Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    1/40

    O R I G I N A L A R T I C L E

    The ABCs of REBT I: A Preliminary Study of Errors

    and Confusions in Counselling and PsychotherapyTextbooks

    Windy Dryden

    Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

    Abstract In this study, a random sample of twenty counselling and psychotherapy

    textbooks were studied with respect to the errors and confusions made by the

    authors of these textbooks with respect to the ABCs of REBT. A total of 240 of

    such errors/confusions were found with most being made about beliefs at B,

    particularly about irrational beliefs. A variety of errors and confusions were also

    made about (i) the relationship between B and C (including whether or not

    such a relationship is causal), (ii) the relationship between irrational beliefs anddisturbed responses at C, (iii) A and (iv) emotional Cs. Twenty errors were

    even made about the name of the therapy! It was suggested that one way of

    addressing this state of affairs would be for the Albert Ellis Institute to commission

    a group of REBT experts to write a document especially for authors of counselling

    and psychotherapy textbooks and for publishers of these works that specifies clearly

    and accurately agreed wisdom about the ABCs of REBT. The weaknesses of the

    current study were noted and suggestions for future research made.

    Keywords REBT

    Errors and confusions

    ABCs of REBT

    A key aspect of Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT) is how this therapy

    conceptualises clients problems and healthy solutions to these problems. Whenever

    REBT is written about by its adherents, taught in professional and academic settings

    and employed with clients, there is a very good chance that what has become

    known as the ABCs of REBT will be used to show how this therapy assesses

    psychological problems and how it views healthy, but realistic alternatives to theseproblems.

    W. Dryden (&)

    Goldsmiths University of London, New Cross, London SE14 6NW, UK

    e-mail: [email protected]

    123

    J Rat-Emo Cognitive-Behav Ther

    DOI 10.1007/s10942-011-0137-1

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    2/40

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    3/40

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    4/40

    and self-, other- and life-acceptance beliefs) are secondary in that they are said to be

    derived from the non-dogmatic preferences.

    Partial Versus Full Rational Beliefs

    There is one important point that guides my thinking about rational beliefs. It is

    Elliss idea that people easily transform rational beliefs into irrational beliefs,

    particularly when their rational beliefs are strongly held. Thus, it is very important

    that rational beliefs are carefully distinguished from irrational beliefs. To do this one

    needs to discriminate between partial rational beliefs and full rational beliefs. Partial

    rational beliefs are beliefs that assert the rational component of the belief, but do not

    negate the irrational component. Thus, it is easy for partial rational beliefs to be

    implicitly transformed into irrational beliefs, whereas this is not possible with full

    rational beliefs since these latter beliefs are the antithesis of irrational beliefs. InTable 1, I outline and illustrate the differences between partial and full rational

    beliefs.

    C

    C stands for the consequences of the beliefs held at B about the critical A.

    This is often shown as A 9 B = C. There are three such consequences at

    C: emotional, behavioural and cognitive.

    Emotional Cs

    REBT theory distinguishes between healthy negative emotions (HNEs) and

    unhealthy negative emotions (UNEs). Healthy negative emotions are deemed to

    stem from rational beliefs and unhealthy negative emotions from irrational beliefs.

    As such the REBT theory of emotions is a qualitative one and not a quantitative one.

    This means that HNEs and UNEs are placed on separate continua of intensity and

    not on a single continuum of intensity.

    There is some difference of opinion among REBT theorists concerning the length

    of the separate continua. Ellis (1994) argues that they are of the same length and that

    HNEs can be as intense at their zenith as UNEs, whereas my position is that the

    HNE intensity continuum is shorter than the UNE continuum in that at their height

    UNEs are more intense than HNEs at their height. For example, blind rage (100%)

    will be more intense than healthy anger at the highest possible level of intensity.

    Figure 1 presents these two positions.

    Leaving aside the above differences, the point that distinguishes REBT from

    other approaches to CBT is that UNEs and HNEs are qualitatively not quantitatively

    different (Dryden 2009).

    Health and Flavour of Negative Emotions I made the point above that beliefs

    largely determine the health of negative emotions with rational beliefs underpinning

    healthy negative emotions (HNEs) and irrational beliefs underpinning unhealthy

    W. Dryden

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    5/40

    negative emotions (UNEs). However what gives emotions their flavour and whatdistinguishes among the different emotional flavours are the different inferences that

    people make at A.

    Let me provide an example. When a person is anxious (UNE) then, according to

    REBT theory, they hold an irrational belief. The healthy alternative to anxiety in

    Table 1 Distinguishing between partial and full rational beliefs

    Partial rational belief Full rational belief

    Partial preference Flexible belief (or non-dogmatic preference)

    Preference asserted

    Demand not negated

    Preference asserted

    Demand negated

    This leaves open the possibility that the person will

    transform his partial preference into a demand

    (e.g. I want to do well..and therefore I have to

    do so)

    (e.g. I want to do well.but I dont have to do so)

    Partial non-awfulising belief Full non-awfulising belief

    Badness asserted,

    Awfulising not negated

    Badness asserted

    Awfulising negated

    This leaves open the possibility that the person willtransform his partial non-awfulising belief into an

    awfulising belief

    (e.g. It would be bad if I dont do well.and

    therefore it would be awful)

    (e.g. It would be bad if I dont do well.but it

    wouldnt be awful )

    Partial discomfort tolerance belief Full discomfort tolerance belief

    Struggle asserted

    Intolerability not negated

    Struggle asserted

    Intolerability negated

    Tolerability asserted

    Worth it component asserted

    This leaves open the possibility that the person willtransform his partial discomfort tolerance belief

    into a discomfort intolerance belief

    (e.g. It would be hard for me to tolerate not doing

    well.and therefore it would be intolerable)

    (e.g. It would be hard for me to tolerate not doing

    well. but it wouldnt be intolerable. I could

    tolerate it and it would be worth it to me to do

    so)

    Partial acceptance belief Full acceptance belief

    Negative evaluation of part asserted

    Negative evaluation of whole negated,

    Acceptance of whole not asserted

    Negative evaluation of part asserted

    Negative evaluation of whole negated.

    Acceptance of whole assertedThis leaves the possibility that the person will

    transform the partial acceptance belief into a

    depreciation belief

    (e.g. Not doing well would be bad, but it wouldnt

    prove that I am a failure.but I would be

    worthier if I did well than if I didnt)

    (e.g. Not doing well would be bad, but it doesnt

    prove that I am a failure.I am a fallible human

    being whose worth is based on my aliveness not

    on my performances)

    Errors and Confusions in REBT

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    6/40

    REBT theory is concern (HNE). When a person is concerned but not anxious then

    again according to REBT theory they hold a rational belief. However beliefs, on

    their own, cannot explain why the person feels anxiety or concern rather than, say,depression or sadness. What determines what I call the flavour of an emotion is the

    theme implicit in the inference that the person makes at A which in the case of

    anxiety/concern is threat.

    While inferential themes at A determine the flavour of an emotion, they cannot

    on their own determine whether that resultant emotional flavour is healthy or

    unhealthy. As we have seen, beliefs are the significant determining factor on this

    point. Thus, we need to understand both the inferential theme at A and the belief

    that the person holds at B if we are to understand the precise negative emotion

    that the person experiences at C. These points are shown in the formulaepresented below.

    A 9 B = C

    Inferential theme 9 Belief= Emotion

    A 9 iB = C

    Inferential theme 9 Irrational Belief= UNE

    A 9 rB = C

    Inferential theme 9 Rational Belief= HNE

    Table 2 presents the eight main unhealthy negative emotions for which clients

    seek help, their healthy alternatives and the relevant inferential themes and beliefs

    associated with each emotional pairing.

    Ellis version

    0______________________________________________________________________________100

    Unhealthy Unhealthy

    Anger Anger

    0______________________________________________________________________________100

    Healthy Healthy

    Anger Anger

    Dryden version

    0______________________________________________________________________________100

    Unhealthy Unhealthy

    Anger Anger

    0__________________________________________________________100

    Healthy Healthy

    Anger Anger

    Fig. 1 Two versions of the two continuum model of unhealthy and unhealthy negative emotions with

    reference to levels of intensity (as exemplified in anger)

    W. Dryden

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    7/40

    Behavioural Cs

    The second set of consequences of beliefs at B about A concerns behaviour.

    Here, I distinguish between overt behaviour and action tendencies. The latterconcern an impulse to act in a certain way, but without that impulse being converted

    into overt action. This distinction is important for we know that clients suppress

    their action tendencies and can manage to act healthily despite holding irrational

    beliefs. Thus, by their behaviour they appear to hold rational beliefs, but their

    suppressed action tendencies reveal that, in fact, they are holding irrational beliefs.

    For example, consider Ed, who is unhealthily angry towards his boss for criticising

    his work. What Ed did was to thank his boss for the latters helpful feedback.

    What he felt like doing, but suppressed, was to pin his boss to the wall and beat him

    to a pulp. Here, the clients action tendencies revealed his true beliefs and his actualbehaviour revealed his ability to suppress his impulses and act counter to how he

    was feeling.

    Table 3 shows the most common behaviours / action tendencies associated with

    the major UNEs and HNEs.

    Table 2 The eight main unhealthy negative emotions for which clients seek help, their healthy alter-

    natives and the relevant inferential themes and beliefs associated with each emotional pairing

    Inferential theme Belief Emotion

    Threat IrrationalRational

    Anxiety (UNE)Concern (HNE)

    Loss

    Failure

    Undeserved plight

    Irrational

    Rational

    Depression (UNE)

    Sadness (HNE)

    Immoral act

    Failing to act morally

    Hurting others

    Irrational

    Rational

    Guilt (UNE)

    Remorse (HNE)

    Falling short of ones ideal

    Behaviour reflects badly on social groupOthers negative judgment of self

    Irrational

    Rational

    Shame (UNE)

    Disappointment (HNE)

    Others bad treatment of self; self undeserving

    Relationship devaluation

    Irrational

    Rational

    Hurt (UNE)

    Sorrow

    Frustration

    Goal obstruction

    Other transgresses personal rule

    Self transgresses personal rule

    Threat to self-esteem

    Irrational

    Rational

    Unhealthy anger (UNE)

    Healthy anger (HNE)

    Threat posed to relationship by a third personUncertainty about partner in the contest of the above threat

    IrrationalRational

    Unhealthy jealousy (UNE)Healthy jealousy (HNE)

    Another person possesses and enjoys something

    desirable that one does not have

    Irrational

    Rational

    Unhealthy envy (UNE)

    Healthy envy (HNE)

    Errors and Confusions in REBT

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    8/40

    Cognitive Cs

    So far, I have discussed the emotional and behavioural consequences of irrational

    beliefs. These are the most frequently discussed consequences in the REBT

    literature. However, it is also recognised (e.g. Ellis and Dryden 1997) that

    consequences of irrational beliefs can be cognitive in nature. Thus, Ellis and Dryden

    (1997) argue that the cognitive distortions often discussed by cognitive therapists

    (e.g. Beck et al. 1979; Burns 1980) are produced not just by a tendency for people to

    think distortedly, but mainly as a result of their tendency to think irrationally. REBT

    theory argues that irrational thinking is characterised by rigid and extreme thinking,

    while distorted thinking is largely inferential in nature. Thus, the REBT position is

    that when thinking is very skewed and distortedas they tend to be in the cognitive

    distortions outlined by cognitive therapiststhey stem from rigid and extreme

    thinking. Thus, if I believe that I must do well and I am faced with the prospect of

    not doing well my subsequent thinking at C will be far more distorted than if I

    were to hold a non-dogmatic preference about not doing well. Thus, holding an

    irrational belief, I may well think that I will never do well if I fail to do well on this

    occasion whereas I am far less likely to make such a distorted conclusion if I held

    the alternative rational belief. In the late 1980s, I conducted a series of experiments

    which demonstrated just this very point (e.g. Dryden et al. 1989).

    My view is that in addition to the differential effect that rational and irrational

    beliefs have on cognitive content at C they also have a differential effect on

    Table 3 The most common behaviours/action tendencies associated with the major UNEs and HNEs

    Negative emotion Dysfunctional behaviours and action tendencies

    Anxiety (UNE) Withdrawing from threat; avoiding threat; seeking reassurance

    even though not reassurable; seeking safety from threatConcern (HNE) Confronting threat; seeking reassurance when reassurable

    Depression (UNE) Prolonged withdrawal from enjoyable activities

    Sadness (HNE) Engaging with enjoyable activities after a period of

    mourning or adjustment to the loss

    Guilt (UNE) Begging for forgiveness

    Remorse (HNE) Asking, not begging, for forgiveness

    Shame (UNE) Withdrawing from others; avoiding eye contact with others

    Disappointment (HNE) Keeping in contact with others, maintaining eye contact with others

    Hurt (UNE) SulkingSorrow (HNE) Assertion and communicating with others

    Unhealthy anger (UNE) Aggression (direct and indirect)

    Healthy anger (HNE) Assertion

    Unhealthy jealousy (UNE) Prolonged suspicious questioning of the other person;

    checking on the other; restricting the other

    Healthy jealousy (HNE) Brief, open-minded questioning of the other person; neither

    checking on the other nor restricting them

    Unhealthy envy (UNE) Spoiling the others enjoyment of the desired possession

    Healthy envy (HNE) Striving to gain a similar possession for oneself if it is truly what you want

    W. Dryden

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    9/40

    cognitive processes such as attentional focus and memory. Thus, if I were to hold an

    irrational belief about failure, my attentional focus would be far more on those

    aspects of my environment that I associate with failure than it would be if I held a

    rational belief about failure. Similarly, if I held an irrational belief about failure, I

    would access more failure-related memories than I would if I thought rationallyabout failure. These hypotheses about the differential effects of rational and

    irrational beliefs on cognitive processes such as attentional focus and memory await

    empirical enquiry.

    Table 4 shows the most common cognitive consequences associated with the

    major UNEs and HNEs.

    Putting It All Together: Some Examples of the ABCsof REBT

    Having discussed the major elements of my ABC framework, let me put all this

    together by providing two examples. These examples will reflect specific examples

    of two clients target problems (i.e. the problems that have been targeted for change

    at that particular time).

    Table 4 The most common cognitive consequences of irrational beliefs associated with the major UNEs

    and HNEs

    Negative emotion Cognitive consequences of irrational beliefs

    Anxiety (UNE) Overestimating the negative consequences of the threat if it occursConcern (HNE) Realistically appraising the negative consequences of the threat if it occurs

    Depression (UNE) Hopelessness; helplessness

    Sadness (HNE) Viewing the future with hope; seeing self as able to cope with adversity

    Guilt (UNE) Assigning too much responsibility to self and too little to others

    Remorse (HNE) Assigning appropriate level of responsibility to self and to others

    Shame (UNE) Overestimating the negativity of others reactions to self and the

    extent of these reactions

    Disappointment (HNE) Realistically appraising others reactions to self and the

    extent of these reactions

    Hurt (UNE) Thinking that the other has to put things right of their own accord

    Sorrow (HNE) Not thinking that the other has to put things right of their own accord.

    Thinking that one can initiate the healing process oneself

    Unhealthy anger (UNE) Thinking that the other has malicious intent; thoughts of exacting revenge

    Healthy anger (HNE) Only thinking that the other has malicious intent when there is clear evidence

    of this; thoughts of assertion rather than of exacting revenge

    Unhealthy jealousy

    (UNE)

    Tending to see threats to ones relationship in the absence of evidence

    Healthy jealousy (HNE) Tending to see threats to ones relationship only when there

    is clear evidence that such threats exist

    Unhealthy envy (UNE) Tending to denigrate the value of the desired possession

    Healthy envy (HNE) Honestly admitting to oneself that one wants the desired possession

    for its own sake and not because the other person has it

    Errors and Confusions in REBT

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    10/40

    Sally: An ABC of Hurt

    Sally reported that she felt hurt that her friends had gone to a party and had not

    invited her. She thought that they did not care about her so she decided to withdraw

    from them and resolved never to get close to a group of women again.Her ABC is as follows:

    Situation = My friends went to a party and did not invite me to go with them

    A = They dont care for me

    iB = My friends must care for me and I cant stand it when they dont. Poor me!

    C Emotional = Hurt

    C Behavioural = Sulky withdrawal

    C Cognitive = I will never get close to a group of women again

    Here is Sallys ABC after I had worked with her on challenging and changing

    her irrational belief to its rational alternative.

    Situation = My friends went to a party and did not invite me to go with them

    A = They dont care for me

    rB = I want them to care for me, but sadly they dont have to do so. I can stand them not caring for me

    although it is difficult to do so. I am not a poor person, but a non-person in a poor situation

    C Emotional = Sorrow

    C Behavioural = Talk to my friends about how I feel and ask then why they did not ask me to the

    partyC Cognitive = I will get close to another group of women again. Just because this group of women

    dont care for me, it doesnt mean that others wont

    Sally talked to her friends about her feelings. Her friends were shocked that she

    thought they did not care for her. They explained that they did not invite her because

    they knew she was working on an essay and did not want to provide her with a

    temptation which might interfere with her work.

    Frank: An ABC of Health Anxiety

    Frank had always had a brownish mark on his arm. One day he thought that it had

    darkened and he panicked that he had a malignant melanoma and was going to die.

    He kept checking the mark and looked on skin cancer websites for reassurance

    which only served to further convince him that he had cancer. Being too scared to

    consult his doctor, he sought reassurance from his friends instead, but was not

    reassured when they assured him that it was not serious.

    Situation = Thinking that the brown mark on my arm had darkened

    A = Not knowing if the change was benign or malignant

    iB = I must know right now that the change is benign and its terrible not knowing this

    C Emotional = Anxiety

    C Behavioural = Frequently checking the mark; looking at skin cancer websites for reassurance;

    seeking reassurance from my friends that it wasnt serious; avoiding going to my doctor

    C Cognitive = It must be a malignant melanoma and I am going to die

    W. Dryden

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    11/40

    Here is Franks ABC after I had worked with him on challenging and changing

    her irrational belief to its rational alternative.

    Situation = Thinking that the brown mark on my arm had darkened

    A = Not knowing if the change was benign or malignant

    rB = I would very much like to know right now that the change is benign, but I dont have to have

    such an assurance. It is bad not knowing, but it is not terrible

    C Emotional = Concern

    C Behavioural = Refraining from checking the mark; not looking at skin cancer websites for

    reassurance; not seeking reassurance from my friends that it wasnt serious; consulting my doctor

    C Cognitive = It might be a malignant melanoma, but it is more likely to be a benign change. It is very

    unlikely that I am going to die

    On consulting his doctor, Frank was reassured that the change was minor and notindicative of anything sinister.

    Having outlined the yardstick against which textbook portrayals presentations of

    the ABCs of REBT can be judged, let me discuss how the present study was

    conceptualised.

    Method

    Materials

    In this study, I randomly selected twenty textbooks on counseling and psychother-

    apy published in North America and Britain in a 10-year period between 1998 and

    2008 that had a chapter or major section on REBT. There are two types of textbooks

    in the field of counseling and psychotherapy: i) edited books containing chapters

    written by experts on the presented therapy approaches and ii) authored books

    where all such approaches are written by one or more authors. In my view, there are

    advantages and disadvantages of both types of book. The main advantage of the

    edited text is that each approach is written by an expert, while its main disadvantage

    is lack of consistency of writing style. The main advantage of the authored text is

    consistency of writing style and subject perspective, while its main disadvantage is

    variability of subject knowledge and possible subject bias.

    Perhaps the major purpose of a textbook in the counseling and therapy field,

    irrespective of whether it is authored or edited, is to provide readers with accurate

    information on the subject covered. While it may be assumed that experts on REBT

    selected to contribute to edited texts would portray REBT accurately (an assumption

    that needs, however, to be tested), I decided to select authored rather than edited

    texts for investigation for two reasons. First, authored textbooks on counseling and

    therapy are more common than edited texts and second, since authors of such texts

    are not likely not to be REBT experts, the chances for errors and confusions aboutthe ABCs of REBT are increased.

    Errors and Confusions in REBT

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    12/40

    Procedure

    I read each chapter and/or major section on REBT as well as every other reference

    that was made to this therapy approach in the twenty textbooks and detailed major

    errors and confusions in the following five categories: (1) The name of the therapy;(2) General issues related to the role of cognition in REBT; (3) The nature of A;

    (4) The nature of B; (5) The nature of C.

    While errors and confusions overlap in the sense that any error about REBT that

    an author makes will result, ultimately, in the reader having an incorrect and

    confused picture of that aspect of the approach, I differentiate an error and a

    confusion in the following way. An error occurs when the author provides incorrect

    information about REBT and does so clearly. On the other hand, a confusion occurs

    when it is not clear what the author meant or there is sufficient ambiguity that the

    reader will be unclear about some key aspect of the ABC framework. Put anotherway, an error occurs when an REBT expert says: This is wrong and a confusion

    occurs when that same expert says: I am not sure what the authors is saying on this

    point or the reader may be misled on this point.

    Results

    The Name of the Therapy

    REBT has undergone three major name changes since its inception. Originallyknown as Rational Therapy (RT), Ellis changed its name to Rational-Emotive

    Therapy (RET) in 1961 and to Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT) in

    1993. Since the textbooks I appraised in this study were published between 1998

    and 2008, they could all be expected to get the name of the therapy correct, whether

    present or past. Surprisingly, this was not the case.

    I discovered twenty incorrect instances of the name of the therapy covering seven

    incorrect names as shown below:

    Rational-emotive behavior therapyError: inclusion of hyphen (n = 6)

    Rational emotive therapyError: omission of behavior or old term usedwithout the hyphen (n = 4)

    Rational emotive behavioral therapyError: use of behavioral not behav-

    ior (n = 4)

    Rational-emotive therapyError: old term used or if referring to the current

    name, omission of behavior and inclusion of hyphen (n = 3)

    Rational emotive behavioral therapiesErrors: use of behavioral not

    behavior and use of therapies not therapy (n = 1)

    Rational-emotive behavioral therapyErrors: inclusion of hyphen and use of

    behavioral not behavior (n = 1) Rational behavioral emotive therapyErrors: use of behavioral not behav-

    ior and behavioral placed before emotive (n = 1)

    Thirteen of the twenty authors of the textbooks surveyed made name errors (see

    Table 6). While eleven of these authors each made one such error and one author (i.e.

    W. Dryden

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    13/40

    Ivey et al. 2007: 227, 230) made two different errors, one other author (i.e. Mobley

    2005) made six errors covering four different incorrect names of the therapy. Thus,

    Mobley (2005) called the therapy Rational emotive therapy (p. ii), Rational

    emotive behavioral therapies (p. 91), Rational behavioural emotive therapy

    (p. 145) and on three different occasions called it Rational emotive behaviouraltherapy (pp. 11, 87, 163). Indeed, this author never called REBT by its correct name!

    General Issues

    Before I consider how each element of the ABCs are portrayed in the selected

    textbooks, let me consider what these authors have to say about REBTs views on

    the relationship between B and C.

    The Relationship Between B and C

    REBT has both a general model about the relationship between B and C and a

    specific model about this relationship. The general model states that C is largely

    determined by cognitive factors at B and this is synonymous with the general

    viewpoint outlined by many approaches within the CBT tradition. The specific

    model states that C is largely determined by beliefs at B and this is a

    distinctive feature of REBT (Dryden 2009).

    If textbook authors are to portray correctly REBTs views on this issue, they

    need, at the very least to describe with accuracy the specific model. If they outlinethe general model, they need also to outline the specific model and make clear that

    while the former shows REBTs allegiance to the CBT tradition, the latter is its

    distinctive view.

    Consequently, it would be an error for authors to state only the general model and

    imply that this is REBTs position on the relationship between B and C. I

    found ten such errors in the textbooks studied. Seven of these made the point that

    thoughts largely determine responses at C. A typical example of this is as

    follows: A primary focus of this approach is teaching clients to realise that feelings

    are derived from thoughts not events (Gladding 2005: 148). In addition, there were

    three errors that stated that these thoughts are interpretative in nature (e.g. A is the

    objective event, B is the persons interpretation of the event and C is the persons

    emotional or behavioural reaction found in Todd and Bohart 2006: 294).

    Six of the twenty authors made this error with four authors each making one

    error, one author making two errors (Archer and McCarthy 2007: 271-twice) and

    one author making four such errors (Gladding 2005: 142-twice, 148-twice)

    The Relationship Between B and Disturbed Responses at C

    The REBT view of the relationship between B and psychological disturbance at

    C is quite clear. It is that such disturbance is underpinned or largely determined

    by irrational beliefs at C. Despite this being clear in the REBT literature, I found

    the following eleven errors/confusions in the textbooks surveyed. Disturbance is

    largely determined by:

    Errors and Confusions in REBT

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    14/40

    thoughts (Archer and McCarthy 2007: 271)

    whatever you are telling yourself inside your head (Kottler 2002: 111)

    views4 (Ford and Urban 1998: 387)

    interpretations (e.g. the way they interpret an event that happens in a way that

    involves them personallyMobley 2005: 149 and Corey 2005: 271-2)

    These five instances may be read as any thoughts, views, personally relevant

    interpretations and what you are telling yourself, can lead to psychological

    disturbance. The following six instances, although problematic do, at least, indicate

    the presence of some negative or irrational cognitive process:

    self-defeating sentences (Corey 2005: 275)

    self-talk or catastrophising5 (Hough 2002: 115)

    irrational thinking (Hough 2002: 115) and irrational thoughts6 (Ivey et al.

    2007: 253) negative thinking and inferences (Hough 2002: 126127)

    irrational statements7 (Ivey et al. 2007: 234)

    These eleven citations can be seen as both errors and confusions for the reader.

    They are incorrect in that none explicitly mentions the term beliefs and they may

    confuse the reader who may think of them as beliefs. As can be seen from the above,

    seven of the twenty authors make such errors/confusions with five each making one,

    two making two (i.e. Corey 2005 and Ivey et al. 2007) and one making three (i.e.

    Hough 2002).

    The Causation Error

    REBT theorists are careful to avoid saying that B causes C. Rather, the

    following terms are preferred: B largely determines C; B underpins C or

    B is at the core of C. From the outset, Ellis (1958) made clear that A, B

    and C are inter-related rather than separate processes and thus B cannot cause

    C since this would imply that they were separate factors. This important point

    needs to be reflected in the language employed by authors of counselling and

    psychotherapy textbooks if the REBT position is to be accurately portrayed.Causation language, therefore, is an error and violates the inter-relatedness

    principle.

    I found eleven instances where authors of counselling and psychotherapy

    textbooks stated that B causes C and in ten of these eleven instances B was

    deemed to cause emotional problems. Two of these authors explicitly and

    4 Epictetus, who is often cited by REBT theorists, claimed that people are disturbed not by things, but by

    their views of things. The REBT version of this is: People are disturbed not by things; rather they disturb

    themselves by the irrational beliefs that they hold about things.5 While catastrophising is the old-fashioned term for the irrational belief known as awfulising, its

    pairing with self-talk here is confusing for the reader.6 It is the words thinking and thoughts that is the problem here, not the word irrational. The first

    two words are too imprecise.7 It is the word statements here that is again the problem not the word irrational. It is too imprecise.

    W. Dryden

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    15/40

    incorrectly stated that this was Elliss position, albeit without citing any published

    evidence (Corey 2005: 274; Todd and Bohart 2006: 294).

    Nine of the twenty authors made the causation error in their textbooks with seven

    authors each making one error and two authors making two respectively (i.e. Archer

    and McCarthy 2007: 271-twice and Hough 2002: 121, 127-7).

    A

    In REBT, A stands for activating event. The term activating event is

    problematic and confusing. It can mean the situation in which the person

    experienced his disturbed feelings or it can be the precise part of the situation to

    which the person responds with disturbance (sometimes called the critical A),

    mediated of course by the persons irrational beliefs. In the framework that I

    presented in the first part of this paper, A stands for the latter and this is itsmodern usage, but authors of textbooks can be forgiven for thinking that it is the

    former. Perhaps the major difference between the objective A and the critical

    A is that the latter is inferential in nature, while the former is not.

    In describing A without direct reference to an example, seven out of the

    twenty authors on one occasion each refer to A as an objective or factual event

    devoid of inferential meaning. For example, Seligman (2006: 320) describes an

    activating event as the external and objective source of discomfort and Ivey

    et al. (2007: 237) say that A represents the objective facts, events, behaviors

    that an individual encounters.When authors refer to A as an objective situation and provide an example, they

    often reveal the critical A in the content of what they put under B in this example.

    Here is an example of what I mean to be found in Sommers-Flanagan and

    Sommers Flanagan (2004: 265):

    Jems activating event: His wife is late for dinner

    Jems belief: His wife doesnt love or respect him any more

    She is probably having an affair

    I found ten instances where the critical A was revealed under B as described

    above. Four authors did so on one occasion each and three authors did so on two

    occasions each (Archer and McCarthy 2007: 272-twice; Fall et al. 2004: 342, 351;

    Hough 2002: 116, 119).

    Making a clearer distinction between the situation in which an episode of

    disturbance occurs (which I refer to as Situation in the framework that I

    introduced earlier) and what the person is most disturbed about (which I refer to as

    A in my framework) will help authors of textbooks have a clear understanding of

    the two ways in which the term activating event is used in the REBT literature

    and thus minimise errors about A.

    B

    Authors in the selected sample made most errors and confused readers most often

    when dealing with beliefs at B and particularly when covering irrational beliefs.

    Errors and Confusions in REBT

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    16/40

    Irrational Beliefs

    In REBT theory irrational beliefs are either rigid (e.g. demands) or extreme (i.e.

    awfulising beliefs, discomfort intolerance beliefsalso known as low frustration

    tolerance beliefsor depreciation beliefs).

    Irrational Belief Errors The authors in the present sample made five types of error

    when discussing irrational beliefs.

    1. Irrational beliefs are really inferences at A. One of the most frequent errors

    that new trainees make when they first learn REBT is to identify distorted inferences

    when they think that they are identifying irrational beliefs. I found fifteen examples

    of this type of error. Here are some examples of inferences that authors state are

    irrational beliefs:

    Charles thinking thatthe divorce was his fault (Archer and McCarthy 2007:

    272)

    Nick doesnt love me anymore (Day 2008: 376)

    The employee who believes the boss is angry with him because she did not say

    hello to him in the hall (Fall et al. 2004: 339)

    The essay must be awful.8 She doesnt even feel its worth mentioning. She

    probably thinks Im not up to much (Hough 2002: 116)

    The reason I have nothing is that the rich have taken it all (Ivey et al. 2007:

    234)

    Life isnt fair (Kottler 2002: 109) My boss must9 be out to get me (Nystul 2006: 245)

    His wife doesnt love or respect him anymore. She is probably having an

    affair (Sommers-Flanagan and Sommers Flanagan 2004: 265)

    Half of the sample of authors (n = 10) made this irrational belief as inference

    error with seven making it once, one making it twice (Day 2008: 376-twice) and two

    making it on three occasions (Archer and McCarthy 2007: 272, 276, 277) and

    Kottler 109, 111, 113).

    2. Irrational beliefs are really cognitive consequences at C. One of the more

    recent developments in REBT theory has been to refer to highly distorted inferences

    that are skewed to the negative and appear to stem from irrational beliefs as

    cognitive consequences at C. An example of the difference between an inference

    at A and an inferential cognitive consequence at C is shown on p. 10 of this

    article where the person infers at A: My friends dont care for me and

    concludes at C: I will never get close to a group of women again. Typical

    examples of cognitive consequences are black and white thinking and making

    always and never statements.

    8 The inclusion of the words must and awful here do not indicate the presence of an irrational belief.

    The meaning seems to be that the person thinks that the essay is very bad.9 Again the presence of the word must does not indicate the presence of an irrational belief. The

    person appears to have a strong conviction that his inference that his boss is out to get him is correct.

    W. Dryden

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    17/40

    Ellis was not consistent in his placing of what I am calling cognitive

    consequences in the ABC framework. Sometimes he stated clearly that they

    stemmed from irrational beliefs and, by definition, are cognitive consequences at

    C (e.g. Ellis and Dryden 1997), while at other times he saw them as irrational

    beliefs at B (e.g. in Ellis and MacClaren 1998). Consequently, authors makingthe irrational belief as cognitive consequence may well be reflecting the

    confusion that appears in the REBT literature on this point.

    Having said that, I found sixteen instances where authors had described or listed

    cognitive consequences as irrational beliefs of which three had made this error in

    general terms and in thirteen other instances, authors demonstrated this error in an

    example.

    From a general perspective, Kottler (2002: 115) argued that overpersonalizing

    (sic), disqualifying and perfectionism are examples of derivative irrational beliefs

    from a basic must, when they are more appropriately viewed as cognitiveconsequences. James and Gilliland (2003: 239) listed a number of cognitive

    distortions such as overgeneralizing and all-or-none thinking and called them hot

    cognitions with which REBT is mainly concerned.10 Finally, Ivey et al. (2007:

    234) stated that the word never is a useful indicator of irrational thinking when it

    is a cognitive consequence of such thinking.

    The following are a few of the thirteen examples where this error was

    demonstrated:

    I will never make my mother happy (Archer and McCarthy 2007: 276)

    No one will ever love me again (Fall et al. 2004: 342) This means that I will never be a good therapist (Kottler 2002: 111)

    Ill always do poorly in job interviews (Nelson-Jones 2006: 322)

    No-one will ever respect me (Sharf 2008: 307)

    Half of the sample (n = 10) committed the irrational belief as cognitive

    consequence error with seven authors demonstrating this error on one occasion,

    three authors demonstrating it twice (Archer and McCarthy 2007: 276, 277); James

    and Gilliland 2003: 239, 246; Kottler, 2002: 111, 115) and one demonstrating it

    three times (Fall et al. 2004: 341, 342, 351).

    3. Failing to distinguish between role evaluation and self-evaluation. In REBT

    theory, a distinction is made between role-depreciation and self-depreciation. The

    latter is an irrational belief, the former is not. Thus, it is possible for someone to say

    I am a poor father and for him to accept rather than to depreciate himself.

    Consequently, it is an error to regard role depreciation to be an irrational belief. I

    found five instances of this error with two authors responsible for the five errors.

    Archer and McCarthy (2007) made four of these errors:

    Charles thinking that he was an inadequate husband (p. 272)

    I was never a good enough husband (p. 274) I am not a good enough daughter (p. 276)

    Im not a real man unless I have a good body (p. 277)

    10 Since REBT is mainly concerned with irrational beliefs, readers could easily infer that these hot

    cognitions are, in fact, irrational beliefs.

    Errors and Confusions in REBT

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    18/40

    The other error was made by Seligman (2006: 317):

    I am a terrible mother Clients often imply self-depreciation when they

    engage in role-depreciation, but in textbooks, authors need to make this point

    clear to readers and clarify this distinction as, interestingly enough, Archer andMcCarthy (2007: 272) did themselves:

    .she is not a perfect daughter (woman) because she doesnt have a family/

    children that she is not a worthwhile person because of this (sic)

    4. Conditional musts as irrational beliefs. Just because the word must appears

    in a sentence does not necessarily indicate the presence of an irrational belief. Thus,

    some musts are conditional whereas the musts that underpin disturbance are

    unconditional. I found only one instance of this error:

    I must have a wife to be happy (Archer and McCarthy 2007: 274)

    5. Catastrophising as irrational belief. REBT theory makes clear distinctions

    between concepts that appear, at first sight to be synonymous or similar, but are not.

    Thus, awfulising is different from catastrophising in that in REBT, the former is seen

    as an irrational belief and the latter as a cognitive consequence from an irrational

    belief. Thus, a person can believe: It is awful that this catastrophe occurred or It

    is very bad that this catastrophe occurred, but not awful. The latter belief is deemed

    to help the person to process the catastrophe, grieve and move on while the latter

    belief interferes with this process. I do concede that this is an understandable error for

    authors to make since it is not prominent in the REBT literature, but it is an errornonetheless and it occurred on two separate occasions in the textbooks surveyed. For

    example, Hough (2002: 121) says: The urge to catastrophise about the inevitable is

    a fundamental cause of unhappiness in clients

    James and Gilliland (2003: 238) compound this error by making it clear in the

    following example of awfulising that catastrophic is worse than awful whereas,

    the reverse is the case in REBT theory: I have no job. Im 45 years old. I cant get

    another job. My family will starve. This is not just awful. It is catastrophic.

    6. Failing to distinguish between cant tolerate and will not tolerate. In

    REBT, there is an important difference between cant tolerate (which is an irrational

    belief and will not tolerate (which appears to be a determination not to put up with

    something passively). In the latter, the person often believes that he can tolerate the

    adversity, but he is determined not to do so. I found one instance of this error. James

    and Gilliland (2003: 238) say: I-cant-stand-it-itis: I will not tolerate them firing me.

    I am totally destroyed by this and I will get even if its the last thing I do.

    Irrational Belief Confusions In this section, I will present eight ways (n = 18) in

    which authors confused readers about irrational beliefs together with four other

    instances where the authors meaning was not clear and therefore these instances of

    confusion were difficult to categorise.

    1. Confusion about the nature of an irrational belief. I found six instances where

    readers would be confused about the nature of an irrational belief after reading the

    author on this point. I will discuss these in turn:

    W. Dryden

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    19/40

    I must have a date for New Years by tomorrow or I will be the laughing stock

    of the community (Seligman, 2006: 317). Here it is not clear if the demand is

    an irrational belief with the statement I will be the laughing stock of the

    community representing the cognitive consequence of this demand or whether

    it is a conditional must as in in order for me to prevent myself from becomingthe laughing stock of the community, I must have a date for New Years by

    tomorrow.

    Gladding (2005: 142) says: Another goal of REBT is to help people change

    self-defeating habits of thought or behavior. He then presents the ABC

    model where B stands for thoughts (1. Positive, 2. Negative, 3. Neutral and 4.

    Mixed). Since Gladding is referring to self-defeating thought, the reader will end

    up thoroughly confused concerning what constitutes such thought, leaving aside

    the more obvious point that Gladding does not mention beliefs, let alone

    irrational beliefs. McLeod (2003: 132) mentions the following belief which he says is irrational

    because it is exaggerated and overstated: I must have love or approval from all

    the significant people in my life. McLeod confuses the reader who will not

    know whether this belief is irrational because of the must or because of the

    all or both. If McLeod had provided the standard criteria for a demand being

    irrational (i.e. it is rigid, false, illogical and helpful) rather than being

    exaggerated and overstated the reader would be clearer, particularly if the

    rigid criterion was emphasized.

    I would never want to bring a child into this insane and awful world. Thisstatement by Nystul (2006: 244) confuses the reader because it appears that the

    inclusion of the word awful here makes the belief an irrational awfulising

    belief. This may not the case. The meaning of the word awful here is

    ambiguous. It could mean very bad. A clear example of an awfulising belief

    would be as follows: I would never to bring a child into this insane world. It

    would be awful if I did so.

    Perhaps the most confusing example of an author presenting an irrational belief

    was that found in Day (2008: 376). She begins by incorrectly stating that belief

    at B is: The often spontaneous, often irrational, thought within the clients

    mind in response to A. She goes on to give the following example of this: Sera

    thinks: Nick knows that I want to watch our favourite television series together

    in fifteen minutes, and that cigar will take all night to finish! He hates to smoke

    just half, and he will never come in on time to watch our show. He is so

    inconsiderate. I just cant stand it. He always does things like this to ruin our

    time together.

    The only statement here that is an irrational belief is: I just cant stand it. The

    other statements seem to be descriptive and inferential in nature. But how is the

    reader to know which is the irrational belief? Day provides no help here.

    Day (2008: 376) goes on to provide disputing interventions designed to help

    Seras therapist to ask questions about the B that came between A and C.

    Perhaps these will help the reader identify Seras irrational belief. Sadly, these

    questions only deepen the readers confusion.

    Errors and Confusions in REBT

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    20/40

    Does Nick know what time it is?

    Does he want to smoke that particular whole cigar?

    Does he do considerate things, ever?

    Could there be another reason for his choice of cigar. Other than to ruin her

    evening? How bad could it really be if they missed watching part of their show

    together?

    Isnt it true that she can stand an incident like this, even if he is ignoring

    her?

    Only the last question is targeted at an irrational belief. The other questions are

    primarily targeted at inferences at A. Strikingly, the one thing that Day does not

    teach her readers is the centrality of the demand in disturbance, an issue I will

    discuss in greater detail later.

    Five authors confused their readers about the nature of irrational beliefs with four

    doing so on one occasion each and with one doing so on two occasions (Day, 2008:

    376-twice).

    2. Confusion between an irrational belief and an inference. I found two instances

    where an author indicated that an inference might be an irrational belief, but this

    was not clear.

    Fall et al. (2004: 339) state that teaching people how to identify irrational

    thinking and how to produce rational thinking is the primary thrust of REBT.

    However, they provide three examples of inferences determining emotional andbehavioural responses (e.g. the adolescent girl who believes He hates me

    when her boyfriend does not call her one evening). Readers are thus given two

    different views of the determinants of emotional and behavioural responses and

    will end up being confused.

    Todd and Bohart (2006: 295) claim that the following is an irrational need: I

    deserve to be well liked. This is confusing since while deservingness is often

    taken as an irrational belief, this statement is best regarded as an inference. The

    irrational belief would be: Since I deserve to be well liked, I must get what I

    deserve.

    3. Confusion between an irrational belief and a cognitive consequence. I found

    four instances where an author indicated that a cognitive consequence might be an

    irrational belief or vice versa, but this was also not clear.

    McLeod (2002: 133) says that the irrational belief leads to catastrophizing.

    Since it is not clear whether McLeod regards catastrophizing as a derivative

    irrational belief or a cognitive consequence, the reader will be confused.

    Sharf (2008: 307) lists the following irrational belief: I must get an A on my

    exam, or I will be a worthless person, and no-one will ever respect me. This is

    confusing because the reader will think that the statement and no-one will

    ever respect me is part of the irrational belief, not a cognitive consequence of it.

    Parrott (2003: 313) says: Once clients begin to identify their irrational beliefs,

    the counselor can help group them into themes and goes on to list the fol-

    lowing: all-or-nothing thinking, mind reading, over-generalization, discounting,

    W. Dryden

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    21/40

    magnification, emotional reasoning and self-blame. Apart from the latter, all the

    above are examples of cognitive consequences of irrational beliefs (Ellis and

    Dryden 1997). However, by recommending that counsellors group clients

    irrational beliefs into themes, it is not clear what the author is saying. Thus

    readers could easily conclude that Parrot is saying that they are irrational beliefs. Colledge (2002: 231) states that Individuals usually make over-generalised and

    unrealistic inferences in response to their absolutistic demands. For example,

    awfulising, terribleising, horribleising; I cant stand it-itis; a feeling of

    worthlessness and self-hatred and a prediction of continuous failure. While it

    is correct that distorted inferences stem from absolutistic demands and are thus

    best regarded as cognitive consequences of these demands, Colledge implies that

    the three major derivatives are over-generalised and unrealistic inferences and

    not derivative irrational beliefs which will be confusing for the reader.

    4. Confusion between an irrational belief and a rational belief. I found two

    instances where an author confused an irrational belief with a rational belief.

    Colledge (2002: 233) notes that Bs and Cs interact as follows. Preferential

    Beliefs (Bs) lead to consequences (Cs) that reciprocally influence Bs. So if

    people are rejected and consequently feel depressed they avoid anyone who may

    reject them because they have constructed the belief that he is an idiot and isnt

    worth bothering with. Leaving aside the point that it is not clear who holds the

    belief outlined (i.e. he is an idiot and isnt worth bothering with), the author

    gives an example that shows the impact of an irrational belief not a rationalbelief that his opening point about preferential beliefs suggests. Thus, the reader

    is likely to be confused on both points.

    Hough (2002: 102) says the following in considering the need for approval:

    Besides people who want to be loved and approved of at all times set

    impossibly high standards for themselves, since such an ambition requires

    continual hard work and expenditure of energy. Insecurity is also generated by

    this constant need for approval, and ingratiating behaviour is likely to follow.

    Hough confuses the reader by seeming to equate wants with needs and also by

    implying that desires lead to disturbed consequences.

    5. Confusion between irrational beliefs. I found one instance where the author

    confuses the reader by making a point about one irrational belief (a rigid belief) and

    then by exemplifying this point with reference to a different irrational belief (an

    awfulising belief). Thus, Corey (2005: 280) says: Clients learn that musts,

    oughts, and shoulds can be replaced by preferences. Instead of saying It would

    be absolutely awful if, they learn to say It would be inconvenient if

    6. Confusion about what is meant by a distorted evaluation. Archer and

    McCarthy (2007: 271) say the following: we have this basic idea that human

    evaluation of events is often distorted and that this distortion causes many of ourproblems and difficulties. Readers will be confused in that they will not know what

    is meant by distorted evaluation. Also, the word distorted is more frequently

    used to describe inferences rather than evaluations (which when extreme are a

    cornerstone of derivative irrational beliefs). This will also add to readers confusion.

    Errors and Confusions in REBT

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    22/40

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    23/40

    McLeod (2003; 136) lists a number of irrational beliefs and then says: The

    belief statements used in RET [sic] reflect the operation of a number of distorted

    cognitive processes. For example, over-generalisation is present if the client

    believes that he or she needs to be loved at all times. This is confusing for two

    reasons. First, McLeod considers the need to be loved to reflect the distortedcognitive process of over-generalisation. In REBT theory this need reflects the

    cognitive operation of rigidity and leads to the distorted cognitive consequence of

    over-generalisation. Second, it is not clear what McLeod regards as distorted (his

    term) in the belief, he needs to be loved at all times. Is it the need to be loved

    component or the at all times component? In REBT, it is the need component

    since a person can believe I want to be loved at all times and this would not be

    irrational as long as the person also believes but I dont have to be.

    Conversely, if the person believes that he needs to be loved by one person, then in

    REBT theory this would be regarded as an irrational belief. McLeod fails to helpreaders understand these subtle but important distinctions made in REBT theory.

    The final instance of how an author can baffle readers concerning the nature of

    irrational beliefs demonstrates just how confused an author can be in their own

    minds about this topic. Thus, Ivey et al. (2007: 234) say the following: In

    counseling and psychotherapy, clients will frequently use such irrational

    statements as12

    1. If I dont pass the course, it is the end of the world

    2. Because my parents have been cruel to me as a child, there is nothing I can

    do now to help myself3. Since the economy is lacking jobs, I cant have a good life

    4. If I cant get a scholarship, all is ended

    5. The reason I have nothing is that the rich have taken it all

    All of these statements at one level are true, but all of them represent helpless

    thinking, a common end result of irrational thought.

    Iveys statement is very confusing for the following reasons:

    (a) It is not clear if the authors intend these statements to be examples of irrational

    beliefs as the meaning of the term irrational statements here is not defined(b) Only statement 1 above is an example of an irrational (awfulising) belief.

    Statements 2 and 3 seem to be cognitive consequences of irrational beliefs,

    statement 4 is confusing (it may be an awfulising belief or it may be a

    cognitive consequence of an irrational belief) and statement 5 is an inference.

    Irrational Beliefs: Theory not Emphasised I found two major areas where REBT

    theory was not emphasised in the discussion of irrational beliefs.

    i. Rigid beliefs. Perhaps the most distinctive feature of REBT theory is the central

    role given to rigid beliefs i.e. demands, musts, absolute should, etc. in

    accounting for psychological disturbance (Dryden 2009). Thus, it is important

    12 I have inserted numbers in this quote to aid comprehension.

    Errors and Confusions in REBT

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    24/40

    for authors of textbooks to make clear the core place of rigid beliefs in such

    disturbance. It is not sufficient, therefore, for authors to make reference to

    derivative irrational beliefs without, at the very least, also mentioning rigid

    beliefs.

    Six authors mentioned derivative irrational beliefs without mentioning rigid

    beliefs on twelve occasions in total.

    1. Archer and McCarthy (2007: 270) state that discomfort disturbances basically

    involve beliefs that lead to an inability to deal with frustration when things in life

    dont go as one wants or expects. Things become terrible and awful when one is

    treated unfairly or blocked in some avenue of life. Here, the authors mention

    awfulising, but omit any mention of rigid beliefs which according to Ellis (1994) are

    the breeding ground for the three other derivative irrational beliefs including

    awfulising. Archer and McCarthy (2007: 272-twice) give two examples where no

    mention is made of a rigid belief, but where reference is made to other derivative

    irrational beliefs.

    Charles thinking that he was an inadequate husband, the divorce was his fault

    and that his has ruined his life forever.

    In the following example, there are two different irrational beliefs provided and

    the demand is present in only the second: Sharons irrational beliefs that she is

    not a perfect daughter (woman) because she doesnt have a family/children that

    she is not a worthwhile person because of this,13 and that her mother must

    approve of the lifestyle choices she (Sharon) makes.

    14

    2. Hough (2002: 115) say that According to the [ABC] model, it is not what

    happens at point A which causes an individual to experience disturbance or

    discomfort; on the contrary, it is the individuals own self-talk or catastrophising

    which inflicts the damage, and this place at point B. Once again the author

    mentions catastrophising,15 but omits any reference to rigid beliefs. Hough

    (2002: 117, 119) goes on to give two examples where no mention is made of a rigid

    belief, but where reference is made to other derivative irrational beliefs.

    Paul believes Its awful that I failed the test and Im useless for having failed

    (Hough 2002: 117).

    There will be a lot of students there. Many have studied the Romantic poets

    before. They are likely to know a lot about the subject. They may not appreciate

    what I have planned to say to them. Some might even leave the lecture hall in

    boredom. My God, it would be awful. I would just dieit would be terrible

    (Hough 2002: 119).

    3. James and Gilliland (2003: 263) say: Rational-emotive therapy theory

    suggests that emotional disturbance may be explained by ABC theory. This theory

    states that when an unpleasant event (activating event) occurs at point A, an

    13 This is the first irrational belief.14 This is the second irrational belief.15 See footnote 5 for the difference between awfulising and catastrophising.

    W. Dryden

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    25/40

    individual may react in two ways. One is to conclude that this event is unfortunate

    and disadvantageous (a rational belief).orthat circumstances are terrible or

    even catastrophic and therefore should not exist (irrational belief). James and

    Gilliland (2003: 235) provide one example where no mention is made of a rigid

    belief, but where reference is made to other derivative irrational beliefs, in this case,awfulising again and other-depreciation.

    In reality, however, Dr. James tells himself, Here is another example of

    stupidity and incompetency insinuated on me, and it is a major catastrophe that

    this jerk is now making me late for my therapy session.

    4. Day (2008) give two examples where no mention is made of a rigid belief, but

    where reference is made to other derivative irrational beliefs, self-depreciation in

    the first example and discomfort intolerance in the second.

    See I am a loser. No one will ever want a long-term relationship with me (Day

    2008: 372).

    Nick knows that I want to watch our favorite television series together in

    15 min, and that cigar will take all night to finish! He hates to smoke just half,

    and he will never come in on time to watch our show. He is so inconsiderate, I

    just cant stand it.16 He always does things like this to ruin our time together.

    (Day 2008: 376).

    5. Fall et al. (2004: 342) give one example where no mention is made of a rigid

    belief, but where mention is made of all three derivative irrational beliefs. That is horrible! I must be a complete loser. No one will ever love me again! I

    am all alone, unloveable, and I cant live without her.

    6. Kottler (2002: 111) give one example where no mention is made of a rigid

    belief, but where mention is made self-depreciation.

    I cant believe I blew this assignment completely. Im such an idiot!

    ii. Unqualified shoulds. As discussed above, REBT emphasises the rigid nature ofirrational beliefs. In this context, the word should is sometimes referred to by

    the authors of counselling and psychotherapy textbooks, but without a qualifier

    (such as absolutely should) and as such REBT theory is not brought to the

    fore. As I have showed elsewhere, REBT theory acknowledged that the word

    should has several different meanings and that only the absolute should is

    deemed to be irrational in REBT (Dryden 2010).

    Six of the selected sample of twenty authors did this, five on one occasion each

    (Archer and McCarthy 2007: 270; Fall et al. 2004: 341; James and Gilliland, 2003:

    263); Kottler 2002: 111; and Seligman (2006: 317) and one Hough (2002) on three

    occasions. Thus Hough refers to demands as unqualified shoulds in the following

    examples:

    16 I just cant stand it is the discomfort intolerance belief in this example.

    Errors and Confusions in REBT

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    26/40

    People should always like me (Hough 2002: 127)

    People should always believe me (Hough 2002: 127)

    Things should always be the way I want them to be (Hough 2002: 137).

    Rational Beliefs

    In REBT theory rational beliefs are either flexible (e.g. non-dogmatic preferences)

    or non-extreme (i.e. non-awfulising beliefs, discomfort tolerance beliefsalso

    known as high frustration tolerance beliefsor depreciation beliefs).

    Rational Belief Errors The authors in the present sample made five types of error

    when discussing rational beliefs.

    1. Rational beliefs are really inferences at A. I mentioned earlier that one of

    the most frequent errors that new trainees make when they first learn REBT is to

    identify distorted inferences when they think that they are identifying irrational

    beliefs. This is also the case when they come to formulate rational belief

    alternatives. I found fifteen instances of this error where rational beliefs are really

    inferences: one as a general point and fourteen in examples. In general, Fall et al.

    (2004: 343) state that: When faced with situations they find difficult, rational

    people interpret the events as hurtful or disappointing but not catastrophic.

    Here are some of the fourteen examples of inferences that authors state are

    rational beliefs:

    I will be a good daughter and do what I can for my mother, but I am not

    responsible for her happiness (Archer and McCarthy 2007: 277)

    I havent found the right way to choose relationships yet (Day 2008: 372)

    My boss seems to have concerns about my report (Nystul 2006: 245)

    My wifes behaviour is independent of my own success and accomplishments

    (Parrott 2003: 315)

    I believe that my supervisor is not really familiar with my work and so is not

    giving me the praise I would like (Seligman 2006: 318).

    Nine of the sample of authors made this rational belief as inference error withfive making it once, three making it on two occasions (Archer and McCarthy 2007:

    277-twice; Mobley 2005: 150-1-twice and Seligman, 2006: 318-twice), and one

    making it on four occasions (Day 2008: 372, 376-three times).

    2. Rational beliefs are really cognitive consequences at C. I mentioned earlier

    in this paper that a recent development of REBT is the emphasis placed on the

    cognitive consequences of beliefs. When these beliefs are irrational, their cognitive

    consequences tend to be highly distorted and skewed to the negative and when they

    are rational, their cognitive consequences are realistic and balanced. I found just one

    instance when an author put forward a rational belief that was really a cognitiveconsequence. Thus, Todd and Bohart (2006: 297) put forward the rational belief: I

    would prefer not to act that way. I will try to do better next time. The latter

    statement forms part of the rational belief, but is really a cognitive consequence of

    that belief.

    W. Dryden

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    27/40

    3. Rational beliefs are really role evaluations. In the section on irrational belief

    errors, I mentioned that it was an error to state that a role evaluation was an

    irrational belief. I found one instance where an author stated that a role evaluation

    was a rational belief: I will be a good daughter and do what I can for my

    mother (Archer and McCarthy (2007: 277).4. Rational beliefs are really feeling statements. On three occasions, McLeod

    (2002) gives examples of rational beliefs which seem to be feeling statements rather

    than beliefs. Thus:

    I enjoy being loved by others. (McLeod 2003: 133)

    I feel most secure when the majority of the people in my life care about me.

    (McLeod 2003: 133)

    I enjoy the feeling of being loved and accepted by another person, and if this is

    not available to me I can sometimes feel unhappy. (McLeod 2002: 136)

    5. Rational beliefs lessen disturbance. REBT theory states that rational beliefs

    are qualitatively, rather than quantitatively different from irrational beliefs. As a

    result, rational beliefs do not lessen disturbance; rather they help people to have a

    healthily negative, but undisturbed set of responses to lifes adversities (Dryden

    2009; Ellis 1994). Hough (2002) makes this error on three separate occasions. Thus:

    .and to substitute rational and less disturbing beliefs in their place (Hough

    2002: 139).

    Homework assignments.are designed to correct the clients irrational beliefs

    and to help him replace them with more realistic and less disturbing language orself-talk (Hough 2002: 128).

    Eleanor was also asked by the counsellor to practise changing her internal

    language so that her thinking would become less rigid and self-sabotaging

    (Hough 2002: 137).

    Rational Belief Confusions In this section, there were four instances where, in my

    view, readers would have been confused about the nature of a rational belief after

    reading what three of the samples authors had to say on this topic. In giving a rational belief alternative to the following belief: I must be

    approved or accepted by people I find important, James and Gilliland ( 2003:

    254) provide the following: While it would be grand if everyone thought I was

    great, thats not going to happen, particularly if Im me. And if I am me and

    people like me, Ill know they really like me for what I really am and not for

    what I pretend to be. Furthermore, Im only anxious about this. It isnt

    dangerous. Its a bit uncomfortable right now, but risk in any new situation is

    distressing.

    The above statement contains a number of different components. Parts of it

    provide partial components of rational beliefs which are not negated (see below),

    while others are inferential. One is even a feeling statement. Consequently readers

    will be confused about why this statement is rational.

    Errors and Confusions in REBT

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    28/40

    Kottler (2002: 111) provides the following: Oh well, so what if the grade

    isnt what I wanted. Ill do better next time. While Kottler does not state that

    this is a rational belief, it is put forward as a contrast to an irrational belief in

    a section under the heading The Irrational Belief. This is confusing in

    itself. In addition, the statement oh well, so what if can be taken as anindifference belief rather than a rational belief. Finally, the statement Ill do

    better next time seems more like a cognitive consequence of a rational belief

    than an integral part of such a belief. In short, the reader will be left very

    confused by Kottler on the nature of a rational belief after reading his

    example.

    Another statement put forward by Kottler (2002: 113) as a rational belief is

    even more confusing for the reader. This statement is as follows: Well it is a

    little disappointing that I didnt do well as I had hoped. And I do think that

    the assignment was rather unclear and inconsistently graded. But that is theway things sometimes go in this imperfect world. Just because I didnt

    perform perfectly doesnt mean I wont do better on the next assignment.

    Even if I dont improve, these tasks in class dont reflect how well I will do

    in the field. But they do provide useful feedback that I can use to improve my

    knowledge and skills. So this is hardly a terrible disaster, merely a very minor

    setback.

    Only the last sentence is an accurate example of a (non-awfulising) rational belief

    where the badness component is acknowledged (albeit in a diluted form i.e. very

    minor) and the awfulising component is negated. The other parts of the statementare partial rational beliefs or inferences.

    Seligman (2006: 322) begins by presenting the following irrational belief: My

    supervisor should praise me more and is a jerk for not seeing how hard I work. It

    is awful that I work so hard and am not valued as I ought to be. Then Seligman

    puts forward the following rational belief alternative to Martins irrational

    belief: Although I am disappointed that my evaluation was average, it is not the

    end of the world. I will take steps to familiarize my supervisor with my work and

    hopefully obtain a more positive evaluation next time.

    This rational belief is not a point-by-point alternative to the irrational belief,

    which it needs to be if readers are to be clear about the nature of the rational belief

    provided. Also, Seligman fails to provide a flexible, non-dogmatic preference

    alternative to the (unqualified) should in the rational belief. Compare my own

    proposed rational belief alternative to that provided by Seligman: I wish my

    supervisor would praise me more, but sadly he does not have to do so. He is not a

    jerk for not seeing how hard I work. Rather, he is a fallible human being who is

    doing what I consider to be the wrong thing. It is bad, but not awful that I work so

    hard and not valued as I would to be, but dont have to be.

    Rational Beliefs: Theory Not Emphasised I found five areas where REBT theory

    was not emphasised in the discussion of rational beliefs.

    W. Dryden

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    29/40

    i. No flexible beliefs mentioned. REBT theory states that at the core of a healthy

    response to adversity is a flexible belief where the person asserts her preference and

    negates her demand. I found six instances where neither a full nor a partial flexible

    belief was put forward in the authors discussion of a rational belief. In the one

    general case of this de-emphasis that I found, James and Gilliland (2003: 263) statethe following: Rational-emotive therapy theory suggests that emotional distur-

    bance may be explained by ABC theory. This theory states that when an unpleasant

    event (activating event) occurs at point A, an individual may react in two ways. One

    is to conclude that this event is unfortunate and disadvantageous (a rational

    belief).or.that circumstances are terrible or even catastrophic and therefore

    should not exist (irrational belief). In this statement, by only mentioning a non-

    awfulising belief (albeit in a partial form) and omitting the flexible belief, James and

    Gilliland (2003) are clearly de-emphasising the REBT theoretical position on the

    central role of flexibility in rational beliefs.I found five examples of this de-emphasis. Here are a couple of examples:

    Too bad I failed the test. Its disappointing. Ill have to take more lessons.

    (Hough 2002: 117)

    How unfortunate it is that I have been fired because it will be inconvenient to

    look for a job. (James and Gilliland, 2003: 238).

    The six instances (five examples and one general point) of the de-emphasis of

    flexibility in rational beliefs were found in the work of three authors with two

    examples found in Hough (2002: 117, 119) and three in James and Gilliland (2003:235, 238, 263).

    ii. Partial rational beliefs presented: Partial flexible beliefs (or partial

    preferential beliefs). In the first part of this paper, I presented my version of the

    ABC framework and was very clear in distinguishing rational from irrational

    beliefs. Thus, the essence of a flexible belief is where the person states his

    preference and notes explicitly that this preference does not have to be met (e.g. I

    want to do well, but I dont have to do so). If he just articulates his preference, this

    is known as a partial flexible or preferential belief and the person can implicitly

    transform this into a rigid belief (e.g. I want to do well.and therefore I have to do

    well).

    I found eight instances of this de-emphasis; one was a general point and the other

    seven were examples of this point. In the one general point that I found, Nelson-

    Jones (2006: 308) states: B Belief system involving preferential thinking: I prefer

    to have my important Goals unblocked and fulfilled. It would have been more

    accurate for Nelson-Jones to have stated: I prefer to have my important Goals

    unblocked and fulfilled, but I dont have to do so.

    Of the seven examples, each was made by one author. Here is a selection of these

    examples with the full version of this flexible belief also provided:

    I would like to make my mother happy (Archer and McCarthy 2007: 276).

    The full version would be: I would like to make my mother happy, but it does

    not follow that I have to do so.

    Errors and Confusions in REBT

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    30/40

    I dont like this failure, and I wish it had not occurred (Ford and Urban 1998:

    390). The full version would be: I dont like this failure, and I wish it had not

    occurred, but that does not mean that my wish must be fulfilled.

    I dont like to fail a test (Sharf 2008: 305). The full version would be: I

    dont like to fail a test, but there is no reason why I must not do so.

    iii. Partial rational beliefs presented: Partial non-awfulising beliefs. A full

    awfulising belief occurs where the person asserts the badness of the adversity, but

    explicitly negates that it is awful (e.g. It would be is bad if I dont do well, but it

    would not be awful). If the person just asserts the badness of the adversity, this is

    known as a partial non-awfulising belief and the person can implicitly transform this

    into an awfulising belief (e.g. It would be bad if I dont do well.and therefore it

    would be awful).

    I found five instances of this de-emphasis: two made in general points and three

    in examples. Dealing with the general points first, Corey (2005: 280) says: Clients

    learn that musts, oughts, and shoulds can be replaced by preferences. Instead of

    saying It would be absolutely awful if., they learn to say It would be

    inconvenient if. What would be more consistent with REBT theory would be

    the statement: It would be inconvenient, but not awful if James and Gilliland

    (2003: 263) say that concluding that an event is unfortunate and disadvantageous

    is a rational belief. Once again they do not make it clear that a full non-awfulising

    belief would involve negating the idea that it would be awful.

    The three examples of a partial non-awfulising belief were as follows:

    Too bad I failed the test. Its disappointing Hough (2002: 117)

    How unfortunate it is that I have been fired because it will be inconvenient to

    look for a job. (James and Gilliland 2003: 238)

    This is too bad; I dont like to fail a test. (Sharf, 2008: 305)

    Four authors de-emphasized the importance of stating a full non-awfulising

    belief, three doing so on one occasion and one doing so on two occasions (James

    and Gilliland, 2003: 238, 263).

    iv. Partial rational beliefs presented: Partial acceptance beliefs17A full

    acceptance belief occurs where the person does three things: (a) acknowledgesthe negativity of an aspect of himself or what has happened to him, (b) negates that

    this aspect or experience means that he is worthless and (c) asserts his fallibility,

    unrateability and that he is in flux (e.g. I have been demoted, but this does not

    mean that I am worthless. I am fallible, unrateable and capable of change and this is

    true whether I was demoted or not). If the person only takes the first two steps, this

    is known as a partial self-acceptance belief and the person can implicitly transform

    this into a self-depreciation belief (e.g. I have been demoted, but this does not

    mean that I am worthless. However, I would be worthier if I hadnt been

    demoted.) I found two instances where the author did not present a full self-acceptance belief.

    17 In this section I will discuss this issue with respect to partial and full self-acceptance beliefs. The same

    points are relevant to partial and full other-acceptance and life-acceptance beliefs.

    W. Dryden

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    31/40

    Even if I do behave foolishly at times, this does not make me a foolish person

    (Corey 2005: 279)

    Just because I dont understand everything about these theories doesnt make me

    stupid (Parrott 2003: 317).

    As can be seen from these two examples, in partial self-acceptance beliefs, the

    person makes clear who he is not, but does not make clear who he is.

    v. The pantomime horse. In what I call the pantomime horse rational belief,

    the person asserts the first part of one rational belief and negates the irrationality of a

    different rational belief. A common pantomime horse belief pairs the front half of a

    flexible belief with the back half of a non-awfulising belief. This pairing appears in

    the two instances of the pantomime horse rational belief that I found in our sample.

    Its nice to be liked, but not everybody will like me, and that isnt the end of the

    world (Corey 2005: 279) It would be nice to live in a warm country abroad, but it isnt awful that I dont

    (Hough 2002: 137).

    To make REBT theory transparent here, both parts of the two rational beliefs

    need to be expressed in their full form (e.g. It would be nice to live in a warm

    country abroad, but I dont have to do so. It would be bad if I dont, but not awful.

    C

    In the first part of the paper, I noted that C can be emotional, behavioural andcognitive. The present sample of authors largely discussed emotional Cs.

    Emotional Cs

    Emotional C Errors I found ten errors when authors discussed emotional Cs

    1. More than one C in an ABC

    In the first part of this paper, I distinguished between what might be called the

    situational A and the critical A. The former represents the situation in which

    the person disturbed himself and latter is that aspect of the situation about which theperson was most disturbed. The latter tends to be used as A in modern REBT.

    This distinction is important when considering C. When A is situational, many

    Cs can be experienced, but it is not clear what these emotions are about.

    However, when A is critical, only one C can be experienced and there is a

    theme in the A that is representation of that emotion (Dryden 2009). It follows

    therefore that a number of critical As can be found within one situational A.

    I found ten instances where the author provided more than one C when

    outlining an ABC example with two authors doing this twice (Archer and

    McCarthy 2007: 272-twice and Colledge 2002: 232-twice).In nine of the ten instances, the ABC described a psychologically disturbed

    response to a negative situational A. In five of these, the author provided three

    unhealthy negative emotions (UNEs), in two the author provided two UNEs

    (Colledge 2002: 232-twice), in one instance the author (Sommers-Flanagan and

    Errors and Confusions in REBT

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Dryden2011 ABC of Rebt 1

    32/40

    Sommers Flanagan 2004: 265) provided four UNEs and one HNE (healthy negative

    emotion) and in one the author provided three UNEs and one HNE (Archer and

    McCarthy 2007: 272). Here are a few examples:

    A=

    Sharons mother showing her the picture of her sisters baby and remarkingthat her grandchildren are the only things that really make her life worthwhile.

    C = anxious, angry and hurt (Archer and McCarthy 2007: 272)

    A = My supervisor gave me an average rating on my evaluation. C = anger,

    shame, and anxiety (Seligman 2006: 321).

    In the one instance where the ABC described a psychologically healthy

    response to a negative situational A, the author provided three HNEs (Hough

    2002: 117) as follows:

    A = Paul fails his driving test; C = regret, disappointment, irritation

    2. Emotional Cs are too vague. REBTs emotional theory requires specificityand clarity in detailing emotional Cs when explaining the ABC model and

    exemplifying this model (see Dryden 2009). I found four instances when such

    specificity and clarity in authors writings on this issue were lacking with three

    authors doing this once and one author doing so twice (Hough 2002: x, 126127). In

    three of these four instances, authors talked about the vague term emotional upset

    instead of the term preferred in REBT theory i.e. emotional disturbance. The

    other vague term employed was serious emotional reaction (Nystul 2006: 244).

    I found two instances where authors were also too vague in exemplifying the

    ABC of emotional disturbance. Hough (2002: 117) mentioned the termunhappy while Mobley (2005: 89) referred to tired of acting shy.

    3. Emotional Cs are really inferences at A. I found four instances where

    authors (n = 3) used inferences to refer to emotional Cs. Thus, the following

    refer to the following inferences as UNEs:

    victimised and ignored (Day (2008: 376)

    betrayal Fall et al. (2004: 351)

    frustration (Parrott 2003: 317)

    Fall et al. (2004: 351) also refer to frustration as a feeling, but this time as a

    HNE. However, as Trexler (1976) noted, frustra