18
http://cjs.sagepub.com/ School Psychology Canadian Journal of http://cjs.sagepub.com/content/27/3/201 The online version of this article can be found at: DOI: 10.1177/0829573512450567 2012 2012 27: 201 originally published online 20 July Canadian Journal of School Psychology M. Catherine Cappadocia, Debra Pepler, Joanne G. Cummings and Wendy Craig Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com On behalf of: Canadian Association of School Psychologists can be found at: Canadian Journal of School Psychology Additional services and information for http://cjs.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts: http://cjs.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Permissions: http://cjs.sagepub.com/content/27/3/201.refs.html Citations: at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014 cjs.sagepub.com Downloaded from at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014 cjs.sagepub.com Downloaded from

Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

  • Upload
    w

  • View
    213

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

http://cjs.sagepub.com/School PsychologyCanadian Journal of

http://cjs.sagepub.com/content/27/3/201The online version of this article can be found at:

 DOI: 10.1177/0829573512450567

2012 2012 27: 201 originally published online 20 JulyCanadian Journal of School Psychology

M. Catherine Cappadocia, Debra Pepler, Joanne G. Cummings and Wendy CraigIntervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander  

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of: 

  Canadian Association of School Psychologists

can be found at:Canadian Journal of School PsychologyAdditional services and information for    

  http://cjs.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

 

http://cjs.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:  

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:  

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:  

http://cjs.sagepub.com/content/27/3/201.refs.htmlCitations:  

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 2: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

What is This? 

- Jul 20, 2012OnlineFirst Version of Record  

- Aug 28, 2012Version of Record >>

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 3: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

Canadian Journal of School Psychology27(3) 201 –216

© 2012 SAGE PublicationsReprints and permission:

sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navDOI: 10.1177/0829573512450567

http://cjs.sagepub.com

450567 CJS27310.1177/0829573512450567Cappadocia et al.Canadian Journal of School Psychology

1York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada2Queen’s University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada

Corresponding Author:M. Catherine Cappadocia, LaMarsh Centre for Child and Youth Research, York University, 4700 Keele Street, TEL 5022, Toronto, ON, Canada, M3J 1P3, Canada Email: [email protected]

Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

M. Catherine Cappadocia1, Debra Pepler1, Joanne G. Cummings1, and Wendy Craig2

Abstract

The aim of the current study was to explore bystander experiences during bullying episodes among children and youth attending a residential summer camp by investigating rates of witnessing and intervention, as well as individual motivations and characteristics associated with bystander intervention. The majority of children had witnessed bullying at least once in the past 3 weeks and reported intervening in some way. Among children who reported intervening, the strongest motivation appeared to be a sense of social justice. Among children who reported not intervening, the strongest motivation appeared to be the feeling that it was not their place to intervene because the bullying situation did not directly involve them and/or was not extremely severe. The determining factors for bystander intervention during bullying episodes differed between genders. Social self-efficacy predicted bystander intervention among girls, while empathy and attitudes about bullying predicted bystander intervention among boys. Implications for bullying prevention are discussed.

Résumé

Cette recherche visait à étudier l’expérience des témoins d’actes d’intimidation survenus entre enfants et jeunes résidents d’un camp d’été. Nous avons comparé le

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 4: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

202 Canadian Journal of School Psychology 27(3)

taux de témoins qui s’interposent et ceux qui demeurent inactifs face à l’intimidation et étudié les motivations et les caractéristiques du témoin qui s’implique. La plupart des enfants affirmaient avoir assisté à au moins un acte d’intimidation au cours des trois dernières semaines. La justice sociale semble la principale motivation des enfants qui se sont interposés; ceux qui n’ont pas réagi ont eu le sentiment de ne pas être directement concernés ou que l’intimidation était sans gravité. On note une différence entre les garçons et les filles qui interviennent; les plus susceptibles de s’interposer sont les filles ayant des compétences sociales affirmées et les garçons ayant de l’empathie et qui refusent l’intimidation. Nous examinons l’impact de ces découvertes au regard de nos programmes de prévention.

Keywords

bullying, peer victimization, bystander, intervention

Bullying is common among Canadian children and associated with many negative outcomes. Roughly one third of children report each of bullying and victimization and most students in middle and high school (63%-73%) report witnessing bullying epi-sodes (Molcho et al., 2009; Oh & Hazler, 2009; Rivers, Poteat, Noret, & Ashurst, 2009; Salmivalli, Lagerspetz, Björkqvist, Österman, & Kaukiainen, 1996). Bullying can take many forms including physical (e.g., hitting), verbal (e.g., name calling), and social (e.g., gossiping; PREVNet Assessment Working Group, 2008). Among school-aged children, verbal and social forms are most common (Scheithauer, Hayer, Petermann, & Jugert, 2006; Woods & White, 2005). Peer victimization is associated with internalizing and externalizing mental health problems, social problems, aca-demic difficulties, and school avoidance (Delfabbro et al., 2006; Forero, McLellan, Rissel, & Bauman, 1999; Mitchell, Ybarra, & Finkelhor, 2007; Nansel et al., 2001; Ybarra & Mitchell, 2004, 2007).

Peer interventions are very effective during bullying episodes. Bystanders have the power to either stop or prolong bullying episodes (Craig & Pepler, 1995, 1997; Craig, Pepler, & Blais, 2007; O’Connell, Pepler, & Craig, 1999; Salmivalli et al., 1996). The more peers who gather to watch a bullying episode, the longer the episode tends to last, as the child who is bullying is reinforced by the bystanders’ attention (O’Connell et al., 1999). Observational research indicates that when bystanders stand up for the child being victimized, these peer interventions immediately stop the bullying over 50% of the time (Hawkins, Pepler, & Craig, 2001). Similarly, when victimized stu-dents report that peers intervened during a bullying episode, they rate the interventions as successful in stopping bullying almost half the time (Fekkes, Pijpers, & Verloove-Vanhorick, 2005).

Although children tend to want to help victimized peers during bullying episodes, they typically do not take action to defend the child being victimized. Most students hold antibullying and provictim attitudes as well as the belief that they would defend

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 5: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

Cappadocia et al. 203

peers if/when witnessing bullying; however, only 17% to 46% of students report having actually intervened during a bullying episode (Nickerson, Mele, & Princiotta, 2008; Poyhonen & Salmivalli, 2008; Rigby & Johnson, 2006; Salmivalli et al., 1996). Helpful bystander behaviours tend to decline and proaggression attitudes tend to increase with age (Endresen & Olweus, 2001; Rogers & Tisak, 1996; Stevens, Van Oost, & de Bourdeaudhuij, 2000; Tisak & Tisak, 1996). Salmivalli and colleagues (1996) have found that most children play one of four participant roles during bullying episodes, in addition to the roles of bully and victim: assistant to the bully, reinforcer of the bully, outsider, and defender of the victim. These researchers found that among middle school students, the distribution of roles was as follows: 20% to 30% were assistants or reinforcers, 26% to 30% were outsiders, and about 20% were defenders (Salmivalli, Lappalainen, & Lagerspetz, 1998).

There is little research on individual motivations for bystander intervention during bullying episodes among children. There has been some speculation among research-ers, however, regarding motivations for failing to intervene. Hazler (1996) asserted that passive bystanders tend to refrain from intervening because they are not sure what to do, they are afraid of becoming the focus of the child who is bullying, or they are afraid they will make the situation worse rather than better. Salmivalli (2010) has sug-gested that lack of intervention during bullying episodes may result from the “classical bystander effect,” whereby bystanders are less likely to intervene if other individuals are also witnessing the potentially harmful situation (Darley & Latané, 1968; Salmivalli, 2010). In the case of bullying among children, bystanders may feel that the presence of others absolves them from being responsible for intervening (i.e., “diffu-sion of responsibility” effect; Darley and Latané, 1968) or they may view the lack of action from others as a sign that intervention is unnecessary (Salmivalli, 2010). It may also be that when peers do not intervene, this modeling creates a group norm that would be difficult for some children to violate.

A number of individual characteristics have recently been identified as potential correlates and predictors of bystander intervention (or lack thereof) among youth. These constructs include empathy, social self-efficacy, and attitudes about bullying and victimization (Gini, Albiero, Benelli, & Altoe, 2008; Nickerson et al., 2008; Stevens et al., 2000). Empathy, also referred to as empathetic concern, refers to one’s ability to emotionally respond with feelings of concern for another and a desire to alleviate that person’s distress (Davis, 1983; Nickerson et al., 2008; Olweus & Endresen, 1998). Social self-efficacy refers to an individual’s belief in his or her abil-ity to influence social relationships (Bandura, 1997; Gini et al., 2008). Attitudes about bullying refer to one’s sympathy (or lack thereof) for children who bully and one’s opinion (or lack thereof) that victimized children are weak. Conversely, attitudes about victimization refer to one’s empathy and supportive feelings toward children who are victimized (Andreou, Vlachou, & Didaskalou, 2005; Stevens et al., 2000). Although all of these characteristics have been found to be associated with bystander behaviour, they have not been investigated together to examine their relative (and potentially cumulative) contributions to bystander intervention among children.

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 6: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

204 Canadian Journal of School Psychology 27(3)

The aim of the current study was to investigate bystander experiences, characteris-tics, and motivations during bullying episodes among children and youth. The first goal was to examine rates of witnessing for physical, verbal, and social forms of bul-lying. The second goal was to investigate bystander intervention rates and motivations during bullying episodes. The third goal was to explore the relative contributions of individual characteristics to bystander intervention behaviours among children and youth. There is a need for more research to help us understand what facilitates and hinders intervention during bullying episodes among children and youth to guide the development of bullying prevention programs that effectively support children to intervene.

MethodParticipants

Participants included 108 children and youth ranging from 8 to 16 years old attending an overnight summer camp in Ontario, Canada (44 girls and 64 boys; mean age = 12.5 years, SD = 1.97 years). This summer camp has explicit policies about bullying as well as comprehensive bullying prevention and intervention education and program-ming for all staff, counsellors, and campers. With respect to bullying prevention, camp counsellors and staff attend an experiential precamp training workshop that focuses on raising awareness, understanding, and empathy regarding bullying, with a concentration on gender differences as well as prevention and intervention tools. The topics of bullying and social safety are also discussed frequently with the campers and confidential safety questionnaires are administered to campers during each camp ses-sion to ensure that everyone feels safe at camp. With respect to intervention, a step-wise protocol is in place that begins with soft “no-blame” relationship counselling and progresses (when necessary) through various consequences (e.g., loss of privileges).

Consent forms describing the current research project were sent to parents before their children attended camp. Children required active parental consent, as well as their own assent, to be eligible to participate in the current research. Every child with a returned parent consent form (regardless of whether or not the parent consented) was entered into a raffle for an iPod. Researchers visited the camp to administer the ques-tionnaires to eligible children 3 weeks after the camp session had started. Due to the sensitive nature of the research, we included a question at the end of the package invit-ing children to indicate whether they would like to talk about issues raised in the questionnaires (Yuile, Pepler, Craig, & Connolly, 2006). Any questionnaire indicating that participants wanted to speak further about their bullying experiences with an adult was to be flagged and passed on to the camp director for follow-up.

MeasuresPromoting Relationships and Eliminating Violence Network Assessment Tool—Child/

Adolescent Version. The PREVNet tool assesses bullying, victimization, and bystander

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 7: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

Cappadocia et al. 205

experiences among children and youth (PREVNet tool; PREVNet Assessment Work-ing Group, 2008). The sections pertaining to bystander experiences were used for this study to assess rates of witnessing and motivations for intervening (or not interven-ing). The survey provides age appropriate definitions of each form of bullying (physi-cal, verbal, and social). Physical bullying is defined as “when someone hits, shoves, kicks, spits at, or beats up on others.” Verbal bullying is defined as “using words to call someone names, make fun of them, or tease them in a mean way, humiliate some-one or make them feel stupid, or threaten someone.” Social bullying is defined as “not letting someone join the group, gossiping or spreading rumours about someone or making sure others don’t make friends with the person.” Cyber bullying was not included because there are strictly enforced camp rules preventing the possession and use of communication devices at camp.

Children are asked to indicate how often they have witnessed each form of bullying in the past month by choosing one of five possible responses: never (0), once or twice (1), two or three times (2), once per week (3), and several times per week (4). In the current study, the time frame was “since camp started,” which was 3 weeks prior. Children are also asked the following question: “Think of the last time you saw or heard someone being bullied. Did you try to stop it?” For each answer option (i.e., yes or no), a list of potential reasons for the decision is provided and children are asked to check off all that apply as well as add any additional reasons not listed. Potential rea-sons for intervening (i.e., answering yes) include, “It was not fair,” “I wanted to help,” and “No one deserves to be bullied.” Potential reasons for not intervening (i.e., answer-ing no) include, “I didn’t want to get involved,” “I was afraid,” “It wasn’t my prob-lem,” and “I was worried I would get bullied next.”

Empathetic Responsiveness Questionnaire (ERQ). This scale is a self-report question-naire for children and youth that assesses empathy regarding bullying and victimiza-tion (Olweus & Endresen, 1998). The ERQ includes 12 items and three subscales: Empathic Concern for Girls (e.g., “When I see a girl who is hurt, I wish to help her,” “Seeing a girl who can’t find anyone to be with makes me feel sorry for her”), Empathic Concern for Boys (e.g., “When I see a boy who is distressed I sometimes feel like cry-ing,” “Seeing a boy who is sad makes me want to comfort him”), and Empathic Dis-tress (e.g., “It often makes me distressed when I see something sad on TV,” “Sometimes I feel a bit distressed when I read or hear about something sad”). Responses are indi-cated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from not true (1) to very true (5). In the current study, a mean score was computed to create an overall empathy score, with higher scores indicating more empathy. Studies have indicated that the ERQ overall mean score has high internal consistency (alphas range from .92 to .95; Manger et al., 2001; Olweus & Endresen, 1998).

Social self-efficacy Scale. This scale measures children’s perceptions of their com-petence and assertiveness during social situations (Pastorelli, Caprara, & Bandura, 1998). The scale includes seven items (e.g., “How easy is it for you to say what you think, even if your friends do not agree with you?,” “How easy is it for you to express your personal opinions in a group?”) and responses are indicated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from not able/easy at all (1) to very able/easy (5). In the

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 8: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

206 Canadian Journal of School Psychology 27(3)

current study, a mean score was calculated to index overall self-efficacy, with higher scores indicating higher levels of perceived self-efficacy. In past studies, internal con-sistency of the scale was adequate (α = .72; Pastorelli et al., 1998).

Probully and Provictim Scales. These scales index children’s attitudes about bullying and victimization (Stevens et al., 2000). The Probully scale includes nine items that reflect sympathy and understanding for children who bully (e.g., “It is fun to encour-age bullying”) and an outlook that children who are victimized are weak (e.g., “Kids who are picked on annoy others”). The Provictim scale includes seven items that reflect empathy toward children who are victimized (e.g., “I’m upset when another kid is being bullied”) and an outlook that these children deserve to be supported (e.g., “Kids who intervene in bullying incidents are brave”). Responses are provided via a 5-point Likert-type scale that ranges from totally disagree (1) to unsure (3) to totally agree (5). In the current study, responses for the nine items in the Probully scale were reverse coded, that is, responses ranged from totally disagree (5) to totally agree (1), such that higher scores represented lower levels of probully attitudes. Mean scores were then calculated for the Probully and Provictim scales, with higher scores indicat-ing more negative (i.e., lower levels of) probully attitudes and higher levels of provic-tim attitudes, respectively. Previous research indicates that internal consistency is adequate for both the probully and provictim scales (α = .71 and .69, respectively; Andreou et al., 2005).

ResultsRates of Bystander Experiences

Social bullying was most common, witnessed by about 67% of children at least once in the past 3 weeks and 23% of children several times per week. Verbal bullying was also prevalent, witnessed by about 62% of children at least once and 18% of children several times per week. Least common was physical bullying, which was witnessed by about 26% of children at least once and 2% of children two to three times per week. Frequencies for each form of bullying witnessed at camp are presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Frequencies of Physical, Verbal, and Social Forms of Bullying Witnessed in the Past 3 Weeks

Frequency (%)

Form of bullying Never Once 2-3 times Once/week Several times/week

Physical 74 18 5 1 2Verbal 38 17 16 11 18Social 33 20 19 5 23

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 9: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

Cappadocia et al. 207

Bystander Intervention Rates and Motivations

When asked whether they intervened during the last bullying episode they witnessed, about 80% of the children reported that they did intervene, while 20% reported they did not. Intervention rates were not associated with child age (r = –.17, p = .09) and did not differ between boys and girls, (χ2(1) = 2.03, df = 1, p = .15).

Children who reported intervening were provided with a list of potential reasons for their decision and asked to check off all that applied. Most (68%) agreed with the statement that no one deserves to be bullied and 50% agreed with the statement that bullying was not fair. In addition, 50% reported that they wanted to help and 45% reported that the person (i.e., the child being victimized) needed help. Endorsement rates for the listed potential reasons for intervening are presented in Table 2.

Children who reported not intervening were also provided with a list of potential reasons for their decision and asked to check off all that applied. Most (57%) of these children indicated they did not want to get involved and a roughly one quarter (24%) reported that they were afraid. Endorsement rates for the listed potential reasons for not intervening are presented in Table 3.

Table 2. Endorsement Rates for Reasons for Intervening during a Bullying Episode

Reasons for intervening Endorsement rate (%)

No one deserves to be bullied 54It was not fair 39I wanted to help 39The person needed help 36Stopping bullying is everyone’s responsibility 29I wanted to make a difference 22

Table 3. Endorsement Rates for Reasons for Not Intervening during a Bullying Episode

Reasons for not intervening Endorsement rate (%)

I didn’t want to get involved 57I was afraid 24I didn’t know what to do 20The bullying wasn’t so bad 20Not my business/problem 20The kid being bullied deserved it 15I didn’t want to get in trouble for telling 15It wouldn’t have made a difference 15Nobody would do anything if I told someone 5I was worried I would get bullied next 5

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 10: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

208 Canadian Journal of School Psychology 27(3)

Predictors of Bystander Intervention by Gender

Binary logistic regression analyses were conducted to examine the prediction of bystander intervention by age, empathy, social self-efficacy, and attitudes about bul-lying and victimization. Descriptive statistics for the scales used to index these vari-ables are provided in Table 4. The regressions were conducted separately for each gender because with respect to proportion of aggressive behaviours, girls and boys tend to engage in different forms of bullying (covert and overt, respectively) most frequently (Card, Stucky, Sawalani, & Little, 2008). All of the variables were entered simultaneously into one model for each gender. The models were significant for both girls, (χ2 = 11.89, df = 5, p < .05), and boys, (χ2 = 25.30, df = 5, p < .001). Regression results are provided in Table 5.

Girls who reported high levels of social self-efficacy were 32 times more likely than other girls to report that they had intervened during the last bullying episode they witnessed. When the roles of social self-efficacy, empathy, and attitude about bullying were all considered simultaneously, empathy and attitudes toward bullying were not significant in predicting intervention. Boys who reported high levels of empathy were about 17 times more likely than other boys to report that they had intervened during the last bullying episode they witnessed, and boys who reported low levels of probully attitudes were about 8 times more likely to report that they had intervened. When all

Table 4. Descriptive Statistics for Regression Predictor Variable Scales

Predictor variable (Scale) N Minimum value Maximum value Mean SD

Empathy (ERQ) 108 2.58 5.00 3.82 0.56Social Self-Efficacy (Social Self-

Efficacy Scale)108 2.43 5.00 3.89 0.60

Probully Attitudes (Probully Scale reverse coded)

108 1.56 4.78 4.00 0.49

Provictim Attitudes (Provictim Scale) 107 2.57 5.00 3.93 0.54

Table 5. Binomial Logistic Regression Results for Predictors of Bullying Bystander Intervention for Girls and Boys

Girls (Boys)

Predictors B SE Wald p Exp(B)

Age –.55 (–.49) .36 (.23) 2.38 (4.79) .12 (.03) .58 (.61)Empathy .30 (2.82) 1.41 (1.09) .05 (6.77) .83 (.01) 1.35 (16.83)Social Self-Efficacy 3.48 (1.19) 1.43 (.72) 5.91 (2.77) .02 (.10) 32.57 (3.30)Lower levels of Probully Attitude 1.52 (2.10) 1.42 (.96) 1.15 (4.81) .28 (.03) 4.57 (8.17)Provictim Attitude –.92 (–.38) 1.34 (.85) .47 (.20) .49 (.66) .40 (.69)

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 11: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

Cappadocia et al. 209

three characteristics were considered simultaneously, the level of boys’ social self-efficacy was not significant in predicting intervention.

DiscussionThe majority of children had witnessed bullying at least once in the past 3 weeks, and reported intervening in some way. Among children who reported intervening, the strongest motivation appeared to be a sense of social justice. Among children who reported not intervening, the strongest motivation appeared to be the feeling that it was not their place to intervene because the bullying situation did not directly involve them and/or was not extremely severe. The determining factors for bystander inter-vention during bullying episodes differed between genders. Social self-efficacy pre-dicted bystander intervention among girls, while empathy and attitudes about bullying predicted bystander intervention among boys.

Almost 70% of the children reported witnessing at least one form of bullying at least once in the past 3 weeks. This rate is higher than those typically found in the bul-lying literature (Molcho et al., 2009). This may reflect the large amount of time spent with peers when attending overnight camp. At the time when this research was con-ducted, the children had been together (and observing peer interactions) all day, every day, over the past few weeks. Consistent with the literature, verbal and social forms of bullying were more common than physical forms, with more than 60% of children witnessing each (Craig & Pepler, 1997; Scheithauer et al., 2006; Woods & White, 2005). Verbal and social forms of bullying are predominant as children become older and realize that these forms are equally or more effective than physical forms for hurt-ing others (Rivers & Smith, 1994; Woods & White, 2005). Verbal and social forms of bullying are also common because they are more likely to escape detection from adults than physical forms (Sutton, Smith, & Swettenham, 1999).

The rate of bystander intervention during bullying episodes (80%) was also high within this sample of children. Previous research studies using observational data, self-report surveys, and peer nomination procedures have all indicated peer interven-tion rates of 17% to 25% during bullying episodes (Hawkins et al., 2001; O’Connell et al., 1999; Salmivalli et al., 1998). The high rate of intervention in the current study (via self-report) is likely attributable to the particular camp setting, which has imple-mented a well-established bullying prevention program for more than 10 years. In addition, there has been considerable attention and strong messaging aimed at chil-dren and youth to stand up to bullying in the past 10 years. Thus this high rate of intervention may also reflect a historical effect, since children in the present study have likely received more education about bullying and may feel a greater sense of social responsibility to intervene when compared to children in previous studies con-ducted a decade ago.

Among the children who reported intervening when witnessing bullying, it appears that a sense of social justice is the strongest motive for standing up for others. When children who reported intervening were asked to indicate the reason(s) for their deci-sion, most reported that they intervened because no one deserves to be bullied and/or

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 12: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

210 Canadian Journal of School Psychology 27(3)

bullying is not fair. The instinct to help others in distress also appears to be a powerful motivator, as many children reported they intervened because they wanted to help the child being victimized and/or that the child being victimized needed help.

Among the children who reported not intervening when they witnessed bullying, most reported that they did not act because they felt it was not their place because the bullying situation did not directly involve them and/or was not extremely severe. This finding speaks to the relevance of the classic bystander effect for bullying among chil-dren (Salmivalli, 2010). Many of these children also indicated that they did not inter-vene because they were afraid. The fear of standing up, and perhaps consequently becoming targeted or voicing a potentially unpopular opinion, likely prevents some children from intervening.

The determining factors for bystander intervention during bullying episodes dif-fered for girls and boys. Social self-efficacy predicted bystander intervention among girls, regardless of empathy and attitudes about bullying. Girls who reported high levels of social self-efficacy were 32 times more likely than other girls to report intervening during the last bullying episode they witnessed. In contrast, empathy and attitudes about bullying predicted bystander intervention among boys, regard-less of social self-efficacy. Boys who reported high levels of empathy were about 17 times more likely than other boys to report intervening during the last bullying epi-sode they witnessed, and boys who reported low levels of probully attitudes were about 8 times more likely to report intervening in bullying. The different character-istics associated with bullying intervention behaviours for girls and boys may reflect well-established gender differences in aggression, since youth tend to intervene more often in same- than in opposite-sex bullying (Hawkins et al., 2001). As a pro-portion of aggressive behaviours, girls are more likely to exhibit more covert forms of aggression (e.g., social) and boys are more likely to exhibit overt forms (e.g., physical; Card et al., 2008).

Implications for Bullying Prevention ProgramsThese findings suggest that bullying prevention programs can encourage bystander intervention by addressing issues of social justice, emphasizing that no one deserves to be bullied and bullying is always wrong and unfair, as well as the importance of helping others when they are in trouble. It may also be effective to focus on bullying as a community problem (i.e., everyone’s business) and implement school policies that increase personal feelings of responsibility and accountability for intervening during bullying episodes in an effort to mobilize children who would not typically feel inclined to intervene.

These findings also suggest that bullying prevention programs would benefit from gender specific components that focus on empowering bystanders. Perhaps targeting social self-efficacy is the most effective intervention goal for the covert forms of bul-lying (i.e., social) predominant among girls, while targeting empathy and negative opinions of bullying are the most important intervention goals for overt forms of bul-lying (i.e., physical and verbal) predominant among boys.

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 13: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

Cappadocia et al. 211

Although empowering children to intervene during bullying episodes is important for bullying prevention, successful bullying prevention and/or intervention requires effective leadership and support from adults. Children cannot intervene successfully unless adults are actively and effectively supporting bystanders as well as children who are at risk for bullying others and/or being victimized. Research from across the world has indicated that teachers must be explicitly trained with respect to healthy and unhealthy peer relationships to be effective in promoting positive relationships and preventing negative peer dynamics such as bullying (Pepler & Craig, 2008). Effective prevention programs should include the following critical components for socializing adults (e.g., parents and teachers): scaffolding, social architecture, and self-awareness (Pepler, 2006; Pepler, Craig, & Cummings, 2009). Scaffolding involves coaching chil-dren and tailoring support as they struggle with complex social issues such as bullying others and/or being bullied (Pepler, 2006). Social architecture involves actively impos-ing organization and structure during children’s interactions and activities. Social architecture requires focusing on the social dynamics within children’s groups and creating social contexts that will likely promote positive peer interactions and reduce the potential for aggressive interactions (e.g., sports teams created at random by a socializing adult, rather than by team captains; Pepler, 2006; Pepler et al., 2009). Self-awareness is crucial among adults who interact with children, to ensure their social interactions and relationships with other adults and children are appropriate for model-ing healthy and positive relationships (Pepler et al., 2009).

Study LimitationsThe convenience sampling employed in this study limits the generalizability of the results. The sample includes children and youth attending a particular overnight sum-mer camp in Ontario, Canada, which has a history of implementing formal policies about bullying for all camp community members (campers, counsellors, etc.). Although the generalizability of these results is limited, the findings provide prelimi-nary information for future studies of bullying bystander intervention among children. Another limitation within this study is the exclusive use of self-report survey data. Children responded to closed-answer questions about bystander behaviours, which did not allow for further qualitative exploration. In addition, findings based solely on self-report data must be interpreted with caution because of shared method variance, given that students were the only ones reporting their own behaviours across all of the scales and items in the survey. Despite these limitations, self-report surveys are effec-tive for assessing behaviours, feelings, and opinions among children. Since involve-ment in bullying represents a private experience that many students do not report to others, self-report data are essential for indexing these experiences.

Future DirectionsFuture research on bullying bystander behaviours among children should focus on comparing rates of bullying and bystander intervention at summer camps that have

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 14: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

212 Canadian Journal of School Psychology 27(3)

formal bullying prevention programs with summer camps that do not have these pro-grams. The depth and scope of the bullying prevention program within the camp cur-riculum and the number of years the camp has employed the program should be considered. Future research should also look at the effectiveness of various interven-tion programs within summer camps based on intervention goals, focusing particu-larly on gender and age effects. More generally, future research on bullying bystander behaviours among children should include prospective data that index stability and change over several data points within a year and over the important developmental stages of childhood and adolescence. If this research is undertaken within overnight camps, the prospective data should index stability and change over several data points within the camp term (often 1 month) and across summers for each camper.

Summary and ConclusionsMost of the children in this study had witnessed bullying at least once in the past 3 weeks and reported intervening. A sense of social justice appeared to be the stron-gest motivation for intervention, while those who did not intervene tended to feel it was not their place. The determining factors for bystander intervention during bully-ing episodes differed between genders, indicating that bullying prevention efforts are likely most effective when gender is considered. It is important to note that although empowering children to intervene during bullying episodes is important for bullying prevention, it is not sufficient. Children cannot intervene successfully unless adults are actively and effectively supporting bystanders as well as children who are at risk for bullying others and/or being victimized.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: M. Catherine Cappadocia was supported during the prepara-tion of this article by The Provincial Centre of Excellence for Child and Youth Mental Health at CHEO Graduate Award, Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) Frederick Banting and Charles Best Canada Graduate Scholarships Doctoral Award, and MITACS Accelerate Research Internship Award. Debra Pepler, Joanne G. Cummings, and Wendy Craig were sup-ported by Networks of Centres of Excellence through its support of PREVNet (Promoting Relationships and Eliminating Violence Network). This research was conducted in collabora-tion with Family Channel and Camp Arowhon.

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 15: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

Cappadocia et al. 213

References

Andreou, E., Vlachou, A., & Didaskalou, E. (2005). The roles of self-efficacy, peer interac-tions and attitudes in bully-victim incidents: Implications for intervention policy-practices. School Psychology International, 26, 545-562.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: Freeman.Card, N. A., Stucky, B. D., Sawalani, G. M., & Little, T. D. (2008). Direct and indirect aggres-

sion during childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic review of gender differences, inter-correlations, and relations to maladjustment. Child Development, 79, 1185-1229.

Craig, W., & Pepler, D. (1995). Peer processes in bullying and victimization: An observational study. Exceptionality Education Canada, 5, 81-95.

Craig, W., & Pepler, D. (1997). Observation of bullying and victimization in the schoolyard. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 13, 41-59.

Craig, W., Pepler, D., & Blais, J. (2007). Responding to bullying: What works? School Psychol-ogy International, 28, 465-477.

Darley, J. M., & Latané, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies: Diffusion of respon-sibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8, 377-383.

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimen-sional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113-126.

Delfabbro, P., Winefield, T., Trainor, S., Dollard, M., Anderson, S., Metzer, J., & Hammarstrom, A. (2006). Peer and teacher bullying/victimization of South Australian secondary school stu-dents: Prevalence and psychosocial profiles. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 71-90.

Endresen, I. M., & Olweus, D. (2001). Self-reported empathy in Norwegian adolescents: Sex differences, age trends, and relationship to bullying. In A. C. Bohart, C. Arthur, & D. J. Stipek (Eds.), Constructive & destructive behavior (pp. 147-165). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Fekkes, M., Pijpers, F. I. M., & Verloove-Vanhorick, S. P. (2005). Bullying: Who does what, when and where? Involvement of children, teachers and parents in bullying behavior. Health Education Research, 20(1), 81-91.

Forero, R., McLellan, L., Rissel, C., & Bauman, A. (1999). Bullying behavior and psychosocial health among school students in New South Wales, Australia: Cross sectional study. British Medical Journal, 319, 344-348.

Gini, G., Albiero, P., Benelli, B., & Altoe, G. (2008). Determinants of adolescents’ active defending and passive bystanding behaviour in bullying. Journal of Adolescence, 31, 93-105.

Hawkins, D. L., Pepler, D. J., & Craig, W. M. (2001). Naturalistic observations of peer interven-tions in bullying. Social Development, 10, 512-527.

Hazler, R. J. (1996). Breaking the cycle of violence: Interventions for bullying and victimiza-tion. Washington, DC: Accelerated Development.

Manger, T., Eikeland, O., & Asbjornsen, A. (2001). Effects of social-cognitive training on stu-dents’ empathy. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 60, 82-88.

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 16: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

214 Canadian Journal of School Psychology 27(3)

Mitchell, K. J., Ybarra, M., & Finkelhor, D. (2007). The relative importance of online victim-ization in understanding depression, delinquency, and substance use. Child Maltreatment, 12, 314-324.

Molcho, M., Craig, W., Due, P., Pickett, W., Harel-Fisch, Y., Overpeck, M., & the HSBC Bullying Writing Group. (2009). Cross-national time trends in bullying behavior 1994-2006: Findings from Europe and North America. International Journal of Public Health, 54, 225-234.

Nansel, T. R., Overpeck, M., Pilla, R. S., Ruan, W. J., Simons-Morton, B., & Scheidt, P. (2001). Bullying behaviours among U.S. youth: Prevalence and association with psychosocial adjustment. Journal of the American Medical Association, 285, 2094-2100.

Nickerson, A. B., Mele, D., & Princiotta, D. (2008). Attachment and empathy as predictors of roles as defenders or outsiders in bullying interactions. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 687-703.

O’Connell, P., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (1999). Peer involvement in bullying: Insights and chal-lenges for intervention. Journal of Adolescence, 22, 437-452.

Oh, I., & Hazler, R. J. (2009). Contributions of personal and situational factors to bystanders’ reactions to school bullying. School Psychology International, 30, 291-310.

Olweus, D., & Endresen, I. M. (1998). The importance of sex-of-stimulus object: Age trends and sex differences in empathic responsiveness. Social Development, 7, 370-388.

Pastorelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Bandura, A. (1998). La misura dell’autoefficacia percepita in eta` scolare: Uncontributo preliminare [The measurement of self-efficacy in school-age children: A preliminary contribution]. Eta`Evolutiva, 61, 28-40.

Pepler, D. (2006). Bullying interventions: A binocular perspective. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 15, 16-20.

Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (2008). Conclusion: Moving forward from lessons learned. In D. Pepler & W. Craig (Eds.), Understanding and addressing bullying: An international perspective (pp. 311-320). Bloomington, IN: Authorhouse.

Pepler, D., Craig, W., & Cummings, J. (2009). Conclusion: Steps to respect for everyone by everyone. In W. Craig, D. Pepler, & J. Cummings (Eds.), Rise up for respectful relation-ships: Prevent bullying (pp. 199-206). Ottawa, Ontario, Canada: National Printers.

Poyhonen, V., & Salmivalli, C. (2008). New directions in research and practice addressing bullying: Focus on defending behaviour. In D. Pepler & W. Craig (Eds.), Understand-ing and addressing bullying: An international perspective (pp. 26-43). Bloomington, IN: AuthorHouse.

PREVNet Assessment Working Group. (2008). PREVNet Bullying Survey: Grades 4-6. Kingston, Ontario, Canada: PREVNet.

Rigby, K., & Johnson, B. (2006). Expressed readiness of Australian schoolchildren to act as bystanders in support of children who are being bullied. Educational Psychology, 26, 425-440.

Rivers, I., Poteat, V., Noret, N., & Ashurst, N. (2009). Observing bullying at school: The mental health implications of witness status. School Psychology Quarterly, 24, 211-223.

Rivers, I., & Smith, P. K. (1994). Types of bullying behaviour and their correlates. Aggressive Behavior, 20, 359-368.

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 17: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

Cappadocia et al. 215

Rogers, M. J., & Tisak, M. S. (1996). Children’s reasoning about responses to peer aggression: Victim’s and witness’ expected and prescribed behaviors. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 259-269.

Salmivalli, C. (2010). Bullying and the peer group: A review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 15, 112-120.

Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Bjorkqvist, K., Osterman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1996). Bully-ing as a group process: Participant roles and their relations to social status within the group. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 1-15.

Salmivalli, C., Lappalainen, M., & Lagerspetz, K. (1998). Stability and change of behaviour in connection with bullying in schools: A two-year follow-up. Aggressive Behavior, 24, 205-218.

Scheithauer, H., Hayer, T., Petermann, F., & Jugert, G. (2006). Physical, verbal, and relational forms of bullying among German students: Age trends, gender differences, and correlates. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 261-275.

Stevens, V., Van Oost, P., De Bourdeaudhuij, L. (2000). The effects of an anti-bullying inter-vention programme on peers’ attitudes and behaviour. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 21-34.

Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999). Social cognition and bullying: Social inad-equacy or skills manipulation? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 435-450.

Tisak, M. S., & Tisak, J. (1996). My sibling’s but not my friend’s keeper: Reasoning about responses to aggressive acts. Journal of Early Adolescence, 16, 324-339.

Woods, S., & White, E. (2005). The association between bullying behavior, arousal levels, and behavior problems. Journal of Adolescence, 28, 381-395.

Ybarra, M. L., & Mitchell, K. J. (2004). Online aggressor/targets, aggressors, and targets: A comparison of associated youth characterictics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-try, 45, 1308-1316.

Ybarra, M. L., & Mitchell, K. J. (2007). Prevalence and frequency of Internet harassment insti-gation: Implications for adolescent health. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41, 189-195.

Yuile, A., Pepler, D., Craig, W., & Connolly, J. (2006). The ethics of peeking behind the fence: Issues related to studying children’s aggression and victimization. In B. Leadbeater, E. Nanister, C. Benoit, M. Jansson, A. Marshall, & T. Riecken. (Eds.), Ethical issues in community-based research with children and youth (pp. 70-89). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: University of Toronto Press.

Bios

M. Catherine Cappadocia is a doctoral student in clinical developmental psychology at York University. Supervised by Dr. Debra Pepler, her research focuses on investigating psychosocial risk factors and outcomes as well as interventions associated with bullying and victimization during childhood and adolescence.

Debra Pepler is a distinguished research professor of psychology at York University and a senior associate scientist at the Hospital for Sick Children. Together with Dr. Wendy Craig, she is leading a national network, PREVNet (Promoting Relationships and Eliminating Violence

at UNIV OF WESTERN ONTARIO on October 31, 2014cjs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 18: Individual Motivations and Characteristics Associated With Bystander Intervention During Bullying Episodes Among Children and Youth

216 Canadian Journal of School Psychology 27(3)

Network), a Networks of Centres of Excellence—New Initiative. PREVNet’s mission is to promote safe and healthy relationships for children and youth (www.prevnet.ca).

Joanne G. Cummings received her PhD in clinical developmental psychology from York University and has been involved in bullying prevention work since 1995, consulting to schools and community organizations to raise awareness about bullying and to create healthy social environments. She is the partnership director of PREVNet (Promoting Relationships and Eliminating Violence), a national network that aims to mobilize knowledge to reduce bullying and peer victimization throughout Canada. As a clinician, she works with very young children (ages 0-6) and their parents at Blueballoon Health Services, where she is the director of clinical training in psychology.

Wendy Craig received her PhD in clinical developmental psychology from York University. Currently she is a professor in the Psychology Department at Queen’s University. She coleads PREVNet (Promoting relationships and eliminating violence), a national network of research-ers and organizations working with children and youth.