30
Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Experimental PsychologyPSY 433

Chapter 8

Attention and Reaction Time

Page 2: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Where’s Waldo?

http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=EvWh6PMi9Ek&feature=player_embedded#

Page 3: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Two Aspects of Attention

Divided attention – what happens when we try to engage in two cognitive processes at once

Selective attention – how we switch mental resources from one cognitive task to another.

Page 4: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Visible Bottleneck Task

http://opl.apa.org/contributions/Pashler/prp.html

This task illustrates how difficult it is to pay attention to two things at the same time. Both tasks require a choice of response and

the same cognitive resource cannot be devoted to both tasks at the same time.

The competition goes away when one task does not involve a choice (e.g., press any button when you hear a tone).

Page 5: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Donders A, B & C Revisited

A is simple RT – see a stimulus and press a key

B is stimulus choice RT – see one of two stimuli and decide whether to press a key or not

C is stimulus and response choice RT – see one of two stimuli and decide which of two keys to press

B-C gives response selection time C-A gives ID time.

Page 6: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Donders Tasks

S1 R1 Donders A

S1 R1 Donders BS2 R2

S1 R1 Donders CS2

Page 7: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Donders A -- Simple

C Reaction Time

C Minus A

A Reaction Time

Baseline Identification Time

Selection Time

Page 8: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Donders B -- Choice

B Reaction Time

B Minus C

C Reaction Time

Page 9: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Testing for Modularity

Donders A, B & C implies that the parts of the task are modules.

How can component modules be identified? When one component module can be

changed without changing the others, it is independent.

If Donders was correct then the three parts should be separately modifiable. Pure insertion – addition of a module without

affecting the duration of the other modules.

Page 10: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Response Force

Donders pure insertion could not be tested so an additional variable was added – response force.

Response force – the amount of pressure exerted on a response key. Force increases with stimulus intensity.

For Ulrich et al. (1999), response force was the same for a Donders A and B comparison, even though RTs were different.

Page 11: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

RT and Integrated Force (Fig 8.3)

This result is consistent with pure insertion.

Page 12: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

B and C Reactions (Fig 8.4)

The B and C RT’s should differ but they do not.

Page 13: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

A and C Reactions (Fig 8.5)

Now RT’s differ as they should but force is not consistent with pure insertion.

Page 14: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Confounding Stimulus Intensity

Confound – when one or more independent variable is simultaneously varied so we can’t tell which is responsible for an effect.

In Ulrich et al.’s experiment (green LED to left or right), two things were varied: Mapping of stimulus to response (one hand vs

two) Apparent brightness (focusing on a single light

instead of both lights) Replaced by letters X and S not lights.

Page 15: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

The Same Experiment using Letters not LED Lights (Fig 8.6)

Now RT differs as it should and Force supports pure insertion.

Page 16: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Results using Letters

When X vs S is used, the confound of stimulus intensity is eliminated (controlled) and the results support pure insertion.

Part of the problem is that all three Donders experiments were not presented: Authors wished to avoid transfer effects that

occur in within-subject experiments. To avoid this, present all three Donders

conditions or do the experiment between-subject.

Page 17: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Speed Accuracy Tradeoffs

RT cannot be used as the only dependent variable because subjects change accuracy to maximize speed: Speed & accuracy are sometimes inversely

related. RT & Accuracy must be jointly considered.

Examining more than one dependent variable may be crucial to understanding the processes involved in a task.

Page 18: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Theios (1973)

Subjects had to name a digit presented visually.

Probability of the digit was varied from 0.2 to 0.8.

Reaction time was unaffected by probability of the digit, but accuracy was greatly affected. Highest error rate with lowest probability.

To increase accuracy (keep error rate constant), RTs would have to increase.

Page 19: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

RT and Error Rate as a Function of Stimulus Probability

RT stays constant but as stimulus probability increases, errors decrease.

Page 20: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Dual Task Processing

Page 21: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff

SOA (S1-S2 Interval in ms)

Page 22: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

A Central Bottleneck

We can only process one thing at a time within a single modality (vision, hearing).

Central cognition may be the most important bottleneck – the central bottleneck.

Whether two tasks can be done at once depends on whether they compete for the same resources. Schumacher dual-task experiment.

Page 23: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Stimulus Onset Asynchrony (SOA)

Pashler presented a modified Donders B task in which S1 and S2 were not presented simultaneously. The interval between them is called stimulus

onset asynchrony (SOA) The shorter the SOA, the greater the effect on

RT and errors. The period where one task interferes with the

other is called psychological refractory period.

Page 24: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Dual Task Processing

Page 25: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Pashler’s Paradigm

Task 1 – hear a tone and press a key with the left hand.

Task 2 – vocally call out the name of the highest digit in a display of eight digits.

When subjects are not required to respond quickly to Task 2, accuracy is not affected. It only occurs with a requirement to make a

speedy response.

Page 26: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Capacity Sharing Explanation

S1

S2

Response selection

Response selection

The resource is shared during the time when the tasks overlap.

Page 27: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Explanations

Central bottleneck models – some common internal processing stage is required by both tasks, creating a bottleneck for resources.

Central capacity sharing models – a resource called capacity must be split across the two tasks, reducing the capacity available to either task and reducing efficiency.

Both models predict the same observed results – the author prefers capacity models.

Page 28: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Stress and Cognitive Control

Two possible effects of stress on cognition: Stress requires attention (capacity) thus

decreasing performance. People adapt to stress by finding more

efficient ways of doing tasks increasing performance (strategies change).

Steinhauser’s experiment: Long/short interval cues to respond to digit or

letter – 6M Is letter a consonant/vowel, is digit odd/even?

Page 29: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Results

For the low stress condition there was an interaction between stimulus interval and task repetition (same task next or different next). No interaction in the high stress condition.

Under low stress there was a relatively higher cost to changing tasks quickly.

Under high stress the cost was the same. This is consistent with the idea that under

stress cognitive strategies change.

Page 30: Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 8 Attention and Reaction Time

Change Blindness Demo

http://viscog.beckman.illinois.edu/flashmovie/23.php