16
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/236233194 Multiple Goals Perspective in Adolescent Students With Learning Difficulties ARTICLE in LEARNING DISABILITY QUARTERLY · NOVEMBER 2011 Impact Factor: 0.77 · DOI: 10.1177/0731948711421763 CITATIONS 7 READS 30 6 AUTHORS, INCLUDING: José Carlos Núñez University of Oviedo 210 PUBLICATIONS 1,194 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Julio Antonio González-Pienda University of Oviedo 90 PUBLICATIONS 780 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Antonio Valle University of A Coruña 147 PUBLICATIONS 1,122 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Pedro Rosário University of Minho 148 PUBLICATIONS 730 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE All in-text references underlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately. Available from: Antonio Valle Retrieved on: 28 March 2016

Learning Disability Quarterly

  • Upload
    mfhfhf

  • View
    10

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

HKTU

Citation preview

Page 1: Learning Disability Quarterly

Seediscussions,stats,andauthorprofilesforthispublicationat:https://www.researchgate.net/publication/236233194

MultipleGoalsPerspectiveinAdolescentStudentsWithLearningDifficulties

ARTICLEinLEARNINGDISABILITYQUARTERLY·NOVEMBER2011

ImpactFactor:0.77·DOI:10.1177/0731948711421763

CITATIONS

7

READS

30

6AUTHORS,INCLUDING:

JoséCarlosNúñez

UniversityofOviedo

210PUBLICATIONS1,194CITATIONS

SEEPROFILE

JulioAntonioGonzález-Pienda

UniversityofOviedo

90PUBLICATIONS780CITATIONS

SEEPROFILE

AntonioValle

UniversityofACoruña

147PUBLICATIONS1,122CITATIONS

SEEPROFILE

PedroRosário

UniversityofMinho

148PUBLICATIONS730CITATIONS

SEEPROFILE

Allin-textreferencesunderlinedinbluearelinkedtopublicationsonResearchGate,

lettingyouaccessandreadthemimmediately.

Availablefrom:AntonioValle

Retrievedon:28March2016

Page 2: Learning Disability Quarterly

¥HAMMILL INSTITUTEO N D r S A B I L I T I E S

Multiple Goals Perspective in AdolescentStudents With Learning Difficulties

José Carlos Nunez', Julio Antonio González-Pienda',Celestino Rodríguez',Antonio Valle', Ramon Gonzalez-Cabanach',and Pedro Rosario'

Learning Disability Quarterly

34(4) 273-286

© Hammiil Institute on Disabilities 2011

Reprints and permission: http://www.

sagepub.com/iournalsPermissions.nav

DOI: I O.I 177/07319487 M 421763

http://idq.sagepub.com

AbstractIn the present work, the hypothesis of the existence of diverse motivational profiles in students with learning difficulties(LD) and the differential implications for intervention in the classroom are analyzed.Various assessment scales (academicgoals, self-concept, and causal attributions) were administered to a sample of 259 students with LD, ages 8 to 15 years,in Spain. The data obtained were analyzed through (a) cluster analysis to study this hypothesis and (b) MANOVAs todetermine the extent to which such profiles were accompanied by significant differences in self-esteem and causalattribution patterns.The results revealed four different motivational profiles and significantly different levels of self-esteemand causal attribution process.

Keywords

motivation, goals, learning difTiculties, self-esteem, causal attribution

Personal goals are a very important framework from whichto explain motivational orientations and behavior patternsin the school setting. The results of past research on the roleof motivational academic orientations have agreed thatleaming goals are beneficial for most learning-relatedresults, including motivational results such as self-efficacy,interest, and value (Barron & Harackiewicz, 2001, 2003;Valle et al., 2008; Wolters, Yu, & Pintrich, 1996), emotionalwell-being (Middleton & Midgley, 1997), help seeking, andcognitive commitment (Pintrich, 2000a). In the area ofleaming difficulties (LD), although motivational and emo-tional variables are relevant (Sideridis, 2005c), only a fewempirical studies have examined the topic of motivation instudents with LD from the perspective of academic goalsand, more specifically, from the perspective of multiplegoals.

Taking into consideration the research to date, two generallines of work were observed: (a) studies that compareddiverse academic goals (leaming goals, performance goals,performance-avoidance goals), one by one, among studentswith and without LD and (b) investigations that attempted todetemiine the relationship between such goals and impor-tant criteria such as achievement in various curricular areas,affective variables, or even psychopathological variables. Inaddition, some current works have examined these children'smotivational environment from the perspective of multiplegoals in comparative and explanatory studies.

Academic Goals

Although the study of personal goals has been addressed fromdifferent perspectives, they all share the idea that peopleestablish goals for themselves, in such a way that cognitiverepresentations of future events become potential motivatorsof behavior in any setting. From such proposals, and withinthe academic sphere, there has been increasing interest inthe idea that the academic goals pursued by students orga-nize and regulate their behavior. Certain goals are closelyrelated to the type of motivation, which is defined by thekind of goal they hope to achieve.

Elliot and collaborators (Elliot, 1997, 1999; Elliot &Church, 1997; Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1996) proposed a tri-dimensional comprehensive framework of academic goals.In their proposal, they differentiated two tendencies withinperfonnance goals, an approach tendency and an avoidancetendency, and as a result defined three independent aca-demic goals:

'University of Oviedo, Oviedo, Spain

Corresponding Author:Celestino Rodriguez, University of Oviedo, Department of Psychology.Plaza Feijoo s/n, 33003, Oviedo, SpainEmail: [email protected]

Page 3: Learning Disability Quarterly

274 Learning Disability Quarterly 34(4)

1. Performance approach, focused on achievingcompetence in comparison to others

2. Performance avoidance, focused on avoidingincompetence in comparison to others

3. Learning approach, focused on the developmentof competence and mastery of the task

Subsequently, a new construct, learning-avoidance goals,was proposed. This is the result of applying the differentia-tion of the tendencies of approach and avoidance towardleaming goals (Pintrich, 2000a, 2000b). Students who havelearning-approach goals are oriented to achieving the goalof leaming and understanding, whereas those with learning-avoidance goals are concemed with not being perfect, notunderstanding the material completely, or failing to meettheir self-referred mastery standards (Linnenbrink &Pintrich, 2002). Nevertheless, such general considerationsshould be taken with precaution because the results of arecent meta-analysis (Hulleman, Schräger, Bodmann, &Harackiewicz, 2010) appeared to indicate diverse dimen-sions within each one of these four types of goals, suggest-ing that the goals maintain different relations with othermotivational, cognitive, behavioral, and performancevariables.

Academic Goals inStudents With LDFrom an explanatory perspective, some investigators haveattempted to find possible links between motivational, cog-nitive, and psychopathological variables and the academicachievement of students with LD. For example, the worksof Georgios Sideridis have contributed interesting data thatconfirmed the significant connection between academic goalsand achievement, and affect, self-esteem, anxiety, and depres-sion (Sideridis, 2003, 2006, 2007).

Although they do not coincide, the studies that haveaddressed this topic from a comparative perspective haveindicated that students with LD display less motivation towardleaming and more fear of failure (Botsas & Padeliadu, 2003;González-Pienda et al., 2(X)0; Pintrich, Anderman, & K lobucar,1994; Sideridis, 2003; Sideridis & Tsorbatzoudis, 2003),and this tendency is more pronounced in boys than in girls(González-Pienda, Núñez, & González-Pumariega, 1996).In terms of academic goals, without considering possiblesex differences, students with LD present lower leaminggoals and higher performance-avoidance goals with no dif-ferences in performance-approach goals. In other studiesin which social reinforcement- or approval-seeking goalswere evaluated, it was found that students with LD tend tobe less motivated in this sense than do their classmates(González-Pienda et al., 1996; González-Pienda et al., 2000).

For example, in a study of 496 children (253 with LD, 243without LD; mean age = 11 years), González-Pienda et al.

(2000) reported the existence of very significant differencesbetween students with and without LD, finding that com-pared to their counterparts without LD, students with LDwere significantly less motivated toward leaming (leaminggoals), less motivated toward achieving others' approval(social goals), but equally motivated toward a certain levelof performance (performance goals). These authors notedthat these results could indicate that students' motivationtoward academic tasks is closely related to the beliefs theyhold about their competence and their expectations of self-efficacy, and they interpreted the results in the sense that,because of their repeated failure experiences, children withLD tend to distrust their ability and to develop low self-efficacy expectations, so that rather than being motiva-tionally oriented toward leaming (leaming goals), they aremotivated toward a certain level of academic performance(perfonnance goals). This is noted in the fact that studentswith and without LD do not display significant differencesin their interest in attaining a certain level of performance(performance goals).

As in other later studies (e.g., Botsas & Padeliadu, 2003;Sideridis, 2003), no differences in performance-approachgoals were found because the groups of students may haveattributed both their successes and their failures mainly toeffort or lack of effort; this allowed students with LD to main-tain a minimum motivational level. At the level of metacog-nitive system functioning, because of their negative academicself-efficacy perceptions and low motivation to gain mean-ingful knowledge, students with LD—particularly those witha tendency to develop maladaptive attributional pattems(Núñez et al , 2005)—will probably engage only superfi-cially in tasks and resort to using repetitive, primitive cogni-tive and self-regulatory strategies, thereby minimizing theirefforts to leam. This leads to new failures; consequently, theirinterest in leaming will progressively decrease. Last, accord-ing to these investigators, students with LD who have areduced interest in achieving social acknowledgment can beexplained by their perception of others' negative expecta-tions about their leaming possibilities, especially those oftheir parents and teachers (Meltzer et al., 1998). Moreover,this decreased interest in approval seeking or even indefending their self-esteem may be the result of the mal-adaptive attributional pattem that is so typical in many ofthese children (Settle & Milich, 1999).

Multiple Goals PerspectiveThe strategy of considering the consequences of each oneof the academic goals individually presents some draw-backs when educational reality indicates that students rarelyadopt a single goal exclusively but instead, in most cases,choose various goals simultaneously or, depending on thecircumstances, give priority to some goals over others. Almostall school situations present the opportunity to pursue diverse

Page 4: Learning Disability Quarterly

Núñez et al. 275

goals simultaneously, and many situations may requirestudents to strive for even various goals at the same time.Therefore, when considering this diversity of goals—manyof them important for students—it is impossible to assert thatthey can be pursued one by one, in an isolated manner.

Taking this into account, in general, the research cardedout from this perspective has focused on the analysis of thecombination of different academic goals and their effects onlearning and on academic performance. For example, Barronand Harackiewicz (2001) suggested four ways by means ofwhich learning goals and perfomiance-approach goalscould be combined;

1. Both types of goals are combined, but each onehas beneficial effects on certain results, that is,their effects are additive

2. Interactive effects result from both types of goals,so that adopting both at the same time will be moreadaptive for a certain outcome than only adoptinga single type of goal

3. Possibility of occurrence of specialized effects,that is, unique effects for both types of goals overmultiple results; for example, mastery goals couldbe beneficial in terms of interest or emotional well-being, whereas perfonnance-approach goals may bemore adaptive in terms of achievement outcomes

4. Possibility of occurrence of selective effects, so thatthe consequences or effects of the goals depend onwhether they coincide with the goals of the context

With regard to the consequences of these diverse combi-nations of learning and performance goals, according toBouffard, Boisvert, Vezeau, and Larouche (1995), the high-est levels of motivation, cognitive strategies, self-regulation,and performance strategies are found in the group thatcombines high learning goals and high performance goals.Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, Carter, and Elliot (2000) foundthat learning goals positively predict interest but not grades,whereas perfonnance-approach goals positively predict gradesbut not interest. These complementary results of learninggoals and perfonnance goals led the authors to conclude thatthe best strategy is for the students to adopt both types ofgoals concurrently. In a longitudinal study carried out withsecondary students, Pintrich (2000a) concluded that studentswho are concerned with their performance and with beingbetter than their classmates but, at the same time, are orientedtoward learning follow a parallel trajectory to students whoare oriented only toward learning. However, Pintrich alsonoted that this trajectory is not equally adaptive in the caseof students concerned only with perfomiance.

Currently, data are emerging that indicate that, as in sam-ples of typical students, the combination of goals predicts theacademic achievement of students with LD more effectivelythan if each one of the academic goals is taken individually

(Sideridis, 2003, 2005b, 2006, 2007), although results inthe opposite direction have also been found (Sideridis &Tsorbatzoudis, 2003). In general, Sideridis has found thatbeing oriented toward multiple goals is less adaptive thanbeing oriented mainly toward learning goals.

These flndings thus have led important investigators tourge the development of research of individual differences instudents with LD, relative to diverse domains (social, motiva-tional, emotional, cognitive) and how they are related to vari-ous intrinsic and extrinsic factors (Margalit & Idan, 2004;Meltzer, 2004). In this sense, for example, Margalit (2004)noted that instead of other extensively studied aspects, weneed to develop investigations to identify subgroups of stu-dents with LD who are capable of improvement and whostill perceive themselves positively (resilient children)like their peers without LD, despite their deficient cogni-tive processing.

In this vein, in the present investigation, we studied themotivational profile (combination of goals) of studentswith LD and how it is associated with significant differ-ences in cognitive-affective variables such as causal attri-butions and self-concept. We attempt to answer the followingquestions:

Are there subgroups of students with LD with signifl-cantly different motivational proflles and similarto those obtained in populations of students with-out LD?

Do LD students who have a multiple goal profile dis-play a more adaptive pattern than students whohave a profile with just one predominant goal(learning or performance)?

PredictionsIn view of the lack of data on the different combinations oftypes of goals in students with LD. and taking as a referencethe results of the studies carried out with populations with-out LD (i,e,, Daniels et al,, 2008; Valle, Núñez, Cabanach,Rodriguez, González-Pienda, & Rosario, 2009), the datafrom the works of Sideridis (2003, 2006, 2007), and thosecontributed by our own work with students with LD(i.e., González-Pienda et al., 2000; Núñez et al., 2005), wemade the following predictions:

1. The three types of goals considered in this study(learning goals, performance goals, and social-reinforcement-seeking goals) will be associatedwith each other, leading to different motivationalproflles (homogeneous groups of participantswith a similar combination of academic goals).

2. Empirical evidence for the existence of four dif-ferent motivational profiles will be obtained: (a) amultiple goal profile (students oriented both toward

Page 5: Learning Disability Quarterly

276 Learning Disability Quarterly 34(4)

leaming and perfomiance or social reinforcementseeking), (b) a low motivational level profile (stu-dents with a low or very low level in all three typesof goals), (c) a profile with predominance of leam-ing goals (students oriented mainly toward mastery,personal progress, and leaming), and (d) a profilewith a predominance of extrinsic goals (studentsoriented mainly toward attaining better performancethan their peers or toward seeking significant others'acknowledgment).

3. These four motivational profiles will be associatedwith significantly different levels of self-conceptand causal attributions. Specifically, on one hand,students with a multiple goal profile will obtainthe highest levels in self-esteem, followed bythe profile with predominance of leaming goals,predominance of performance goals, and, last, alow motivation profile. However, students with amultiple goal profile will attribute their successesto a greater extent—and their failures to a lesserextent—to intemal causes (capacity and effort)than will students with a profile with predomi-nance of leaming goals and/or performance goalsand, particularly, students with a low motivationprofile.

Method

Participants

Initially, we contacted representatives from the 36 educationalcenters of primary schools (second to sixth grade levels) andsecondary schools (seventh and eighth grade levels) in twonorthem regions of Spain to obtain their participation in theinvestigation, requesting them to (a) provide data about thediagnosis of students with LD (screening to determine whichchildren were diagnosed with LD), (b) request parents' per-mission for their children's participation in the investigation,and (c) inform the teachers and request their collaboration inproviding various data. The tests were administered by post-graduates with scholarships who were collaborating with theteam. The participants filled in the three questionnaires indi-vidually and without any time limit. Any doubts expressedby students about terms were cleared up.

Participants were 257 students with LD, ages 8 to 15 years{M= 11.90, SD = 1.703), distributed as follows: 6 eight-year-olds (5 boys and 1 girl), 17 nine-year-olds (12 boys and5 girls), 36 ten-year-olds (14 boys and 22 girls), 42 eleven-year-olds (25 boys and 17 girls), 58 twelve-year-olds(44 boys and 14 girls), 37 thirteen-year-olds (25 boys and12 girls), 55 fourteen-year-olds (37 boys and 18 girls),and 6 fifteen-year-olds (5 boys and 1 girl). The total samplecomprised 167 boys (65%) and 90 girls (35%; see Table 1 ).None of the study participants had an IQ lower than 79

(minimum = 79, maximum = 121, iV/ = 92.02, SD = 6.27),nor did they present with sensory disabilities, severe emotionalproblems (e.g., autism), or attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder. Practically all of the students attended public schoolsin two northem regions of Spain.

In the United States, use of the discrepancy model as amethod of diagnosis of LD is currently being questioned, par-ticularly since 2004, when the Individuals with DisabilitiesEducation Improvement Act oificially introduced the modelof response to intervention (RTI) as an altemative to the dis-crepancy model (Jiménez et al., 2010). Nevertheless, althoughsome authors believe that the RTI model is the most promis-ing method for identifying individuals with LD (Haager,Klingener, & Vaughn, 2007), other researchers have statedthat the effectiveness of RTI is significantly mediated by thechild's age (Vellutino, Scanlon, Zhang, & Schatschneider,2008) and may not be sufficient to identify people with LD(Al Otaiba et al., 2009), or they even doubt that it canbe considered a diagnostic method (Kavale, Kauffman,Bachmeier, & LeFevers, 2008).

In Spain, as in other countries (McLaughlin et al., 2006),use of the RTI model has scarce tradition because there is noclear definition of LD. Specifically, the diagnosis perfonnedby the school district specialists, integrated in psychopeda-gogical teams (see Ntiñez & González-Pienda, 2007), wasused to select the students with LD. The diagnostic processused at the public educational centers in Spain is describedbelow. Once the general education classroom teacher detectsa student with poor performance with no apparent justifica-tion (motivational problems, discipline, etc.), the specialistof the psychopedagogical team addresses the analysis of thereal magnitude of the leaming delay, determining whetherthe student's academic performance is significantly lowerthan his or her intellectual ability. The discrepancy is consid-ered significant when performance is 2 or more years lowerthan the general intellectual ability. Second, in the absenceof some general intellectual deficit and in the presence of asignificant discrepancy between intellectual abilities andperformance, the specialist continues to look for a deficit inbasic cognitive processes that would justify the discrepancy.Third, the specialist attempts to rule out the possibility thatthe existing learning problems are the result of disabilitiesother than LD (visual, auditory, motor, emotional, etc.).Finally, once the first three steps have been completed andthe student is considered as having "potential LD," takinginto account the student's characteristics (deficiencies andabilities), the specialist carries out modifications in the con-ditions of access to the general education curriculum thatseem to be preventing the student from leaming. If the studentdoes not respond to the access adaptation, the specialist, in col-laboration with tutors, proceeds to carry out an "individualcurricular adaptation," which means that the student isassumed to be incapable of following the curriculum—^atleast, at the established requirement level—and, as a result, it

Page 6: Learning Disability Quarterly

Núñez et al. 277

Table I. Participant Demographic Characteristic AcrossGender

Characteristic

Age (years)

8910I I1213H15

Grade level

Second

Third

Fourth

rittnSixthSeventhEighth

Ethnicity

White

GypsyAfrican

(Moroccan)Asian

IQMSDMinMax

Diagnosis time"*MSDHhtMax

Boys'

n

51214254425375

39

1730622521

15772

1

91.74

6.2679

113

23.3421.71

196

%

3.07.28.4

15.0

26.3

15.0

22.2

3.0

1.85.4

10.2

18.0

37.1

15.0

12.6

94.04.21.2

0.6

Girls'"

n

15

22171412181

02

1924249

12

8631

0

92.506.32

80121

25.1822.24

190

%

I.I5.6

24.4

18.9

15.6

13.3

20.0

I.I

0.02.2

21.1

26.7

26.7

10.0

13.3

95.63.3I.I

0.0

Total'

n

6173642583755

3II3654863433

243103

1

92.026.27

79121

2421.87

196

%

2.36.6

14.0

16.3

22.6

14.4

21.4

2.3

1.24.3

14.021.0

33.5

13.212.8

94.63.91.2

0.4

a.n= 167(65%).b. n = 90 (35%).c. N = 257.d. Months students had been diagnosed with learning difficulties at thetime of participation in this research.

must be significantly modified. The adaptation is generallycarried out over 2 years. Except for severe cases, the stu-dents receive instmction in the curriculum adapted to theordinary classroom, and they also participate in supplemen-tal instruction specific to tlieir deficit or deficits, outside ofthe normal classroom.

Measures

Academic Goals Questionnaire (AGQ). This instrument wasbased on an experimental questionnaire used by Hayamizu

and Weiner (1991). It is composed of 20 items: 8 conceminglearning goals (evaluation of students' interest in leaming asa priority goal), 6 concerning performatice goals (students'interest in obtaining a certain goal as a priority aim), and6 referring to motivational orientation toward obtainingsocial reinforcement and acknowledgment. The student rateseach item on a 5-point scale {never to always). Examples ofthe three types of goals assessed by the AGQ are the follow-ing: leaming goals ("I study because, for me, it is interestingto solve problems or tasks"), performance goals ("1 studychiefly because I want to get good grades"), and social rein-forcement goals ("I study because I want my parents andteacher to appreciate me"). The Spanish adaptation of theAGQ shows high reliability coefficients for students withLD, leaming goals (a = .85), performance goals (a = .86),social reinforcement goals (a = .83), total AGQ (a = .87), andexcellent structural validity and predictive validity for vari-ous types of leaming strategies and for academic achieve-ment (González-Pienda et al., 2000; Inglés et al., 2009; Valleet al., 2003). In this study, the reliability of the global AGQwas also high (a = .919).

Self-Description Questionnaire (SDQ-I). This multidimensionalinstrument (Marsh, 1988) consists of 76 items organized intoeight self-concept dimensions: general, academic (general,mathematic, verbal), social (relationships with parents andpeers), and physical (physical appearance, physical capacity),to which participants respond on a 5-point scale. The SDQ-I isa reliable instrument with demonstrated validity (see Byme,1996). In the Spanish population with LD, reliability is high(Núñez, González-Pumariega, & González-Pienda, 1995):general (a = .74), general academic (a = .84), reading (a =.92), mathematics (a = .92), physical appearance (a = .84),physical capacity (a = .81), relations with parent (a = .81),peer relations (a = .73), and total SDQ (a = .92). In thissample of students with LD, reliability of the global SDQwas high (a = .916). In addition, the Spanish version hasconstruct validity and predictive validity for academicachievement (Núñez et al., 1995).

Sydney Attribution Scale (SAS). Developed by Relich et al.(Marsh, Cairns, Relich, Bames, & Debus, 1984), the SAS isa multidimensional scale that assesses individuals' percep-tions of the causes of their academic successes and failures,adopting a dispositional viewpoint. It is made up of 24 hypo-thetical situations, with responses on a 5-point scale. These24 situations involve combinations of two academic areas(mathematics, verbal), three types of causes (ability, effort,extemal causes), and two opposite-hypothetical outcomes(success situations and failure situations). In this study, how-ever, the academic area was not taken into account, so thatsix scores were obtained: Attribution of Success to Ability(ASAB), Attribution of Success to Effort (ASEF), Attribu-tion of Success to Extemal Causes (ASEC), Attribution ofFailure to Ability (AFAB), Attribution of Failure to Effort(AFEF), Attribution of Failure to Extemal Causes (AFEC;

Page 7: Learning Disability Quarterly

278 Learning Disability Quarterly 34(4)

3 types of causes ^ 2 opposite-hypothetical outcomes). TheSAS adaptation for Spanish-speaking populations has beenshown to have acceptable reliability (in Spain, a = .81; seeGonzález-Pienda et al., 2002; and a = .82 in Chile; seeVillalobos, González-Pienda, Núñez, & Mújica, 1997) andconstmct validity and predictive validity for academic achieve-ment (Núñez et al., 1995). The reliability of this scale in thepresent investigation was somewhat lower (a = ,768).

Statistical Analyses

To contrast the first and second hypotheses, we performedseveral cluster analyses. Cluster analysis is not only themost appropriate procedure to establish profiles in an amplesample of study participants (Hair, Anderson, Tatham, &Black, 1998) but also one of the most recommended solu-tions to identify multiple goals (Pastor, Barron, Davis, &Miller, 2004). As in the predictions, we had established thatfour clusters or groups of students with certain levels in thethree kinds of goals should be obtained; we used the K-meanmethod as the procedure to group the participants. The moti-vational profiles were defined from the different combina-tions of the three types of goals assessed by the AGQ scale.To eliminate the effect resulting from differences in themeasurement of the goals (given that the number of items ofeach goal subscale is different), we carried out cluster analysisafter transfomiing the raw scores into standardized scores(z scores).

Once the subgroups of students with LD were identified,MANOVAs were conducted in which the dependent vari-ables were the dimensions of self-concept and attributionalprocesses, and the independent variable was the subgroupsof students established on the basis of their typical motiva-tional profile. When the analysis involved more than twogroups, post hoc Bonferroni tests were used to identifypairwise differences among the groups. Similarly, we per-formed various ANOVAs to verify differences amongthe motivational profiles on the three academic goals. Theeffect sizes were also calculated to determine the portion ofvariance of the dependent variable that was attributable tothe independent variable. Following the recommendationsof authors such as Tabachnick and Fidell (1989), we reportthe partial squared eta (r| ') . According to Aron and Aron(1999), the following criteria should be used: .10 = smalleffect size, .25 = medium effect size, and .40 = large effectsize.

Results

Motivational Profiles in Students With DA

As initially predicted, when carrying out the cluster analy-sis, we indicated that four groups of participants shouldbe extracted (and they should coincide with those obtained

1.0000 -

0.5000 -

S ^.5000 -

-2.0000 -

H Leaming goaisQ Performance goaisI I Soctai Reinforcement

IPSG

I ILowM MG

Group Profile

Figure I . Results of cluster analysis corresponding to the threesubscales from the Academic Goals QuestionnairePSG = profile vsiith prevalence of performance and social-reinforcement-seeking goals; Lov^M = profile of low motivation; MG = profile of multiplegoals; LG = profile with prevalence of learning goals.

Table 2. Final Cluster Centers Corresponding to the Groups of

Students With Learning Difficulties With PSG, LowM, MG, and

LG Motivational Profiles

Cluster

Learning goals

Performance goals

Social reinforcement goals

PSG

-.9628

-.0811

.0476

z Score

LowM

-1.3927-1.3560-1.8528

MG

.8022

.8567

.6847

LG

.1652-.5359-.1193

PSG = profile with prevalence of performance and social-reinforcement-seeking goals; LowM = profile of low motivation; MG = profile ofmultiple goals; LG = profile with prevalence of learning goals.

in previous investigations): a multiple goal profile, a lowmotivational level profile, a profile with a predominance ofleaming goals, and a profile with a predominance of extrin-sic goals.

After the analyses were performed, the four-cluster solu-tion (see Figure 1 ) converged in five iterations and optimallyfit the predictions of this study, based on studies of studentswithout LD. Table 2 shows the centers of the final clusters,and Figure 1 presents their graphic representations.

Cluster analysis thus allowed us to identify four groupscharacterized by significantly different motivational profiles.

Page 8: Learning Disability Quarterly

Núñez et al. 279

which are the result of different combinations of the fourtypes of academic goals. The first group was made up of47 students ( 18.3%; 35 boys and 12 girls) with a motivationalprofile with predominance of social-reinforcement-seekingand performance-approach goals (extemal orientation with asocial comparison referent) and with low levels of leaminggoals (scarcely interested in the acquisition of competenceand learning). This group is thus defined by a motivationalprofile with predominance of performance and social-reinforcement-seeking goals (PSG group).

The second group consisted of 36 students (14%; 27 boysand 9 girls) and was characterized by low scores in each oneof the goals assessed. This group was defined by a.profile withgeneralized low motivation (LowM group). If their behavioris ever motivated, they will probably engage in tasks for rea-sons other than those presented in the questionnaire to assessacademic goals.

The third group, composed of 106 students (41.2%;59 boys and 47 girls), was the largest and was characterizedby high scores in all three types of academic goals, whichindicates this is a multiple goal profile (MG group).

Last, the fourth group was made up of 68 students (26.5%;46 boys and 22 girls); they displayed a clear predominance ofleaming goals (interest in improving as they leam) and a lowlevel of perfonnance and social-reinforcement-seeking goals(LG group). Therefore, this group of students is defined by amotivational profile with predominance of learning goals.

We carried out a MANOVA that yielded statistically sig-nificant differences among the four groups in the three typesof goals that make up the profiles, I = .093, F(9, 611) =112.586, ;? < .001, r) = .548. These general level differencesare even higher if we compare the levels of each one of thethree goals in the four groups of subjects: leaming goals,F(3, 253) = 212.658, p < .001, TI = .716; perfonnance-approach goals, F(3,253) = 153.318,p < .001, r) = .645; andsocial-reinforcement-seeking goals, F(3,253)=180.122,/?<.001, r] = .681. Table 2 shows the means and standard devi-ations of the three types of goals for the four motivationalprofiles. The multiple comparisons were statistically sig-nificant in all cases, except for the comparison of the levelsof social-reinforcement-seeking goals corresponding to thePSG and LG profiles.

Motivational Profiles, Self-Concept,and Causal Attributions

In the third working hypothesis, we proposed that the fourmotivational profiles would be significantly associatedwith the dimensions of self-concept and causal attributions.Table 3 presents the means and standard deviations for thevariables of self-concept and causal attributions of the fourgroups of students.

We performed a MANOVA that yielded statistically sig-nificant differences among the four groups of students in the

eight measures of self-concept, ^ = .691, F(24, 714) = 4.051,p < .001, T] =. 116. Considering the results of each one of thedimensions separately, the data contributed by the multivari-ate analysis indicate that the differences are statistically sig-nificant in all the subscales (except for the two physicalsubscales): general self-concept, F(3, 253) = 6.128, /?<.001, r|" = .068; general academic self-concept, F(3, 253) =14.694, p < .001, ri" = .148; mathematical self-concept,F(3, 253) = 6.062, p=.OQ\,^^ = .067; verbal self-concept,F(3, 253) = 8.276, p < .001, n' = .089; relationship withparents, F(3,253) = 10.466,p < .001, V = .110; peer relation-ships, F(3,253) = 2.822,/? = .039, n' = .032; physical appear-ance, F(3, 253) = 1.721, ;? = .163, If = .020; and physicalcapacity, F(3, 253) = 1.280,p = .282, T]" = .015. The multiplecomparisons were not statistically significant in all possiblecases, except for the general dimensions and the general aca-demic dimension, in which most of the possible comparisonswere significant.

With regard to causal attributions, the results are fairlysimilar to those obtained for self-concept: statistically sig-nificant differences both at the multivariate level, X = .737,F(18, 701) = 4.446, p < .001, ri' = .097, and in each of thespecific dimensions measured by the SAS: attribution ofsuccess to capacity, F(3, 253) = 4.685, p = .003, TI' = .053;attribution of success to effort, F(3,253) = 53.586,/J < .001,ri' = .219; attribution of success to extemal cause, F(3, 253)= 2.912, p = .032,11^ = .034; attribution of failure to lack ofcapacity, F(3, 253) = 3.163, p = .025, x\- = .036; and attribu-tion of failure to lack of effort, F(3, 253) = 3.656,/; = .013,r^ = .042, except for the attribution of failure to extemalcauses, for which the differences did not reach statisticalsignificance, F(3, 253) = 0.521, /? = .668, r( = .006. Last,the multiple comparisons showed that the differences werestatistically significant when comparing the multiplegoal group with the group with predominance of social-reinforcement-seeking or perfonnance goals and low leam-ing goals, except for ASEF, where all the comparisons weresignificant.

Discussion

In accordance with our first and second hypotheses, wefound empirical evidence of the existence of consistentcombinations of diverse types of goals or motivationalorientations. As predicted, the results of this work clearlysupport the existence of four motivational profiles: a mul-tiple goal profile, a low motivational level profile, a profilewith predominance of leaming goals, and a profile with apredominance of extrinsic goals. Therefore, it can be con-cluded that students with LD behave like students withoutLD, insofar as regards motivational profiles. In fact, theseresults may not be at all random because the same fourmotivational profiles were found in independent investiga-tions with populations witli and without LD, from different

Page 9: Learning Disability Quarterly

280 Learning Disability Quarterly 34(4)

Table 3, Means and Standard Deviations for the Four Participant Groups on the Eight SDQ-I Scales, Three AGQ Subscales, and Six

SAS Subscales

Scale and Subscale

Self-concept (SDQ-I Scales)

General'

School"

Reading"

Mathematics"

Physical Appearance"

Physical Abilities"

Parent Relations"

Peer Relations"

Academic goals (AGQ Scale)Learning goals'

Performance goals^

Social reinforcement goals'Causal attributions (SAS Scales)

Attribution of Success to Ability (ASAB)*"Attribution of Success to Effort (ASEF)"*

Attribution of Success to External Causes (ASEC)**

Attribution of Failure to Ability (AFAB)''Attribution of Failure to Effort (AFEF)"*

Attribution of Failure to External Causes (AFEC)**

PSG

M

21.787

20.867

26.141

20.756

28.495

31.125

29.647

30.541

2.4233.219

4.039

24.142

31.663

31.761

30,177

31,81126.494

SD

4.318

6.298

8.712

8.432

7.292

6.485

7.3455.772

0.4167

0.73070.6218

5.7186.699

6.429

5.813

5.199

6.221

Low M

M

18.472

16.441

22.927

18.028

24.951

30.12327.686

26.889

2.031

1.902

2.143

21.81125.287

29.022

28.801

31.353

25.221

SD

5.542

5.247

9.4439.107

6.928

7.5316.186

5.854

0.59560.64910.7217

6.2946.689

7.472

5.908

5.938

6.695

MG

M

22.268

24.336

29.631

24.89326.799

28.819

33.585

29.609

4.0314.189

4.674

26.90136.879

28.304

26.784

28.751

25.137

SD

4.803

6.513

7.484

9.076

7.474

7.651

5.632

6.276

0.5328

0.50820.3899

8.2137.739

6.847

7.157

6.928

6.623

LG

M

22.083

21.852

24.729

22.545

26.415

30.154

32.252

29.769

3.451

2.7503.872

24.741

32.687

29.127

27.829

30.829

25.304

SD

4.5476.479

8.494

9.159

6.817

6.561

5.8825.664

0.3894

0.6664

0.6605

7.879

7.467

5.978

5.549

5.668

6.158

SDQ-I - Self-Description Questionnaire-, AGQ = Academic Goals Questionnaire; SAS = Sydney Attribution Scale: PSG = profile with prevalence of perfor-mance and social-reinforcement-seeking goals (n = 47); Lov^M = profile of low motivation (n = 36); MG = profile of multiple goals (n = 106); LG =profile with prevalence of learning goals (n = 68).a. Minimum = 6, maximum = 30.b. Minimum = 8, maximum = 40.c. Minimum = I, maximum = 5.d. Minimum = 10, maximum = 50.

school levels, and using different instruments to assess aca-demic goals (i.e., Daniels et al, 2008; Valle, Núñez, Cabanach,Rodriguez, González-Pienda, & Rosario, 2009).

Besides conflrming the existence of motivational profilesformed by the combination of various goals, these resultsalso reveal the possibility of profiles that integrate certaintypes of goals that, although theoretically counterpoised, maybe a resource and an adequate complement in some circum-stances for students with LD, even though this could lead tocoordination problems or even to incompatible goals. Thecombinations of goals mean we must accept that studentshave diverse reasons and motives for engaging in learning.From this perspective, students' motivation is seen as a pro-cess of managing multiple goals, which requires coordina-tion and effective matching of one's personal motives to thespeeifle demands of the learning context (Valle, Núñez,Cabanach, Rodriguez, González-Pienda, & Rosario, 2009).Moreover, each one of the combinations of goals identifiedrepresents a certain motivational profile, in the sense thatthey lead to a particular and different way of being moti-vated academically.

Nevertheless, adequate management of a broad spec-trum of motives is more complex and involves more diffl-culties when students face a cluster of goals that can easilycome into conflict under eertain eircumstances because oftheir peculiarities. There is usually no problem unless suchcircumstances involve some kind of comparison of a stu-dent's results with those of his or her classmates. However,as students who are oriented toward perfonnance goals tryto display competence or to avoid seeming incompetentto others—classmates and teachers—they may sometimesengage in behaviors that are opposed to successful learning(Gabriele, 2007). Thus, when academic results and the mean-ing they have for many students interact, in the sense thatthey are a good indicator of high or low capacities in com-parison to others, the expression of approach and avoid-ance tendencies (Sideridis, 2003, 2007)—to achieve positiveresults and avoid negative results—may entail obvious risksthat often lead to higher levels of anxiety, more fear of failure,and, as a result, poorer academic results. In fact, as pointed outby some authors (e.g., Kaplan & Maehr, 2007; Middleton,Kaplan, & Midgley, 2004), one of the possible problems of

Page 10: Learning Disability Quarterly

Núñez et al. 281

40.000 n

35.000-

30.000-

25.000-

20.000-

15.000.ASAB ASEF ASEC AFAB AFEF AFEC

Figure 2. Motivational profiles and dimensions of causalattributions in students with learning difficultiesPSG = profile with prevalence of performance and social-reinforcement-seeking goals, n = 47; LowM = profile of low motivation, n = 36;MG = profile of multiple goals, n = 106; LG = profile with prevalence of

learning goals, n = 68; ASAB = Attribution of Success to Ability;ASEF = Attribution of Success to Effort; ASEC = Attribution of Successto External Causes; AFAB = Attribution of Failure to Ability;AFEF = Attribution of Failure to Effort; AFEC = Attribution of Failureto External Causes. Pairs with significant differences: ASAB (LMP-MGP),ASEF (AGPP-LMP, AGPP-MGP, LMP-MGP, LMP-LGPP, MGP-LGPP),ASEC (AGPP-MGP). AFAB (AGPP-MGP), AFEF (AGPP-MGP). AFECwas not significant

performance-approach goals occurs when these goals—because of changes in the students' experiences, perceivedcompetence, or probability of failure—^become performance-avoidance goals (Valle, Núñez, Cabanach, Rodriguez,González-Pienda, Rosario, et al., 2009).

With regard to the third hypothesis of this investigation,in which we expected that the four motivational profileswould be associated with significantly different levels of self-concept and causal attributions, in general the data obtainedsupport these predictions.

According to most of the investigation carried out onmultiple goals in students with LD (i.e., Botsas & Padeliadu,2003; Sideridis, 2003, 2007), and in accordance with thehypothesis of interactive effects (Barron & Harackiewicz,2001), the multiple goals profile is the most adaptive moti-vational profile. Members of this group attribute their suc-cesses significantly more to intemal causes (capacity and,mainly, effort), and they attribute their academic failures, toa lesser extent, to these same causes (see Figure 2). Followingattributional logic, this is an adaptive attributional pattembecause it protects these students so they will persist whenfaced with a possible failure and will be more resilient to thedevelopment of emotional and affective problems (Núñezet al., 2005). The data about self-concept point in this samedirection (see Figure 3): These students display a good general

GSC RSC MSC PAPSC PABSC PARSC PERSC

Figure 3. Motivational profiles and self-concept dimensions instudents with learning difficultiesPSG = profile with prevalence of performance and social-reinforcement-seeking goals, n = 47; LowM = profile of low motivation, n = 36; MG =profile of multiple goals, n = 106; LG = profile with prevalence of learninggoals, n = 68; GSC = general self-concept; SSC = general school self-concept; RSC = reading self-concept; MSC = mathematics self-concept;PAPSC = physical appearance self-concept; PABSC = physical abilitiesself-concept; PARSC = parent relations self-concept; PERSC = peerrelations self-concept. Pairs with significant differences: GSC (AGPP-LMP, LMP-MGP, LMP-LGPP), SSC (AGPP-LMP,AGPP-MGR LMP-MGP,LMP-LGPP), RSC (LMP-MGP. MGP-LGPP), MSC (LMP-MGP), PARSC(AGPP-MGP, LMP-MGP, LMP-LGPP), PERSC (AGPP-LMP).

self-concept, a higher academic self-concept than any ofthe other three motivational groups, a higher family self-concept (positive relationship with parents), and a high socialself-concept (excellent peer relations). Consequently, we canconclude that having a multiple goals motivational profile(MGP group) ensures an attributional pattem and self-dimensions that are highly protective against future motiva-tional and emotional maladaptation.

The exact opposite is observed in students with a lowmotivational profile (LowM group), who display a maladap-tive pattem because (a) they hardly ever believe that theirsuccesses are the result of intemal or personal causes (capacityand/or effort), but instead think they are the result of exter-nal causes, and (b) they believe their failures are mainly theresult of their lack of capacity and effort. In addition, (c) theirgeneral self-concept is very negative, and their academicself-concept, family self-concept, and social self-concept areall low. Our data thus indicate that students with a LowMprofile are in a clear situation of vulnerability, so they aremuch more susceptible to future failures and to their cogni-tive, motivational, and affective consequences.

Between these two groups of students are those individu-als with a profile witli predominance of leaming goals and the

Page 11: Learning Disability Quarterly

282 Learning Disability Quarterly 34(4)

students with predominance of social-reinforcement-seekingand perfonnance goals. Although the post hoc analyses didnot reveal statistically significant differences between thesetwo groups in most of the attributional and self-conceptdimensions assessed, a detailed observation of Figures 2and 3 suggests that, of the two, the group with a predominanceof leaming goals (LG group) is the most similar to the multiplegoals profile (MG group), although somewhat less adapted.

These results are in accordance with most of those obtainedin past research (e.g., Barron & Harackiewicz, 2003; Pintrich,2003; Sideridis, 2003,2006; Valle, Núñez, Cabanach, Rodriguez,González-Pienda, & Rosario, 2009) because a multiple goalsprofile seems more adaptive than a profile with predomi-nance of learning goals, although the opposite patternhas also been found (Sideridis & Tsorbatzoudis, 2003).However, as not many differences in causal attributionprocesses and self-dimensions were found between stu-dents with a predominance of leaming goals and studentswith a predominance of social-reinforcement-seeking andperfonnance goals, this leads us to conclude that although theformer profile is more adaptive, the latter is not clearly vulner-able. Consequently, although some researchers initially advo-cated planning contexts that favor leaming goals in studentswith and without LD (e.g., Sideridis, 2005a), accordingto other researchers, learning contexts that favor perfor-mance goals (i.e., Sideridis, 2007) or social-reinforcement-seeking goals, in addition to these teaching structures, may alsobe adaptive. We agree with the latter suggestion. In fact, forstudents in general, and in particular for students with LD, itmay be a question of knowing the profile of the student'sneeds and his or her level of associated vulnerability and,accordingly, establishing motivational contexts that allow thestudent to engage academically with some chance of success.

Implications for Practice

Research of motivational profiles could have some relevantimplications from the educational viewpoint in general and forstudents with LD in particular. The analysis of goals providesa way to understand the dynamics of behavior as it develops ina certain situation as well as a framework frotn which to inte-grate the diverse sociocognitive variables and to understand theinfluence of students' cognitions, behavior, and adaptationsto the academic context.

The results of this type of investigation are also an impor-tant step forward in the attention to diversity, in this case,attention to motivational diversity. Just as students are dif-ferent in their knowledge and competences, they are alsodifferent in their motivational level. To accept these differ-ences means that the teacher should start with students' realmotives. And as we have seen in this investigation, thesemotives are multiple and diverse. Such attention to diversitymeans that the teacher has to work in that "zone of proximaldevelopment" (Brophy, 1998). Moreover, such diversity ofmotives can also mean that from the motivational viewpoint.

there are diverse ways to achieve leaming and academic suc-cess. Although it is true that some are more desirable thanothers, not all students must necessarily follow the samemotivational track, and this is especially tme in the case ofstudents with LD.

A teacher's capacity to adapt the academic activities andleaming contexts to the multiple motivational itineraries ofstudents with LD is one of the key elements to guaranteeingsatisfactory results from the motivational viewpoint. Futuremotivational research should study in depth these multipleitineraries and the ways that teachers can effectively matchthem.

Future Research

In recent years, the paradigm of research in the field of LDhas moved away from a model based on deficit toward aperspective that emphasizes the "capacity to adapt and over-come difficulties"—called the "risk and resilience model"—underlining self-esteem, motivation, effort, and persistenceas personal, central variables to explain the success of somestudents with LD (Meltzer, Reddy, Pollica, & Roditi, 2004;Wong, 2003).

Morrison and Cosden (1997) were among the first inves-tigators who applied the terms risk factors and protectionfactors to the field of research on LD. They argued that thepresence of a learning disability constitutes a risk factor forthe psychological well-being of the person who has one, butat the same time, diverse intrinsic and extrinsic factors are setoff to make the person more resilient to the negative effects. Incontrast to the deficit model (which underestimates students'capacity to grow and feel good because it concentrates ontheir deficits instead of their potential), under this new para-digm investigators focus all their energy on identifyingthe critical predictors than can tap students' true potentialfor leaming and prospering in diverse areas or contexts,despite their particular leaming difficulties (Damon, 2004;Murray, 2003).

In recent research based on this new perspective (i.e.,Cosden, Brown, & Elliot, 2002; Meltzer et al., 2004; Murray,2003; Núñez et al., 2005; Stone, Bradley, & Kleiner, 2002),various factors have been identified that contribute signifi-cantly to the characteristic behavior of a person who canadapt and overcome adverse conditions, among which areawareness of difficulties and skills, level of self-concept,capacity expectations, causal attributions, and degree ofperceived control over their achievements, their teachers'expectations and attributions, and their trust in the availabil-ity of social support (i.e., parents, peers, teachers). Therefore,first of all, more research is needed to attempt to determinehow these intemal factors (personal goals, self-esteem, causalattributions, capacity expectations, achievement expectations,etc.) interact with extemal factors (curricular and instructionaladjustments, family support, etc.) to lead to risk and/or pro-tection contexts for these students.

Page 12: Learning Disability Quarterly

Núñez et al. 283

Second, the results obtained in this study reveal the needto continue to investigate from the viewpoint of motivationalprofiles rather than only goals considered individually. Also,the students' response to the perceived requirements of thecontexts in interaction with their own needs and interests isimportant. Future research needs to take into account vari-ables such as gender, the structure of the academic tasks, orsome aspects of the family context (Meece, Glienke, &Burg, 2006). Some of the very interesting issues to be inves-tigated within this sphere could be the following: How dostudents prioritize their goals within the motivational pro-file? What is the relationship between inappropriate goalprioritization and adaptation to the academic context? Howdo unexpected events like failure, confiicts, and rejection bypeers or by teachers affect the goal profile? How can differ-ent goals be coordinated in different situations? Regardingthe last question, most of the difficulties could arise whenstudents (a) are not aware of the goal or goals that are mostappropriate to activate in each situation or (b) are incapableof maintaining the same goal profile in the face of adversityor confiict.

Third, a new line of investigation with great potential isthe one already initiated by some investigators (e.g., Sideridis,2003, 2006, 2007), which consists of using structural equa-tion models to obtain information about the link betweenvariables fi'om different settings (personal, contextual, task).This methodology may provide enough infonnation to estab-lish intervention models adapted to different groups of studentswith LD, depending on their competences and deficits, theirdiversity of motivations and affects, and—more important—the relationships among all of them.

Study Limitations

The results obtained and the conclusions derived therebyshould be considered with some caution for several reasons,among which we emphasize three. First, although the num-ber of participants is large, the results should be interpretedwith caution because there were considerable differences inthe age variable (which ranged between 8 and 15 years).The data may vary somewhat as a function of this variable.Second, the data derived from this investigation should beinterpreted prudently because some variables were not con-trolled (e.g., type of deficit that causes the LD—perceptive,attentional, working memory, linguistic, etc.—current aca-demic achievement, family attitude and involvement, schoolcontext), and this could significantly affect the results obtained,although the size of the sample and the variety of centers andplaces where the data were collected may attenuate the effectof these extraneous variables. Last, as noted by Hullemanet al. (2010), the conclusions derived from studies of aca-demic goals should be interpreted by taking into account thetype of instrument used to obtain the data. Therefore, weencourage investigators in the field of LD to replicate thisstudy with different scales fi-om the one used here to contrast

whether our results are mediated by tlie nature of the items ofthe AGQ or whether, as we expect, they may be generalized.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest withrespect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of thisarticle.

Funding

The author(s) received no financial support for the research,authorship, and/or publication of this article.

References

Al Otaiba, S., Petscher, Y., Pappamihiel, N. E., Willians, R. S.,Dyrlund, A. K., & Connor, C. (2009). Modeling oral readingfluency development in Latino students: A longitudinal studyacross second and third grades. Journal of Educational P.sy-chology,¡0¡,3\5-329.

Aron, A., & Aron, E. N. (1999). Statistics for psychology. UpperSaddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Barron, K. E.,& Harackiewicz, J. M. (2001). Achievement goalsand optimal motivation: Testing multiple goals models.Journal of Personality and Social Psvchology, 80, 706-722.

Barron, K, E,, & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2003). Revisiting the ben-efits of performance-approach goals in the college classroom:Exploring the role of goals in advanced college courses. Inter-national Journal of Educational Research, 35, 357-374.

Botsas, G., & Padeliadu, S. (2003). Goal orientation and readingcomprehension strategy use among students with and with-out reading difficulties. International Journal of EducationalResearch, 39,411-495.

Bouffard, T., Boisvert, J., Vezeau, C , & Larouche, C. (1995). Theimpact of goal orientation on self-regulation and perfonnanceamong college students. British Journal of Educational P.iy-chology, 65, 211-329.

Brophy, J. (1998). Motivating students to learn. New York, NY:McGraw-Hill.

Byrne, B.M. (1996). Measuring self-concept across the life span:Issues and instrumentation. Washington, DC: American Psy-chological Association.

Cosden, M., Brown, C, & Elliot, K. (2002). Development of self-understanding and self-esteem in children and adults with leam-ing disabilities. In B. Wong & M. Donahue (Eds.), The socialdimensions al Learning disabilities (pp. 33-52). Mahwah, NJ:Lawrence Erlbaum.

Damon, W. (2004). What is positive youth development? Annalsof the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 591,13-30.

Daniels, L. M., Haynes, T. L., Stupnisky, R. H., Perry. R. P.,Newall, N. E., & Peknm, R. (2008). Individual differences inachievement goals: A longitudinal study of cognitive, emo-tional, and achievement outcomes. Contemporary Educa-tional Psychology, 33, 584-608.

Elliot, A. J. (1997). Integrating the "classic" and "contempo-rary" approaches to achievement motivation: A hierarchical

Page 13: Learning Disability Quarterly

284 Leaming Disability Quarterly 34(4)

model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation.In M. L. Machr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motiva-tion and achievement (pp. 143-179). Greenwich, CT: JAI.

Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation andachievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34, 169-189.

Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. A. (1997). A hierarchical model ofapproach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal ofPersonality' and Social Psychology, 72, 218-232.

Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and avoidanceachievement goals and intrinsic motivation: A mediational analy-sis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 461^75.

Gabriele, A. J. (2007). The influence of achievement goals on theconstructive activity of low achievers during collaborativeproblem solving. British Journal of Educational Psychology,77,121-141.

González-Pienda, J. A., Núñez, J. C , & González-Pumariega, S.(1996). Motivación intrínseca versus extrínseca en niños con ysin dificultades de aprendizaje escolar [Intrinsic versus extrin-sic motivation in children with and without academic leamingdifficulties]. Magister, ¡4, 245-261.

González-Pienda, J. A., Núñez, J. C , González-Pumariega, S.,Alvarez, L., Roces, C , & Garcia, M. (2002). A structural equa-tion model of parental involvement, motivational and attitu-dinal characteristics and academic achievement. Journal ofExperimental Education, 70, 257-287.

González-Pienda, J. A., Núñez, J. C , González-Pumariega, S.,Alvarez, L., Roces, C , Garcia, M Valle, A. (2000). Self-concept, causal attribution process and academic goals in chil-dren with and without learning disabilities. Psicothema, 12,548-556.

Haager, D., Klingener, J., & Vaughn, S. (2007). Evidence-basedreading practices for response to intervention. Baltimore, MD:Brookes.

Hair, J. F, Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1998).Multivariate data analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., Carter, S. M., &Elliot, A. J. (2000). Short-term and long-term consequences ofachievement goals: Predicting interest and perfonnance overtime. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 316-330.

Hayamizu, T., & Weiner, B. (1991). A test of Dweck's model ofachievement goals as related to perceptions of ability. Journalof Experimental Education, 59, 226-234.

Hulleman, C. S., Schräger, S. M., Bodmann, S. M., &Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). A meta-analytic review ofachievement goal measures: Different labels for the same con-structs or different constructs with similar labels? Psychologi-cal Bulletin, ¡36, 422^449.

Inglés, C. J., García-Fernández, J. M., Castejón, J. L., Valle, A.,Delgado, B., & Marzo, J. C. (2009). Reliability and validityevidence of scores on the achievement goal tendencies ques-tionnaire in a sample of Spanish students of compulsory sec-ondary education. Psychology in the Schools, 46,1048-1060.

Jiménez, J. E., Rodriguez, C , Crespo, P., González, D., Artiles, C ,& Alfonso, M. (2010). Implementation of response to inter-

vention (Rtl) model in Spain: An example of a collaborationbetween Cañarían universities and the Department of Educa-tion of the Canary Islands. Psicothema, 22, 935-942.

Kaplan, A., & Maehr, M. L. (2007). The contributions and pros-pects of goal orientation theory. Educationai PsvchologyReview, 19, U\-\%4.

Kavale, K. A., Kaufïhian, A. S., Bachmeier, R. J., & LeFevers, G. B.(2008). Response-to-intervention: Separating the rhetoric of self-congratulation from the reality of specific leaming disability iden-tification. Learning Disabilities Quarterly, 31, 135-150.

Linnenbrink, E. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2002). Achievement goaltheory and affect: An asymmetrical bidirectional model. Edu-cational Psychologist, 37, 69-78.

Margalit, M. (2004). Second-generation research on resilience:Social-emotional aspects of children with leaming disabilities.Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 19, 45-48.

Margalit, M., & Idan, O. (2004). Resilience and hope theory: Anexpanded paradigm for leaming disabilities research. Thalamus,22, 58-65.

Marsh, H. W. (1988). Self-Description Questionnaire 1. San Antonio,TX: Psychological Corp.

Marsh, H. W., Caims, L., Relich, J., Bames, J., & Debus, R. ( 1984).The relationship between dimensions of self-attribution anddimensions of self-concept. Journal of Educational Psychology,76, 3-32.

McLaughlin, M. J., Dyson, A., Nagle, K., Thurlow, M., Rouse, M.,Hardman, M., . . . Perlin, M. (2006). Cross-cultural perspec-tives on the classification of children with disabilities. Journalof Special Education, 40,46-58.

Meece, J. L., Glienke, B. B., & Burg, S. (2006). Gender and moti-vation. Journal of School Psychology, 44, 351-373.

Meltzer, L. (2004). Resilience and leaming disabilities: Research onintemal and extemal protective dynamics. Introduction to the spe-cial series. Leaming Disabilities Research & Practice, 19, 1-2.

Meltzer, L., Reddy, R., Pollica, L. S., & Rodítí, B. (2004). Academicsuccess in students with leaming disabilities: The roles of self-understanding, strategy use, and effort. Thalamus, 22,16-32.

Míddleton, M., Kaplan, A., & Mídgley, C. (2004). The change inmiddle school students" achievement goals in math over time.Social Psychology of Education, 7,289-311.

Meltzer, L. J., Rodítí, B., Houser, R. F, & Perlman, M. (1998).Perceptions of academic strategies and competence in studentswith leaming disabilities. Jbur/ia/q/'Z,earn/>igDi.sa6i7/i(e5. 31,437-451.

Míddleton, M., & Mídgley, C. (1997). Avoiding the demonstrationof lack of ability: An unexplored aspect of goal theory. Journalof Educational Psychology, ¡99, 710-718.

Morrison, G., & Cosden, M. (1997). Risk, resilience and adjustmentof individuals with leaming disabilities. Leaming DisabilityQuarterly, 20,43-60.

Murray, C. (2003). Risk factors, protective factors, vulnerability,and resilience. Remedial and Special Education, 24, 16-26.

Núñez, J. C , & González-Pienda, J. A. (2007). School psychologyin Spain. In S. R. Jimerson, T. D. Oakland, & P. T. Farrell (Eds.),

Page 14: Learning Disability Quarterly

Núñez et al. 285

The handbook of intemational school psychology (pp. 381-390),Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage,

Núñez, J. C , González-Pienda, J. A., González-Pumariega, S.,Roces, C , Alvarez, L., González, P . , . . . Rodríguez, S. (2005).

• Subgroups of attributional profiles in students with learningdifficulties and their relation to self-concept and academicgoals. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 20, 86-97.

Núñez, J. C , González-Pumariega, S., & González-Pienda, J. A.(1995). Self-concept in children with and without learning dis-abilities. Psicothema, 1, 587-604.

Pastor, D., Barron, K, E,, Davis, S, L,, & Miller, B. J. (2004,April). College students ' achievement goal orientation pro-files. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educa-tional Research Association, San Diego, CA.

Pintrich, P R. (2000a). Multiple goals, multiple pathways: Therole of goal orientation in learning and achievement. Journalof Educational Psychology, 92, 544—555.

Pintrich, P. R. (2000b). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner(Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp, 451-502), San Diego,CA: Academic Press,

Pintrich, P. R. (2003). A motivational science perspective on therole of student motivation in learning and teaching contexts.Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 667-686.

Pintrich, P R., Anderman, E. M., & Klobucar, CH. (1994). Intra-individual differences in motivation and cognition in studentswith and learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabili-ties, 27, 360-370.

Settle, S. A., & Milich, R. (1999). Social persistence followingfailure in boys and girls with LD. Journal of Learning Dis-abilities, 32, 20\-2\2.

Sideridis, G. D. (2003). On the origins of helpless behaviour ofstudents with learning disabilities: Avoidance motivation?International Journal of Educational Research, 39. 497-517.

Sideridis, G. D. (2005a). Classroom goal structures and hope-lessness as predictors of day-to-day experience at school:Differences between students with and without learningdisabilities. International Journal of Educational Research,43, 308-328.

Sideridis, G. D. (2005b). Goal orientations, academic achieve-ment, and depression: Evidence in favour of revised goal the-ory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 366-375.

Sideridis, G. D. (2005c). Social, motivational, and emotional aspectsof learning disabilities. Editors' introduction to special issue.Intemational Journal of Educational Research, 43, 209-214.

Sideridis, G. D. (2006). Achievement goal orientations, "oughts,"and self-regulation in students with and without learning dis-abilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 29, 3-18.

Sideridis, G. D. (2007). Why are students with LD depressed?A goal orientation model of depression vulnerability. Journalof Learning Disabilities, 40, 526-539.

Sideridis, G. D., & Tsorbatzoudis, C. (2003). Intra-group motiva-tional analysis of students with leaming disabilities: A goal

orientation approach. Learning Disabilities: A ContemporaryJoumal,}, S-19.

Stone, C. A., Bradley, K., & Kleiner, J. (2002). Parental under-standing of children with language leaming disabilities and itsrole in the creation of scaffolding opportunities. In B. Wong& M. Donahue (Eds.), The social dimensions of learning dis-abilities (pp. 133-160). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L, S. (1989). Using multivariatestatistics (2nd ed.). New York: Harper & Row.

Valle, A., Cabanach, R. G., Núñez, J. C , González-Pienda, J. A.,Rodriguez, S., & Pineiro, I. (2003). Multiple goals, motivationand academic leaming. British Journal of Educational P,iv-chology,73,l\-Sl.

Valle, A., Núñez, J.C., Cabanach, R.G., González-Pienda, J.A.,Rodriguez, S., Rosario, P., Cerezo, R. y Muñoz-Cadavid, M.A,(2008). Self-Regulated Profiles and Academic Achievement.Psicothema, 20, 4, 724-731.

Valle, A., Núñez, J. C , Cabanach, R. G., Rodriguez, S,, González-Pienda, J. A., & Rosario, P. (2009). Perfiles motivacionales enestudiantes de Secundaria: Análisis diferencial en estrategiascognitivas, estrategias de autorregulaeión y rendimiento aca-démico [Motivational profiles in secondary students: Differ-ential analysis of cognitive and self-regulation strategies andof academic achievement]. Revista Mexicana de Psicología,26, 113-124.

Valle, A., Núñez, J. C , Cabanach, R. G., González-Pienda, J. A.,Rodriguez, S., Rosario, P., . . . Cerezo, R, (2009), Academicgoals and leaming quality in higher education students. SpanishJournal of Psychology, 12, 96-105.

Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., Zhang, H., & Schatschneider, C.(2008). Using response to kindergarten and first grade inter-vention to indentify children at-risk for long-term reading dif-ficulties. Reading and Writing: .4n Interdisciplinary Journal,27,437^80.

Villalobos, M. V, González-Pienda, J. A., Núñez, J. C, & Mújica, A. D.(1997, March). Sydney Attribution Scale: Facture structureand psychometric properties in Chilean students. Presented atthe Portuguese-Spanish Congress of Educational Psychology,Coimbra, Portugal.

Wolters, C. A., Yu, S. L., & Pintrich, P R. (1996). The relationbetween goal orientation and students' motivational beliefsand self-regulated learning. Leaming and Individual Differ-ences, 8,21 \-23S.

Wong, B. (2003). General and specific issues for researchers' con-sideration in applying the risk and resilience framework to thesocial domain of leaming disabilities. Leaming DisabilitiesResearch & Practice, 18, 68-76.

About the Authors

José Carlos Núñez Pérez, PhD is a Leaming DisabilitiesProfessor in Psychology Department at the University of Oviedo.Research lines focus about motivational and emotional compo-nents in leaming disabilities and self-regulation processes.

Page 15: Learning Disability Quarterly

286 Learning Disability Quarterly 34(4)

Julio Antonio González-Pienda, PhD, is an EducationalPsychology Professor in Psychology Department at the Universityof Oviedo. Research lines focus in thinking styles and leamingdisabilities.

Celestino Rodríguez, PhD. is an Educational Psychology andLearning Disabilities Assistant Professor in PsychologyDepartment at the University of Oviedo. Research lines focus inADHD and leaming disabilities.

Antonio Valle, PhD, is an Educational Psychology Professor inEducational and Development Psychology Department at the

University of Coniña. Research lines focus in academic motiva-tion, self-regulation processes and leaming disabilities.

Ramón Gonzalez-Cabanach, PhD, is an Instructional PsychologyProfessor in Educational and Development PsychologyDepartment at the University of Coruña. Research lines focus inself-regulation processes, learning disabilities and stress.

Pedro Rosario, PhD, is an Educational Psychology AssistantProfessor in Psychology Department at the University de Minho.Research lines focus in self-regulation processes and schoolfailure.

Page 16: Learning Disability Quarterly

Copyright of Learning Disability Quarterly is the property of Council for Learning Disabilities and its content

may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express

written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.