56
Western Michigan University Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU ScholarWorks at WMU Master's Theses Graduate College 12-1990 Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated Vestibular Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated Vestibular Stimulation on Vocalization in Children with Autism Stimulation on Vocalization in Children with Autism Carol L. Maddox Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/masters_theses Part of the Rehabilitation and Therapy Commons, and the Speech and Rhetorical Studies Commons Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Maddox, Carol L., "Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated Vestibular Stimulation on Vocalization in Children with Autism" (1990). Master's Theses. 1029. https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/masters_theses/1029 This Masters Thesis-Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate College at ScholarWorks at WMU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at WMU. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

Western Michigan University Western Michigan University

ScholarWorks at WMU ScholarWorks at WMU

Master's Theses Graduate College

12-1990

Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated Vestibular Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated Vestibular

Stimulation on Vocalization in Children with Autism Stimulation on Vocalization in Children with Autism

Carol L. Maddox

Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/masters_theses

Part of the Rehabilitation and Therapy Commons, and the Speech and Rhetorical Studies Commons

Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Maddox, Carol L., "Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated Vestibular Stimulation on Vocalization in Children with Autism" (1990). Master's Theses. 1029. https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/masters_theses/1029

This Masters Thesis-Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate College at ScholarWorks at WMU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at WMU. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Page 2: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

EFFECT OF THERAPIST-INITIATED VERSUS SELF-INITIATED VESTIBULAR STIMULATION ON VOCALIZATION IN

CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

byCarol L. Maddox

A Thesis Submitted to the

Faculty of The Graduate College in partial fulfillment of the

requirements for the Degree of Master of Science

Department of Oooupational Therapy

Western Michigan University Kalamazoo, Michigan

December 1990

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 3: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

EFFECT OF THERAPIST-INITIATED VERSUS SELF-INITIATED VESTIBULAR STIMULATION ON VOCALIZATION IN

CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Carol L. Maddox, M.S.Western Michigan University, 1990

Therapist-initiated vestibular stimulation was com­pared to self-initiated stimulation to determine their relative effects on vocalization in children with autism. Vocalizations produced during table-top activities were also compared to those produced during vestibular stimulation. Two children with autism participated in the study. Vestibular stimulation was provided by a hammock swing. All vocalizations were recorded during a total of thirteen 15-minute test sessions.

Results indicated that in this study: Ca) There wasno significant difference in quantity of vocalizations produced during self-initiated versus therapist-initiated vestibular stimulation, and (b) there was a significant inarease in vocalizations during vestibular stimulation as compared to during table-top activities.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 4: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

'1 wish to express my appreciation to my advisor, Doris Smith, for her unending encouragement, support, and patience throughout the process of this study; and to my committee members, Sandra Edwards and.Cindee Peterson, for their guidance and assistance. Thanks are also due to the staff and students of the Doris Klaussen Developmental Center, in Battle Creek, Michigan, for their participation in the research.

Carol L. Maddox

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 5: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

INFORMATION TO USERS

The most advanced technology has been used to photograph and reproduce this manuscript from the microfilm master. U M I films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer.

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction.

In the unlikely event that the author did not send U M I a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand corner and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book.

Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

University Microfilms International A Bell & Howell Information Company

300 North Zeeb Road. Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 USA 313/761-4700 800-521-0600

Page 6: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 7: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

Order Number 1342626

Effect of therapist-initiated versus self-initiated vestibular stimulation on vocalization in children with autism

Maddox, Carol Lee, M.S.

Western Michigan University, 1990

UMI300 N. ZeebRd.Ann Arbor, MI 48106

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 8: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 9: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS i iLIST OF FIGURES............................................ ivINTRODUCTION................................................ 1METHOD....................................................... 19

Subjects................................................. 19Apparatus................................................ 20Procedure................................................ 20Data Analysis........................................... 23

RESULTS...................................................... 25DISCUSSION.................................................. 28SUMMARY..................................................... 32APPENDIX A .................................................. 35BIBLIOGRAPHY................................................ 36

iii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 10: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

LIST OF FIGURES

1. Number of Vocalizations Produced During Vesti­bular Stimulation in Therapist-Initiated Vs. Self-Initiated Conditions............................. 26

2. Number of Vocalizations Produced During Table Top Activities Vs. During VestibularStimulation............................................. 27

iv

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 11: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

INTRODUCTION

Increased vocalization is frequently observed in cor­relation with vestibular stimulation. The latter is com­monly provided as part of sensory integrative treatment in clients with autism and many other diagnoses. As Ayres remarked: "Vestibular stimulation usually elicits vocali­zation" (1972b, p. 243).

A number of researchers have hypothesized a connec­tion between the vestibular and auditory-language systems although limited cliniaal research has been performed to support this hypothesis. Ayres (1972a) stated that audi­tory-language dysfunation is one of the syndromes which is most responsive to sensory integrative treatment, while DeQuiros (1967) identified syndromes of vestibular dysre­flexia which involved abnormalities of language.

It has been noted that, phylogenetically, the audi­tory system evolved out of the vestibular system, there­fore, a close connection has been assumed (Ayres, 1972b).In support of this connection, Moore (1973) reports that hearing deficits may interfere with equilibrium reactions.

Decreased post-rotary nystagmus, considered to be one of several indications of vestibular dysfunation, has been documented in children with a variety of language disor­ders, including autism (Ayres, 1979; Ayres & Mailloux,

1

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 12: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

1981; Ornitz, 1974; Ornitz, Brown, Mason & Putnam, 1974; Stilwell, Crowe & MoCallura, 1978). Wing (1976) mentioned a laok of dizziness--another indication of depressed ves­tibular functioning--following spinning as characteristic of children with autism.

Studies of abnormal reflexes in children with lan­guage disorders provide additional evidenae linking the vestibular system v?ith language. Rider (1974) documented a significantly increased number of abnormal reflexes in children with dysphasia. Stilwell et al. (1978) hypothe­sized that if speech/language development is indeed relat­ed to vestibular functioning, the child with disabilities in this area would exhibit deviations in vestibular func­tioning. In fact, they did find "decreased postural sta­bility" (p. 226) to be characteristic of their subjectswith speech/language disabilities. Studies have documen­ted improvement in reflex integration utilizing vestibular stimulation (Chee, Kreutzberg & Clark, 1978; Montgomery & Richter, 1977; Ottenbaoher, 1983; Ottenbacher, Short & Watson, 1981) .

Language has been noted to be an essential prerequi­site for verbal instruction (Clark & Steingold, 1982) as hearing is for the development of language (Ayres, 1972b). In other words, one must be able to understand language in order to comprehend verbal commands; similarly, one should be able to hear in order to develop adequate spoken

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 13: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

language. Condon (1975), in his work with autistic chil­dren, speculated that infants with defects in auditory processing might encounter difficulties learning speech as well a» in developing human relationships.

DeQuiros and Schrager (1978) believe that vestibular stimulation is necessary for development of the ability to exclude excess information about posture, movement and equilibrium from consciousness in order that learning may occur. DeQuiros (1976) postulates that language develop­ment progresses as the child gains the ability to close out much of the sensory input which is received. Con­versely, inability to modulate this incoming information would interfere with the development of language.

Autism is a condition characterized by disturbances of language, perceptual integration, motility, relation­ships with others, and development. It is felt that the pathology may be present at birth in most oases, rendering the individual virtually incapable of effectively utiliz­ing inaoming stimuli (Ornitz & Ritvo, 1968).

Ornitz (1970) suggests that autism is primarily a disorder of faulty sensorimotor integration. He states that, under normal conditions, the vestibular system sup­presses sensory input during motor discharge, while vesti­bular influence on motor output may in turn be suppressed during excessive sensory input. However, in children with autism, qualitative observations reveal a number of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 14: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

anomalous sensorimotor reactions, such as abnormal motoroutput in reaction to sensation, or motor inhibition which may be sought in order to reduce sensory input.

In the auditory area, autistic children have been noted to be overreaotive or abnormally attentive to cer­tain specific auditory stimuli, such as self-producedsounds, while at the same time failing to react visibly to other stimuli, such as very loud noises. In fact, very often infants with autism may at first be thought to bedeaf, an assumption which is reinforced by the fact thatthey frequently lack the "babbling" behavior typical of this developmental stage, which is the normal precursor to speech. Rutter (1968) hypothesizes that this lack of re­sponse to certain sounds may indicate an inability to un­derstand them, which could indicate a dysfunation in audi­tory processing. Yet another characteristic is the appar­ent inability to accommodate to continuous sound (Ayres, 1979) or an exaggerated lack of tolerance to common sounds.

A number of language anomalies are typically observed in autistic children and delayed development of speech is common in this population. According to Wing (1976) half of all children with autism never develop speech at all. Also common is eoholalia, whioh for 75% of all autistic children is the first language to develop (Wing, 1976). Other limitations include failure to use gestures (such as

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 15: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

signed language) as a substitute for spoken language, and spontaneous speech whioh is produced only with great ef­fort and marked by immaturity and/or abnormalities of grammar. Frequently, the speech whioh is produced resem­bles that seen in individuals with receptive disorders; indeed, the comprehension problems found in autism are similar to those characteristic of receptive aphasia. However, aphasio children are generally able to understand gestures, an ability which autistic children seem to lack (Ornitz & Ritvo, 1968; Wing, 1976). Though articulation disorders may be present in autism, probably more signifi­cant is the apparent lack of an inner language. In addi­tion, children with autism seem to experience a great deal of difficulty in dealing with symbols and symbolic thought. The latter would seem to cause the most severe disability (Savage, 1970).

The speech which is produced by autistic children is often "stereotypical and parrotlike, with much echolalia" (Bloom & Lahey, 1978, p. 516). The voice tends to be high-pitched--often hoarse or very nasal; there is poor control of volume and speech tends to be monotonous (Fay & Schuler, 1980). Wing (1976) described it as resembling the speech of the congenitally deaf, citing deficient con­trol of pitch and volume, atypical intonations and uneven delivery. Koupernik (1970) observed that autistic chil­dren seem incapable of monitoring their own voices, an

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 16: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

ability whioh most children acquire by the age of six months. Although memory and the ability to memorize and repeat phrases (demonstrated by echolalia) are areas of relative strength in many autistic children, these chil­dren, in addition to exhibiting a delay in the development of speech, often fail to develop communicative speech or to use the words already learned (Ornitz & Ritvo, 1968).

According to Rutter (1968), Kanner, who first de­scribed the syndrome of autism in 1943, considered failure to relate to others, or social withdrawal, to be it9 most important characteristic. Rutter presents a number ofpieces of evidence to refute this view, including the fol­lowing: (a) that social withdrawal tends to decrease withage, although the individual remains autistic; (b) that it is impossible to explain the characteristic intelligence test scores or the specific language defects encountered in terras of social withdrawal; (c) that other conditions which involve social withdrawal do not show a similar pat­tern of intelligence test saores; (d) that some autisticchildren may never develop language and yet, as they ma­ture, they become less socially withdrawn; and (e) that the most important predictors of eventual prognosis in­clude IQ and degree of language improvement, while social withdrawal has only a weak correlation with prognosis.

Rutter (1968) proposes the theory that speech is the primary defect in autism. His evidenae for this point of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 17: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

view includes the following points: (a) that retardedspeech is inevitably associated with autism, indeed, in many cases, it is the first sign to be noticed in the au­tistic infant; (b) that scores tend to be poor on all tests which involve verbal concepts, abstraat and/or sym­bolic processes, whether or not those tests actually re­quire speech; and (c) that studies have suggested deficits in verbal coding and patterning.

Other researchers concur with Rutter's theory, in­cluding Schopler and Reichler C1970) who believe that the primary defeat in autism is impaired communication and comprehension. Wing (1970) reports that disorders in per­ception, understanding and language (both verbal and non­verbal) are basic, with behavior playing a secondary role. In addition, Wing (1976) states that most of the observed behavior problems may be blamed on the child's problems with symbolic function and non-verbal communication.

Vestibular system dysfunction is a characteristic of autism (Ayres & Hesket, 1972; Wing, 1976). Autistic chil­dren frequently engage in self-stimulatory behavior such as rooking, whirling, head banging and spinning of ob­jects, all of whioh stimulate the vestibular system either through direct input, optokinetic effects, or both (Ayres, 1979; Kinnealy, 1973; Knickerbocker, 1980; Ornitz, 1970, 1974; Ornitz & Ritvo, 1968). Knickerbocker (1980) be­lieves that this behavior represents an excessive need for

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 18: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

vestibular stimulation as well as a means of facilitating sensory integration. Ornitz, Atwell, Kaplan and Westlake (1985) suggest that the abnormalities in nystagmus whioh they observed indicated brain stem dysfunction, which could be sufficient to explain many of the symptoms and behaviors of autism.

Ayres and colleagues (Ayres, 1979; Ayres & Hesket, 1972; Ayres & Mailloux, 1981) have noted that their autis­tic clients tend to either strenuously avoid or excessive­ly seek vestibular stimulation, both of which may indicate defective processing. Some degree of gravitational inse­curity is also commonly observed (Ayres, 1979; Kinnealy, 1973; Ornitz, 1969; Ornitz & Ritvo, 1968). Ayres (1979) describes two categories of faulty sensory processing which are relevant here. First, she disausses the failure of the nervous system to properly register sensory input whiah leads to hypo- or hyper-reactions to sensation; se­cond she explains gravitational insecurity and tactile de­fensiveness as due to a laak of modulation of input.

Observation suggests that varying states of arousal may exist in the same individual at different times (Ornitz & Ritvo, 1968). Another characteristic is the in­ability of autistic children to modulate perceptual input and thereby maintain constancy. Ornitz and Ritvo state that "identical percepts from the environment are not ex­perienced as the same each time" (p. 88) a fact which they

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 19: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

attribute to a failure of the homeostatic regulation mech­anism of the central nervous system. Similarly, Bergman and Esaalona, as cited by Ornitz and Ritvo (1968), postu­lated that there are low neurological thresholds to exter­nal stimulation, whioh could lead to either too much or too little input being received into the system. In 1974, Ornitz postulated that a vestibular defect might, by remov­ing the vestibular system's normal modulating influence on sensory input, cause the sensorimotor dysfunctions commonly observed. These dysfunctions oould in turn lead to lan­guage dysfunation.

It seems clear that some degree of vestibular malfunc­tion is involved in autism and is responsible, at least in part, for the language deficits experienced by individuals with the disorder. Much research has documented the util­ity of vestibular stimulation in increasing verbal output in individuals with a variety of conditions whioh limit their language ability. A number of these studies are des­cribed below.

Kawar (1973), studying children with learning disabil­ities, found that sensory integrative therapy which includ­ed vestibular stimulation, produced improvement in right ear scores on tests of dichotia listening in a group of 18 subjects. This indicated improved lateral specialization for language functions.

Brody, Thomas, Brody, and Kucherawy (1977) reported in

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 20: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

their study of 27 profoundly retarded, marginally vocal adults, that sensory integrative treatment, whioh incorpor­ated vestibular stimulation, produced a significant in­crease in rate of vocalizations. They felt that sensory integrative methods might be an appropriate means of elici­ting vocalizations in clients whose rate of vocalization is too low to employ behavioral methods.. Similarly, Clark, Miller, Thomas, Kuoherawy, and Azen (1978) reported signi­ficant increases in frequency of vocalization following sensory integrative treatment incorporating vestibular stimulation in their study of 27 profoundly retarded, mini­mally vocal, institutionalized adults.

King (1974) employed vestibular stimulation to produae increased verbalizations in adult chronic, non-paranoid schizophrenics. She hypothesized that an underreaotive vestibular system might be an etiological factor in this type of schizophrenia. In 1973, King (Henderson & Coryell, 1973) reported that she had found that gains achieved through the application of vestibular stimulation were gen­erally retained as opposed to the regression which may oc­cur with other treatment modalities. In 1978, Bailey, also working with chronic schizophrenics, reported an increase in quality of language in her group of seven subjects, though no difference was noted in either quantity or rate.

Magrun, Ottenbacher, McCue, and Keefe (1981) reported increases in spontaneous verbalizations in five trainable

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 21: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

mentally impaired and five developmentally delayed chil­dren as a result of self-initiated, active vestibular stimulation, along with a decrease in verbal responsesfollowing withdrawal of the stimulation. They also noted that the strongest effect was found in the youngest sub­jects with more severe delays, a group whioh is typically least responsive to traditional therapy. Kantner,Kantner, and Clark (1982) provided controlled, passive,serai- circular canal stimulation to ten mentally retarded children. Some improvement in language was noted, al­though the results were not statistically significant.

Morrison and Pothier (1972) reported that nine men­tally retarded pre-schoolers who participated in sensori­motor training incorporating vestibular stimulation demon­strated significant improvement in their language as meas­ured by the Denver Developmental Screening Test (DDST) language subsoale (Frankenburg & Dodds, 1967). It is important to recognize that the DDST is a screening test whiah is not considered to be an optimal tool for the measurement of language. In addition, the reliability and validity of the DDST are considered to be low.

Researchers specifically studying children with au­tism have also reported significant results. Ayres and Hesket (1972) provided a aourse of sensory integrativetherapy to a seven-year-old girl with autism and reportedan increase in auditory-language ability. In 1977,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 22: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

Wolkowicz, Fish, and Sohaffer UBed sensory integrative treatment incorporating vestibular stimulation with four autistic children and documented an increase in spontan­eous vocalizations.

Karsteadt (1983), in a single-case design with a child with autism, compared vestibular stimulation with table-top activities and reported a marked increase in verbalizations during vestibular stimulation as opposed to table-top activities. The subject worked one-on-one with a tutor both prior to and following each of twelve activ­ity sessions.

In a follow-up study, Reilly, Nelson, and Bundy(1983) studied 18 children with autism in a counter­balanced/crossover design. They compared vestibular stim­ulation and fine-raotor/table-top activities and failed to replicate Karsteadt's success, reporting instead no sig­nificant differences in most areas measured, and in fact significantly more variety and greater average length of utterances in the fine-motor group.

Reilly et al. (1983), in discussing their findings, mention several possible reasons for the difference in re­sults. Most relevant here is the fact that the children in their study had had previous experience with the speci­fic table-top activities used, in which their teachers had encouraged verbal interaction related to some of the spe­cific objects employed in the study. They suggest that

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 23: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

these experiences might have biased the subjects in some way toward increased verbalization during these aotivities as opposed to the aotivities involving vestibular stimula­tion. They also cited the short time frame of their study. Essentially only four sessions were conducted for each child, amounting to a total of one hour of vestibular stimulation per subject, whioh may not have been suffi­cient to produce results.

Ray, King, and Grandin (1988) reported a significant increase in vocalizations in a nine-year-old child with autism, with the application of self-initiated vestibular stimulation which was administered by spinning in a rotary fashion on a platform swing with a bouncer attachment for a five-minute period once daily for 17 days over a period of four weeks.

In a related study, Kreutzberg (1976) performed ves­tibular stimulation with normal, unimpaired infants and reported subjective observations of increased vocaliza­tions in many of the subjects.

A significant characteristic of autism is a strong resistance to change and the related lack of self-direc­tion which is so pervasive. Ayres (1979) states that the therapist may not be able to rely on the autistic child to self-direct his or her activity, as is generally recomen­ded in sensory integrative therapy, but instead, may have to encourage the child to participate. Pettit (1980)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 24: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

stresses the importance of recognizing when the child is in need of encouragement or is able to direct her/his own activity. Keeping this faat in mind, along with the prin­ciples of sensory integrative therapy as promoted by Ayres, it seems that the method of administration remains an important factor to consider when examining the impact of vestibular stimulation on language. It is worth noting that whereas Magrun et al. (1981) reported statistically significant increases in verbalizations in mentally retar­ded children as a result of self-initiated, active stimu­lation, Kantner, Kantner, and Clark (1982) reported less than significant results in a similar population, where passive, controlled stimulation was employed. Also rele­vant is the Ray et al. (1988) study, which produced in­creased vocalizations during self initiated stimulation, as well as an increase in vocabulary of 13 words over the term of the study. Cermak and Henderson (1989) state that one of the variables whioh may affect sensory integration is whether stimulation is self- or therapist-initiated.

Stimulation may be provided to the vestibular system in many ways and positions. Ottenbacher and Peterson(1984) reviewed 14 studies of the effects of controlled vestibular stimulation, defined for their purposes as "procedures designed to stimulate vestibular receptors... rotatory stimulation combined with impulsive starts and stops and/or linear or vertical accelerations or some

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 25: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

combination of rotatory, linear, or vertical acceleration or movement" (p. 429), on a total of 533 subjects, ranging in age from infancy to adolescence. Sixty-five percent of the subjects were "normal" without any "identified handi­capping condition" (p. 430), 15% were preterm infantsclassified as "at risk," and the remaining 20% were chil­dren with developmental delays such as cerebral palsy and mental retardation. The investigators reported that "the mean effect size for rotatory vestibular stimulation was approximately twice that of 1 ir,ear/vertical vestibular stimulation" (p. 431). Kantner, Clark, Allen, and Chase (1976) suggested that not all accelerations in linear and angular directions create vestibular stimulation. In practice, many of the purposeful activities which provide vestibular stimulation, such as riding a scooter board down a ramp or swinging on a swing set, provide it in a linear fashion. However, in addition, these aotivities typically incorporate other types of sensory stimulation such as proprioceptive or tactile input. For the purposes of sensory integrative therapy, such a combination of stimulation to more than one system with the purposeful aspeot of the activity would be ideal; however, to insure experimental control, it is necessary first to isolate the form of stimulation, and seaond to assure that the stimu­lation received is equivalent across conditions.

In order to satisfy the first condition, swinging in

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 26: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

a hammock swing was selected for this study as the means of providing vestibular stimulation. With reference to the second condition, although it would have been prefer­able to limit the stimulation to rotary spinning, it was not possible to maintain control over the subject during the self-initiated condition. Therefore, an alternating pattern was utilized, which incorporated both linear and rotary movement.

One consideration in using vestibular stimulation is the "concern that vestibular stimulation may induce sei­zures in seizure-prone children" (Kantner, Clark, Atkin- sons & Paulson, 1982, p. 16) which may include children with autism, according to Rutter (1968). A search of the literature failed to produce documentation of any instance in which seizures aould be directly attributed to vestibu­lar stimulation. This issue has been addressed by Ayres (Henderson & Coryell, 1973). Ayres (Henderson & Coryell, 1973) stated that, although she felt "a great deal of con­cern and responsibility over the possibility that certain types of vestibular stimulation will possibly precipitate seizures... this has never happened to any of the ahildren that I have worked with" (p. 122). According to Kantner, Clark, et al. (1982) this concern "has been based on hear­say and unconfirmed clinical impressions of practicing therapists" (p. 16).

Kantner, Clark, Atkinsons, and Paulson (1982) looked

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 27: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

at the effects of vestibular stimulation on EEG readings in children prone to seizures. They reported that the stimulation did not exacerbate abnormal brain-wave pat­terns; in fact they observed a significant decrease in paroxysmal aativity in six of their ten subjects. The au­thors concluded "available literature, supported by our findings, indicates that a seizure directly induced by semicircular canal stimulation is an extremely rare event"(p. 20).

Kantner, Clark, Atkinsons, and Paulson (1982) pro­posed a number of possible explanations for the assumption that vestibular stimulation might promote seizure activ­ity, among them (a) the possibility that inexperienced observers might mistake the manifestation of spinal level reflexes, which may be evoked by spinning, for seizure activity; (b) that seizures may be caused by fatigue, ex­citement or hyperventilation; or <o) that they might be a result of the stroboscopic visual effect created by spin­ning in a lighted room. For the latter reason, as well as in the interest of more specifically isolating the effects of vestibular stimulation, Kantner, Clark, Atkinsons, and Paulson (1982), along with several other researchers, sug­gest keeping eyes closed and/or spinning in darkness (Kantner, Clark, Atkinsons & Paulson, 1982; Montgomery, 1985; Palmer 1980; Reilly et al. , 1983; Stilwell et al. , 1978).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 28: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

This recommendation was not considered to be a viable option for the purposes of this study for two reasons. First, the basic principle of sensory integration, indeed of occupational therapy itself, centers on the concept of purposeful activity. It is questionable whether it would ever be possible to incorporate vestibular stimulation performed in a completely darkened room or with the cli­ent's eyes closed into a purposeful activity or one re­quiring an adaptive response. Second, as Montgomery(1985) acknowledges, a child may resist being blindfolded, especially a child who has gravitational insecurity, as so many children with autism do.

Two hypotheses were tested in this study. It was hy­pothesized that vocalizations would increase during vesti­bular stimulation compared to table top activities and that the number of vocalizations produced would be greater in response to self-initiated stimulation than when the stimulation was initiated by the therapist.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 29: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

METHOD

Subjects

Two subjects were selected from volunteers in the Autistic Impaired classrooms at the Doris Klaussen Devel­opmental Center in Battle Creek, Michigan. Selection cri­teria were that subjects: Ca) be between the ages of fiveand fifteen years; (b) have a primary diagnosis of autism; (o) be essentially non-verbal, that is have a delayed dev­elopment of language; and (d) have no history of seizure disorders, no EEG abnormalities, and no inner ear dysfunc­tion.

Subject A was a nine-year, nine-month old male who had no spoken language (non-verbal). The subject was re­sponsive to verbal commands and there was no reason to suspect hearing impairment. The classroom teacher report­ed that the subject had at one time had tubes surgically implanted to relieve a fluid build-up in the inner ear. An audiological screening was unsuccessfully attempted by the school nurse shortly after the initiation of this study.

Subject B was a 15-year old male, also non-verbal. Subject B would not remain seated in the swing long enough to participate in the study after the second session.

19

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 30: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

Apparatus

Equipment included a post-rotary nystagmus testing board, a mat, a hammock swing, a stopwatch, and a hand­held cassette recorder.

Procedure

The experimental design employed was a single-sub­ject, alternating treatment design (Ottenbacher, 1986a). A total of 13 experimental sessions were conducted two times per week over a nine-week period, with a two-week break imposed by the school holidays. During a third week, school conferences necessitated an alteration in the schedule and only one session was completed. The initial session was eliminated from data analysis due to adjust­ments in the procedure which were made following this ses­sion. The two conditions--therapist-initiated versus self-initiated--were alternated every third session (i.e., two sessions of one condition, followed by two sessions of the second condition), beginning with the therapist-initi­ated condition.

Each subjeat initially received an assessment of ves­tibular system integrity. The Southern California Post- Rotary Nystagmus Test (SCPNT) (Ayres, 1975) was adminis­tered. Integration of the asymmetria (ATNR) and symmetric tonic neck reflexes (STNR), and the tonic labyrinthine re­flex (TLR) in prone and supine were assessed. In

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 31: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

addition, the subjects were observed for the presence of equilibrium reactions in quadruped, kneel-standing, and seated positions. Ottenbacher (1978) believes that post-rotary nystagmus is easily influenced by other factors, most notably visual fixation; therefore, he investigated other means of identifying vestibular dysfunction. The inability to assume the prone extension posture (essentially, the test for TLR, prone) was found to be an effective indicator of "vestibulo-proprioceptive dysfunction" (p. 221). Montgomery (1985) citedcorrelations between hypo-reactive nystagmus and poor postural mechanisms and recommended assessing prone extension, righting and equilibrium responses, and the to­nic neck reflexes.

Results of the SCPNT test on Subjeat A were equivo­cal. The subject appeared to receive definite pleasure from the rotation, and was cooperative in assuming and maintaining the testing posture; however, it was not pos­sible to prevent visual fixation while attempting to mea­sure nystagmus, and no nystagmus could be observed. The SCPNT had previously been administered by the sahool oaau- pational therapist on three occasions when the subject was between six-years, five-months and six-years, ten-months of age, at which time aombined scores of 13, 40 and 5 se­conds were obtained. Notwithstanding the high score of 40 seconds, the reliability of which was questioned by the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 32: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

administering therapist due to the subject's behavior dur­ing administration, the remaining scores obtained at that time were indicative of significantly depressed nystagmus as the mean total score for six year old males is 20.1 se­conds while the subject's mean score, excluding the anoma­lous score of 40, was 9. The combined SD for the scores of 13 and 5 were calculated at 1.0 and 2.1 respectively, again significantly different from the norm of 8.7.

Subjeat A was resistant to assuming the posture for testing of the ATNR and STNR; therefore it could not be determined that these were present. The subjeat was ap­parently unable to assume or maintain either the prone or supine postures for observation of the TLR, indicating a possible unintegrated reflex, although this may have sim­ply resulted from lack of compliance.

Vestibular stimulation was provided by swinging for five minutes in a suspended hammock swing. During the therapist-initiated stimulation the subject sat passively while the examiner moved the swing. For the self-initiat- ed condition, the subject propelled himself by pushing with his feet against the floor. The subjeat tended to alternate between rotary and linear swinging in a fairly even pattern. As there was no way to control this self­initiated condition, the therapist attempted to imitate this pattern as closely as possible during the therapist- initiated stimulation; therefore, the pattern was as

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 33: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

follows. For 30 seconds, the swing was propelled in a linear fashion, i.e., forward and back. During the next 30 seaonds, the swing was rotated to the left, followed by another 30 seconds of linear motion. Finally, the swing was rotated to the right for 30 seaonds.

Each experimental session consisted of a five-minute pre-intervention stage during which the subject engaged in his usual classroom activities, which generally consisted of assembling an assortment of puzzles or completing a co­loring project; a five-minute intervention session during which vestibular stimulation was provided by the subject or therapist; and a five-minute post-intervention stage during whiah the subjeat returned to the table-top activ­ities. The experimenter was present in the room at all times, without speaking, except to indicate to the client when it was time to change activities. During the table- top activities the investigator sat at the table with the client; during the self-initiated swinging sessions, the therapist stood within view of the client, again without speaking; and during the therapist-initiated sessions, the therapist stood in front of the client to propel the ham­mock. In this way an effort was made to keep the thera­pist's presence as constant as possible.

Data Analysis

During the entire 15-minute session, all sounds the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 34: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

child produced were audio-recorded. The tapes were then analyzed by a graduate student in Speech/Language Pathol­ogy, Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo, MI, who counted the number of vocalizations produced. Vocaliza­tions were defined as intentional phonations produaed from the larynx. Excluded from the count were sounds such as tongue clicks, "whimpers" and inhalatory stridors. In or­der to maintain a single blind control of the data and avoid any effect of experimenter bias on the results, this assistant was not aware of the purposes of the study, the experimental conditions, or the hypotheses. The number of vocalizations produced during each phase of each session were tallied and graphed to compare the effeat of thera­pist-initiated versus self-initiated vestibular stimula­tion as well as any differences between the three phases.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 35: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

RESULTS

Variability in the subject's responses was observed throughout the study, with marked fluctuation from session to session. However, plotting on a graph (see Figure 1) indicated a large difference between the mean number of voaalizations produaed during the table-top activities [pre- and post-intervention phases (x = 10.5)] and that produaed during vestibular stimulation (x = 29).

On the second graph the quantity of vocalizations du­ring the therapist-initiated sessions was compared to the quantity produced during self-initiated sessions. As may be observed in Figure 2, the mean number of vocalizations during the self-initiated phase was marginally higher than during therapist-initiated sessions (x = 31 as compared to x = 27), but, due to the high degree of variability within the sessions, the difference cannot be considered signifi­cant .

25

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 36: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

Number

of Vo

cali

zati

ons

26

10 ■

SessionsLegend: o= Self-Initiated, 0= Therapist InitiatedFigure 1. Number of Vocalizations Produced During

Vestibular Stimulation in Therapist- Initiated vs. Self-Initiated Conditions.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 37: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

Reproduced

with perm

ission of the

copyright ow

ner. Further

reproduction prohibited

without

permission.

00

Phase II: Vest ibular StimulationPhase I:

Table-topActivities

Phase III:Table-topActivities

20

to

i ! 3 t 5 i 1 i 1 in ii s t i 3 i s n m » ri i n 4 5 n m » n 12Sessions

Legend: S = Self-Initiated, T = Therapist InitiatedFigure 2. Number of Vocalizations Produced During Table-top

Activities vs. During Vestibular Stimulation. to-a

Page 38: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

DISCUSSION

The high level of variability observed in the sub­ject's responses contributed to difficulty in drawing con­clusions with regard to either hypothesis. The results appear to support the hypothesis that vocalizations would increase during vestibular stimulation as compared to ta­ble-top activities. However, the second hypothesis that the number of vocalizations would be greater in response to self-initiated stimulation than to stimulation initia­ted by the therapist could not be supported.

Several factors may have contributed to the varia­bility of the data. The classroom teacher noted that the subject had been experiencing difficulties in the home en­vironment. On several occasions, the subject appeared distressed when the experimenter arrived to begin the ses­sion. This was indicated by either depressed or agitated affect. On those days when the subject appeared depres­sed, vocalizations were increased as can be noted on Fig­ure 1 (see days 2 and 7). On days when the subject was visibly agitated, indicated by vigorous head shaking, hit- ing self lightly on the forehead, or resisting table-top activities, vocalizations were decreased (see Figure 1, days 3, 5, and 12). In each of these instances, the sub­ject’s affect had visibly improved by the end of the

28

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 39: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

intervention, suggesting a positive effect of the vestibu­lar stimulation. In general, the subjeat evidenced enjoy­ment of the stimulation through smiles and laughter. Dur­ing one of the sessions, the vestibular stimulation seemed to have a calming effect on the subject. This effect was indicated by a relaxed posture and drooping eyelids and resulted in fewer vocalizations (see Figure 1, day 12).

Another confounding factor was related to the diffi­culty encountered in controlling the testing environment due to the fact that the research was conducted in a busy school building. The swing which was available and appro­priate for use in this study was located in a classroom which was generally vacant during the experimental ses­sions. However, on three occasions, a schedule change re­sulted in unavailability of this room, necessitating a change in location and on two of these location changes, a different swing was used. There were also frequent inter­ruptions caused by other students entering the room or by announcements being made over the public address system. Any of these phenomena could have affected the subject's responses. The changes in schedule due to sahool holidays and the subject's absence from school may also have af­fected the subject's performance.

In reference to the lack of a measurable difference in response between the therapist-initiated and self-ini­tiated stimulation, several possible explanations may be

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 40: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

projected. It is conceivable that any effect may have been mitigated by the fact that when the therapist was providing vestibular stimulation, there was neaessarily more interaction between the subject and the examiner than during the self-initiated phases. Such interaction, al­though non-verbal on the part of the examiner, may have induced more vocalizations by the subjeat.

A second possibility is that the slight degree of gravitational insecurity whiah was initially observed in the client may have created an increased number of vocali­zations during the therapist-initiated session, since, in order for the examiner to control the swing, it was neces­sary for the subjeat to lift his feet off the floor. It was also noted that the swing was slightly less stable un­der this condition which may have resulted in increased vestibular input.

It is also possible that the frequent alteration be­tween conditions prevented an effect from developing or becoming measurable. It may have been more effective to employ each condition for six sessions in sequence, rather than alternating every third session.

No qualitative differences were apparent in the sub­ject's voaalizations reaorded on the tape; however, the classroom teachers reported that the subjeat had been in­creasingly vocal in the classroom over the course of the study. They also voluntarily reported a significant

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 41: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

improvement in his affect. Finally, just prior to termination of the study, they reported that the subject had begun to produae some babbling sounds, i.e., sounds such as "ba-ba-be."

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 42: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

SUMMARY

In considering these results, there seems to have been some sign of increase in the number of vocalizations which the subject produced during the vestibular stimula­tion as compared to table-top activity sessions. The high degree of variability in the subject's responses from ses­sion to session interfered with establishment of a consis­tent trend. While it would be impossible to control all of the factors such as those related to the subject's home life, illnesses, or affective changes, whiah may have con­tributed to this variability, a future study might main­tain better control of environmental faators suah as in­terruptions and changes of site if a more adequate facili­ty were available.

More frequent sessions could also be employed on a more regular schedule. Due to a combination of faators, the sessions in the present study were conducted each Thursday and Friday, at different times of the day. Dai­ly, or at least thrice weekly, sessions conducted at the same hour each day would have been preferable. Such a schedule might have produced a more consistent pattern of responses. In addition, an increased amount of data could allow for more in-depth analysis of the subject's vocal productions. In this study, anecdotal reports indicated

32

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 43: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

that a possible improvement was occurring in the quality of the subject's vocalization; however, given the limited time frame and number of sessions, no evidence of this was noted in the tape recordings.

With reference to the lack of difference in vocaliza­tions during self-initiated versus therapist-initiated stimulation, a future study might employ a means of pro­viding vestibular stimulation which requires less direct contact by the experimenter for operation in order to mi­tigate possible effects of the increased interaction dur­ing this condition. It would also be advisable to control the meanB of vestibular stimulation such that the stimula­tion provided is equal during both conditions.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 44: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

Appendix AResearch Protocol Clearance

Human Subjects Institutional Review Board

34

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 45: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

35Human Subjects Institutional Review Board Kalamazoo. Michigan 49008-3899

W estern M ic h ig a n University

Data January 24,1990

To: Carol L. Maddox

From: Mary Anna Bunda, Chair

This letter w ill serve as confirmation that your research protocol, "Therapist-Imposed Versus Self-Imposed Vestibular Stimulation in Terms of Their Effects on Verbalization in Children with Autism", has been approved asfyllby theHSIRB. The conditions and duration of this approval are specified in the Policies of Western Michigan University. You may now begin to implement the research as described In the approval application. You must seek reapproval for any change in this design.

The Board wishes you succsss in the pursuit of your research goals.

xc: D. Smith, Occupational Therapy

HSIRB Project Number 90-01-07

End Dote of Approval_______ January 24.1991

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 46: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

BIBLIOGRAPHY

Arendt, R.E., MacLean, W.E., & Baumeister, A.A. (1980). Critique of sensory integrative therapy and its appli­cation in mental retardation. American Journal on Men­tal Retardation, 92, 401-411.

Ayres, A.J. (1972a). Improving academic scores through sensory integration. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 5, 338-343.

Ayres, A.J. (1972b). Sensory integration and learning dis­orders . Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Ayres, A.J. (1972c). Types of sensory integrative dysfunc­tion among disabled learners. American Journal of Occu­pational Therapy, 26, 13-18.

Ayres, A.J. (1973). An interpretation of the role of the brain stem in intersensory integration. In A. Hender­son & J. Coryell (Eds.), The body senses and perceptual deficit (pp. 81-89). Boston: Boston University.

Ayres, A.J. (1975). Southern California post-rotary nys­tagmus test. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Servi­ces .

Ayres, A.J. (1977). Effect of sensory integrative therapy on the coordination of children with choreoathetoid movements. American Journal of Occupational Therapy,31, 291-293.

Ayres, A.J. (1978). Learning disabilities and the vestibu­lar system. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 11,18-29.

Ayres, A.J. (1979). Sensory integration and the child. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Ayres, A.J. & Hesket, W.M. (1972). Sensory integrative dysfunction in a young schizophrenic girl. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2, 174-181.

Ayres, A.J. & Mailloux, Z. (1981). Influence of sensory integrative procedures on language development. Ameri­can Journal of Occupational Therapy, 35, 383-390.

36

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 47: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

37Ayres, A.J. & Tickle, L.S. (1980). Hyperresponsivity to

touch and vestibular stimulation as a predictor of pos­itive response to sensory integrative procedures by au­tistic children. American Journal of Occupational Ther­apy , 33, 375-381.

Bailey, D.M. (1979). The effects of vestibular stimulation on verbalization in chronic schizophrenics. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 32, 445-450.

Beaker, M. (1980). Autism: A neurologiaal model. Sensory Integration Specialty Section Newsletter, ^(1), 1,4.

Benaroya, S., Klein, L.S., & Monroe, D. (1977, May). An evaluation of the role of multisensory input in treat­ing autistic children. Paper presented at the Third na­tional conference of the Canadian Society for Autistic Children, Regina, Saskatchewan, Canada.

Bender, L. (1956). Schizophrenia in childhood: Its recog­nition, description and treatment. American Journal of Orthopsyahiatry, 26, 499-506.

Bhatara, V. , Clark, D.L, & Arnold, L.E. (1978). Behavioral and nystagmus response of a hyperkinetic child to vest­ibular stimulation. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 32, 311-316.

Bhatara, V. , Clark, D.L., Arnold, L.E., Gunsett, R . , &Sraeltzer, D.J. (1981). Hyperkinesis treated by vestibu­lar stimulation: An exploratory study. Biological Psy­chiatry , 16, 269-279.

Bloom, L. & Lahey, M. (1978). Language development and language disorders. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Brody, J.F., Thomas, J.A., Brody, D.M., & Kucherawy, D.A. (1977). Comparisons of sensory integrative and operant methods for production of vocalization in profoundly retarded adults. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 1283- 1296.

Cermak, S.A. & Henderson, A. (1989). The efficacy of sen­sory integrative procedures. Sensory Integration Quar­terly. 17(3), 1-3.

Chee, F.K.W., Kreutzberg, J.R., & Clark, D.L. (1978). Semicircular canal stimulation in cerebral palsied children. Physioa1 Therapy, 58, 1071-1075.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 48: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

Clark, D.L. (1985). The vestibular system: An overview. In K. Ottenbacher & M.A. Short (Eds.), Vestibular proces­sing dysfunction in ohilden (pp. 5-32)., New York: The Hawthorne Press.

Clark, D.L., Kreutzberg, J.R., & Chee, F.K.W. (1977). Vestibular stimulation influence on motor development in infants. Soienae, 196, 1228-1229.

Clark, F.A., Miller, L.R., Thomas, J.A., Kucherawy, D.A.,& Azen, S.P. (1978). A comparison of operant and sensory integrative methods on developmental parameters in profoundly retarded adults. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 32, 86-92.

Clark, F. & Pierce, D. (1987). Synopsis of pediatria occu­pational therapy effectiveness. In S. Vulpe & R. Van Benschoten (Eds.) Occupational therapy for maternal and child health research and leadership development (pp. 226-244). Los Angeles: Center for Development and Developmental Disorders.

Clark, F.A. & Shuer, J. (1978). A clarification of sensory integrative therapy and its application to programming with retarded people. Mental Retardation. 16, 227-232.

Clark, F.A. & Steingold, L.R. (1982). A potential rela­tionship between occupational therapy and language ac­quisition. American Journal of Occupational Therapy,36, 42-44.

Condon, W.S. (1975). Multiple response to sound in dys­functional children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Sahizophrenia, j>, 37-56.

Condon, W.S. & Sander, L.W. (1974). Synchrony demonstrated between movements of the neonate and adult speech.Child Development, 45, 456-462.

DeGangi, G.A., Berk, R.A., & Larsen, L.A. (1980). The mea­surement of vestibular-based functions in pre-school children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 34, 452-459.

DeMyer, M.K. (1975). The nature of the neuropsychological disability in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia. Jj, 109-127.

DePauw, K.P. (1978). Enhancing the sensory integration of aphasio students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 11,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 49: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

39142-146.

DeQuiros, J.B. (1967). Vestibular-proprioaeptive integra­tion: Its influence on learning and speech in children. In C. F. Hereford & Luiz Natalioio (Eds.), Proceedings of the 10th Inter-American Congress of Psychology (pp. 194-202). Mexico City, Mexico: Editorial F. Trillas.

DeQuiros, J.B. (1976). Diagnosis of vestibular disorders in the learning disabled. Journal of Learning Disor­ders , 9, 39-58.

DeQuiros, J.B. & Sohrager, O.L. (1978). Neuropsyahological fundamentals in learning disabilities. San Rafael, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.

Dutton, R.E. (1985). Reliability and clinical significance of the SCPNT. In K. Ottenbacker & M.A. Short (Eds.), Vestibular processing dysfunction in ohilden (pp. 57- 67). New York: The Hawthorne Press.

Edwards, S.J. & Yuen, H.K. Effects of vestibular-stimula- tion on neurophysiological responses in children with Down Syndrome. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Fay, W.H. & Sahuler, A.L. (1980). Emerging language in au­tistic children. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

Franois-Wi11iaras, J. (1976). Early identification of chil­dren likely to have specific learning difficulties: Re­port of a follow-up. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 18, 71-77.

Frankenburg, W.K. & Dodds, J.B. (1967). The Denverdevelopmental screening test. Journal of Pediatrics, 2, 181-191.

Freeman, B.J., Frankel, F., & Ritvo, E.R. (1976). The ef­fects of response contingent vestibular stimulation on the behavior of autistic and retarded children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia. 6, 353-357.

Gabert, T.E., Jee, A., & Collins, W.E. (1982). Influence of passive and active vestibular stimulation on balance of young children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54,548- 550.

Gillberg, C., Steffenberg, S., & Jakobssen, G. (1987). Children with Kanner-type autism or Asperger syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 29, 641-

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 50: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

40649.

Gregg, C.L., Haffner, M.E., & Korner, A.F. (1976). Therelative effioaoy of vestibular-propriooeptive stimula­tion and the upright position in enhancing visual pur­suit in neonates. Child Development, 47, 309-314.

Henderson, A, & Coryell, J. (1973). The body senses and perceptual deficit. Boston, MA; Boston University.

Hirt, S. (1967). The TNR mechanism in the normal human a- dult. American Journal of Physical Medicine, 46, 362- 369.

Horak, F.B., Shumway-Cook, A., Crowe, T.K., & Black, F.O. (1988). Vestibular function and motor proficiency of children with impaired hearing, or with learning dis­ability and motor impairments. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 30, 64-79.

Jacobson, R., LeCouteur, A., Howlin, P., & Rutter, M.(1988). Selective subcortical abnormalities in autism. Psychological Medicine, 18, 39-48.

Jenkins, H.A. & Goldberg, J. (1988). Test-retest reliabil­ity of the rotatory test in normal subjects. Advances in Oto-Rhino-Laryngology, 41, 190-95.

Kantner, R.M., Clark, D.L., Allen, L.C., & Chase, M.F.(1976). Effects of vestibular stimulation on nystagmus response and motor performance in the developmentally disabled infant. Physical Therapy, 56, 414-421.

Kantner, R.M., Clark, D.L., Atkinsons, J., & Paulson, G. (1982). Effects of vestibular stimulation in seizure- prone children. Physioal Therapy, 62, 16-21.

Kantner, R.M., Kantner, B., & Clark, D.L. (1982). Vestibu­lar stimulation effect on language development in men­tally retarded children. American Journal of Occupa­tional Therapy, 36, 36-41.

Karsteadt, K. (1983). The effeats of vestibular stimula­tion on verbalization and attending behavior of an au­tistic child. Sensory Integration Special Interest Sec­tion Newsletter, 6(1), 4.

Kawar, M. (1973). The effeats of sensorimotor therapy on diohotic listening in children with learning disabili­ties. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 27,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 51: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

41

226-231.Kimball, J.6. (1988). The emphasis is on integration, not

sensory. American Journal of Mental Retardation. 92, 423-424.

King, L.J. (1974). A sensory-integrative approaah to schi­zophrenia. American Journal of Occupational Therapy.28, 529-536.

Kinnealy, M. (1973). Aversive and non-aversive responses to sensory stimulation in mentally retarded children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 27, 464-471.

Knickerbocker, B.M. (1980). A holistic approach to thetreatment of learning disorders. Thorofare, N J : Charles B. Slack.

Korncr, A.F. & Thoraan, E.B. (1970). Visual alertness in neonates as evoked by maternal care. Journal of Experi­mental Child Psychology, 10, 67-78.

Koupernik, C. (1970). A pathogenic approach to infantile autism. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Infantile autism: Concepts, characteristics and treatment (pp. 27-42). London, England: Churchill Livingstone.

Kreutzberg, J.R. (1976). Effects of vestibular stimulation on the reflex and motor development in normal infants. (Doctoral Dissertation, Ohio State University, 1976). Dissertation Abstracts International, 37, 2013B.

Larrington, G.G. (1987). A sensory integration-based pro­gram with a severely retarded/autistic teenager: An oc­cupational therapy case report. In Zoe Mailloux (Ed.), Sensory integrative approaches in occupational therapy (pp. 101-117). New York: The Haworth Press.

Magrun, W.M., Ottenbacher, K., MoCue, S., & Keefe, R.(1981). Effects of vestibular stimulation on spontan­eous use of verbal language in developmentally delayed children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 35, 101-104.

Miller, A. & Miller, E.E. (1973). Cognitive-developmental training with elevated boards and sign language. Jour­nal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 21, 65-85.

Montgomery, P. (1985). Assessment of vestibular function in children. In K. Ottenbacher & M.A. Short (Eds.),

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 52: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

42

Vestibular processing dysfunction in children (pp. 33- 55). New York: The Hawthorne Press.

Montgomery, P. & Riohter, E. (1977). Effect of sensory in­tegrative therapy on the neuroraotor development of re­tarded children. Physiaal Therapy, 57, 799-806.

Moore, J.C. (1973). Cranial nerves and their importance in current rehabilitation techniques. In A. Henderson & J. Coryell (Eds.), The body senses and perceptual deficit (pp. 102-120). Boston: Boston University.

Morrison, D. & Pothier, P. (1972). Two different remedial motor training programs and the development of mentally retarded pre-schoolers. American Journal of Mental De­ficiency , 77, 251-258.

Nelson, D . , Nitzberg, L., & Hollander, T. (1980). Visually monitored post-rotary nystagmus in 7 autistic children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 34, 382-386.

Nelson, D.L. (1984). Children with autism and other perva­sive disorders of development and behavior: Therapy through activities. Thorofare, N J : Charles B. Slack.

O ’Brien, V., Cermak, S.A., & Murray, E. (1988). The rela­tionship between visual-perceptual motor abilities and clumsiness in children with and without learning disa­bilities. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 42, 359-463.

Ornitz, E.M. (1969). Disorders of perception common toearly infantile autism and schizophrenia. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 10, 259-274.

Ornitz, E.M. (1970). Childhood autism: A disorder of sen­sorimotor integration. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Infantile autism: Conaepts, characteristics and treatment (pp. 50-68). London, England: Churchill Livingstone.

Ornitz, E.M. (1974). The modulation of sensory input and motor output in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 4, 197-215.

Ornitz, E.M., Atwell, C.W., Kaplan, A.R., & Westlake, J.R. (1985). Brain-stem dysfunction in autism: Results of vestibular stimulation. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 1018-1025.

Ornitz, E.M., Brown, M.B., Mason, A., & Putnam, N.H.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 53: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

A3

(1974). Effect of visual input on vestibular nystagmus in autistic children. Archives of General Psyohiatrv. 31, 369-375.

Ornitz, E.M. & Ritvo, E.R. (1968). Perceptual inconstancy in early infantile autism. Archives of General Psychia­try , 18, 76-98.

Ornitz, E. & Ritvo, E. (1976). The syndrome of autism: A critical review. American Journal of Psychiatry, 133, 609-621.

Ottenbacher, K. (1978). Identifying vestibular processing dysfunction in learning disabled children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 32, 217-221.

Ottenbacher, K. (1980). Excessive post-rotary nystagmus duration, in learning disabled children. American Jour­nal of Occupational Therapy, 3A, A0-A4.

Ottenbacher, K. (1982). Sensory integration therapy: Af­fect or effect? American Journal of Occupational Thera- ££> 36, 571-578.

Ottenbacher, K. (1983). Developmental implications of clinically applied vestibular stimulation: A review. Physical Therapy, 63, 338-3A2.

Ottenbacher, K.J. (1986a). Evaluating clinical change: Strategies for occupational and physical therapists. Madison, W I : Williams and Wilkins.

Ottenbacher, K.J. (1986b). Reliability and accuracy ofvisually analyzing graphed data from single-subject de­signs. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, AO, A6A-A69.

Ottenbacher, K.J. & Peterson, P. (1983a). Quantitative re­viewing of medical literature: An approach to synthesi­zing research results in clinical pediatrics. Clinical Pediatrics, 23, A23-A27.

Ottenbacher, K.J. & Peterson, P. (1983b). The efficacy of vestibular stimulation as a form of specific sensory enrichment. Clinical Pediatrics, 23, A28-A33.

Ottenbacher, K. & Short, M.A., Eds. (1985). Vestibular processing dysfunction in children. New York: The Haworth Press.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 54: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

Ottenbacher, K., Short, M.A., & Watson, P.J. (1981). The effects of a clinically applied program of vestibular stimulation on the neuromotor performance of ahildren with severe developmental disabilities. Physical and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, _1, 1-11.

Ottenbacher, K., Watson, P.J., Short, M.A., & Biderman, M.D. (1979). Nystagmus and ocular fixation difficulties in learning disabled children. American Journal of Oc­cupational Therapy, 33, 717-721.

Ottenbacher, K. & York, J. (1984). Strategies for evalua­ting clinical change: Implications for practice and re­search. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 38, 647-659.

Palmer, L.L. (1980). Auditory discrimination development through vestibulo-ooohlear stimulation. Academic Thera­py, 16, 53-63.

Parraenter, C.L. (1975). The asymmetrical tonic neck reflex in normal first- and third-grade children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 29, 463-468.

Perozzi, J.A. (1972). Language acquisition as adaptive be­havior. Mental Retardation, 10, 32-34.

Pettit, K. (1980). Treatment of the autistic child: A de­manding challenge. Sensory Integration Special Interest Section Newsletter, 3(1), 1-3.

Raver, S.A. (1980). The effeats of antecedent vestibular stimulation on the receptive language learning of re­tarded toddlers. Child Study Journal, 10, 77-86.

Ray, T.C., King, L.J., & Grandin, T. (1988). The effec­tiveness of self-initiated vestibular stimulation in producing speech in an autistic child. Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 8, 186-190.

Reilly, C., Nelson, D.L., & Bundy, A.C. (1983). Sensorimo­tor versus fine motor activities in eliciting vocaliza­tions in autistic children. Occupational Therapy Jour­nal of Research, 3 , 199-212.

Rider, B.A. (1972). Tonia neck reflexes. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 26, 132-134.

Rider, B.A. (1972). Relationship of postural reflexes to learning disabilities. American Journal of Occupational

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 55: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

Therapy, 26, 239-243.Rider, B.A. (1974). Abnormal postural reflexes in dyspha-

sio children. Amerioan Journal of Oooupational Therapy, 20, 351-353.

Ritvo, E.R., Freeman, B.J., Soheibel, A.B., Duong, T., Robinson, H., Guthrie, D., & Ritvo, A. (1906). Lower Purkinje Cell aounts in the aerebella of four autistic subjects: Initial findings of the UCLA-NSAC autopsy re­search report. Amerioan Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 062-066.

Rutter, M. (1960). Concepts of autism: A review of re­search. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 9, 1-25.

Rutter, M. (Ed.). (1971). Infantile autism: Concepts, characteristics and treatment. London, England: Churchill Livingstone.

Rutter, M . , Bartak, L., & Newman, S. (1970). Autism: A central disorder of cognition and language? In M.Rutter (Ed.), Infantile autism: Concepts, characteris­tics and treatment (pp. 140-171). London, England: Churchill Livingstone.

Savage, V.A. (1970). An approach to treatment in the young autistic child. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Infantile autism: Concepts, characteristics and treatment (pp. 297-307). London, England: Churchill Livingstone.

Schmid, R. & Zarabarbieri, D. (1900). The role of the vest­ibular system in eye-head coordination and the genera­tion of vestibular nystagmus. Advances in Oto-Rhino- Laryngology, 41, 09-94.

Schopler, E. & Reiahler, R.J. (1970). Developmental ther­apy by parents with their own autistic child. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Infantile autism: Concepts, characteris­tics and treatment (pp. 206-227). London, England: Churahill Livingstone.

Selliak, K.J. & Over, R. (1900). Effeats of vestibular stimulation on motor development of cerebral-palsied ahildren. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,22, 476-403.

Shuer, J., Clark, F., & Azen, S.P. (1900). Vestibular function in mildly mentally retarded adults. Amerioan

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 56: Effect of Therapist-Initiated Versus Self-Initiated

46Journal of Oooupational Therapy, 34, 664-670.

Silva, P.A., Williams, S., & McGee, R. (1987). A longitu­dinal study of children with developmental language de­lay at age three: Later intelligence, reading and beha­vioral problems. Developmental Medicine and Child Neu­rology , 29, 630-640.

Slavik, B.A. (1982). Vestibular function in children with nonparalytic strabismus. Oooupational Therapy Journal of Research, 2, 220-233.

Steinberg, M. & Rendle-Short, J. (1977). Vestibular dys­function in young children with minor neurological im­pairment. Developmental Mediaine and Child Neurology, 19, 639-651.

Stilwell, J.M., Crowe, T.K., & McCallum, L.W. (1978).Postrotary nystagmus duration as a function of communi­cation disorders. Amerioan Journal of Occupational Therapy, 32, 222-228.

Weeks, Z.R. (1979). Effects of the vestibular system on human development, part 1: Overview of functions and effects of stimulation. Amerioan Journal of Oooupation­al Therapy, 33, 377-381.

Weeks, Z.R. (1979). Effects of the vestibular system on human development, part 2: Effects of vestibular stimu­lation on mentally retarded, emotionally disturbed, and learning disabled individuals. American Journal of Oo­oupational Therapy, 33, 450-457.

Wing, L. (1970). Perceptual and language development in autistic children: A comparative study. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Infantile autism: Concepts, oharaoteristios and treatment (pp. 173-195). London, England: Churchill Livingstone.

Wing, L. (Ed.) (1976). Early childhood autism. Oxford, England: Pergaraon Press.

Wing, L., Gould, J., Yeates, J.R., & Brierly, L.M. (1977). Symbolic play in severely mentally retarded and in au­tistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy­chiatry , 18, 167-78.

Wolkowicz, R., Fish, J., & Schaffer, R. (1977). Sensory integration with autistic children. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 44, 171-75.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.