20
Typology and Universals in Word-Formation III Košice, Slovakia 2015-06-28 Steve Pepper Department of Linguistics University of Oslo <[email protected]> Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages in Africa and elsewhere in the world are poorly studied, there is obviously still a lot to be learned about the typology of languages. (Dimmendaal 2011: 298).

Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    9

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

Typology and Universals in

Word-Formation III

Košice, Slovakia

2015-06-28

Steve Pepper

Department of Linguistics

University of Oslo

<[email protected]>

Head position in nominal compounds

A lesson from Africa

Given the fact that so many languages in Africa and elsewhere in

the world are poorly studied, there is obviously still a lot to be learned

about the typology of languages. (Dimmendaal 2011: 298).

Page 2: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

2 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Introduction

Far away, in the West African

village of Galim, there is a

language called Nizɑɑ

It exhibits a feature which

appears to be unique in the

published literature

This has ramifications for the

typology of compounding…

● (and perhaps also for the

classification of compounds) Nizɑɑ (a.k.a. Suga); sgi; Niger-Congo; Cameroon.

Spoken by ca. 10,000 people in Adamawa Province.

Page 3: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

3 >

The head in compounding

A prototypical compound consists of two or more

lexical roots, one of which is the “head”

● (Here I quietly ignore co-compounds, “exocentric” compounds,

and the question of exactly what we mean by the head)

The head generally determines the (grammatical and

semantic) “profile” of the compound

● e.g. blackbird, snail mail, football, apron string, taxi driver,

futbalový štadión, lesopark

One of the questions posed by language typologists is

● Is there a linguistic universal that governs the position of the

head in compounds?

Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Page 4: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

4 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Proposal #1: Fixed position

“Righthand Head Rule” (RHR)

● “The head of a morphologically complex word is the

righthand member of that word” (Williams 1981)

● Works for English and other Germanic languages (mostly)

Falsified by evidence from Romance, Hebrew, Celtic, etc.

● French: bateau mouche ‘river boat’ < ‘boat’ + ‘fly’

● Hebrew: delet bayit ‘house door’ < ‘door’ + ‘house’

● Welsh: drws tŷ ‘house door’ < ‘door’ + ‘house’

All of these languages have left-headed compounds

Page 5: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

5 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Proposal #2: Parametric variation

The “principles and parameters” approach

● Each language is either left-headed or right-headed

– “Stando alle nostre conoscenze attuali, non sembra che una lingua

possa avere liberamente composti con testa a sinistra e composti

con testa a destra” (Scalise 1994)

Falsified by two kinds of evidence

1) Vietnamese: LH native tradition vs. RH Chinese tradition

– xe lưaN ‘train’ < xeN ‘vehicle’ + lưaN ‘fire’

– tô-quôc ‘fatherland’ < 祖國 zǔguó ‘ancestor’ + ‘country’

2) Mandarin: RH nominal compounds vs. LH verbal compounds

– 食品 shípǐnN ‘food’ < shíV ‘eat’ + pǐnN ‘product’

– 开刀 kaidaoV ‘operate’ < kaiV ‘open’ + daoN ‘knife’

Page 6: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

6 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Proposal #3: Canonical position

Discounting contact phenomena, it seems there is

● “a canonical position for the head in each compound

type in a given language” (Scalise & Fabregas 2010)

Where “compound type” means “output category type”

Enter Nizɑɑ…

● Reported to have a mixture of left-headed and

right-headed nominal compounds

– i.e. compounds of the same output category type

● Not attributable to language contact

Page 7: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

7 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Nizɑɑ: A Mambiloid language

Mambiloid: Fam, Kwanja, Mambila, Mbongno, Mvanip, Ndoro, Ndunda, Nizɑɑ, Njerep, Somyev, Twendi, Vute, Wawa

(Ble

nch 1

993)

Page 8: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

8 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Sample of N+N nominal compounds

sii yim ‘dispensary’

sii jaŋgide ‘school’

sii ɓʉʉ ‘roof’

ɓag ɓʉʉ ‘hat’

nim ɓʉʉ ‘source/spring’

siŋw nim ‘duck’

yir nim ‘tear’

< ‘house’ + ‘medicament’

< ‘house’ + ‘reading’

< ‘house’ + ‘head’

< ‘clothing’ + ‘head’

< ‘water’ + ‘head’

< ‘bird’ + ‘water’

< ‘eye’ + ‘water’

LH

LH

RH

LH

RH

LH

RH

Page 9: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

9 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Rolf Theil and his language consultant Hamadicko Daniel

Research questions

Is it really the case

that Nizɑɑ exhibits no

particular preference

for either left-headed

or right-headed

nominal compounds?

If so, can a semantic

analysis provide an

explanation?

Page 10: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

10 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Data

Field notes collected by

Rolf Theil (genannt

Endresen) in 1983–84

1300-page word list

(including notes)

No access to native

speakers

Page 11: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

11 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Methodology

Identify nominal compounds

● 534 candidates

Identify constituents

● 314 transparent

● 116 semi-transparent

● 104 opaque

Classify by input type

● Predominance (63%) of

noun + noun compounds

● 203 NN, 35 NA, 33 NV, ...

Determine head position

● 60-40 split between LH and

RH compounds

Statistical analysis of

recurrence patterns

● Heads recur significantly

more often in left-headed

compounds

● Non-heads recur significantly

more often in right-headed

compounds

Semantic analysis...

Page 12: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

12 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Analysing semantic relations

Jackendoff (2010) not directly usable

● Some functions too general, e.g. CLASSIFY(x, α)

● Some functions too culture-specific, e.g. PROTECT(α, x)

● Some functions too language-specific, e.g. HAVE(α, x)

Warren (1978) and Ryder (1994) equally difficult to apply

● (Adopting someone else’s classification is not easy)

Devised own ad-hoc classification inspired by the above

● Incorporated insights from knowledge modelling

● The methodology is prescientific, but still valid

The results were quite startling…

Page 13: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

13 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Examples of semantic relations

ɓów nyiiŋ ‘canine’

sii jaŋgide ‘school’

sii ɓʉʉ ‘roof’

ɓag ɓʉʉ ‘hat’

ɗɔɔŋ nim ‘soup’

siŋw nim ‘duck’

yir nim ‘tear’

< ‘dog’ + ‘tooth’

< ‘house’ + ‘reading’

< ‘house’ + ‘head’

< ‘clothing’ + ‘head’

< ‘sauce’ + ‘water’

< ‘bird’ + ‘water’

< ‘eye’ + ‘water’

POSSESSION

ACTIVITY

PART

LOCATION

APPEARANCE

LOCATION

LOCATION LOCATION? LOCATED

Page 14: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

14 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Distribution by semantic relation

22 relation types

● LOCATION,

PURPOSE,

ACTIVITY, etc.

Left-hand column

● 15 types, all of

which occur in LH

compounds only

Right-hand column

● 7 types, all of

which occur in RH

compounds only

One special case

● LOCATION and

LOCATED are the

inverse of each

other

Relation type Total LH RH Relation type Total LH RH

LOCATION 26 26 0 PART 45 0 45

PURPOSE 13 13 0 KIN 9 0 9

ACTIVITY 9 9 0 LOCATED 8 0 8

APPEARANCE 9 9 0 POSSESSION 5 0 5

IDEM 9 9 0 PRODUCT 5 0 5

STATE 7 7 0 CONTAINER 3 0 3

GENDER 6 6 0 RESPONSIBLE 1 0 1

EVENT 5 5 0

MATERIAL 5 5 0

CONTENTS 4 4 0

PROPER 3 3 0

EMOTION 2 2 0

DOMAIN 1 1 0 (analysed) 190 111 79

FEATURE 1 1 0 (unanalysed) 13 10 3

FOOD 1 1 0 Totals 203 121 82

Page 15: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

15 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Summary of results

Right-headed compounds

● PART-WHOLE and KINSHIP relations predominate

– also: LOCATED, POSSESSION, PRODUCT, CONTAINER

Left-headed compounds

● APPEARANCE, GENDER, LOCATION, PURPOSE, ACTIVITY

– largely attributive

Clearly, Nizɑɑ does exhibit both left- and right-headed

compounds…

And there is a systematic difference between the two,

expressible in terms of semantic relations

Page 16: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

16 >

Towards an explanation

My explanation for this phenomenon is framed in terms

of Langacker’s Cognitive Grammar

● (Time does not permit me to go into the details)

● (The paper is available at Academia.edu)

It hinges on the notion of “mental access routes”

● The patterns in Nizɑɑ represent two such routes

● They correspond (roughly) to the division of compounds

into ”subordinate” and “attributive” (Scalise & Bisetto 2009)

Essentially, in Nizɑɑ

● Subordinate compounds are right-headed

● Attributive compounds are left-headed

Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

Page 17: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

17 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

A syntactically mixed language

These two mental access routes are probably universal

but they surface rather clearly in Nizɑɑ

Nizɑɑ is a little unusual (but far from unique) in that it has

postposed modifiers and preposed possessors

From a diachronic perspective:

● LH compounds develop from the one

– lexicalization of noun phrases in which adjectival nouns

act as postposed modifiers

● RH compounds develop from the other

– lexicalization of possessive phrases

Page 18: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

18 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

In conclusion

Nizɑɑ is very unusual (but surely not unique?) in having

both left-headed and right-headed compounds in equal

measure

The two kinds are characterized by different semantic

relations

● “Subordinate” compounds are right-headed

● “Attributive” compounds are left-headed

We need to revise the Canonical Position Hypothesis

● “Compound type” has to be defined in terms of semantic

relation as well as output type

Page 19: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

19 > Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper

References

Bauer, L. 2001. Compounding. In M. Haspelmath et al. (eds.) Language typology and language universals: an

international handbook. Berlin: de Gruyter.

Blench, R. M. 1993. An Outline Classification of the Mambiloid Languages, Journal of West African

Languages 23(1).

Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. 2011. Historical linguistics and the comparative study of African languages.

Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Jackendoff, R. 2010. The ecology of English noun-noun compounds. In Meaning and the lexicon. The parallel

architecture 1975-2010. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Pepper, S. 2010. Nominal compounding in Nizɑɑ: A cognitive perspective. SOAS: MA thesis. (Available from

http://uio.academia.edu/StevePepper.)

Ryder, M. E. 1994. Ordered chaos: the interpretation of English noun-noun compounds. Berkeley: University

of California Press.

Scalise, S. 1994. Morfologia. Bologna: il Mulino.

Scalise, S. & A. Bisetto. 2009. The Classification of Compounds. In R. Lieber & P. Štekauer (eds.) The Oxford

Handbook of Compounding. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Scalise, S. & A. Fábregas. 2010. The head in compounding. In S. Scalise & I. Vogel (eds.) Cross-disciplinary

issues in compounding. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Theil, R. n.d. Field notes on Nizɑɑ: Word lists and accompanying notes. MS.

Warren, B. 1978. Semantic patterns of noun-noun compounds. Gothenburg: Acta Universitatis

Gothoburgensis.

Williams, E. 1981. On the notions ‘lexically related’ and ‘head of a word’, Linguistic Inquiry 12:2.

Page 20: Head position in nominal compounds: A lesson from Africafolk.uio.no/stevepe/TUWF3.pdf · Head position in nominal compounds A lesson from Africa Given the fact that so many languages

20 >

Ďakujem vám – Thank you!

My PhD project (2015-2018)

Nominal compounding in cross-linguistic perspective

Please contact me if you are interested in the topic

<[email protected]>

…especially if you already have (or intend to acquire)

specialist knowledge about compounding in a lesser

known language…

Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies Steve Pepper