145
Social Competence 1 Running Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCE Social Competence in Peer-Accepted Children with Learning Disabilities Andrea E- Brown Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology McGi11 University, Montreal June, 1999 A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research in partial firlfilment of the requirements of the degree of PhD in Educational Psychology Major in SchooVApplied Child Psychology O Andrea E, Brown, 1999

Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

1

Running Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCE

Social Competence in Peer-Accepted Children with Learning Disabilities

Andrea E- Brown

Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology

McGi11 University, Montreal

June, 1999

A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research in partial firlfilment of the requirements of the degree of

PhD in Educational Psychology Major in SchooVApplied Child Psychology

O Andrea E, Brown, 1999

Page 2: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

National Library I*I of Canada Bibliothèque nationale du Canada

Acquisitions and Acquisitions et Bibliographic Services services bibliographiques

395 Wellington Street 395, rue Wellington OnawaON K l A O N 4 Ottawa ON K1A ON4 Canada Canada

Yow KI8 Votre rëfërence

Our fi& Notre refdr~nce

The author has granted a non- L'auteur a accordé une licence non exclusive licence allowing the exclusive permettant à la National Library of Canada to Bibliothèque nationale du Canada de reproduce, loan, distribute or sell reproduire, prêter, distribuer ou copies of this thesis in microform, vendre des copies de cette thèse sous paper or electronic formats. la forme de microfiche/nlm, de

reproduction sur papier ou sur format électronique.

The author retains ownership of the L'auteur conserve la propriété du copyright in this thesis. Neither the droit d'auteur qui protège cette thèse. thesis nor substantial extracts fkom it Ni la thèse ni des extraits substantiels may be printed or otheMise de celle-ci ne doivent être imprimés reproduced without the author7s ou autrement reproduits sans son permission. autorisation.

Page 3: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

2

Acknowledgements

1 would iike to achowledge and express my sincere appreciation to the many

individuals who assisted, supported, and inspird me in completing this thesis.

F k t , gratitude is extended to the Lakeshore and Baldwin Cartier School Boards

for supporting this research. 1 am thankful to all of the principals, teachers, students, and

parents who particpated in this study. The wrllingness of students and teachers to share

their experiences and feelings with me made this project both possible and pleasurable.

Thank you to the members of Dr. Nancy Heath's research laboratory (Christina,

Daisy, Stephanie, Shana, Loretta) and the faithfiul 'stand-bys' (Marla, Alyssa, Lisa, Julie,

Cindy, and Tina) who diligently assisted with many phases of this project, Special thanks

to Cathy Masden and Linda Grey who assisted in data andysis, Marguerite and Litsa for

their discerning statisticd guidance, and Marie-Hélène for her translation skïlls.

I wouid like to thank the doctoral cornmittee members who have offered helpfid

suggestions and insights dong the way: Dr. Judy Dr. Bruce Shore, Dr. Evy

Lusthaus, and Dr. Mark Aulls-

1 would also like to acknowledge the hancial support provided by the Fonds pour

la formation de chercheurs et L'aide à la recherche (FCAR) and the McGili University

Social Sciences Research subcommittee (McGiU University interna1 grant) in completing

this research.

1 am especially gratefbl to my supervisor, Dr. Nancy Heath, for challenging me to

think and providing the sincere support and guidance needed in completing this thesis.

To my family, thank you for your patience, support, and endurance through the

years. Fkally, a very special thank you to Lisa, Annie, and Linda for knowing where 1

was headed before I even knew myself and for helping to point me in the right direction.

Page 4: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

3

Abs tract

Since the time of Brym's (1974) seminal work on the social status of children with

learning disabilities GD), research has consistently shown that children with LD stniggle

to eam social acceptance fiom non-LD peers. Recently, investigators have uncovered

within-group varïability among children with LD suggesting that some children with LD

are well accepted and even popular among non-LD peers. An appeal in the field of

leaming disabilities has emerged, calling for a shift fkom deficit-mode1 research focusing

on the deficiencies of children with LD, to the investigation of how children with LD

obtain positive social outcornes. Accordingly, a study was undextaken addressing this

request by examining the characteristics of peer-accepted children with LD from a muIti-

rater and multi-method perspective. Using the comprehensive mode1 of social

competence proposed by Vaughn and Hogan (1990) as the theoretical fhmework, data

were gathered fiom teachers, peers, and peer-accepted chiIdren with and without LD in

important areas of social functioning. Participants were grade four and five mainstreamed

students meeting the followïng criteria: (a) having a researcher-identified leaming

disability in at least one academic area (reading, speiling, or arïthmetic) and (b) peer-rated

social acceptance as determined via a modified version of the Asher and Dodge (1986)

sociometnc classification system. Statistical analyses consisted of multivariate and

univariate techniques. Findings indicated few significant diffierences between peer-

accepted children with and without LD in specific areas of social competence as rated by

peers. Significant interactions, however, between LD status and gender revealing variable

profiles of social-behavioural characteristics for boys and girls with and without LD did

emerge fiom the perspective of teachers. Implications for special education referral and

placement, inclusive education, and interventions are discussed.

Page 5: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

4

Résumé

Depuis l'époque du travail séminal de Bryan (1974) sur le statut social des enfants atteint de

difficultés d'apprentissages (DA), les recherches ont régulièrement démontré que ces

enfants luttent pour obtenir l'approbation sociale de leurs pairs qui n'ont pas de D A

Récemment, une recherche avec un groupe d'enfânts ayant des DA a démontré une certaine

variabilité dans ce groupe, ce qui suggère que certains enfants ayant des DA soient bien

accueillis et jouissent même d'une popularité auprès de leurs pairs sans DA. Une nouvelle

ligne de pensée dans le domaine des DA amène un revirement du modèle de recherche se

concentrant sur les déficiences de ces enfants vers une méthode de recherche axée sur les

moyens à travers lesquels ces enfants atteint de DA obtiennent des résultats positifs. Pour

répondre à cette demande, une étude a été entrepnse pour examiner les caractéristiques des

enfants avec des DA qui jouissent d'une bonne intégration sociale. Utilisant le modèle

compréhensif des compétences sociales proposé par Vaughn et Hogan (L990) comme base

théorique, des données sur d'important domaines du fonctionnement social ont été amassées

a partir de professeurs, de pairs et d'enfants avec et sans DA qui sont acceptés socialement.

Les participants étaient des élèves de quatrième et cinquième années intégrés dans des

classes régulières rencontrant Ies critères suivants: (a) ils avaient des DA reconnues par la

recherche dans au moins une matière (lecture, orthographe ou mathématiques) et (b) ils

étaient classés par leurs pairs selon leur acceptation sociale via une version modifiée du

système de classincation sociornétrique de Asher et Dodge (1986). Les analyses statistiques

consistaient de techniques à variables multiples et à variables simples. Les résultats

indiquent peu ou aucune différence entre les enfants acceptés socialement avec ou sans DA

sur la mesure globale d'acceptation sociale. Par contre, des interactions significatives entre

les variables de difficultés d'apprentissage et genres sont ressorties, ce qui révèle des

profIles variable des caractéristiques sociales et comportementales pour les garçons et les

filles avec et sans DA. Une discussion sur les implications des interventions et des

références en éducation adaptée ainsi que des placements est entrepnse.

Page 6: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

5

Table of Contents

............................................................................................................ Aclmowledgements ..2

Abstract ................................................................................................................................ 3

Résumé ................................................................................................................................ -4

................................................................................................................ Table of Contents .5

.............................................................................. Chapter I r Literature Revïew .............. .. 9

Sociometric Profiles ............................................................................................... IO

Sociometric Profiles of Children with Learning Disabilities ............~.................... 14

..................................................................... Onginal Contribution to Knowledge 17

Theories of Social Cornpetence in Children with Leaming Disabilities ............... 18

Gresham and Reschly Mode1 of Social Cornpetence ............ ... .............. 18 ........ ....................... Vaughn and Hogan Mode1 of Social Cornpetence .... 19

............................................... .......................... Peer Relations ... -20

............................................................ Self-concept ................... .. 27

...................................................................... Behaviour Problems 3 1

Social Skills ................................................................................... 34

................................................................. Chapter II: Rationale and Research Questions -39

...................................................................... Hypotheses and Research Questions 40

................................................................. A- Peer Relations: Social Status 40

........................................................................................... B . Self-concept 40

............................................................................. C . Behavioural Conduct 41

D . Sociai Skills ................... ....... ............................................................ 41

........................................................................ E . Distinguishing Features -41

........................................................................................................... Chapter III: Method 43 . . ............................................................................................................ Participants -43

................................................................... ........ ...................... Measures ... ........ -47

IQ and Achievement .................... .... .............. 4

Page 7: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

6

Sociometric Measures .......................... .... ....... ...................................... -48

.............................................................. .. Self-perceptions ..... ............. .. 51

Social Skills and Behaviour Problems .................................................. ...... 52

Peer and SeIf-ratings .. ................................................................................ 53

Open-ended InteMew ................................................................................ 54

Procedure ............................................................................................................... 54

Phase 1 ..................................................................................................... 55

....................................................................................................... Phase 2 57

Unitisation of data .......................................................................... 59

Unit sorting ..~..~.......~..~..................~.~...........~.............~.....~.............~ 59

Negotiation of categories ............................................................ - 3 9

Re-coding and validation .................. ..... .................................... -60

................................................................... Quantification of data -60

............................................................................................................ Chapter IV: Resuits 62

................................................................................................ Data Analysis .... . .... 62

.................................................................... LD and Non-LD Classification Rates 64

Sociometric Results ............................................................................................... 65

........................................................... Overall Analyses for Initial Sample 65 .

Descriptive Statistics for Target Sample ............................................................... -66

................................................ Social Cornpetence Mode1 ............................. .... 68

.......................... Sociornetric-s tatus Equivalence Between Target Groups 68

........................................ Intercorrelations Between Dependent Variables 68

Revised Class Play Rating: Peer Ratings ............................................................. 73

....... ................................. .... Original Factors ,... .... 74

Nonacademic Factor ...................................... ......................................... 75

.......................................................... .... Academic Factor .......................... 76

Revised Class Play Rating: Self-ratings ................... .. ....................................... 77

Page 8: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

7

Original Factors .............. ........ ................................................................. 77 . .

Sensitive-Isolated Factor ............................................................................ 79

Nonacademic Factor ................................................................................. -79

Academic Factor ................................ ,.,,. ................................................... 81

Peer Raters Interview ................... ,., ................ .., .............................. 83 .................................................................................. Self-raters Interview .......... .,, -83

Chapter V: Discussion ....................................................................................................... 86

............................................................................ ..... ...................... Peer Status ... ... 87

Vaughn and Hogan Mode1 of Social Competence .........,,..................................... 90

...................................................... Social SUIS and Behavioural Conduct 90

.............................................................................................. Self-concept -95

Social, Emotional, and Behavioural Characteristics ..................... ..-. ................... 97 Peer Ratings ........................................................... ... -97 ....

................................................................................................ SeIf-ratings -97

Nonacademic and Academic Factors ..................................... ..................... 98

................................................................................................. Interview Findings 99

................................................ Conclusions and Contributions to Knowledge -99 . . * ................................................................................................. Study Limitations 102

.......................................................................... Educational Implications ......... .. 105

...........*.......*........................................... .......................*............................. References ,,.. 108

AppendYr A: Sociometrks Forms .................................................................................. 124

Appendix B: Revised Class Play Rating Questionnaire ................................................ 128

........................................................................................... Appendix C: Consent Fonns -133

Appendbc D: Sociometncs Distracter Tasks .................................................................... 137

Appendix E: Source Table for RCPR: Peer Repeated Measures MANOVA .............. .... 140

Appendix F: Breakdown of Peer-pleasing vernis Teacher-pleasing items on the

Social Skills factor of the Social Skills Rating System-Teacher Form (SSRS-T) ........... 142

Page 9: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

8

List of Tables

Table 1 : Summary of Early Research Examining the Behavioural Correlates of

Popular and Rejected Status Children .............................................. ..,, .............................. 1 1

Table 2: Select Studies Showing Social Skills Deficits in Children with LD ................... 36

Table 3 : Open-ended I n t e ~ e w Categoïy Titles and Descriptions ................................... -6 1

Table 4: Sociometric Classifications for Initial Sample by LD Status and Gender ........... 66

............................ Table 5: Demographic and Achievement Data for Target Sample ........ 67

Table 6: S tandardised g-scores for Sociometric and Means and Social Competence

Model Variables ........-.......... ,., ...................................................... ...-. ..................... ..70 Table 7: Means and Standard Deviations for RCPR: Peer Factors,-- ................................. 75

Table 8: Mean Responses to RCPR: Peer Nonacademic Factor Items ............... .. ..... ..,..76 ........................ Table 9: Means and Standard Deviations for RCPR: Self-ratings Factors 78

......... Table 10: Mean Responses to RCPR: Self-ratings Sensitive-Isolated Factor Items 80

Table 11: Nonsimiificant F-tests for RCPR: Self-ratings Sensitive-Isolated

Factor Items .......................................................... ....................................................... 8 1

Table 12: Mean Responses to RCPR: SeK-ratings Nonacademic Factor Items .....-. ........ -82

Table 13: Frequency and (Percentage) of Peer Responses in Open-ended

Interview Categories ........................................................................................................ A4 Table 14: Frequency and (Percentage) of Self-rated Responses in Open-ended

InteMew Categories ...................-..S... ........... -.-.... .............-............................................. 85

Table 15 : Mean Scores and DEerence Scores Between Peer-pleasing and Teacher-

pleasing Social Skills on the SSRS-T ................... .. .... .. ........... .. ........................................ 93

List of Figures

Figure 1 : Significant LD Status-by-Gender univariate interactions for Social Skills and

Problem Behaviours and nonsignificant LD Status-by-Gender interaction for

Nonacademic Self-concept ....................................................... ... ........... 7 1

Page 10: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

9

Chapter 1: Literature Review

Over twenty years ago Bryan (1974) examined the peer popularity and social

relationships of children with learning disabilities 0) and their peers in a classroom

setting and concluded that children with LD were not well-liked and were rejected by

their peers without LD. Two years later, Bryan (1976) replicated these findings and added

that children with leaming disabilities continueci to be rejected by their peers over t h e .

Bryanrs pioneering research on the social status of children with LD has had an enormous

impact on the amount and quality of empirical research devoted to the social and

emotional hctioning of children with leaming disabilities,

Extensive research in the past two decades has consistently documented the

difficulties students with leaming disabilities have in fonning and maintainhg social

relationships (Bursuck, 1989; Gresham & Reschly, 1986; Horowitz, 198 1; La Greca &

Stone, 1990; Siperstein, Bopp, & Bak, 1978; Vaughn, Zaragoza, Hogan, & Walker,

1993). While reviews conducted in the area of social status and learning disabilities (e.g.,

Wiener, 1987) have served to confirm Bryanls early Sndings, more recently researchers

have discovered the merits of complementing between-group research designs with the

examination of within-group diffiences focusing on subgroups of students with learning

disabilities. Using two-dimensional classification schemes based on social impact and

social preference construct scores, researchers have detemiined that within-group

variability in sociometric status exists in children with LD, suggesting that some children

with LD are socially accepted and even popular among their non-LD peers (Kïstner &

Gatlin, 1989; La Greca & Stone, 1990; Ochoa & Palmer, 1991; Stone & La Greca, 1990).

These hdings have influenced researchers to make an appeal for moving away £kom pure

between-group designs and the "deficit model" of research in the field of learning

disabilities (i-e., research which focuses on what children with LD lack in cornparison to

non-LD peers) and investigating instead what exemp lary social and personal qualities

children with LD possess (Coleman & Minnett, 1992; Dudley-Marling & Edmiaston,

Page 11: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

10

1985; Haager & Vaughn, 1995; Ochoa & Olivarez, 1995).

Hence, while Bryan's (1974, 1976) early hypotheses conceming the low social

status and peer rejection of children with LD continue to be actively investigated (e.g.,

Conderman, 1995; Swanson & Malone, 1992; Wiener, Harris, & Shirer, 1990), there is a

movement in the field to supplement our lmowledge of poor peer status and social

maifùnctioniug in some chïldren with LD with an additional understanding of the nature

of peer acceptance and hedthy social fiinctioning in other children with LD. In keeping

with this recent trend, the present study utilises the comprehensive mode1 of socid

competence proposed by Vaughn and Hogan (1990) as the theoretical k e w o r k to

examine the social, behavioural, and emotional features of socially-accepted children

with LD in an attempt to iliuminate the nature of social competence for these children.

Sociometric Profiles

The task of deciphering the social-behavioural correlates of sociaily-accepted

children with learning disabilities must begin with a thorough understanding of the

features associated with sociaily success fiil and unsuccessful childm without LD.

Identification of the behaviours associated with normally-achieving children in various

sociometric status groups (i.e., popular, average, controversial, neglected, and rejected)

has been systematicdy approached by many investigators. Table 1 briefly summarises

the early studies exarnining the social-behavioural correlates of popular and rejected peer

status in normdly-achieving children.

Increasingly, studies using between-group cornparisons have also been conducted

comparing the behavioural correlates of students in different sociometxic categones. Ladd

(1983) studied the social networks and behaviour of third and fourth grade children with

differing sociometric status and found that popular and average status children engaged in

more cooperative play, more social conversations, less onlooking behaviour (Le.,

observing but not joinUlg peers), and spent more tirne interacting in groups compnsed o f

older (upper-grade) peers than rejected status children. Coie and Dodge (L988) examined

Page 12: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

Table 1.

Summarv of Earh Research Examining the Behavioural Correlates of Pouular and Reiected Status Children

Rejected Status Study

Conceitedness Feinberg, Smith, & Schmidt, 1958

Unattractiveness Elkins, 1958

Disruptive behaviour Bemdt, 1983; Carlson, Lahey, & Neeper, 1984; Coie, Dodge, & Coppoteili, 1982; Feinberg, Smith, & Schmidt, 1958

Aggressiveness Bemdt, 1983; Carlson, Lahey, & Neeper, 1984; Dodge, 1983

MaIadaptive social Carlson, Lahey, & Neeper, 1984 behaviour

OE-task behaviou. Gottman, Gonso, & Rasmussen, 1975; Vosk, Forehanci, Parker, & Rickard, 1982

- - --

Popular Status Study . - - - -

Intelligence Feinberg, Smith, & Schmidt, 1958

Athletic ability Feinberg, Smith, & Schmidt, 1958

Friendliness/ Bemdt, 1983; Coie, Dodge, & Coppotelli, 1982; Dodge, Cooperativeness 1983; Kuhlen & Lee, 1943; Rubin & Daniels-Behess, 1983

Sense of humour Elkins, 1958; Feinberg, Smith, & Schmidt, 1958; Gronlund & Anderson, 1957; Kuhlen & Lee, 1943

CheerfllVEnthusiasm Kuhlen & Lee, 1943

Faimesdhonesty Feiuberg, Smith, & Schmidt, 1958

On-task behaviour Vosk Forehand, Parker, & Rickard, 1982

Attractive Appearance Gronlund & Anderson, 1 957

Leadership Coie, Dodge, & Coppotelli, 1982; Dodge, 1983

Page 13: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

12

the behaviour pattern of various social status groups in first and third grade boys. Both

peers and teachers were asked to rate students dong specified factors. Results indicated

that peers view popular classmates as more prosocial, better at sports, and more h y

than average status classmates. A review by Coie, Dodge, and Kupersmidt (1990)

examined data fiom peers, teachers, and observers concerning the behavioural profiles of

8- to 12-year-old normally-achieving children in each sociometric group. The authors

reviewed many early and more recent investigations on behaviour and social status and

concluded that, in normal populations, being helpfiil, cooperative and considerate,

following rules, and demonstrating athletic ancl acadernic cornpetence were the reasons

that accounted for popular children's hi& acceptance.

More recently, research has not only sought to expand our understanding of

popular and rejected children in the normal but has begun increasing efforts

to better understand children classified as neglected and controversial. Newcomb,

Bukowski, and Pattee (1993) evaluated empirical support on sociometric status profiles in

a meta-analysis of 41 studies examining the behavioural differences among groups of

elementary children (aged 5-12 years) categorised into sociometric categories. The four

other status groups (Le., popular, neglected, rejected, and controversial) were compared

with the average status group. Criteria for study selection included use of peer

nominations or peer ratings, inclusion of a cornparison group of children with average

sociometric status, and elementary-school-age parlicipants. The results of the meta-

analysis indicated that children in each sociometric group had distinct behavioural

profiles when contrasted with children in the average status category. Generally, children

classified in the neglected category exhibited few differences f?om average status peers,

however, they appeared to be less aggressive, less socidy interactive, and not as well

known in cornparison to their average peers. Controversial children, on the other hancl,

were found to share characteristics of both rejected and popular children. For example,

controversial children had higher levels of aggression than students in the rejected

Page 14: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

13

category but levels of sociability sirnilar to those in the popular group than average

shidents. The authors concluded that controversial children, compared to rejected

children, have better cognitive and social abilities that buffer them fiom peer rejection

and social exclusion. Rejected children were found to be more aggressive, less sociaily

and cognitively skilled, more socially withdrawn, and exhibithg higher levels of

depression and anxiety than their average status counterparts. Finaliy, popular children

showed higher levels of sociability and cognitive abïlities and lower levels of aggression

and withdrawal, and positive social actions compared to average children. Newcomb et

al. (1993) concluded that the behavioural repertoire of popular children mainly contains

socially skilled and academicdy competent behaMour which lead to positive social

experiences and outcomes, thus making these children candidates for status as a preferred

peer.

Studies have been conducted examining less observable aspects of social status in

children as well. One of the few studies conducted to evaluate the self-perceptions of

popular children was canied out by Boivin and Bégin (1989). The authors evaluated the

relationship among peer status and self- and other-perceptions of social competence

among 9- and 1 1-year-old boys and girls. ResuIts revealed that the self-perceptions of

popular children were significantly more positive than the average children on the

academic, social acceptance, athIetic, and self-esteem dimensions of the Perceived

Cornpetence Scde (Harter, 1982). No significant differences were found between popular

and average children in self-perceived physical appearance or behavioural conduct. No

significant differences were f o n d between neglected and average children, while

controversiai children were sigdicantly more negative than average children on

academic, behaviour, and self-esteem dimensions.

Patterson, Kupersmidt, and Griesler (1990) studied the relationship among

children's subjective reports and objective measures of their competence in behavioural,

scholastic, social cornpetence, and global self-esteem according to sociomehic status.

Page 15: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

14

Participants in the study were thkd and fourth grade boys and girls. The Self-Perception

Profile for Children (Harter, 1985) was used to assess self-perceptions. Results reveal that

(a) popular children perceived themselves as signifïcantly higher in behavioural

competence than rejected and neglected children, (b) popular children reported

signifïcantly higher self-perceived social competence than neglected children, (c) popular

and average children underestimated their =tuai liking by other children, and (d) no

status group effect in global self-worth was found In general, the trend was for popular

children to report hïgher, but not always significantly so, self-perceptions of competence

than al1 other status groups.

In summary, distinct behavioural profiles exist and can be rneasured in children of

varyîng sociometric status classifications. Popular children are seen as cooperative, better

leaders, fair and honest, good looking, athletic, able to stay on-task, funny, and generally

competent in social, cognitive, and academic domains. Rejected children are seen as

aggressive, dimptive, socially unskilled, weak a~adernically~ and socially withdrawn.

Neglected and controversial status children fall somewhere between the behavioural

extremes of rejected and popular chiIdren, while average-statu children have often been

used for cornparison purposes and infiequently studied on their own. Given our

understanding of the social-behavioural profles of average-achieving children, it is

important to determine whether simiIar behavioural profiles exist for students with LD, as

this knowledge can broaden our understanding ofthe social competence in this

population.

Sociometric Profiles of Children with Learning Disabilities

Despite a need for research in the area (Ochoa & Olivarez, 1995), very few

researchers have exarnined the social-behavioural profiles of different sociometric groups

of children with LD. Additionally, virtually no evidence exists regarding the self-

perceptions of competence of sociometrical1y classified children with LD. One of the

earliest studies addressing the relationship between child attributes and peer popularity

Page 16: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

was conducted by Siperstein, Bopp, and Bak (1978). The authors studied the relationship

of social status to athletic ability, physical appearance, and academic ability in 22

children with LD attending regular classrooms for at least 75% of the &y. Participants

with LD were compared to non-LD classrnates (0=155) on a series of peer ratings asking

children to nominate the best athlete, the smartest, and the best looking student in the

class. Children without LD were voted smartest in thek class, while an equal proportion

of children with and without LD received nominations for best athlete and best looking.

Of further interest, six of the twenty-two children with LD in the sample were liked by

one-third or more of the boys or girls in their classes. Of these children, five received

several nominations for best athlete in the class. The authors postulated that certain

nonacademic talents of children with LD may be considered as redeeming qualities

promoting positive peer status.

Another important study on the social-behavioural profiles of children with LD

was conducted by Kistner and G a t h (1989)- These authors administered a positive and

negative sociogram (PEI: Peer Evaluation Inventory; Pekarîk, Prinz, Liebert, Weintraub,

& Neale, 1976) and coiiected ratings of dependence, unassertiveness, and passivity fkom

44 mainstreamed children with LD in grades three through five. Higher scores on the

PEI'S aggression and withdrawal scales were positively correlated with negative

nominations. As well, negative nominations were positively correlated with peer mtings

of dependence, unassertiveness, and passivity . Lastly, rejected children with LD were

more Likely to be considered as aggressive and withdrawn than popular or accepted

children with LD. Ackerrnan and Howes (1 986) examined the relation between

sociometric status and participation in der-school social activïties ;in 6- to 1 3 -year-old

boys attending a private school for children with LD. Results indicated no signifïcant

relation between popularity and the nurnber or fiequency of after-school programmes

attended. PopuIarity was, however, significantly related to the nequency of informal get-

togethers with fkiends. Children not receiving nominations for being best liked (i.e.,

Page 17: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

unpopuiar children) were involved in fewer after-school programs, played with fiends

less often, and engaged in fewer social activities than other children in the sample.

Finally, Perlmutter, Crocker, Cordray, and Garstecki (1983) investigated peers' views of

weli-iiked and disliked grade ten adolescents with LD and found that well-üked students

with LD were rated as significantly more independent and withdrawn, and had more

insight into how others viewed themselves than disliked peers with LD.

One study which recognised the utility of sidtaneously examinuig behavioural

profiles of children with and without LD was conducted by Wiener, Harris, and Shirer

(1990). The authors e x h e d how non-LD peers view the social behaviour of their LD

peers and the extent to which these views are related to peer status. Participants for the

study were 9- to 12-year-old students with (@ = 90) and without LD (n = 499). Results

ftom the seven scaies of the peer-rated Social Behaviour Nomination Scale (adapted fiom

Dodge, 1983) indicated that overall, non-LD children were nominated by their peers

signincantly more often than children with LD as cooperative, as leader, and as clown.

Results M e r revealed significant between-group differences for the various social

status groups. For example, popular children without LD were viewed as more like

leaders, more cooperative, and more cornical than neglected, rejected, and average

children without LD- Unfortunately, due to extremely small cell sizes for certain social

status categones (popular-LD, = 1) the authors were unable to compare LD and non-LD

groups along the seven dimensions of the Social Behaviour Nomination Scale. Finally,

Nabuzoka and Smith (1993) attempted to i d e n w the behavioural profiles associated with

each of the sociometric status groups in 8- to 12-year-old children with and without LD.

The authors found that peer nominations for cooperation were significantly related to

being rated as "liked most" for children with LD and that nominations for being

disruptive, fighting, and bullying were sigd5cantly related to being rated as "liked least."

Few studies have been conducted examining the behavioural profiles of

sociometric groups of children with LD, especially those children with LD who are

Page 18: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

17

popular or weU-accepted. Of the existing studies, many used very small sample sizes and

lacked theoretical b e w o r k s . Thus, unlike the wealth of investigations dealing with the

social hctioning of socially-accepted chilciren without LD, M e r research is clearly

needed on the nature of social competence in weil-liked children with LD. m a t remains

to be detennined is whether the attributes associated with average-achieving children's

popularity and social acceptance are simïiar to or different fkom the attributes which

facilitate social acceptance in children with LD. Although Kistner and G a t b (1989)

concluded that the correlates of social status for children with LD are similar to those

reported for non-LD peers, they based this conclusion on minimal empirical evidence.

TUT-Kaspa and Bryan (1 995) have recently reiterated the notion that one of the paths to

social competence in children is academic achievement. Thus, as children with LD have a

Iower likelihood of being perceived as acadernicaüy competent by their peers, it is

possible they May not have the same opportunities for displaying socially competent

behaviour as their non-LD peers. Hence, there May be a greater potential for peer-

accepted students with LD to compensate for their Iack of academic ski11 wïth strengths in

nonacademic areas such as social skills, athletics, physical appearance, and leadership

skills.

Original Contribution to Knowled~e

This study compared the behavioural and emotional profiles of socially-accqted

children with and without LD in order to detennine the nature of social acceptance and

social competence in children with LD. Limited research has been conducted to

determine what behaviour or emotional characteristics distinguish accepted children with

LD fiom accepted children without LD. Whereas previous studies in this area have

utilised peer ratings or teacher ratings to determine correlates of popularity and social

acceptance in chWren with LD, this study provided an original contribution to the area

by also soliciting personal i n t e ~ e w s with peer-accepted children with and without LD

and thek peers. Objective instruments tapping peer and teacher ratings and self-ratings

Page 19: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

18

were also admuustered in an attempt to better understand the nature of peer acceptance in

children with LD h m a multidimemional standpoint This study offered an essential and

unique theoretical contribution to the body of Iiterature on the social fhctioning of

children with LD by systematicaily examining the global social competence of peer-

accepted children with LD in comparkon to their peer-accepted non-LD peers within the

theoretical fiamework provided by Vaughn and Hogan's (1 990) mode1 of social

competence.

Theories of Social Competence in Children with Leaming Disabilities

Social competence is a broad term which is often used to describe a varie@ of

social behaviours ranging fkom understanding and using social skills to social acceptance

(Haager & Vaughn, 1995). Social competence has long been regarded as a basic

characteristic of human hctioning (Gresham, 1 988). For example, in its early

conception, social competence was declared by Thomdike (1927) as one of three kinds of

intelligence. Over fifty years later a similar notion of social competence as an

independent form of intelligence (Le., interpersonal intelligence) ernerged in the work of

Gardner (1983). SociaI competence and subsequent social adjustment can be considered

important and needed goals for ail school-aged children (Parker & Asher, 1987). Two

popuiar models of social competence to emerge in the literaîure on leaming disabiiïties

are the models proposed by Gresham and ReschIy (1988) and Vaughn and Hogan (1990).

Gresham and Reschlv Model of Social Cornpetence

Gresham (1 98 8) and Gresham and Reschly (1 98 8) conceptualised social

competence as encompassing two fundamental components, -each emphasising different

aspects of social behaviour: (a) adaptive behaviour and @) social skïlls. Adaptive

behaviour includes independent functioning skills, physical development, language

development, and academic competencies. Social skills, on the other hand, include: (a)

interpersonal behaviours (e-g., accepting authority, conversation skills, cooperative

behaviours, play behaviours), (b) self-related behaviours (e-g., expressing feelings, etbical

Page 20: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

19

behaviour, positive attitudes towards self), and (c) task-related behaviours (e.g., attending

behaviour, completing tasks, following directions, independent work). This model of

social competence appears to be useful at a conceptud level because it distinguishes

between tangible behaviour within the context of adaptive, interpersonai, and task-related

fllnctioning and intangible self-related behaviours. However, it is a static model in that it

does not directly take into consideration the interaction between adap tive behaviour and

social skills nor the resuits of socially skilIed behaviour such as acceptance by peers.

Vau* and Hogan Mode1 of Social Competence

A second model of social competence has been advmced by Vaughn and Hogan

(1990). The authors proposed a similar, yet more comprehensive, model in which they

view social competence as a higher order construct, similar to intelligence, that includes

the following four components: (a) positive relations with others, (b) seK-evaluations of

competence, (c) absence of maladaptive behaviour, and (d) effective social skills. Positive

relations with others c m include general peer status, patterns of fiïendships and intimate

relations, or family relations and can be represented by meaSuTes of peer acceptance such

as peer rathgs and peer nominations. Self-evaluations of competence refer to the notion

of self-concept and can be measured by self-appraisal instruments tapping self-

perceptions in various domains of competence. Absence of maiadaptive behaviour can be

evaluated through behaviour-problem rathg scales including self-, peer-, and teacher-

ratings of behaviour. Finally, effective social skiils include a variety of cornpetencies

fkequently targeted for intervention withïn the school social context and appraised via

social skills teacher- or parent-rating systems (Vaughn & Haager, 1994).

Vaughn and Hogan (1990) asserted that the interaction and interrelationship

between the four components @eer relations, self-concept, behaviour problems, and

social skills) are likely to yield the truest understanding of socially competent behaviour.

They also stated that, for the purpose of assessrnent and research, the four components

must be isolated and measured separately, but that it is important in terms of

Page 21: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

20

interpretation and understanding of social competence that each component be considered

in consort with the others (Kaager & Vaughn, 1995). Despite this message, there bas been

no study conducted to date which simultaneously examines these components in a

multivariate model. Further, past studies referring to the Vaughn and Hogan model of

social competence have considered children with LD as one homogeneous group and

have not malysed the social competence of children with different social status.

Consequently, this study is unique in that it utilises the Vaughn and Hogan model as a

fhmework for examining the social competence of socidy-accepted children with and

without LD fÏom a rnultidimensional and multirater perspective. The model w i . be used

as a k e w o r k for organising the review of literature related to this study and for

generating research questions to be investigated.

Peer relations. Relationships with peers constitute a central element in chifdren's

social fives &hrîup, 1 983) and healthy peer relations are considered to be critical aspects

of sound social adjustment (Parker & Asher, 1987). Cbildren who expenence serious

problems with peer relationships are more at risk for developing additional adjustment

problems later in Life, including academic and behavioural problems during adolescence

(Roff, Sells, & Golden, 1972) and mental health difficulties (Cowen, Pederson, Babigian,

h o , & Trost, 1973).

Peer relations is a large concept which can be broken down into two related but

distinct categorïes (a) peer acceptance (popularity) and (b) fiiendship (dyadic relations).

Popularity and niendship are seen as distinct aspects of children's peer relations

(E3ukowski & Hoza, 1989; Parker & Asher, 1987; Schneider, Wiener, & Murphy, 1994)-

Popularity is a uni-directional construct related to general group perceptions towards a

particular individual, whereas friendship is bidirectional, specific, and refers to a de f i t e

type of expenence between two individuals (Bukowski & Hoza, 1989). Thus, it is

possible to be generally popular among peers while not having any really close fiends or

to be unpopular with peers at large and stiil have close, mutual fiïendships.

Page 22: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

It is well known that level of group acceptance and overall social status is a

consistent predictor of a wide range of positive and negative social-ernotional outcornes

@OU, 1996; Parker & Asher, 1987); however, our knowledge of the important social,

emotional, and behaviourai hks to group acceptance in children with LD rem-

Limited. As such, it was deemed necessary for this investigation to focus on gaining a

better understanding of the general, unilateral view (Le., peer acceptance) of the group

toward children with LD, rather than focus specifically on dyadic processes (Le.,

fiiendships) . Several assessment procedures are available to determine the existence of peer-

relation ~ c u l t i e s . The most fiequently used procedues are sociomeûics, rankings or

ratings b y others (e.g . , teachers, parents), and self-report. Sociometrïc assessrnent

procedures were developed by Moreno (1934) who used peer-nomination methods to

evaluate children's fïiendship patterns. Two basic types of sociometric procedures, each

of which appears to measure different aspects of sociometric status, have been used since

that time: (a) peer nominations and (b) peer ratings. Peer nomination procedures involve

asking children to name members of their class or playgroup who have certain

characteristics such as helpfulness, shyness, iXendliness or meet certain criteria such as

most or least liked, most or least desirable to play or work with, or as fiend, The basic

procedure in nominations is to have children name a specified number of peers according

to certain nonbehaviourd criteria, These criteria are temied nonbehavioural because they

are based upon activities such as work or play, or attributes such as being withdrawn or

pleasant, rather than on specific behaviour (e-g., shares the most). Nomination procedures

therefore tap children's attitudes toward or preferences for engaging in certain activïties

with peers rather than the speci£ic behaviourd performance of peers (Gresham &

Reschly, 1988)-

Nominations can be keyed to both positive or most liked and negative or least

liked criteria. Research using peer nominations indicates that positive and negative

Page 23: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

nominations are measuring two distinct dimensions of sociometrïc statu (Asher &

Hymel 198 1). Positive peer nominations measure popularity or fiiendship (when

reciprocal) in the peer group and negative nominations rneasure rejection in the peer

group (Gresham & Reschly, 1988). Asher and Renshaw (198 1) point out that acceptance

and rejection are not poles on the same continuum because some children receive few

nominations fiom their peers and are considered neglected and isolated because they do

not invoke much attention in their peers.

Rating-scale procedures require children to objectively rate every member of their

group on a continuum ranging fÏom a statement of strong acceptance (a rathg of 4 or 5)

to one that expresses strong rejection (a rating of 1) according to some specified criterion

(e.g., play partner, work partnery likability) rather than the preferential selections

associated with nomination techniques. The benefit of this technique is that each

participant in the class is rated, and because the students are provided with a roster of

consenting classmates, there is little chance that any child will be forgotten (Gresham &

Reschly, 1988). Peer nominations and peer ratings appear to measure independent

dimensions of social fhctioning (Asher & Hymel, 198 1); however, not all researchers

agree on this point (see Bukowski & Hoza, 1989). Positive peer nominations appear to

measure popularity among feliow peers or even friendships when considered reciprocdyY

as the rater is asked to nominate (or choose) a finite number of classmates for a given

interpersonal criterion. On the other hand, peer ratings appear to capture the child's

overd level of acceptance or Iikability among peers, as this fonn of memernent

requires that each classrnate be rated by fellow classmates (Gresham & Reschly, 198 8).

Used in combination, peer nominations and peer ratings provide a two-dimensional

sociometric approach to determine the sociai preference or sociai likability and social

impact or social salience of children.

Numerous studies have utilised sociometrics to study the peer diffïculties of

students with LD. In an extensive review of the relevant peer-status literature, Wiener

Page 24: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

23

(1987) observed that 14 of 18 of the reported studies found children with LD to be lower

in peer status than their nondisabled peers. Seven of these 14 studies used the peer

nomination sociometrÏc technique (Bryan, 1974; Bryan, 1976; Garrett & Crump, 1980;

Horowitz, 198 1; Scranton & Ryckman, 1979; Siperstein, Bopp, & Bak, 1978; Siperstein

& Goding, 1983); three used the Peer Acceptame Scale (Bniininks, 1978% 1978b;

Sheare, 1978); and four used rating-scale procedures to measure peer status (Bender,

Wyne, Stuck, & Bailey, 1984; Gresham & Reschly, 1986; Hutton & Polo, 1976;

Perhutter, Crocker, Cordray, & ~arsteCki, 1983). The remaining four studies did not

iden- differences between children with LD and without LD in peer status (Bursuck,

1983; PriIlUnan, 1981; Sabomie & KauEinan, 1986; Sainato, Zigmond, & Strain, 1983).

Many of the early studies reviewed by Wiener (1987) conceming peer statu

among children with LD provided somewhat misleading results due to various

methodological shortcomings. Some smered fiom fiaws in their choice of samples. For

example, Bursuck (1 983) compared extremely s m d @ = 12)) exclusively male, and

poorly defïned samples of children with LD to low-achieving children in terms of their

sociometrîc status. At least two other studies were methodologically flawed in their use

of sociometric procedures. For example, Prilliman (1981) examined the social acceptance

of 362 students in fkst through sixth grade, 28 of whom had leamhg disabilities. While

results showed no ciifferences between children with and without LD in terms of

acceptance, these hduigs must be viewed with caution because the researcher employed

a nonspecific sociometric téchnique-children were simply asked to indicate their k t ,

second, and third choice of classrnates next to whom they would like to sit. Garrett and

Cnimp (1980) may have obscured their data by numerically combining acceptance and

rejection ratings in calculaîhg a total social status score. Finally, certain shidies in the

review presented misleading results. For example, Horowitz's (198 1) hypothesis that

children with LD are less popular than their normal peers is only confïrmed when the

subject's intelligence is not controlled for by means of an ANCOVA. Scranton and

Page 25: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

24

Ryckman's (1979) conclusion is thought to be based upon misinterpretation of statistical

interactions and a nonsignificant main effect for gender (see Dudley-Marling &

Edmiaston, 1985 for a complete critique). Despite the methodological problems in these

few studies, Wiener's overall obse~ation that the preponderance of h d i n g s indicated

that children with LD obtained lower peer status scores than their non-LD peers remains

accurate, since the majonty of studies cited in Wienefs review did not suffer fiom

methodological flaws.

Since the t h e of Wiener's (1987) review, many studies have aOtempted to dari@

the nature of peer acceptmce and social status in children with LD. Stone and La Greca

(1990) compared the sociomettric classification of 57 (38 boys, 19 girls) mainstreamed

fourth- to sixth-graders with school-identifïed LD to 490 (233 boys, 257 girls) non-LD

peers. The researchers found that LD students obtained significantly lower sociometric

scores relative to their nondisabled peers. As weli, students with LD were

overrepresented in the rejected and neglected categories and underrepresented in the

popular and average groups. More recently, Conderman (1995) compared the social status

of 74 school-identified sixth- and seventh-graders with LD (48 boys and 26 girls) to a

matched sample of 74 students without LD. Aii students with LD in the study were

receiving between 30 and 120 minutes of pull-out resource-room assistance daily and al1

were mainstreamed into a general social studies class at the t h e of investigation. Results

indicated that overall, when compared to their nondisabled peers, students with LD

received more negative and fewer positive votes on the sociometric technique. Swanson

and Malone (1992) reviewed over 20 studies relating to the social skills deficits of

children with learning disabilities and concluded that children with LD were less liked

and more rejected than non-LD peers. Likewise, Ochoa and Olivarez (L 995) conducted a

meta-analysis of the Literature concerning the social skills and sociometric status of

children with LD. The studies in their review were selected fiom between-group design

investigations that compared the peer acceptance or social status of children with and

Page 26: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

without LD via the use of peer-rathg sociometric techniques. The authors concluded,

based on the results of 17 studies, that pupils with LD have lower sociomeûic status

when compareci with their nondisabled comterparts-

There is considerable evidence that low-achieving children in general are Iess

accepted than high-achieving peers (Hartup, 1983). Dehitive evidence indicating that

the social ~ c u l t i e s of chiidren with LD are mainly attributable to low achievement,

however, is lacking and the evidence that has been umvered is questionable due to

methodological problems involved in differentiating students with LD eom low-

achieving students (Wiener, personai cornmunication)- For example, Sater and French

(1989) found that both elementary level students with LD and low-achieving non-LD

pupils received signincantly lower ratùigs than normdly-achieving peers on a peer-rating

sociometnc measure, but the participants with LD did not m e r significantly h m low-

achieving children, indicating that achievement level rather than having a leaming

disability may be an important determinate of peer status, In contrast, Wiener, Harris, and

Shirer (1 990) examined the rdationship between IQ, achievement, and peer status in 9- to

12-year-old students with LD usïng positive and negative peer nominations. Students

with LD were less Ucely to be popular and more likely to be rejected and neglected than

students without LD. Additionally, both achievement Ievel and ZQ were deemed non-

meaningful predictors of peer status in childrm with LD.

Wiener, Harris, and Duval (1 993) re-analysed the data described in the Wiener et

al. (1990) study and determined that another variable to consider in examining the social

status of children with LD is the issue of identification, Identification was dehed as the

method and critena by which students were deemed to have met criteria for having a

leaming disability. Wiener et al. (1990) had pointed out that there was a relationship

between LD identification and peer preference in that children who are not school-

identified as having LD but who othenvise meet the cnteria for having LD obtain higher

peer preference scores than the school-identified children with LD. Vaughn, Hogan,

Page 27: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

26

Kouzekanani, and Shapiro (1990) examined the issue of identification, social status, and

social skills in (a) students with LD prior to identification, (b) low-achieving students, (c)

average-achievùig students, and (d) high-achieving kindergarten students. Peers'

perception of social status and teacheç' assessrnent of behaviour problems and social

skills were gathered in the fd and spring of the students' kindergarten year. Results

showed that as early as eight weeks &er entering kindergarten, children who were later

identified as LD received Iower peer acceptance ratings than their average-achieving and

high-achieving peers. The authors concluded that later social ~ c u l t i e s of some students

with LD are not exclusively due to a history of low achievement and low teacher

acceptance. Thus, as opposed to the conclusions that can be drawn regarding the poor

social acceptance of low-achieving children without LD (e.g., Hartup, 1983), the

relationship between social status and achievement level in children with LD is unclear at

this tirne.

FinalIy, as a challenge to some of the aforementioned investigations, numerous

studies have indicated that certain children with learning disabilities are as accepted as

their non-LD peers. Perhutter, Crocker, Cordray, and Garstecki (1983) studied 55 ninth

through twelfth grade students (41 boys, 14 girls) diagnosed by their school as havuig a

learning disability and spending a portion of each day in special education classes. These

researchers found that nearly a quater of the students with LD were among the most

popular classrnates of the non-LD students when using a peer-rating sociometrîc

procedure. Kistner and Gatlin (1989) studied the peer acceptance of children with and

without LD in third, fourth, and fifth grade c1assrooms. Children with LD (N = 44; 26

boys, 18 girls) were school-identified and enrolled in a resource specialist program for

not more than L2 hours a week. Positive and negative peer nominations indicated that

over 40% of their sarnple with LD was classifïed as either accepted or popular. Similarly,

Wiener, et al. (1990) reported that about half (48 out of 90) of their students with

research-identified LD (nine- to twelve-year-olds) had at least average sociometric status.

Page 28: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

27

Stone and La Greca (1990) utiliseci a positive peer nomination measure procedure with a

sample of 57 maimtreamed students with LD (38 boys, 19 girls) and 490 non-LD

students (233 boys, 257 girls) and found that while the children with LD were

overrepresented in the rejected and neglected groups (75%), at least 17% of theçe

children were classified as average or popular by îheir non-LD peers. Vaughn, McIntosh,

Schumm, Haager, and Callwood (1993) also found that 50% of students with LD in their

study were cfassified as either average or popular using the peer nomination sociornetric

technique, FinalIy, Conderman (1995) employed forced-choice peer-nomination

sociometrics with sixth and seventh grades with @ = 74) and without = 74) LD. Using

two social status classification systems (Asher & Dodge, 1986; Siperstein, Bopp, Bak,

1978), the author found that 50 to 70% of the students with Ieaming disabilities held

positions of at least average social status. Conderman (1995) concluded that certain

school-identified children with LD experience acceptable social standing among their

nondisabled peers.

Self-concept. Self-concept is difficult to define and even more difficult to

operationalise. Despite its elusive nature, the construct of seK-concept ments significant

interest in educational research because of its dual importance as both a consequence oÇ

and infiuence on, educational experiences (Shavelson, Hubner, & Stanton, 1976).

Generally, self-concept c m be defined as how a child perceives himself or herself and as

such can be used interchangeably with the term self-perception menick & Harter, 1988).

Specifically, self-concept or self-perception refers to the perception of ourselves

involving our attitudes, feelings, and knowledge about our skills, abilities, appearance,

and social accep tability (B yme, 1984).

Researchers have dBered with respect to the conceptuaiisation and measurement

of children's self-concepts (Clever, Bear, & Juvonen, 1992). Before the 1970s most

research in the area of children's self-concept used unidimensional instruments that

measured widely ranging percep fions or general self-concept (Chapman, 1 98 8).

Page 29: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

28

Shavelson, Hubner, and Stanton (1976), however, attempted to improve the usefiilness of

the self-concept construct by advancing a mdtifaceted model of self-perceptions. They

developed a hierarchical, multi-dimensional theory of sekoncept which places general

self-concept at the apex and more context-specifïc components (e-g., self-perception of

athletic or mathematical ability) at the bottom. A number of years later, Harter (1985)

b d t upon hm previous work (Harter, 1982) and the work of Shavefson and colleagues

(1976) and developed the SelfTerception Profile for Children (SPPC) to investigate the

relationship that specific cornpetencies bear to children's overd evaluation of the seK

Harter's (1985) SPPC utilises a multidimensional model assessing how competent an

individual feels in the following domains: (a) general intellectual ability, (b) scholastic

competence, (c) social acceptance, (d) athletic competence, (e) physical appearance, ( f )

behavioural conduct, and (g) global self-worth. It is important to note that Harter does not

view global self-worth as an additive fiinetion of one's self-concept across the specifïc

domains (Kloomok & Cosden, 1994). Consequently, the scale contains separate sets of

items for each domain, with only moderate interdomain correlations.

A more recent version of Harter's (1985) SPPC is the SeE-Perception Profile for

Leaming Disabled Students (SPPLD; Renick & Harter, 1988). The SPPLD is a self-

report measme for assessing domain-specZc judgements of competence and perceived

worth as a person by children both with and without learning disabilities (Renick &

Harter, 1 98 8). The scale contains ten subscales (general intellectual ability, reading,

spelling, writing, mathematics, athletics, social acceptance, physical appearance,

behavioural conduct, and global selE-worth), each tapping a separate aspect of an

individual's self-concept. The validity of the SPPLD (Renick & Harter, 1988) and its

multidimensional mode1 has been supported by studies finding that children with leaming

disabilities are able to make distinctions between theu perceptions of overd1 self-worth

and other domain-specific self-concepts (Kistner, Haskett, White, & Robbins, 1987; Prie1

& Lesham, 1990; Renick & Harter, 1989). The SPPLD remains one of the most widely

Page 30: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

29

utilised self-concept measures for children with leaming disabilities.

Several researchers have investigated the academic and nonacademic (i-e., social

acceptance, athletic ability, behavioillal conduct, physical appearance, and global self-

worth) self-perceptions of preadolescent students with leamhg disabilities (see Chapman,

1988, for a review). Results have consistently shown that students with LD report lower

self-concepts in academic domains, but comparable self-perceptions of general setf-

worth, relative to non-LD peers (Heath, 1995). For example, Kloomok and Cosden

(1994) explored how some children with LD maintain a positive global self-concept

despite academic difEculties. The study investigated the relationship between global self-

concept and perceived competence in general intellectual ability and specific academic

domains in third through sixth grade children with LD. Most of the children with LD in

the study were found to have a positive global self-concept (67%) and a negative

academic seif-concept (85%). Kistner, Haskett, White, and Robbins (1 9 87) found LD

students (grades 4 and 6) to hold lower opinions of their cognitive abilities and similar

ratings for general self-esteem compared to normally-achieving peers. Grolnick and Ryan

(1 990) studied the self-perceptions of third through six grade students with and without

LD and found more negative self-perceptions of academic competence in the LD group,

but no differences in global self-worth. The results of these studies suggest that children

with LD are able to make distinctions between global self-worth (i-e., self-esteem) and

academic self-concepts (Kloomok & Cosden, 1994) and that they may hold self-

perceptions of general self-worth comparable to their non-LD peers.

In the social acceptance domain the hdings have been inconsistent (Heath,

1995). For example, some researchers have found Iower self-perceptions of social

acceptance in children with LD, relative to non-LD peers. For example, Margalif Raviv,

and Pahn-Steinmetz (1 988) investigated the structure of social cornpetence in fourth

through seventh grade children with and without LD and found that children with LD

viewed themselves as less acceptable socially than non-LD peers. La Greca and Stone

Page 31: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Compet ence

30

(1990) studied the self-perceptions of social acceptance in fourth through sixth grade low-

achievers, average- to high-achievers, and children with LD. Children with LD perceived

themselves as significantly lower in social acceptance than low-achieving or average- to

high-achieving children. Stiehr-Smith and Nagle (1995) recently compared the self-

perceptions of children with LD to non-LD peers by administe~g the Self-Perception

Profile for Learning Disabled Students (Renick & Harter, 1988) to tbird and fourth grade

students with LD and a comparison group of non-LD peers. The groups dif5ered on the

domain of social acceptance, with the non-LD goups' self-perception ratings being

significantly higher.

Other studies have found no differences between students with LD and non-LD

students' self-perception of socid acceptance as measured by the Renick and Harter

(1988) scale. For example, Kistner, Haskett, White, and Robbins (1987) studied the self-

perceptions of eiementary and middle school chddren with LD in comparison to their

normally-achieving peers and found that the groups did not differ in evaluations of their

social competence. Coleman, McHam, and Minnett (1992) investigated the self-perceived

social cornpetencies of third through sixth grade children with LD in comparison~to their

low-achieving peers without LD and found that children with LD considered themselves

comparable to other iow-achieving children in terms of socid self-concept. Vaughn and

Haager (2994) examined the self-perceived social competence ratings of students with

LD, low-achievers, and average- to hi&-achievers fiom kindergarten through fifth grade

using Harteis measures (Harter, 1985; Karter & Pike, 1984; Renick & Harter, 1988) and

found no differences among any of the groups. Finally, some authors have even found

higher reported self-perceptions of social acceptance in children with LD, relative to non-

LD peers (Wime, Woodlands, & Wong, 1982). Thus, out understanding of self-perceived

social competence in chiidren with LD is unclear at this time.

Self-perceptions of nonacademic quaiities have been studied less fkequently.

Wime, Woodlands, and Wong (1982) contrasted the self-concept of children with LD,

Page 32: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

3 1

normaliy-achieving, and gifted children- They found that students w i ~ LD had better

self-perceptions of athletic ability than their gifted cohorts, In contrast t o the Winne et al.

(1 982) finduigs, Kistner and colleagues (1987) found that students with LD reported

significantly lower self-perceptions of athletic ability than non-LD peeas. Only a few

researchers have found no differences between LD and non-LD groups on self-perceived

athletic competence (Margalit, Raviv, & Pahn-Steinmetz, 1988; Renick & Harter, 1 988;

Stiehr-Smith & Nagle, 1995). Studies examining the self-perception o f physicai

appeamnce report no differences between LD and non-LD groups as a whole

(Montgomery, 1994; Renick & Harter, 1988; Stiehr-Smith & Nagle, 1995)- FinaUy, self-

perceived behavioural conduct has been investigated by very few investigators; however,

both Renick and Harter (1988) and Stiehr-Smith and Nagle (1995) have found that

children with LD report significantly lower self-perceptions of behavioural conduct than

their average-achieving peers.

Recently, Heath and Wiener (1996) combined self-perceptions of athletic ability,

social acceptance, physical appearance, and behavioural conduct into a global

nonacademic selkoncept score with fifth and eighth grade children with and without LD

using data fiom the Self-Perception Profile for Learning Disabled Students (Renick &

Harter, 1988). While the researchers were primarily interested in the moderathg effects

of self-reported depressive symptomatology on nonacademic self-perceptions, they also

reported no significant multivariate group effects on the nonacademic self-perceptions

between children with and without LD indicating that these groups don't differ in these

areas of self-percep tion. In silmmary, the study of nonacademic self-perceptions in

children with and without LD is fairly lhnited and generally contradictory in nature.

Behavioural ~roblerns. The third element of the Vaughn and Hogan (1990) mode1

of social competence is rnaladaptive behaviour. Behaviour problems in children c m be

classified into two broad general categones (a) environmental conflict Cextemalising

problems) and (b) persond disturbance (internalising problems) (Achenbach &

Page 33: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

32

Edelbrock, 1978). Extemalising difficulties include aggression, acting out, poor attention,

or conduct problems, while internalising problems include inhibition, shy-withdrawn

behaviour, anxiety, or depression. Early research indicated that children with leaming

disabilities have behavioural profiles that are similar to children with externalising

behaviour disorders (KaufEnan, 1981; Moore & Simpson, 1983).

Measurement of children's behaviour varies along at least two dimensions (a) rater

(parent, teacher, peer, experimenter) and (b) setting (home and school). T ypically,

children's classroom behaviour is measured using checklists, questionnaires,

observational techniques, and teacher interviews. Children's behaviour in nonacademic

settings can be assessed by similar means. Research on the behavioural patterns of

children with leaming disabilities suggests an elevated prevalence of several kinds of

behavioural difficulties among children with LD compared to non-LD controls (Eliason

& Richman, 1988). These behaviours include attention problems, hyperactivity, and off-

task behaviow (McKinney & Feagans, 1984; McKinney, McClure, & Feagans, 1982), as

well as anti-social behaviour, aggression, and conduct problems (Cornwall & Bawdcn,

1992; La Greca & Stone, 1990; Stanton, Feehan, McGee, & Silva, 1990)-

The classroom behaviour of students with LD has received a great deal of

attention from researchers. Bryan (1974) observed third grade boys with and without LD

in their regular and special education ciassrooms and found that boys with LD attended

less than their non-LD peers during certain academic activities. M c h e y and colleagues

examined the classroom behaviours of early elementary-aged children with LD using

observational techniques and concluded that children with LD were more off-task and

more likely to interact with teachers than non-LD peers @fcKinney, McClure, &

Feagans, 1982; McKinney & Speece, 1983; Richey & McKinney, 1978). Haskins,

Walden, and Ramey (1983) found children with LD to be more disruptive in groups than

non-LD peers. In a meta-analysis of 25 studies using either teacher ratings or classroom

observations of classroom behaviour to directly compare children with LD and

Page 34: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

33

nonhandicapped children, Bender and Smith (1990) identified Iack of on-task behaviour

and excessive off-task behaviour, conduct disorders, distractibility, and shy-withdrawn

behaviour as problem areas for students with LD. Evidence gathered fiom the meta-

analysis supported earlier ideas that children with LD tend to behave inappropriately in

the classroorn (Bender, 1985; McKinney & Feagans, 1983). In summary, early studies in

the area consistently report that children with LD display more behaviourai diEculties in

the classroorn than non-LD peers. It is important to note that in the majority of cases boys

with LD outnumbered girls with LD in these studies' d e s i p .

Since Bender and Smith's (1990) meta-analysis, many researchers have continuai

to study the behaviour problems of children with leaming disabilities. For example,

Vaughn, Zaragoza, Hogan, and Walker (1993) conducted a longitudinal study

investigating the social skills and behaviour problems of children with LD, low-

achievers, and average- to hi&-achievers in kindergarten through third grade. Significant

group differences in problem behaviour were found, with the average- to hi&-achievers

obtaining significantly lower levels of behaviour problems than both the low achieving

and LD groups. However, analysis of simple effects showed no sigxiificant differences

among the three groups for conduct disorder, socialised aggression, anxiety, withdrawal,

or motor excess. Significant group differences in attention-problem scores were found

with the low-achieving group obtaining simiificantiy higher scores than both the LD and

average to high-achieving groups, while differences between the average- to hi&-

achieving and LD groups were not significant. The authors point out, in a later summq

of the study (Vaughn & Haager, 1994), that these results contrast with previous research

yielding higher behaviour problems for students with LD, compared to non-LD students.

The authors expressed caution about the vdidity of thek results, however, since most

research examining the behaviou. problems of youngsters with LD had been conducted

with children in at least the fourth grade rather than kindergarten to third grade and the

behaviour problems investigated in their study may not be evident at such early ages

Page 35: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

34

(Hogan, Quay, Vaughn, & Shapiro, 1989). Haager and Vaughn (1995) investigated the

extent to which third through sixth graders with LD, low achievers, and average- to hi&-

achievers ciiffer on ratings of behaviour problems fiom the perspective of parents and

teachers. The Social SkilIs Rating System (for Teachers and Parents) yielchg three

subscales (externalishg problems, internalising problems, and hyperactivity) was used in

the study. Significant group differences were found for ail three of the behaviour problem

scales as rated by teachers, with children with LD and low-achievers receiving more

problematic behaviour ratings than the average- to high-achievers. Results M e r

indicate a significant group difference for two of the behaviour problems as rated by

parents. The LD and low-achievers groups received signincantly higher ratings of

hyperactivity and internalising behaviour problems such as acting sad or depressed or

appearing anxious, shy, and withdrawn than the average to high-achieving group.

In summary, there have been numerous studies conducted comparing the

behavioural conduct of children with and without LD. The majority of these studies have

uncovered significant ciiffer ences between the behavioural characteristics of these

leamers (Bender & Wall, 1994), indicating that children with LD receive more

problematic behaviour ratings from parents and teachers than non-LD peers.

Socid skills. Social skills refer to positive social behaviours that contribute to the

initiation and maintenance of positive social interactions and to the avoidance of negative

responses (Elliott & Gresham, 1993; La Greca, 1993). The constmct of social skills has

often erroneously been considered synonymous with social competence (Elliott &

Gresham, 1993). Social competence should, however, be viewed as a summary phrase

referring to social judgement about the quality of an individual's performance in a given

situation (Elliott & Gresham, 1 993). Social skills refer more specifically to actual positive

behaviours such as cooperating, sharing, following instructions, complimenting, taking

turns in conversations, or joining ongoing peer groups. Elliott and McKùinie (1 993) point

out that "social skills are the bridge that connects the individual and his or her environ-

Page 36: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

35

ment and the mechanisrn by which social interactions are initiated, acted on, and

maintained" (p. 2).

. The development of appropnate social skills is a very important outcome of the

schooling process. Social skills facilitate a child's engagement in social interactions with

peers while integrating those skiiis needed to resolve conflict, maintain respect, affection,

and positive interactions with peers (Elliott & McKinnie, 1993). Fortunately, most

children are able to acquire and employ appropriate social skills in social interactions,

thus enabling positive relations with peers to develop. However, there is a large group of

children who have been found to be at risk for establiskg poor peer relations due to

social skills deficits (Elliott & Gresham, 1993). A number of investigators have

documenteci that students with mild handicaps such as learning disabilities, behaviour

disorders, or mild inteliectual impairments exhibit notable deficits in social skills (Coie,

1985; Gresham & Elliott, 1989; Gresham & Reschly, 1986).

Researchers have used several methods to assess social skills in school-aged

children, such as (a) self-report instruments, @) sociometric measures, (c) parent ratings,

(d) behaviourd observations, and (e) teacher ratings (Gresham & Elliott, 1984). R e g s

or ratings by others (e-g., teachers and parents) have been used fiequently in research on

the sociai skills of children with Iearning disabilities (e-g., Bursuck, 1989; Elliott &

McKinnie, 1993; Gresham & Reschly, 1986; Vaughn & Haager, 1994; Vaughn,

Zaragoza, Hogan, & Walker, 1993). One of the most comprehensive teacher-rating

procedures developed in recent years is the Social Skills Rating System-Teacher rating

fonn (SSRS-T; Gresham & Elliott, 1990). The SSRS-T is a nationally standardised rating

scale designed to measure a broad range of social behaviours for children between the

ages of 3 and 18 years. It consists of three broad domains with associated subscales: (a)

Social S kiils (cooperation, assertion, self-control); (b) Behaviour Problems (extemalising,

intemalking, hyperactivity); and (c) Academic Competence.

A number of researchers have provided empirïcal evidence on the social skills

Page 37: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

36

deficits of children with LD. Table 2 contains a List of select studies in this area and the

specific social skills deficits exhibited by children with LD in relation to non-LD peers,

Research has consistently documenteci that many children with LD suffer serious social

skilis deficits that may lead to impaired social interactions. Other studies suggest,

however, that some children with LD are not deficient in social skills (Horowitz, 198 1;

Perlmutter, Crocker, Cordray, & Garstecki, 1983; Sater & French, 1989). Hence, children

Table 2.

Select Studies Showine: Social Skills Deficits in Children with LD

Area of Deficit Study

Task-related social skills such as attendkg, completing tasks, following directions, on-task behaviour

Helping others

Accepting authority

Expressing feelings appropriately

Social cognition and social problem-solving

Cooperation, Assertion, Self-control

Responding to the feelings of others

Interpersonal communication skills;

Deciphering social cues

Social perspec tive-taking

Self-control in social situations

Gresham & Reschly, 1986; Bender & Smith, 1990

Gresham & Reschly, 1986

Gresham & Reschiy, 1986

Gresham & Reschly, 1986

Schumaker, Hazel, Shennan, & Sheldon, 1982; Vau- Mchtosh, & Spencer-Rowe, 1991

Haager & Vaughn, 1995

Bruck & Hebert, 1982

. Axelrod, 1982; Bryan, Sherman, & Fisher, 1980; Soenksen, Flagg, & Schmits, 1981

Wong & Wong, 1980

Elliott & McKinnie, 1993

Page 38: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

37

with LD should be considered a population at heightened risk for certain social skills

nifftculties, and not as a population which is consistently charocterised by such problems

(Sater & French, 1989). In fact, a general consensus is emerging in the field whereby it is

concluded that social skius deficits may coexist with learning disabilities but are not

necessarily caused by them (Kavale & Fomess, 1996)-

In summary, the Vaughn and Hogan (1990) four-domain model of social

competence (Le., relations with others, self-concept, behavioural conduct, and social

skills) is a very usefiil means by which researchers may study the overall social

competence of children. The model provides for the interrelationship among the four

components in effectively understanding and deteminhg the nahire of socially

competent behaviour exhiiited by youngsters. Shidies conducted on the peer status of

children with LD tend to affirm the notion that although groups of studentts with LD

remain either socially rejected or neglected, many children with LD experience

acceptable social standing among their non-LD peers. Recently, researchers are realising

that chiidren with LD rnay hold lower self-perceptions in academic areas, but comparable

self-perceptions of general self-worth, relative to non-LD peers, Studies conducted on the

behavioural patterns of children with LD have consistently revealed more behavioural

problems in students with LD (Bender & Smith, 1990); however, it has recently been

suggested that this may not always be the case (Vaughn & Haager, 1994). Finally,

numerous studies have shown that many children with LD dispIay social skills deficits

and subsequent low peer acceptance (Hazel& Schumaker, 1988). Despite this growing

body of knowledge concerning the social fiuictioning of children with LD in general,

Little is h o w n about the profile of social competence in children with LD who are well

accepted b y their peers.

Zn order to clar* the nature of social competence in children with LD it is

necessary to study whether distinguishing behaviours, characteristics, or cognitions exist

between peer-accepted children with and without LD. The present investigation

Page 39: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

38

comparing the social, emotional, and behavioural feiztures of average to high social statu

children with and without LD will enhance the existing body of literature on the social

competence of children with LD. Haager and Vaughn (2995) have recently conducted a

study investigating the sociai hctioning of students with leaming disabilities, low

achievement, and average to high achievement in which they concluded that because the

population of students with LD is heterogeneous with respect to many social competence

variables, it would be interesting t o h o w more about the characteristics of those children

with LD who exhibit high sociai functioning. This c d has been echoed by a number of

prominent researchers in the field Coleman & Minnett, 1992; Juvonen & Bear, 1992,

Vaughn & Hogan, 1994) and is addressed in the present study.

Page 40: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

39

Chapter IX: Rationale and Research Questions

Since the time of Bryan's research on the social functioning of children with LD,

research has consistently documenteci the diffïculties students with LD have in forming

and maintaining social relationships (Vaughn, Zaragoza, Hogan, & Walker, 1993). More

recently, researchers have discovered a certain amount of within-group variability ui

children with LD, suggestïng that some children with LD are socially accepted and even

popular amongst their non-LD peers (Kistner & Gatlin, 1989; La Greca & Stone, 1990;

Ochoa & Palmer, 1991; Stone & La Greca, 1990). Cuxrently, very little is known about

the social, emotional, or behavioral functioning of socially-accepted children with LD,

thus prompting researchers to call for M e r investigation of the exemplary qualitties

possessed by these children with LD (Coleman & Minnett, 1992; Haager & Vaughn,

1995).

Investigating the social competence of socially accepted children with LD is a

very important endeavour for a number of reasons, First, by studying the social

competence of accepted children with LD, more may be leamed about the socialising

conditions and personal attributes or skills inf2uencuig interpersonal and intrapersonal

success in the developing child with LD. Second, in planning intervention programmes

for children with LD who display or are at risk for low social status, information

regarding the key s u s and characteristics of accepted children with similar academic

profiles c m be very helpful. Finally, an increased understanding of the social-behavioral

profile of socially-accepted children with LD cm better inforni inclusionary efforts and

programming decisions.

In an attempt to address the need for such research, this study offers a unique

investigation into the nature of social acceptance in children with LD based on the four

domains of the Vaughn and Hogan (1990) mode1 of social competence: (a) peer relations,

(b) seif-concept, (c) behavioural conduct, and (d) social skills. The extent to which peer-

accepted children with LD differ fiom non-LD peers in social, emotional, and

Page 41: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

40

behavioural areas were specifically examined, in addition to investigation of the

distinguishing social, exnotional, or behavioural features between peer-acceptai children

with and without LD.

Hwotheses and Research Ouestions

Using the h e w o r k of the Vaughn and Hogan model of social competence,

specifk hypotheses and research quetions for this exploratory study were presented.

-4. Peer Relations: Social Status

Based upon previous research (Kistner & Gatlin, 1989; Stone & La Grma, 1990;

Vau& McIntosh, Schumm, Haager, & Caliwood, 1993; Wiener, Hams, & Shirer,

1990), it was hypothesised that:

k(i) Within the initial screening sample of children with LD all five categories of

sociometric social status (Le., popular, average, conhoversial, rejected, neglected)

would be represented.

A(@ A moderate proportion of students with LD (Le., roughly 40%) would

receive a favourable social status rating (i-e., sociometncally average or popular),

and approximately 10-20% of all students with LD wodd be rated as

sociometrically "popular" by their regular class peers.

B. Self-concmt

Considering that the Vaughn and Hogan (1990) model does not clearly allow for

an analysis of self-perceptions of academic competence and because the sample under

investigation in this study was considered, by definition, to display poor academic

achievement, this study focused exclusively on nonacademic self-concept.

Few studies to date have exarnined the nonacademic self-perceptions of children

with LD in high status sociometric groups (Le., average and popular). Because children

with LD may not have the opportunity of enhancing their social standing and self-

concepts through average or high average achievement in academic areas, it is possible

that they are more likely to engage in positive nonacademic self-perceptions as a way to

Page 42: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

41

compensate for reduced opportunities to gain social acceptance and feel positive self-

concepts through traditional academic means (see Hamip, 1983). Thus, it was suggested

that :

B.(i) Socially-accepted children with LD would have higher self-perception scores

in nonacademic domains than their matched non-LD geers,

C. Behavioural Conduct

In keeping with previous research indicating that many cbildren with LD exhibit

off-task behaviour, distractibility, and aggression (La Greca & Stone, 1990; McKinney &

Feagans, 1984), it was believed that:

C.(i) SocialIy-accepted children with LD would be rated by teachers as exhibiting

higher levels of disruptive classroom behaviour than matched non-LD peers.

D. Social Skills

Contrary to previous studies utilising the SSRS-T (e.g., Elliott & McKinnie,

1993), in which children with LD were rated by their teachers as less cooperative, less

assertive, and less able to exhibit self-control in social situations than non-LD peers, it is

likely that peer-accepted children with LD can compensate for their relative lack of

academic cornpetence and be perceived as more sociaily skilied by their teachers, relative

to non-LD counterparts. Inus, it was beiïeved that:

D.(i) Socially-accepted children with LD would be rated by their teachers as

exaibiting higher levels of sociaUy skilied behaviour than matched non-LD peers-

E. Distinmishinn Features

Research is limited in the area of peer ratings on sociometrically classified

children with LD. A few studies have shown, however, îhat well-liked children with LD

may be seen as more independent, as having more insight into how others feel about

hem, as more assertive, and as more cooperative than theu disliked peers with LD (e-g.,

Kistner & Gatlin, 1989; Nabukoza & Smith, 1993; Perlmutter, Crocker, Cordray, &

Garstecki, 1983). Kistner and Gatlin (1 989) suggest that the social, emotional, and

Page 43: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

42

behavioural correlates of social status for children with LD may be similar to those

reported for their non-LD peers, although this conclusion remains to be tested directly.

Thus, the question becomes:

E-(i) To what extent do socially-accepted children LD differ fiom their non-LD

peers on specific aspects of social, emotional, and behavioural fiinctioning?

Page 44: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

43

Chapter III: Method

Particioants

The onginal sample of participants consisted of 320 fourth and fifth graders in

four elementary schools withh the Montreal area AU four schools upheld inclusion

policies and practices followhg regular education initiatives wherein students with

academic or emotional difficulties were mainstreamed for the majorïty of the school day.

Schools varied, however, in how services were provided children with identified needs,

with only one school in the sample offering pull-out services for students identified with

special needs, and the other schools offering Limited identification and remedial senrices.

Participants were screened in areas of cognitive, academic, and social fünctioning

to obtain a final sample of socially-accepted children with LD and their matched controls

without LD. All participants attended regular (80% English, 20% French instruction) or

French Immersion (50% English, 50% French instruction) programmes. The schook and

participants represented low-middle to high socioeconomic status comunities. The

children came- fkom a variety of ethnic backgrounds (e.g., Anglo-Saxon, Greek, Italian)

and al l participants had Engtish as their fkst language. Participating children were

screened in order to obtain two target groups of students: (a) Socially Accepted-LD (LD)

and @) Socially Accepted non-LD (non-LD) matched controls. Participants were matched

on the basis of gender, age, IQ estimate, and classroom.

Extensive criteria were used in determinhg group membership. To be considered

as a student with a leamhg disability participants had to be meet two major cnteria: (a)

regular cIassroom placement with no resource room withdrawal or mainstreamed in the

regular class with resource room withdrawal of no more than one hour per day and (b)

independently classified as having a leaming disability on the basis of researcher-

administered standardised achievement and intelligence tests results. A researcher-

identification procedure for detemrining students with leaming disabilities was utilised in

place of a school-identified procedure due to the unique manner in which children with

Page 45: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

44

academic difficulties are identified and s e ~ c e d in the province of Quebec.

In Canada, education is a provincial jurisdiction where each province has its own

education act and policies p e r t e g to special education. The Ministry of Education in

the province of Quebec defines major categones of students with handicaps, leaming, or

adjustment d~cu l t i e s . The label learning disability is not found within the Quebec

special education regulations; rather, the concept of students with learning diffcuZties is

used. Under Quebec regulations, students with academic difficulties and delays are coded

as belonghg to one of the following sub-categories (a) code 1: mild leaming difficulty or

minor academic delay, dehed by an academic delay exceeding one year in the language

of instruction or mathematics and @) code 2: serious leaming difficulty, defined as either

a severe academic delay exceeding two years in the language of instruction or

mathematics, or as a specific and persistent learning difficulty in one or more of the

following processes that are essential to the development, use, or comprehension of

language: reading, writing, communication, or reasoning. Exclusionary criteria relatuig to

leaming difficulties not being primarily caused by sensory, mental, or physicai handicaps

are also recognised under Quebec special education guidelines.

The concept and definition of a learning disability as hrst described in Public Law

94-142 of the Education for Al1 Handicapped Children Act and updated in Public Law

101-476 of the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (DEA) in the United States is

well-understood in educational training and practice within Canada However, special

education identification and funding policies in Quebec follow a different set of

guidelines fkom those in the United States. In recent years, students in Quebec schools

identified (Le., coded) as expenencing academic delays have not always been able to

receive remedial instruction or seMces due to major hancial cut-backs to speciai

education fiinding. As a result, and in keeping with desired inclusionary practices, one of

the School Boards (goveming the majority of schools participating in the present study)

has drastically reduced psychoeducational assessments that once identified children with

Page 46: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

45

academic delays. As a consequence, very few students in these schools are fo rmdy

identified by school personnel as experiencing learning disabilities. Thus, in an effort to

secure the highest level of generalisability of results to past and £üture research conducted

in the broad field of leaming disabilities, participants in the present study were c1assifÏed

as having a leaming disability according to the following criteria parallehg the

interpretation of standards outlined in IDEA and endorsed by the Learning Disabilities

Association of Canada &DAC): (a) average or above average intelligence (Le., an IQ

score estimate of 85 and above) as measured by the IQ estimate comprïsing the WISC-III

subtest dyad of Block Design and Vocabulary; @) a discrepancy between the IQ estimate

and measured achievement on standardised measures (Le., WRAT-3) of 18 standard

points or more in at Ieast one area of achievement (i-e., spelling, arithmetic, o r reading);

(c) measured achievement in at least one area of the m T - 3 at or below one standard

deviation below the mean (i.e., standard score of 85 or below), and (d) absence of other

concomitant disabilities (e-g., senous emotional disturbance) or insuffr.cient o r

inappropriate instruction.

A significant discrepancy between IQ and measured achievernent is a

conventional method for determining classification as having a learning disability in

research of this nature. Researchers commonly utilise a discrepancy Eom 1 to 1.5

standard deviations between IQ and achievement in their criteria for having a leamïng

disability (Conderman, 1995; Stone & La Greca, 1990; Vaughn, McIntosh, Schumm,

Haager, & Callwood, 1993). In this study, a discrepancy of 18 standard points between

the IQ estimate and measured achievement corresponds to a difference of more than one

standard deviation (Le., 15 points) between standardised measures and is utilised since it

takes into account the approximate standard error of measurement (SEM) on the WRAT-

3 of between 3 and 5 points; that is, with a discrepancy of 18 points and a SEM of

approximately 3 points, the "true" discrepancy between eamed scores is free to vary

between 15 and 21 points which is roughly a difference of 1 to 1.5 standard deviations

Page 47: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

46

(i.e., the amount needed for a significant discrepancy).

Students were classified as non-LD based on the following criteria: (a) average

intelligence (Le., standard score estimate between 85 and 114) as measured by the IQ

estimate; and (b) average achievement (Le., standard score above 84) in a l l three areas of

the WRAT-3 (i-e-, spelling , reading, and arithmetic).

Using the aforementioned criteria, 38 (1 1.87%) students out of 320 were

classified as having a leaming disability, 156 (48.75%) as not having a learning

disability, and 126 (39.37%) as not classinable as either having or not having a learning

disabiiity- While the percentage of students classifTed as having speciflc LD in this study

is higher than the approximately five percent estimate reported by the U.S. Department of

Education (1994), conceptual and methodological variations in the definition and

measurement of LD have lead to inconsistent estimations of the number of students

experiencing LD in any one school population, and incidence rates as high as ten percent

have been reported (Ingersoll& Goldstein, 1993). Thus, the present incidence rate for LD

of nearly twelve percent is not seen as out of the ordinary, especially when considering

that some of the schools targeted by the researcher attracted high numbers of students

with learning dificulties due to their excellent inclusiooary practices.

The majonty (70.59%) of participants were classified as having LD based on

discrepancies between cognitive ability and reading or speliing achievement (35.29% for

reading and spellïng, 3 5.29% for spelling alone), while 17.65% had significant

discrepancies in al1 three achievement areas (reading, spelling, and arïthmetic), with the

remaïnder (1 1.76%) of students experiencing significant difficulty in arithmetic. The

relatively high number of students classified as having LD based on IQ-achievement

discrepancies in spelling alone raised concern given the number of participants attending

French Immersion schools, as these students may have expenenced less opportunity for

written English than non-French immersion populations. Conceni was considerably

lessened, however, in detemiining that mean WRAT-3 Spellùlg standard scores for the

Page 48: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Compet ence

47

three French Immersion schools were solidly in the average range (M = 103.48 where =

83, M = 109.22 where n = 74, and M = 102.51 where = 83).

Mesures

IO and Achievement

A short form of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IIi (WISC-III;

Wechsler, 199 1) and a standardised test of achievernent, the Wide Range Achievement

Test-3 (WRAT-3; Wiikinson, 1993), were used to identi& children with learning

disabilities fkom those without learning disabilities. The Block Design and Vocabulary

subtests of the WISC-III were utilised to provide a short form IQ estimate for each child

in the study. The Block Design subtest assesses the ability to perceive and analyse forms

by breaking down a whole into its component parts and thea assembling the components

into the identical design (Sattler, 1992)- The task involves visual-motor coordination,

spatial visualisation, percephial organisation, logic, and reasoning- The Vocabulary

subtest assesses word knowledge and verbal reasoning by having children explain orally

the meaning of various words. The subtest involves a variety of cognitive-related factors

including ability to leam, depth of information, richness of ideas, memory, concept

formation, and language development (Sattler, 1992). Both subtests have excellent

reliabiiity and used together represent a popular short form estimate of IQ corretating

highly with the Full Scale IQ (Sattler, 1992).

The WRAT-3 was used to determine achievement in spelling, arithmetic, and

reading for each subject. The WRAT-3 is a bnef achievement test with a mean of 100, a

standard deviation of 15, and a standard error of meastuement of 3 points. The WRAT-3

contains three subtests (a) Reading: the ability to name letters and pronounce words of

increasing phonological and orthographie difficulty; (b) Spelling: the ability to write

single wordç nom dictation; and (c) Anthmetic: mathematical calculation skills such as

counting, reading number sytnbols, and perfonning written computations Test-retest

coefficients for the WRAT-3 are reported in the manual (Wilkinson, 1993) as being

Page 49: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

48

higher than .90 for individuals ranging in age fiom 6 to 16 years old. Interna1 reliability

coefficients are also reljorted in the -80s and .90s for most age groups.

The short form of the WISC-III (e-g., Vocabulary and Block Design) has been

found to be highly correlated = -89) to the WISC-III F d Scde IQ (Sattler, 1992) and

has been fiequently utilised md supported in the literature as a valid and reliable IQ

estimate (e-g., Heath, 1995; Heath & Wiener, 1996; Masten, Morison, & Pellegrini, 1985;

Morison & Masten, 199 1). Likewise, the WRAT-3 has been a popular measure of

achievement in this area of research because it (a) is relatively quick to administer, (b) is

scaled in a manner which is comparable to the WISC-III (Le., M = 100, SD = 15), and (c)

measures three essential areas of academic achievement (reading, spelling, and

arithmetic) in which leaming disabilities are commoniy manifest. Both the Education for

AU Hmdicapped Children Act (PL 94- 142) and the Learning Disabilities Association of

Canada endorse the idea that spelling alone c m be a manifestation of a leaming disability

and that a learning disability is a disorder in one or more of the basic processes involved

in understanding or using language, spoken or M e n .

Two types of sociometric measures based upon the Asher and Dodge (1 986)

method of sociornetxic classification were employed in the present study. The Asher and

Dodge (1986) method utilises a combination of positive norriinations with peer ratings in

order to replicate the social status groups described in Coie, Dodge, and Coppotelli

(1982). The k s t part of the Asher and Dodge sociometric technique involved the peer-

rating process. Children were given a roster of al1 consenthg peers in their classroom and

were asked to indicate how much they liked to "play with" each peer by circluig a number

on a Likert-type scale from 1 to 4, where 1 indicated "not at dl" and 4 indicated "very

much." The primary advantage of this method is that important sociometric data are

obtained for al1 participating children and not just for those students nominated as a

preferred classrnate.

Page 50: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

49

To assess peer popuiarity, positive nominations were obtained by providing

participants with a class list of participating children and asking thern to circle the names

of three individuals fiom the list that they "Iïked the most" (see Appendix A). This

procedure was chosen over the standard Coie, Dodge, and Coppotelli (1982) method of

soliciting both positive nominations ("like the most") and negative nominations ("like the

least") due to ethical concems regarding the administration of negative peer nominations.

Furthemore, the nature of the present study does not require that rejected and neglected

children be explicitiy identified; rather, the interest of the present study is in those

children rated as average or better by their peers (i-e., those students who obtain more

"liked the most" nominations than feiïow classrnates).

Membership in social status groups was initially determined in the following

manner (cf. Asher & Dodge, 1986). First, the number of positive nominations received by

each child was calculated and standardised within each classroom- Next, the total number

of 1 ratings ("not liked at ail") received by each subject on the peer-rating scale was

tabulated and standardised withuz each classroom to index negative nominations. This

procedure yielded standardised Eed-most (Lw scores fiom the positive nomination data

and standardised liked-least P L ) scores from the number one ratings for each child. To

detemiine the sociometric status of the participants, social preference (SP) and social

impact (SI) scores were detennined fiom the liked-least and like&-rnost data Social

preference was computed as the LM score minus the LL score, while the social impact

score was computed as the sum of the LM and LL scores. These scores were then

standardised by classroom and used to denve the following five sociometric

classifications: (a) popular, children with an SP score greater than 1 .O, a LM score greater

than 0, and a LL score less than O; (b) rejected, children receiving a SP score less than - 1.0 (e-g., -1.2), a LM score less than 0, and a LL score greater than O; (c) neglected,

children receiving a SI score less than -1 .O (e.g., -1.2), and LM and LL scores each less

than O; (d) controversial, children receiving a SI score greater than 1.0, and LM and LL

Page 51: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

50

scores each greater than O; (e) average, children receiving a SP score between 0.0 and 1.0,

and a SI score between -1 -0 and 1 .O.

Studies utilising sociornetric procedures tend to Vary in how they operationalise

the traditional sociometric categories (Le., popular, average, rejected, neglected, and

controversial). The most variability appears to centre around the criteria for mernbership

in the average sociometric category. Some authors (e-g., Vaugbn, McIntosh, Schumm,

Haager, & Callwood, 1993) have followed the earliest mode1 set forth by Coie, Dodge,

and Coppotelli (1982) whereby the average sociometric category is sirnply defked as a

social preference score greater than -.5 and less than +S. Other authors (e-g., Conderman,

1995; Wiener, Harris, & ShÏrer, 1990) have chosen to define average social status

according to the Coie and Kupersmidt (1983) method, that is, as a "catch-ail" category

wherein average group social preference and socid impact -scores range fkom - 1 .O to

1.0. The Coie and Kupersmidt method is credited with creating a more traditional

definition of average status that allows for a greater inclusion of sociograrn data

(Conderman, 1995). The Coie and Kupersmidt (1 98 3) variation for determinkg average

social status participants was used in thîs study, however, one important change

narrowing the average category lllnits was adopted. In this study, the criteria of having a

social preference score between 0.0 and +1.0, rather than between -1 -0 and +I .O, was

employed to avoid including in the average category those participants receiving liked

least scores higher than the classroom mean and liked most scores lower than the

classroom mean, as would be the case were social preference scores below 0.0 included.

Utilising the sociometric classification cnteria, five students with LD out of 38

were classified as popular and 12 as average in social status. Together, these 17 children

with LD were redefined as target children belonging to the Socially Accepted category of

social status. Subsequent analyses do not distinguish between popular and average target

children due to the small number of children with LD who were classified as

sociometrically popular. Therefore, in the current study, the terms "sociaily accepted" and

Page 52: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

SociaI Cornpetence

51

"peer accepted" are used to refer to those children obtaining average or better (i-e.,

popular) social status ratings.

Self-perceptions

The SelfTerception Profile for Leatning Disabled Students (SPPLD; Renick &

Harter, 1988) was used to assess self-perception of competence in specific areas of

ability. The SPPLD is a self-report measure for assessing domain specific judgements of

competence and perceived self-esteem as a person in both children with and without

leaming disabilities. The SPPLD consists of 46 questions with a format designed to

reduce the incidence of socidy desirable responses (Renick & Harter, 1988). Each item

is comprised of two contrasting statements (e-g., "Some kids know how to speU most

words they corne across BUT Other kids h d it bard to spell most words" or "Some kidç

are happy with themselves as a person BUT Other kids are often not happy with

themselves"). The child is asked to decide which of two contrasting statements best

describes him or her and then check if that statement is "Really hue for me" or "Sort of

true for me." Items are scored fiom a low self-evaluation (a score of 1) to a high self-

evaluation (a score of 4). The SPPLD has ten subscales, nine assessing nine specific

domains of academic and nonacademic self-percep tion (generd intellectual ability,

mathematics ability, reading abdïty, spelling ability, writing ability, social acceptance,

athletic competence, behavioural conduct, and physical appearance) and one global self-

worth subscale. Each of the nine subscales and the global self-worth subscale are distinct

fkom each other, with very low to moderate subscale intercorrelations (Renick & Harter,

1988). Interna1 consistency reliabilities for each of the ten subscales, based on Cronbach's

alphas, range fkom .78 to .89. Test-retest retiability coefficients for the ten SPPLD

domains on a sample of 104 grade 7 and 8 students over a seven week period have been

found to be high (Heath & Brown, 1999): general intellectual ability, -72; reading

competence, -85; spelling competence, -84; writing competence, .69; math competence,

.83; social acceptance, .79; athletic competence, -81; physical appearance, .78;

Page 53: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

52

behaviourai conduct, -73; and global self-worth, -76.

Heath and Brown (1999) also determined the naturd groupings of SPPLD

subdomains by a principal components andysis on the ten domains of the SPPLD using a

sample of 102 grade five and six children. As expected, a two-factor solution emerged

fiom the ten subscales: Nonacademic SelfTerceptions (Factor 1) and Academic Self-

Perceptions Factor 2). In the present study, only the Factor 1 aggregate score fiom the

five nonacademic subscales (social acceptance, behavioural conduct, athletic abilîty,

physical appearance, and global self-worth) was examined, as nonacademic self-concept

is a key component of the Vaughn and Hogan model of social cornpetence.

Social Skills and Behaviour ProbIems

The Social Skills Rathg System-Teacher (SSRS-T; Gresham & Elliott, 1990) was

utilised in the present study to assess teacher-rated social skills and extemalising

behaviour problems of children with and without LD. The SSRS-T is a standardised

rating scale measuring a broad range of social behaviours for children between the ages

of 3 and 18 years and can serve as a v h b l e measure in the identification of children

with internalishg and extemalising difficulties @&tg, Vasa, Reid, & Torrey, 1995).

Teachers are asked to rate the fiequency with which students evidence 30 specific sociai

skill items on a 3-point Likert scale (O = never, 1 = sometimes, 2 = very often). Each item

is categorised into a sociai skills factor comprising three separate dimensions of skill:

cooperation, assertion, and self-control. Cooperation items include behaviours such as

helping others, sharing materials, and complying with rules and directions. Assertion

items include behaviours such as asking others for information, introducing oneself, and

responding appropriately to actions of others. Self-control items consist of behaviours

such as responding appropnately to teasing, arguing with others, and embarrassrnent in

social situations.

Three additional scales on the SSRS-T indicate the extent to which youngsters

display problem behaviours such as externalising problems, hyperactivity, or intemalking

Page 54: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

problems. Extemalisïng problems entail verbal or physical aggression toward others, poor

control of temper, and arguing. Items on the Hyperactivity subscale reflect excessive

movement, fidgeting, and impulsive reactions. Intemalising problems encompass

behaviom indicating sadness, loneLiness, anxiety, and poor self-esteem. In addition, the

SSRS-T inchdes an academic competence domain in which the teacher rates the student

on a five-point scde in reading, mathematics, motivation, parental support, and general

cognitive abilities. Extemalising, intemalising, and hyperactivity subscaies combine to

form an overail Uidicator of Problem Behaviours, while the cooperation, assertion, and

self-control subscales combine to indicate level of Social Skills. The Academic Problems

subscale was incorporated in data analysis as an indication of teacher-perceived academic

competence. The interna1 reliability (Cronbach's alpha) for the three scales of the SSRS-T

(Problem Behaviours, Social SkiLls, and Academic Competence) are very high, ranging

fiom -73 to -95. Test-retest reliabilities over a four-week period for the SSRS-T are also

very high, ranging fkom -84 to .93 (Gresham & Elliott, 1990).

This study employed a modification of Masten, Monson, and Pellegrini's (1985)

Revised Class Play (RCP). In its standard administration, the RCP is a peer- nomination

technique asking raters to select classrnates fÏom a roster whom they feel are best suited

for 15 positive (e.g., 'good leader,' 'plays f&') and 15 negative roles (e.g., 'gets into a lot

of fights,' 'shows off a lot') in an imaginary class play. Three factors emerge fkom the 30

questions--one positive factor called Sociability-Leadership and two negative factors

called Aggressive-Disruptive and Sensitive-Isolated. The RCP has intemal consistency

coefficients of -81 to .95, 17-month stability comelates of -77 to 30, and cross-sex

reliability of .78 to -86 (Masten et al., 1985). Two author-modified versions (Peer and

Self) of the Revised Class Play were employed,

Items fiom the original RCP were reworded into positive staternents for the

Sociability-Leadership scale and negative statements for the Aggresçive-Disruptive and

Page 55: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

54

Sensitive-Isolated scales. Further, five new positively phrased items were added to the

measure tapping academic (school work and intelligence) and nonacademic areas

(athletic ability, physical appearance, and personai possessions) of functioning (see

Appendix B). The questionnaire was renamed the Revised Class Play & h g (RCPR) in

order to distinguish it fiom the original RCP-

Target children and the peers who rated the target children as prefmed playmates

chose their level of agreement for each RCPR statement via a 5-point Likert scale (1 =

very false to 5 = very true). This method yielded two types of scores: (a) peer ratings of

target children and @) target children's self-ratings, in five domains of functioning: (a)

sociability-leadership, @) aggressive-disruptive, (c) sensitive-isolated, (d) academic

competence, and (e) nonacademic competence.

%en-ended Interview

An open-ended interview question was included at the end of the administration

of the RCPR askuig (a) the peer rater to describe in thek own words why they chose the

target child as a preferred playmate and @) the target child why they thought they were

favourably rated by their peers. The open-ended question is an important component of

the data gathering process as it promotes ecological validity through its allowance for

relevant information in the child's own words to be obtained - information that is

otherwise difficult to capture via structured questionnaire items. By including an open-

ended interview question, it was felt that important social-behavioural characteristics not

otherwise accounted for in the standardised data g a t h e ~ g procedure could be ascertained

and understood fiom peer and self-raters.

Procedure

Consent forms were initially distributed to al1 students (N = 510) in the

participating classrooms (see Appendix C). Consent forms were retumed by 464 (9 1 %) of

the students. Of these students, 320 consented to participate in the study; thus the overall

level of participation from the original sample was 63% (Le., 320 out of 510). Data

Page 56: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

55

collection proceeded in two distinct phases as describeci below. Phase 1 was considered

the screening phase.

Phase 1

During the initial screening phase, the WRAT-3 S p e h g and fithmetic subtests

were administered to the participants in their classrooms. The standardised instructions

for group testing discussed in the manual (Wilkinson, 1993) were adhered to during

administration. Nonparticipating students remained in the classroom and were asked to

work quietly on their homework while testing took place with the consenting students.

Within two weeks of the initial assessrnent of spelling and arithmetic, students were

individually administered the short form IQ test (VUSC-III: Block Design and

Vocabulary) and the Reading portion of the UrRAT-3, again following standardised

procedures. Results of the achievement testing and short form IQ screenulg phase were

then analysed to determine student eIigibility in one of the three categories: (a) LD, (b)

non-LD, and (c) no category.

A few weeks later, students having met criteria for LD or non-LD group

membership completed the SPPLD in their classroorns, Nonparticipants were asked to

work quietly at their desks while testing was underway. Before administering the SPPLD,

children were provided with extensive instructions on how to complete the SPPLD forms.

A larninated poster containhg three SPPLD-like items was displayed at the fiont of the

classroom. A trained research assistant guided the participants through three sample

questions. Al1 SPPLD items were read out loud while the participants followed dong and

marked their answers. Children completed their questionnaires uidependentiy at their own

desk and were periodically reminded of task instructions and not to share their answers

with children seated near them.

The sociometric procedure was administered to al1 classroorns in the months of

May and June, 1996 thus ensuring that al1 children were very familiar with the traits and

charactenstics of their fellow classrnates. To set the stage for the sociometrics, children

Page 57: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

56

were told that they would be taking part in an activity designed to help the researchers

understand why some children have an easy time making fiiends and getting dong with

people, and why other children seem to have difficdty in these areas. They were told that

the study dedt with children's fkiendships and social acceptance and that they would be

filling out questionnaires and nominating classmates as in a pretend voting situation.

Children participated in the peer rating and nomination process as a group in their

classrooms. They were told that they were about to participate in a simulated vote

concerning how much they liked to play with certain classmates and how much they liked

to engage in certain play activities. The following measures were taken to assure that al1

answers pertaining to classmates were private and confidentid: (a) Desks were separated

before the voting process began, (b) AU forms relating to the vote were identified by

subject numbers rather than by proper names, and (c) AU materials pertauùng to the peer

rathg and peer nomination procedures were distributed and collected in unmarked

envelopes. Consenting students were provided with a letter-sized envelope containing

instructions as to the nature of the sociometric task and al l materials needed in order to

complete the evduation. Each envelope contained: (a) an example of the peer rating task,

@) the peer rating fom, (c) an example of the peer nomination task, (d) the peer

nomination task, and (e) a distracter task (see Appendix D). Students marked theu

answers on typed, non-alphabetical class lists which had their own name blackened-out.

Children were given a roster of aLl participating peers (Le., cross-gender ratings)

in their classroom and were asked to ûidicate how much they liked to play r i th each peer

by circling a number on a scale &orn 1 to 4, where 1 indicated "not at dl" and 4 indicated

"very much." Next, children were asked to complete the peer-nominations portion of the

sociometnc procedure. Children were provided with a list of participating classmates and

were instnicted to circle the names of three individuals they "like rnost" fiom this page.

Finally, following termination of the voting task, students were given a distracter task in

which they rated various play activities on a four-point Likert scale and chose three

Page 58: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Coqetence

57

activities which they Ued to play the most. This distracter task paralleled the primary

voting tasks in form and structure; however, participants rated how much they liked to

engage in certain play activities and which play activities they Liked the best, rather than

asking how they felt about certain classmates. The distracter tasks were designed to divert

the child's attention away fkom thicking about his or her classmates and toward ratings of

play activities instead,

The major@ of participants completed the sociometric measure in their own

classroom. However, in some schools, due to space restrictions, the participating children

left their classrooms and went to an empty classroom to complete the sociometric portion

of data coilection. Throughout the testing session, trained graduate assistants

simultaneously guided all students through the sociometric task booklet and answered

my questions that arose.

Lastly, the teacher of each target child with whom they spent the greatest amount

of class time was asked to complete the Social Skills Rating System-Teacher (SSRS-T)

measure on the student. Since many target students came from the same classroorn, each

teacher involved in this portion of data collection completed between one and five SSRS-

T forms. Teachers were given an envelope containing the tkneline, instructions on how to

complete the forms, and the forms themselves in the late Spring and asked to return them

before the end of the school year in June. Teachers were not provided with any data

concerning specific students before or after they completed the SSRS-T- At the end of the

school year, the sociometric data were analysed and participating students were

categorised into one of the five sociometric categories (i-e., popular, average, rejected,

neglected, and controversial).

Phase 2

Phase two of data collection took place in early Fall of the subsequent academic

school year and involved (a) detemilnation of same-gender peer raters for target children,

(b) obtaining same-gender peer ratings on the target children with the RCPR: Peer and the

Page 59: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

58

open-ended question, and (c) obtaining self-ratings fiom target children on the RCPR:

Self and the open-ended question. As it was not possible to determine who the target

children of the study were pnor to ariministering tbe sociometric instrument, the initial

consent form distributed to parents in phase one did not mention that follow-up data

collection sessions and short interviews would be requested fiorn certain students-

Consequentiy, a second consent form was issued to parents of children who met the

following criteria: (a) a target child with or without LD, classified as socially accepted or

(b) one of two same-gender peers choosing either target children with or without LD as a

preferred playmate (see Appendix C). Potentiai consenting same-gender raters had to

have circled the target peers' name on the peer nomination form and rated the target child

with a 3 or 4 on the peer-rating scale to be eligible for continued participation This

procedure ensured that those children chosen to complete the RCPR: Peer nominated the

target child as someone they liked the most and rated the child as someone they liked to

play with either ''pretty much" or "very much." As such, each target child received peer

ratings on the RCPR-Peer fkom two same-sex classrnates. Of the 17 consent forms

distributed to target children with LD, al1 but one came back positive. T'us, for phase

two of the study the number of target children with and without LD was 32 (Le., 16 in

each group) and the number of peer raters was 64 (Le., two raters for each of the 32 target

children) . Consenthg participants left the classroom for approxirnately 20 minutes to

complete the RCPR: Peer or RCPR: Self and be interviewed by the researcher. The

session took place in a private room in the child's school in which a tape recorder was

unobtnisively positioned. Participants were reminded that this study involved trying to

better understand why some children have an easy thne making fiends and how they go

about doing this. Peer raters and target children were told that al1 information was stnctly

confidentid and that no one except the interviewer would h o w how they answered the

questions. Peer raters and target chilâren were provided with thorough instructions and an

Page 60: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Compet ence

59

example as to how the RCPR scale worked. Items on the RCPR were then read to each

student and the responses were noted by the researcher. Periodic checks on

comprehension were made by the investigator to ensure accuracy of completion,

Following administration of the RCPR items, the tape recorder was tumed on and raters

were asked a final open-ended question. Peer raters were asked, "In your own words,

please tel me why you chose as someone you lïke to play with the most." Prompts

for clarifications and elaborations were used as needed. Target children were asked, "In

your own words, please teli me why you think you were chosen as sorneone many kids

like to play with; what is it about you that other kids like?"

Responses to the open-ended interview questions were transcribed verbatim and

then analysed. Analysis of this qualitative data followed procedures outlined by Lincoln

and Guba (1985). This method was chosen as it allows categories to emerge fkom the data

rather than imposing the category structure a priori. A surnrnary of the procedures that

were used follows.

1. Unitisation of data Initial categories for the responses were developed

independently by the author and one &ed graduate assistant. The researchers fïrst read

the interview transcript and identified salient bits of information or meaningfùl idea units

within each interview. Units varied in length fiom single to severai words and each unit

represented a single thought or idea Units were underlined on the transcript and iisted in

a paraphrased manner on a separate sheet by each researcher.

2. Unit sortinrr. Researchers then independently sorted the iisted units into more

refined categories consisting of similar content. Categories were named and initiai mies

established for belonging to the category. Categories were then checked for thoroughness

and completeness.

3. Negotiation of categories. The two researchers met to discuss the coding

process and negotiate the categories. The author acted as Ieader, amouncing her

categones to determine whether the other researcher had fonnulated a similar category.

Page 61: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

60

The two researchers then discussed the category, the rules for categorisation, and the

exernplar units for each category. Rules for category membership were discussed and

revised and those categones deemed very similar in nature were collapsed and merged as

needed. A single system for categorisation resulted and is presented in Table 3.

4. Re-codin~: and validation. The author coded ail of the transcnpts a second time

using the system for categorisation redting fiom steps 1,2, and 3. A superscript numeral

was written beside each meaningfül unit within the transcript pertaining to one of the

eight categories fkom Table 3. Next, a second trained graduate student was asked to apply

the revised categorisation system to a representative sample of transcripts (Le., 40% of

peer interviews and target child interviews). Inter-rater reliability was established

between the two researchers and was equivalent to 89% agreement between independent

raters.

5. Quantification of data After the re-coding of transcripts, each interview was

evaluated for the presence or absence of a reference to each of the eight categories by

noting the superscript numerals within the transcrïpt and then endorsing the appropriate

category on the data spreadsheet. The number of categones endorsed for each interview

was calculated for al1 target children. Thus, in total, 64 peer (Le., two peers per target

child) and 3 1 target child interviews were categorised via this procedure (one target child

declined the interview process).

Page 62: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

61

Table 3-

Open-ended I n t e ~ e w Category Titles and Descriptions

Category Title Category Description

Humourous

Talent

Enjoyable Company

Positive Traits

Outside Affiliation

LeadershipKharisma

Non-descrip t

Reference to having shared interests or partaking in cornmon activities; having a lot in common with the peer; feeling that the other person is similar to themselves,

Reference to the peer's sense of humour or ability to make people Iaugh; description as funny or cornical.

Reference to the peeis ability in academic (e-g., school) and nonacademic (e.g., sports) endeavours; description as being "good" or able to "help" in certain activities.

Reference to the idea that the peer is kind to others and fh to be around; ideas such as having an easy-going and positive attitude; treating people with respect; and not performing discourteous behaviours-

Reference to speczflc positive qualities, like being loyal, reliable, helpfid, generous, or ûushirUrthy.

Reference to an association the peer has with something or someone else (e-g., reference to nice belongings, pleasant farnily members, many other fïiends, or being popdar),

Reference to the peers' ability to get others to listen; having an unique and engaging personality.

Any non-descriptive reference without clanfication (i-e., "is nice," "is a good fiiend").

Page 63: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

62

Chapter N: Resnlts

Data Analvsis

A section on data analysis is provided as clarification for the choice of statistical

procedures utilised in this study. It is understood that a study of this nature, containhg

relatively small sample sizes, will involve certain limitations depending on the statistical

analyses performed. Accordingly, a number of measures have been taken to reduce error

variance in this study, particularly when the statistical analyses involve small nlmibers:

(a) Extraneous variables such as age of child, grade in school, and type o f school have

been controlled; @) Participants have been matched on relevant variables (e-g., gender,

IQ, age, classroom); and (c) Statistical power has been increased when sample size is

srnall (i.e., less than 20) according to the Stevens' (1 986) recommendations.

Stevens (1986) points out that statistical power (i-e., the ability to find a

significant difference) is considered to be questionable with less than 20 participants per

group. He suggests, however, that meaningful parametric analyses can be performed on

srnail data sets when the following guidelines to increase statistical power are followed:

(a) adopting a more lenient alpha level (Le., p c -10); @) using a one-tailed test where the

- literature supports a directional hypothesis; and (c) reducing within-group variability by

choosing a more homogeneous sample (i-e., matching on essential variables or choosing

resûicted age groups). The first and third recommendations have been foliowed in this

study, while the second was not possible due to a lack of prior research.

Stevens (1986) fürther points out that the alpha level set by the experimenter is a

subjective decision and that there are situations, such as when sample sizes are small,

when it makes sense to use alpha levels other than the standard .O5 and .O 1 levels.

Accordingly, and especially in consideration of the dearth of prior research in this area,

the present study utilised a less stringent alpha level of E = .IO. The potential drawback to

using a lenient alpha (e-g., a greater chance of a type I error where there is a greater risk

of rejecting a tn ie n d l hypothesis) is well understood by the researcher; however, in Light

Page 64: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

63

of cautions fiom prominent authors in the field of statistics (e-g., Glass & Hopkins, 1996;

Stevens, 1986) concerning the seriousness of committting a type II error (i.e., saying that

the groups don't differ when they do) and the desire to obtain adequate levels of statistical

power given such a small sample size, it is felt that the previously mentioned advantages

of a lenient alpha outweigh the disadvantages, particularly for an exploratory study of this

nature.

Stevens (1986) draws an important distinction between exploratory and

c o ~ a t o r y studies. An exploratory study is one in which the investigator first has to

establish that an effect may exist, while a c o ~ a t o r y study is one in which the

experimenter, based upon past research, has specific questions wishing to be asked of the

data In confirmatory studies, it is reasonable to Lunit the number of comparisons made in

order to improve power; however, in exploratory studies it is understood that although the

comparisons made are planned, due to the lack of prior research in the area, they are not

necessarily limited in number. The present study follows the guidelines of an exploratory

study and consequently the statistical analyses reflect this investigative focus by not

reducing the number of planned cornparisons to be made.

Stevens (1986) M e r notes that in exploratory studies it is wise to fïrst

demonstrate an overall significance in the data and then to foilow this up with assessrnent

of the subsources of variation (Le., group dinerences). Similarly, Huberty and Morris

(1 989) point out that multiple univariate analyses are appropriate when the research being

conducted is exploratory in nature. Consequently, in the present study, where appropriate,

multivariate analyses are conducted and then followed by univariate analyses. An initial

multivariate approach to data analysis is warranted in the present study since the

measures chosen in this investigation both theoretically and practically measure one

underlying construct, namely social cornpetence (see Harwell, 1988).

Finally, significant multivariate effects at the g = -10 level in this study were

followed by univariate analyses, also conducted at the E = -10 level. This procedure,

Page 65: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

64

suggested by Stevens (1986), was chosen over the Bonferroni inequality post hoc method

because it offered greater power in detecting group differences given the small sample

and moderate levels of statisticd power in this study* Further, as has been suggested by

Glass and Hopkins (1 996), taking a slightly greater risk of a Type 1 error in order to gain

a substantial increase in the probability of detecting tme differences (i.e., power) is wise

practice. As such, and in keeping with the exploratory nature of thÏs study, a Bonferroni

inequality @mm, 196 1) lowering alpha levels and subsequently reducing statistical

power was not employed in this study.

LD and non-LD Classification Rates

Using the categonsation criteria mentioned pre&ously, 38 children (1 1.87%; 22

mdes, 16 females) fiom the initial sample of 320 were classified as having a leamhg

disability and 156 (48.75%; 85 mdes, 71 females) were classified as not having a

leaming disability. Of the 38 children with LD, 17 were classifïed as target children

having average or better social status. A group of matched peers without LD was

subsequently determined fiom the remaining sample of 156 non-LD peers. IndividuaI

participants were matched for age, gender, IQ, and classroom.

A manipulation check on the classification of LD and non-LD groups was

obtained via the Academic Competence subscale of the SSRS-T. A two-way ANOVA

was conducted with LD Status (LD, non-LD) and Gender as the independent variables

and teacher-rated overall academic competence as the dependent variable. Results

revealed a non-significant LD Status-by-Gender interaction (1,30) = 1.62, a, a non-

significant Gender main effect (F (1,30) = -78, &, but a significant main effect for LD

Status (F (1,30) = 9.25, g < .O 1). Teachers rated research-identified children with LD as

signincantly Iower (M = 86.06, SD = 9.44) than non-LD peers a = 97.00, SD = 7-37) in

overall academic competence. Results were in the anticipated direction and indicated that

participants identified as having LD were rated by their teachers as lower in achiai

academic cornpetence than those identified as not having LD.

Page 66: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

65

Sociometric Results

Overall Analvses for Initial S m l e

Sociometric categorisation procedures resulted in the categorisation of 78.3 5%

(152 out of 194) of the entire sarnple into one of five sociometric categories. Similar

categonsation rates have been found by other authors (e.g., Asher & Wheeler, 1985;

Stone & La Greca, 1990)- Overall, 79.49% of the initial non-LD group (124 out of 156)

and 73 -68% of the initial LD group (28 out of 3 8) were classifiable into one of the five

sociometrk categories (popular, average, rejected, neglected, or controversial). Overall,

classification rates were comparable for both groups (1, N = 194) = -607, os) and are

acceptable as they indicate that approlamately three-quarters of the initial LD and non-

LD populations in the shidy were classified into a sociometric category. Propotions of

participants in each of the five specifk sociometric categories by LD Status and Gender

are found in Table 4-

An overd chi-square analysis on the LD and non-LD group proportions was

conducted for the sociometric categories (excluding the controversial category due to low

fkequencies) to determine whether the participants with and without LD were

disproportionately represented in one of the remaining four sociometnc categories. A 2 x

4 chi-square analysis ushg LD status (LD or non-LD) by sociometric category (popular,

average, rejected, and neglected) was not significant OZ (3, N = 143) = -856, nS),

indicating that participants with and without LD had comparable representations across

each of the sociometric categories. Based on these results it is concluded that, in the

initial sample of students, the likelihood of being classified into one of the four

sociometric categories did not appear to be associated with the presence or absence of a

learning disability. A chi-square analysis examinuig gender effects in the sociometnc data

was not possible due to smdl cell frequencies.

Page 67: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

66

Table 4.

Sociometric Classifications for Initial Sample bv LD S tatus and Gender

.-

Initial Sample POP Avg Rej Neg Cont - - - - - - -- - -

LD - Boys(o=17) f 2 8 4 3 O

P - (1 1.76) (47.06) (23 253) (17.65) (0)

Girls (n = I l ) f 3 4 2 2 O

P (27.27) (36.3 6) (18.18) (18.18) (0)

Total (n = 28) f 5 12 6 5 O

P - (17.86) (42.8 6) (2 1 -43) (17.86) (0)

Total &L= 124) f 27 43 20 25 9

P - (21.77) (34.68) (16.13) (20.16) (7 -26)

Note. LD = leaming disabilities; f = fiequency; P = percentage; Pop = popular; Avg = average; Rej = rejected; Neg = neglected; Cont = controversial

Descri~tive Statistics for Target Sample

Descriptive statistics for target sample participants (Le., socially-accepted children

with LD, = 27 and non-LD matched peers, fi = 17) were calculated and appear in Table 5.

Separate 2 x 2 ANOVAs were conducted with demographic variables (Age, IQ estimate,

and WRAT-Spelling, Reading, and Anthmetic) as dependent variables and LD Status (LD

Page 68: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

67

Table 5.

Demosa~hic and ,',chievernent Data for Target S a m ~ l e

Target Sample Age in IQ Speiiing Reading Arithmetic

months Estimate SS SS SS

LD - Boys&=lO) M 132.20 1 02 -40 76.60 87.90 95.10

SD - (9.03) (5 -97) (3 -84) (1 1.73) (1 5.07)

M Girls (n= 7) - 130.43 106.43 82.29 91.57 99.43

SD - (1 0.23) (5 -03) (2-36) (10.45) (14.77)

Total(n=17) M 126.59 99.94 102.59 105.41 105.94

SD - (7.53) (8 -62) (1 1.05) (7.65) (9.68)

Note. LD = learning disabiiities; SS = standard scores (NJ = 100; SD = 15)

or non-LD) and Gender as independent variabIes. Results revealed no significant LD Status-

by-Gender interaction @ (1,30) = .42, ns) and no LD Status @ (1,30) = 2.26, ns) or

Gender @ (1,30) = 0.00, ns) main effects for Age. Sirnilarly, no si@cant LD Status-by-

O Gender interaction @ (1,3 0) = .O 1, nsJ, LD Status main effect @ (1,3 0) = 2.66, ns), or

Page 69: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

68

Gender main effect (1,3 0) = 2.17, ns) were found for IQ estimate.

LD Status-by-Gender interactions for S p e b g 7 Reading, and Arithmetic were not

signïficant (F (2,30) = 1.03, -02, -00, ns, respectively), Additionally, no Gender main

effects were found for Spelling, Reading7 or Anthmetic @ (1,30) = -85, .89, .84, ns,

respectively). A significant main effect for LD Status, however, was found in ai l three

achievement areas. As expected, children with LD eamed significantly lower standard

scores than the non-LD group in Spelling @ (1,30) = 62.31,~ < .01), Reading @ (1,30)

= 22.34, c .O l), and Anthmetic (F (1,30) = 4.18, p_ < -05).

Social Com~etence Mode1

Sociometric-status Equivalence Between Target Groups

Before statistical analyses codd be perfonned on the Vaughn and Hogan model

data, it was k t necessary to determine the statistical equivalence of Sociometric S tatus

between target samples. Consequently, a 2 x 2 MANOVA was conducted to determine

whether LD and non-LD groups were statistically equivalent in measured social status,

with LD Status (LD, non-LD) as the independent variable and Social Preference (SPZ)

and Social Impact (SIZ) -scores as dependent variables. The social preference and social

impact -scores were chosen as the most appropriate numerical representation of the

sociometrïc classification of participants because these numbers were used to numencally

delineate socid status soup membership.

Resuits of the MANOVA did not indicate a si@cant multivarîate effect for LD

Status, (F (2,3 1) = -717, &. Results reveded that LD and non-LD target groups did not

differ in measured social status; thus, it was deemed appropriate to nin al1 subsequent

statistical analyses without Sociometric S tatus as a dependent variable-

Intercorrelations Behiveen Dependent Variables

To evaluate the Vaughn and Hogan model of social cornpetence, a series of

multivariate and univariate analyses were conducted comparing target participants with

and without LD on data gathered nom the administration of the SPPLD (nonacademic

Page 70: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

69

self-concept) and the SSRS-T (teacher-rated social skills and behavioural conduct). As an

additional preliminary step before conducting the multivariate analyses, it was necessary

to jusw the theoretical intercorrelations between dependent variables. Preiiminary

Pearson product-moment correlations were therefore conducted with the three dependent

variables (Social Skills, Problem Behaviours, and Nonacademic Self-concept)- Overail,

the Pearson resdts were in the expected direction. Social Skills and Problem Behaviours

were sigdicantly correlated @ = 4 2 , E c . O l ) , as were Social Skills and Nonacademic

Self-concept = -40, E (.OS), and ProbIem Behaviours and Nonacademic Self-concept (r

= 4 4 , p c.01). niese results support the theoretical overlap between dependent variables.

Mean sociometric 2-scores and LD Status-by-Gender standard scores for tbe three

dependent variables of Social Skills, Problem Behaviours, and Nonacademic Self-concept

are found in Table 6* To detennine whether significant differences existed between the

two groups of target children, a 2 x 2 x 3 MANOVA was undertaken with LD Status (LD

and non-LD) and Gender as independent variables and Social Skills, Problem

B ehaviours, and Nonacademic Self-concept as dependent variables.

Results indicated no significant multivarïate LD Status-by-Gender interaction

effect (Wirks' Lambda = -848, F (3,28) = 1.68, ns), Likewise, no si@cant multivariate

main effect for LD Status ('Wih' Lambda = ,868, F (3,28) = 1.42, ns) was found, A

significant multivariate Gender main effect (Wilks' Lambda = -764, F (3,28) = 2.88, E <

.IO) was found, however, suggesting that socially-accepted boys and girls are different in

overall Social Skills, Problem Behaviours, and Nonacademic Self-concept, regardless of

LD status.

To clariQ the multivariate Gender main effect and to M e r explore the

possibility of univariate differences, a post hoc procedure examining the univariate F- tests was conducted- At the univariate level, a significant LD Status-by-Gender

interaction was found for the dependent variables of Social S W s (1,30) = 4 . 7 4 , ~ <

-10) and Problem Behaviours @ (1,3 0) = 2.97, g < -10)- No significant results emerged,

Page 71: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

70

however, for the dependent variable of Nonacademic SelFconcept, (F (1,30) = .01, oS)

These results are displayed in Figure 1.

A significant univariate main effect for LD Status was found for the Problem

Behaviours variable @ (1,3 0) = 3 -66, == -1 0). Specifically, children with LD were rated

Table 6.

Standardised z-scores for Sociometric and Means for Social Competence Mode1 Variables

Target Sample SPZ S E SS PB NASC

Boys(n=10) M -35 .48 84.90 118.70 3.13

SD - (-51) (-68) (1 5.72) (16.67) (-75)

Girls (n= 7) &J .96 .5 1 105-14 97.57 3 -46

SD - (-69) (-34) (1 4.6 1) (7.57) (-43)

Total @ = 17) M -60 .49 93 -24 11 0.00 3 -27

SD - (-65) (-55) (1 8.02) (17.1 1) (-64)

Non-LD

Boys@=lO) M -90 .42 200-90 102.30 3.18

SD - (-75) (-49) (15.86) (1 1.32) (-56)

Girls @= 7) M -84 .84 99.43 96.71 3 -46

SD - (-59) (-52) (7-96) (1 3 -06) (-47)

0 Note. LD = learning disabilities; SPZ = Social Preference; SIZ = Social Impact; SS = Social Skills; PB = Problem Behaviours; NASC = Nonacademic Self-concept

Page 72: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

71

Figure Caption

F i m e 1 - Significant LD Status-b y-Gender univariate interactions for Social Skills and

Problem Behaviours and nonsignificant LD Statu-by-Gender interaction for

Nonacademic Self-concept.

Page 73: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

Social Skills

-

LD non-LD

Problem Behaviours

Nonacademic Self-concept Males [ -a- h m m h

Page 74: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

73

by their teachers as exhibiting more problematic behaviours = 1 10.00; SD = 17.1 1)

than their non-LD counterparts = 100.00; SD = 12-01}, No significant ciifferences in

either Social Skills @ (1,30) = 1-06, & or Nonacademic Self-concept @ (1,30) = -01,

ns) were found between LD and non-LD.

Gender main effects also emerged for both the Social Skills and Problem

Behaviours scales, revealing signifïcant differences between boys and girls in teacher-

rated Social Skills, @ (1,30) = 3.54, c -10) and Problem Behaviours @ (1,30) = 8.78,

p -C -10). SpecikaUy, teachers rated males as sig-nificantly lower in Social Skills than

females (M = 92.90; SD = 17.43 and M = 102.28; SD = 11.68, respectively) and as

signîfïcantly higher in Problem Behaviours than females = 110.50; SD = 16.22 and M = 97.1 4; SD = 10.27, respectively). No simiificant Werences between males and females

(F(1,30) = 2.29, nsJ in Nonacademic Self-concept emerged (males &J = 3-14; SD = -64,

fernales M = 3.46; SD = -43). Overd, LD Status and Gender univariate main effects for

Social Skills and Problem Behaviours c m ody be interpreted, however, when

considerhg the significant LD Status-by-Gender univariate interactions.

Revised Class Play Ratinn: Peer Ratinas

Before anaiyses could be conducted on the Revised Class Play Rating: Peer

Ratings (RCPR: P) questionnaire, it was f b t necessary to establish intercorrelations and

interna1 reliabilities for the original RCPR- P factors (Sociability-Leadership, Aggressive-

Disruptive, Sensitive-Isolated). A Pearson product-moment correlation conducted on the

three original factors of the RCPR: P indicated significant intercorrelations at the E < .O1

Ievel. Next, a series of Cronbach alpha coefficient calculations was conducted to

detennine intemal reliabilities for each target group. Aipha coefficients for the

Sociability-Leadership, Aggressive-Disruptiveness, and Sensitive-Isolated factors were

.87, -85, and -77, respectively for target participants with LD and .88, .87, and -54,

respectively for target participants without LD. These alpha levels indicate substantiai

intemal reliability within factors.

Page 75: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

74

Coefficient alpha's were also determined for the two additional RCPR: P factors

of Academic and Nonacademic cornpetence. Alphas for the Academic scaie were .82 for

the group with LD and -78 for the non-LD group, while alphas for the Nonacademic

scales were -18 for the group with LD and .73 for the non-LD group. The Iow alphas for

the Nonacademic scale in the group with LD supported the decision to examine

individual item responses, rather than overall factor scores. The three items in the

Nonacademic scale are: (a) Physical Appearance ( - is good looking); (b) Personal

Possessions ( - has a lot of neat st-; and (c) Athletic Ability ( - is better than others

hidher age in sports). Raw scores fkom the RCPR: P were summed and averaged across

all five factors (Le., the original three RCPR factors plus the Academic and Nonacademic

factors). Means and standard deviations for each group, broken down by gender, appear

in Table 7.

Scores on the RCPR: P range fiom 1-00 (statement is very fdse) to 5.00

(statement is very true), with higher scores indicating greater endorsement for a paaicuiar

factor. It should be pointed out that the Aggressive-Disruptive and Sensitive-kolated

factors are scaled in the opposite direction to the Sociability-Leadership, Academic, and

Nonacademic scales (Le., they contain negatively phrased statements) and hence higher

scores on these two factors indicate an endorsement of more negative characteristics (Le.,

higher levels of aggressive-disruptive or sensitive-isolated behaviours).

Onginal Factors

A between-within repeated measures MANOVA was conducted on the RCPR: P

data to determine whether group differences existed between children with and without

LD on the three original factors of the RCPR: P. LD Status and Gender were the two

crossed between-subjects variables, while Questionnaire Factor (Sociability-Leadership,

Aggressive-Disruptive, Sensitive-Isolated) and Peer Rater were the two within-subjects

variables. The Peer Rater factor was included to measure whether significant differences

existed between peers' ratings of individual target participants. Overall, there were no

Page 76: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

75

Table 7.

Means and Standard Deviations for RCPR: Peer Factors

- --

LD LD Total Non-LD Non-LD Total

Boys Girls LD Boys Girls non-LD

(n = 9) @=7> (g = 16) @= 9) @= 7) (O= 16)

Factor M SD M SD M SD - M m - M SD - M SD

SL 3.77 (-64) 3.74 (-56) 3.76 (-73) 3.67 (-73) 4.26 (-23) 3.93 (-71)

NAC 3.57(.54) 3.69(.56) 3.63 (-72) 3.39(.73) 4.02 (-88) 3.67(.95)

Note. RCPR = Revised Class Play Rating; LD = learning disabilities; SL = Sociabiiity- Leadership; AD = Aggressive-Disniptive; SI = Sensitive-Isolated; AC = Academic; NAC = Nonacademic

signincant multivariate repeated measures m a h effects or interactions for the RCPR: P

factors dong any of the variables (see Appendur E for MANOVA source table). N o

further analyses were conducted with the RCPR: P factors.

Nonacademic Factor

Separate two-way ANOVAs with LD Status and Gender as independent variables

were conducted for the three Nonacademic factor items. Means and standard deviations

for target participants are presented in Table 8. Results reveal no significant LD Status-

by-Gender interaction @ (1,60) = -09, nS), no significant main effect for LD Status fF (1,

60) = -49, a, and a significant main effect for Gender (F (1,60) = 4.18, E < -10)

on the Physical Appearance item. In general, girls (M = 3.86; = 1.18) rated their

same-sex peers as beirig more "good looking" than did boys (M = 3.27; = 1.06). No

Page 77: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

76

Table 8.

Mean Responses to RCPR: Peer Nonacademic Factor Items

Items LD LD Total non-LD non-LD Total

Boys Girls LD Boys Girls non-LD

(n= 9) (n = 7) (n= 16) &=9) @ = 7) (g = 16)

M SD - - M SD - M SD - M SD - M SD - - M SD

Note. RCPR = Revised Class Play Rating; LD = IeamiBg disabilities; PA = Physicai Appearance; PP = Personal Possessions; AA = Athletic Ability

other interactions @ (1,60) = 2.35, -; F (1,60) = -73, a, LD Status main effects @ (1,

60) = -54, -; F (1,60) = -47, -), or Gender main effects (F (1,60) = 2.35, -; F (1,60) =

-17, -) were found for the Personai Possessions o r Athletic Ability items, respectively.

Academic Factor

A two-way ANOVA with Gender and LD Status as independent variables was

conducted on the Academic factor of the RCPR: P- Results indicated significant main

effects for Gender (F (1,28) = 6.59, Q c -10) and LD Status (F (1,28) = 3.39, c .IO),

with no significant interaction between the variables (F (1,28) = -25, as). On this scale,

participants without LD (M = 3.50; = -98) were rated by their peers as higher in

academic competence than participants with LD = 2.98; = 1.06), while females

(M = 3 -66; = -94) were rated higher than males &f = 2.92; = 1 .O 1). These results

provide a manipulation check for the study's design by indicating peer-perceived

differences in academic ability, with the group with LD rated as least competent.

Page 78: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

77

Revised Class Plav rat in^: Self-ratinns

Before analyses could be conducted on the Revised Class Play Rating: Self-

ratings (RCPR: S) questionnaire, it was k t necessary to establish the intercorrelations

and interna1 reliabilities for the RCPR: S and its component factors. To begin, a Pearson

product-moment correlation was conducted on the three original factors of the RCPR- S.

Results indicate that the Sociabiw-Leadership scale was sîgnificantly negatively

correlated to both the Aggressive-Dîsruptive (g = -.35, g c -05) and Sensitive-Isolated =

-.38, p c -05) scales, but that the Aggressive-Disniptive and Sensitive-Isolated scales

were not significantly correlated with each other & = .04, ns)- Next, a series of Cronbach

coefficient alpha's was calculated to determine the internai reliability for the three RCPR:

S factors for each target group. Alpha coefficients for the Sociability-leadership,

Aggressive-Disruptive, and Sensitive-Isolated factors were determined to be -76, .8 0, and

-73, respectively, for participants with LD, and -64, -74, and .06, respectively, for

participants without LD. The low alpha for the SI scale for non-LD pupils supported the

decision to examine individual item responses on this scale for both participants with and

without LD. The seven items in the SI scde are: (a) 1 am often left out; (b) My feelings

get hurt easily; (c) 1 am usually sad; (d) 1 would rather play alone than with others; (e) I

have trouble making fkiends; (f) 1 can't get others to listen; and (g) 1 am very shy.

Coefficient alpha's were also determined for each target group for the two

additional Academic and Nonacademic cornpetence factors included in the questiomaire.

Alphas for the Academic scale were detemiined to be -66 for both the group with and

without LD. Alphas for the Nonacadernic scale were detemiined to be -44 for the group

with LD and -18 for the non-LD group. Again, the low coefficeint alphas for the

Nonacadernic scale supported the decision to examine individual item responses.

Orifinal Factors

Raw scores fiom the RCPR: S were summed and averaged across al1 factors for

both groups of target participants. Scores on the RCPR: S range fkom 1.00 (statement is

Page 79: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

78

very false) to 5.00 (statement is very true), with higher scores indicating greater

endorsement for a particular factor. Means and standard deviations on di RCPR factors,

broken down by LD Status and Gender, appear in Table 9.

Table 9.

Means and Standard Deviations for RCPR: Self-ratin~s Factors

- --

LD LD Total non-LD non-LD Total non-

Boys Girls LD Boys Girls LD

@ = 8 ) @=7) (n = 15) @ = 9 ) (n = 7) b= 16)

Note. RCPR = Revised Class Play Rating; LD = leaming disabilities; SL = Sociability- Leadership; AD = Aggressive-Disruptive; SI = Sensitive-Isolated; AC = Academic; NAC = Nonacademic

To determine whether group ciifferences existed between children with and

without LD on the factors of the RCPR: S, a LD Status-by-Gender MANOVA was

conducted on the Sociability-Leadership (SL) and Aggressive-Disruptive (AD) subscales,

as only these subscales were highly correlated and had adequate Cronbach alpha levels to

a justiQ a multivariate analysis. No signiflcant multivariate LD Status-by-Gender

Page 80: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

79

interaction ( W W Lambda = -897, F (2,26) = 1.49, ns) or LD Status multivariate main

effect (Wilks' Lambda = -993, F (2,26) = -09, ns) was observed. However, a significant

multivariate main effect for Gender (Wilks' Lambda = -775, F (2,26) = 3 -78, g < -10) was

found, thus necessitating a univariate follow-up procedure to detennine where significant

ciifferences existe&

A post-hoc procedure examining the univariate F-tests for the Gender main e h t

was found to be signifïcant only for the Sociability-Leadership factor @ (2,27) = 4 . 3 6 , ~

c .1 O), where males (M = 4.28; SD = -32) rated themselves lower than fernales (&l= 4.50;

SD = -24). It shodd be pointed out that the mean self-ratings received by males, although - significantly lower than fernales, can nonetheless be considered very positive, as the

scores on this particular scale ranged f?om 1 (stating "very false" to positive qualities) to

5 (stating "very tme" to positive categories). When considering Gender as a variable,

males and femdes did not differ in self-ratings on the Aggressive-Disruptive @ (1,27) =

-45, ns) factor.

Sensitive-Isolated Factor

Means and standard deviations for participants with and without LD, broken

down by gender, on the seven items of the Sensitive-Isolated factor are presented in Table

10. Scores range fkom 2.00 to 5.00, with higher scores indicating increased agreement

with negative self-statements.

Separate two-way ANOVAs with LD Status and Gender as independent variables

and questionnaire item as dependent variables were conducted on the seven Sensitive-

kolated (SI) subscale items.

Results reved no signincant LD Status-by-Gender interactions and no LD Status

or Gender main effects on any of the seven items. ANOVA results (i.e., F-values) for .

each of the seven tests are presented in Table 11.

Nonacademic Factor

Separate two-way ANOVAs with LD Status and Gender as independent variables

Page 81: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

80

Table 10.

Mean Remonses to RCPR: Self-ratings Sensitive-Isolated Factor Items

LD LD Total non-LD non-LD Total

Boys Girls LD Boys Girls Non-LD

(n = 8) (n = 7) (n = 15) (n=9) @ = 7) (&= 16)

- - - - - -- - -

(a) 1.50(.54) 1.43(.79) 1.47(.64) 1.33(.50) 1.43(.79) 1.38(.62)

(b) 2.25 (1.58) 1.71 (-76) 2.00 (1.25) 2.56 (1.33) 2.14 (1.46) 2.38 (1-36)

(c) 1-50 (1.07) 1.29 (-49) 1-40 (33) 1.33 (-71) 1-14 (-39) 1-25 (-58)

(d) 1-13 (.35) 1.43 (-79) 1.27 (-59) 1.44 (1.33) 1.14 (-39) 1.31 (1-01)

(e) 1.12 (.35) 1.00 (-00) 1.07 (-26) 1.22 (-44) 1.14 (-39) 1.19 (-40)

(0 2.25(1.04) 1.43(.79) 1.87(.99) 1.44(.53) 1.57(1.13) 1.50(.82)

(g) 2.38 (1.51) 2.57 (1.72) 2.47 (1.56) 2.1 1 (1.54) 2.71 (1.70) 2.38 (1.59)

Note. RCPR = Revised Class Play Rating; LD = learning disabilities; Item: (a) 1 am often left out; (b) My feelings get hurt easily; (c) 1 am usually sad; (d) 1 would rather play done than with others; (e) 1 have trouble making fi-ïends; ( f ) 1 can't get others to lïsten; and (g) I am very shy.

and questionnaire item as dependent variables were conducted for Nonacademic factor

items: (a) Physical Appearance, (b) Personal Possessions, and (c) AthIetic Ability.

Means and standard deviations for both groups on the three items are presented in

Table 12. For the Physical Appearance item, results reveal no LD Status-by-Gender

interaction @ (1,27) = 2.61, ns), no LD Status main effect (F (1,27) = .49, a, but a

significant main effect for Gender @ (1,27) = 7.49, < .IO). In general, girls = 3 -7 1 ;

Page 82: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

81

Table 11.

Nonsiwcant F-tests for RCPR: S elf-ratîngs Sensitive-Isolated Factor Items

Item LD S tatus-by-Gender LD Status Gender Interaction Main Effect Main Effect

(g) - F (L,27) = -12 - F (1,27) = -01 F (1,27) = -47 Note. RCPR = Revised CIass Play Rating; LD = learning disabilities; Item: (a) 1 am often lefi out; @) My feelings get hiut easily; (c) 1 am usually sad; (d) 1 would rather play alone than with others; (e) I have trouble making Ken&; (f) 1 can't get others to listen; and (g) 1 am very shy-

SD = -99) rated themselves as more "good lookuig" than did boys @ = 2.88; = .78). - For the Personal Possessions item, resdts reveal no LD Status-by-Gender interaction

(1,27) = .87, ns), no LD Status main effect (F (1,27) = -30, ns), and no signincant

main effect for Gender @ (1,27) = -30, ns). Likewise, for the Athletic Ability item,

results reveal no LD Status-by-Gender interaction @ (1,27) = 1.03, ns), no LD Status

main effect @ (1,27) = .53, ns), and no significant main effect for Gender (1,27) =

.05, ns).

Acadernic Factor

0 A two-way ANOVA with LD Statu and Gender as independent variables was

Page 83: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

82

Table 12.

Mean Responses to RCPR: Self-ratiners Nonacademic Factor Items

LD LD Total non-LD Non-LD Total

Boys Girls LD Boys Girls Non-LD

(&= 8 ) @ = 7) @= 15) @= 9) (n = 7) (&= 16)

Note. RCPR = Revised Class Play Rating; LD = learning disabilities; PA = Physical Appearance; PP = Personal Possessions; kA = Athletic Ability

conducted on the Academic factor of the RCPR: S- Results reveai a significant LD - Status-by-Gender interaction @ (1,27) = 3.25, p < .IO), a main effect for LD Status (1,

27) = 4 . 3 9 , ~ c .IO), and amain effect for Gender @ (1,27) = 5.10, p c .IO). Overall,

girls = 3.96; SD = -66) had higher academic self-perceptions than boys a = 3.41; SD

= .8 1) and children without LD (M = 3 -94; SD = -63) had higher academic self-

perceptions than children with LD @ = 3.43; = = -85). More importantly, when the

interaction between LD Status and Gender was examined, it became clear that gender was

an important variable to consider for participants with LD, but not for those without LD.

Results indicate that boys with LD &X = 2.94; = -62) held lower self-perceptions of

academic competence than girls with LD = 3.93; SD = .79), with no corresponding

gender differences noted in the non-LD group (boys M = 3.89, SD = -70; girls M = 4.00,

SD = 33). Thus, the presence of LD was related to a relatively lower self-perception of - academic functioning only for boys.

Page 84: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

83

Peer Rater Interviews

Ail peer raters for children with and without LD were able to provide reasons as

to why they chose the target peer as a preferred playmate. Interviewers were periodically

required to probe rater's responses and encourage exp lanation of vague statemmts;

however, extensive probing was discouraged.

In general, responses to the open-ended question ("What is it about that made

you choose as someone you like to play with?") corroborated the r d t s fiom both

the MANOVA on the Vaughn and Hogan mode1 and the RCPR data. No differences were

found between the raters of children with LD and the raters of cbildren without LD with

regard to why the target children were chosen as prefmed playmates, as each group of

peer raters endorsed al1 eight categories of responses to the interview question.

Differences between groups did emerge in the fiequency with which certain categories

were endorsed, Table 13 contains the fiequencies of responses per category for

participants with and without LD, broken down by gender.

To test for LD Status and Gender differences in peer raters' responses to the

i n t e ~ e w question, a test for the signifrcance of merences betweem proportions was

conducted on comparisons yielding the highest relative discrepancies (Le., largest

absolute merence between percentages), until no differences were found at which point

no M e r comparisons were made. A -score of 1.645 (Le., p c -10) was used as the cut-

off for significance. Results indicated significant differences between the proportion of

girls and boys with LD ea&g responses in the Non-descript (z = 2.05, E < -10) and

Leadership (& = 2.03, p < .IO) categories, with girls with LD receiv3ng relatively more

responses than boys with LD. One other significant clifference was found between boys

and *Is without LD where boys without LD received relatively more responses in the

Outside Associations category than girls without LD (Z = 1.66, Q < -10).

Self-raters Interview

Al1 target children with and without LD (except one boy with LD declining the

Page 85: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

84

Table 13.

Frequencv and (Percentage) of Peer Remonses in @en-ended Interview Categories

LD LD Total non-LD non-LD Total

Girls Boys LD Girls Boys Non-LD

Category @ = 14) (n=18) (n=32) @=14) @=18) @=32)

ND 13 (92.86) 11 (61.1 1) (75.00) 11 (78.57) 14 (77.77) (78.12)

Note. LD = learning disabïIities; Category: S = Similady; H = Hurnourous; T = Talent; EC = Enjoyable Company; PQ = Positive Qualities; OA = Outside Associations; L = Leadership; ND = Non-descript

interview process) self-reported reasons as to why they felt fellow peers had chosen them

as preferred playmates. Table 14 contains these fiequencies and percentages, broken

down by gender.

To test for LD status and gender differences in self raters' responses to the

i n t e ~ e w question, tests for the significance of differences between proportions were

conducted using a Z-score of 1.645 (Le., I) c -10) as the cut-off for significance. Results

Page 86: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

85

Table 14.

Frequencv and Wercentage) of Self-rated Responses in Open-ended Interview Categories

LD LD To ta1 Non-LD non-LD Tot al

Category Girls Boys LD Girls Boys Non-LD

b=7) @=8) @=15) @=7) b=9) @=i6)

Note. LD = leamhg disabilities; Category: S = Similarity; H = Humornus; T =Talent; EC = Enjoyable Company; PQ = Positive Qualities; OA = Outside Associations; L = Leadership; ND = Non-descript

indicated a significant clifference between the proportion of girls with and without LD,

and boys without LD in the Positive Qualities response category, with girls with and

without LD offering more responses related to Positive Qualities than boys without LD &

= 2.09, p < -10). One other significant difference was found between girls and boys with

LD in the Non-Descript category, where girls with LD provided relatively more

responses in this category ttian boys with LD (z = 2.35, Q c -10).

Page 87: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

86

Chapter V: Discussion

Overview

To secure an understanding of the social-emotional fiinctioning of socially

successful children with LD in cornparison to their equally accepted peers without LD,

this study examined the characteristics of peer-accepted students with LD from the

multiple perspectives of the peer, self, and teacher. Social fiuictioning was investigated

f?om a multi-domain perspective by using the muitidimensional model of social

competence proposed by Vaughn and Hogan (1990) as a framework.

Despite extensive research dealing with the social status of children with and

without LD, a void clearly existed in the literature on social competence in socialiy-

accepted children with l e h g disabilities. The riterature failed to examine the extent to

which pathways to peer acceptance for children with LD were similar to or different fÏom

those of their non-LD peers. It has been shown (e-g., Coie, Dodge, & Kupersmidt, 1990;

Newcomb, Bukowski, & Pattee, 1993) that socialiy-accepted children without LD exhibit

certain noteworthy and measurable characteristics related to their social success such as

acadernic competence, cooperation, honesty, good looks, humour, and athletic ability.

However, it was not known whether the attributes associated with normally-achieving

children's popularity and social acceptance were similar to the attributes which facilitate

social acceptance in children with LD. Further, since children with LD, by dennition,

have a lower likelihood of being perceived as academically competent b y their peers, it

followed that children with LD may not have the same opportunities, academically, for

gaining social accep tance as their non-LD peers. This investigation detennined whether

peer-accepted students with LD must compensate for their lack of academic ski11 with

abilities in nonacademic areas (e-g., social skiils, athletics, appearance, behavioural

conduct) that are at least equal to, if not better than, those of their non-LD peers.

Results are discussed as they relate to the components of the Vaughn and Hogan

(1990) model of social competence. The statisticd effectiveness of the ~ a u g h n and

Page 88: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

87

Hogan model was not directly assessed in this study; rather, the model was utilised as an

organisational heuristic in the examination of social competence in children with and

without LD. To review, the Vaughn and Hogan model of social competence suggests that

there are four important areas to consider in the social functioning of children with LD

and that an individual's social competence is comprised of a unique pattern of strengths

and weaknesses across these four components waughn & Hogan, 1990). This model

allows for the logical hypothesis that if a particular group of children (i.e., those with LD)

differs in social competence fiom another group, then group ciifferences should emerge in

the areas covered by the model- The pragmatic nature of this model lent itself to the

development of one underlying question guiding this investigation: To what extent do

socially-accepted children with LD differ £iom their sociaily-accepted non-LD

couterparts in social competence areas such as selficoncept, behaviourd conduct, and

social skills? Finaiiy, the need to determine whether socially-accepted children with LD

possess unique social-behavioural characteristics relative to non-LD peers prompted the

inclusion of various objective and qualitative tools in this study.

Peer Status

In this study, peer status was used as an independent variable rather than a

dependent variable and as such this component of the Vaughn and Hogan model was not

included in multivariate analyses. Nonetheless, a hypothesis was offered as to the

proportion of children with and without LD being represented in each of the five

sociometric categones. The hypothesis that children with LD would be represented in al1

five sociometric categories was not c o h e d , as no children with CD were classifïed in

the controversial category. Further analysis, however, reveaied that children with and

without LD did not ciiffer significantly in the rates with which they were assigned to the

remaining four sociometric categories. These results were unexpected, however, given the

wealth of research in this area finding that children with LD eam proportionaliy more

rejection classifications and less popular classifications than non-LD peers (Condeman,

Page 89: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

88

1995; Stone & La Greca, 1990; Wiener, Harris, & Shirer, 1990)- That this study failed to

find significant differences in the classification rates of LD and non-LD peers may be

explained in three important ways related to (a) sampling methods involving sample size

and composition, (b) identification procedures, and (c) classification procedures.

First, the small sample @ = 32) of students identifled as having LD in the initial

sample of participants dong with the relatively high number of participants within this

sample falling in the unclassifiable category resulted in small numbers of participants

with LD being compared to the larger non-LD sample. Thus, small sample size may have

prevented the emergence of significant differences in sociometric classification rates (cf.

Vaughn, McIntosh, Schiimm, Haager, & Callwood, 1993). Additionaliy, the possibility

that some children with very severe learning and behavioural difficulties were not being .

serviced by the two school boards in this study as a resdt of pnvate school placement

must be considered when interpreting the non-significant sociometric results.

Secondly, the participants in the present study were non-school-identified children

with learning disabilities attending regular classrooms for the majority of the school day.

Consequently, the greater inclusion and minimal segregation of these children (compared

to others following a pdl-out or separate class model) may be important factors

contributing to the lack of significant Werence between LD and non-LD participants'

sociometric ratings. Bryan and Bryan (1986) point out the importance of situational

influences and social context in detennlliing the social status of children with LD;

however, inconsistent ernpirical evidence exists for the assumption that students with LD

will be better accepted by peers if placed in general education classrooms (Vaughn,

Elbaum, Schumm, & Hughes, 1998) despite research that indicates the social benefits of

inclusion for students with disabilities (Baker, Wang, & Walberg, l994/l995). Within the

context of inclusive settings, some studies have revealed different social status

classification patterns between students with and without LD (Sale & Carey, 1995;

Madge, Affleck & Lowenbraun, 1990; Vaughn, Elbaum, & Schumm, 1996; Wiener,

Page 90: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

89

Hams, & Shirer, 1990), whereas other studies have found no differences between these

groups (Juvonen & Bear, 1992).

The situational variable of whether participants have been school- or researcher-

identified as having LD is important to consider. For instance, Wiener, Harris, and Duval

(1993), utilising a similar classification procedure as was used in this study, found that

children who were not school identified as having LD but who met the criteria for LD on

relevant measures, obtained significantly higher peer preference and popularity ratings

than school-identified children with LD. Further, the authors found no differences

between the school- and non-school-identified groups in peer rejection. Wiener et al.

(1993) concluded that school-identiifcation of LD may not directly contribute to peer

rejection but it does appear to make being rated as popular very difficdt. Previous studies

(Stone & La Greca, 1990) have found over 12 percent of school-identifid mainstreamed

children with LD as sociometricaiIy popular, however, this number was simüficantly less

than that of non-LD peers earning popuhr status. Thus, the variable of school-

identification of LD appears influentid in whether similar or dissimilar sociometric

classification rates are found between LD and non-LD peers. It is plausible that as the

target children with LD in this study were not school-identified as having learning

di fficulties, they were less at-risk for davourable sociornetric status outcornes compared

to school-identified peers.

Thirdly, there is a lack of consistency in sociometric classification procedures in

past research, especially in relation to whether peer ratings or nominations are used, how

the "average" status category is defined, and whether same-gender or cross-gender ratings

are gathered (see Teny & Coie, 199 1). For example, in the 19 studies pert-g to the

peer status of children with LD reviewed by Wiener (1987), four different types of

sociomeûic methodologies were utilised Likewise, Swanson and Malone's (1992) review

cites 22 studies in this area, six of which utilised peer nominations and 16 of which

utilised peer ratings. Terry and Coie (1991) pointed out that there has been no clear trend

Page 91: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

90

arnong researchers in the use of same- or cross-gender ratings and that no evidence exists

that different status designations result fiom the two methodologies. Unfortunately, the

lack of consistent methodology in social status research detracts kom k t cornparisons

between p s t studies and the present investigation.

It was also hypothesised that at least 40% of children with LD would receive a

favourable social status rating (Le., average or popular) and that 10 to 20% would receive

a popular rating. Both hypotheses were c o h e d as 12 out of 28 children with LD

(42.86%) received a sociomeûic rating of average or better and 5 out of 28 (1 7.86%)

children with LD received a sociometric rating of popular, When interpreting the

classification rates for the LD and non-LD groups in light of normal ctwe distribution

rates, it appears that both groups are normaily distributed within the sociometric

categories (excluding the controversial category where there were no children with LD).

In conclusion, that no differences in sociometric classification rates between

children with and without LD were found suggests that at lest fiom a sociometnc stand-

point children with researcher-identifïed LD may be more similar to than different h m

their non-LD peers. Further, children classitied as having LD in uiis study were not

categoncally rejected by their peers and some were even the most popular children in

their cIassrooms. These results support the growing realisation that many children with

LD are able to eam positive peer ratings and nominations fkom their cIassmates and that

the variables of identification, labeliing, and classroom placement must be considered

when drawing conclusions on the peer relations of students with LD. Future studies need

to clarie the roIe that school versus non-school identification plays in the social status of

children with LD and the extent to which sociometric status varies as a fünction of

programming differences such as full inclusion, mainstreaming, or segregation.

Vaughn and Honan Mode1 of Social Cornnetence

Social Skills and Behavioural Conduct

Comparing children with and without LD on the three social competency domains

Page 92: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

91

of the Vaughn and Hogan mode1 yielded a small number of significant group and gender

rnultivariate and univariate main effects and interactions. The question of how

participants with and without LD would compare on teacher-ratings of social skiils and

problem behaviours indicated that, consistent with previous research @ender & Smith,

1990), children with LD were rated by their teachers as higher in problem behaviours

than non-LD peers- Significant group differences were not found in teacher-rated social

skills, however, oniy boys with LD were rated by teachers as having low average social

skills, based on instrument nonns. These results are most meanin@ when viewed within

the context of the significant LD Status-by-Gender univariate interactions illustfating that

boys with LD were rated as the highest in problem behaviour and the lowest in social

skills of auy other group.

Past research related to gender differences in ctassroom behaviour has found that

boys tend to be rated by teachers as behaving more poorly than girls and to be referred

more often than girls for acadeanic difficulties (e-g., Bryan, 1974; Cullinau, Epstein, &

Lloyd, 1981; Offord, Boyle, & Racine, 1991; Shaywitz, Fletcher, & Shaywitz, 1994).

This study supports this notion and m e r reveals that boys experiencing academic

difficulties may be rated by their teachers as demonstrating the lowest social and

behavioural skills in any given classroom.

The extent to which low academic standing influences teachers' perceptions of

students' sociai and behaviouraf skills is unclear at this time as few studies have

simultaneously investigated children with LD, low-achieving, and average-achieving

children in these areas. One study which did such a cornparison waager, 1992) found that

teachers rated both the students with LD and the low-achieving students more negatively

than average-achieving peers on the Social Skills scale of the SSRS-T. The present

flndings, however, reveal that low achievement may not be the decisive factor in negative

teacher ratings because girls with LD, who did not difEer significantly fiom boys with LD

in measured achievement, were rated by teachers as possessing relatively positive sociai

Page 93: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

92

skills and few problem behaviours. Thus, it appears that the presence of leaming

disabilities is an especiaily potent factor related to teacher-ratings of boys, more so than

of girls who experience simiIar academic difficulties,

To more M y explain the results fiom the SSRS-T, it is helpfid to take a closer

look at the specific skills contained within this measure. Most skills tapped by the SSRS-

T deal with either social skills related to classroom success (Le., teacher-pleasing

behaviours such as listenhg, cooperating with peers, completing assignments) or social

skills related to making and ma in tahg fiends (Le., interpersonal social skills such as

being polite, fnendly, caring). Researchers have used factor analysis to create altemate

versions of the SSRS-T (1986 version), containing only those items fiom the Sociai Skilis

scale that load on one of two factors labelled (a) Cooperative/Responding and @)

Outgoinghitiating (Vaughn & Hogan, 1990; Vaughn, Zaragoza, Hogan, & Waiker,

1993). This procedure has been used to more precisely analyse the interpersonal or peer-

pleasing social skills of children with LD. In appreciation of the Vaughn et al. (1990;

1 993) methodology, it was deemed important to determine whether students with LD in

the present sttidy were rated by teachers as particulady weak in classroom social skills

(Le-, teacher-pleasing skills), fkiendship making and maintaining social skïlls (i. e., peer-

pleasulg skills), or both Spes of skills- Recently, Bryan (1 997) has no ted that teachers are

often more lïkely to attend to behaviours that inteifere with a child's academic progress

than to social relations between peers. As such, it is possible that the boys with LD in this

study received negative teacher ratings because they were more likely to engage in

academic-interfering (Le., non-teacher-pleasing) behaviours within the classroom setting

than other participants. To examine this question, a post hoc analysis on teacher-rated

peer-pleasing versus teacher-pleasing behaviours was conducted.

Due to the small sample size in the present study a factor analytic examination of

SSRS-T (1990) items was not feasible; nonetheless, post hoc procedures were undertaken

to fùrther examine teacher-pleasing versus peer-pleasing behavîours on the SSRS-T. Each

Page 94: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

93

of the 30 items on the SSRS-T Social Skills factor was recategorised by the author, based

on face validity7 as ref&ng to either a peer-pleasing (e-g., gives compliments to peers) or

teacher-pleasing (e.g., attends to your instructions) behaviour (see Appendix F for a

breakdown of peer-pleasing and teacher-pleasing items). Next, mean peer-pleasing and

teacher-pleasing scores were caicdated for each target group (scores range fkom 0-2-00)

and were followed by the calculation of difference scores between rneans. Table 15

summarïses this data

Table 15.

Mean Scores and Difference Scores Between Peer-deasing and Teacher-deashg Social

Skills on the SSRS-T

Peer-pleasing Teacher-pleasing DBerence

Target Group - M - SD - M - SD Score I)

LD boys .95 -39 -92 -44 0.03 ns

LD girls 1.59 -27 1-56 -31 0.03 ns

non-LD boys 1.44 -43 1.28 -49 0.16 11s

non-LD girls 1.35 .32 1.63 -59 -0.28 ~ < . 1 0

Note. S S ~ T : Social Skills Rating System-Teacher, LD = learning disabilities

T-tests for related measures using mean peer-pleasing and teacher-pleasing scores

were calculated for each target group and revealed a significant difference between peer-

pleasing and teacher-pleasing scores only for girls without LD, t (6) = -2.14, c -10.

Interpretation of this post hoc data suggests that girls without LD are rated by teachers as

displaying more teacher-pleasing than peer-pleasing behaviour, although they are rated

Page 95: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

94

relatively high on both types of behaviour. The supposition that boys with LD would be

xated as displayhg less teacher-pleasing than peer-pleasing behaviour was not confirmed;

however, consistent with MANOVA results, boys with LD were rated as displaying the

lowest levels of social skills of ali groups- More research using larger samples and factor

analysis is needed in this area as it rernains unclear whether boys wîth LD have more

difficulties displaying teacher-pleasing behaviours within the classroom than girls with

LD. For example, Wiener, Harris, and Shirer (2990) found that boys with LD have a

tendency to take on the role of class clown and that this disruptive classroom behaviour is

looked upon with high regard by peers, but not by teachers. Classroom disruptiveness

may be best understood on a continuum, where small amounts of disruptive behaviour are

not troublesome, but where increasing levels of negative behaviours are associated with

unfavourable teacher perceptions. Thus, there may exist a threshold Ievel of disruptive

behaviour that when surpassed withh the classroom context and among peers carries with

it negative consequenees for teacher relations (Le., negative teacher-perceptions), but

which preserves or even fost ers positive p eer relations. This speculative conclusion

remains to be more fully investigated.

A M e r point must be made conceming the possibility of gender biases in

teacher's perceptions of students with academic nifficulties. It is possible that teachers

perceive the behaviours of boys with LD in a more negative light than they do for girls

with similar deficits, despite fêw differences in actual behaviour. For example, it has been

shown that certain teachers have a differential attitude towards males and females and

that they favour referriag males for academic or behaviourai probtems even when females

have identical problems ('Vogel, 1990). In this study it was not possible to determine

whether teacher perceptions of problem behaviours and low social skills reflected actual

differences in behaviour or negative perceptual biases by teachers towards boys

experiencing academic difficulties. Future studies utilising teacher-ratings of social skills

and problem behaviours may wish to look more closely at the relationship between actual

Page 96: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

95

and perceived behaviour of boys and girls with LD in understanding the significance of

teacher bias.

Finally, longitudinal studies conducted by Vaughn and coileagues have explored

teacher acceptance as influencing the peer acceptance of children with LD. The

researchers found that when controllhg for teacher acceptance by choosing students ftom

classes where teachers were identÏfied as "accepting" and "effective" with mainstreamed

students, no signifïcant ciifferences emerged among achievement groups (LD, low-

achieving, averagelhigh-achieving) in overall peer acceptance or peer rejection (Vaughn,

McIntosh, Schumm, Haager, & Callwood, 1993). These hdings suggest that teacher

acceptance is associated with, and may be a facilitaor of, peer acceptance for children

with LD. The present results, however, indicate that positive teacher perceptions towards -

children with LD are not necessarily au essential cornponent in the pathway toward peer

acceptance, as peer-accepted boys with LD in this study were not favourably viewed by

their teachers mese conflicting hdings speak to the multiple potential pathways to

social acceptance in children with LD and to the need for fiirther research in this area

In conclusion, peer-accepted boys in this study were rated by their teachers as less

sociaily skilled and more prone to problematic school behaviours- This outcome diiffers

fkom past research indicating that higher teacher ratings of problem behaviour and lower

teacher ratings of social skills are inversely related to social success and positive social

status (Cantrell & Prinz, 1985; Coie & Dodge, 1988; French & Waas, 1985). Direct

cornparisons between these studies and the present investigation are cautioned, however,

as previous studies were conducted with nomaliy-achieving children, did not use a

restricted social status range, and did not utilise similar measures.

Self-concep t

Research has consistently shown that children with LD have lower academic self-

concept but comparable general self-worth, in cornparison to their non-LD peers

(Grolnick & Ryan, 1990; Heath, 1995; Kistner, Kaskett, White, & Robbins, 1987;

Page 97: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

96

Kloomok & Cosden, 1994). Further, children with LD have reduced prospects of

enhancing their social standing and seif-concepts via normal or high achievement in

academics and rnay therefore be more likely to engage in positive self-perceptions in

nonacademic domains in compensahg for their inability to gain acceptance and positive

self-perceptions through traditional academic means (cf. Bear & Minke, 1996; Hartup,

1983). Thus, it was questioned whether children with LD in this study wodd hold higher

overall self-perceptions in nonacademic domains than their non-LD peers. This

hypothesis was not codhned, however, as children with LD in this sample did not ciiffer

fiom their non-LD cornterparts in overall nonacademic self-concept. It is important to

note that the mean nonacademic self-concept domain raw scores for both groups of

participants c m be described as "positive" (i-e., above a score of 3-00). As Kloomok and

Cosden (1994) point out, a mean score of 3-00 or more on any of the SPPLD domains

indicates that the student has endorsed positive items as either "sort of true for me" or

"really true for me," whereas a value of less than 3.00 indicates that the student has

endorsed negative items as being self-relevant- Thus, a mean score above 3.00 is

interpreted as reflecting a relatively positive nonacademic self-concept. Results indicate

that in tems of nonacademic selfkoncept, sociaîiy-accepted children with LD hold self-

perceptions that are positive and comparable to their non-LD peers.

The possibility that cfüldren with LD will develop higher nonacademic self-

concepts over time as a self-preserving and compensatory rnechanism in response to

repeated academic shortcomings is important to consider. As Harter (1982) and Marsh

(1989) point out, children's self-concepts tend to become more differentiated with age.

That is, as a result of continued exposure to the skills and abilities of other students of the

same age, children develop an understanding that there are some things at which they are

good and some things at which they are not. Thus, with t h e the nonacademic self-

concept of socially-accepted children with LD may become increasingly differentiated

(Le., more distinguished fiom their academic self-concepts) and more positive. With this

Page 98: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

97

possibility comes the reaIïsation that nonacademic self-concept should continue to be

assessed in children with LD weli into the hi& school years, as it is possible that

nonacademic self-concept will change in response to fluctuations in academic

hctioning, experiences, and settings.

Social, Emotional, and Behavïoural Characteristics

Peer Ratinns

No statistically significant group differences in the peer ratings of positive and

negative social characteristics of children with and without LD were found on the

Revised Class Play Rating (RCPR) scale, ït was originally questioned whether children

with LD need to excel in at least one measurable area of nonacademic hctioning in

order to garner sirnilar levels of peer acceptance as children without LD. Resdts,

however, do not lend themselves to thïs interpretation. In contrast, the present results

support Kistner and Gatlids (1989) suggestion that the correlates of social status for

mainstrearned children with LD are similar to those reported for their non-LD peers.

S imilarly, Siperstein, Bopp, and Bak (1 978) also found that an qua1 proportion of

students with and without LD received positive nominations for athletic ability @est

athlete) and physical appearance (best Iooking), The authors concluded that nonacademic

talents possessed by children with LD may be the necessary redeeming qualities in

obtaining peer acceptance that is on par with non-LD peers- Support for this view cornes

fiom evidence in this study that children with and without LD with similar peer status

profiles display positive nonacademic skills and do not differ in peer-rated social,

emotional, and behavioural characteristics.

S elf-ratings

Overall, s i m c a n t merences were found on the RCPR: S between boys and

girls in self-rated skïlls on the Sociability-Leadership scale, with girls rating themselves

higher (Le., more favourably) on this factor than boys. This outcome may be explained in

part when considering the research examining gender differences (Le., Maccoby &

Page 99: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

98

Jacklin, 1974) suggesting that the dynamics within and between girls' and boys' social

groups differ. Finally, whiIe no other simiificant results were found for the RCPR: S, it is

interesthg to note the pattern that emerged whereby boys with LD provided lower (Le.,

more favourable) self-ratings on the Aggressive-Disruptive scale than a l l other peers. The

disparity in rathgs between boys with LD and their teachers is puzzhg and may be

explained by self-perceived or teacher-perceived biases, Studies comparing the actual and

perceived behavioural conduct of children with LD are needed to cl- this issue.

Nonacademic and Academic Factors

The question of whether children with LD wodd

RCPR: P as possessing unique strengths in nonacademic

group differences on any of the three nonacademic items

be rated by their peers on the

skiIls was not supported. No

of the RCPR- P were found. In

academic areas, the fÏnding that children with LD were rated as lower in academic

competence than children without LD supported the intended group differences in

academic functioning between children with and without LD,

The question of whether children with LD wodd have higher self-perceptions,

relative to non-LD peers, in nonacademic areas as rneasured by the RCPR: S was not

supported. Resuits h m the RCPR: S support the fïndings fkom the SPPLD Nonacademic

domain analyses showing no group or gender differences in nonacademic self-

perceptions. The notion that chiltiren with LD would have lower self-perceptions in

academic areas, however, was supported- Further, the significant LD Status-by-Gender

interaction on the Acadernic competence factor was interesting as it indicated that boys

with LD held the lowest self-perceptions in academic areas. Thus, despite poor academic

hctioning and accurate peer and self-perceptions ofsame, children with LD in this

study were able to secure positive peer and self-perceptions in nonacademic areas which

were comparable to ratings for non-LD peers. Previous research (Heath & Wiener, 1996;

Kloomok & Cosden, 1994) suggests that despite actual, peer-perceived, and self-

perceived diflEicuIties in academic areas, children with LD are able to obtain peer

Page 100: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

99

perceptions and hold self-perceptions in nonacademic areas that are positive and

comparable to those held b y their non-LD peers. Sirnilar patterns of academic and

nonacademic self-perceptions were found in the present study.

I n t e ~ e w Findin~s

In general, results of the peers' and target child's open-ended interviews

corroborated the fïndings from the quantitative analyses of this study. Boys and girls with

and without LD in this sample were described by their peers and by themselves as fbn to

be around, fkiendly, kind, humourous, good leaders, and as having things in common with

fellow peers. The few significant ciifferences that emerged in the interview data suggest

that girls with LD are seen by their peers, more often than boys with LD, as leaders. This

hding is noteworthy considering that girls with LD rated themselves as somewhat

higher on the Sociability-Leadership factor of the RCPR: S than did boys with LD. These

fïndings suggest that leadership may be an important pathway toward peer acceptance for

girls with LD.

In general, that few differences in the ftequency of category endorsement were

found within peer and self-raters lends M e r support to the contention that peer-

accepted children with LD are more similar to than diEaent fiom their non-LD

counterparts. It appears fiom the results of the i n t e ~ e w process that social acceptance is

a condition which is achieved and described by peers in similar ways for both children

with and without LD.

Conclusions and Contributions to Knowledge

Early research with children with LD identified that a disproportionate number of

youngsters with leaming difficulties were not well liked by their peers (Bryan, 1974;

1976). Since that time, the focus of most research has been on confinning the low

acceptance and social difficulties of students with LD rather than on examining positive

social-emotional outcornes. Because children and adolescents with LD represent a

heterogeneous group characterised by underachievement, it is surprising that more

Page 101: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

100

research has not been conducted with subgroups of children with LD in sociornetric status

categones. This is an area in need of further study.

Despite few significant differences between LD and non-LD students in peer

ratings, interesting gender and LD status interactions did emerge in teacher-ratings.

Generally, results show a variable pattern of social-emotional features for peer-accepted

boys and girls with LD in cornparison to non-LD counterparts. SpecZcally, boys were

distinguishable fiom girls in teacher-rated social skills and problem behaviour and boys

with LD were distinguishable fiom aLi other peers in teacher-rated problem behaviour.

The most important contribution of this study cornes fiom the conclusion that

children with LD are able to secure healthy social fiinctioning without having to

necessarily excel beyond their non-LD peers in nonacademic or social-emotional realms,

Findings support the presence of minimal distinguishing features between socially

successfiil children with and without LD in terms of their self-perceived and other-

perceived social, emotional, and behavioural characteristics- Results suggest that there

may not be a "secret recipe" for social success that eludes children with LD; rather, it

appears that heaithy social fiuictioning is possible for many children with LD. Research

conducted by Siperstein, Bopp, and Bak (1 978) pointai to the importance of children

with LD possessing positive nonacademic traits as possible avenues toward peer

acceptance. The present study supports this conclusion and fürthers our understanding of

the quality and quantity of nonacademic traits or abilities that are needed by children with

LD in securing social acceptance fiom peers.

Cornparison of teacher and peer ratings obtained in this study suggests that a

certain amount of variability exists between teacher's and peer's evaluation of the social-

behavioural fünctioning of socially-accepted children with LD. This conclusion is

consistent with past research indicating a fiequent lack of inter-rater agreement in social-

emotional domains between raters such as teachers, peers, parents, and students

(Achenbach, McConaughy, & Howeii, 1987; Conne11 & ïlardi, 1987). It is clear from the

Page 102: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

101

present results that students with LD are perceived as demonstrating strengths in social

fiuictioning in some, but not all settings, and fkom the perspectives of some, but not al1

raters. As Achenbach, McConaughy, and Howell(l987) pointed out, one would not

expect cross-situational ratings to converge. Rather, pieces of information provided by

different raters with perspectives on different settings should be combined to pauit a more

coruprehensive picture of the child's socio-behavioural charactenstics (Haager & Vaughn,

1995)-

In this study, teachers' perceptions of target chiIdren with LD tended to ciiffer

fiom peers' perceptions of these children, especiaily in social skills areas. These results

are similar to those found by Lancelotta and Vaughn (1989) who found that teacher's

view of peer acceptance had only a modest correlation to the views held by students, and

by Nabuzoka and Smith (1993) who found that peer assessments of chitdren with LD

were not related to those of classroom teachers. As Achenbach et al. pointed out, reasons

for clifferences in ratings between raters may be due in part to one of several factors. First,

differences between children and adults in the b e s of reference used is an important

variable to consider as teachers and peers tend to view other students in different ways,

through different lenses (Landau, Milich, & Whitten, 1984). For example, teachers are

Likely to hold expectations for school behaviour which are clearly defined (i.e., progress

toward academic success) and based on their prior teaching experiences, whereas peers

may not hold such well-defied behavioural expectations. Also, teachers have reiatively

limited opportunity to observe children in unstructured play with their peers and to

become privy to the intricacies of these social interactions. Finally, differences in ratings

between teachers and peers may arise as a consequence of the differential values placed

on particular abilities or skills by teachers and classrnates. For example, the same

behaviour by a student may be seen as making fellow peers laugh (peer perception) or as

disrupting the cIass (teacher perception), depending on who is doing the evaluating. In

this study, the finding that socially-accepted boys with LD were seea by teachers as more

Page 103: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

102

problematic and as less socidy skilled in the school setîing than other children lends

support for the contention that dimptive classroom behaviour may be valued by peers

and a potential route to peer popularity, despite the negative perceptions this behaviour

induces in teachers,

Study Limitations

This investigation is one of the few studies focusing exclusively on socidly

successful students with LD and as such assurned an exploratory, rather than

conknatory, b c t i o n in the field. The author was aware of the dangers inherent in

exploratory-type research (i-e., increased chance of Type 1 errors and reduced statistical

power) and necessary adjustments were made accordingly. Nonetheless, other limitations

regarding sample size, definition, rneasurement, and identification techniques are

important to consider.

First, the small sample size is a concem that requires the study to be replicated.

With recognition of the difficulty in hding large samples of socially successful children

with LD, it is suggested that fûture studies with adequate sample sizes compare the social

and behavioural profiles of average and popular studenk with LD to students without LD,

rather than combining these two categories of students into a homogeneous socialiy-

accepteci group as was done in this study. Also, fbture studies might wish to include

rejected status control groups in order to compare the profiles of students with LD in

various status groups. In so doing, it will be possible to compare past research

investigating the correlates of social status in average-achieving participants to sùnilar

studies with subgroups of children with LD.

In this study, participants classified as having LD were so defined on the basis of

significant discrepancies between estirnates of cognitive fiinctioning and measured

achievement in spelling, reading, and arithmetic. While the majonty of participants were

so defined on the basis of at least a significant discrepancy between IQ and reading

(52.94%), it is important to consider the hi& number of participants with the

Page 104: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

103

classification of LD havhg significant discrepancies in spelling achievement aione

(35.29%). This is an especially important concem when considering that many

participants attended French Immersion programmes where the majority of subjects were

taught in French. Concern regarding this Limitation is lessenecl, however, when

considering the mean spelling achievement scores fiom the schools where these children

attende& In al1 three schools where children were emolled in French Immersion

classrooms, mean WRAT-3 spelling achievement standard scores for participaihg

students were solidly in the average range, given the normal curve distribution. Th,

children scoring below the defïned standard score of 85 were still performing

significantly below the expected average for chrldren in their curriculum.

A m e r limitation of this study was the variable intemai reliabilities and

resultant inability of the Revised Class Play Rating (RCPR) scale to pinpoint the subtle

différences that may egst between sociaUy successfid children with and without LD. The

small sample size precluded a cornparison of dl individual RCPR item scores between

the two groups in this study. It is possible that a more sensitive measure and one which

has strong interna1 reliability for both groups would be able to detect differences in

samples of LD and non-LD chiidren.

Results from the interview potion of this study supported the quantitative

hdings of minimal merences found between socially-accepted boys and girIs with and

without LD. These results are somewhat contrary to expectations for it was postulated

that children with LD would be perceived by their peers as excelhg in at least one or

more nonacadernic areas. It is possible that the Iack of structured probes to accompany

the open-ended interview, however, detracted fiom the ability to pinpoint clifferences

between target participants and gain a better understand of vague responses (e-g., "she's a

good fiend"). While each child was able to provide appropriately phrased responses to

the interview question, it was nonetheless noted that many children had difficulty putting

their sentiments into words. This difficulty was apparent in the numerous phrases

Page 105: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

104

assigned to the Nomdescript category. Also, it was obsewed that many of the children's

responses to the interview question reflected personal characteristics similar to the items

contained in the RCPR questionnaire- It is possible that as the RCPR was administered in

close temporal proximity to the interview that peer raters' responses were influenced by

RCPR item content. Future investigations cm circumvent this difficulty by carefûlly

timing the interview process and by devising probes facilitating oral expression of ideas.

Finally, one of the most important limitations of the present study was the

exclusive use of researcher-identifiecl children with LD and the resulting impact this

methodology has on the generalisability to past and fiiture studies. Studies examining the

social fiinctioning of children with LD typically utilise preselected, school-identified

populations of students with LD, identified as such based on either local, state, or national

identification criteria for having LD. Researcher-identified populations, where

identification of students as having LD is performed for research purposes, are

hûequently used. In the present study, however, the use of school-identified participants

with LD w 2 not feasible given the inclusive and non-labelling philosophy of targeted

schooIs. According to teacher feedback (formal school records were not available), only a

small number of students in the present sample (4 out of 17) had been foxmally referred

or evaluated for Learnïng difficulties by school personnel and as such the majority of

participants in this study had not been officially labelled as having iearnîng dificulties.

While non-labehg practices are admirable for a number of social and emotional reasons

(see KLiewer & Biklen, 1996) they nonetheless diminishes the generalisability of the

present results to previous studies in the area using school-identified populations.

However, it is important to note that despite its widespread use, there are severai

problems inherent in using system-identified samples of children with LD, namely, the

trend in underidentification of girls with LD and the biasing influence of student

behaviour in teacher referral rates (Vogei, 1990). Future studies may wish to combine

both school and researcher identification procedures in their designs to avoid potential

Page 106: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

105

genedisability or sampling-bias limitations.

Educationai IniDlications

This study expands our understanding of the processes involved in social success

for chiIdren with LD and helps in providing a number of implications for educational

practice. Extending the interpretation of fïndùigs to the areas of educational programming

and interventions, two ideas are offered. First, the results of the present study suggest that

there may be social and personal advantages for chiidren with leaming diniculties who

attend completely integrated settings- Despite research showing that social problems

related to LD are not entirely alieviated by simply placing a child with LD in a fùlly-

integrated classroorn (Madge, Afûeck, & Lowenbraun, 1990), research does show that

integrated settings provide better opportunities for special education students to blend in

socially with their peers and estabiïsh reciprocal fiendshïps (Baneji & Dailey, 1995). In

fact, researchers have recently suggested that students themselves prefer inclusive to pull-

out models because ofthe increased opportunities to make fiends that are present in

incIusive classrooms (Klinger, Vaughn, Schumm, Cohen, & Forgan, 1998).

The social benefits of inclusive classrooms and the negative academic and social

effects of labelling students with LD have been illuminated by many professionals in the

field (Baker, Wang, & Walberg, l994/1995; Baneji & Dailey, 1995; Kliewer & Biklen,

1996; Wang, Reynolds, & Walberg, 19941 1 995). A number of researchers, however,

continue to emphasise the importance of rnaintaining a continuum of support seMces for

students with special needs (Fuchs & Fuchs, 1994; Vaughn & Schumm, 1995). This

study, while unabIe to offer conclusions regarding the academic benefits or drawbacks of

inclusion, does support the notion of social advantages of inclusive classrooms (e.g.,

more opportunities for developing social competence). Although not specifically studied

in the present investigation, results speak to the conclusion that when labeliing practices

are decreased and inclusive practices are increased, students with LD do not differ fjrom

non-LD peers in representation across social statu categories. Contrary to previous

Page 107: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

106

research indicating the lower social status of children with LD in mainstreamed

classrooms (Madge, Afneck, & Lowenbraun, 1990; Sale & Carey, 1995), no difference in

the proportions of children falling in the sociometrïc categories was found in this shidy

between children with and without LD,

AdditionaUy, the results of the present study can better inform pre-service and in-

s e ~ c e teachers on the nature of effective inclusion in relation to social development-

Fbt, recent publications have emerged describing best practices for teaching students

with disabilities in inclusive settings (Friend, Bursuck, & Hutchinson, 1998) and despite

effective qualitative research in this area (e-g., Hutchinson, 1996), there remains a need to

understand how the promotion of social cornpetence and social acceptance take place in

inclusive settings,

Secondly, when plauning social skills interventions for students, caution shouId

be afXorded when considering who is rating the social skills of the student (teachers,

parents, self, or peers) and the Iand of social skills to include in intemention programs

(i-e., classroom skills, interpersonal skills, task management skills). G e n d education

teachers are typically responsible for reporting students' classroom difficulties and

making refenals for remedial assistance, making their perspective critical when

examining the sociai-emotiond hctioning of children. If teacher ratings are the sole

method utilised in choosing who will benefit fkom social skills training programs, many

children chosen to receive such services may be those with a tendency toward displaying

teacher-displeasing behaviours, but who otherwise function well with their peers. Thus, a

caution is warranted when using teachers as exclusive infonnants regarding the social and

behavioural fUnctioning of students with academic difficulties, as it is possible that

children who otherwise experience social success with their peers may be identified by

teachers as lacking in social skiils.

Additionally, the teacher-perceived behavioural difnculties experienced by boys

or girls with LD in the classroom may be important avenues through which these children

Page 108: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

107

gain acceptance h m their peers. It is possible that mildly disruptive, class-clown

behaviour is highly valued arnong students and that this type of classroom behaviour is

associated with positive social outcomes among peers (Wiener, Harris, & Shirer, 1990).

Thus, it is conceivable that teacher-identified children with social and behavioural

weaknesses may actually experience social success among peers and be most in need of

classroom, rather than interpersonal, social skiils remediation.

In summary, results of the present study inform us that students with LD possess

many positive social-behwioural traits and that, despite th& academic s horicomings, are

able to achieve nonacademic success s d a r to non-LD peers. Hence, in their efforts to

enhance the scholastic success of students with LD, educators are rerninded of the

importance of i d e n w g and distinguishing between peer-perceived versus teacher-

perceived social fiinctioning strengths and weaknesses of students with LD. Finally, it is

important to consider the present raults as providing support for non-labelling and

inclusive educational practices for students with LD.

Overall, the paucity of significant differences between socially-accepted children

with and without LD in the present study, whiie not entirely expected, cm nonetheless be

seen as encowaging as it appears fkom these r e d t s that cbildren with LD do not

necessarily have to excel above and beyond their non-LD peers in social and

nonacademic areas in order to gain peer acceptance. A large amount of literature has been

devoted to the topic of social competence in general, and social skills deficits in

particular, in children with LD. Although social skills deficits are argued to be an integral

part of the constellation of learning disabilities profile @Cavale & Fomess, 1996), it is

imprudent to overlook the proportion of the LD population that manages to succeed

socialiy. It is now well established that not al1 children with LD expenence problems in

their social lives (Bryan, 1997); the time is right to broaden our scope in the study of the

social functioning of children with LD by complementing deficit-model research with

competency-mode1 investigations.

Page 109: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

108

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1978). The classification of child

psychopathology. A review and analysis of empirical efforts. Psvchologicai Bulletin. 85,

1275-1301.

Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. &, & Howell, C . T. (1 987). Child/adolescent

behaviourai and emotional problems: Implications of cross-informant correlations for

situational specincity. PsychoIonical Bulletin, 10 1,2 13-232.

Ackerman, Dey & Howes, C. (1986). Sociometric status and after-school social

activity of children with learning disabilities. Journal of Leamine. Disabilities. 19,416-

419,

Asher, S. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1986). Identïfying children who are rejected by

their peers. Developmental Psycho 10 W. 22,444449.

Asher, S. R, & Hymel, S. (1981). ChiIdren's social cornpetence in peer relations:

SociornetTic and behavioral assessrnent In J. D. Wine & M.D. Smye (Eds.), Social

cornpetence @p. 125-1 57). New York: Guilford.

Asher, S. R., & Renshaw, P. D. (198 1). Children without fnends: Social

knowledge and social skills training In S. R Asher & I. M. Gottman (Eh.), The

develovrnent of children's fi-iendships @p. 273-296). New York: Cambridge Universi@

Press.

Asher, S. R., & Wheeler, V. A- (1985). Children's loneliness: A cornparison of

rejected and neglected peer status. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psvchologv. 53,

500-505.

Axelrod, L. H. (1982). Social perception in learning disabled adolescents.

Journal of le am in^ Disabilities, 15,610-613,

Baker, E. T., Wang, M. C., & Walberg, H. J. (1994/1995). The effects of inclusion

on leaming. Educational Leadership. 52,33-35.

Banerji, M., & Dailey, R. A. (1995). A study of the effects of an inclusion mode1

on students with specific leaming disabilities. Journal of Leamine Disabilities. 28,s 1 1 -

Page 110: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

109

522.

Bear, G. G., & Minke, K. M. (1996). Positive bias in maintenance of self-worth

among children with LD. Leamin~ Disabilitv Ouarterlv. 19,23-32.

Bender, W. N. (1 985). Differences between learning disabled and non-learning

disabled children in temperament and behavior. Leaming Disability Ouarterly- 8,1148.

Bender, W, N., & Smith, J. K (1990). Classroom behavior of children and

adolescents with learning disabilities: A meta-analysis. Journal of Learnin~ Disabilities,

23,298-305.

Bender, W. N., & Wall, M. E- (1994). Social-emotional development of students

with l e d g disabilities- Leaminn Disabilitv Ouarterlv. 17,323-341.

Bender, W. N., Wyne, M. D., Stuck, G. B., & Bailey, D. B. (1984). Relative peer

status of leaming disabled, educable mentally handicapped, low-achieving and normally

achieving children. ChiId Studv Journal, 13,209-2 16.

Berndt, T. J- (1983). Correlates and causes of sociometric status in childhood: A

commentary on six current studies of popular, rejected, and negIected children- M e d l

Palmer Ouarterlv. 29,43 9-448.

Boivin, M., & Bégin, G. (1989). Peer status and self-perception among early

elementary school children: The care of rejected childrem Child Develo~ment, 60,59L-

596.

Bruck, M., & Hebert, M. (1982)- Correlates of leaming disabled students' peer

interaction problems. Learnlli~ Disability Ouarterlv. 5,353-362.

Bniininks, V. L. (1978a). Actual and perceived peer status of leaming-disabled

students in mainstream programs. Journal of S~ecial Education. 12,51-58.

Bnllninks, V. L. (1978b). Peer status and personality characteristics of leaming

disabled and nondisabled students. Journal of Leamhg Disabilities. 1 M8), 29-34.

Bryan, T. H. (1 974). Peer popularity of learning disabled children. Journal of

Leaming; Disabilities, T l O), 3 1-35,

Bryan, T. H. (1976). Peer popularity of Ieaming disabled children: A replication.

Page 111: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

110

Journal of Learning Disabilities, 9(5), 49-53,

Bryan, T. H- (2997). Assessing the personal and social skills of students with

learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, t2,63-76.

Bryan, T. H. & Bryan, J. (1986). un der stand in^ learning disabilities. PaIo Alto,

CA: Mayfîeld-

Bryan, J. H., Sherman, R., & Fisher, A. (1980). Learning disabled boys' nonverbal

behaviors with a dyadic interview- Learnin~ - Disabilitv Ouarterly, 3,6572,

Bukowski, W. M., & Hoza, B. (1989). Popularïty and fiiendship: Issues in theory,

measurement, and outcorne. In T. Bemdt & G. Ladd (Eds.), Peer relationships in child

development (pp.15-45). New York: Wiley.

Bursuck, W. D. (1983). Sociometric status, behavior ratings, and social

knowledge of learning disabled and low-achieving students. Leamine Disabilitv

OuarterIv. 6,329-33 8.

Bursuck, W. (1989). A comparison of students with learning disabilities to Iow

achieving and higher achieving students on three dimensions of social competence.

Journal of Learninn Disabilities. 22,188-194.

Byme, B. M. (1984). The generaVacademic self-concept nomological network: A

review of constmct validation research, Review of Educational Research, 54,427-456.

Cantrell, V. L., Prinz, R. J. (1985). Multiple perspectives of rejected, neglected,

and accepted children: Relationship between sociorneûic status and behavioral

characteristics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psvchologv. 53,8 84-8 89.

Carlson, C. L., Lahey, B. B., & Neeper, R (1984). Peer assessrnent of the social

behavior of accepted, rejected, and neglected children. Journal of Abnormal Child

PSVC~O~OEY. 12,189-198,

Chapman, J. W. (1988). Learning disabled children's self-concepts. Review of

Educational Research. 58,347-37 1.

Clever, A., Bear, G., & Juvonen, J. (1992). Discrepancies between competence

and importance in self-perceptions of children in integrated classes. Journal of Soecial

Page 112: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

111

Education, 26,125-13 8.

Coie, J. D. (1985). Fitting social skills intervention to the target group. In B.

Schneider, IL Rubin, & J. Ledingham (Eds.), Children's peer relations: Issues in

assessrnent and intervention (pp. 141-156), New York: Springer-

Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1988). Multiple sources of data on social behavior

and social status in the school: A cross-age cornparison. Child Development, 59,815-

829.

Coie, 1. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). Dimensions and types of

social status: A cross-age perspective. Develo~rnenta.1 Psvcholoev. 1 8,557-570.

Coie, J. D., Dodge, K A., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1990). Peer group behavior and

social status. In S. R. Asher & J- D. Coie (Eds.), Peer reiection in childhood (pp. 17-59).

New York: Cambridge University Press.

Coie, J. D., & Kupersmidt, J. (1983). A behaviourai analysis of ernerging social

status in boys' groups. Child Development, 54,1400-1416.

Coleman, J. M., McHam, L. A., & Minnett, A. M. (1992)- Similarities in the

social cornpetencies of learning disabled and low-achieving elementary school children.

JoumaI of Leaniinrr Disabilities. 25,671-677.

Coleman, J. M., & Minnett, A. M. (1992). Learning disabilities and social

competence: A social ecological perspective. Exceptional Children, 59,234246-

Conderman, G. (1995). Social status of surth-and-seventh-grade students with

learning disabilities. Learning Disabilitv Ouarterlv. 1 8,13 -24.

Connell, J. P. & flardi, B. C- (1987). Self-system concomitants of discrepancies

between children's and teachers' evaluation of academic competence. Child Development,

58 1297-1307. d

Cornwall, A-, & Bawden, H. N. (1992). Reading disabilities and aggression: A

critical review. Journal of Leamine; Disabilities. 25,28 1-288.

0 Cowen, E. L., Pederson, A., Babigian, H., Izzo, L. D., & Trost, M. A. (1973).

Long-term follow-up of early detected vulnerable children. Journal of Consulting and

Page 113: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

112

Clinical P S V C ~ O ~ O W ~ 41,438-446,

CuUinan, D., Epstein, M. IL, & Lloyd, J. (1 98 1). School behavior problems of

leaming disabled and nomial girls and boys. Learnïne Disabiiity Quarterlv. 4,163-169.

Dodge, K. A. (1983). Behaviord antecedents of peer social status. Child

Develo~ment. 54,1386-1399-

Doll, B. (l996). Children without fnends: Implications for practice and policy.

School Psvcholow Review, 25,165-183-

Dudley-Marling, C. C., & Edmiaston, R (1985). Social status of learnuig disabled

chïldren and adolescents: A review. Learning Disability Ouarterly. 8,189-204.

Eliason, M- J., & Richman, L. C. (1988). Behavior and attention in LD children-

Learning Disability Quarterly, 1 1,360-369.

Elkins, D. (1958). Some factors relate to the choice status of ninety eighth-grade

children in a school society. Genetic Psycholom Monopphs. 58,207-272.

Eliiott, S. N., & Gresham, F. M. (1993). Social skills interventions for children.

Behavior Modification, 17,287-3 13-

Elliott, S. N., & McKùinie, D. M. (1993). Relationships and differences among

social skills, problem behaviors, and academic cornpetence for mainstrearned learning-

disabled and nonhandicapped students. Canadian Journal of School Pmcholow, 10,l-14.

Feinberg, M. R., Smith, M., & Schmidt, R (1 958). An analysis of expressions

used by adolescents at varying economic levels to descnbe accepted and rejected peers.

Journal of Genetic Psychologv, 93,133-148,

French, D. C., & Waas, G. A- (1985). Behavior problems of peer-neglected and

peer-rej ected elementary-age children: Parent and teacher perspectives. Child

Development. 56,246-252.

Fnend, M., Bursuck, W., & Hutchinson, N. L. (1996'). Including- students with

mecial needs: A practical mide for classroom teachers. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Fuchs, D., & Fuchs, L. S. (1994). Inclusive schools movement and the

radicalization of special education reform. Exceotional Children 60,294-309.

Page 114: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

113

Gardner, H. (1983). Multi~le intellicrences: The theorv in ~ractice. New York:

Basic Books.

Garrett, M. K, & C m p , W. D. (1 980). Peer acceptance, teacher preference, and

self-appraisal of social status among leaming disabled students. Learninn Disabil i~

Ouarterk3,42-47.

Glass, G.V. & Hopkins, K.D. (1996). Statisticd methods in education and

psvcholonv (3rd ed.). Toronto: AIlyn & Bacon.

Gottman, J. M., Gonso, J., & Rasmussen, B. (1975). Social interaction, social

competence, and fiendship in children. Child Development. 46,709-71 8.

Gresham, F. M. (1 98 8). Social competence and motivational characteristics of

1 e a . g disabled students. In M C Wong, M.C. Reynolds, & H. J. Walberg (Eds.).

Handbook of special education: Research and practice, Wol, 2): Mildlv handica~ued

conditions @p. 283-302). Toronto: Pergarnon.

Gresham, F., & Elliott, S- (1984). Advances in the assessment of children's social

skills. School of Psvcholonv Review. 13,292-301,

Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S- N. (1989). Social skills deficits as a primary

learning disability. Journal of Leaming Disabilities. 22, L20-124-

Gresham, F. M., & Eiliott, S. N. (1990)- Social Skills Rating Svstem- Circle

Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

Gresham, F. M., & Reschly, D. J. (1986). Social skiU deficits and low peer

acceptance of maïnstreamed learning disabled children. Leaming Disabilitv Ouarterlv. 9,

23-32.

Gresham, F. M., & Reschly, D. J. (1988). Issues in the conceptualisation,

classification, and assessment of social skills in the mildly handicapped. In T. R

Kratochwill (Ed.), Advances in school ~svcholom. Vol. 6 @p. 203-247). Hillsdale, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1990). Self-perceptions, motivation, and

adjustment in children with leaming disabilities: A multiple group cornparison study.

Page 115: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

114

Journal of Learninn Disabilities, 23(3), 177-1 84.

Gronlund, N. E., & Anderson, L. (1957). Personality characteristics of socially

accepted, socially neglected, and socidly rejected junior hi& school pupils. Educational

Administration and MuItivle Pemectives Suvervision, 43,329-338.

Haager, D. S. (1992). M u h i e perspectives on the social functioning; of students

with Iearning disabilities: Teacher. parent, self, and peer reports- Dissertation Abstracts

International, 53(6), 1868-A (DA922720 1)

Haager, D., & Vaugbn, S. (2995). Parent, teacher, peer, and self-reports of the

social competence of students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities,

28,205-225,231.

Harter, S - (1982)- The Perceived Competence Scale for Children. Child

Develo~ment. 53,87-97.

Harîer, S. (1 985). Manual for the Self-Percmtion Profile for Children (Revision of

the Perceived Competence Scale for Children). Unpublished manuscript, University of

Denver, Denver, CO.

Harter, S., & Pike, R (1984). The pictorial scale of perceived competence and

social acceptance of young children. Child Development, 55,2969-1982.

Hartup, W. W. (1983). Peer relations. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of Child

Psvcholow, Vol- 4. Toronto: John Wiley & Sons.

Harwell, M. R (1988). Univariate and multivariate tests: Anova versus Manova,

Educational Research Ouarterlv, 12,20-28.

Haskins, R., Walden, T., & Ramey, C. T. (1983). Teachers and student behavior

in hi&- and low-ability groups. Journal of Educational Psvcholoav. 25,865-876.

Kazel, J. S., & Schumaker, J. B. (1988). Social skills and leaming disabilities:

Currents issues and recommendations for fiiture research. In J. Kavmaugh & T. J. Tniss

(Eds.), Learnina disabilities: Proceedings of the national conference @p. 293-344).

Parkton, MD: York Press.

Heath, N. L. (1995) Distortion and deficit: Self-perceived versus actual acadernic

Page 116: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

115

cornpetence in depressed and nondepressed children with and without learning

disabilities. Learning Disabilities: Research and Practice. 10,2-f 0-

Heath, N. L., & Brown, A. E- (1999). Self-concept differentiation and depressive

symptomatology International Journal of Psvcholonv. 34,95-105.

Heath, N. L., & Wiener, J. (1996). Depression and non-achievement self-

perception in children with and without learning disabilities, Learning Disabilitv

Chmrteriy. L 9,34-44.

Hogan, A, E., Quay, H. C,, Vaughn, S., & Shapiro, S , K. (1989)- Revised

Behavior Problem Checklist : Stability, prevalence, and incidence of behavior problems in

kindergarten and ht-grade children. PsychoIonical Assessment. lXlO3-ll 1.

Horowitz, E. C. (198 1)- Popdarity, decentering ability, and role-taking skills in

leaming disabled and normal children, Learning Disabilitv Ouarterlv. 4,23-3 0.

Huberty, C. J., & Moms, J. D. (1989). Multivariate analysis versus univariate

analyses. PsycholoeJcal Bulletin, 105,302-308.

Hutchinson, N. L. (1996)- Creating an inclusive classroom with young

adolescents in an urban school, Exceptionality Educatïon Canada. 6,51-67.

Hutton, J., & Polo, L. (1976). A sociometric study of leaming disability children

and Spes of teaching strategy. gr ou^ Psvchothemv and Psvchodrama 29,113- 120.

Ingersoll, B. D.& Goldsteïn, S. (1993)- Attention Deficits and Learning

Disabilities: Realities. Mvths. and Controversial Treatments, NY: Doubleday.

Juvonen, J., & Bear, G. (1 992)- Social djustment of children with and without

learning disabilities in integrated classrooms. Journal of Educational Psvchologv, 84,

322-330.

Kaufhan, J- M. (198 1). Characteristics of children's behavior disorders (2nd-

edJ, Columbus, OH: Merrill,

Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S- R; (1996)- Social skills deficits and leaming

a disabilities: A meta-analysis. Journal of Leaming Disabilities. 29,226-237.

Kistner, J. A., & Gatlin, D. (1989). Correlates of peer rejection among children

Page 117: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

116

with leaming disabilities. Learninn Disabilitv Ouarterl~, 12,133-1 40.

Kistner, J., Haskett, M., White, R7 & Robbins, F. (1987)- Perceived competence

and self-worth of l e d g disabled and nomaliy achieving students- Learnin~ Disabilitv

Quarterlv. 1 O, 37-44.

Kliewer, C-, & Biklen, D- (1996). Labelling: Who wants to be cailed retarded? In

W. Stainback & S. Stainback (Eds.), Controversial issues codinnting. mecial education:

Divergent perspectives. (RD, 83-951- Toronto: Allyn & Bacon.

Klingner, J. K, Vaughn, S., Schumm, J. S., Cohen, P., & Forgan, J. W. (1998).

Inclusion or puli-out: Which do students prefer? Jownal of Learninn Disabilities. 3 1,

148-158.

Kloomok, S., & Cosden, M. (1994). Self-concept in children with learning

disabilities: The relationship between global self-concept, academic "discounting,"

nonacademic self-concept, and perceived social support. Learning Disabilitv Ouarterlv,

17 140-153. d

Kuhlen, R G., & Lee, B. 5. (1943). Personality characteristics and socid

acceptability in adolescence- Journal of Educational Psvcholog;v. 34,321-340.

Ladd, G. W. (1983). Social networks of popular, average, and rejected children in

school settings. Merrill Palmer Ouarterlv. 29,283-307.

La Greca, A. M. (1993). Social skills training with children: Where do we go fiom

here? Journal of Clinical Child Psvcholow. 22,288-298.

La Greca, A. M., & Stone, W L (1990). LD status and achîevement:

Confomding variables in the study of children's social status, self-esteem, and behaviord

hctioning- Journal of L e h g Disabilities, 23,483-490.

Lancelotta, G. X. & Vaughn, S. (1989). Relation between types of aggression and

sociometric stahis: Peer and teacher perceptions. Journal of Educational Psycholow. 8 1,

86-90.

Landau, S., Milich, R., & Whitten, P. (1984). A cornparison of teacher and peer

assessrnent of social status. Journal of Clinical Child Psycholow. 13,4449.

Page 118: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

117

Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1485)- Naturalistic InauuVuuv Beverly Hills, CA:

Sage.

Maag, J. W., Vasa, S. F., Reid, R7 & Torrey, G. K- (1995). Social and behaviorai

predîctors of popular, rejected, and average children Educational and Psvcholoeical

Measurement, 55,196-205.

Maccoby, Et E. & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The Psvcholow of Sex Differences.

SWord, CA: S tdord University Press.

Madge, S., Afneck, J., & Lowenbraun, SS. (1990). Social effects of integrated

classrooms and resource room/regular class placements on elementary students with

learning disabilities. Journal of Leaming Disabilities. 23,439-444,

Margalit, M., Raviv, A., & Pahn-Steinmetz, N. (1988). Social cornpetence of

learning disabled children: Cognitive and emotiond aspects. The Excmtional Child. 35,

179-189.

Marsh, H. W. (1989). Age and sex effects in multiple dimensions of self-concept:

Preadolescence to early adulthood. Journal of Educational Psychologv. 8 1,4 17-430.

Masten, A., Morison, P., & Pellegrhi, D. (1985). A revised cIass play method of

peer assessment. Developmentai Psvcholoav, - - 2 1,523-533.

McKinney, J. D., & Feagans, L. (1984). Acadernic and behavioral characteristics

of learning disabled children and average achievers: Longitudinal studies. Learning

Disabilitv Ouarterly. 7,25 1-265.

McKinney, J. D., McClure, S., & Feagans, L. (1982). Classroom behavior of

learning disabled children. Learninn Disabilitv Ouarterlv. 5,4542.

McKinney, J. D., & Speece, D. L. (1 983). Classroorn behavior and the acadernic

progress of Iearning disabled students. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychoiogv. 4,

149-161.

Montgomery, M. S. (1994). Self-concept and children with leamhg disabilities:

Observer-child concordance across six context-dependent domains. Journal of Leaminq

Disabilities, 27,254-262.

Page 119: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

118

Moore, S. R-, & Simpson, R L. (1983). Teacher-pupil and peer verbal

interactions of leaming disabled, behavior-disordered, and nonhandicapped students.

Learning; Disability Quarterlv. 6,273-282-

Moreno, J. (1934). Who shall survive? A new amroach to the ~roblem of human

interrelations. Washington, DC: Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing Co.

Morison, P. & Masten, k S- (199 1). Peer reputation in middle childhood as a

predictor of adaptation in adolescence: A seven-year follow-up. Child Development. 62,

991-1007.

Nabuzoka, D., & Smith, P. K. (1993). Sociornetric status and social behavior of

children with and without learning difficulties. Journal of Child Psvcholo~y and

Psvchiatrv. 34,1435-1448.

Newcomb, A. F., Bukowski, W. M., & Pattee, L. (1993). Children's peer

relations: A meta-analytic review of populae, rejected, neglected, controversid, and

average sociometric status. Psvcholo-&al Bulletin. 1 l3,99-128.

Ochoa, S. H., & Olivarez, A. (1 995). A meta-analysis of peer rating sociometnc

studies of pupils with leaming disabilities. Journal of S~ecial Educatioa 29,149.

Ochoa, S. H., & Palmer, D. J. (1991). A sociometric analysis of between-group

ciifferences and within-group status variability of Kispank learning disabled and

nonhandicapp ed pupils in academic and play contexts. Learning Disabilitv Ouarterlv, 14,

208-218.

Offord, D. R, Boyle, M. H., & Racine, Y. A (1991). The epedemiology of

antisocial behaviours in children and adoIescents. In: D. J. Pepler & K. H. Rubin (Eds.),

The development and treatment of childhood anmession @p. 3 1-54), Hillsdale, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaurn.

Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and later persona1 adjustment:

Are low-accepted children at risk. Psychological Bulletin 102,357-3 89.

Patternon, C. J., Kupersrnidt, J. B., & Gnesler, P. C. (1990). Children's

perceptions of self and of relationships with others as a fiinction of sociomeûic status.

Page 120: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

119

Child Development, 61,1335-1349,

Pekarik, E. G., Prinz, R J., Liebert, D. E., Weintraub, S., & Neale, J. M. (1976)-

The Pupil Evaluation Inventory: A sociometric technique for assessing children's social

behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psvcholo~. 4,83-97.

Perlrnutter, B. F., Crocker, J- Cordray, D., & Garstecki, D. (1983). Sociomeû5c

status and related personality characteristics of mainstreamed leaming disabled

adolescents. Learninp; Disability Quarterly. 6,20-30.

Priel, B., & Lesham, T. (1990). Self-perceptions of first- and second-grade

children with learning disabilities. Journal of Leamhg Disabilities, 23,637-642.

Prillunan, D. (1981)- Acceptance of learning disabled students in the mainstream

environment: A failure to replicate. Journal of Learnin~ Disabilities, 14,334-346,368.

Renick, M. J., & Harter, S. (1988)- Self-Percemtion Profile for Learning Disabled

Students. Denver: University of Denver.

Renick, M. J., & Harter, S. (1989). Impact of social cornparisons on the

developing self-perceptions of learning disabled students. Journal of Educational

Psvcholonv. 8 1,63 1-63 8.

Richey, D. D., & McKinney, J. D. (1978). Classroom behavior styles of leaming

disabled boys. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 1 1,297-301.

Roff, M., Seiis, S. B., & Golden, M. W. (1972). Social adiustment and ~ersonalitv

development in children. Minneapolis: Univerisity of Minnesota Press.

Rubin, K. H., & Daniels-Beirness, T. (1983). Concurrent and predictive correlates

of sociornetnc status in kindergarten and grade one children. Merrïll-Palmer Ouarterlv,

29,337-352.

Sabomie, E. J., & Kauffinan, J. M. (1986). Social acceptance of learning disabled

adolescents. Learning; Disability Ouarterlv, 9,55-60.

Sainato, D. M., Zigmond, N., & Strain, P. S. (1983). Social status and initiations

m on interaction by learning disabled students in a regular education setting, Anahsis and

Intervention in Develo~mental Disabilities, 3,71-87.

Page 121: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

120

Sale, P. & Carey, D. M. (1995)- The sociometric status of students with

disabilities in a full-inclusion school. Excmtional Children. 62,6- 19.

Sater, G. M., & French, D. C. (1989). A cornparison of the social cornpetencies of

learning disabled and low-achieving elementary-aged children. Journal of Soecial

Education. 23,2942,

Sattler, J. M. (1992). Assessrnent of children (4th ed.). San Diego, CA: J M.

Sattler,

Schneider, B. H., Wiener, J., & Murphy, K. (1994). Children's riendships: The

giant step beyond peer acceptance. Joumal of Social and Personalitv Relationshbs. 11,

323-340.

Schumaker, J. B., Hazel, J. S., Sherman, J. A., & Sheldon, J. (1982). Social skill

pefiormances of leaming disabled, non leamïng-disabled, and delinquent adolescents.

Learning Disabilitv Ouarterlv, 5,409-414.

Scranton, T. R-, & Ryclanan, D. B. (1979). Sociometric status of learning

disabled children in an integrative program. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 12,49-54.

Shavelson, R. J., Hubner, J. J., & Staton, G. C . (1976). Self-concept: Validation of

constmct interpretations- Review of Educational Research, 46,407-441.

Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Shaywïtz, S. E. (1994). A conceptual

framework for leamhg disabilities and attention-deficithyperactivity disorder. Canadian

Journal of Special Education, 9,l-32.

Sheare, J- B. (1978). The impact of resource programs upon the self-concept and

peer acceptance of learning disabled children. Psvcholow in the Schools. 15,40642.

Siperstein, G. N., Bopp, M. J., & Bak, J. J. (1978). Social status of learning

disabled children. Journal of Learning Disabifities, 1 1.49-53.

Siperstein, G. N., & Goding, M. J. (1983). Social integration of leamuig disabled

children in regular classroorns. Advances in Learnin~ and B ehavioral Disabili ties, 2,227-

263.

Soenksen, P. A., FIagg, C. L., & Schmits, D. W. (1981). Social communication in

Page 122: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

121

learning disabled students : A pragrnatic analysis. Journal of Learninn Disabilities. 2 4,

283-286.

Stanton, W. R., Freehan, M., McGee, R, & Silva, P. A. (1990)- The relative

values of reading abïlity and IQ as predictors of teacher-reported behavior problems.

Journal of L e d g Disabilities, 23,5 14-517,

Stevens, J. (1986). Applied multivarïate statistics for the social sciences-

Hïllsdale, NJ: Erlbaum-

Stiehr-Smith, D., & Nagle, R. i, (1995). Self-perceptions ancL social cornparisons

among children with leaming disabilities. Ioumai of Learning Disabilities. 28,364-371.

Stone, W. L., & La Greca, A. M- (1990). The social status of children with

leaming disabilities: A re-examination. Journal of Leaming Disabilities. 23,32-37.

Swanson, H. L., & ~aloxie, S. (1992). Social skiils and leamÉng disabilities: A

meta-analysis of the fiterature. School Psvcholow Review. 2 1,427-443.

Terry, R., & Coie, J. D. (1991). A cornparison of methods for definhg

sociometric status among children- Develo~rnental Psvchologv. 27,867-880.

Thomdike, E- L. (1927). Intelligence and its uses. Harper's Magazine. 140,227-

235.

TUT-Kaspa, H., & Bryan, T. (1 995). Teachers' ratings of the social competence

and school adjustment of students with leaming disabilities in elementary and junior high

school. Journal of Learninn Disabilities, 28,4442.

Vau-, S., Elbaum, B. E., & Schumm, J. S. (1996). The effeçts of inclusion on

the social hctioning of students with Leamuig disabilities. Jounial o f Leaming

Disabilities, 29,598-608.

Vaughn, S., Elbaum, B. E., Schumm, J- S., &Hughes, M. T. (1998). Social

outcornes for students with and without leaming disabilities in inclusive classrooms.

Journal of Learning; Disabilities, 3 1,428436.

Vaughn, S., & Haager, D. (1994). Social competence as a rndtifaceted construct:

How do students with learning disabilities fare? Leamhg Disabilitv Ouarterlv. 17,253-

Page 123: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

122

Vaughn, S., & Hogan, A. (1 990)- Social competence and leaming disabilities: A

prospective study. In H- L. Swanson & B. K. Keogh (Eds.), Learning disabilities:

Theoretical and research issues (pp. 175-1 9 1)- Hilisdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Vaughn, S., & Hogau, A. (1994). The social competence of students with Iearning

disabilities over time: A within-individual examination. Journal of Learnin~ Disabilities,

27,292-303 - Vaughn, S., & Hogan, A., Kouzekanani, K-, & Shapiro, S. (1990). Peer

acceptance, seIf-perceptions, and social skills of learning disabled students prior to

identification. Journal of Educational Psvcholoav, 82,101-106.

Vaughn, S., McIntosh, R, Schumm, J_ S., Haager, D., & Cdlwood, D. (1993).

Social status, peer accep tance, and reciprocal fiiendships revisited. Leaming Disabilities:

Research and Practice, 8,82-88.

Vaughn, S., McIntosh, R, & Spencer-Rowe, J- (1991). Peer rejection is a

stubborn thing: Increasing peer accep tance of rejected students with learning disabilities.

Leaming Disabilities: Research and Practice, 6,83 -88-

Vaughn, S., & Schumm, J. S. (1995). Responsible inclusion for students with

learning disabilities. Journal of Learnin~ Disabilities. 28,264-270,290.

Vaughn, S., Zaragoza, N., Hogan, A., & Walker, J- (1 993). A four-year

longitudinal investigation of the social skills and behavior problems of students with

leamhg disabilities. J o u a l of Learning Disabilities. 26,404412.

Vogel, S. A. (1990). Gender differences in intelligence, language, visual-motor

abilities, and academic achievement in students with learning disabifities: A review of the

literature. Journal of Learnine Disabilities. 23,4452.

Vosk, B., Forehand, R, Parker, J I B., & Rickard, K. (1982). A multimethod

cornparison of popdar and unpopular children. Develo~mentd Psvcholom. 18,57 1-575.

Wang, M. C., Reynolds, M. C., & Walberg, H. J- (L994/1995). Serving students at

the margins. Educational Leadership, 52, 12-17.

Page 124: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

123

Wechsler, D. (1991). Manual for the Wechsler Intelli~ence Scale for Children - Third Edition. New York: Psychological Corporation.

Wiener, J. (1 987). Peer status of Iearnùig disabled children and adolescents: A

review of the literature. Learning Disabilities Research. 2,62-79.

Wiener, J., Harris, P. J., & Shirer, C. (1990). Achievement and social-behavioral

correlates of peer status in learning disabled children. Leaming Disabil& Ouarterlv. 13,

114-127.

Wiener, J., Hams, P. J., & Duval, L. (1993). Placement, identification and

subtype correlates of peer status and social behaviour of children with leaming

disabïlities. Exceptionality Education Canada 3,129-1 55.

Wiikinson, G. S- (1993). The Wide Range Achievement Test-Revision 3.

Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.

Winne, P. H., Woodlands, M- H., & Wong, B. Y. L. (2982)- Comparability of

self-concept among leaming disabled, normal and gified students. Journal of Leaming

Disabilities, 1 5,470-475.

Wong, B. Y. L., & Wong, R (1980). Role-taking skills Ui nomal achieving and

le&g disabled children. Learninn Disabilitv Quarterlv, 3,ll-18. \

Page 125: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

124

Appendix A: Sociometrics Forms

Page 126: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

125

1. This is an EXAMPLE of what you will be asked to do:

How much do you like to PLAY with J o h n ~ ?

l=not at aU

2= not much

3= pretty much

4= very much

Choose one number fiom below to show how much you like playing with

Bobby. CUcle one number.

Bobby Black

not at al1 not much pretty much very much

1 2 3 4

... DO NOT GO ON UNTIL TOLD TO

Page 127: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

126

2. Rate EACH of the CLASSMATES listed below by using this 4-number scale. Circle ONE number only for each classrnate- Rate ALL classrnates. Your answers are PRIVATE.

l=not at al1 2=not much 3=pretty much W e r y much

... HOW MUCH DO YOU LIKE TO PLAY WITH

not at al1 not much ' urettv much verv much

(names)

Did you give an Answer for each person ??

Turn the page and wait for instructions ...

Page 128: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

127

3. Here is an EXAMPLE of what you will be asked to do next.

CIRCLE the names of the THREE (3) FAMOUS PEOPLE/ CHARACTERS fkom this list that you LIKE THE MOST. Circle ONLY 3 names. Your answers are PRIVATE.

J h Carrey

Michael Jordan

Oprah W ~ e y

Bart Simpson

Pauly Shore

Madonna

CeLine Dion

Mariah Carey

Babe

Pocahontas

Roseanne

Whoopee Goldberg

Michael Jackson

Alanis Momsette

Robin Williams

Jocelyn Thibeault

4. CIRCLE the names of the THREE (3) CLASSMATES fkom this Iist that you LIKE THE MOST. Circle ONLY 3 names. Your answers are PRIVATE.

- STOP -

CHECK YOUR WORK - TWEN SLIDE YOUR PAPERS IN YOUR ENVELOPE.

DO NOT LICK THE ENVELOPE.

Page 129: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

128

Appendùr B: Revised Class Play Rating Questionnaire:

a) Peer-rater Fonn

b) Target Form

Page 130: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

129

Revised Class PIay Rating-Peer

a) Please read each of the following 35 statements and decide how much you agred disagree with what it says. Remember to think about the person you are rating very carefùlly when making your choices. Do not put your name on this questionnaire. Ali of your answers are private and no one in this school wiil ever know how you answered these questions. Please answer as honestly as possible and do not skip any items. Let's do an example to start,

Il Example: "My (da& mom, sister, brother) is good at playing video games"

1 2 3 4 5 VerY a Little almost a lïttle VerY false false even true tme

very a iittle almost a iittle very Items: false false even true true

1- - is a good Ieader

2, Everyone likes to be with

3- - has many fiiends

4- - has a good sense of humour

5. Everyone listens to

6- - has good ideas for things to do

7- - makes new fiends easiiy

8- - is someone you c m trust

9- - helps other people when they need it

10- can get things going

I l . plays fair

12, is poiïte

13. Wces to pIay with others rather than alone

14. is usualiy happy

15. waits hisher turn

Page 131: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

130

very alittle almost alittle very Items: false fdse even true true

16. picks on other kids

17. is too bossy

18- teases other kids too much

19. gets mto a lot of fights

20. Ioses temper e a d y

21. shows off a lot

22. intemipts when other children are speaking

23. acts Iike a little kid (immature)

24. is often left out

25- feelings' get easily hurt

26. is usually sad

27. would rather play alone than with others

28. has trouble making fnends

29 - can't get others to listen

30. is very shy

31. is good looking

32. is more intelligent than others his/her age

33- has a Iot of neat s t u f f

34. does very well in school work

35. is better than others hisher age in sports

In your own words, please tell me why you chose as someone you like to play witb the most What

is it about them that you Iike?

Page 132: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

13 1

Revised Class Play Rating-Target

b) Please read each of the followuig 35 statements and decide how much you agreddisagree with what it says. Think very carefiilly about how you feel about yourself in each of these areas and then make your choice. Do not put your name on this questionnaire. AU of your answers are private and no one in this school will ever know how you answered these questions. Please answer as honestIy as possible and do not skip any items. Let's do an example to starl.

Example: "1 am good at playing video games"

1 2 3 4 5 VerY a little almost a Little VerY

false fdse even true tme

Items: very a IittIe almost a litde very false false even true true

1.1 am a good leader

2, Everyone &es to be with m e

3- 1 have many firiends

4-1 have a good sense of humour

5. Everyone listens to me

6.1 have good ideas for things to do

7.1 make new fnends easily

8.1 am someone you can trust

9- 1 help other people when they need it

10.1 cm get things going

1 1.1 play fair

12.1 am polite

13.1 like to play with others rather than alone

14.1 am usuaUy happy

15.1 wait my tum

Page 133: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

132

very a Iittle almost a Little very Items: false fdse even tme Inie

16-1 pick on other kids

17.1 am too bossy

18.1 tease other kids too much

19.1 get into a lot of fights

20- 1 Iose my temper easily

21. I show off a lot

22. L intermpt when other children are speaking

23. I act like a little kid (immature)

24.1 am often lefi out

25, My feelings get easily hurt

26, I am usuaIiy sad

27, I would rather play alone than with others

28-1 have trouble making fiiends

29.1 can't get others to ïisten

30.1 am very shy

3 1.1 am good looking

32.1 am more intelligent than others my age

33.1 have a lot of neat sNfi

34. I do very weii in school work

35.1 am better than others my age in sports

In your own words, please tell me why you think you were chosen as someone many kids Iïke to play with

What is it about you that other kids me?

Page 134: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

133

Appendùc C: Consent Forrns

a) Screening Phase

b) Peer-raters (P)

c) Target Group (T)

Page 135: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

McGill Social Cornpetence

134

Page 136: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

135

Dear Parent(s) or Guardian(s),

As you are already aware, your soddaughter participated in the McGill University/ Faculty of Education study at hidher school this past year- My name may sound farniliar to you because 1 am Dr. Nancy Heath's graduate student who has been, and will continue tu be, the Research Director for the McGiii University study in which your chiid is involved As a follow-up to the phone c d which you will receive or have received fiom either Dr. Nancy Heath or myself, here is a brief description of my study and a request for consent regarding participation- Please ask your child to retum the attached consent fonn (once you have signed it) to hidher teacher as soon as possible.

While the individual results for your son/daughter cannot be provided to you, 1 am able to tell you that yow soddaughter has indicated that there is a certain child in hismer grade that hdshe particularly Iikes as a fiend, E wodd like your child to fill-out a quesüonnalre that will help me better understand why he/she has chosm this child as a prefmed playmate. As part of the larger project currently underway with myself and Dr, Heath, 1 a m particularly interested in why some children are weil-liked and accepted by their peers- Unhappily, much of the research conducted in schools today focuses on the problems or the failures experienced by children, My research project is a welcomed change to this trend in that 1 am interested in what children are doing weil and where they are succeeding, rather than where are they failing. Because your child has rated certain of hisher peers as chifdren he/she particularly likes to play wiîh, 1 am w-riting to you to request your and your child's pennission to briefly meet wiîh your child to leam more about why he/she likes certain studeats ia his class.

What is involved? If you gant your childs' permission to participate in m y study, your child will be asked to complete ONE short questionnaire (35 items) pertaining to the classrnate with which your soddaughter has indicated he/she mes to play. Your chiid will be asked to circle a number fÏom one ("very false") to five ("very tme") indicating how much helshe agrees with each of the 35 statements (sample statements are: " has a good sense of humour?" or "- helps other people when they need it?"). Finally, your child wiU be asked one last question where hefshe gets a chance to explain m their own words why they like to play with the classrnate in question.

The entire session with your child will take approximately 15-20 minutes and wiU be conducted by either rnyselfor a trained doctoral student in a private area at your child's school- The session will be audiotaped to ensure accurate recording of your chdd's responses, There are no nght or wrong answet-s to these questions, rather the questions sirnply ask your child to express hidher thoughts concerning these fnendship related topics.

Scheduling of the session will be done in agreement with your child's teacher. Confidentiality of a l i information gathered from your child remains assure& Your child's responses will be identified by a code number, and not by name, on rnaterial associated with the study. Your child will be fiee to withdraw hisher participation fiom the study at any time.

1 would very much appreciate your child's participation in this activity in order to help me, and others, better understand the nature of fnendships and social acceptance in children- If you have any questions or concerns please do not hesitate to c d me at 845-2170 and 1 will return your c d within 24 hours, Please complete the consent form below. Than . you!

SincereIy, Andrea Brown, M.Sc. McGill University Doctoral Student

Page 137: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

136

As you are already aware, your soddaughter participated in the McGiU University/ Faculty of Education study at hidha schoo1 this past year. My name may sound famiiiar to you because 1 am Dr. Nancy Heath's graduate student who has been, and will continue to be, the Research Director for the McGiIi University study in which your child is involved, As a foUow-up to the phone c d which you will receive or have received fiom either Dr. Nancy Heath or myself, here is a brief description of my study and a request for consent regarding participation, Please ask your chiId to retuni the attached consent form (once you have signed it) to his/her teacher as soon as possible.

While the individual results for your soddaughter cannot be provided to you, 1 am able to tell you that your sorddaughter has been chosen by some of his /ha classmates as someone many children me to play with, As part of the larger project currentiy underway with myseIf and Dr, Heath, 1 am particularly interested in why some children are weII-liked and accepted by their peers. Unhappily, much of the research conducted in schools today focuses on the problems or the failures experienced by children, My research project is a welcomed change to this trend in that 1 am interested in what children are doing well and where they are succeeding, rathm than where are they failing- Because your child has been rated by his peers as someone that is experiencing social success, 1 would like to get a better understanding of how your cMd perceives hisher own social skills and how your child's peers perceive hidher social abilities.

M a t is involved? If you grant your child's permission to participate in my study, your child will be asked to complete ONE short questionnaire (35 items) about his/her self-perceptions in various areas of social fiuictioning. Your chiid will be asked to circie a number fkom one ("very fdse") to five ("very îrue") indicating how much hekhe agrees with each of the 35 statements (sample statements are: "1 have a good sense of humour?" or "1 help other people when they need it?"). Your chiId will be asked one last question where he/she gets a chance to explain in heir own words why they think other classmates picked t'hem as someone fun to play with Finally, permission is also requested in having two of your child's classmates (those who have indicated that they like to play with your child) complete a similar questionnaire that your child wili £ill out, with the only change being that the "I" will be replaced by your child's name. Peer raters wiil be asked to think about your child and histher social skills as they complete the brief questionnaire-

Scheduling of the session will be doue in agreement with your child's teacher. The entire session with your chiid wiU take approximately 15-20 minutes and will be conducted by either myselfor a trained doctoral student in a pnvate area at your child's school. The session will be audiotaped to ensure accurate recording of your child's responses. There are no right or wrong answers to these questions, rather the questions sirnply ask your child to express m e r thoughts concerning these. fiiendship related topics.

Confldentiality of al1 information gathered corn your child and the peer raters remains assured- Ail responses obtained fkom the participants wiU be identified by a code number, and not by name, on the material associated with the study. Your child will be kee to withdraw M e r participation fiom the study at any time.

1 would very much appreciate your child's participation in this activity in order to help me, and others, better understand the nature of fiiendships and social acceptance in children. Eyou have any questions or concerm pIease do not hesitate to call me at 845-2170 and 1 will return your call within 24 hours- Please complete the consent form below. Thank you!

Sincerely, Andrea Brown, M.Sc, McGiil University Doctoral Student

Page 138: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

137

Appendix D: Sociometrics Distracter Tasks

Page 139: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

5. Rate EACH of the ACTMTIES Iisted below by using this 4-number scale. Circle ONE nurnber only for each activity. Rate ALL activities.

HOW MUCH DO YOU LIKE TO DO THE FOLLOWING:

not at al1 not much arettv much verv much

Riding Bikes

Playing vide0 games in arcades

Reading

Watching TV

Talking on phone to fiends

Playing Hackie-Sack

Watching movies

Playing computer games

Staying &et school for sports

Skateboarding

Snowboarding

Spendùig time on the Internet

Goin% fiien s t" house

Spending time alone

Shopping

Pla g games WI X" friends

Music Lessons

Page 140: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

139

6. CIRCLE the names of the THREE (3) PLAY ACTIVITIES fkom this list that you LIKE THE MOST. Circle ONLY 3 activities. Your answers are PRIVATE.

Riding Bikes

Playing video games in arcades

Reading

Watching TV

Talking on phone to Eends

Playing Hackie-Sack

Watching movies

Playing cornputer games

Staying after school for sports

S kateboardhg

Snowboarding

Spending t h e on the Internet

In-line skating

Going to fiiends house

Spending time alone

Shopping

Playing games with fiends

Music Lessons

- STOP EERE - PLEASE CHECK YOUR WORK AND PUT YOUR

PAPERS IN THE ENVELOPE. THANK YOU!

Page 141: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

140

Appendix E: Source Table for RCPR: Peer

Repeated Measures MANOVA

Page 142: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

141

Source Df - F - Wilks' Lambda

Between ~ d c i p a n t s

Questionnaire Factor (QF)

QF x Gender

QF x LD Status

QF x LD Status x Gender

Within Participants

QF x Peer

QF x Peer x Gender

QF x Peer x LD Status

QF x Peer x Gender x LD Status

Page 143: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Competence

142

Appendix FI Breakdown of Peer-pleasing versus Teacher-pleasing items on the Social

Skills factor of the Social Skius Rating System-Teacher Form (SSRS-T)

Page 144: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

Social Cornpetence

Peer-Pleasing Behaviours Teacher-Pleasing B ehaviours

1. Controls temper in conflict situation with

peers

2. htroduces herself or himself to new people

without being told

4. Compromises in confIict situation

5- Responds approprÏately to peer pressure

6. Says nice things about hirnself or herseif

7. Invites others to join in activities

10, Makes firiends easily

1 1. Responds appropriately to teasing by peers

14. Initiates conversations with peers

18, Accepts peers' ideas for group activities

19, Gives compliments to peers

24- Joins ongoing activity or group without

being told to do so

25. Responds appropriately when pushed or

hit by other children

30- Gets along with people who are différent

3, Appropnately questions d e s that may be

d a i r

8, Uses fkee time in an appropriate manner

9. Finishes class assignments within tirne

limits

12- Controls temper in conflict situations with

adults

13. Receives crïticism well

15. Uses time appropriately while waiting for

help

16. Produces correct school work

17. Appropriately tells you when thinking he

or she was treated unfairly

20, Foilows your directions

2 2 , Puts school materiaIs properly away

22. Cooperates with peers without prornpting

23. Volunteers to help peers with ckssroom

tasks

26- Ignores peer distractions when doing class

work

27. Keeps desk clean and neat without being

reminded

28, Attends to your .instructions

29, Easily makes transitions

Note, Numbw refers to the actual item number fkom the SSRS-T (Gresham & Elliott, 1990)

Page 145: Head: SOCIAL COMPETENCEcollectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape8/PQDD_0017/... · 2005-02-10 · Social Cornpetence 2 Acknowledgements 1 would iike to achowledge and express my

MCGiLL UNIVERSIN FACULTY OF EDUCATION

A review commit& consisting of three of the following members:

1, Prof. E. Lusthaus 1. Prof. M. ~aguire

2, Prof, R Ghosh 2. Prof. G. Isherwood

3- Prof. M. Downey 3. Prof- R. Tumtte

has examined the application for certification of the ethical acceptability of the project nmemplary fea -ues of s o c i u y accepted chLl*en e t h l e m i n g disabil i tg '

The review cornmittee considers the research procedures as explained by the applicant in this application. to be acceptable on ethical grounds,

(Signatures)

March Y996