Good Cognition Emotion7

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/30/2019 Good Cognition Emotion7

    1/3

    Introduction to the Special Section on EmotionCognition Interactions andthe Aging Mind

    Fredda Blanchard-FieldsGeorgia Institute of Technology

    This editorial previews the articles that appear in the special

    section on emotion cognition interactions and the aging mind.

    These articles examine in older adults the degree to which intact

    emotional processing affects, and perhaps enhances, what has

    more traditionally been considered purely cognitive processing.

    Thus, they help advance researchers understanding of the com-

    plex interplay between emotion and cognition and raise important

    questions for future research in this area.

    Decades of research have yielded an abundance of empirical

    studies demonstrating that older adults show decline in comparisonto young adults across a wide variety of cognitive tasks (for

    review, see Zacks, Hasher, & Li, 2000). These declines occur in a

    number of areas of cognitive functioning, such as sensory func-

    tioning, working memory, attention, and executive abilities that tax

    deliberative, effortful information processing (Park, 2002; Zacks et

    al., 2000). Of course, not all types of memory are affected equally.

    For example, systems that require less deliberative processing,

    such as semantic memory, do not necessarily decline with age. Of

    particular interest to this special section is that, along with the

    research on cognitive decline, there are a growing number of

    studies that demonstrate that emotional experience and emotion

    regulation remain intact and may even improve across adulthood

    (Blanchard-Fields, 1998; Charles, Mather, & Carstensen, 2003;Rahhal, May, & Hasher, 2002). Given the different developmental

    trajectories of the emotion system and cognitive systems that

    require deliberative processing, there has been a recent surge of

    interest in investigating their interactions in the aging mind

    (Carstensen & Mikels, 2005; Mather, 2004). Accordingly, evi-

    dence is mounting that suggests that emotional variables improve

    performance on cognitive tasks (Carstensen & Mikels, 2005). At

    the same time, it is important to note that there are also inconsis-

    tencies in the literature. On the one hand, there are quite a few

    memory studies that have found that older adults remember pro-

    portionally more positive relative to negative information more so

    than do young adults, despite the fact that the absolute amount of

    information retained is lower in older adults (Carstensen & Mikels,

    2005; Charles et al., 2003; Mather & Carstensen, 2003). Thisphenomenon is typically referred to as a positivity effect. On the

    other hand, there are studies that demonstrate a memory enhance-

    ment for negative information in older adults (Kensinger, Brierley,

    Medford, Growdon, & Corkin, 2002; Denburg, Buchanan, Tranel,

    & Adolphs, 2003). There are two major issues that arise from these

    findings and that are the focus of this special section. First, what

    are the conditions that determine when a positivity effect can be

    observed in older adults? In other words, what are the systematic

    differences in studies where these effects are found and where they

    are not found?

    Second, given the evidence for a positivity effect in older adults,

    what are the mechanisms that account for this preferential treat-

    ment of positive information over negative information? Is this

    relative preference compensatory in nature? In other words, do

    intact emotional processes ameliorate cognitive decline? For ex-ample, what is the role of emotion regulation in processing infor-

    mation? Or do these interactions represent a constellation of spared

    processes? That is, do the mechanisms responsible for these emo-

    tional performance advantages operate independently of cognitive

    functioning?

    The articles proposed for this special section present varied

    research projects that examine how emotional factors influence

    cognitive performance in long-term memory, working memory,

    source memory, and social judgments. The goal of this compila-

    tion of work is to begin to provide some answers to the above

    questions, while underscoring a recent surge in emotioncognition

    and aging investigations.

    The first two articles provide evidence for a positivity effect inolder adults emotional memory. The first article focuses on work-

    ing memory for affective versus visual information, and the second

    article examines the role of executive processes in an observed

    positivity effect in older adults memory for pictures. Mikels,

    Larkin, Reuter-Lorenz, and Carstensen (2005) extend research on

    the positivity effect in older adults to working memory. First, they

    found evidence of a cognitive process that appears to remain intact

    in older adults. Working memory involving judging the intensity

    of affective pictorial information showed no age differences, in

    contrast to working memory tasks involving intensity judgments of

    visual information, which did demonstrate the typical age differ-

    ences (i.e., young adults performed better than did older adults).

    Second, a positivity effect in older adults was found. In the

    affective working memory task, older adults performed better onpositive relative to negative emotion trials, whereas young adults

    displayed the opposite pattern. The contributions of this article are

    twofold. First, the authors have developed an elegant new task to

    tap cognitive processing of emotional material. Second, they dem-

    onstrate not only the positivity effect in older adults but also a

    corresponding negativity effect in young adults. Although not

    directly examined, their findings are suggestive of the fact that

    executive processes associated with emotion-related working

    memory may be unimpaired with age. In addition, it may be the

    case that older adults superior ability to regulate emotions influ-

    ences their success at this task. This is also taken up in the Mather

    Fredda Blanchard-Fields, School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of

    Technology.

    Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Fredda

    Blanchard-Fields, School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Psychology,

    Atlanta, GA 30332-0170. E-mail: [email protected]

    Psychology and Aging Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association2005, Vol. 20, No. 4, 539 541 0882-7974/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.539

    539

  • 7/30/2019 Good Cognition Emotion7

    2/3

    and Knight (2005) article. What remains to be seen is whether this

    age difference involves differential allocation of limited resources

    to emotionally gratifying or positive information (Carstensen &

    Mikels, 2005), perhaps at the cost of other forms of functioning or

    a spared cognitive process in itself.

    Mather and Knight (2005) also demonstrate a positivity effect in

    older adults and a negativity effect in young adults in pictorialemotion memory. In addition to demonstrating this effect, they

    also focus on the role executive control processes play in the

    positivity effect. In fact, they find that age-related differences in

    emotion memory are associated with higher levels of cognitive

    control. As in the Mikels et al. (2005) study, they also speculate

    that although there may be a motivational preference for encoding

    positive information on the part of older adults, such processing

    goals are necessary but not sufficient to produce the positivity

    effect. Only those older adults with higher levels of cognitive

    control exhibited this effect. In addition, when cognitive control

    was reduced in older adults, the positivity effect was attenuated.

    These two studies demonstrate nicely that it is not emotion-related

    goals or cognitive decline alone that can account for these findings.

    Instead, it is an interaction between the two systems. In particular,Mather and Knight find that cognitive capacity in the form of

    control processes is needed to implement ones processing goals.

    Again, similar to Mikels et al., Mather and Knight further suggest

    that such cognitive control processes are necessary for emotion

    regulation and can easily deplete resources. This becomes relevant

    when discussing the Mienaltowski and Blanchard-Fields (2005)

    article. First, the next two articles are previewed in that they were

    less successful in demonstrating the positivity effect. Thus, they

    provide important information as to the conditions under which the

    positivity effect is observed.

    The article by May, Rahhal, Berry, and Leighton (2005) exam-

    ines the differential preferences for emotional information in older

    adults on source memory. They focused primarily on the degree towhich older adults spontaneously focus on meaningful, emotion-

    ally laden dimensions when encoding information. They effec-

    tively teased apart whether older adults focus on the emotional or

    conceptual nature of the information presented. Indeed, they found

    a memorial advantage for emotion-related source memory relative

    to neutral information. It is interesting that, as in the Mikels et al.

    (2005) study, older adults ability to process and retain contextual

    information that was emotionally laden remained intact, whereas

    older adults ability to retain contextual information that was

    strictly perceptual or conceptual information that was neutral in

    valence was worse than that of young adults. However, it is not

    clear whether the advantage was based on older adults tendency to

    focus on negative, positive, or both types of information. Thus far,

    their conclusion was that the emotional nature of the stimuli, ingeneral, influenced source memory in older adults. An important

    implication of May et al.s findings is that the memorial advantage

    of processing emotion-laden information may be driving increased

    elaborative processing of the emotion-laden material. Thus, again

    examining the interaction between emotion and cognition is an

    optimal method toward understanding older adults cognitive pro-

    cessing. It raises the following important question: Does this

    motivational tendency to engage emotion-related information

    evoke more elaborative processing, leading to better recall?

    With a novel approach to examining emotioncognition inter-

    actions in memory, Gruhn, Smith, and Baltes (2005) examined the

    positivity effect for lists of words that were either homogeneous in

    valence (e.g., all negative or all positive) or heterogeneous in

    valence (e.g., including positive, negative, and neutral words). The

    authors argue that heterogeneous, but not homogeneous, lists

    evoke selective processing on the part of older adults, (i.e., they

    provide strong cues to select words for processing based on va-

    lence). Thus, if a positivity effect exists, it should be observedunder these conditions. However, their findings offer no evidence

    of a positivity effect in older adults under either condition. Instead,

    they found that both older and young adults recalled more negative

    than positive words. In addition, this emotion-based prioritization

    (negativity effect) was greater for young adults than for older

    adults. This finding, of course, coincides nicely with a large

    literature on young adults indicating that, compared with other

    stimuli, negative information attracts more processing resources.

    The contrasting age differences found in this study, as compared

    with the Mikels et al. (2005) study and the Mather and Knight

    (2005) study, yield important information regarding researchers

    understanding of emotioncognition interactions. First, why is it

    that the negativity effect for older adults was attenuated? Similar to

    all of the authors in this special section, these authors argue that ifolder adults indeed display intact emotion regulation abilities, it

    may be the case that they down-regulate negative affect, and this

    may reduce the ability to make effective comparisons between

    negative and positive stimuli. The interesting question raised is

    whether the positivity effect is actually a reduced negativity effect

    on the part of older adults. In addition, an important question to ask

    is whether older adults actively divert attention away from nega-

    tive information or whether they just are not able to process this

    information as well as young adults. These questions, again, apply

    to all of the studies thus far and warrant future research.

    In all of the articles previewed thus far, reference has been made

    to a possible mechanism for the positivity effect, the resource

    demands encountered when engaging in emotion regulation. Thefinal article by Mienaltowski and Blanchard-Fields (2005) in this

    special section directly examines emotion induction and its effect

    on social judgments. They examined the age-related differential

    influence of induced positive and negative emotion on the ability

    to objectively make social judgments. Contrary to research sug-

    gesting that negative mood induction evokes more elaborative

    processing, resulting (in this case) in a reduced judgment bias, a

    reduced judgment bias was more prominent for older adults in the

    positive mood condition. In other words, they were, for example,

    appropriately less confident in a tenuous judgment when their

    mood was positive than when it was negative. The authors argue

    that it may be the case, as with the above studies, that older adults

    were engaging in emotion repair in the negative mood condition.

    Given that emotion regulation is resource depleting, they may nothave processed the details necessary to make a more appropriate

    social judgment. Again, the possibility that older adults spontane-

    ously engage in the emotion regulation of negative mood or

    information may be an important factor influencing their cognitive

    processing. In this case, its influence is disadvantageous to making

    appropriate social judgments.

    The articles in this special section begin to answer the questions

    put forth in this introduction. They offer two conditions under

    which a positivity effect in older adulthood is observed. First, it

    appears to depend on the older adults cognitive capacity level, in

    this case, executive cognitive control. Second, it appears to depend

    540 BLANCHARD-FIELDS

  • 7/30/2019 Good Cognition Emotion7

    3/3

    on the nature of the task, such as whether information is presented

    in a comparative framework (i.e., heterogeneous lists) or not (i.e.,

    homogeneous lists). In addition, what is not addressed in the above

    studies is that the nature of the stimuli under investigation also

    differed (e.g., pictures vs. words). This also could affect when the

    positivity effect is observed. With respect to mechanisms, concep-

    tually relevant, emotion-laden information, in general, may serveas a trigger for more elaborative processing on the part of older

    adults. Finally, age-related differences in emotion regulation may

    also play an important role in determining when a positivity effect

    will be observed. Although none of the studies directly assessed

    how emotion regulation may produce a positivity effect, they will

    serve as catalysts for future research to examine this possibility.

    This set of articles highlights the importance of the recent

    increase in attention to emotioncognition interactions in the aging

    mind. They raise important new critical questions to be posed

    about cognitive aging when taking into consideration the function-

    ing emotional system. In addition, they also offer novel and

    valuable frameworks for providing empirical answers to those

    questions.

    References

    Blanchard-Fields, F. (1998). The role of emotion in social cognition across

    the adult life span. In K. W. Schaie & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), Annual

    review of gerontology and geriatrics: Vol. 17. Focus on emotion and

    adult development (pp. 238 265). New York: Springer.

    Carstensen, L. L., & Mikels, J. A. (2005). At the intersection of emotion

    and cognition: Aging and the positivity effect. Current Directions in

    Psychological Science, 14, 117121.

    Charles, S. T., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Focusing on the

    positive: Age differences in memory for positive, negative, and neutral

    stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 85, 163178.

    Denburg, N. L., Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., & Adolphs, R. (2003).

    Evidence for preserved emotional memory in normal elderly persons.

    Emotion, 3, 239253.

    Gruhn, D., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (2005). No aging bias favoring

    memory for positive material: Evidence from a heterogeneity

    homogeneity list paradigm using emotionally toned words. Psychology

    and Aging, 20, 579588.

    Kensinger, E. A., Brierley, B., Medford, N., Growdon, J. H., & Corkin, S.(2002). The effect of normal aging and Alzheimers disease on emo-

    tional memory. Emotion, 2, 118134.

    Mather, M. (2004). Aging and emotional memory. In D. Reisberg & P.

    Hertel, (Eds.), Memory and emotion (pp. 272307). New York: Oxford

    University Press.

    Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and attentional biases for

    emotional faces. Psychological Science, 14, 409415.

    Mather, M., & Knight, M. (2005). Goal-directed memory: The role of

    cognitive control in older adults emotional memory. Psychology and

    Aging, 20, 554570.

    May, C. P., Rahhal, T., Berry, E. M., & Leighton, E. A. (2005). Aging,

    source memory, and emotion. Psychology and Aging, 20, 571578.

    Mienaltowski, A., & Blanchard-Fields, F. (2005). The differential effects

    of mood on age differences in the correspondence bias. Psychology and

    Aging, 20, 589600.Mikels, J. A., Larkin, G. R., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Carstensen, L. L.

    (2005). Divergent trajectories in the aging mind: Changes in working

    memory for affective versus visual information with age. Psychology

    and Aging, 20, 542553.

    Park, D. C. (2002). Judging meaning improves function in the aging brain.

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 227229.

    Rahhal, T. A., May, C. P., & Hasher, L. (2002). Truth and character:

    Sources that older adults can remember. Psychological Science, 13,

    101105.

    Zacks, R. T., Hasher, L., & Li, K. Z. H. (2000). Human memory. In T. A.

    Salthouse & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Handbook of aging and cognition

    (2nd ed., pp. 293357). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    541INTRODUCTION TO SPECIAL SECTION