10
Emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence of children with cerebral palsy in special education Marieke Peeters a, *, Jan de Moor b , Ludo Verhoeven b a Science Hub, Institute for Science Innovation and Society, Radboud University, the Netherlands b Behavioral Science Institute, Radboud University, the Netherlands 1. Introduction Before children learn tot read, they have acquired emergent literacy skills, such as auditory perception, phonological awareness, vocabulary, and syntactic skills. These skills turn out to be good predictors of early reading, and children who lack behind in these skills have often a hard time acquiring literacy skills. This is especially true for children with cerebral palsy. According to Bax et al. (2005, p. 572), cerebral palsy can be described as ‘‘a group of disorders of the development of movement and posture, causing activity limitations, that are attributed to non-progressive disturbances that occurred in the developing fetal or infant brain. The motor disorders of CP are often accompanied by disturbances of sensation, cognition, communication, perception, and/ or behavior, and/ or by seizure disorders’’. Disturbances in speech-related abilities and intellectual disabilities (Dormans & Pellegrino, 1998) occur in the majority of children with CP (cf. Mirenda & Mathy-Laikko, 1989). Peeters, Verhoeven, van Balkom, and de Moor (2008) have shown that children with CP lack behind in emergent literacy skills compared to their normally developing peers. Moreover, Peeters, Verhoeven, and de Moor (2009) have shown that the level of speech intelligibility in children with CP has a long-term impact on emergent literacy and word decoding. Although the child’s impairments play an important role in their literacy development, they are not the only factor of influence. External factors influencing literacy learning in these children such as the school environment turn out to be important as well. Recently, studies in classrooms have highlighted the importance of examining the curriculum or the activities that promote literacy learning, such as the child’s access to printed materials and opportunities to learn (Koppenhaver & Yoder, 1992). Katims (1991, 1994) conducted a study to explore Research in Developmental Disabilities 32 (2011) 659–668 ARTICLE INFO Article history: Received 23 November 2010 Received in revised form 1 December 2010 Accepted 4 December 2010 Available online 15 January 2011 Keywords: Emergent literacy Instruction Cerebral palsy Special education ABSTRACT The goal of the present study was to get an overview of the emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence of children with cerebral palsy (CP) compared to normally developing peers. The results showed that there were differences between the groups regarding the amount of emergent literacy instruction. While time dedicated to storybook reading and independent picture-book reading was comparable, the children with CP received fewer opportunities to work with educational software and more time was dedicated to rhyming games and singing. For the children with CP, the level of speech, intellectual, and physical impairments were all related to the amount of time in emergent literacy instruction. Additionally, the amount of time reading precursors is trained and the number of specific reading precursors that is trained is all related to skills of emergent literacy. ß 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. * Corresponding author at: Science Hub, Institute for Science Innovation and Society, Radboud University, Room HG02.812, PO BOX 9010, 6500 GL Nijmegen, the Netherlands. Tel.: +31 0243652119. E-mail address: [email protected] (M. Peeters). Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Research in Developmental Disabilities 0891-4222/$ – see front matter ß 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2010.12.002

Emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence of children with cerebral palsy in special education

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence of children with cerebral palsy in special education

Emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and schoolabsence of children with cerebral palsy in special education

Marieke Peeters a,*, Jan de Moor b, Ludo Verhoeven b

a Science Hub, Institute for Science Innovation and Society, Radboud University, the Netherlandsb Behavioral Science Institute, Radboud University, the Netherlands

1. Introduction

Before children learn tot read, they have acquired emergent literacy skills, such as auditory perception, phonologicalawareness, vocabulary, and syntactic skills. These skills turn out to be good predictors of early reading, and children who lackbehind in these skills have often a hard time acquiring literacy skills.

This is especially true for children with cerebral palsy. According to Bax et al. (2005, p. 572), cerebral palsy can bedescribed as ‘‘a group of disorders of the development of movement and posture, causing activity limitations, that areattributed to non-progressive disturbances that occurred in the developing fetal or infant brain. The motor disorders of CPare often accompanied by disturbances of sensation, cognition, communication, perception, and/ or behavior, and/ or byseizure disorders’’. Disturbances in speech-related abilities and intellectual disabilities (Dormans & Pellegrino, 1998) occurin the majority of children with CP (cf. Mirenda & Mathy-Laikko, 1989). Peeters, Verhoeven, van Balkom, and de Moor (2008)have shown that children with CP lack behind in emergent literacy skills compared to their normally developing peers.Moreover, Peeters, Verhoeven, and de Moor (2009) have shown that the level of speech intelligibility in children with CP hasa long-term impact on emergent literacy and word decoding. Although the child’s impairments play an important role intheir literacy development, they are not the only factor of influence. External factors influencing literacy learning in thesechildren such as the school environment turn out to be important as well. Recently, studies in classrooms have highlightedthe importance of examining the curriculum or the activities that promote literacy learning, such as the child’s access toprinted materials and opportunities to learn (Koppenhaver & Yoder, 1992). Katims (1991, 1994) conducted a study to explore

Research in Developmental Disabilities 32 (2011) 659–668

A R T I C L E I N F O

Article history:

Received 23 November 2010

Received in revised form 1 December 2010

Accepted 4 December 2010

Available online 15 January 2011

Keywords:

Emergent literacy

Instruction

Cerebral palsy

Special education

A B S T R A C T

The goal of the present study was to get an overview of the emergent literacy activities,

instructional adaptations and school absence of children with cerebral palsy (CP)

compared to normally developing peers. The results showed that there were differences

between the groups regarding the amount of emergent literacy instruction. While time

dedicated to storybook reading and independent picture-book reading was comparable,

the children with CP received fewer opportunities to work with educational software and

more time was dedicated to rhyming games and singing. For the children with CP, the level

of speech, intellectual, and physical impairments were all related to the amount of time in

emergent literacy instruction. Additionally, the amount of time reading precursors is

trained and the number of specific reading precursors that is trained is all related to skills

of emergent literacy.

� 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

* Corresponding author at: Science Hub, Institute for Science Innovation and Society, Radboud University, Room HG02.812, PO BOX 9010, 6500 GL

Nijmegen, the Netherlands. Tel.: +31 0243652119.

E-mail address: [email protected] (M. Peeters).

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Research in Developmental Disabilities

0891-4222/$ – see front matter � 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2010.12.002

Page 2: Emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence of children with cerebral palsy in special education

and document ways in which literate behaviors may be promoted in a group of preschool children with mild to moderatedisabilities. The classroom procedures that were conducted to promote literate behaviors were immersing the children in aliterate environment with repeated daily reading of familiar and predictable books. These activities resulted in theemergence of literate behaviors such as increases in emergent storybook reading, and better understanding of the conceptsof print. Moreover, Mike (1995) conducted an ethnography study of one self-contained classroom at a school for childrenwith CP. Factors that inhibited literacy learning of these children was restriction of instruction time, over reliance onindividual instruction, and lack of student literate interactions. In addition, factors that facilitated literacy learning were theroom as a text-rich environment, the latitude often given to children to govern their own literate behavior, regularlyconducting story reading sessions, and the constructive use of computers. Moreover, the role of computers in promotingliteracy for children with severe disabilities has been documented (Steelman, Pierce, & Koppenhaver, 1993) and research hasindicated that using computerized interactive books, i.e., interactive multimedia stories, is a valuable tool for enhancing thedevelopment of emergent literacy of children with severe disabilities (Hetzroni & Schanin, 2002). These studies show thatliterate behaviors and skills can be promoted in children with impairments such as in the case for children with CP, in orderto prevent later literacy failure. Furthermore, these studies stress the need for early curriculum programs that consist ofclassroom activities that promote these skills, before the start of formal reading.

Yet, studies are conflicting whether the often occurring additional impairments of children with CP influence theopportunities these children receive for literacy learning. Light and Kelford Smith (1993) stated that the educationalopportunities may often compete with other priorities, such as medical or therapeutic appointments. In a similar vein, Smith(2001) suggested that the physical limitations may restrict the degree of active child-initiated or independent learning.Furthermore, according to Bishop, Byers Brown, and Robson (1990), school absence in children with disabilities turns out tobe more frequent than in children without disabilities. In addition, research of Peeters, Verhoeven, de Moor, and van Balkom(2009b) indicated that teachers’ literacy expectations are lower for children with CP compared to normally developing peers.However, Jenks, de Moor, van Lieshout, and Withagen (2010) showed that children with CP in special education receiveddramatically less instruction time in arithmetic than children with CP and normally developing children in regular schools,not because of more school absence, but because less instruction time was planned due to paramedical therapy.

Although previous studies have shown that literacy skills of children with CP can be promoted by providing themadequate instruction, little is known about what classroom activities kindergarten children with CP are exposed to incomparison with their normally developing peers. In order to more fully comprehend the literacy learning opportunities ofchildren with CP, a nation-wide exploratory study was conducted in the Netherlands of 52 kindergarten children with CPattending special education, and a comparison group of peers without impairments attending mainstream schools. First, aninventory of the children’s curriculum was made by investigating the amount of emergent literacy activities the groups areexposed to, together with the amount of instructional adaptations, and amount of school absence. For children with CP, datawas also collected regarding the amount of type of therapies. Additionally, the emergent literacy activities, instructionaladaptations, and school absence were all related to their level of emergent literacy skills and their level of speech, physical,and intellectual impairments. The following questions were the focus of the present study:

1. Are there differences in the amount of emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence betweenthe groups? If so/yes, what nature are these differences?

2. To what extent are the amount of emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations, and school absence of childrenwith CP related to their current level of emergent literacy and the child’s physical, speech, and intellectual impairments?

2. Method

2.1. Participants

All 32 schools for children with physical and multiple disabilities in the Netherlands were asked to participate. Writtenconsent of the children’s parents with CP was obtained.

Inclusion criteria for the children with CP were: Dutch as their native language, an intellectual level ranging from a mildintellectual disability to average intelligence or above, hearing and vision within the normal range, being able to respondintentionally, either through speaking or by means of alternative communication (e.g., looking, pointing or gesturing) and achronological age between 60 and 75 months old.

Fifty-two children with CP participated in the present study: 33 boys (63.5%) and 19 (36.5%) girls. Fifty children (96.1%)had spastic CP and 2 children had ataxia (3.8%). Of the children with spastic CP, 13 children (26.0%) had quadriplegia, 22children (44%) had diplegia, 8 children (16%) had hemiplegia, 5 children (10%) had a combination of spastic-ataxic CP, 1 child(2%) had spastic-hypotonia CP, and 1 child (2%) had spastic-dyskinetic CP. Nine of the 52 children (17.3%) had seizures. Thespeech-language therapists of the children reported that 24 children (46.2%) had no speech difficulties, 10 children (19.2%)had mild dysarthria, 6 children (11.5%) had moderate dysarthria, 9 children (17.3%) had severe dysarthria and were unable tospeak, 1 child (1.9%) had moderate dyspraxia, and 2 children (3.8%) had a combination of severe dysarthria and dyspraxia.Twelve children (23.1%) use some sort of Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) to communicate. The averageage of the group children with CP was 72.00 months (SD = 5.70). All children with CP attended special schools for childrenwith physical and multiple disabilities across the Netherlands. The comparison group consisted of 71 children without

M. Peeters et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 32 (2011) 659–668660

Page 3: Emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence of children with cerebral palsy in special education

impairments attending the second year of kindergarten in five different mainstream schools. The average age of comparisongroup was 72.21 months (SD = 3.69). The groups did not differ significantly in chronological age, t(121) =�.26, p> .05. Allchildren were participating in a longitudinal study to the emergent literacy development of children with CP (cf. Peeters,Verhoeven, & de Moor, 2009; Peeters et al., 2008).

2.2. Materials

A teacher questionnaire was used concerning questions about emergent literacy activities of children with cerebralpalsy that occurred in the context of Dutch kindergarten, instructional adaptations and school absence. Regarding child’simpairments, standardized tasks for speech intelligibility and intelligence were assessed, and two questionnaires wereused to assess both the fine as well as the gross motor skills. Also, standardized task of emergent literacy wereadministrated

2.2.1. Teacher questionnaire

A written questionnaire was sent to teachers to obtain information about: amount of time dedicated to emergent literacyactivities, instructional grouping, method adaptations, school absence and paramedical therapy. This self-administratedquestionnaire was composed of open-ended and multiple-choice items, and was constructed partly based on earlier research(Jenks et al., 2010). Fifty of the 52 (91.16%) of the questionnaires were returned.

2.2.1.1. Amount of time dedicated to emergent literacy activities. Four classroom activities were chosen as they wereconsidered important activities for the emergent literacy of children and occur frequently in Dutch Kindergarteneducation. These classroom activities were: storybook reading, training reading precursors, independent picture-bookreading and using educational software. First, the teachers were asked to mark for each activity separately if the childparticipated in that specific activity. Then, the teachers had to indicate how many times a week the child participated in thatactivity and to indicate the average amount of time the child spent on a that specific activity. To get an impression of the total

amount of time per week the child spent doing a specific activity, these numbers were multiplied resulting in a score thatreflected the average amount of time per week (in minutes) the child participated in that activity. For example, if teacherreported that a child participated in the activity independent picture-book reading three times a week for 20 min, the childwas considered to do this activity on average 60 min/week. A short description of these emergent literacy activities wasprovided to the teachers. Also, the amount of time the child received emergent literacy instruction outside the classroomwas asked.

Storybook reading. During this activity the teacher reads a familiar or unfamiliar storybook to the children. Usually,the children sit in a circle and listen to the story, are able to ask questions about the story or the teacher may ask thechildren questions about the story. Through communicating about aspects of the story children learn to understand thestory line.

Independent picture-book reading. If the child is not yet able to read the text of the story, the child can look through thepicture-book while looking at pictures and making up a story. Sometimes classrooms have a reading-corner where childrencan sit comfortably and read picture-books independently.

Training reading precursors. During this activity the teachers give instruction to the whole class or to an individual child, orchildren do exercises. Teachers were asked to indicate which reading precursors the child is training in the last half year ofkindergarten. The following reading precursors were verified: auditory perception, rhyme, memory, auditory synthesis,auditory analysis, and letter knowledge.

Type of reading precursors. Six yes–no questions addresses which of the mentioned reading precursors the child wastraining.

Educational software. During this activity the child sits in front of a computer and plays language games on the computer,such as matching a word with a picture, making a rhyme with pictures, listening to a story told by the computer andanswering questions about the story, or learning the alphabet.

Instruction time outside classroom. One open-ended question concerned the amount of emergent literacy instruction timethe child received outside the classroom, for example, during remedial teaching or during speech and language therapy.

2.2.1.2. Instructional adaptations. Three questions were asked regarding instructional grouping and method adaptations.Instructional grouping. This categorical question concerned the type of instructional grouping the child received most

often during instruction aimed at promoting emergent literacy skills such as training reading precursors. Response optionswere: whole class or group instructions, individual instruction, and both group and individual instructions.

Types of method. This categorical question addressed the type of method that is most often used for the particular child.Response options ranged from: official method, official method as a source book, self-constructed materials. Combinationswere possible.

Method adaptations. Five questions concerned the instructional adaptations that applied to the particular child. The firstyes–no question concerned if the particular child received any adaptations. If so, the teacher could mark which of thefollowing adaptations applied to the child: slower speed, additional instruction, additional exercises, or enlarging thematerials. A combination of two or more adaptations was possible.

M. Peeters et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 32 (2011) 659–668 661

Page 4: Emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence of children with cerebral palsy in special education

2.2.1.3. School absence and therapy. School absence. One categorical question concerned the amount of school absence sincethe start of the school year. The response options ranged from: 1 (almost) no absence, very little absence (0–25% of the time),small part absence (around 25%), half of the time absent (around 50%), large part absent (around 75%), to 6 almost completelyabsent (around 100%).

Therapy. One open-ended question concerned the amount average amount of therapy the child has in a regular week, oneopen-ended questionnaire regarding the average total therapy time the child has in a regular week, and one open-endedquestion in which the amount of instruction the child missed on average cause of therapy time.

2.2.2. Emergent literacy skills

2.2.2.1. Auditory perception. To assess auditory perception abilities, the auditory perception task of the standardized DutchLanguage Proficiency Test was administrated (Verhoeven & Vermeer, 2001). In this task, the child was presented withminimally differing word pairs and had to indicate whether the words in a pair sounded alike. Response adaptations forchildren with speech difficulties consisted of nodding or pointing to left or right to indicate if the words sounded the same ordifferent. All items were tested and the maximum score was 50. The task was highly reliable with a Cronbach’s alpha of .97(Verhoeven & Vermeer, 1999).

2.2.2.2. Rhyme. The rhyme perception (Irausquin, 2001) task was used to measure emergent phonological awareness. Thetask consisted of 10 highly frequent Dutch CVC word pairs. The child had to decide, aided or unaided, whether the auditorilypresented word pairs rhymed or not. After four successive failures the task was ended. The internal consistency in this studywas sufficient with a Cronbach’s alpha of .73.

2.2.2.3. Phonological awareness. To test phonological awareness skills, the first-phoneme recognition task was used (deJong, van Otterloo, & Regtvoort, 2006). This task consists of 10 items with CVC words. Each item consists of five pictures,one stimulus picture and four response pictures. During the exercise items, the test assistant pointed at the stimuluspicture and named that picture (e.g., roof). Subsequently, the test assistant explained that the stimulus word could besplit up in two parts, the first-phoneme of that word versus the rest of the word (e.g., rrrrr-oof). The test assistantsubsequently named all four response pictures with explicitly emphasis on the first-phoneme of the words. The childhad to point at one of the four response pictures that started with the same first-phoneme as the stimulus word (i.e., r).During the test items, the test assistant named the stimulus picture and the response pictures without explicitlyemphasizing the first-phoneme of the word. The internal consistency of this task in this study was good with aCronbach’s alpha of .83.

2.2.2.4. Verbal working memory. The task was based on a serial-recognition experiment of Gathercole, Pickering, Hall, andPeaker (2001) and did not require physical or speech production abilities. In the newly constructed task, the child heard astring of words and after 2 s the child heard another string of words (e.g., [boat], [knife], [cap], versus [boat], [window], [cap]).The child had to decide whether the two successive strings of words were identical or not. The task consisted of strings ofwords that increased in length, starting with a length of one word and increasing to a length of eight words. For this task, a setof 10 highly frequent monosyllabic consonant–vowel–consonant (CVC) words were used which occurred in a list of wordsused in the context of kindergarten education (Schaerlaekens et al., 1999) and differed phonologically and semantically asmuch as possible from each other. There were a total 48 items; 6 items of each string length. If the child had only three or lessitems of a string length correct, the task was ended. The internal consistency of this task in this study was very high with aCronbach’s alpha of .95.

2.2.2.5. Vocabulary. Receptive vocabulary was assessed using a Dutch version (Schilichting, 2005) of the Peabody PictureVocabulary Test III (Dunn & Dunn, 1997). In this standardized test, the task of the child was to point to one of the four picturesthat corresponded with the word spoken by the test assistant. Raw scores were converted to standard scores (M = 100,SD = 15). The test manual described an internal consistency interval of Cronbach’s alpha between .92 and .94.

2.2.2.6. Syntactic skills. To assess the syntactic skills a Syntactic Pattern subtest of the Dutch Language Proficiency Test wasadministrated (Verhoeven & Vermeer, 1986). In this task, the child heard a sentence which constitutes a syntactic pattern,and had to indicate which of the three pictures matches that syntactic pattern. All 32 items were scored. The task was highlyreliable with a Cronbach’s alpha between .90 and .97 (Verhoeven & Vermeer, 1999).

2.2.3. Child’s impairments

2.2.3.1. Speech intelligibility. In order to assess children’s speech production ability, the standardized Word Articulation taskof the SLI Screening test was administrated (Verhoeven, 2006). In the Word Articulation task the child was asked to repeatreal words. Words were presented one-by-one by a computer with recorded voice, whereby the task started with wordscontaining only one syllable and increased to words containing up to five syllables. When a child made five successive errors,the task was ended. Eighty-four percent of the children with CP were able to do this task since they had some level of

M. Peeters et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 32 (2011) 659–668662

Page 5: Emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence of children with cerebral palsy in special education

understandable speech; the other children who were given a score of zero. The maximum score was 40. The test manualreported a good internal consistency with Cronbach’s alpha of .94 for the Word Articulation task.

2.2.3.2. Intelligence. Nonverbal reasoning was measured with the Raven Coloured Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1956). Thistask measures nonverbal reasoning with a minimal interference of language and is a commonly used instrument to assessintelligence, or general reasoning ability in the nonspeaking population (cf. Dahlgren Sandberg, 1998). Children were askedto point to the correct one of the six pictures that completed the presented figure. The task consisted of 36 items. Raw scorewere converted to standard scores ranging from .5 to 9.5 using Dutch norms (Van Bon, 1986). The reliability of the test isconsidered satisfactory (Evers, van Vliet-Mulder, & Groot, 2000). This task was administrated six months earlier during ameasurement of the longitudinal study.

2.2.3.3. Fine motor function. The fine motor function skills were assessed by means of the Dutch version of the Manual AbilityClassification System (MACS) for children with CP (Eliasson et al., 2006). The purpose of the MACS is to assess the child’sability to handle objects and his or her need for assistance or adaptation in order to be able to perform tasks in everyday lifeon five different levels. A particular emphasis is placed on handling objects in an individual’s personal space. According to themanual, the higher the level that is applicable the more the child is impaired in his or her fine motor skills, but to makeinterpretations easier the levels were recoded. Therefore, level one refers to children who cannot handle objects and haveseverely limited abilities to perform even simple actions, while level five refers to handling objects easily and successfully.Research has indicated that the MACS has good validity and reliability (Eliasson et al., 2006).

2.2.3.4. Gross motor function. To assess the gross motor function of children with CP, a Dutch version of the Gross MotorFunction Classification System (GMFCS) (Palisano et al., 2000) was used. The GMFCS consists of five levels based on self-initiated movement with particular emphasis on sitting and walking. The purpose of the GMFCS is to determine what levelbest represents the child’s present abilities and limitations in motor function. Level one refers to walking withoutrestrictions, while in level five self-mobility is severely limited even with the use of assistive technology. Research hasindicated that the GMFCS has good reliability and validity. The interrater reliability of two blinded raters applied four timesduring the study was high (G = .93), even as the test–retest reliability (G = .79).

2.2.4. Procedure

Concerning the motor functions of the children with CP, a small questionnaire was sent to a pediatric neurologist or aspecialist in rehabilitation medicine who was familiar with the child. Intelligence, speech intelligibility, and emergentliteracy task were assessed by individually testing the children in a quiet room in their schools by an instructed test assistant;an assistant teacher was present as well for the children with CP. Prior to these tasks, there was a training phase, to make surethe children understood the tests. Response adaptations for children with speech difficulties for were nodding or pointing,aided or unaided.

2.2.5. Statistical method

Statistical analyses were performed to provide answers to the research questions. First of all, descriptive statistics(means, standard deviations) were computed for all emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations, and schoolabsence. Then, multivariate analyses of variance between the groups were calculated regarding the amount and time ofemergent literacy activities. Chi-square analyses were conducted to investigate if the groups differed in the percentage ofreading precursors that were trained in the classrooms and in the type of method adaptations. A Mann–Whitney test wasused to compute if the groups differed in the amount of school absence. For the group of children with CP, it was examined towhat extent the emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence were related to their level ofemergent literacy skills and the level of child’s impairments by means of spearman’s correlation analyses.

3. Results

3.1. Amount of emergent literacy training

MANOVA analyses showed that there is a main effect of GROUP regarding the time dedicated to the specific emergentliteracy activities in the classrooms, Wilk’s lambda = .500, F(5, 95) = 18.99, p< .001,and hp

2 = .50. Univariate tests pointed outthat this main effect of Group applied only to the following two activities: Rhyming games and singing, and Educationalsoftware. Children with CP engaged in more rhyming activities and were using less educational software. Table 1 also showsthat there were differences between the groups regarding the amount of book reading activities, such as storybook readingand independent picture-book reading. In addition, children with CP were receiving more emergent literacy instructionoutside the classroom. In addition, the group of children with CP on average spent more time outside the classroom foradditional instruction, such as remedial teaching or speech-language therapy. As much as 45% of the children with CPreceived emergent literacy instruction outside the classroom compared to only 4% of the comparison group.

Although the time dedicated to reading precursors did not differ significantly between the groups (p< .05), this does notmean that all children were training the same reading precursors. Table 2 shows the percentage of children of each group

M. Peeters et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 32 (2011) 659–668 663

Page 6: Emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence of children with cerebral palsy in special education

that were training specific reading precursors. The percentage of children with CP that were learning the different readingprecursors was significantly lower for almost all reading precursors (all ps <.05), except for Letter Knowledge (p> .05). Thepercentage of children in both groups that were learning the graphemes of the alphabet was comparable. The average totalamount of reading precursors that were trained in the group of children with CP was 3.69 (SD = 1.69), while the children inthe comparison group trained an average 5.28 (SD = .72) reading precursors, these differences were significant,t(117) =�7.06, p< .001. From Table 2, we can conclude that although the amount of time the groups were training thediverse reading precursors was comparable, children with CP were mostly training the more easy reading precursors such asauditory perception and rhyme. Skills of phonemic awareness, such as auditory synthesis and auditory analysis were trainedin less then half of the children with CP.

3.2. Instructional adaptations

There were no differences regarding instructional grouping, x2(1, N = 120) = 4.37, p = .113, and h = .17. Most children ofboth the comparison (76.1%) and the group of children with CP (63.3%) were receiving instruction with the whole class orgroup, i.e., 54 and 31 children respectively, followed by both whole class or group instruction, and individual instruction, andonly a minority group of children with CP received mostly individual instruction.

However, there were significant differences between the groups regarding the instructional method used for the children,x2(6, N = 119) = 30.99, p< .001, and h = .51. A large percentage of children in the comparison group received a combination ofan official method and self-constructed materials, while the sort of method for the children with CP was more divided overthe different types and combinations.

Table 3 shows that children with CP received more adaptations in emergent literacy method. This applied to all types ofadaptations (all ps <.05). Furthermore, the children with CP received in total more types of adaptations (M = 1.94, SD = 1.31)compared to the comparison group (M = .53, SD = .94), t(117) = 6.80, p< .001.

Table 1

Amount of time dedicated to emergent literacy activities and instruction time outside the classroom for the children with CP and the comparison group.

Comparison CP F df hp2 p

M SD N M SD N

Emergent literacy activities in class (min)a

Storybook reading 70.16 36.21 64 63.24 18.52 37 1.17 (1,101) .01 .282

Rhyming games and singing 43.98 12.99 64 66.89 23.40 37 40.15 (1,101) .29 <.001

Independent picture-book reading 27.80 13.15 64 25.89 21.35 37 .308 (1,101) .00 .580

Educational software 18.24 14.71 64 4.73 10.67 37 23.90 (1,101) .19 <.001

Training reading precursors 31.63 20.47 64 38.45 29.42 37 1.87 (1,101) .02 .174

Instruction time outside classroom (min) 2.11 10.55 71 22.33 30.50 47 26.26 (1,117) .19 <.001a Missing values were excluded list wise.

Table 2

Percentage of reading precursors trained in both groups.

Reading precursors trained Comparison CP x2 N h p

Yes % N Yes % N

Auditory perception 63 88.7 71 32 66.7 48 8.66 119 .27 .003

Rhyming 71 100 71 45 93.8 48 4.55 119 .20 .033

Memory games 71 100 71 40 83.3 48 12.69 119 .33 <.001

Auditory synthesis 69 97.2 71 22 45.8 48 41.97 119 .59 <.001

Auditory analysis 67 94.4 71 18 37.5 48 45.38 119 .62 <.001

Letter knowledge 34 47.9 71 21 42.9 49 .30 120 .05 .587

Table 3

Differences in method adaptations for the comparison group (N = 71) and the children with CP (N = 48).

Group x2 N h p

Comparison CP

N % N %

Method adaptations 23 47.9 40 56.3 29.83 119 .50 <.001

Tempo/speed (i.e., slower) 7 14.6 33 46.5 44.51 119 .62 <.001

Additional instruction 13 27.1 27 38.0 18.47 119 .39 <.001

Additional exercises 17 35.4 23 32.4 7.38 119 .25 .007

Enlarging materials 1 2.1 10 14.1 12.88 119 .33 <.001

M. Peeters et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 32 (2011) 659–668664

Page 7: Emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence of children with cerebral palsy in special education

3.3. School absence and therapy

Regarding school absence, the Mann–Whitney test indicated that the group of children with CP were more absent duringa school year compared to the comparison group, U(49, 71) = 1185.50, p = .001 (two-tailed). Furthermore, on average, thechildren with CP were having 5.30 (SD = 2.17, range 1–11) therapies a week, i.e., a total of 163.15 min (SD = 99.62). Whilesome children were only having one therapy, some children were having a total of 11 therapies a week. Ten children (20.40%)were missing emergent literacy instruction cause of different therapies, with an average of 41.00 min (SD = 16.63)instruction time each week.

3.4. Relationship between emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations, and school absence with emergent literacy skills

for children with CP

Table 4 shows that there are systematic positive relationships between the amount of instruction time regardingstorybook reading and training reading precursors with different skills of emergent literacy. The better children with CP arein, for example, Rhyming and Memory, the more instruction they receive in training these reading precursors. The better thechildren are in different skills of emergent literacy, the more reading precursors are trained. Or in other words, the morereading precursors are trained the better the children are in different emergent literacy skills. These results suggest that theamount of instruction children with CP is not suited to skills of the children. Children who have difficulty with, for example,auditory perception or phonemic awareness skills do need relatively more instead of less instruction compared to childrenwho are already good at these skills.

3.5. Relationship between amount of emergent literacy activities and instructional adaptations with child impairments for the

children with CP

The same patterns are visible when the child’s levels of impairments are related to the amount of instruction time,instructional adaptations, and school absence. Table 5 shows that children with relatively good skill in speech intelligibility

Table 4

Spearman correlations between aspects of the amount of instruction, instructional adaptations, and school absence with emergent literacy skills for

children with cerebral palsy.

Aud Rhyme Phon STM Voc Syntax

Storybook reading (time) .19 .26*,y .33* .32* .11 .25y

Rhyming games and singing (time) .03 �.09 .01 �.13 .12 .03

Independent picture-book reading (time) .13 .19 .18 .09 .03 .18

Educational software (time) .09 �.03 �.05 �.02 �.05 .00

Training reading precursors (time) .46** .37* .49*** .44** .49*** .44**

Amount of reading precursors trained (total) .53** .33* .34* .41** .36* .42**

Instruction outside classroom �.07 .02 �.07 �.11 �.07 �.06

Amount of therapy �.06 .02 �.10 �.19 �.22 �.05

Missing instruction cause of therapy �.03 .03 �.06 .00 .04 .06

School absence .09 .04 �.12 �.06 �.10 .11

Aud, auditory perception; Phon, phonemic awareness; STM, auditory STM; Voc, vocabulary; and Syntax, syntactic awareness.* p< .05.** p< .01.*** p< .001.y p< .10.

Table 5

Relationship (Spearman’s rho) between the amount of instruction, instructional adaptations, and school absence with child’s impairments.a

Speech intelligibility Intelligence Fine motor skills Gross motor skills

Storybook reading (time) .26 .44** �.19 �.31*

Rhyming games and singing (time) �.04 .11 .05 �.06

Independent picture-book reading (time) .12 .19 �.18 �.26

Educational software (time) �.17 .06 .14 .05

Training reading precursors (time) .38* .33* �.32* �.48**

Amount of reading precursors (total) .29* .34* �.26 �.33*

Instruction outside classroom �.31* .08 �.06 .05

Number of adaptations in method �.38** �.23 .16 .24

Amount of therapy .19 .10 �.19 .12

Missing instruction cause of therapy .00 �.06 �.01 �.21

School absence �.14 .06 .32* .35*

a Missing variables are excluded analysis by analysis.* p< .05.** p< .01.

M. Peeters et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 32 (2011) 659–668 665

Page 8: Emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence of children with cerebral palsy in special education

receive relatively more instruction time in training reading precursors and are trained more diverse reading precursorscompared to children with speech impairments. The same pattern applies to their intelligence and physical skills.Furthermore, the more impaired the fine or gross motor skills, the more the child was absent in the last school year. Childrenwith impairments in fine motor function have more adaptations in the methods. Furthermore, children with good speechintelligibility receive more instruction inside the classroom, while children with relatively less good speech intelligibilitiesreceive more instruction time outside the class, such as with a speech-language therapist or remedial teacher. Table 5 showsfurther that there are marginally significant relationships between the level of speech intelligibility with the amount ofstorybook reading, and the level of gross motor skills with independent picture-book reading (all ps <.01)

4. Conclusions and discussion

From the present study, several conclusions can be drawn. First of all, instruction time for children with cerebral palsy andtheir normally developing peers in regular schools differs for some emergent literacy activities. Educational software is anactivity that takes place less often in the classrooms of children with CP, and more importantly, only one out of five childrenwith CP play with educational software in the classroom. Previous research has indicated that educational software is avaluable tool for promoting emergent literacy skills, such as phonological awareness (Hetzroni & Schanin, 2002). Speechfeedback in computer-aided instruction can improve the literacy skills of nonspeaking children, as it gives them theopportunity to map oral language onto written language (Steelman et al., 1993). Furthermore, assistive technology, such asswitches and interfaces, can assist in reducing some of the physical barriers and promote opportunities for independence inliteracy development (Hetzroni & Schanin, 2002). The fact that children with CP rarely use this tool cannot fully be explainedby the fact that most educational software is not adapted to the physical needs of these children, since more and moreoptions and tools have become available to use educational software for children with physical disabilities (cf. Segers,Nooijen, & de Moor, 2006; Voort, Hoenkamp, Hulstijn, & de Moor, 1993). However, it seems that most children for whomthese tools would be necessary or supportive to learn independently have no access to assistive technology. An increased useof educational software constitutes an opportunity for schools for children with physical disabilities to promote theemergent literacy development of these children.

Another difference in instruction time between the groups is that children with CP were spending relatively more timedoing rhyme and auditory perception activities and less time doing phonemic awareness activities such auditory analysisand auditory synthesis. An explanation for the fact that children with CP were relatively more time spending in rhymingactivities and auditory perception can be found in the fact these skills are the first phonological awareness skills that emerges(Wagner et al., 1997). Rhyme, which indicates onset-rime awareness, is one of the first phonological awareness skills thatemerge. Auditory analysis and auditory synthesis require the awareness of the individual phonemes of the words, which ismuch more difficult that onset-rime awareness. The same explanation is true for the fact that more children with CP wereonly training auditory discrimination and rhyme abilities, while only a minority of the children is learning skills of phonemicawareness. Phonemic awareness is only trained in less than half of the children with CP while it is one of the best predictorsof early reading success. The results show further that within the classrooms of children with CP; relatively much time isdedicated to storybook reading. Reading storybooks to children is a meaningful en enjoyable activity for most children,probably also for the children with CP. Although storybook reading in general is considered to be a stimulant for the languagedevelopment of the children such as book orientation skills and vocabulary growth, is has less clear relationships with worddecoding skills (de Jong & Leseman, 2001). Conclusively, emergent literacy instruction in special education classrooms forchildren with CP is mostly concerned with relatively easy, meaningful and less abstract skills, such as phonemic awareness.

The present study has pointed out that the amount of instruction time of the children with CP is related to their level ofspeech, intellectual, and physical impairments. The more severe these impairments, the lesser instruction time in emergentliteracy and the less specific reading precursors are trained. For children with speech impairments, the results showed thatthe more severe their speech impairments the more instruction they were receiving outside the classroom, probably inremedial teaching or in speech language therapy, and the more adaptations take place in methods. These results indicate thatthe instruction for children with CP with speech impairments is adapted to their abilities and needs.

Another finding from the present study is that for children with CP the amount of time they receive instruction in readingprecursors and the more different reading precursors are trained the better their skills of emergent literacy are. The resultsindicate that it is effective to train reading precursors in the way it is done in special education.

The present study has shown that especially children with severe speech, intellectual, and physical impairments are atrisk for receiving insufficient instruction time in emergent literacy skills, as the amount of instruction time in the class intraining reading precursors is related to the level of child’s impairments. Although the children with CP with severe speechimpairments receive additional instruction time outside the classroom, this is insufficient for these children to develop theemergent literacy skills as a similar level as their classmates. Especially these children who are at risk for limited literacylearning should receive all the possible support to prevent them for emergent literacy and reading problems.

The present study has some limitations. To begin with, correlation analyses pointed out that the levels of the child’sspeech intelligibility, intelligence, and motor function are strongly related to the amount of diverse reading precursors thatthey trained and the amount of instruction time for training reading precursors. From these results it could be speculatedthat teachers were only training the skills which they think the child is ready to learn, suggesting that they only react on whatthey see the child is able to do, instead of looking to the learning potential of the children. Longitudinal studies are needed to

M. Peeters et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 32 (2011) 659–668666

Page 9: Emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence of children with cerebral palsy in special education

shed more light on the influence instruction and training time of reading precursors have of the level of abilities. Anotherlimitation of the present study is that we looked at the different classroom activities that took place during a school weekglobally. Children with CP and accompanying speech difficulties often have speech-language therapy which stimulates theiremergent literacy skills as well, although these activities were not taken into account. Furthermore, the studied activitieswere very global indicators of what happened in the classrooms. There may be important differences between theclassrooms in what exactly is done during these activities and the quality of the activities, so these aspects should be studiedmore detailed.

With an eye on educational practice, we suggest that teachers should collect information about the IndividualEducational Plan (IEP) of the children with CP. With reference to a study on the quality of arithmetic education for childrenwith CP, Jenks et al. (2010) concluded that the majority of the IEPs did not include well-formulated arithmetic goals andmany were not based on optimal assessment. It can thus be recommended that school teachers should look more closely atthe quality of arithmetic and literacy instruction by including information about the IEPs of these children.

Another clinical implication of this study is that instruction time in emergent literacy skills and paramedical therapyshould be more closely adapted to each other in order to make sure that children with CP will miss as less literacy instructionas possible. This is especially important to make sure these children will not lack behind in their literacy development.

Acknowledgements

This study is funded by the European Union, Interreg 4-BMG-V-1=31 and Viataal (St. Michielsgestel, The Netherlands).Special appreciation is expressed to the children and teachers who participated, and the clinical experts of the participatingschools.

References

Bax, M., Goldstein, M., Rosenbaum, P., Leviton, A., Paneth, N., Dan, B., et al. (2005). Proposed definition and classification of cerebral palsy. Developmental Medicineand Child Neurology, 47, 571–576.

Bishop, D. V. M., Byers Brown, B., & Robson, J. (1990). The relationship between phoneme discrimination, speech production, and language comprehension incerebral-palsied individuals. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33(2), 210–219.

Dahlgren Sandberg, A. (1998). Reading and spelling among nonvocal children with cerebral palsy: Influence of home and school literacy environment. Reading andWriting, 10(1), 23–50.

de Jong, P. F., & Leseman, P. M. (2001). Lasting effects of home literacy on reading achievements in school. Journal of School Psychology, 39(5), 389–414.de Jong, P. F., van Otterloo, S. G., & Regtvoort, A. G. F. M. (2006). Fonologische bewustzijn taken voor kleuters: Rijm en beginfoneem [Phonological awareness tasks for

kindergarten children: Rhyme and first-phoneme]. Unpublished manuscript, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands.Dormans, J. P., & Pellegrino, L. (1998). Caring for children with cerebral palsy: A team approach (1st ed.). Baltimore: Paul H Brookes.Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Eliasson, A. C., Krumlinde-Sundholm, L., Rosblad, B., Beckung, E., Arner, M., Ohrvall, A. M., et al. (2006). The Manual Ability Classification System (MACS) for

children with cerebral palsy: Scale development and evidence of validity and reliability. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 48(7), 549–554.Evers, A., van Vliet-Mulder, J. C., & Groot, C. J. (2000). Documentatie van tests en testresearch in Nederland Documentation of tests and test research in the

Netherlands. Assen/Amsterdam: Van Gorcum/NIP Dienstencentrum.Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Hall, M., & Peaker, S. M. (2001). Dissociable lexical and phonological influences on serial recognition and serial recall. Quarterly

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 54A, 1–30.Hetzroni, O. E., & Schanin, M. (2002). Emergent literacy in children with severe disabilities using interactive multimedia stories. Journal of Developmental Physical

Disabilities, 14(2), 173–190.Irausquin, R. (2001). Rijmperceptie, Rijmproductie en Beginrijm [Rhyme perception, rhyme production and alliteration]. Unpublished Manuscript, Radboud

University of Nijmegen, The Netherlands.Jenks, K. M., de Moor, J., van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. , & Withagen, F. (2010). Quality of arithmetic education for children with cerebral palsy. International Journal of

Rehabilitation Research, 33(1), 19–25.Katims, D. S. (1991). Emergent literacy in early childhood education: Curriculum and instruction. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 11(1), 69–84.Katims, D. S. (1994). Emergence of literacy in preschool children with disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 17, 58–69.Koppenhaver, D. A., & Yoder, D. E. (1992). Literacy learning of children with severe speech and physical impairments in school setting. Seminars in Speech and

Language, 31, 143–153.Light, J., & Kelford Smith, A. K. (1993). Home literacy experiences of preshoolers who use AAC systems and of their nondisabled peers. Augmentative and Alternative

Communication, 9(1), 10–25.Mike, D. G. (1995). Literacy and cerebral palsy: Factors influencing literacy learning in a self-contained setting. Journal of Reading Behavior, 27(4), 627–642.Mirenda, P., & Mathy-Laikko, P. (1989). Augmentative and alternative communication applications for persons with severe congenital communication disorders:

An introduction. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 5(1), 3–13.Palisano, R. J., Hanna, S. E., Rosenbaum, P. L., Russel, D. J., Walter, S. D., Wood, E. P., et al. (2000). Validation of a model of gross motor function for children with

cerebral palsy. Physical Therapy, 80(10), 974–985.Peeters, M., Verhoeven, L., & de Moor, J. (2009). Teacher literacy expectations of kindergarten children with cerebral palsy in special education. International

Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 32(3), 251–259.Peeters, M., Verhoeven, L., de Moor, J., & van Balkom, H. (2009). Importance of speech production for phonological awareness and word decoding: The case of

children with cerebral palsy. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30(4), 712–726.Peeters, M., Verhoeven, L., van Balkom, H., & de Moor, J. (2008). Foundations of phonological awareness in preschool children with cerebral palsy: The impact of

intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 52(1), 68–78.Raven, J. C. (1956). Guide to using the coloured progressive matrices. London: HK Lewis.Schaerlaekens, A., Kohnstamm, D., Lejaegere, M., de Vries, A., Peeters, L., & Zink, I. (1999). Streefwoordenslijst voor 6-jarigen (3rd ed.). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.Schilichting, L. (2005). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III-NL. Amsterdam: Harcourt Test Publisher.Segers, E., Nooijen, M., & de Moor, J. (2006). Computer vocabulary training in kindergarten children with special needs. International Journal of Rehabilitation

Research, 29(4), 343–345.Smith, M. M. (2001). Simply a speech impairment? Literacy challenges for individuals with severe congenital speech impairments. International Journal of

Disability, Development and Education, 48(4), 331–353.

M. Peeters et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 32 (2011) 659–668 667

Page 10: Emergent literacy activities, instructional adaptations and school absence of children with cerebral palsy in special education

Steelman, J. D., Pierce, P. L., & Koppenhaver, D. A. (1993). The role of computers in promoting literacy in children with severe speech and physical impairments(SSPI). Topics in Language Disorders, 13(2), 76–88.

Van Bon, W. H. J. (1986). Raven’s coloured progressive matrices: Manual of Dutch norms. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.Verhoeven, L. (2006). ESM-screenings instrument SLI-screening test. Arnhem: Cito.Verhoeven, L., & Vermeer, A. (1986). Taaltoets Allochtone Kinderen Language proficiency test for children with Dutch as a second language. Arnhem, The

Netherlands: Cito.Verhoeven, L., & Vermeer, A. (1999). Cognitive factors in early first and second language proficiency. In (Series Ed.) & C. Perdue, & R. Rast (Vol. Eds.), La personne

bilingue: Vol. 1. Actes du 8e colloque EUROSLA-conference Paris (pp. 97–110). Paris: Universite Paris.Verhoeven, L., & Vermeer, A. (2001). Taaltoets Alle Kinderen (TAK) Language proficiency test for all children. Arnhem: Cito.Voort, R., Hoenkamp, E., Hulstijn, W., & de Moor, J. (1993). An evaluation on the use of computer applications for severely physically handicapped children. In F. J.

Maarse, A. E. Akkerman, A. N. Brand, L. J. M. Mulder, & M. J. van der Stelt (Eds.), Computers in psychology: Applications, methods, and instrumentation (pp. 30–39).Berwyn, PA: Lisse.

Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., et al. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities andword-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled reading: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33(3), 468–479.

M. Peeters et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 32 (2011) 659–668668