13
ABSTRACT Driver distraction is a topic of considerable interest, with the public debate centering on the use of cell phones and texting while driving. However, the driver distraction/overload issue is really much larger. It concerns specific tasks such as entering destinations on navigation systems, retrieving songs on MP3 players, accessing web pages, checking stocks, editing spreadsheets, and performing other tasks on smart phones, as well as, more generally, using in-vehicle information systems. Five major problems related to distraction/overload research and engineering and their solutions are addressed in this paper. Problems include (1) the misuse of the term distraction (and possible misdirection of effort), (2) driving performance measures and statistics that are either undefined or poorly defined (to be resolved by an SAE practice), (3) the workload of the driving task is not quantified (for which an equation is proposed), (4) the demand characteristics of in-vehicle tasks are not quantified (for which a scheme is proposed), and (5) too often, standards specify only measurement methods, not compliance criteria (which must be developed). INTRODUCTION Driver distraction/overload/workload is now the topic of considerable scientific and public debate. For the public, this debate has led to the passage of laws in cities and states restricting the use of cell phones and other devices while driving. Most recently, the province of Ontario, in Bill 118, took this step, becoming effective at the end of 2009 ( Legislative Assembly of Ontario (2008). However, this topic has been one of scientific inquiry for some time (e.g., Brown, Tickner, and Simmonds, 1969). Cell phones are but one example of nomadic devices that provide a wide range of features that are carried into motor vehicles. They supplement information and driver assistance systems now becoming commonplace. Each of these systems and their features is being provided for a valid reason, and collectively, they increase complexity of vehicle operation. With many more tasks for the driver to do, these systems could also collectively diminish usability and safety. This is particularly a concern for the carried in equipment, especially phones, which are rarely tested for use while driving, even though use while driving is expected. Some perspective of how the current situation has arisen is useful. Consider the top- five selling vehicles in the U.S. in January 2010 per the Wall Street Journal ( http:// online.wsj.com/mdc/public/page/2_3022- autosales.html#autosalesC, retrieved February 25, 2010) ( Table 1). Table 1. Top Five Vehicles Sold in the U.S., January 2010 These vehicles are all reasonably well equipped. Among them, the 2010 Nissan Altima 2.5 with the SL package, a typical contemporary sedan, was selected as an example for which technical details are readily available. How does the current vehicle differ from the 2000 model? The 2.5S with the SL package and the technology option includes a navigation system with a 6.5-inch, touch-screen unit, traction control, and vehicle stability control. The navigation system Driver Distraction/Overload Research and Engineering: Problems and Solutions 2010-01-2331 Published 10/19/2010 Paul A. Green University of Michigan Copyright © 2010 SAE International

Driver Distraction/Overload Research and …driving/publications/GreenConvergence2010.pdfproblem for driving research and engineering, especially for work on distraction/overload

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    4

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Driver Distraction/Overload Research and …driving/publications/GreenConvergence2010.pdfproblem for driving research and engineering, especially for work on distraction/overload

ABSTRACTDriver distraction is a topic of considerable interest, with thepublic debate centering on the use of cell phones and textingwhile driving. However, the driver distraction/overload issueis really much larger. It concerns specific tasks such asentering destinations on navigation systems, retrieving songson MP3 players, accessing web pages, checking stocks,editing spreadsheets, and performing other tasks on smartphones, as well as, more generally, using in-vehicleinformation systems.

Five major problems related to distraction/overload researchand engineering and their solutions are addressed in thispaper. Problems include (1) the misuse of the term distraction(and possible misdirection of effort), (2) driving performancemeasures and statistics that are either undefined or poorlydefined (to be resolved by an SAE practice), (3) the workloadof the driving task is not quantified (for which an equation isproposed), (4) the demand characteristics of in-vehicle tasksare not quantified (for which a scheme is proposed), and (5)too often, standards specify only measurement methods, notcompliance criteria (which must be developed).

INTRODUCTIONDriver distraction/overload/workload is now the topic ofconsiderable scientific and public debate. For the public, thisdebate has led to the passage of laws in cities and statesrestricting the use of cell phones and other devices whiledriving. Most recently, the province of Ontario, in Bill 118,took this step, becoming effective at the end of 2009(Legislative Assembly of Ontario (2008). However, this topichas been one of scientific inquiry for some time (e.g., Brown,Tickner, and Simmonds, 1969).

Cell phones are but one example of nomadic devices thatprovide a wide range of features that are carried into motorvehicles. They supplement information and driver assistancesystems now becoming commonplace. Each of these systemsand their features is being provided for a valid reason, andcollectively, they increase complexity of vehicle operation.With many more tasks for the driver to do, these systemscould also collectively diminish usability and safety. This isparticularly a concern for the carried in equipment, especiallyphones, which are rarely tested for use while driving, eventhough use while driving is expected.

Some perspective of how the current situation has arisen isuseful. Consider the top- five selling vehicles in the U.S. inJanuary 2010 per the Wall Street Journal (http://online.wsj.com/mdc/public/page/2_3022-autosales.html#autosalesC, retrieved February 25, 2010)(Table 1).

Table 1. Top Five Vehicles Sold in the U.S., January2010

These vehicles are all reasonably well equipped. Amongthem, the 2010 Nissan Altima 2.5 with the SL package, atypical contemporary sedan, was selected as an example forwhich technical details are readily available. How does thecurrent vehicle differ from the 2000 model? The 2.5S withthe SL package and the technology option includes anavigation system with a 6.5-inch, touch-screen unit, tractioncontrol, and vehicle stability control. The navigation system

Driver Distraction/Overload Research andEngineering: Problems and Solutions

2010-01-2331Published

10/19/2010

Paul A. GreenUniversity of Michigan

Copyright © 2010 SAE International

Page 2: Driver Distraction/Overload Research and …driving/publications/GreenConvergence2010.pdfproblem for driving research and engineering, especially for work on distraction/overload

includes speed limit advisories, Zagat restaurant guide, XMtraffic and weather data, a DVD player, and provides forseveral ways to accept auxiliary audio/video input. TheAltima does not have adaptive cruise control, yet. Nothinglike this was available on the 2000 model Altima, or for manyother vehicles of that time period. The operation of theseadded systems is something drivers must now learn and willuse while driving. If improperly designed, these systems canbe distracting and their use overwhelming. Further, for any ofthe other vehicles in this list or a larger list, the same trend ofincreasing functionality should be apparent.

Each of the systems just mentioned, at least if it wasdeveloped by an automaker, was probably subjected to somesort of human factors analysis or evaluation. However, thosedoing the work are sometimes so close to those evaluationsthat they may not be aware of the larger problem ofincreasing total vehicle complexity. Moreover, because of thefocus on very specific interface issues, engineers are notaware of the larger shortcomings of current work, which mayfall below the quality of research and engineering being donein other industries. The author does not have any statisticalevidence to support this assertion, though many humanfactors professionals will agree that the problems identified inthe sections that follow are real.

Specifically, five major problems and their solutions areaddressed in this paper. Those problems include (1) themisuse of the term distraction (and potential misdirection ofeffort), (2) driving performance measures and statistics thatare either undefined or poorly defined (to be resolved by anSAE practice), (3) the workload of the driving task is notquantified (for which an equation is proposed), (4) thedemand characteristics of in-vehicle tasks are not quantified(for which a scheme is proposed), and (5) too often, standardsonly specify measurement methods, not compliance criteria(that must be developed).

PROBLEM 1: MISUSE OF THE TERMDISTRACTIONDriver distraction and workload are often usedinterchangeably, but are not the same. Part of the problem isdefining what is the problem. As has been stated before(Oberholtzer, Yee, Green, Eoh, Nguyen, and Schweitzer,2007; Green, 2008), in the popular press but also in thescientific literature, the term “distraction” is often used todescribe the topic addressed here. Two examples are the well-done series in the New York Times, “Driven to Distraction,”(http://topics.nytimes.com/top/news/technology/series/driven_to_distraction/index.html, retrieved March 2, 2010)and the most comprehensive book on the topic, DriverDistraction (Regan, Lee, and Young, 2009). Distractiongenerally refers to something that attracts and retainsattention, whereas workload or overload refer to theindividual and aggregate demands of the tasks a driver

performs. In practice, sometimes the consequences of bothare the same, but nonetheless distraction persists as the labelfor both phenomena, probably because it is easier to getattention and funding.

The naming/identification of the problem is importantbecause of its implications for what one thinks the problem isand which performance measures should be collected. Keepin mind that there is just something compelling aboutanswering a ringing phone, keeping a phone conversationgoing, responding to a text message, or completing an in-vehicle task such as entering a destination. When these tasksare conducted while driving, they become a safety issue.

PROBLEM 2: DRIVINGPERFORMANCE MEASURES ANDSTATISTICS ARE EITHERUNDEFINED OR POORLY DEFINED.Contemporary practice is to establish knowledge, such ashow distraction/overload affects driving, based on thescientific method, a method that has existed for centuries.Richard P. Feynman, the Nobel prize-winning physicist atCaltech, in his well-known textbook Feynman Lectures onPhysics (volume 1, page 2-1), said, “Observation, reason, andexperimentation make up what we call the scientific method.”Following the scientific method involves creating ahypothesis to explain a phenomenon, collecting observable,quantifiable data in experiments to test the hypothesis, andusing reasoning to interpret the results.

Those quantifiable data, measurements, must be repeatableand reliable. The lack of such measures has been a majorproblem for driving research and engineering, especially forwork on distraction/overload. A few examples from Savino(2009), his master's thesis, make the point. Savino reviewedthe refereed human factors literature relating to driving, withthe goals of determining the names used to identify commondriving performance measures and statistics and how theywere defined. He examined every issue of Human Factorsand Ergonomics from 2000 to 2005, as well as the HFES andDriving Assessment Conference Proceedings, and otherreferences. He did not examine SAE or ISO standards, asthose interested in the research were familiar with theircontent. Overall, Savino examined 498 references, of which111 were relevant to his research. Three examples of problemmeasures and statistics follow.

EXAMPLE 1: LANE DEPARTURESThe number of lane departures per unit of time or trial is avery common safety statistic. If drivers are distracted, theyare more likely to depart the lane (Zhang and Smith, 2004).Table 2, from Savino's research, shows how often variousnames for this statistic appear in the literature. Notice that the

Page 3: Driver Distraction/Overload Research and …driving/publications/GreenConvergence2010.pdfproblem for driving research and engineering, especially for work on distraction/overload

most common name is used only one-third of the time, and itis not the preferred name.

Table 2. Names Used for Lane Departure in theLiterature

However, what is most critical is that this measure was rarelydefined and when it was, the definition was imprecise.Contrast the definition of Jenness, Lattanzio, O'Toole, andTaylor, 2002 (page 594) of a lane departure beginning when“the automobile crossed the white sidelines on the roadway,”with that of Blanco, Hankey, and Chestnut, 2005 (page 1977)as beginning “when the vehicle's tire came into contact withthe lane marker.”

So, what then is a lane departure? There are two aspects thatneed to be defined, what part of the lane is considered theboundary and what part of the vehicle is considered to havedeparted. In the U.S., as defined by the Manual of UniformTraffic Control Devices (U.S. Department of Transportation,2009), a lane marking is typically four inches wide.Depending upon whether the boundary is the inside edge ofthe line in the lane in question or the midline of the line is atwo-inch difference. Further, the question is if departing isrelative to any part of the vehicle, for example the outsideedge of the mirror, or the outside of a front tire (Figure 1).For passenger cars and pick up trucks, the outside edge of themirror is three to six inches farther outboard than the edge ofthe tire patch.

Thus, there are at least two candidate criteria for a lanedeparture, (1) the outer edge of the exterior mirror passesover the midline of the lane marking, and (2) the front tiretouches the inside edge of the lane marking.

The first criterion is the most crash relevant. The second iseasier to detect (when using a side-mounted camera). Simplemath suggests there is a one to four inch difference betweenthe two criteria. In reality, the difference is slightly lessbecause most vehicles approach the lane marking at an angle.As a footnote, the definitions in SAE J2808 (Society ofAutomotive Engineers, 2007) and ISO 17631 (International

Standards Organization, 2007) refer to the tire contacting thelane marking.

Figure 1. What is a Lane Departure? Source: Savino,2009, page 25.

EXAMPLE 2: TIME-TO-LINECROSSINGTime-to-line crossing, a key safety measure, reflects thesafety margin for lateral control. When drivers are distracted,the minimum time-to-line crossing over a time windowdecreases (Jamson, Westerman, Hockey, and Carsten, 2004).Fortunately, this is a term for which only that name is used.At a superficial level, it seems simple to define, basically howlong it takes the vehicle to reach the lane boundary. Time-to-line crossing is defined in standards associated with lanedeparture warning systems, SAE J2808 (Society ofAutomotive Engineers, 2007) and ISO 17631 (InternationalStandards Organization, 2007), but papers on lane departurerarely reference those standards or their definitions as well.

There are actually at least three different ways time-to-linecrossing can be defined: (1) as lateral distance divided bylateral velocity, (2) as an expression that includes lateralacceleration, and (3) as the complete trigonometric solutionthat considers the radius of curvature of the vehicle's path andthe radius of curvature of the road. Of the values provided bythe three expressions, the first two of which areapproximations, and all three can differ considerably. (SeeGodthelp, 1984; Van Winsum, Brookhiuis, and de Waard,2000).

Further, as with lane departure, there is the issue of what isconsidered a departure. For time-to-line crossing, it istypically when the tire touches the lane boundary, but it doesnot have to be.

Page 4: Driver Distraction/Overload Research and …driving/publications/GreenConvergence2010.pdfproblem for driving research and engineering, especially for work on distraction/overload

EXAMPLE 3: HEADWAYThe more closely one follows a vehicle ahead, the morelikely a crash (Ervin, Sayer, LeBlanc, Bogard, Mefford,Hagan, Bareket, and Winkler, 2005). Of the various types ofcrashes, rear-end collisions are much more likely whendrivers are distracted (Wang, Knipling, Goodman, 1996).

In the civil engineering literature, for example, the HighwayCapacity Manual (Transportation Research Board, 2010),headway refers to the time difference between when twosuccessive vehicles pass by the same point on a road.Generally, it is the front bumper to front bumper difference(Figure 2). Civil engineers are interested in measuring trafficvolume, how many vehicles/lane/hour occur on a road. Theinverse of the interarrival time of successive vehicles is ameasure of traffic volume.

Figure 2. Gap vs. Headway

However, human factors engineers are interested in the spacebetween vehicles, the stopping distance available to avoid acrash. They often call that distance (or time) “headway,” asshown in Table 3, event though gap is the classically usedand appropriate name (as in gap acceptance studies; Farber,Landis, and Silver, 1968; Chan, Ragland, Shladover,Misener, and Marco, 2005; Hwang and Park, 2005; Farah,Bekhor, Polus, Toledo, 2009). Strangely, SAE and ISOdocuments refer to gap as clearance, a name no one seems touse in the literature.

Table 3. Terms Used for Distance Gap in the Literature

However, the problem is not just that the name headway isinconsistently used, but the intended use is uncertain becauseit is not defined. Savino (2009) found 10 definitions fordistance headway and 18 definitions for time headway in theliterature. The definition of Strayer, Drews & Crouch, 2003,page 27, is typical. Following distance as “the distancebetween the pace car and the participant's car.” Notspecifying the points on the vehicle leads to ambiguity. In asimulator, it is sometimes easiest to give the distance fromcenter of gravity (CG) to center of gravity, since CG relatedmeasures are often computed. However, the differencebetween gap and headway, as defined here, depends on thelength of the lead vehicle. The difference in length betweenand car and a tractor-trailer is close to 40 ft (12 m), quitesubstantial. Further, it would be awkward to define severalgap values, each for a different length lead vehicle or relativeto a different part of the vehicle. (The CG-to-CG distance isnot equal to either gap or headway, but some weightedcombination that depends upon the length of the lead vehicle,an undesired complication.)

So, what is the solution to this problem? The first step wasSavino's (2009) review that provided data to show the lack ofconsistency and provided a basis for defining terms. Usingthat information, a subcommittee of the Society ofAutomotive Engineers Safety and Human Factors Committeeis developing a set of standard term names and operationaldefinitions for driving performance measures and statistics(SAE Recommended Practice J2499). The terms initiallybeing considered are: accelerator response time, acceleratorto brake transition time, brake response time, steering wheelreversal, distance gap, time gap, headway time, headwaydistance, time to collision, lane departure, lane change, laterallane position, and time to line crossing.

There are several key points to be made about thesedefinitions and the process to develop them. First, the SAEsubcommittee is being diligent to make sure the definitionsare based on current practice and the research literature, aswell as criteria for good definitions, not mere opinions.Second, to document how and why particular terms andspecific definitions were selected, a rationale document isbeing written. Third, in several situations, such as for lane

Page 5: Driver Distraction/Overload Research and …driving/publications/GreenConvergence2010.pdfproblem for driving research and engineering, especially for work on distraction/overload

departure and time-to-line crossing, there is a need formultiple definitions to satisfy user needs, which will beprovided. To avoid ambiguity, the intent is for authors in thefuture to identify which definition was used in each case (forexample, as defined in SAE document such and such,definition A).

However, the most difficult part will be getting engineers andresearchers to use those definitions. As was noted earlier, theauthor would assert that the frequency with which research injournal articles on lane departure to collision avoidance citethe relevant SAE and ISO standards is low, though thestatistical analysis has yet to be done. This is a problem, andmay be even one worthy of further discussion. A solution isfor those who review manuscripts to be extremely diligentabout commenting on or rejecting manuscripts that do notrefer to the SAE practice.

This problem of undefined or inconsistently defined measuresis quite serious, making automotive human factorsengineering and research appear second-rate, and makes itdifficult to consistently assess the effects of distraction. Whatwould chemistry be like if there were ten different names foracidity, the pH value could be computed three different ways,and when used, the authors did not identify how the acidity/pH characteristic, or whatever they called it, was determined?

PROBLEM 3: THE WORKLOAD OFTHE DRIVING TASK IS NOT WELLQUANTIFIED.To date, the demand of the primary driving task in moststudies is typically described in general terms, for example,as demanding, or in some studies, as low workload and highworkload. Other times, it is measured, but no single or evensmall set of measures or statistics is consistently used in themajority of studies.

As an example, one of the author's studies (Tsimhoni, Green,and Watanbe, 2001) evaluated the effects of workload onHead Up Display (HUD) use in a driving simulator.Workload was manipulated by varying the radius of the curvedriven, with the implication being that smaller radius curvesrepresented a higher workload. However, there was no directmeasurement of workload.

Keep in mind that what is low, moderate, or high workload isrelative. In parts of the upper peninsula of Michigan,moderate traffic is when a driver sees another vehicle. InTokyo, moderate traffic is when traffic is moving.

Consequently, until now, there has been no consistent basisfor comparing, say a study on a four-lane road in the U.S.with a study on a two-lane road conducted in a drivingsimulator. There have been studies where researchers have

attempted to measure primary task workload, using peripheraldetection tasks, the standard deviation of lane position, etc.Again, no single measure is used consistently, and in somecases, such as for the peripheral detection task; there is noagreement on the procedure. Fortunately, ISO is developing aprocedure for peripheral detection.

This lack of consistent and reliable measures to quantify testconditions also does not reflect favorably on automotivehuman factors work.

Workload depends primarily on road geometry, traffic,visibility, and the road surface condition, each of which canbe quantified. For example, civil engineers describe traffic interms of volume (vehicles/lane/hour) and the percentage ofvehicles that are trucks. Commonly, traffic volume isdescribed in terms of Level of Service, which maps trafficvolume into letter grade categories of A through F, where Acorresponds to excellent driving conditions and F to failingconditions.

As part of the SAVE-IT project, UMTRI developed asubjective rating method to quantify the demands of thedriving task and equations to estimate workload from adescription of the road geometry and traffic (Schweitzer andGreen, 2007). (For other ideas, see Hulse, Dingus, Fischer,and Wierwille, 1989; Green, Lin, and Bagian, 1994; andKondoh, Yamamura, Kitazaki, Kuge, and Boer, 2007).

In brief, researchers presented drivers with clips of roadscenes that drivers rated relative to two anchor clips showinglight and heavy traffic. Those anchor clips were assignedvalues of 2 and 6 respectively. Subjects then rated a largenumber of clips of expressways, urban, and rural roads,varying in terms of the lane in which the driver waspositioned, the Level of Service, and other characteristics. Itis important to note that the clips were taken from theAdvanced Collision Avoidance System (ACAS) fieldoperational test (Ervin, Sayer, LeBlanc, Bogard, Mefford,Hagan, Bareket, and Winkler, 2005), and associated witheach clip were 400 engineering variables. Using regressionanalysis, equations were developed relating rated workload todriving statistics associated with each clip. One equationworthy of note is as follows:

Page 6: Driver Distraction/Overload Research and …driving/publications/GreenConvergence2010.pdfproblem for driving research and engineering, especially for work on distraction/overload

All statistics were computed over a moving 30-s timewindow.

This equation accounted for 85% of the variance of theworkload ratings, remarkably good.

Research is in progress to validate this equation and extend itto other driving situations. Certainly, over time, this and otherequations will evolve, and the measures in them may changeas research is conducted. However, what is notable about thisequation is that all of the measures of interest can be readilycollected and computed in most driving simulators. Further,most vehicles equipped with an adaptive cruise controlsystem should be able to generate the data needed by theequation as well. This is important because in many casescomputing workload will not require much additional effort.

The hope is that in the future, when someone does a study orperforms an evaluation, they would report estimatedworkload (e.g., 5.5 on the UMTRI workload scale,identifying the equation used). Admittedly, just because twodriving situations have similar workload estimates does notmake them identical, but having some basis for comparisonof studies is better than none at all.

The approach proposed here is a simplification of thedemands of real driving. It is well known that humanperformance involves use of at least four types of resources,though many partition what people do more finely (Wickensand McCarley, 2008). Commonly, the literature refers tovisual, auditory, cognitive, and psychomotor (VACP)demands, an idea popularized by McCracken and Aldrich(1984). VACP demands are discussed further later in thispaper.

What is interesting about driving, is that most of the time, onecould argue that the four resources occur in a particularcombination. Driving is clearly visual, and without seeing theroad, one cannot drive (Senders, Kristofferson, Levison,Dietrich, and Ward, 1967; Sivak, 1996). Further, visualdemands are tightly coupled with cognitive demands toprocess the road scene, and motor output to steer the vehicleand control speed. Although one could create test conditionswhere they are decoupled, those combinations do not occurvery often. Hence, for determining the primary demands of

the driving task, measuring the aggregate demand or theprimary visual demand should indicate the demand ofdriving. Thus, the simplification of only measuring overalldemand is reasonable.

To date, these equations have only been examined in a singleexperiment (actually two by the time this paper appears) andthey are far from perfect. However, they are the bestestimates available and they are good. Quite frankly, it couldbe a decade or more before the research basis for these oralternative expressions is complete, and other five years or sobefore adoption occurs. When that finally occurs, how willthe research conducted over the next 15 years be integratedinto future work if there is no basis for comparison of theprimary driving task?

PROBLEM 4: THE DEMANDCHARACTERISTICS OF IN-VEHICLETASKS IN QUESTION ARE NOTWELL QUANTIFIED.A topic of significant debate in the literature is what levels oftask demands (especially visual, cognitive, and psychomotor)are excessive. However, because tasks are only describedqualitatively, a quantitative answer is unlikely to appear.Further, when efforts are made to quantify the VACPdemands of secondary tasks, they are often with reference tothe interference of other secondary tasks (Wierwille andGutmann, 1978; Casali and Wierwille, 1983; Zeitlin, 1995;Verwey, 2000; Harms and Patten, 2003; Jahn, Oehme,Krems, and Gelau, 2005). However, there are few tasks usedas benchmarks consistently across experiments. The Allianceof Automobile Manufacturer guidelines uses manual radiotuning as a benchmark, but across studies, the total time forthat task varies by a factor or six, hardly a stable value (Shahand Green, 2003).

There is reason to believe that in contrast to the primarydriving tasks, the combination of VACP demands vary withthe secondary task. Yee, Nguyen, Green, Oberholtzer, andMiller (2007) examined a large number of naturally occurringsecondary driving tasks that drivers often do, such assmoking, eating, etc. These tasks had been identified in aprior evaluation of real-world driving (Oberholtzer, Yee,Green, Eoh, Nguyen, and Schweitzer, 2007). Tasks were splitinto subtasks (reach for cigarette, light cigarette, etc.), andtheir VACP demands were identified relative to UMTRI-enhanced scales that are part of the Army IMPRINT(Improved Performance Research Integration Tool, http://www.arl.army.mil/www/default.cfm?Action=445, retrievedFebruary 28, 2010) benchmarks. IMPRINT is an applicationdeveloped by the Army used to assess the workload of a widevariety of systems including future strike fighters (Brett,Doyal, Malek, Martin, Hoagland and Anesgart, 2002), futuretanks (Mitchell, Samms, Henthorn, and Wojciechowski,

Page 7: Driver Distraction/Overload Research and …driving/publications/GreenConvergence2010.pdfproblem for driving research and engineering, especially for work on distraction/overload

2003), and unmanned aerial vehicles (Walters, French, andBarnes, 2000) to determine manning requirements forsystems and where interfaces and tasks need to be redesignedto optimize combat performance.

As part of the SAVE-IT project, the VACP scales wereenhanced to make them easier to use. As an example, thevisual scale is shown in Table 4.

Note that these scales are strictly ordinal, though it iscommon to use them as ratio scales. It may not be the casethat to visually locate/align (selective orientation) is fourtimes as demanding as visually register/detect image, onlythat it is more.

Furthermore, it is unknown how much demand is too much,either on a single scale or in combination. Under whatcircumstances is a visual demand of 6.0 excessive? That ofcourse will depend on the workload of the primary task, theduration of the secondary tasks, and the cognitive, auditory,and psychomotor demands of the task as well. Of course,since these have yet to be quantified in a common manner inthe automotive literature, there is no direct, quantitativeanswer to the excessive visual demand question just posed.However, should research begin to quantify tasks, then overtime enough data will be accumulated to answer it.

As a first step to explore these scales, Yee, Nguyen, Green,Oberholtzer, and Miller (2007) examined the relationshipbetween the VACP demands for a variety of naturallyoccurring tasks while driving. As an example, Figure 3 showsthe relationship between subtask demands (again, lighting acigarette was a subtask) for the visual and cognitivedimensions, the two most highly correlated. Thesecorrelations were computed assuming the scales were ratioscale, which they technically are not, but they commonly areused as ratio scales. In this analysis, subtasks were usedbecause the combination of VACP values varied fromsubtask to subtask that formed a task. For example, thesubtasks for a phone consist of preparing to use a phone todial, answering a handheld phone, dialing a phone (case a-handheld, case b-hands-free), conversing on a phone, holdinga phone but not conversing, and hanging up/ending a call.

Figure 3. Visual vs. Cognitive Demand for VariousSubtasks Large dots signify multiple responses at the

same data point.

PROBLEM 5: TOO OFTEN,STANDARDS ONLY SPECIFYMEASUREMENT METHODS, NOTCOMPLIANCE CRITERIA.Standards, guidelines, rules, and regulations fall into twocategories, design oriented and performance oriented. Designoriented specifications identify specific physicalcharacteristics for some feature and may specify values for it,such as a bumper height, a minimum acceptable contrast ratiofor a letter, or a minimum intensity for a sound, say awarning. Performance specifications identify how well asystem should do in a test, such as the maximum load onsome body part in a crash, or the maximum allowable timefor drivers to perform certain tasks while driving.

There are numerous standards, guidelines, rules andregulations that relate to driver distraction. The mostimportant ones including U.S. Federal Motor Vehicle SafetyStandards and NCAP regulations (U.S. Department of

Table 4. Visual Demand Scale

Page 8: Driver Distraction/Overload Research and …driving/publications/GreenConvergence2010.pdfproblem for driving research and engineering, especially for work on distraction/overload

Transportation, 2008a,b), national laws of countries otherthan the U.S., International Standards Organization standards,SAE standards and practices such as SAE J2364 (Green,1999a,b; Society of Automotive Engineers, 2004), Allianceof Automobile Manufacturer Guidelines (Alliance ofAutomobile Manufacturers, 2003), state and provincial laws(Legislative Assembly of Ontario, 2008, CaliforniaDepartment of Motor Vehicles, 2010; New York StateGovernor's Safety Commission, 2010), and local ordinances(City of Ann Arbor, 2010; http://www.clickondetroit.com/news/9231667/detail.html, retrieved March 2, 2010). This is areflection of the complexity of producing a product that ismanufactured internationally and sold in many jurisdictions,where many organizations have a rightful say in safety.

Which of these sets of documents is most important dependson this situation. However, it is apparent that ISO documentsare consistently important, so they are discussed here.

Table 5 contains a list of the currently applicable ISOstandards to driver distraction/driver workload. Of the 10standards listed, four provide compliance criteria now andothers may in the future. However, most of those criteria arevery simple, easy to meet, and pertain to design-for charactersizes, warning intensity, and so forth. Other than for theocclusion procedure (ISO 16673), what is excessivelydistracting, a performance characteristic, is left for themanufacturer or supplier to determine.

Not providing criteria, leaving up to the user to determinewhat is distracting, has some interesting consequences. Themajor automakers with a human factors staff have thecapability to decide what is excessive, but where there is noperformance criterion, there is no incentive to conduct thesetests, so they may not do it. In the organizations with few orno human factors staff, they lack appropriate performancecriteria, and accordingly will not perform the evaluationunless required to do so. To put it plainly, if there is noperformance criterion for distraction testing, tests fordistraction will not be conducted.

As a practical matter, automakers producing vehicles in theUnited States have agreed to comply with the AAMguidelines (Alliance of Automobile Manufacturers, 2003),and those in Japan comply with JAMA requirements (JapanAutomobile Manufacturers Association, 2004; Nakamura,2008). Aftermarket manufacturers should comply with SAERecommended Practice 2364, but they do not, and there areconsequences as a result (Canadian Broadcasting Company,2010). Thus, when the critical testing occurs, it is primarily tomeet the ISO 16673 requirements, similar requirements in theAAM guidelines, and the requirements of SAE J2364.

For those unfamiliar with them, the AAM guidelines for thedesign, installation, and use of telematics are based upon a setof 24 high-level principles (e.g., Principle 1.3 “No part of the

physical system should obstruct any vehicle controls ordisplays required for the driving task.”). The AAM guidelineswere derived from the 1999 European Statement ofPrinciples, which have since been updated (Board andStevens, 2001; Commission of the European Communities,1999, 2006). The AAM guidelines go one step further byelaborating on their use and in some cases, providing testprocedures. Of these principles, principle 2.1 matters most,(“Systems with visual displays should be designed such thatthe driver can complete the desired task with sequentialglances that are brief enough not to adversely affectdriving.”)

The AAM guidelines need to be improved. As noted byMorton and Angel (2005), page i “In the assessment of theAAM guidelines, principles appeared to be valid but ofteninsufficiently detailed and too vague in the accompanyingelaborations. This led to poor reliability of results betweeninspectors.” They further note on the same page, “Withrevisions the AAM guidelines may be sufficient to ensuresafe operation of telematic systems, but insufficient in currentform due to inadequate scientific support, incompleteness,and poor reliability of results.” Particular concerns wereexpressed with regards to the definition of measures and theselection of performance criteria.

So what should be done? ISO TC 22/SC 13/WG 8 needs toconsider what it development program should be. If thepurpose is to aid research by developing standard protocols,then that goal should be clearly stated. However, that will notoccur if the committee consists largely of industry members,as it does now, with scant representation from the academiccommunity. If the goal is to develop standards to aid industryand government in assessing the safety and usability of newproducts and services, then performance criteria must beincluded in standards.

Further, the AAM guidelines need revision, but that isunlikely at the moment given the current state of theautomotive industry.

SUMMARY/CONCLUSIONS ANDCLOSING THOUGHTSThis paper provides a rather critical perspective of someaspects of research and associated engineering pertaining todriver distraction/driver workload. It should not be taken toimply that those engaged in this research are not capableindividuals nor that their intentions are not noble. In fact,many of the criticisms voiced apply to the author's ownresearch. Certainly, the research to date has been informative.

Over time, research evolves, and the quality of work shouldimprove as studies build upon the knowledge of prioractivities. For that to occur, five things need to be done:

Page 9: Driver Distraction/Overload Research and …driving/publications/GreenConvergence2010.pdfproblem for driving research and engineering, especially for work on distraction/overload

1. Authors need to clearly define what they mean bydistraction to distinguish between the scientific and popularinterpretations.

2. SAE needs to complete its work to define drivingperformance measures, and authors need to begin using them.Some means to induce use may be needed.

Table 5. ISO Standards Produced by ISO TC 22/SC 13WG 8

Page 10: Driver Distraction/Overload Research and …driving/publications/GreenConvergence2010.pdfproblem for driving research and engineering, especially for work on distraction/overload

3. Studies of driving need to quantify the demands of theprimary task of driving. The author has proposed onepotential equation for that purpose.

4. The visual, auditory, cognitive, and psychomotor demandssecondary tasks need to be quantified. The author hasproposed a method that uses the U.S. Army IMPRINT scales.The last three points have a common goal-to improve therigor of evaluations conducted so they can be compared.

5. Compliance criteria need to be developed for several ISOstandards and refined for the AAM guidelines. Withoutexplicit criteria, everything is acceptable, and driving safetysuffers.

REFERENCES1. Alliance of Automobile Manufacturers, (2003). Statementof Principles on Human-Machine Interfaces (HMI) for In-vehicle Information and Communication Systems, Version3.0. Washington, D.C.: Alliance of AutomobileManufacturers, (www.umich.edu/∼driving/guidelines/AAM_Statement2003.pdf, retrieved July 15, 2007).2. American Association of State Highway andTransportation Officials (AASHTO) (2004). A Policy onGeometric Design of Streets and Highways, Washington,D.C.: American Association of State Highway andTransportation Officials.3. Blanco, M., Hankey, J. M., & Chestnut, J. A. (2005). ATaxonomy for Secondary In-Vehicle Tasks Based on EyeGlance and Task Completion Time, Proceedings of the 49thHuman Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting,Orlando, FL.4. Board, A.C. and Stevens, A. (2001). Assessing theCompliance of Telematics Automotive Products with theEuropean Principles of Human-Machine Interaction, Journalof Navigation, 54(3), 365-370 (http://journals.cambridge.org/production/action/cjoGetFulltext?fulltextid=86162, retrievedMarch 3, 2010).5. Brett, B.E., Doyal, J.A., Malek, D.A., Martin, E.A.,Hoagland, D.G., Anesgart, M.N. (2002). The CombatAutomation Requirements Testbed (CART) Task 5 InterimReport: Modeling a Strike Fighter Pilot Conducting a TimeCritical Target Mission, (AFRL-HE-WP-TR-2002-0018),Wright-Patterson AFB, OH: Air Force Research Laboratory.6. Brown, I.D., Tickner, A.H., and Simmonds, D.C.V.(1969). Interference between Concurrent Tasks of Drivingand Telephoning, Journal of Applied Psychology, 53(5),419-424.7. California Department of Motor Vehicles (2010). Hand-Held Wireless Telephone: Prohibited Use (http://www.dmv.ca.gov/pubs/vctop/d11/vc23123.htm, retrievedMarch 2, 2010).8. Canadian Broadcasting Company (2010). ConvenienceOver Safety? (Marketplace episode, aired January 2010),

(http://www.cbc.ca/marketplace/2010/gps_distraction/main.html, retrieved March 2, 2010).

9. Casali, J. G. and Wierwille, W.W. (1983). A Comparisonof Rating Scale, Secondary Task, Physiological, and Primary-Task Workload Estimation Techniques in a Simulated FlightTask Emphasizing Communications Load, Human Factors,25, 623-641.

10. Chan, C-Y, Ragland, D.R., Shladover, S.E., Misener,J.A., and Marco, D. (2005). Observations of Driver Time-TapAcceptance at Intersections in Left-Turn Across-PathOpposite-Direction Scenarios, 2005 TRB Annual Meeting,Paper No. 05-2159, (path.berkeley.edu/∼cychan/Research_and_Presentation/Intersection_Decision_Support/TRR2005_05-2159.pdf, retrieved March 2, 2010).

11. City of Ann Arbor (2010). Use of Cell Phone or OtherPortable Electronic Device While Operating Motor Vehicleor Bicycle Prohibited (new section 10:1010a to be added tochapter 126 (Traffic of Title X of the Code of the City of AnnArbor), version of February 28, 2010.

12. Commission of the European Communities (1999).Statement of Principles on Human Machine Interface (HMI)for In-Vehicle Information and Communication Systems(“EU Principles), (Annex 1 to Commission Recommendationof 21 December 1999 on safe and efficient in-vehicleinformation and communication systems: A Europeanstatement of principles on human machine interface)Brussels, Belgium: European Union, (http://www.umich.edu/∼driving/documents/EU_guidelines.pdf, retrieved March 3,2010).

13. Commission of the European Communities (2006).Updated Statement of Principles on Human MachineInterface (HMI) for In-Vehicle Information andCommunication Systems Annex 1 to CommissionRecommendation of 22 December 2006 on safe and efficientin-vehicle information and communication systems: AEuropean statement of principles on human machineinterface) Brussels, Belgium: European Union, (http://eur-lex.europa.eu/LexUriServ/site/en/oj/2007/1_032/1_03220070206en02000241.pdf, retrievedMarch 3, 2010).

14. Ervin, R., Sayer, J., LeBlanc, D., Bogard, S., Mefford,M., Hagan, M., Bareket, Z., and Winkler, C. (2005).Automotive Collision Avoidance System (ACAS) FieldOperational Test Methodology and Results (technical reportUS DOT HS 809 901), Washington, DC: Department ofTransportation.

15. Farah, H., Bekhor, S., Polus, A., and Toledo, T (2009). APassing Gap Acceptance Model for Two-Lane RuralHighways, Transportmetrica, 5(3), 159-172.

16. Farber, E., Landis, D. and Silver, C.A. (1968).Knowledge of Closing Rate Versus Knowledge of OncomingCar-Speed As Determiners of Driver Passing Behavior, paperpresented at Highway Research Board 47th Annual Meeting.

Page 11: Driver Distraction/Overload Research and …driving/publications/GreenConvergence2010.pdfproblem for driving research and engineering, especially for work on distraction/overload

17. Feynman, R.P. (1964). Feynman Lectures on Physics,Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

18. Godthelp, J. (1984). Studies on Human Vehicle Control(Ph.D. thesis), Delft, Netherlands: Delft University ofTechnology.

19. Green, P. (1999a). Estimating Compliance with the 15-second Rule for Driver-interface Usability and Safety,Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society43rd Annual Meeting (CD-ROM), Santa Monica, CA:Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.

20. Green, P. (1999b). The 15-second Rule for DriverInformation Systems, ITS America Ninth Annual MeetingConference Proceedings (CD-ROM), Washington, D.C.,Intelligent Transportation Society of America.

21. Green, P., “Driver Distraction, Telematics Design, andWorkload Managers: Safety Issues and Solutions,”Convergence Technical Paper 2004-21-0022, 2004.

22. Green, P., “Driver Status and Implications for CrashSafety,” Convergence Technical Paper 2006-21-0028, 2006.

23. Green, P., “Driver Interface/HMI Standards to MinimizeDriver Distraction/Overload,” Convergence Technical Paper2008-21-0002, 2008.

24. Green, P. (2008). Driver Interface Safety and UsabilityStandards: An Overview, chapter 24 in Regan, M.A., Lee,J.D., and Young, K.L., Driver Distraction: Theory, Effects,and Mitigation, Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

25. Green, P., Lin, B., and Bagian, (1994). Driver Workloadas a Function of Road Geometry: A Pilot Experiment(technical report GLCTTR 22-91/01), Ann Arbor, MI:University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute.

26. Harms, L. and Patten, C. (2003). Peripheral Detection asa Measure of Driver Distraction. A Study of Memory-basedversus System-based Navigation in a Built-up Area,Transportation Research, Part F, 6(1), 23-36.

27. Hulse, M., Dingus, T., Fischer, T., and Wierwille, W.W.(1989). The Influence of Roadway Parameters on DriverPerception of Attentional Demand, Advances in IndustrialErgonomics and Safety I, 451-456.

28. Hwang, S-Y and Park, C.H. (2005). Modeling of the GapAcceptance Behavior at a Merging Section of an UrbanFreeway, Proceedings of the Eastern Asia Society forTransportation Studies, 5, 1641-1656.

29. International Standards Organization (2007). IntelligentTransport Systems -- Lane Departure Warning Systems --Performance Requirements and Test Procedures (ISOstandard 17361:2007), Geneva, Switzerland: InternationalStandards Organization.

30. Japan Automobile Manufacturers Association (2004).Guideline for In-Vehicle Display Systems-Version 3.0,Tokyo, Japan: Japan Automobile Manufacturers Association,

(www.jama.or.jp/safe/guideline/pdf/jama_guideline_v30_en.pdf, retrieved March 3, 2010).

31. Jahn, G., Oehme, a., Krems, J.F., and Gelau, C. (2005).Peripheral Detection as a Workload Measure in Driving:Effects of traffic Complexity and Route Guidance SystemUse in a Driving Study, Transportation Research, Part F,8(3), 255-275.

32. Jamson, A.H., Westerman, S.J., Hockey, G.R.J., andCarsten, O.M.J. (2004). Speech-Based E-Mail and DriverBehavior: Effect of an In-Vehicle Message System Interface,Human Factors, 46(4), 625-639.

33. Jenness, J. W., Lattanzio, R. J., O'Toole, M., & Taylor,N. (2002). Voice-Activated Dialing or Eating aCheeseburger: Which is More Distracting during SimulatedDriving? Proceedings of the 46th Human Factors andErgonomics Society Annual Meeting, Baltimore, MD,592-596.

34. Kondoh, T., Yamamura, T., Kitazaki, S., Kuge, N., andBoer, E.R. (2007). Identification of Visual Cues andQuantification of Drivers' Perceptions of Proximity Risk tothe Lead Vehicle in Car-Following Situations, Journal ofMechanical Systems for Transportation and Logistics, 1(2),170-180.

35. Legislative Assembly of Ontario (2008). An Act toamend the Highway Traffic Act to prohibit the use of deviceswith display screens and hand-held communication andentertainment devices and to amend the Public Vehicles Actwith respect to car pool vehicles (Countering DistractedDriving and Promoting Green Transportation Act, 2008, Bill118). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Legislative Assembly ofOntario. (http://www.ontla.on.ca/bills/bills-files/39_Parliament/Sessionl/b118.pdf, retrieved February 27,2010).

36. McCraken, J.H., & Aldrich, T.B. (1984). Analyses ofSelected LHX Mission Functions: Implications of OperatorWorkload and System Automation Goals (report TNAASI479-24-84). Fort Rucker, AL: Anacapa Sciences, Inc.

37. Mitchell, D. K.; Samms, C.; Henthorn, T. &Wojciechowski, J. (2003). Trade Study: A Two- VersusThree-Soldier Crew for the Mounted Combat System (MCS)and Other Future Combat System Platforms, (technical reportARL-TR-3026), Aberdeen Proving Ground, MD: U.S. ArmyResearch Laboratory.

38. Morton, A.H., and Angel, H. A. (2005). Evaluation ofIndustry Safety Principles for In-Vehicle Information andCommunication Systems (technical report prepared forTransport Canada), Guelph, Ontario: Humansystems, Inc.

39. Nakamura, Y., “JAMA Guideline for In-Vehicle DisplaySystems,” Convergence Technical Paper 2008-21-0003,2008.

40. New York State Governor's Safety Commission (2010).Distracted Driving, Talking & Texting (http://

Page 12: Driver Distraction/Overload Research and …driving/publications/GreenConvergence2010.pdfproblem for driving research and engineering, especially for work on distraction/overload

www.nysgtsc.state.ny.us/phon-ndx.htm, retrieved March 2,2010).

41. Oberholtzer, J., Yee, S., Green, P. A., Eoh, H., Nguyen,L., and Schweitzer, J. (2007). Frequency of Distracting TasksPeople Do While Driving: An Analysis of the ACAS FOTData (technical report UMTRI-2006-17), Ann Arbor,Michigan: University of Michigan Transportation ResearchInstitute.

42. Regan, M., Lee, J.D., and Young, K.L. (2009). DriverDistraction: Theory, Effects, and Mitigation, Boca Raton, FL:CRC Press.

43. Savino, M.R. (2009). Standardized Names andDefinitions for Driving Performance Measures (unpublishedmasters thesis), Medford, MA: Tufts University.

44. Senders, J.W., Kristofferson, A.B., Levison, W.H.,Dietrich, C.W., and Ward, J.L. (1967). The AttentionalDemand of Automobile Driving, Highway Research Record #195, Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences,Transportation Research Board, 15-33

45. Schweitzer, J. and Green, P.A. (2007). Task Acceptabilityand Workload of Driving Urban Roads, Highways, andExpressway: Ratings from Video Clips (technical reportUMTRI-2006-6), Ann Arbor, MI: University of MichiganTransportation Research Institute.

46. Sivak, M. (1996). The Information That Drivers Use: IsIt Indeed 90% Visual? Perception, 25(9), 1081-1089.

47. SAE International Surface Vehicle RecommendedPractice, “Navigation and Route Guidance FunctionAccessibility While Driving,” SAE Standard J2364, IssuedAug. 2004.

48. SAE International Surface Vehicle Information Report,“Road/Lane Departure Warning Systems: Information for theHuman Interface,” SAE Standard J2808, Issued Aug. 2007.

49. Shah, R. and Green, P. (2003). Task Time and GlanceMeasures for Telematics and Other Functions: A TabularSummary of the Literature, (Technical ReportUMTRI-2003-33). Ann Arbor, MI: University of MichiganTransportation Research Institute.

50. Strayer, D. L, Drews, F. A., & Crouch, D. J. (2003).Fatal Distraction? A Comparison of the Cell-Phone Driverand the Drunk Driver, Proceedings of the SecondInternational Driving Symposium on Human Factors inDriving Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design, Park City,UT, 25-30.

51. Transportation Research Board (2010). HighwayCapacity Manual, Washington, D.C.: TransportationResearch Board, National Academy of Sciences.

52. Tsimhoni, O., Green, P. and Watanabe, H. (2001).Detecting and Reading Text on HUDs: Effects of DrivingWorkload and Message Location, Paper presented at the 11thAnnual ITS America Meeting, Miami, FL, CD-ROM.

53. U.S. Department of Transportation (2008a). ForwardCollision Warning System Confirmation Test, Washington,D.C.: U.S. Department of Transportation, National HighwayTraffic Safety Administration, Office of Crash AvoidanceStandards, Office of Vehicle Safety.

54. U.S. Department of Transportation (2008b). LaneDeparture Warning System Confirmation Test, Washington,D.C.: U.S. Department of Transportation, National HighwayTraffic Safety Administration, Office of Crash AvoidanceStandards, Office of Vehicle Safety.

55. Verwey, W. B. (2000). On-line Driver WorkloadEstimation. Effects of Road Situation and Age on SecondaryTask Measures. Ergonomics, 43, 187-209.

56. Walters, B., French, J., and Barnes, M.J. (2000).Modeling the Effects of Crew Size and Crew Fatigue on theControl of Tactical Unmanned Aerial Vehicles (TUAVS),Proceedings of the 2000 Winter Simulation Conference,920-924.

57. Wang, J.-S., Knipling, R.R., and Goodman, M. J. (1996).The Role of Driver Inattention in Crashes: New Statisticsfrom the 1995 Crashworthiness Data System, Association forthe Advancement of Automotive Medicine 40th AnnualConference Proceedings, Association for the Advancementof Automotive Medicine Des Plaines, Iowa, 377-392.

58. Wickens, C.D. and McCarley, J.S. (2008). AppliedAttention Theory, Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

59. Wierwille, W. W., & Gutmann, J. C. (1978). Comparisonof Secondary and Primary Task Measures as a Function ofSimulated Vehicle Dynamics and Driving Conditions,Human Factors, 20, 233 - 244.

60. U.S. Department of Transportation (2009). Manual ofUniform Traffic Control Devices, Washington, D.C.: U.S.Department of Transportation, Federal HighwayAdministration.

61. Yee, S., Nguyen, L., Green, P.A., Oberholtzer, J. andMiller, B. (2007). Visual, Auditory, Cognitive, andPsychomotor Demands of Real In-Vehicle Tasks (technicalreport UMTRI-2006-20), Ann Arbor, MI: University ofMichigan Transportation Research Institute.

62. Zhang, H. and Smith, M. (2004). Safety Vehicle UsingAdaptive Interface Technology (Task 7): A Literature Reviewof Visual Distraction Research (technical report prepared forthe U.S. Department of Transportation), Kokomo, IN: DelphiCorporation (http://www.volpe.dot.gov/hf/roadway/saveit/docs/dec04/litrev_7a.pdf, retrieved March 4, 2010).

63. Zeitlin, LR. (1995). Estimates of Driver MentalWorkload: A Long-Term Field Trial of Two SubsidiaryTasks, Human Factors, 37(3), 611-621.

Page 13: Driver Distraction/Overload Research and …driving/publications/GreenConvergence2010.pdfproblem for driving research and engineering, especially for work on distraction/overload

CONTACT INFORMATIONPaul GreenUniversity of Michigan Transportation Research InstituteDriver Interface Group2901 Baxter RoadAnn Arbor, Michigan USA [email protected] 734 763 3795

The Engineering Meetings Board has approved this paper for publication. It hassuccessfully completed SAE's peer review process under the supervision of the sessionorganizer. This process requires a minimum of three (3) reviews by industry experts.

All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in aretrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical,photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the prior written permission of SAE.

ISSN 0148-7191

doi:10.4271/2010-01-2331

Positions and opinions advanced in this paper are those of the author(s) and notnecessarily those of SAE. The author is solely responsible for the content of the paper.

SAE Customer Service:Tel: 877-606-7323 (inside USA and Canada)Tel: 724-776-4970 (outside USA)Fax: 724-776-0790Email: [email protected] Web Address: http://www.sae.orgPrinted in USA