Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/9/2019 Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy

    1/11

    Neuropsychology

    Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy: Evidencefor Self-Orientation as a Neuropsychological Foundationof EmpathyBrick Johnstone, Dan Cohen, Kirk R. Bryant, Bret Glass, and Shawn E. Christ

    Online First Publication, November 17, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000155

    CITATION

    Johnstone, B., Cohen, D., Bryant, K. R., Glass, B., & Christ, S. E. (2014, November 17).

    Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy: Evidence for Self-Orientation as a

    Neuropsychological Foundation of Empathy. Neuropsychology. Advance online publication.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000155

  • 8/9/2019 Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy

    2/11

    Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy: Evidence forSelf-Orientation as a Neuropsychological Foundation of Empathy

    Brick Johnstone, Dan Cohen, Kirk R. Bryant, Bret Glass, and Shawn E. ChristUniversity of Missouri

    Objective:To evaluate a model that hypothesizes that empathy is associated with decreased right parietal

    lobe (RPL)-related self-orientation (i.e., increased selflessness), which allows individuals to more easily

    empathize with others.Methods:Participants: Thirty one individuals with documented neuroradiological

    abnormalities due to traumatic brain injury (TBI) referred for clinical evaluations. Measures: Cerebral

    integrity was measured with both functional (i.e., neuropsychological tests) and structural indices (i.e.,

    MRI). Participants were administered comprehensive neuropsychological tests associated with general

    bilateral frontal, temporal, and parietal lobe functioning, a self-report measure of empathy (i.e., Penners

    Prosocial Personality Battery), and an objective measure of empathy (i.e., Prisoners Dilemma). Twenty

    participants also completed structural MRI analysis of the bilateral frontal, temporal, parietal, and insular

    cortices measured in terms of volume. Results:Pearson correlations indicated that empathy was related

    toincreasedneuropsychological indices of RPL and frontal lobe (primarily left frontal) functioning. The

    only MRI indices associated with empathy were the bilateral insula. Neither functional nor structuralcerebral indices were significantly related to objective measures of empathy. Conclusions:Contrary to

    hypotheses, empathy appears to be associated with increased RPL functioning. It is suggested that to

    incorporate the experiences of others into the experience of the self (i.e., to be empathetic), one must have

    an intact sense of the self.

    Keywords:affective empathy, cognitive empathy, self-orientation, right parietal lobe, neuropsychology

    Neuroanatomy of Empathy

    There has been growing interest in determining the neuroanat-

    omical and neuropsychological foundations of empathy (hereby

    defined as the action of understanding, being aware of, being

    sensitive to, and vicariously experiencing the feelings, thoughts,and experience of another). A meta-analysis of empathy research

    concluded that empathy is a complex construct that is related to

    multiple neurologic networks and cognitive and emotional abilities

    (Zaki & Ochsner, 2012). Specifically, empathy has been shown to

    be related to the inferior parietal lobe, temporoparietal junction,

    anterior insula, posterior superior temporal sulcus, temporal pole,

    premotor cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, anterior cingulate cor-

    tex, medial prefrontal cortex, insula, right temporoparietal region,

    and various regions of the frontal lobes (Banissy, Kanai, Walsh, &

    Rees, 2012; Decety & Jackson, 2004; Decety & Jackson, 2006;

    Eslinger, 1998; Grattan, Bloomer, Archambault, & Eslinger, 1994;

    Grattan & Eslinger, 1989; Grattan & Eslinger, 1992; Mutschler,

    Reinbold, Wankerl, Seifritz, & Ball, 2013; Preston & de Waal,2002; Shamay-Tsoory, Tomer, Goldsher, Berger, & Aharon-

    Peretz, 2004; Zaki & Ochsner, 2012). Considered together, these

    studies implicate most of the brain in empathy, which is not

    surprising given its complexity. This variability in findings is

    likely related to the lack of clear definitions of empathy, method-

    ological weaknesses inherent in neuroradiological scanning (Logo-

    thetis, 2008), and other methodological issues (e.g., use of self-

    report vs. objective measures of empathy; artificiality of

    objective empathy measures; structural vs. functional measures of

    cerebral integrity; use of clinical and nonclinical samples; Zaki &

    Ochsner, 2012).

    The majority of research on the neurology of empathy has

    involved neuroradiological evaluation of nonclinical populations

    engaged in objective computer-based tasks. However, determining

    associations between empathy and cerebral regions is complicated

    given the numerous other cognitive processes that are engaged

    during objective tasks of empathy (e.g., attending to stimuli, lis-

    tening to instructions, perceiving stimuli, cognitively processing

    economic games commonly used in empathy research, etc.; Logo-

    thetis, 2008). As a result, the neuroanatomical basis of empathyhas been difficult to determine. An alternative research method has

    involved the investigation of populations with brain dysfunction in

    which persons with various lesions/disorders are compared with

    one another (e.g., persons with left vs. right frontal lobe lesions),

    or to healthy control groups, in terms of empathy (Beadle &

    Tranel, 2013; de Sousa et al., 2010; Eslinger, 1998; Grattan et al.,

    1994; Grattan & Eslinger, 1989; Grattan & Eslinger, 1992; Rankin,

    Kramer, & Miller, 2005; Rankin et al., 2006; Shamay-Tsoory et

    al., 2004; Shamay-Tsoory, Aharon-Peretz, & Perry, 2009). Using

    this method, empathy can be associated with different cerebral

    areas (i.e., damage to areas associated with empathy will be

    Brick Johnstone, Department of Health Psychology, University of Missouri;

    Dan Cohen, Department of Religious Studies, University of Missouri; Kirk R.

    Bryant, Department of Health Psychology, University of Missouri; Bret Glass,

    Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri; Shawn E.

    Christ, Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri.

    Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Brick John-

    stone, Department of Health Psychology, University of Missouri, DC116.88,

    Columbia, MO 65212. E-mail: [email protected]

    y

    p

    y

    Neuropsychology 2014 American Psychological Association2014, Vol. 29, No. 1, 000 0894-4105/14/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000155

    1

  • 8/9/2019 Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy

    3/11

    indicative of decreased empathy). However, a weakness in such

    research is that only general relationships can be inferred given the

    diffuse nature of most brain disorders. Whereas the first method

    may lead to overidentification of the cerebral areas/functions spe-

    cifically associated with empathy, the latter one can only be used

    to make general inferences regarding brain-empathy relationships.

    In fact, it has been suggested that the two methodologies areappropriate complements to one another (Rorden & Karnath,

    2004).

    Neuropsychology of Empathy

    In addition to identifying the cerebral areas associated with

    empathy, research has also focused on determining the specific

    affective and cognitive components of empathy. Empathy has been

    conceptualized as relating to numerous cognitive processes includ-

    ing attention, memory, perspective taking, theory of mind, abstract

    reasoning, cognitive flexibility, generational fluency, and set shift-

    ing (Rankin et al., 2005). More concisely, empathy is generally

    conceptualized as involving two primary neuropsychological pro-

    cesses including the ability to cognitively take the perspective of

    others (i.e., cognitive empathy) and the ability to feel the emotions

    of others as if they were ones own (i.e., affective empathy;

    Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009; Shamay-Tsoory, 2011). Studies of

    empathy involving persons with brain dysfunction have primarily

    focused on the associations between empathy and cognitive flex-

    ibility (Grattan et al., 1994; Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009; Grattan &

    Eslinger, 1989), with suggestions that increased cognitive flexibil-

    ity allows individuals to take the perspective of others. Other

    studies suggest that empathy is related to declarative memory

    (Beadle & Tranel, 2013), as well as empathetic concern (Rankin et

    al., 2005; Rankin et al., 2006).

    Right Hemisphere, Self-Orientation, and Empathy

    Empathy has been hypothesized to be related to self/other

    differentiation and self/other overlap (Zaki & Ochsner, 2012).

    However, the specific nature of such self/other neuropsychological

    processes has not been fully elaborated. Recent research on other

    complex human experiences/traits (i.e., transcendence, forgive-

    ness) suggests that empathy may have its neuropsychological

    foundations in decreased self-orientation associated with the right

    hemisphere, and particularly the right parietal lobe (RPL). Re-

    search indicates that persons with increasing RPL dysfunction,

    from either brain injuries or tumors, report increased spiritual

    transcendence (Johnstone, Bodling, Cohen, Christ, & Wegryzn,

    2012; Johnstone & Glass, 2008; Urgesi, Aglioti, Skrap, & Fabbro,

    2010). This increased transcendence is hypothesized to be related

    to decreased self-orientation, or what can be termed increased

    selflessness. These studies are further supported by single-

    photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) research on Bud-

    dhist monks and Franciscan nuns who demonstrate decreased RPL

    activity during advanced spiritual practices while reporting in-

    creased feelings of selflessness (Newberg, Alavi, Baime, Mozley,

    & dAquili, 1997; Newberg et al., 2001; Newberg, Pourdehand,

    Alavi, & dAquili, 2003).

    Two recently published studies suggest that decreased RPL

    functioning is also related to increased willingness to forgive in

    populations with both traumatic brain injury (TBI; Johnstone et al.,

    2012) and seizures (Johnstone et al., 2014). These studies suggest

    that to forgive, one must be willing to decrease focus on the

    perceived wrong to the self.

    Based on these studies, it is hypothesized that empathy may also

    be related to decreased RPL-related self-orientation. Specifically,

    it is suggested that one must be selfless to experience spiritual

    transcendence, be willing to forgive, and to be empathetic. Thishypothesis is also based on studies that indicate that the right

    hemisphere, and particularly the RPL, is primarily related to pro-

    cessing information that is related to the self (Austin, 2009;

    Northoff et al., 2006). Numerous studies have consistently dem-

    onstrated that the right hemisphere is related to several self-

    referential processes, such as recognizing pictures of ones self and

    processing autobiographical information/memories (Decety &

    Sommerville, 2003; Keenan, McCutcheon, & Pascual-Leone,

    2001; Keenan, Nelson, OConnor, & Pascual-Leone, 2001; Leigh

    et al., 2013; Lou et al., 2004; Platek, Wathne, Tierney, & Thom-

    son, 2008; Uddin, Molnar-Szakacs, Zaidel, & Iacoboni, 2006).

    The relationship between the RPL and self-orientation is also

    supported by research with individuals with RPL dysfunction.

    They often experience disorders of the self, including anosag-

    nosia and left-sided neglect, in which they have difficulties

    creating/processing a coherent sense of the physical and/or

    psychic self (Feinberg & Keenan, 2005b; Feinberg & Keenan,

    2005a; McGlynn & Schacter, 1989; Mesulam, 2000). When

    considered together, these studies suggest that decreased RPL-

    related self-orientation (which can be conceptualized as in-

    creased selflessness) may also allow individuals to more

    easily understand and relate to the experiences of others (i.e., to

    be empathetic).

    Rationale for Current Study

    It was hypothesized that decreased integrity of the RPL (whichis associated with decreased self-orientation) would be related to

    increased empathy. To address weaknesses in empathy research to

    date, empathy was measured by subjective and objective measures,

    and cerebral integrity was measured in terms of both functional

    and structural indices. Although the focus of the study was on the

    relationship between the RPL and empathy, relationships among

    empathy and indices of other cerebral areas were also investigated

    to assist in clarifying the neurologic and neuropsychological foun-

    dations of empathy.

    Methods

    ParticipantsThe sample included 31 individuals with TBI referred for out-

    patient neuropsychological evaluations at a Midwestern university.

    All participants had abnormal clinical radiologic evaluations of the

    brain. Neuropsychological tests and self-report measures of empa-

    thy were completed by all 31 participants. MRI data for 20

    participants was obtained and analyzed for research purposes. Data

    were unavailable for the remaining participants due to either

    preexisting counterindications (e.g., metal in body; n 7) or

    technical problems at the time of testing (n 4). Chi-square and

    ttests conducted between the 20 participants with and 11 partic-

    ipants without MRI scanning indicated there were no significant

    y

    p

    y

    2 JOHNSTONE, COHEN, BRYANT, GLASS, AND CHRIST

  • 8/9/2019 Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy

    4/11

    differences in neuropsychological, demographic, or outcome mea-

    sures between the groups, other than marital status (with the group

    not receiving MRI having a higher percentage of divorce).

    The average age of the sample was 34.6 years (SD 16.2; range

    1787). The mean Wechsler Adult Scale of Intelligence III, Full Scale

    IQ (WAIS-3 FSIQ) of the sample was in the average range (WAIS-3

    FSIQ 93.4;SD 15.9; Wechsler, 1997a). The WAIS-3 FSIQ wasnot significantly correlated with any measure of empathy. Demo-

    graphic characteristics of the entire sample are presented in Table 1.

    TBI injury severity characteristics are presented in Table 2.

    Procedure

    All participants were referred for clinical neuropsychological

    evaluations and had evidence of abnormal neuroradiological eval-

    uations at the time of their injuries. They were administered

    measures of the functional integrity of the brain including intelli-

    gence, memory, language, visual-spatial skills, attention, and

    sensory-motor skills by psychometricians (see Table 3). Partici-

    pants provided informed consent and completed subjective mea-

    sures of empathy (see Table 4). Participants were then scheduledfor MRIs (i.e., measures of the structural integrity of the brain),

    where they were screened for contraindications to scanning and

    also completed an objective measure of empathy. Participants werecompensated $100. The present study was approved by the uni-

    versitys research review board.

    Measures of Empathy

    Penner Prosocial Personality Battery (Penner, Fritzsche,

    Craiger, & Freifeld, 1995). This measure was used to assess

    several dimensions of empathy and has been used in several

    peer-reviewed studies (Finkelstein, Penner, & Brannick, 2005;

    Penner, 2002; Penner & Finkelstein, 1998; Skoe, Cumberland,

    Eisenberg, Hansen, & Perry, 2002). The Empathy scale assessesthe ability to feel responsibility for and concern about the welfare

    of others. This scale is divided into four different scores, including

    a total empathy score, as well as individual scores for each of the

    following three dimensions of empathy: Empathetic Concern, Per-

    spective Taking, and Personal Distress. All questions are rated on

    Table 1

    Sample Demographics

    N Percent

    GenderMale 21 67.7Female 10 32.3

    HandednessRight 28 90.3Left 3 9.7

    EthnicityWhite 28 90.0African American 1 3.2Biracial 2 6.5

    Marital StatusMarried 9 29.0Divorced 5 16.1Separated 1 3.2Single 16 51.6

    EducationSome HS 4 12.9HS Diploma 13 41.9Some College 7 22.6Bachelors Degree 3 9.7Masters Degree 3 9.7PhD/JD/MD 1 3.2

    Employment StatusCurrently Employed 12 38.7Student 5 16.1Unemployed, not on disability 12 38.7Unemployed, on disability 1 3.2Retired 1 3.2

    Annual Income$20,000 25 80.6$20,001$40,000 3 9.7$40,001$60,000 1 3.2$60,001$80,000 1 3.2$80,000 1 3.2

    Table 3

    Neuropsychological Measure Descriptive Statistics

    N Min. Max. Mean SD

    TMT (z score) 31 19.52 1.18 3.58 4.86WMS-3 LM I

    (scaled score) 31 2.00 13.00 7.35 3.01BVMT-R Delay

    (tscore) 30 20.00 66.00 46.27 13.77JOLO (z score) 31 1.75 1.08 0.02 0.86Letter Fluency

    (scaled score) 31 3.00 19.00 8.42 3.85Agnosia RH

    (errors) 30 0 4 0.70 1.06Agnosia LH

    (errors) 30 0 2 0.37 0.56

    Note. TMT Trail Making Test Part B; WMS-3 LM I WechslerMemory Scale III Logical Memory I; BVMT-R Benton Visual MemoryTest Revised; JOLO Judgment of Line Orientation; RH right hand;LH left hand.

    Table 2

    Sample Brain Injury Characteristics

    N Percent

    Loss of ConsciousnessYes 18 58.1

    No 5 16.1Unknown/Missing 8 25.8Retrograde Amnesia

    Yes 9 29.0No 0 0.0Unknown/Missing 22 71.0

    Posttraumatic AmnesiaYes 14 45.2No 0 0.0Unknown/Missing 17 54.8

    Effected Brain Regions on ImagingLeft Frontal 9 29.0Right Frontal 7 22.6Left Temporal 7 22.6Right Temporal 7 22.6Left Parietal 6 19.4Right Parietal 4 12.9

    Occipital 2 6.5

    y

    p

    y

    3SELF-ORIENTATION AND EMPATHY

  • 8/9/2019 Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy

    5/11

    a five point scale, from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).

    The score for each scale is the total number endorsed for each

    respective scale. The Total Empathy score is the sum of each of the

    following three subscales.

    Empathetic concern. This subscale includes four questions

    and assesses the degree to which individuals are emotionally

    concerned with the well-being of others. It includes questions such

    as: When I see someone being taken advantage of, I feel kind of

    protective toward them; I am often quite touched by things that

    I see happen. To be consistent with the terminology of previous

    studies, Empathetic Concern is conceptualized as affective empa-

    thy. Higher scores suggest more empathetic concern. Cronbachs

    alpha for the measure was .67 for the original sample (Penner,

    2002; n 1,111).

    Perspective taking. This subscale includes five questions and

    assesses the degree to which individuals can take the perspective

    of others. It includes questions such as: I sometimes try to

    understand my friends better by imagining how things look from

    their perspective; When I am upset at someone, I usually try to

    put myself in their shoes for a while. To be consistent with the

    terminology from previous studies, Perspective Taking is concep-tualized as cognitive empathy. Higher scores suggest a greater

    capacity to take others perspective. Cronbachs alpha for the

    measure was .66 for the original sample (Penner, 2002; n

    1,111).

    Personal distress. This subscale includes three questions and

    assesses the degree to which individuals have difficulties dealing

    with stressful situations. Example questions include: I tend to lose

    control during emergencies; I am usually pretty effective in

    dealing with emergencies; and When I see someone who badly

    needs help in an emergency, I go to pieces. Higher scores suggest

    a higher degree of distress. Cronbachs alpha for the measure was

    .77 for the original sample (Penner, 2002; n 1,111).

    Prisoners Dilemma task. This computer task that has been

    used as an objective measure of empathy/altruism (Batson &

    Moran, 1999). Participants are engaged in a task in which they are

    told they can cooperate with, or not cooperate with (i.e., defect),

    another person (i.e., the same confederate face on the computer

    screen for each trial), not knowing if the confederate will them-

    selves cooperate or defect. If the participant and confederate both

    choose to cooperate, then they both earn $5. If the participant

    defects but the confederate cooperates, then the participant earns

    $6 and the confederate $1. If the participant cooperates but the

    confederate defects, then the participant earns $1 and the confed-

    erate $6. The participants are administered 15 trials twice, and the

    participant is informed of the confederates response after each

    trial. The total score is the amount of money earned over all trials.

    Higher dollar amounts are indicative of higher levels of coopera-

    tion (and theoretically empathy).

    Functional and Structural Indices of Cerebral

    Integrity

    The current study evaluated both the functional and structural

    integrity of gross cerebral structures that have been shown to be

    related to empathy (i.e., bilateral frontal, temporal, parietal, insular

    cortices). Both structural and functional indices of cerebral integ-

    rity were used given that neuropsychological tests can provide

    different information than neuroradiological techniques (Gomar et

    al., 2011; Schmand et al., 2014). For example, cerebral abnormal-

    ities were detected in only 18% of MRI scans of persons with TBI,

    compared with 40% of SPECT scans and 86% of magnetoen-

    cephalography evaluations (Lewine et al., 2007).

    Neuropsychological Tests

    The following neuropsychological tests were administered as

    indices of thegeneralfunctional integrity of each of the following

    cerebral lobes. Specifically, the tests were not used as indicators of

    specific cognitive functions, but rather to infer the functional

    integrity of the cerebral lobe that is generally associated with the

    test (i.e., finger agnosia tests used as indices of parietal lobe

    functioning, rather than measures of tactile sensitivity; Lezak,

    Howieson, & Loring, 2004). However, it is acknowledged that

    only general relationships could be inferred regarding empathy and

    neuroanatomical foundations, which was determined to be appro-

    priate given the unclear nature of the neurological foundations of

    empathy (Zaki & Ochsner, 2012). For all tests, higher scores

    indicate more intact ability, other than for the finger agnosia tests

    (i.e., higher scores equal worse performance). Age was controlled

    for by calculating standard scores for all tests based on age, otherthan for finger agnosia.

    RPL. The Judgment of Line Orientation (JOLO; Benton,

    Hamsher, Varney, & Spreen, 1983) and left-hand finger agnosia

    tests were used to infer RPL functioning. The JOLO is a measure

    of spatial perception with the total score equaling the number

    correct of the 30 total items. For this study, the score is presented

    as a z-score calculated according to normative data published in

    the test manual, with higher z -scores indicative of better perfor-

    mance. The JOLO, and similar measures of the perception of

    angulation, have been associated with right hemisphere and par-

    ticularly RPL functioning (Benton, Hannay, & Varney, 1975;

    Lezak, Howieson, & Loring, 2004; Meador et al., 1993). Left-

    sided finger agnosia is a measure of tactile perception that is

    associated with the RPL (Lezak et al., 2004). On this test, each of

    the five fingers is touched four times and identified by the partic-

    ipant, with the score equaling the total number of errors. More

    agnosia errors suggest greater RPL dysfunction.

    Left parietal lobe. Right-sided finger agnosia is a measure of

    tactile perception associated with the left parietal lobe (Lezak et

    al., 2004), during which each of the five fingers is touched four

    times and identified by the participant, with the score equaling the

    total number of errors. More agnosia errors suggest greater left

    parietal dysfunction.

    Right temporal lobe. The Brief Visuospatial Memory Test

    Revised (BVMT-R; Benedict, 1997) is a test of visual memory that

    Table 4

    Subjective and Objective Empathy Measures

    Descriptive Statistics

    N M in. Max. Mean SD

    Prosocial Personality Battery

    Total Empathy 31 32 46 39.93 3.79Perspective Taking 31 10 22 17.32 2.91Empathic Concern 31 12 19 15.61 2.08Personal Distress 31 3 12 7.0 2.56

    Prisoners Dilemma Total Score 20 61 79 71.50 4.35

    y

    p

    y

    4 JOHNSTONE, COHEN, BRYANT, GLASS, AND CHRIST

  • 8/9/2019 Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy

    6/11

    has been associated with right temporal lobe functioning (Tranel &

    Damasio, 2002). It involves presenting the participant with three

    trials during which they are visually exposed to six geometric

    figures for 10 s, after which they are asked to draw the figures from

    memory. The score is the total number of details recalled for all

    three trials, which is presented as a tscore based on age-related

    normative data (average tscore 50, SD 10).Left temporal lobe. The Wechsler Memory Scale III

    (WMS-3) Logical Memory (LM) I subtest (Wechsler, 1997b) is a

    measure of verbal memory that has been associated with left

    temporal functioning (Tranel & Damasio, 2002). It involves read-

    ing two narrative stories to the participant that they are asked to

    remember and repeat immediately after presentation. The score is

    the standard score according to age-related norms (i.e., M 10,

    SD 3).

    Left frontal lobe. The Delis Kaplan Executive Function Sys-

    tem Letter Fluency Test (DKEFS; Delis & Kaplan, 2001) is a

    measure of verbal fluency that has been associated with left frontal

    lobe functioning (Johnstone, Leach, Hickey, Frank, & Rupright,

    1995). On this measure, participants state as many words as

    possible in a 1-min period for three different letters. The total score

    is the total number of words generated over the three trials, which

    is expressed as a scaled score based on age-related normative data

    (M 10, SD 3).

    Frontal lobe. The Trail Making Test Part B (Reitan, 1992)

    has been associated with general frontal lobe functioning (John-

    stone et al., 1995). It is a measure of divided attention (also

    described as cognitive flexibility) that involves the participant

    completing a connect-the-dot test, alternating between a series of

    numbers and letters (i.e., 1 to A, A to 2, 2 to B, B to 3, etc.). The

    result is based on time to complete the measure. For this study, the

    score is presented as a z score according to normative data (Hea-

    ton, Miller, Taylor, & Grant, 2004), with higherz scores indicative

    of better performance.

    MRI

    Data acquisition. A 3T Siemens Trio MRI scanner with a

    standard eight-channel head coil was used to obtain high-

    resolution (1 mm3) T1-weighted structural images of the brain.

    Images were collected using a standard T1-weighted pulse se-

    quence (MP-RAGE sequence: repetition time [TR] 1,920 ms,

    echo time [TE] 4 ms, flip angle 8, number of slices 160,

    resolution 1 mm3).

    Data processing and analysis. Data processing and analysis

    was carried out using a surface-based approach as implemented

    within BrainVoyager QX software (Brain Innovation, Maastricht,

    The Netherlands). (For a more extensive description of the Brain-

    Voyager processing pipeline, see http://support.brainvoyager.com

    or Geuze et al., 2008.) First, the structural MRI data for each

    participant was rotated into anterior-posterior commissures (AC-

    PC) coordinates. The skull and dural tissue were then removed by

    manually deleting voxels containing non-neural tissue. Next, a

    Sigma filter and BrainVoyagers automatic intensity inhomogene-

    ity correction tool (IIHC) were applied to enhance grey/white

    matter (GM/WM) tissue contrast and correct for spatial intensity

    inhomogeneities, respectively. Noncortical structures (e.g., ventri-

    cles, subcortical nuclei) were then removed, and the resulting

    image was upsampled from 1 to 0.5 mm3 using sinc interpolation

    (the upsampling is required for use of BrainVoyagers advanced

    segmentation tools). BrainVoyagers built-in segmentation pro-

    cess, which uses local intensity histograms and computed gradient

    fields to adaptively calculate WM, GM, and cerebrospinal fluid

    (CSF) boundaries, was then applied. Importantly, before proceed-

    ing, the resulting segmentations were visually inspected (slice-by-

    slice) for accuracy and manually corrected where necessary. Cor-tical thickness measurements were calculated using a Laplace

    method (Jones, Buchbinder, & Aharon, 2000) as implemented

    within BrainVoyager. For the current study, cortical volume was

    chosen as an index of structural cerebral integrity (i.e., cortical

    thickness surface area), consistent to other neuroradiological

    studies of empathy (Banissy et al., 2012; see Table 5).

    Output from the previously described segmentation process was

    also utilized to create cortical surface reconstructions of each

    hemisphere for each participant. Anatomical alignment of these

    surface representations (and thereby the aforementioned cortical

    thickness measurements) across participants was accomplished

    using BrainVoyagers automatic cortex-based alignment (CBA)

    process (Fischl, Sereno, Tootell, & Dale, 1999; Goebel, Esposito,& Formisano, 2006). CBA represents an iterative adaptive process

    whereby curvature information (representing gyral and sulcal fold-

    ing patterns) is used to align macroanatomical structures (gyri and

    sulci) of each participants brain to a standard reference brain

    provided in BrainVoyager. Following alignment, average cortical

    thickness, surface area, and volume measurements were extracted

    for each major cortical region (frontal, parietal, temporal, insular)

    of each hemisphere (left, right) of each participant.

    Analyses. Pearson productmoment correlations were con-

    ducted among the neuropsychological measures and the Penner

    Prosocial Personality Battery Empathy scales, and the Prisoners

    Dilemma total score (see Table 6). It is noted that age was

    accounted for in the analyses through the use of age-corrected

    normative data for the neuropsychological measures, other than for

    finger agnosia measures. Partial-correlations using age as a cova-

    riate were conducted among MRI-derived measures of cortical

    volume and the Penner scores, and the Prisoners Dilemma total

    score (see Table 7). Statistical significance was set at 0.05, ac-

    knowledging weaknesses inherent in using the relatively small

    sample size.

    Table 5

    MRI Cortical Volume Descriptive Statistics

    N Min. Max. Mean SD

    Left HemisphereLeft Temporal 20 30,628.05 53,710.81 42,241.44 5,425.12Left Frontal 20 41,317.70 79,798.30 59,528.84 8,539.29Left Parietal 20 23,028.63 37,681.60 31,215.89 3,722.84Left Insula 20 6,319.09 9,724.08 8,121.69 900.11

    Right HemisphereRight Temporal 19 30,052.48 47,168.65 40,602.12 4,330.72Right Parietal 20 44,251.37 81,739.61 62,146.05 8,641.45Right Frontal 20 23,802.39 34,333.18 28,024.57 3,026.48Right Insula 20 8,123.95 11,413.30 9,915.95 894.49

    y

    p

    y

    5SELF-ORIENTATION AND EMPATHY

  • 8/9/2019 Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy

    7/11

    Results

    Neuropsychological Indices

    Contrary to hypotheses, the results suggest that empathy is

    generally related to increased functional integrity of the RPL.

    Specifically, measures of increased RPL functioning (i.e., JOLO

    and left-hand finger agnosia tests) were significantly correlated

    with both increased affective empathy and less personal distress.Measures associated with frontal lobe functioning, and particu-

    larly the left frontal lobe (i.e., Trail Making Test and DKEFS

    Letter Fluency subtest), were significantly and positively corre-

    lated with cognitive empathy.

    A measure of left temporal lobe functioning (i.e., WMS-3 LM I

    scale) was significantly and positively associated with cognitive

    empathy.

    No neuropsychological measures were related to Prisoners Di-

    lemma scores.

    MRI Indices

    Right and left insular cortices were the only cerebral structures

    shown by MRI to be significantly related to subjective measures of

    empathy (i.e., Total Empathy, Personal Distress). No MRI indices

    of cerebral integrity were significantly related to Prisoners Di-

    lemma scores.

    Discussion

    Empathy and the RPL

    The primary goal of the study was to determine if decreased

    RPL functioning was related to increased empathy. Contrary to the

    hypothesis, the results suggest that certain aspects of empathy are

    related to increased rather than decreasedRPL functional integ-

    rity. The process of being empathetic apparently is not associated

    with a decreased focus on the self, which allows for an increased

    focus on others. Instead, it appears that to empathize with others

    that it is necessary to have an intact and relatively strong sense of

    the self. That is, if one does not have an intact sense of the self,

    then one cannot fully understand and emotionally relate to theexperiences of others. As stated by Cozolino:

    empathy is an attempt to experience the inner life of another while

    retaining objectivity. In other words, we hold our own perspective in

    mind while simultaneously imagining what it is like to be the other. In

    order to have empathy, we need to maintain an awareness of our inner

    world as we imagine the inner world of others. (Cozolino, 2006, p.

    203)

    Although the results were contrary to those hypothesized, they

    support others studies that suggest that relatively stronger right

    hemisphere functioning, and particularly RPL functioning, is re-

    lated to increased empathy (Decety & Batson, 2009; Grattan &

    Eslinger, 1989; Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2004; Shamay-Tsoory et al.,

    2009). For example, Shamay-Tsoory and colleagues (2004) re-ported that individuals with RPL lesions, but not left parietal lobe

    lesions, demonstrated significantly decreased empathy. Similarly,

    Grattan and Eslinger (1989) found that the JOLO (i.e., the same

    measure used in the current study to infer RPL dysfunction) was

    significantly and positively related to empathy in a sample of

    individuals with brain dysfunction (r .30 vs. r .38 in the

    current study). However, they conceptualized this test as a measure

    of perceptual judgment and did not mention this finding in the

    abstract given the focus of their study was on the frontal lobes. In

    contrast, in the current study, the JOLO was used to infer the

    functional integrity of the RPL. Whereas Grattan and Eslinger

    concluded that impairments in visual-spatial perception could

    potentially be a shared variable among the flexibility and empathymeasures, it is suggested that it may be more appropriate to

    interpret these tests as indicators of general RPL functional integ-

    rity. Based on this premise, it can then be inferred that empathy is

    significantly related to two measures of RPL integrity (i.e., JOLO,

    left finger agnosia), rather than suggesting that empathy is related

    Table 7

    Correlation Analysis Among MRI Cortical Volume and

    Empathy Measures

    MRI Brain Region Total EC PT PDPrisonersDilemma

    Left HemisphereLeft Temporal 0.32 0.18 0.19 0.05 0.04Left Frontal 0.12 0.08 0.13 0.20 0.11Left Parietal 0.33 0.27 0.25 0.04 0.04Left Insula 0.48 0.06 0.25 0.46 0.15

    Right HemisphereRight Temporal 0.12 0.10 0.13 0.26 0.07Right Frontal 0.15 0.17 0.23 0.03 0.12Right Parietal 0.02 0.04 0.02 0.07 0.12Right Insula 0.43 0.18 0.29 0.56 0.04

    Note. Total Total Empathy; EC Empathetic Concern; PT Per-spective Taking; PD Personal Distress; Prisoners Dilemma Prison-ers Dilemma Total Earnings. p .05.

    Table 6

    Correlation Analyses Among Neuropsychological Indices and

    Empathy Measures

    Neuropsychological Total EC PT PDPrisonersDilemma

    Left TemporalWMS-3 LM I 0.24 0.16 0.36 0.14 0.28Left Frontal

    DKEFS Letter Fluency 0.52 0.42 0.42 0.08 0.04Left Parietal

    Right Finger Agnosia 0.07 0.11 0.06 0.08 0.30Right Temporal

    BVMT-R total 0.03 0.24 0.09 0.12 0.07Right Parietal

    JOLO 0.04 0.38 0.12 0.49 0.20Left Finger Agnosia 0.21 0.37 0.27 0.37 0.15

    FrontalTrail Making Test 0.35 0.01 0.39 0.31 0.16

    Note. Total Penner Prosocial Personality Total score; EC Empa-thetic Concern; PT Perspective Taking; PD Personal Distress; Pris-oners Dilemma Prisoners Dilemma Total Earnings; WMS-3 LM I

    Wechsler Memory Scale III Logical Memory I; BVMT-R Benton VisualMemory Test Revised; DKEFS Delis Kaplan Executive FunctionScales; JOLO Judgment of Line Orientation. p .05.

    y

    p

    y

    6 JOHNSTONE, COHEN, BRYANT, GLASS, AND CHRIST

  • 8/9/2019 Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy

    8/11

    to perceptual judgment and left-sided tactile sensitivity. If concep-

    tualized in this manner, it can then be inferred that other RPL

    functions, such as general self-orientation processes (Austin, 2009;

    Decety & Sommerville, 2003; Leigh et al., 2013; Lou et al., 2004;

    Uddin et al., 2006; Feinberg & Keenan, 2005b, 2005a; McGlynn &

    Schacter, 1989; Mesulam, 2000) are also likely to be diminished.

    Of note, the results also indicated that decreased RPL function-ing (i.e., JOLO, left-sided finger agnosia) is associated with higher

    levels of personal distress. That is, less intact RPL functioning

    (and inferentially the sense of self) is associated with a decreased

    ability to keep composure under stressful situations (e.g., I tend to

    lose control during emergencies; When I see someone who

    badly needs help in an emergency, I go to pieces). This suggests

    that a stronger sense of the self allows individuals to retain com-

    posure in stressful situations and increase their ability to focus on

    others. This finding is consistent with beliefs that one must have a

    strong sense of the self to empathize with others (Cozolino, 2006).

    Empathy and Other Cerebral

    Structures/Neuropsychological FunctionsIn the current study, empathy (i.e., affective, cognitive, total)

    was associated with frontal lobe functioning (and particularly the

    left frontal lobe), consistent with several other studies (Decety &

    Batson, 2009; Decety & Jackson, 2004; Grattan et al., 1994;

    Grattan & Eslinger, 1989; Grattan & Eslinger, 1992; Rankin et al.,

    2005; Rankin et al., 2006; Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2004; Shamay-

    Tsoory et al., 2009). In general, current and previous results

    consistently suggest that cognitive flexibility associated with fron-

    tal lobe functioning is necessary for individuals to take the cogni-

    tive perspective of others.

    The results also support previous studies that indicate that the

    temporal lobes are involved in empathy (Decety & Batson, 2009;

    Leigh et al., 2013; Rankin et al., 2006). It has been suggested thatempathy may be related to the emotional processing that has been

    shown to be related to both the temporal lobes and limbic system,

    given their close neural connectivity (Lee & Siegle, 2012; Lezak et

    al., 2004; Rankin et al., 2005). However, somewhat unexpectedly,

    in the current study, cognitive empathy was associated with in-

    creased left temporal lobe functioning, whereas affective empathy

    was not. If anything, it could be hypothesized that affective em-

    pathy would be related to temporal lobe functioning. The rationale

    for this finding is unclear.

    Empathy and Structural Indices of Cerebral Integrity

    The results indicate that the bilateral insular cortices were the

    only structural indices that were significantly associated with

    empathy (i.e., Total Empathy and Personal Distress). This finding

    is generally consistent with several other studies that implicate the

    insula in the experience of empathy (Banissy et al., 2012;

    Mutschler et al., 2013). It is speculated that the insula may be

    related to empathy given that it has been associated with emotional

    processing (and particularly negative emotions such as personal

    distress), awareness of internal bodily states (i.e., self-orientation

    of internal states), and connecting physiological states of the body

    with social emotions such as empathy (Craig, 2009; Critchley,

    Wiens, Rotshtein, Ohman, & Dolan, 2004; Karnath, Baier, &

    Ngele, 2005).

    The lack of significant findings between empathy and other

    cerebral areas does not suggest that empathy is solely based in the

    insula. The lack of findings is more likely related to the fact that

    many individuals with even severe TBI have normal neuroradio-

    logical findings (Brasure et al., 2012; Lobato et al., 1986). This

    suggests that quantitative structural MRI data may help differen-

    tiate groups with brain dysfunction for comparison (e.g., frontal vs.nonfrontal lesions; Grattan, Bloomer, Archambault, & Eslinger

    1994), but may not be appropriate for correlational analyses such

    as those conducted in the current study. Regardless, the current

    results clearly suggest that future empathy research will benefit

    from focusing on the role of the insula.

    Self-Report Versus Objective Measures of Empathy

    The results indicate that self-report measures of empathy (i.e.,

    Penners Prosocial Personality measure) were significantly related

    to neuropsychological indices of cerebral integrity, although the

    objective measure (i.e., Prisoners Dilemma) was not. This finding

    may be reflective of the artificiality associated with using ex-

    perimental tasks to measure empathy (Zaki & Ochsner, 2012), andparticularly for persons with TBI. Although subjective and objec-

    tive measures of a construct may frequently be administered to-

    gether to assure the construct validity of an attribute (e.g., empa-

    thy), in this case the constructs measured by the Penner Prosocial

    Personality Measure and Prisoners Dilemma appear to be differ-

    ent.

    Limitations and Future Directions

    It is acknowledged that empathetic processes are much more

    complicated from neuroanatomical and neuropsychological per-

    spectives than the conclusions presented in this article. However,

    given the weaknesses in empathy research to date (Zaki &

    Ochsner, 2012), the current results can provide suggestions forfuture research, and particularly how self-orientation relates to

    empathy. The current study suggests that different neuropsycho-

    logical processes associated with different neurological networks

    interact in the experience of empathy (e.g., RPL-related self-

    orientation; frontal lobe-related cognitive flexibility; temporal

    lobe-related emotional processing; insula-related processing of

    internal physical states and social emotions).

    To date researchers have generally conceptualized empathy as

    involving processes such as self/other discrimination, self-

    reflection, self/other overlap, and theory of mind (Shamay-

    Tsoory et al., 2009; Zaki & Ochsner, 2012). However, these terms

    are relatively nonspecific and such terminology may not best

    explain the manner by which individuals incorporate the experi-

    ences of others into the experience of the self. A more nuanced

    explanation for the manner by which specific dimensions of self-

    orientation relate to empathy is needed. Just as language can be

    used as a general umbrella term for other more specific commu-

    nication skills (e.g., expression, comprehension, semantics, syntax,

    etc.), self-orientation may be best conceptualized as a general

    umbrella descriptor for more specific self-oriented processes (e.g.,

    autobiographical memory, body schema perception, self-

    awareness, self-recognition, self-evaluation, etc.). In fact, a general

    model of self-orientation has been proposed, with suggestions that

    the self can be conceptualized in terms of a physical self, mental

    self, and autobiographical self (Northoff et al., 2006).

    y

    p

    y

    7SELF-ORIENTATION AND EMPATHY

  • 8/9/2019 Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy

    9/11

    Further research regarding the expression of increased and de-

    creased self-orientation processes is also warranted given the mul-

    tiple studies that suggest that decreased RPL functioning (as mea-

    sured by the JOLO and/or finger agnosia tests) is associated with

    increased transcendence (Johnstone et al., 2012; Johnstone &

    Glass, 2008) andincreasedwillingness to forgive (Johnstone et al.,

    2012; Johnstone et al., 2014), but decreasedempathy (Grattan etal., 1994; current study). Future research may benefit from deter-

    mining how increased and decreased self-orientation may be re-

    lated to other human experiences (e.g., altruism, antisocial person-

    ality, etc.), as well as the manner in which self- and other-oriented

    neuropsychological processes interact in different human experi-

    ences/relationships (Austin, 2009; Fox et al., 2005; Ochsner et al.,

    2004).

    A primary limitation of the current study is the relatively small

    sample size, as the use of a larger sample may lead to a more clear

    understanding of the specific nature of empathy (e.g., allow for

    investigation of more specific brain regions). The results are fur-

    ther limited in that the current sample included persons with TBI,

    which is an injury that typically involves generalized impairmentthroughout the brain (Lezak et al., 2004). As a result, only general

    inferences can be drawn regarding the neuroanatomical and neu-

    ropsychological bases of empathy. Relatedly, the current study is

    limited as the MRI data involving cortical volume was viewed by

    examining large regions as a whole (i.e., left frontal lobe) rather

    than examining more specific regions within each brain region that

    have been suggested to be implicated in different dimensions of

    empathy (Eslinger, 1998; Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009). Not focus-

    ing on more specific brain regions may have decreased the strength

    of the observed relationships and may have contributed to the

    insignificant MRI findings.

    References

    Austin, J. H. (2009). Selfless insight. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Banissy, M. J., Kanai, R., Walsh, V., & Rees, G. (2012). Inter-individual

    differences in empathy are reflected in human brain structure. Neuro-

    Image, 62, 2034 2039. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05

    .081

    Batson, C. D., & Moran, T. (1999). Empathy induced altruism in a

    prisoners dilemma. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 909

    924. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199911)29:7909::

    AID-EJSP9653.0.CO;2-L

    Beadle, J. N., & Tranel, D. (2013). Empathy in hippocampal amnesia.

    Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 091.

    Benedict, R. (1997).Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised professional

    manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Benton, A. L., Hamsher, K. D., Varney, N. R., & Spreen, O. (1983).

    Judgement of line orientation manual. New York, NY: Oxford Univer-

    sity Press.

    Benton, A., Hannay, H. J., & Varney, N. R. (1975). Visual perception of

    line direction in patients with unilateral brain disease. Neurology, 25,

    907910. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.25.10.907

    Brasure, M., Lamberty, G. J., Sayer, N. A., Nelson, N. W., MacDonald, R.,

    Ouellette, J., . . . Wilt, T. J. (2012). Multidisciplinary postacute rehabil-

    itation for moderate to severe traumatic brain injury in adults. Agency for

    Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) Comparative Effectiveness

    Reviews, 72,ES1ES20.

    Cozolino, L. (2006).The neuroscience of human relationships. New York,

    NY: Norton.

    Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feelNow? The anterior insula and

    human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 5970. http://dx

    .doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555

    Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J.

    (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness.Nature Neu-

    roscience, 7, 189 195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1176

    Decety, J., & Batson, C. D. (2009). Empathy and morality: Integrating

    social and neuroscience approaches. In J. Braeckman, J. Verplaetse, & J.De Schrijver (Eds.), The moral brain (pp. 109127). Berlin: Springer-

    Verlag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-6287-2_5

    Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of lesion

    empathy.Behavior, Cognition, & Neuroscience, 3, 71100.

    Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2006). A social-neuroscience perspective on

    empathy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 5458.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00406.x

    Decety, J., & Sommerville, J. A. (2003). Shared representations between

    self and other: A social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends in Cogni-

    tive Sciences, 7, 527533. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.004

    Delis, D., & Kaplan, E. (2001). Delis Kaplan Executive Function System.

    San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    de Sousa, A., McDonald, S., Rushby, J., Li, S., Dimoska, A., & James, C.

    (2010). Why dont you feel how I feel? Insight into the absence ofempathy after severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 48,

    35853595. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.008

    Eslinger, P. J. (1998). Neurological and neuropsychological bases of em-

    pathy. European Neurology, 39, 193199. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/

    000007933

    Feinberg, T. E., & Keenan, J. P. (2005a).The lost self: Pathologies of the

    brain and identity. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. http://dx

    .doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195173413.001.0001

    Feinberg, T. E., & Keenan, J. P. (2005b). Where in the brain is the self?

    Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 14, 661678.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.01.002

    Finkelstein, M. A., Penner, L. A., & Brannick, M. T. (2005). Motive, role

    identity, and prosocial personality as predictors of volunteer activity.

    Social Behavior and Personality, 33, 403418. http://dx.doi.org/

    10.2224/sbp.2005.33.4.403Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., Tootell, R. B., & Dale, A. M. (1999). High-

    resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical

    surface. Human Brain Mapping, 8, 272284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/

    (SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4272::AID-HBM103.0.CO;2-4

    Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C.,

    & Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into

    dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the Na-

    tional Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102,

    96739678. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0504136102

    Geuze, E., Westenberg, H. G. M., Heinecke, A., de Kloet, C. S., Goebel,

    R., & Vermetten, E. (2008). Thinner prefrontal cortex in veterans with

    posttraumatic stress disorder. NeuroImage, 41, 675681. http://dx.doi

    .org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.007

    Goebel, R., Esposito, F., & Formisano, E. (2006). Analysis of functional

    image analysis contest (FIAC) data with BrainVoyager QX: From

    single-subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis

    and self-organizing group independent component analysis. Human

    Brain Mapping, 27,392 401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20249

    Gomar, J. J., Bobes-Bascaran, M. T., Conejero-Goldberg, C., Davies, P.,

    Goldberg, T. E., & the Alzheimers Disease Neuroimaging Initiative.

    (2011). Utility of combinations of biomarkers, cognitive markers, and

    risk factors to predict conversion from mild cognitive impairment to

    Alzheimer disease in patients in the Alzheimers disease neuroimaging

    initiative. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 961969. http://dx.doi

    .org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.96

    Grattan, L. M., Bloomer, R. H., Archambault, F. X., & Eslinger, P. J.

    (1994). Cognitive flexibility and empathy after frontal lobe lesions.

    y

    p

    y

    8 JOHNSTONE, COHEN, BRYANT, GLASS, AND CHRIST

  • 8/9/2019 Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy

    10/11

    Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology, 7, 251

    259.

    Grattan, L. M., & Eslinger, P. J. (1989). Higher cognition and social

    behavior: Changes in cognitive flexibility and empathy after cerebral

    lesions.Neuropsychology, 3, 175185.

    Grattan, L. M., & Eslinger, P. J. (1992). Long-term psychological conse-

    quences of childhood frontal lobe lesion in patient DT. Brain and

    Cognition, 20, 185195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626

    (92)90068-W

    Heaton, R. K., Miller, W., Taylor, M. J., & Grant, I. (2004). Revised

    comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-Reitan Battery. Lutz,

    FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Johnstone, B., Bayan, S., Gutierrez, L., Lardizabal, D., Lanigar, S., Yoon,

    D. P., & Judd, K. (2014). Neuropsychological correlates of forgiveness.

    Religion, Brain & Behavior. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2014

    .880069

    Johnstone, B., Bodling, A., Cohen, D., Christ, S. E., & Wegrzyn, A.

    (2012). Right parietal lobe-related selflessness as the neuropsycholog-

    ical basis of spiritual transcendence. International Journal for the Psy-

    chology of Religion, 22, 267284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10508619

    .2012.657524

    Johnstone, B., & Glass, B. A. (2008). Evaluation of a neuropsychologicalmodel of spirituality in persons with traumatic brain injury. Zygon, 43,

    861874. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9744.2008.00964.x

    Johnstone, B., Leach, L. R., Hickey, M. L., Frank, R. G., & Rupright, J.

    (1995). Some objective measurements of frontal lobe deficits following

    traumatic brain injury.Applied Neuropsychology, 2,2428. http://dx.doi

    .org/10.1207/s15324826an0201_4

    Jones, S. E., Buchbinder, B. R., & Aharon, I. (2000). Three-dimensional

    mapping of cortical thickness using Laplaces equation. Human Brain

    Mapping, 11, 1232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-0193(200009)11:

    112::AID-HBM203.0.CO;2-K

    Karnath, H. O., Baier, B., & Ngele, T. (2005). Awareness of the functioning

    of ones own limbs mediated by the insular cortex? The Journal of Neuro-

    science, 25, 7134 7138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI

    .1590-05.2005

    Keenan, J. P., McCutcheon, N. P., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Functional

    magnetic resonance imaging and event-related potentials suggest right

    prefrontal activation for self-related processing. Brain and Cognition,

    47, 8791.

    Keenan, J. P., Nelson, A., OConnor, M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2001).

    Self-recognition and the right hemisphere. Nature, 409, 305. http://dx

    .doi.org/10.1038/35053167

    Lee, K. H., & Siegle, G. J. (2012). Common and distinct brain networks

    underlying explicit emotional evaluation: A meta-analytic study. Social

    Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 521534. http://dx.doi.org/

    10.1093/scan/nsp001

    Leigh, R., Oishi, K., Hsu, J., Lindquist, M., Gottesman, R. F., Jarso, S., . . .

    Hillis, A. E. (2013). Acute lesions that impair affective empathy.Brain:

    A Journal of Neurology, 136, 25392549. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/

    brain/awt177Lewine, J. D., Davis, J. T., Bigler, E. D., Thoma, R., Hill, D., Funke, M.,

    . . . Orrison, W. W. (2007). Objective documentation of traumatic brain

    injury subsequent to mild head trauma: Multimodal brain imaging with

    MEG, SPECT, and MRI. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation,

    22,141155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.HTR.0000271115.29954.27

    Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsycho-

    logical assessment(4th ed.). New York, NY: Oxford Press.

    Lobato, R. D., Sarabia, R., Rivas, J. J., Cordobes, F., Castro, S., Muoz,

    M. J., . . . Lamas, E. (1986). Normal computerized tomography scans in

    severe headinjury. Prognostic and clinical management implications.

    Journal of Neurosurgery, 65, 784789. http://dx.doi.org/10.3171/jns

    .1986.65.6.0784

    Logothetis, N. K. (2008). What we can do and what we cannot do with

    fMRI.Nature, 453, 869 878. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature06976

    Lou, H. C., Luber, B., Crupain, M., Keenan, J. P., Nowak, M., Kjaer,

    T. W., . . . Lisanby, S. H. (2004). Parietal cortex and representation of

    the mental Self.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the

    United States of America, 101, 68276832. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/

    pnas.0400049101

    McGlynn, S. M., & Schacter, D. L. (1989). Unawareness of deficits inneuropsychological syndromes. Journal of Clinical and Experimental

    Ne uro ps yc hol ogy , 11, 143205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/

    01688638908400882

    Meador, K. J., Moore, E. E., Nichols, M. E., Abney, O. L., Taylor, H. S.,

    Zamrini, E. Y., & Loring, D. W. (1993). The role of cholinergic systems

    in visuospatial processing and memory. Journal of Clinical and Exper-

    imental Neuropsychology, 15, 832842.

    Mesulam, M.-M. (2000). Behavioral neuroanatomy. In M.-M. Mesulam

    (Ed.),Principles of behavioral and cognitive neurology (2nd ed.). New

    York, NY: Oxford Press.

    Mutschler, I., Reinbold, C., Wankerl, J., Seifritz, E., & Ball, T. (2013).

    Structural basis of empathy and the domain general region in the anterior

    insular cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 177. http://dx.doi

    .org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00177Newberg, A. B., Alavi, A., Baime, M., Mozley, P. D., & dAquili, E. G.

    (1997). The measurement of cerebral blood flow during the complex

    cognitive task of mediation using HMPAO-SPECT imaging.Journal of

    Nuclear Medicine, 38,95.

    Newberg, A., Alavi, A., Baime, M., Pourdehnad, M., Santanna, J., &

    dAquili, E. (2001). The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow

    during the complex cognitive task of meditation: A preliminary SPECT

    study. Psychiatry Research, 106, 113122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/

    S0925-4927(01)00074-9

    Newberg, A., Pourdehnad, M., Alavi, A., & dAquili, E. G. (2003).

    Cerebral blood flow during meditative prayer: Preliminary findings and

    methodological issues.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 97, 625630. http://

    dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.2003.97.2.625

    Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., &

    Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brainA meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage, 31, 440457.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002

    Ochsner, K. N., Knierim, K., Ludlow, D. H., Hanelin, J., Ramachandran,

    T., Glover, G., & Mackey, S. C. (2004). Reflecting upon feelings: An

    fMRI study of neural systems supporting the attribution of emotion to

    self and other. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 17461772.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/0898929042947829

    Penner, L. A. (2002). Dispositional and organizational influences on sus-

    tained volunteerism: An interactionist perspective. Journal of Social

    Issues, 58, 447 467. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1540-4560.00270

    Penner, L. A., & Finkelstein, M. A. (1998). Dispositional and structural

    determinants of volunteerism. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-

    chology, 74, 525537. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.525

    Penner, L. A., Fritzsche, B. A., Craiger, J. P., & Freifeld, T. S. (1995).

    Measuring the prosocial personality. In J. Butcher & C. D. Spielberger

    (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (Vol. 10). Hillsdale, NJ:

    LEA.

    Platek, S. M., Wathne, K., Tierney, N. G., & Thomson, J. W. (2008).

    Neural correlates of self-face recognition: An effect-location meta-

    analysis. Brain Research, 1232, 173184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j

    .brainres.2008.07.010

    Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and

    proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 120.

    Rankin, K. P., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Allison, S. C., Stanley, C. M., Glenn,

    S., Weiner, M. W., & Miller, B. L. (2006). Structural anatomy of

    empathy in neurodegenerative disease. Brain: A Journal of Neurology,

    129, 29452956. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl254

    y

    p

    y

    9SELF-ORIENTATION AND EMPATHY

  • 8/9/2019 Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy

    11/11

    Rankin, K. P., Kramer, J. H., & Miller, B. L. (2005). Patterns of cognitive

    and emotional empathy in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Cognitive

    and Behavioral Neurology, 18, 28 36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.wnn

    .0000152225.05377.ab

    Reitan, R. M. (1992). Trail making test: Manual for administration and

    scoring. Tucson, AZ: Reitan Neuropsychological Laboratory.

    Rorden, C., & Karnath, H.-O. (2004). Using human brain lesions to infer

    function: A relic from a past era in the fMRI age? Nature Reviews

    Neuroscience, 5, 812819. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1521

    Schmand, B., Rienstra, A., Tamminga, H., Richard, E., van Gool, W. A.,

    Caan, M. W., & Majoie, C. B. (2014). Responsiveness of magnetic

    resonance imaging and neuropsychological assessment in memory clinic

    patients. Journal of Alzheimers Disease, 40, 409418.

    Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2011). The neural bases for empathy. The Neuro-

    scientist, 17, 18 24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073858410379268

    Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Perry, D. (2009). Two systems

    for empathy: A double dissociation between emotional and cognitive

    empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions.

    Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 132, 617 627. http://dx.doi.org/

    10.1093/brain/awn279

    Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Goldsher, D., Berger, B. D., & Aharon-

    Peretz, J. (2004). Impairment in cognitive and affective empathy inpatients with brain lesions: Anatomical and cognitive correlates. Journal

    of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 11131127. http://

    dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803390490515531

    Skoe, E. A., Cumberland, A., Eisenberg, N., Hansen, K., & Perry, J.

    (2002). The influences of sex and gender-role identity on moral cogni-

    tion and prosocial personality traits. Sex Roles, 46, 295309. http://dx

    .doi.org/10.1023/A:1020224512888

    Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (2002). Neurobiological foundations of

    human memory. In A. D. Baddeley, M. D. Kopelman, & B. A. Wilson

    (Eds.),The handbook of memory disorders(2nd ed., pp. 1756). Chich-

    ester: Wiley.Uddin, L. Q., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Zaidel, E., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). rTMS

    to the right inferior parietal lobule disrupts self-other discrimination.

    Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 6571. http://dx.doi.org/

    10.1093/scan/nsl003

    Urgesi, C., Aglioti, S. M., Skrap, M., & Fabbro, F. (2010). The spiritual

    brain: Selective cortical lesions modulate human self-transcendence.

    Neuron, 65,309 319. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.026

    Wechsler, D. (1997a). WAIS III. Administration and scoring manual. San

    Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Wechsler, D. (1997b). WMS-III. Administration and scoring manual. San

    Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Zaki, J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2012). The neuroscience of empathy: Progress,

    pitfalls and promise. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 675680. http://dx.doi

    .org/10.1038/nn.3085

    Received March 7, 2014

    Revision received July 24, 2014

    Accepted September 22, 2014

    y

    p

    y

    10 JOHNSTONE, COHEN, BRYANT, GLASS, AND CHRIST