderationofanimalwelfare,wefollowedthe"3R"framework(Russelletal.,1959).148Todecreasethenumberofstudyanimalsneededweusedpredatoranimationsinsteadof149livepredatorsandtestmalesformatechoicetrialswereusedtwice.Duringexperiments,150noanimalswereharmedorexposedtoactualpredationrisk.Preyfishandpredatorswere151keptseparatelyanddidnothavevisualcontactduringfishmaintenance.Thestudywas152permittedbytheGerman"BehördefürGesundheitundVerbraucherschutzHamburg".153154

  • 8

    StudyAnimalsandHoldingConditions155StudyindividualswereobtainedfromacaptivebreedingstockattheUniversityof156Hamburgandlocalsuppliers.Malesandfemalesusedinthisstudywere1‐2yearsoldand157sexuallyinexperienced.Individualsweremaintainedinsame‐sexsiblinggroupsunder158standardisedholdingconditions(100x50x25cmand200x50x25cmtanks,26±1°C159watertemperature,aeratedandfilteredwater,weeklywaterchanges,12:12hours160light:dark)andwerefedonceadayon5daysaweekwithArtemiaspec.On161experimentationdays,fishwerefedafterobservations.Onedaybeforethefirstpersonality162test,individualsweremeasuredfortheirstandardlength(males:3.8‐6.2cm,females:3.5‐1635.1cm)usingImageJ(Schneideretal.,2012)andtransferredintoindividualtanks(25cmx16425cmx50cm)forthedurationofexperimentaltrials(5daysperindividual).Tankswere165endowedwithsand,halfaclaypotasshelterandaninternalfilter.Foridentification,all166individualsweremarkedwithVIEs(visibleimplantelastomers;VIE‐NorthwestMarine167Technology,ShawIsland,Washington,USA).Suchartificialcolourmarkshavenoinfluence168onmatechoiceinourpopulation(Schuettetal.,2017).169170ExperimentalOutline171Duringpersonalitytestingandmatechoicetrialsboldnesswasmeasuredasactivityunder172simulatedpredationriskusingcomputeranimationsofanaturallysympatricoccurring173predator,theAfricanobscuresnakehead,Parachannaobscura.Allmales(N=48)and174females(N=45)usedduringmatechoiceexperimentsweretestedfortheirboldnessthree175times(day0,day4,day33)inordertoassessthebehaviourallevelandconsistencyforall176individuals,andshort‐andlong‐termrepeatabilityinthepopulation.Thefirstandsecond177

  • 9

    testseriesofmaleboldnesstestswereintegratedintomatechoicetrials(N=45),allowing178femalestoobservetwomalesexpressingtheirnaturalboldness.Aftertheobservation,179femaleswereallowedtochoosebetweenthetwomalestheyhadjustobservedina180standardmatechoicetest(seeMateChoiceTrials).Fortheremainingboldnesstrials(third181seriesofmaleboldnesstestsandallfemaleboldnesstests)thetestprocedurewasidentical182tothoseintegratedintomatechoicetrialstoensureequaltestconditionsthroughout.183184BoldnessTest185Boldnesstestswereconductedinatesttank(waterlevel10cm,watertemperature26±1861°C;Figure1),whichwasdividedintothreecompartments:twoparalleltestcompartments187inwhichtwoindividualscouldbetestedfortheirboldnessatthesametimeandanadjacent188observercompartment.Aone‐waymirrorbetweentheobserverandthetestcompartments189allowedtheobservertoseethetestindividualsbutinhibitedtestindividualstoseethe190observer.Ontheothershortside,testcompartmentsfacedacomputermonitor(Dell,191UltraSharpU2412M61cm,24”)forthepresentationofpredatoranimations.Removable192opaquedividersbetweenthetestandtheobservercompartmentsaswellasbetweenthe193testcompartmentsandthemonitorallowedvisualseparationduringacclimationbefore194trials.195196Priortoaboldnesstest,weintroducedtwosame‐sexindividuals(fordetailsseealsoMate197ChoiceTrials)intoaclearcylinder(diameter=11cm)each,onepertestcompartment(test198compartmentswerepermanentlyvisuallyseparatedfromeachother).Anobserverofthe199oppositesexwasintroducedintotheobservercompartmentbeingallowedtofreelyswim200

  • 10

    around.Anobserverwasalwaysintroduced(eveninmaleandfemalepersonalityteststhat201werenotintegratedintomatechoicetrials)becauseitmaybepossiblethatchemicalcues202weretransmittedfromtheobservertothetestcompartmentsdespitephysicalseparation.203Aftera15minacclimation,theopaquedividerswereremovedallowingfreeviewofthe204animation(testindividualsandobserver)andtestindividuals(observer).Afteranother1205minthecylinderswereremovedandthetestperiodof11minstarted.Trialswerevideo‐206recordedfromabovewithnohumanbeingpresentduringtrialsandthetesttankwas207surroundedwithwhitePlexiglastoavoiddisturbances.Individualswerealwaysboldness‐208typedatthesametimeofday±30mintoaccountforpotentialeffectsoftimeofdayand209hungerlevelonindividualactivitypattern(Ariyomoetal.,2015;MacPhailetal.,2009).In210eachboldnesstest,individualswereexposedtoarandomlychosenanimationshowinga211predatorspecimentheyhadnotseenbefore.212213Predatoranimations(N=4,eachusinganotherspecimen)werepreparedusing214PowerPoint©followingFischeretal.(2014).Animationsdisplayedastillphotographofthe215predatorswimmingbackandforthinfrontofawhitebackground.Wehavevalidatedthis216method:P.pulcherdecreasedtheiractivityinresponsetopredatoranimationscomparedto217acontrolwhilenodifferenceinresponsetowardsalivepredatorandtheanimationwas218found(Schereretal.,2017).219220Boldnesswasmeasuredasindividualactivity(totaldistancemoved;cm)fromthevideo221recordingsusingthetrackingsoftwareEthovisionXT11(Noldus,Wageningen,The222Netherlands).Theactivitywasassessedforatestperiodof10min,beginning1minafter223

  • 11

    thestartofthevideo.Forallindividualsthebehaviourallevelwasdefinedasthemean224activityofthefirstandsecondtestseries.Behaviouralconsistencywascalculatedfollowing225Ioannouetal.(2016)astheabsolutevalueofthedifferenceinactivitybetweenthefirstand226secondboldnesstest.WefurtherdividedthemeasureofIoannouetal.(2016)bythetotal227variationinthepopulation(rangeofactivitywithinfirstandsecondboldnesstest).As228suggestedbyDingemanseetal.(2009),suchanindexwouldprovideameasurethatis229standardisedinrelationtothepopulation.Wecalculatedbehaviouralconsistencyformales230andfemalesseparately.Valuesforconsistencycanrangefrom0(highconsistency)to1231(lowconsistency).232233MateChoiceTrials234Matechoicetrialsconsistedoftwoparts:theabovedescribedobservationanda235subsequentchoice.Duringobservation,thefemalecouldobservetwomalesshowingtheir236naturalboldness(seeBoldnessTest).Subsequentmatechoicewasconductedimmediately237aftertheobservationinastandarddichotomouschoicetest,suitabletopredictmate238preferencefromtheamountoftimespentwithamaleincichlids(Dechaume‐Moncharmont239etal.,2011;Thünkenetal.,2007).Thechoicechamber(35x100x25cm,waterlevel=10240cm)wasseparatedintothreecompartmentswiththefemalecompartmentbeinginthe241middle(60x35x25cm)andamalecompartmentateachside(20x35x25cm).242243Tobeginthechoicetest,wetransferredthefemaleandthetwomalesshehadjustobserved244fromtheboldnesstesttanktothechoicechamber.Maleswererandomlyassignedtothe245twomalecompartments.Allindividualswereallowedtoacclimatefor10minwhilebeing246

  • 12

    visuallyseparatedfromeachother.Then,opaquedividerswereremovedandthefirsttest247periodof12minbegan.Thereafter,theprocedurewasrepeatedwiththemalesswitching248sidestotakeaccountforapotentialsidebias(again10minacclimationfollowing12min249testperiod).Toavoiddisturbancesthechoicechamberwassurroundedwithwhite250Plexiglasandnohumanwaspresentduringtrials.Trialswerevideo‐recordedfromabove.251252Eachfemalewasusedonceduringmatechoicetrials.Thetwomalesusedinamatechoice253trialwerematchedforsize(standardlengthdifference≤5%,i.e.≤3mm)andfamilybut254otherwiserandomlychosen.Thefemaleobserveroriginatedfromadifferentfamilythan255themales.256257Theassociationtimeforthetwomaleswasdeterminedfrombothtestperiods(i.e.20min)258usingEthovisionXT11.Testperiodswereanalysedfor10min,starting2minafterthestart259ofthevideo.Theassociationtimewasdefinedasthetimethefemalespentwithin5cm260distancetoeachmalecompartment(whichcorrespondstoca.onefishlength;hereafter261“preferencezone”).Femalestrengthofpreferencewasthenquantifiedastherelative262amountoftimeshespentinthepreferencezoneoftheboldmale(associationtimeforthe263boldmalewasdividedbytheassociationtimeforbothmales;e.g.Dugatkin,1996;264Makowiczetal.,2010).Foreachmatechoicetest,theboldmalewasdefinedasthemale265beingmoreactiveduringtheboldnesstestandtheshymalewasdefinedasbeingtheless266activemale(mean±SEforabsolutesimilaritybetweenshyandboldmales:behavioural267level=975.95±147.81;behaviouralconsistency=0.11±0.02;pleaseseeStatistical268Analysesforcalculationofsimilarityindices).Also,wecalculatedthesidebiasforall269

  • 13

    femalesandconsideredafemalebeingside‐biasedwhenshespentmorethan80%ofthe270totaltimespentinpreferencezones(bothtestperiods)injustonezone,regardlesswhich271malewasthere(Poschadeletal.,2009;Schlüteretal.,1998).272273StatisticalAnalyses274AlldataanalyseswereconductedinR3.2.3(RCoreTeam,2015).Totestforpersonality275differencesrepeatabilityofourmeasureforboldness(activityundersimulatedpredation276risk)wasassessedwithlinearmixedeffectmodels(LMMs)usingtherptR‐package277(Schielzethetal.,2013).Weassessedshort‐termrepeatability(boldnesstest:day0,day4)278aswellaslong‐termrepeatability(boldnesstest:day4,day33)forsexesseparatelywith2791000bootstrappingrunsand1000permutations.Significancewasinferredwhenthe95%280CIdidnotincludezero.Activitywassquareroot‐transformedfornormalityandmodels281werefitforGaussianerrorstructure.282283Totestforageneralpreferenceforboldorshymales,weranaLMMwithfemalestrengthof284preferenceforboldmalesastheresponseandmaleIDasrandomeffect.Wedidnotinclude285anyfixedeffects.Tocheckforadeviationfromrandomchoice(i.e.strengthofpreference=28650%)weobtainedthe95%CIoftheestimatedmean.Apreferenceforeitherboldorshy287maleswouldbeindicatediftheCIdoesnotinclude0.50.Similarly,wetestedforageneral288preferenceforbehaviouralconsistencybyrunninganullmodelwithfemalestrengthof289preferenceforthemaleshowingthehigherconsistencyduringtheobservationasthe290responseandmaleIDasrandomeffect.Apreferenceforeitherconsistencyorinconsistency291wouldberevealedifthe95%CIofthemeandoesnotinclude0.50.292

  • 14

    293Totestfor(dis)‐assortativefemalematechoicewefittedaLMMwithfemalestrengthof294preferenceforboldmalesastheresponsevariableandmaleIDasrandomterm.Asfixed295effectsweincludedrelativesimilarityforthebehaviourallevelandrelativesimilarityfor296thebehaviouralconsistencybetweenthefemaleandthemalesshesawduringthe297observationphaseandmatechoicetest.Tocalculaterelativesimilarity(forleveland298consistency,respectively),wefirstcomputeddifference‐scorebasedsimilaritybetweenthe299femaleandeachofthetwomales(boldandshy)astheabsolutevalueofthedifferencein300therespectivebehaviour(e.g.Gaunt,2006;Luoetal.,2005;Montiglioetal.,2016)between301thefemaleandtheboldmale,andthefemaleandtheshymale.Thus,similarity(inleveland302consistency,respectively)ishighestatzeroanddis‐similarityincreaseswithincreasing303values.RelativesimilaritywasthencalculatedfollowingGasparinietal.(2015):the304similaritybetweenthefemaleandtheboldmalewassubtractedfromthesimilarity305betweenthefemaleandtheshymale.Positivevaluesforrelativesimilarity(inleveland306consistency,respectively)indicatehighersimilaritybetweenthefemaleandtheboldmale307whilenegativevaluesindicatetheshymaleismoresimilartothefemalethanthebold308male.Priortotheanalysis,wez‐transformedbothrelativesimilarityforthebehavioural309levelandforthebehaviouralconsistencyforstandardisation.310311Weusedthelme4‐package(Batesetal.,2015)forLMMs.Weusedstepwisebackward312modelsimplificationtofittheminimumadequatemodel.PartialR2withCL(confidence313level)werecalculatedforexplanatoryvariablesusingtheapproachsuggestedbyNakagawa314etal.(2013),implementedinther2glmm‐package(Jaeger,2016).Fornon‐significant315

  • 15

    explanatoryvariableswereportedregressionestimatesandpartialR2ofthemodelbefore316thetermwasdropped.Modelassumptionswerevisuallyensuredthroughmodeldiagnosis317plots.Forallanalyses,femalestrengthofpreferencewasarcsine‐squareroot‐transformed318fornormality.Wehadaprioridecidedtoexcludeside‐biasedfemales(N=6)from319preferenceanalyses(Dosenetal.,2004;Hoysaketal.,2007;Knieletal.,2015;Schluppetal.,3201999;Schlüteretal.,1998;Williamsetal.,2010).Bydefinition,aside‐biasedfemaleshows321contradictorypreferencesduringthetwotestperiodsofachoicetest.Theremovalofsuch322inconsistentbehaviourthatappearsrandominregardtothepresentedmalesiscrucialas323toremovefemalesthatwouldnotexpressamatingpreferenceforthepresentedmalesbut324ratherapreferencefor(oragainst)aspecificsideofthechoicechamber(e.g.becauseofa325lackofmotivation).Leavingsuchbiasedpreferencedatainthedatasetwouldartificially326increasethesamplesizeanddistorttheactualpreferencepattern.Ontheotherhand,327removingside‐biasedfemalesfromthedatasetcanlowerthebehaviouralrange328representedinthisstudy.Astherearedifferentapproachesbutnocommonagreementin329howtohandlesidebiasesinmatechoicetrials,weperformedallpreferenceanalysestwice,330oncewithandoncewithoutremovingside‐biasedfemales(N=45).Thoughwehere331considerbothapproaches,weadvocatetheremovalofclearlybiasedpreferencedatafrom332analysesandwillthereforemainlyfocusonthepresentationofpreferenceanalyses333performedwithoutobvioussidebiasesinthedata.334335RESULTS336337

  • 16

    Malesandfemalesweresignificantlyrepeatableintheirboldnessovertheshort‐term338(LMMmales:R=0.507,SE=0.110,CI=[0.246,0.686],N=48;LMMfemales:R=0.604,SE339=0.097,CI=[0.380,0.763],N=45)andlong‐term(LMMmales:R=0.463,SE=0.113,CI=340[0.233,0.657],N=48;LMMfemales:R=0.557,SE=0.111,CI=[0.311,0.732],N=42).341342Wefoundnogeneralpreferenceforeitherboldorshymales(meanpreferenceforbold343males:46.5%;95%CI=[40.8,52.1%]).Also,wedidnotdetectageneralpreferencefor344maleconsistency(meanpreferenceforconsistentmales:53.5%,95%CI=[47.8,58.9%]).345346Femalestrengthofpreferencefortheboldmalesignificantlydecreasedwithincreasing347relativesimilarityinthebehaviourallevel(LMM:χ21=10.572,N=39,P=0.001,coefficient348±SE(standardised)=‐0.091±0.026;R2=0.242,CL=[0.056,0.475];Figure2a).Further,349femalestrengthofpreferenceincreasedwithincreasingrelativesimilarityinbehavioural350consistency(LMM:χ21=4.528,N=39,P=0.033,coefficient±SE(standardised)=0.058±3510.026;R2=0.114,CL=[0.003,0.341];Figure2b).352353Whenperformingpreferenceanalysiswithouttheremovalofside‐biasedfemales,we354receivedsimilarresultswithregardtofemalestrengthofpreferenceforboldmales(mean355preference:46.5%;95%CI=[41.5,51.6%])andforconsistentmales(meanpreference:35653.9%;95%CI=[49.1,59.1%])notshowingadeviationfromrandomchoice.However,357differenttotheanalysiswithremovedsidebiases,relativesimilarityinthebehavioural358leveltendedtonegativelyinfluencefemalepreferenceforboldmales(LMM:χ21=2.885,N=35945,P=0.089,coefficient±SE(standardised)=‐0.043±0.034;R2=0.066,CL=[0.001,360

  • 17

    0.258])andrelativesimilarityinbehaviouralconsistencydidnotaffectfemalepreference361(LMM:χ21=2.279,N=45,P=0.131,coefficient±SE(standardised)=0.040±0.025;R2=3620.052,CL=[0.000,0.235]).363364365DISCUSSION366367BothsexesofP.pulchershowedconsistentshort‐andlong‐termpersonalitydifferencesfor368boldness.Wedidnotdetectanoverallagreementinfemalematingpreferenceforeither369malelevelorconsistencyofboldness.However,wefounddis‐assortativefemalechoicefor370thelevelofboldness.Also,femalepreferenceincreasedwithsimilarityinbehavioural371consistency,suggestingassortativechoiceforconsistencyinboldness(whenside‐biased372femaleswereremoved).373374Thedis‐assortativepreferenceforthebehaviouralleveliscontradictorytotheresultsof375mostothermatechoicestudiestestingforbehavioural(dis‐)assortmentthatmainly376reportedassortativematingpreferences(e.g.Montiglioetal.,2016;Schuettetal.,2011).At377thispoint,wecanonlyspeculateaboutpossibleadaptivebenefitsofadis‐assortative378preference.Behaviouraldis‐similaritycouldpossiblyincreasewithin‐pairbehavioural379and/orgeneticcompatibility(Schuettetal.,2010).Behaviouralcompatibilityhasprimarily380beendiscussedforbiparentalspecieswhenbothparentsperformmoreorlessthesame381parentalactivity,forinstanceoffspringprovisioninginsomebirds(Royleetal.,2010).In382zebrafinches,Taeniopygiaguttata,forinstance,similarityinthebehaviourallevelhasbeen383

  • 18

    showntoincreasepaircompatibility(e.g.Schuettetal.,2011).However,whenspecies384performvariousparentalactivitiestheymightsometimesbenefitfromexpressingadis‐385assortativematingpreference,facilitatingroleallocationduringoffspringcare.InP.pulcher,386parentstypicallydividethelabourwithoneindividualstayingmorewiththeoffspringand387theotheronedefendingtheterritory.Thoughsexualdimorphisminrolespecialisationhas388beendescribedformanycichlids(McKayeetal.,2008;Neil,1984;Richteretal.,2010),sex389rolesmightnotbeentirelystrictinthespeciesandmayratherdependontheinterplay390betweenmaleandfemalepersonality.Itzkowitzetal.(2005)haveshownthatmaleand391femaleparentconvictcichlids,Archocentrusnigrofasciatum,changedtheirdefense392behaviourinresponsetothemate'sbodysize,regardlessofthesex.Thisresultindicates393thatparentalroleallocationmayinsomespeciesratherdependonthemate'sbehaviour394andphysiologythanonthesexitself.Behaviouraldis‐similarityinboldnessmayfacilitate395labourdivisionwiththebolderindividualdefendingtheterritoryandtheshyerindividual396stayingwiththeyoung,regardlessofthesex.Hence,dis‐assortativematingforpersonality397couldsometimesleadtoinvertedparentalcarerolesthoughthishasnotbeeninvestigated398yet.Also,anincreasedgeneticcompatibilitythroughdis‐similaritycouldbepossibleifdis‐399assortativematingleadstoheterozygoteoffspringthataremoreviable(Charlesworthetal.,4001987;Dingemanseetal.,2004).Forexample,Marshalletal.(2003)showedastrong401correlationbetweenindividualgeneticdiversityandabehaviouraltrait,songcomplexity,in402sedgewarblers,Acrocephalusschoenobaenus.Femaleschosetomatewithmalesthat403increasedoffspringgeneticdiversity(Marshalletal.,2003).Seddonetal.(2004)foundmale404heterozygositytobecorrelatedwithterritorysizeandsongstructureinmale(butnot405female)subdesertmesite,Moniasbenschi.406

  • 19

    407Further,wefoundassortativematechoicefortheconsistencyofboldness.Thefewstudies408thathaveassessedthelinkbetweenbehaviouralconsistencyandsexualselectionfounda409positiverelationshipbetweenconsistencyandreproductivesuccess(Boteroetal.,2009;410Byers,2006)andahigherreproductivesuccessofpairsmatchedforbehavioural411consistency(Schuettetal.,2011).Schuettetal.(2011)haveshownthatpairsmatchedfor412consistencyraisedfosterfledglingsofbetterbodycondition,indicatingthepossible413mechanismdrivingassortmentforbehaviouralconsistencymightbeahigherefficiencyin414theprovisionofparentalcare.415416Clearly,ourstudyislimitedbythecorrelativedesign,notallowingtospecificallyaddress417thecausalityunderlyingthepreferencepattern.Furtherexaminationsusingbehavioural418manipulationsarenowneededtodecoupleboldnessfrompotentiallycorrelatedtraitsthat419mightinfluencematechoice,toensurethepreferencepatternwefoundisunequivocally420relatedtoindividualbehaviour.Moreover,itshouldbementionedthatourmeasurefor421behaviouralconsistencyderivedfromonlytwomeasurements.Weareherefacingacritical422trade‐off.Whilemultiplemeasurementscanleadtoachangeinbehaviourcausedbythe423numberoftimestested,e.g.throughhabituationorsensitization(Belletal.,2009;Stampset424al.,2012),themeasurementerrorishigherwhenonlytestedtwice.Inthisparticularstudy,425wetestedindividualresponsestowardsunfamiliarpredatoranimations,presentedina426novelsituation.Ourmeasurementforboldnesswouldlikelybeaffectedbypriorexperience427andfamiliaritywithtestconditions,makingitdifficulttoreceivethesamenatureof428measureforboldnesswhentestedmultipletimes.However,thestrengthofourstudyis429

  • 20

    thatfemalescouldobservemaleboldnessdirectlybeforematechoicetrialswhiletheywere430hiddenbehindone‐wayglassandpartitions.Thisway,malescouldexpresstheirnatural431behaviourwithoutbeingaffectedbythefemale'spresence.Adecouplingofobservationand432choiceensuredfemalepreferencenotbeingconfoundedbythepresenceofapredator.433434Conclusions435Insummary,weprovidesuggestiveevidencethatsexualselectionmayrepresentakeyrole436intheevolutionofpersonalitydifferences.Femalesshowedadis‐assortativemating437preferenceforthelevelofboldnessandanassortativepreferenceforthedegreeof438behaviouralconsistency.Ourresultsindicatematechoiceforbehaviouraland/orgenetic439compatibilitythoughonlyassessedinacorrelativeapproach.Suchamatingpreference440mightimproveparentalcareefficiencythroughfacilitationofparentalroleallocation441and/ortoincreaseoffspringfitnessthroughgeneticbenefits.Noticeable,thehandlingof442sidebiasessignificantlyaffectedourresults.Whilewefoundaneffectofbehavioural443similarityinlevelandconsistencywhenremovingsidebiases,wecouldnotdetectsuch444effectswithoutremovingside‐biasedfemalesfromthedata.Thisdiscrepancyinresults445underlinestheimportanceoftakingtheapproachusedintoconsiderationwhencomparing446theresultsofdifferentmatechoicestudies.Thehandlingofsidebiasesinmatechoice447studiesisnottrivialandcanlargelyaffectexperimentaloutcomes.448449450ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS451452

  • 21

    ThisresearchwasfoundedbyDeutscheForschungsgemeinschaft(SCHU‐2927/2‐1,grantto453W.S.).WethankF.X.Dechaume‐Moncharmontandtwoanonymousreviewersfortheir454constructivecomments.455456457REFERENCES458459Ariyomo,T.O.,Carter,M.,&Watt,P.J.(2013).Heritabilityofboldnessandaggressivenessin460

    thezebrafish.BehaviorGenetics,43,161‐167.461 462Ariyomo,T.O.,&Watt,P.J.(2012).Theeffectofvariationinboldnessandaggressivenesson463

    the reproductive success of zebrafish. Animal Behaviour, 83(1), 41‐46.464doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.10.004465

    466Ariyomo, T. O., & Watt, P. J. (2013). Disassortative mating for boldness decreases467

    reproductive success in the guppy. Behavioral Ecology, 24(6), 1320‐1326.468doi:10.1093/beheco/art070469

    470Ariyomo,T.O.,&Watt,P.J.(2015).Effectofhungerlevelandtimeofdayonboldnessand471

    aggression in the zebrafishDanio rerio. Journal of FishBiology, 86(6), 1852‐1859.472doi:10.1111/jfb.12674473

    474

  • 22

    Bates,D.,Mächler,M.,Bolker,B.,&Walker,S. (2015).FittingLinearMixed‐EffectsModels475Usinglme4.JournalofStatisticalSoftware,67(1),1‐48.doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01476

    477Bell,A.M.,Hankison,S. J.,&Laskowski,L.(2009).Therepeatabilityofbehaviour:ameta‐478

    analysis.AnimalBehaviour,77,771‐783.479 480Boissy,A. (1995).Fearand fearfulness inanimals.TheQuarterlyReviewofBiology,70(2),481

    165‐191.482 483Botero, C. A., Rossman, R. J., Caro, L. M., Stenzler, L. M., Lovette, I. J., de Kort, S. R., &484

    Vehrencamp,S.L.(2009).Syllabletypeconsistencyisrelatedtoage,socialstatusand485reproductive success in the tropical mockingbird. Animal Behaviour, 77, 701‐706.486doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.20487

    488Both, C., Dingemanse,N. J., Drent, P. J., & Tinbergen, J.M. (2005). Pairs of extreme avian489

    personalities have highest reproductive success. Journal of Animal Ecology, 74(4),490667‐674.doi:10.1111/j.1365‐2656.2005.00962.x491

    492Budaev,S.V.,Zworykin,D.D.,&Mochek,A.D.(1999).Individualdifferencesinparentalcare493

    andbehaviourprofileintheconvictcichlid:acorrelationstudy.AnimalBehaviour,58,494195‐202.495

    496

  • 23

    Byers, B. E. (2006). Extrapair paternity in chestnut‐sided warblers is correlated with497consistent vocal performance. Behavioral Ecology, 18(1), 130‐136.498doi:10.1093/beheco/arl058499

    500Charlesworth, D., & Charlesworth, B. (1987). Inbreeding depression and its evolutionary501

    consequences.AnnualReviewofEcologyandSystematics,18,237‐268.502 503Dahlbom, S. J., Lagman, D., Lundstedt‐Enkel, K., Sundstrom, L. F., & Winberg, S. (2011).504

    Boldness predicts social status in zebrafish (Danio rerio).PLoSOne,6(8), e23565.505doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023565506

    507Dall,S.R.X.,Houston,A.I.,&McNamara,J.M.(2004).Thebehaviouralecologyofpersonality:508

    consistentindividualdifferencesfromanadaptiveperspective.EcologyLetters,7(8),509734‐739.doi:10.1111/j.1461‐0248.2004.00618.x510

    511Dechaume‐Moncharmont,F.X.,Cornuau, J.H.,Keddar, I., Ihle,M.,Motreuil,S.,&Cezilly,F.512

    (2011). Rapid assessment of female preference formale size predicts subsequent513choiceof spawningpartner ina sociallymonogamouscichlid fish.ComptesRendus514Biologies,334(12),906‐910.doi:10.1016/j.crvi.2011.08.004515

    516Dingemanse,N. J.,Both,C.,Drent,P. J.,&Tinbergen, J.M. (2004).Fitnessconsequencesof517

    avianpersonalitiesinafluctuatingenvironment.ProceedingsofTheRoyalSocietyB,518271(1541),847‐852.doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2680519

  • 24

    520Dingemanse,N. J.,Kazem,A. J.,Réale,D.,&Wright, J. (2009).Behaviouralreactionnorms:521

    animalpersonalitymeetsindividualplasticity.TrendsinEcologyandEvolution,25(2),52281‐89.doi:10.1016/j.tree.2009.07.013523

    524Dingemanse,N.J.,&Réale,D.(2005).Naturalselectionandanimalpersonality.Behaviour,525

    142,1165‐1190.526 527Dosen, L. D., &Montomerie, R. (2004). Female size influencesmate preferences ofmale528

    guppies.Ethology,110,245‐255.529 530Dugatkin, L. A. (1996). Interface between culturally based preferences and genetic531

    preferences: Femalemate choice inPoecilia reticulata.Proceedingsof theNational532AcademyofSciencesUSA,93,2770‐2773.533

    534Dyer, J.R.G.,Croft,D.P.,Morrell,L. J.,&Krause, J. (2008). Shoal compositiondetermines535

    foraging success in the guppy. Behavioral Ecology, 20(1), 165‐171.536doi:10.1093/beheco/arn129537

    538Fischer,S.,Hess,S.,Oberhummer,E.,Burlaud,R.,Fernandez,A.A.,Frommen,J.G.,&Taborsky,539

    B.(2014).Animatedimagesasatooltostudyvisualcommunication:acasestudyina540cooperatively breeding cichlid. Behaviour, 151(12‐13), 1921‐1942.541doi:10.1163/1568539x‐00003223542

  • 25

    543Gasparini,C.,Congiu,L.,&Pilastro,A. (2015).Majorhistocompatibilitycomplexsimilarity544

    andsexual selection:differentdoesnotalwaysmeanattractive.MolecularEcology,54524(16),4286‐4295.doi:10.1111/mec.13222546

    547Gaunt, R. (2006). Couple similarity andmarital satisfaction: are similar spouses happier?548

    JournalofPersonality,74(5),1401‐1420.doi:10.1111/j.1467‐6494.2006.00414.x549 550Godin,J.‐G.J.,&Dugatkin,L.A.(1996).Femalematingpreferenceforboldmalesintheguppy,551

    Poeciliareticulata.ProceedingsoftheNationalAcademyofSciencesUSA,93,10262‐55210267.553

    554Gonzaga,G.C.,Carter,S.,&Buckwalter, J.G. (2010).Assortativemating,convergence,and555

    satisfaction in married couples. Personal Relationships, 17(4), 634‐644.556doi:10.1111/j.1475‐6811.2010.01309.x557

    558Gosling,S.D.(2001).Frommicetomen:Whatcanwelearnaboutpersonalityfromanimal559

    research?PsychologicalBulletin,127,45‐86.560 561Guerra,M.,&Drummond,H. (1995).Reversedsexualsizedimorphismandparental care:562

    minimaldivisionoflabourintheblue‐footedbooby.Behaviour,132,479‐496.563 564

  • 26

    Harris,M.R.,&Siefferman,L.(2014).Interspecificcompetitioninfluencesfitnessbenefitsof565assortativematingforterritorialaggressionineasternbluebirds(Sialiasialis).PLoS566One,9(2),e88668.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0088668567

    568Hoysak,D.J.,&Godin,J.‐G.J.(2007).Repeatabilityofmalematechoiceinthemosquitofish,569

    Gambusia holbrooki. Ethology, 113(10), 1007‐1018. doi:10.1111/j.1439‐5700310.2007.01413.x571

    572Ihle, M., Kempenaers, B., & Forstmeier, W. (2015). Fitness benefits of mate choice for573

    compatibility in a socially monogamous species. PLoS biology, 13(9), e1002248.574doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248575

    576Ioannou,C.C.,&Dall,S.R.(2016).Individualsthatareconsistentinrisk‐takingbenefitduring577

    collectiveforaging.ScientificReports,6,33991.doi:10.1038/srep33991578 579Itzkowitz,M.(1984).Parentaldivisionof laborinamonogomousfish.Behaviour,89,251‐580

    260.581 582Itzkowitz, M., Santangelo, N., Cleveland, A., Bockelman, A., & Richter, M. (2005). Is the583

    selectionofsex‐typicalparentalrolesbasedonanassessmentprocess?Atestinthe584monogamous convict cichlid fish. Animal Behaviour, 69(1), 95‐105.585doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.12.027586

    587

  • 27

    Jaeger,B. (2016). r2glmm:ComputesRsquared formixed(multilevel)models.Rpackage588version0.1.1.Retrievedfromhttps://CRAN.R‐project.org/package=r2glmm589

    590Jiang,Y.,Bolnick,D.I.,&Kirkpatrick,M.(2013).Assortativematinginanimals.TheAmerican591

    Naturalist,181(6),125‐138.doi:10.1086/670160592 593Kniel,N.,Durler,C.,Hecht,I.,Heinbach,V.,Zimmermann,L.,&Witte,K.(2015).Novelmate594

    preference throughmate‐choice copying in zebra finches: sexes differ. Behavioral595Ecology,26(2),647‐655.doi:10.1093/beheco/aru241596

    597Kortet,R.,Niemelä,P. T., Vainikka,A.,&Laakso, J. (2012). Femalespreferboldmales; an598

    analysisofboldness,matechoice,andbacterialresistanceinthefieldcricketGryllus599integer.EcologicalParasitologyandImmunology,1,1‐6.doi:10.4303/epi/235580600

    601Kralj‐Fišer, S., Sanguino Mostajo, G. A., Preik, O., Pekár, S., & Schneider, J. M. (2013).602

    Assortativematingbyaggressivenesstypeinorbweavingspiders.BehavioralEcology,60324(4),824‐831.doi:10.1093/beheco/art030604

    605Kralj‐Fišer, S., & Schuett,W. (2014). Studyingpersonality variation in invertebrates:why606

    bother?AnimalBehaviour,91,41‐52.doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.02.016607 608

  • 28

    Laubu,C.,Dechaume‐Moncharmont,F.X.,Motreuil,S.,&Schweitzer,C.(2016).Mismatched609partners that achievepostpairingbehavioral similarity improve their reproductive610success.ScienceAdvances,2(3),e1501013.doi:10.1126/sciadv.1501013611

    612Lavery,R.J.,&Reebs,S.G.(2010).Effectofmateremovaloncurrentandsubsequentparental613

    care in the convict cichlid (Pisces: Cichlidae). Ethology, 97(4), 265‐277.614doi:10.1111/j.1439‐0310.1994.tb01046.x615

    616Luo, S., & Klohnen, E. C. (2005). Assortativemating andmarital quality in new‐lywed: A617

    couple‐centeredapproach.JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology,88,304–326.618 619MacPhail, R. C., Brooks, J., Hunter, D. L., Padnos, B., Irons, T. D., & Padilla, S. (2009).620

    Locomotion in larval zebrafish: Influence of time of day, lighting and ethanol.621Neurotoxicology,30(1),52‐58.doi:10.1016/j.neuro.2008.09.011622

    623Makowicz,A.,Plath,M.,&Schlupp,I.(2010).Maleguppies(Poeciliareticulata)adjusttheir624

    mate choice behaviour to the presence of an audience.Behaviour, 147(13), 1657‐6251674.doi:10.1163/000579510x528206626

    627Marshall,R.C.,Buchanan,K.L.,&Catchpole,C.K. (2003). Sexual selectionand individual628

    genetic diversity in a songbird. Proceedings of TheRoyal Society B, 270, 248‐250.629doi:10.1098/rsbl.2003.0081630

    631

  • 29

    Mascie‐Taylor,C.G.N.,&Vandenberg,S.G.(1988).AssortativematingforIQandpersonality632duetopropinquityandpersonalpreference.BehaviorGenetics,18,339‐345.633

    634Mays,H.L.,Jr.,&Hill,G.E.(2004).Choosingmates:goodgenesversusgenesthatareagood635

    fit.TrendsinEcologyandEvolution,19(10),554‐559.doi:10.1016/j.tree.2004.07.018636 637McKaye,K.R.,&Murry,B.A.(2008).SexroledifferentiationinbrooddefensebyNicaraguan638

    cichlidfish,Amphilophusxiloanensis.CaribbeanJournalofScience,44,13‐20.639 640Montiglio,P.O.,Wey,T.W.,Chang,A.T.,Fogarty,S.,&Sih,A.(2016).Multiplematingreveals641

    complexpatternsofassortativematingbypersonalityandbodysize.JournalofAnimal642Ecology,85(1),125‐135.doi:10.1111/1365‐2656.12436643

    644Nakagawa,S.,&Schielzeth,H.(2013).AgeneralandsimplemethodforobtainingR2from645

    generalizedlinearmixed‐effectsmodels.MethodsinEcologyandEvolution,4(2),133‐646142.doi:10.1111/j.2041‐210x.2012.00261.x647

    648Neil,S.J.(1984).FieldstudiesofthebehavioralecologyandagonisticbehaviorofCichlasoma649

    meeki(Pisces:Cichlidae).EnvironmentalBiologyofFishes,10,59‐68.650 651Ophir,A.G.,&Galef,B.G.(2003).FemaleJapanesequailthat‘eavesdrop’onfightingmales652

    prefer losers to winners. Animal Behaviour, 66(2), 399‐407.653doi:10.1006/anbe.2003.2230654

  • 30

    655Patrick, S. C., Charmantier, A., & Weimerskirch, H. (2013). Differences in boldness are656

    repeatableandheritableinalong‐livedmarinepredator.EcologyandEvolution,3(13),6574291‐4299.doi:10.1002/ece3.748658

    659Poschadel,J.R.,Plath,M.,&Schlupp,I.(2009).Divergentfemalematingpreferenceinaclonal660

    fish.actaethologica,12(1),55‐60.doi:10.1007/s10211‐009‐0055‐8661 662R Core Team. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna,663

    Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.R‐664project.org/665

    666Reif,A.,&Lesch,K.‐P.(2003).Towardamoleculararchitectureofpersonality.Behavioural667

    BrainResearch,139,1‐20.doi:10.1016/S0166‐4328(02)00267‐X668 669Richter,M.,Santangelo,N.,&Itzkowitz,M.(2010).Biparentaldivisionofrolesintheconvict670

    cichlidfish:influenceofintrudernumbersandlocations.EthologyEcology&Evolution,67117,1‐15.doi:10.1080/08927014.2005.9522611672

    673Royle,N.J.,Russell,A.F.,&Wilson,A.J.(2014).Theevolutionofflexibleparenting.Science,674

    346(6198),776‐781.675 676

  • 31

    Royle,N.J.,Schuett,W.,&Dall,S.R.X.(2010).Behavioralconsistencyandtheresolutionof677sexual conflict over parental investment. Behavioral Ecology, 21(6), 1125‐1130.678doi:10.1093/beheco/arq156679

    680Russell,W.M. S., &Burch, R. L. (1959).Theprinciplesofhumane experimental technique.681

    LondonW.C.I.:MethuenandCo.,Ltd.682 683Sasvari,L.(1986).Reproductiveeffortofwidowedbirds.JournalofAnimalEcology,55,553‐684

    564.685 686Scherer,U.,Godin, J.G. J.,&Schuett,W. (2017).Validationof2D‐animatedpictures as an687

    investigativetoolinthebehaviouralsciences–acasestudywithaWestAfricancichlid688fish,Pelvicachromispulcher.submittedmanuscript.689

    690Schielzeth,H.,&Nakagawa, S. (2013). rptR: Repeatability forGaussian andnon‐Gaussian691

    data.https://R‐Forge.R‐project.org/projects/rptr/. 692693Schlupp, I., Waschulewski, M., & Ryan, M. J. (1999). Female preferences for naturally‐694

    occurringnovelmaletraits.Behaviour,136,519‐527.695 696Schlüter,A.,Parzefall,J.,&Schlupp,I.(1998).Femalepreferenceforsymmetricalverticalbars697

    inmalesailfinmollies.AnimalBehaviour,56,147‐153.698 699

  • 32

    Schneider,C.A.,Rasband,W.S.,&Eliceiri,K.W.(2012).NIHImagetoImageJ:25yearsof700imageanalysis.Naturemethods,9(7),671‐675.doi:PMID22930834701

    702Schuett, W., Dall, S. R. X., & Royle, N. J. (2011). Pairs of zebra finches with similar703

    ‘personalities’ make better parents. Animal Behaviour, 81(3), 609‐618.704doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.12.006705

    706Schuett,W.,Godin,J.G.J.,&Dall,S.R.X.(2011).Dofemalezebrafinches,Taeniopygiaguttata,707

    choose their mates based on their ‘personality’? Ethology, 117(10), 908‐917.708doi:10.1111/j.1439‐0310.2011.01945.x709

    710Schuett,W,Nava,TF,Rahmlow,T&UScherer(2017).ArtificialVisibleImplantElastomer711

    (VIE) tags of different colour and symmetry do not influence mate choice in a712cichlid.Behaviour,inpress.713

    714Schuett, W., Tregenza, T., & Dall, S. R. (2010). Sexual selection and animal personality.715

    BiologicalReviews,85(2),217‐246.doi:10.1111/j.1469‐185X.2009.00101.x716717Seddon, N., Amos,W.,Mulder, R. A., & Tobias, J. A. (2004). Male heterozygosity predicts718

    territorysize, songstructureandreproductivesuccess inacooperativelybreeding719bird. Proceedings of The Royal Society B, 271(1550), 1823‐1829.720doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2805721

    722

  • 33

    Sih,A.,Bell,A.,&Johnson,J.C.(2004).Behavioralsyndromes:anecologicalandevolutionary723overview. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 19(7), 372‐378.724doi:10.1016/j.tree.2004.04.009725

    726Sih,A.,Bell,A.M.,Johnson,J.C.,&Ziemba,R.E.(2004).Behavioralsyndromes:anintegrative727

    overview.TheQuarterlyReviewofBiology,79(3),241‐277.728 729Smith,B.R.,&Blumstein,D.T.(2008).Fitnessconsequencesofpersonality:ameta‐analysis.730

    BehavioralEcology,19(2),448‐455.doi:10.1093/beheco/arm144731 732Smith, B. R., & Blumstein, D. T. (2010). Behavioral types as predictors of survival in733

    Trinidadian guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Behavioral Ecology, 21(5), 919‐926.734doi:10.1093/beheco/arq084735

    736Solomon,N.G. (1993).Comparisonofparentalbehavior inmaleand femaleprairievoles737

    (Microtus ochrogaster). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 71(2), 434‐437.738doi:10.1139/z93‐061739

    740Stamps,J.A.,Briffa,M.,&Biro,P.A.(2012).Unpredictableanimals:individualdifferencesin741

    intraindividual variability (IIV). Animal Behaviour, 83(6), 1325‐1334.742doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.02.017743

    744

  • 34

    Storey,A.E.,Bradbury,C.G.,&Joyce,T.L.(1994).Nestattendanceinmalemeadowvoles:the745roleofthefemaleinregulatingmaleinteractionswithpups.1994,47,1037‐1046.746

    747Thünken,T.,Bakker,T.C.M.,Baldauf,S.A.,&Kullmann,H.(2007).Active inbreeding ina748

    cichlidfishanditsadaptivesignificance.CurrentBiology,17,225‐229.749 750van Oers, K., de Jong, G., van Noordwijk, A. J., Kempenaers, B., & Drent, P. J. (2005).751

    Contributionofgeneticstothestudyofanimalpersonalities:areviewofcasestudies.752Behaviour,142,1185‐1206.753

    754vanOers,K.,Drent,P.J.,Dingemanse,N.J.,&Kempenaers,B.(2008).Personalityisassociated755

    withextrapairpaternityingreattits,Parusmajor.AnimalBehaviour,76(3),555‐563.756doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.03.011757

    758Williams,T.H.,&Mendelson,T.C.(2010).Behavioralisolationbasedonvisualsignalsina759

    sympatricpairofdarterspecies.Ethology,116(11),1038‐1049.doi:10.1111/j.1439‐7600310.2010.01816.x761

    762Wilson,D.S.(1998).Adaptiveindividualdifferencewithinsinglepopulations.Philosophical763

    TransactionsoftheRoyalSocietyB,353,199‐205.764 765Wilson, D. S., Clark, A. B., Coleman, K., & Dearstyne, T. (1994). Shyness and boldness in766

    humansandotheranimals.TrendsinEcology&Evolution,9,442‐446.767

  • 35

    768Wolf, M., & Weissing, F. J. (2010). An explanatory framework for adaptive personality769

    differences.PhilosophicalTransactionsoftheRoyalSocietyB,365(1560),3959‐3968.770doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0215771

    772 773

  • 36

    FIGURES774

    Figure1:Experimentalset‐upfortheboldnesstest.Twosame‐sexfocalindividuals(visuallyseparated)wereexposedtoavideoanimationofapredator.Testindividualswereobservedbyafishoftheothersexbutcouldthemselvesnotseetheobserver:theobservercompartmentwasendowedwithaone‐waymirroralignedwithanangleof45°towardsthetestcompartmentsprovidingavisualcoverfortheobserver.Fishnottoscale.775

  • 37

    Figure2:Femalestrengthofpreferencefortheboldmaleindependenceofrelativesimilarityin(a)theleveland(b)theconsistencyofboldness.Positivesimilarityvaluesindicatetheboldmalewasmoresimilartothefemalethantheshymale,negativevaluesindicatehighersimilaritybetweenthefemaleandtheshymale.Datavisualisationonoriginaldata,strengthofpreferencewasarcsine‐squareroot‐transformedforanalyses.776