21
OPYRIGHT NOTICE: | | | | You may forward this document to anyone you think might be interested. | | | | The only limitations are: | | A) You must copy this document IN ITS ENTIRETY, WITHOUT MODIFICATIONS, | | including this copyright notice. | | You do NOT have permission to change the contents or make extracts. | | B) You do NOT have permission to copy this document for commercial | | purposes. | | | | The contents of this document are copyright (c) 1993 by The American | | Society for Psychical Research. | | | | It was posted on the University of California at Davis ftp server by | | permission of the copyright holder. This ftp server contains ASCII | | files of published articles by Professor Charles T. Tart. Individuals | | wishing to obtain other documents there (which are added to from time | | to time) should | | Connect to ftp server, "ftp.ucdavis.edu". | | Log in as username "anonymous". Send your e-mail address | | as the ident/password string. | | cd to /pub/fztart. | | A "dir" command will show you what is available. | | A "get" command will retrieve documents. | | The file "currentcontents" will be updated regularly, showing | | what papers are available, perhaps with an abstract of each. | |_____________________ ________________________ ____| Marijuana Intoxication, Psi and Spiritual Experiences Charles T. Tart University of California Davis, California 95616 & Institute of Noetic Sciences Sausalito, California 94965 Published in the "Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research," volume 87, pp. 149-170, *word* indicates italicization in printed version -------------------------------------- Abstract General social beliefs that are acquired and operate in our ordinary state of consciousness usually deny the reality of psychic phenomena and thereby probably inhibit psychic functioning. Anecdotal reports, however, suggest that ost ensibly paranormal phenomena often occur in association with altered states of consciousness. This study focuses on the altered state of marijuana intoxication. A ques tionnaire study of 150 experienced marijuana users found that 76% believed in ESP, with frequent reports of experiences while intoxicated that were interpreted as psychic . Sixty nin e percent reported t hat they

Marijuana and Psi

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 1/21

OPYRIGHT NOTICE: || || You may forward this document to anyone you think might be interested. || || The only limitations are: || A) You must copy this document IN ITS ENTIRETY, WITHOUT MODIFICATIONS, || including this copyright notice. || You do NOT have permission to change the contents or make extracts. || B) You do NOT have permission to copy this document for commercial || purposes. || || The contents of this document are copyright (c) 1993 by The American || Society for Psychical Research. || || It was posted on the University of California at Davis ftp server by || permission of the copyright holder. This ftp server contains ASCII || files of published articles by Professor Charles T. Tart. Individuals || wishing to obtain other documents there (which are added to from time || to time) should || Connect to ftp server, "ftp.ucdavis.edu". || Log in as username "anonymous". Send your e-mail address || as the ident/password string. || cd to /pub/fztart. || A "dir" command will show you what is available. |

| A "get" command will retrieve documents. || The file "currentcontents" will be updated regularly, showing || what papers are available, perhaps with an abstract of each. ||__________________________________________________________________________|

Marijuana Intoxication, Psi and Spiritual Experiences

Charles T. Tart

University of CaliforniaDavis, California 95616

&Institute of Noetic SciencesSausalito, California 94965

Published in the "Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research,"volume 87, pp. 149-170,

*word* indicates italicization in printed version

--------------------------------------Abstract

General social beliefs that are acquired and operate in ourordinary state of consciousness usually deny the reality ofpsychic phenomena and thereby probably inhibit psychicfunctioning. Anecdotal reports, however, suggest that ostensiblyparanormal phenomena often occur in association with alteredstates of consciousness. This study focuses on the altered stateof marijuana intoxication. A questionnaire study of 150experienced marijuana users found that 76% believed in ESP, withfrequent reports of experiences while intoxicated that wereinterpreted as psychic. Sixty nine percent reported that they

Page 2: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 2/21

had experienced telepathy while intoxicated, 32% reportedprecognition and 13% reported psychokinesis. Fifty percent hadexperienced seeing auras around people and 44% reportedout-of-the-body experiences. These findings suggest thatmarijuana, used under the proper psychological conditions, mightfacilitate the manifestation of psi. No studies are known inwhich ESP performance was tested under laboratory conditionswhile percipients were intoxicated with marijuana, but a 1975study (Tart, 1975; 1976) found a positive correlation betweenlaboratory ESP scoring and frequency of marijuana use outside thelaboratory in a student population. This study also found anegative correlation between ESP scoring and frequency of alcoholuse in everyday life. A 1977 laboratory study (Tart, 1977)failed to confirm these findings. Differences between thestudies are discussed, as is the importance of the ostensibleparanormality of various experiences associated with marijuanaintoxication on belief systems, irregardless of whether suchexperiences are actually paranormal.

--------------------------------------

Ordinary life experiences that apparently involvemanifestations of psi (i.e., telepathy, clairvoyance,precognition, or psychokinesis) can be of great psychological

intensity and meaning, sometimes to the point of producing anexperience of insight and spiritual blessedness, at one extreme,,or suffering, fear of going crazy, and maladaptive behavior atthe other extreme. Psychological help may occasionally berequired. In the past and, unfortunately, still too often in thepresent, such help was often irrelevant or worsened the client'sstate. This was due to professional ignorance about psi and theautomatic interpretation of ostensible psi experiences aspathological delusions. The recent founding of the SpiritualEmergence Network (see note 1) (Grof & Grof, 1989), which is designed toeducate mental health professionals and provide referrals forpotential clients, is a useful effort to improve the situation1.

The average manifestation of psi in laboratory experiments,by contrast, is usually unreliable and quantitatively weak. Thisis a major problem in experimental parapsychology: thesignal-to-noise ratio is psi research is disappointingly low.Thus I and others have long been interested in the possible usesof *discrete altered states of consciousness* (*d-ASCs*); see Tart,1983a for a full theoretical explication of the nature of d-ASCsboth to facilitate and understand the operation of various psi

 _______________________________ Note 1 The Spiritual Emergence Network (formerly the SpiritualEmergency Network) can be contacted at their national headquarters at 250 Oak Grove Avenue, Menlo Park, CA 94025, 415-327-2776.I urge all mental health professionals to become affiliated with

the Network, as concerns about psychic and spiritual matters arebecoming increasingly widespread.abilities. I have written extensively on these issues (Tart,1967, 1968, 1970a, 1970b, 1974, 1977, 1978, 1980, and 1983b), ashave others (see, for example, Honorton, 1974; 1975; Honorton &Krippner, 1969; Kelly & Locke, 1981; Krippner, 1975; LeShan,1974; Masters, 1974; Mishlove, 1983; Parker, 1975; Schmeidler,1982, 1988; Ullman & Krippner, 1970; Wookey, 1982). See Pekala(1991) for a review of recent empirical research on measuring theeffects of altered states.

Page 3: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 3/21

------------------------------end of footnote

Hypnosis, meditation-induced d-ASCs, drug-induced d-ASCs,and controlled or guided emotional states all seem to bepotential candidates for psi facilitation. Facilitation of theoperation of psi abilities involves helping them manifest morereliably and at stronger intensities of functioning. Suchfacilitation would have the potential of leading to betterunderstanding because more reliable psi would allow more fruitfulprocess experimentation. Better understanding might also comeabout through *state-specific understandings* of psi functioningresulting from the development of *state-specific sciences* (Tart,1972a). These would be testable understandings dependent on thealtered logics and perceptions of some d-ASCs, which are notfully comprehensible in our ordinary state.

This paper will discuss ostensibly psychic experiencesassociated with marijuana use, considering both theirpsychological impact and the possible use of marijuanaintoxication for facilitating psi performance under laboratoryconditions.

Drug Induced Altered States

Psychoactive drugs are convenient means of inducing d-ASCs.Although there is much intriguing speculation on thepossibilities for using psychoactive drugs for inducing psi,numerous anecdotal reports, and many reviews of the generalliterature on psychedelics (Blewett, 1963; Cavanna & Ullman,1968; Clark, 1967; Dobkin de Rios, 1984a; Dobkin de Rios, 1984b;Drury, 1984; Fair, 1975; Garrett, 1961; Kern, 1964; Krippner,1964; Krippner & Davidson, 1976; Long, 1976; Nicol & Nicol, 1961;Osmond, 1961; Smythies, 1960, 1983; Wasson, 1962; Wilson, 1949),there is only a sparse, older experimental literature ondeliberately or experimentally using major psychedelics likeLSD25 to facilitate psi functioning (Cavanna & Servadio, 1964;

Masters & Houston, 1966; Osis, 1961; Paul, 1966; Puharich, 1962;van Asperen de Boer, Barkema, & Kappers, 1966; Vasiliev, 1965;Whittlesey, 1960). This experimental literature has beenreviewed by Krippner and Davidson (1974), with a recent note bySmythies (1987). The tremendous variability of reactions to thepowerful psychedelics, however, combined with a lack ofsophisticated knowledge on *guiding* psychedelic experiences atthat early stage of research, produced results that wereencouraging but not solid. A mild psychedelic such as marijuana,which can be used in a relatively controlled and reliable way bypsychologically balanced people, offers more promise. Marijuanaintoxication is also an excellent candidate for the developmentof a state-specific science. This latter aspect, however, falls

outside the scope of the present paper.

Alcohol is the most popular (and probably the most generallydangerous) recreational drug in our culture, having been tried atleast once by 86% of Americans (164 million people in our 1985population of 191 million). Alcohol is reported to have beenused within the last 30 days by 59% of the population (NationalInstitute of Drug Abuse, 1986). Marijuana, in spite ofcontinuing illegality, is the second most popular recreationaldrug, having been tried at least once by 32% of the population

Page 4: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 4/21

and used within the last 30 days by 10%. Thus, somewhere between18 million and 62 million people have varying degrees offamiliarity with the d-ASC that typically results from marijuanause.

Marijuana's effects are of interest to experimentalparapsychology insofar as it has any psi-facilitating potential.In terms of its interest to the evolving field of clinicalparapsychology, marijuana experiences may foster illusions aboutpsychic phenomena as a result of normal experiences beingmisinterpreted during the intoxicated state as psychic, or it mayfacilitate genuine psychic experiences, often of an intensevariety. In either case, these experiences, mistaken or genuine,must be integrated into a person's life. Some ostensible psiexperiences may also arouse normally unconscious fears about psi(Tart, 1982, 1986a; Tart & LaBore, 1986). The techniques (Tart,1984) for acknowledging and dealing with fears of psi may becomerelevant, as well as other clinical methods.

In this paper, the focus is on ostensible psi and spiritualexperiences occurring with marijuana intoxication in populationsof marijuana *users,* that is people who use marijuana in everydaylife without its use being, by their own report, significantlydisruptive to their general happiness and adaptation. The

additional clinical issues arising in marijuana *abusers*, whoseuse is part of a maladaptive psychological addiction, are beyondour present scope.

The Altered State Induced by Marijuana

Marijuana is what Weil (1972) has called an *active placebo*.That is, there is a definite pharmacological effect due to thechemical nature of the drug, but at the same time psychologicaland situational factors are so important in determining what thenature and content of the intoxicated state is that marijuana, atlow to moderate doses, is like a placebo, with no particulareffects at all other than those psychologically induced. The

pharmacological effects create a range of *possibilities*; thepsychological and situational factors determine which of thesepossibilities are likely to manifest. These non-drug factors arediscussed at length elsewhere (Becker, 1967; Tart, 1971a; Weil,1972).

When I became interested in the nature of the d-ASC producedby marijuana intoxication (popularly referred to as being*stoned*), I first reviewed the laboratory studies of marijuana.It became clear that such studies involved a narrow andspecialized selection of psychological and situational factors,so that only a fraction of the range of possible altered mentalfunctioning with marijuana occurred in most laboratory settings.

Further, this selection seemed atypical of what people ordinarilyexperienced when using marijuana in settings of their choice.Further, the laboratory studies had certainly not attempted tofoster a state conducive to psi functioning.

To examine the fuller and more typical range of marijuanaeffects, I conducted informal interviews with a number of highlyeducated users in the late 1960s. This led to the development ofa 220-item formal questionnaire. It was distributed informallyby giving batches to students and associates and asking that they

Page 5: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 5/21

pass them on until they ended up in the hands of experiencedusers, who could then mail them back anonymously. "Experienced"was defined on the questionnaire as having used marijuana atleast a dozen times.

Questions were of the general form, *During the stateproduced from using marijuana I experience X, more so than if Ihad not used marijuana.* Users were asked to rate the frequencyof each described effect during their previous six months of usein categories of Never, Rarely, Sometimes, Very Often, andUsually. They were also asked to rate the minimal level ofintoxication necessary to experience each phenomenon incategories of Just, Fairly, Strongly, Very Strongly, and Maximum.These categories were defined more clearly in the instructions.A subsequent confirmation study (Tart & Kvetensky, 1973) hasshown this experiential rating of thresholds for effects to bereliable.

Some illustrative questions are:

*I can see new colors or more subtle shades of colorthan when I'm straight.

When listening to stereo music or live music, the

spatial separation between the various instrumentssounds greater, as if they were physically furtherapart.

I get so lost in fantasy or similar trips in my headthat I completely forget where I am, and it takes awhile to reorient after I come back and open my eyes.*

There were 153 questionnaires returned, which representapproximately 37,000 episodes of marijuana intoxication underordinary life, non-laboratory conditions. The respondents weremainly Californians (67%), with 87% under 30 years of age. Sixtyseven percent were students, 71% were unmarried, and there were

twice as many male as female respondents. Their educationallevel was very high, with 21% having some graduate work oradvanced degrees and only 7% having no college training. Manywere actively interested in self-improvement, with 36% reportingthat they practiced some sort of meditative or other spiritualtraining method. Frequency of marijuana use ranged from "almostevery day" for 19% of the respondents to "less than once a week"for 39%. Most (72%) had tried more powerful psychedelic drugssuch as LSD at least once, but only 7% had tried hard narcoticsor drugs such as amphetamines: these drugs were considered toodangerous to be worthwhile by most of the educated drug cultureat that time.

Fourteen of the questionnaire items comprised a validityscale that contained invented experiences that had not beenreported by the interviewees and that I considered unlikely tooccur, such as, *The force of gravity seems to alternate betweenpushing me up and pushing me down*. Three questionnaires withmore than 6 positive responses on the validity scale werediscarded because they probably were filled out carelessly. Thisresulted in 150 questionnaires remaining for analysis.

This sample is not necessarily representative of the general

Page 6: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 6/21

population, of course, and thus generalization of the resultsmust be cautious. Further, there have been major changes in thedrug culture since these data were collected in 1970 that alsoqualify the results. Drug users in 1970 tended to be highlyintelligent and educated rebels and visionaries, interested inself-knowledge and improving themselves and the world. Havingimportant insights into oneself and others was one of the mostcharacteristic effects of the marijuana d-ASC in this group, forexample. Using marijuana was not a mass fad yet.

Once it became a fad, many people who were not thatinterested in psychology or self-improvement per se started usingmarijuana because it was the thing to do. Their motivation wasprobably quite different. Given the active placebo nature ofmarijuana discussed above, differing motivations and backgroundswould probably lead to significantly different qualities of thed-ASC produced by marijuana. For example, my informants in theinitial interviews indicated that they did not use alcohol at thesame time that they used marijuana because it dulled the d-ASCproduced by marijuana (see note 2). Today studies show that simultaneousmarijuana and alcohol use is common, suggesting many people areseeking escape rather than insights. Hochhauser (1977), forexample, surveying 365 undergraduates found that of the 42% whoadmitted to polydrug use, 84% used marijuana and alcohol in

combination. I shall return to the issue of the changing natureof the marijuana using population later.

Nevertheless, I believe my 1970 respondents are fairlytypical of the kind of person who may volunteer for psychological

 _______________________________ Note 2 With the exception of using small amounts of wine for itsenhanced taste, not for its intoxicating effects.and parapsychological testing, namely college students andcollege-educated people with a strong curiosity about the mindwho are seeking various kinds of self-improvement. Such peoplealso represent a subclass of clients often seen by counselors andpsychotherapists. Thus, the findings of the 1970 study, applied

thoughtfully in individual cases, can still be useful today.------------------------------end of footnote

The general results of the study, as well as details of theanalytical procedure, have been presented briefly (Tart, 1970b)and in detail (Tart, 1971a) elsewhere. They constitute aphenomenological description of the general nature of the d-ASCresulting from marijuana intoxication as it was widely practicedby educated people in our culture in the late 1960s. I willhighlight the effects relevant to experimental and clinicalparapsychology in this article. These have been partiallypresented in a more popular form elsewhere (Tart, 1971b).

Belief in Extrasensory Perception

Forty-six percent of the respondents indicated strongagreement to the question, *I believe in the existence ofextrasensory perception (ESP), i.e., that people can sometimesacquire knowledge about things happening at a distance in spaceor time, or about other people's thoughts, when there is nopossibility of this knowledge having been acquired through theknown senses (sight, hearing, etc.).* Another 30% believed

Page 7: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 7/21

somewhat, and only 3% disbelieved strongly (see note 3).

This total of 76% who believe in ESP is quite high. In1973, when there was considerably more overt cultural acceptanceof ESP than in 1970, a representative poll of the American

 _______________________________ Note 3 This question had its own response scale, not the one listedearlier. Note also that percentages given will not always add upto 100% due to occasional skipping of questions by respondentsand rounding errors.------------------------------end of footnote

population by the National Opinion Research Center of theUniversity of Chicago found that 58% of people believed they hadpersonally experienced ESP (Greeley, 1975). This is not the samequestion as *belief* in ESP, of course, but is the closestavailable in survey data. The belief that one had personallyexperienced some kind of ESP had risen to 67% in a 1987 poll("Polls Indicate," 1987).

Telepathy

To the question, *I feel so aware of what people are thinkingthat it must be telepathy, mind reading, rather than just being

more sensitive to the subtle cues in their behavior*, 30%responded that they had never experienced this and 22% rarely,but 31% responded Sometimes, 12% Very Often, and 4% Usually.Higher levels of intoxication were usually indicated as thethreshold for this effect, and heavy users indicated theyexperienced it more frequently than moderate or light users (see note 4).Marijuana users who had also used psychedelic drugs did not haveto be as intoxicated to believe they had experienced telepathywhile intoxicated with marijuana.

Precognition

Experiencing apparent precognition was a much rarer

phenomenon than telepathy. To the question *I can foretell thefuture by some kind of precognition, more than just predictinglogically from present events*, 64% responded Never, 19% Rarely,11% Sometimes, and only 1% Very Often and 1% Usually. Becausemost users had never experienced this, there were few ratings ofminimal level of intoxication, but those who did rate the level

 _______________________________ Note 4 All the differences I will report from this study werestatistically significant at the .05 level or less, two-tailed.considered precognition a high level effect. Heavier usersreported precognition more frequently than lighter users.------------------------------end of footnote

The comparative rarity of ostensible precognitionexperiences compared with ostensible telepathic ones isinteresting in light of a later finding that the frequency andinformation transmission rate of precognition in laboratorystudies is much lower than for present-time telepathy (Tart,1983b). I do not believe that such a difference is reflected inspontaneous psi cases.

Psychokinesis

Page 8: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 8/21

Ostensible psychokinesis (PK) was reported even more rarelythan ostensible precognition. In response to *I can performmagical operations that will affect objects or people whileintoxicated with marijuana*, 83% indicated they had neverexperienced this, 6% Rarely, 6% Sometimes, 1% Very Often, and noone Usually. The few who rated threshold level indicated PK didnot occur until very high levels of intoxication were reached.Users were also asked to describe experiences in this category.Responses suggested that the usual parapsychological concept ofPK was not communicated well, so responses to this questionshould be taken guardedly. Nevertheless, it is interesting thatboth ostensible precognition and PK are reported much lessfrequently than ostensible telepathy by marijuana users; thisparallels a finding that precognition and PK appear at asignificantly lower rate of manifestation than present time ESP(telepathy or clairvoyance) in laboratory studies (Tart, 1983c).

Auras

The aura is a field of colored light sometimes experiencedas outlining a person, and it is often reported to change inresponse to a person's health or emotional state. Although it isundoubtedly an experiential reality for some people, mostparapsychologists would put it in the "maybe but almost no

evidence to that effect" category as to whether it has anyparapsychological reality. The methodology for research to beginto establish whether there are any parapsychological aspects ofthe aura has been presented elsewhere (Tart, 1972b), but almostno research has been done.

Some kind of aura around people is occasionally experiencedby marijuana users when intoxicated with marijuana. *I seefringes of colored light around people (not objects), what peoplehave called the "aura"* was reported Never by 50%, Rarely by 23%,Sometimes by 19%, Very Often by 5%, and Usually by 1%. A closelyrelated phenomenon, *I see fringes of colored light around objects(not people), what people have called the "aura,"* is reported

with almost identical frequency (46%, 21%, 20%, 8%, 1%). Thesimilarity in frequency suggests we are dealing primarily, if notexclusively, with a change in the way the nervous systemprocesses visual information. Nevertheless, experiencers mayinterpret this aura as a psychic phenomenon, and perhaps it is attimes. Both of these effects have a high level of intoxicationthreshold.

Out-of-the-Body Experiences

*Have you ever had the experience of being"located" outside your physical body, i.e., of *you*being at a different location in space than the one you

knew your body was at? Dreams aren't included here, orsituations where you just lost consciousness of yourbody. This is where you consciously feel located at adifferent place and know at the time that you areconscious but at a different location. Has thishappened to you? At all? While stoned? Happenedbefore started smoking grass? Happened after startedsmoking grass?*

Although 53% indicated that they had not had an out-of-body

Page 9: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 9/21

experience (OBE), 23% reported they had experienced one OBE, and21% reported that they had experienced multiple OBEs. Threepercent did not answer this question. Fewer males reported OBEsthan females, but males were more likely to report multiple OBEs.Respondents more involved in personal growth tended to reportmore OBEs. Some of the OBEs were while intoxicated withmarijuana, others not, but more than twice as many usersindicated their OBEs had occurred after they had started usingmarijuana as those who indicated their OBEs had preceded use. Arelated marijuana effect is *I have lost all consciousness of mybody and the external world and just found myself floating inlimitless space (not necessarily physical space).* Twenty-ninepercent report this Rarely, 30% Sometimes, 10% Very Often, and 4%Usually.

OBEs are probably more common in our culture than suspected,but only a few small-scale surveys have been conducted. In themost comprehensive, a mail survey of students at the Universityof Virginia and townspeople in Charlottesville, where theuniversity is located, Palmer (1979) found the 25% of thestudents and 14% of the townspeople believed they had experiencedat least one OBE. Thus the marijuana-using respondents in thissurvey show a very high incidence of ostensible OBEs. Marijuanause, then, may either induce OBEs and/or make a person more

likely to remember or report them.

Reactions to having an OBE range from worrying that one iscrazy (especially if there is no cultural support for theexperience) to considering it of profound religious significance(see Gabbard & Twemlow, 1984). The recent literature onnear-death experiences also illustrates this latter point clearly(see, e.g., Moody, 1975, 1977; Ring, 1980, 1984; Sabom, 1982).

An experience reported by one of the respondents illustratessome of the classical features of OBEs (see note 5).

I had quite an interesting experience while

camping. I got stoned on grass, and as I was about togo to sleep, I came completely awake and aware of mysurroundings. It was pitch black in the tent, yet Icould see as if it were daylight. I felt as if my bodywere covered with eyes and I could see in alldirections. I slowly floated up through the top of thetent, looking at the whole area. I got farther away,moving towards space. I got very realistic views ofthe earth. I kept moving up until I could see half ofthe earth, then the earth and the moon, continuing

 _______________________________ Note 5 The respondent reported this under the spiritual experiencessection of the questionnaire rather than the ESP portion.

------------------------------end of footnote

until I stood at the edge of space, inspecting thewhole universe.

I was all of a sudden struck by man'sinsignificance. Then I proceeded to move until I couldsee hundreds of universes glinting like stars. None ofthese universes was any larger than the head of a pin.It was incredibly beautiful. I began laughing almosthysterically because now our own universe, immense as

Page 10: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 10/21

it seems to us, was no bigger than the head of a pinand one among millions besides.

I described the whole experience as it happened toseveral other people; and I believe, from the reactionI got, I thoroughly scared the hell out of them.

There were a number of important spiritual experiencessurveyed in the questionnaire, considered parapsychological ininexact popular usage of the term, that should be mentioned,although they are not strictly parapsychological in the modern,scientific use of the term. By "spiritual" I mean experiencesthat seem to take the experiencers beyond the boundaries of theirordinary selves and physical limitations and connect them with aGreater Reality of intelligence, meaning, and purpose.

Contact With the Divine

Although 39% had never experienced contact with the divinewhile intoxicated with marijuana, most of my 1970 respondentsanswered positively to *I feel in touch with a Higher Power or aDivine Being to some extent when intoxicated with marijuana; Ifeel more in contact with the "spiritual" side of things.* Thiswas rated Usual for 10%, Very Often for 12%, Sometimes for 24%,and Rarely for 13%. This effect tends to occur at higher levels

of intoxication.

Spiritual Experiences

One third of the respondents replied Yes to the question, *Ihave spiritual experiences, discrete experiences which have had apowerful, long-term religious effect on me, while stoned.* Thoseresponding positively were asked to briefly describe suchexperiences. The primary components reported were a sense ofunity with the cosmos, stimulation of a long-term interest inspiritual matters, contact with divine beings, long term andpositive changes in lifestyle, and a sense of deep peace and joy.These qualities are typical of nondrug induced mystical

experiences (Pahnke, 1966; Pahnke & Richards, 1969).

Marijuana and Psi in the Laboratory

Whenever people are polled about their ostensible psychicexperiences, problems of definition and reporting arise. Doesthe respondent have the same criteria for ruling out ordinaryexplanations, thus qualifying an event as ostensibly psychic, asthe researcher does? Is the respondent accurate in reporting?

These problems are particularly acute when ostensiblepsychic events occurring during periods of marijuana intoxicationare asked about, as there are major changes in styles of

perception, emotion, and evaluation (Tart, 1970b, 1971a) as wellas possible problems in the way an event was remembered for laterreporting and evaluation. An event that might appearunimportant, coincidental, or explicable by ordinary means to anon-intoxicated person might appear important and mysterious tothe intoxicated person. Thus the experience might be falselyevaluated as psychic. Contrariwise, an event that is actuallypsychic, but might be misperceived as ordinary by anon-intoxicated person, might be correctly perceived as psychicby an intoxicated person.

Page 11: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 11/21

These confusions will often be unimportant in dealingclinically with a person who is emotionally affected by what heor she considers psychic, irregardless of the actual case. It isof theoretical interest to the clinician, however, as well as ofgreat practical importance to the laboratory worker wondering ifmarijuana intoxication might boost psi performance, to determineif the apparent boost of psi by marijuana is real. Ideally,straightforward laboratory experimentation with percipientsintoxicated at times and not intoxicated at other times wouldprovide a clear answer. Given the illegality of marijuana, thepoor funding in parapsychology, and the bureaucratic and socialcomplexities of doing research with marijuana, however, I do notanticipate any laboratory research on this in the immediatelyforeseeable future. I do have some less direct research materialthat bears on these questions, however.

Results From The 1975 Confirmation Study

In the fall of 1975, John Palmer, Dana Redington, myexperimental psychology class students, and I carried out a largestudy on the effects of immediate feedback on ESP performance inthe laboratory, based on a theory that immediate feedback wouldeliminate the typical declines found in laboratory ESP work and

provide an opportunity for some percipients to learn improved ESPperformance (Tart, 1966). An initial study had supported thesepredictions (Tart, 1975, 1976).

The overall results of the entire study have been presentedelsewhere (Tart, Palmer & Redington, 1979). Briefly, 1835 UCDavis students were given two paper and pencil tests of generalESP (GESP) ability in their classes. Results of this classroomprocedure are described fully elsewhere (Palmer, Tart, &Redington, 1976). The more successful student percipients wereinvited to participate in six individually supervised laboratorytests of GESP ability, which constituted the Confirmation Study.Students who continued to show strong signs of positive GESP

performance in the Confirmation Study were invited to work in amore extensive Training Study.

In the Confirmation Study, a percipient did four tests onone kind of testing machine and two on another. The *Aquarius*4-choice machine task (Targ & Hurt, 1972) called for pushing oneof four response buttons to try to identify a target numberselected by the machine's electronic random number generator.The selected target number was displayed to an experimenter in adistant room, who tried to "send" it to the student percipient.The 10-Choice training machine (*TCT*) was similar, but with 10target choices.

In the 1975 Confirmation Study, 72 students completed atleast one run on the Aquarius 4-choice machine. Overall, theyshowed significant ESP hitting (1,554 hits for 5,951 trials when1,487.75 hits were expected by chance, p < .05, one-tailed).Although statistically significant, the results represented arelatively low level of psi functioning, with a *psi coefficient*(Timm, 1973) of .01; that is, psi was only being manifested onabout one percent of the trials, after chance hitting is factoredout. This low level of functioning is, unfortunately, typical inparapsychological studies.

Page 12: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 12/21

The 73 students who completed at least one run on the TCTmade 467 hits in 4,520 trials, 15 more hits than expected bychance, which was not significant. Low or zero amounts ofoverall ESP in a group such as this decrease the sensitivity ofcorrelational tests to pick up real relationships, but they donot destroy it completely. A group average not significantlydifferent from chance may conceal genuine ESP that is canceledout at the group level by very high and very low scoringpercipients.

As far as we knew, student percipients were not intoxicatedwith marijuana or any other drug during the laboratory testing.To test whether prior use of marijuana, major psychedelic drugs,and/or alcohol use might be related to GESP scoring in spite ofthe low level results we distributed a questionnaire about druguse to participants in the Confirmation Study. Through anelaborate coding system, we were able to correlate theirresponses with their Confirmation Study GESP scores whileprotecting their anonymity. Fifty-five student percipientsreturned completed questionnaires.

The drug-use questionnaires asked 14 questions. Question 1concerned personal experience with marijuana on a 4-choice scale,

ranging from never to more than a dozen times. Question 2 asked:"Do you regularly practice any sort of meditation or othernon-drug discipline for spiritual or personal growth?". Question3 inquired about the length of time marijuana or hashish has beenused. Questions 4 and 5 asked for ratings of marijuana use inall the time the respondent had used it and in the last 6 months,in the same question form that the original 1970 marijuana studyasked. Question 6 asked what percentage of the time a respondentwould choose alcohol over marijuana to alter his state ofconsciousness if both were freely available. Question 7 askedhow many times a more powerful psychedelic such as LSD,mescaline, peyote, psilocybin, dimethyltryptamine (DMT) ordiethyltryptamine (DET) had been used. Question 8 asked how long

alcohol had been used to get tipsy or drunk (rather than just fortaste with meals), and Questions 9 and 10 rated frequency of useover total time and the last 6 months for alcohol, in the sameform as for marijuana. Questions 11, 12, 13 and 14 reproducedthe telepathy, precognition, magic, and out-of-body experiencequestions from the original study.

Although these student respondents had been selected forpotential GESP ability, not on the basis of their drug use, theyhad an extensive history of drug use. Although 40% had nevertried marijuana and 82% had never used one of the more powerfulpsychedelic drugs, 44% had used marijuana at least a dozen timesand 78% had used alcohol to get tipsy or drunk at least once.

The marijuana users averaged 4 years of use, over a range of onemonth to 10 years, the alcohol users averaged 3, years of useover a range of 1 month to 13 years.

The results of this study were most intriguing and mostfrustrating. The intriguing result is that Palmer, Redington,and I all clearly recall (personal communications with each,October, 1986) that we found that students who used marijuanamoderately to heavily, but seldom used alcohol, scoredsignificantly above chance in the Confirmation Study. Further,

Page 13: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 13/21

students who were heavy alcohol users scored significantly belowchance. We decided not to write this result up at the time as wethought it best to wait for confirmatory results in futurestudies we hoped to carry out. The frustrating aspect of thisresult was that lack of funds for further research broke up ourresearch team before this planned joint research, and in thechaos of moves resulting neither I nor my colleagues can findcopies of the analyses our memory is based on, nor can we locateenough of the original data to present detailed results. Thus,these findings about marijuana and alcohol use and GESPperformance must fall in a category of "better than an anecdotebut less than a proper experiment." I would normally not publisha finding based on memory, but the virtually total lack of dataon this important question justifies recording our jointrecollection of what occurred.

Results From The 1977 Confirmation Study

Working with just my UC Davis students, I was able to carryout a similar study 2 years later in the fall of 1977. Followinga large scale Selection Study with new students, an identicaldrug-use questionnaire was returned by sixty students in aConfirmation Study design that was similar to the previous oneexcept that all the testing for the drug questionnaire part of

the study was collected on a newer version of the TCT, the *ADEPT*(Advanced Decimal Extrasensory Perception Trainer). Six tests of20 trials each were done with each student percipient.

As with the TCT results of the previous study, there was noevidence of ESP for the overall group, with 747 hits occurring in7,200 trials when 720 were expected by chance. Although thesensitivity of correlational tests is decreased because of theoverall chance results, analyses were carried out because theseare the only data that I know of bearing on the question ofmarijuana use and laboratory psi performance.

The 1977 percipients, like the 1975 ones, had been selected

for potential GESP ability, not drug use, yet again showedextensive drug experience. Seventy-seven percent of them hadused marijuana from 1 month to 12 years, about 4.5 years on theaverage. This was about 7 months longer on the average than theyhad used alcohol for altering their state of consciousness.Sixty percent had tried one of the more powerful psychedelics atleast once, compared to 18% in the 1975 sample.

Our remembered positive correlation in the 1975 ConfirmationStudy between GESP scoring and marijuana use, and the negativecorrelation between GESP scoring and alcohol use, received noconfirmation in this 1977 sample. Spearman rank ordercorrelations between total marijuana use and GESP were close to

zero, although there was a small negative, but insignificant,correlation between frequency of marijuana use over the last 6months and GESP scoring (R = -.25, t[36] = 1.52, p < .20,two-tailed). The correlation between GESP performance andprevious experience with major psychedelic drugs wasinsignificant (R = .14). Correlations with alcohol use all ranaround an insignificant -.10.

I did find three significant correlations with GESPfunctioning, however. The more frequently a percipient

Page 14: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 14/21

self-reported experiencing ostensible telepathy, precognition, ormagical effects while intoxicated with marijuana, the lower theirGESP score in the laboratory. The Spearman rank ordercorrelation coefficients were -.47, -.45, and -.33, respectively,the first two significant at p < .01, two-tailed and the thirdsignificant at p < .05, two-tailed. As the probability offinding two correlations significant at the .01 level in 19 testsis less than .02, these results are unlikely to be an artifact ofmultiple testing (see note 6).

 _______________________________ Note 6 The growth practice item, being binary, was not suitable fortesting with the Spearman test. It was tested by a Mann-WhitneyU-Test and showed no significant difference in GESP scores ofthose who did or did not report practicing meditation or someother non-drug growth practice.------------------------------end of footnote

Comparing the Studies

The recalled results of the 1975 sample, a positivecorrelation between marijuana use and GESP, and a negative onebetween alcohol use and GESP received no support from the 1977sample. Indeed, the results of the 1977 sample could be

interpreted to mean that ostensible psychic experiences whileintoxicated with marijuana, because they are negatively relatedto actual laboratory GESP performance, may well be illusory muchof the time. This may be the case, but I will suggest someadditional considerations to flesh out the picture.

Because of the illegality of marijuana, the attitude ofrebellion connected with using it, and the rapidly changingfashionability of marijuana use in the time periods underconsideration, there may be significantly different populationsof users in the 1970, 1975, and 1977 samples. In 1968, forexample, an annual survey of college freshmen sponsored by theAmerican Council on Education, showed that fewer than one in five

students supported legalization of marijuana, but by 1977 amajority (53%) favored it (Astin, Green, & Korn, 1987). That

year was a high point of support, as it had fallen back to 22% in1985. A similar peaking of approval of marijuana in high schoolstudents in 1975 to 1978, followed by a decline, was reported byJohnston, O'Malley, and Bachman (1986). An occasional survey ofattitudes toward marijuana in upper division psychology studentsin his classes by Sommer at UC Davis found similar changes(Sommer, 1988).

My data allows some comparison of the 1970, 1975 and 1977samples. The 1977 sample of marijuana users (who were not

selected for being marijuana users but for promise of GESPability) differed from the original users in the 1970 study intheir reported ostensible psychic experiences. The 1977respondents reported ostensible telepathic experiences whileintoxicated with marijuana significantly more frequently (p< .05, two-tailed, by chi-square test) than the original sample,viz. 17% Never, 43% Rarely, 17% Sometimes, 20% Very Often, and 3%Usually. The 1977 respondents also reported significantly lowerminimal intoxication thresholds than the 1970 respondents (p< .02, two-tailed, by chi-square test) for experiencing

Page 15: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 15/21

ostensible telepathy (10% Just, 45% Fairly, 27% Strongly, and 17%Very Strongly).

The 1975 sample (for whom we recall a significant positivecorrelation of GESP scores and marijuana use and a significantnegative one with alcohol use) did not differ from the original1970 sample in terms of frequency of ostensible telepathicexperiences while intoxicated with marijuana, but did indicate asignificantly lower minimal intoxication threshold forexperiencing this (p < .03, two-tailed, by chi-square test). The1975 and 1977 samples did not differ significantly from eachother on these two measures, but it should be noted that thesmall sample sizes involved in comparing these two reduce thesensitivity of the tests.

The 1977 respondents indicate a higher frequency ofostensible precognition and of magical operations whileintoxicated with marijuana, with lower minimal thresholds forthese phenomena, compared to the original 1970 sample, but thesetrends do not reach statistical significance. Comparisonsbetween the 1975 and 1977 samples are not practical here due tolow numbers.

Another alternative interpretation of the apparent negative

correlation between laboratory psi performance and frequency ofostensible psi experiences while intoxicated with marijuana isone suggested by Osis (personal communication, August 1990).People who believe they have strong psychic ability may be moredefensive about being tested in the laboratory, and theirconsequent anxiety may inhibit psi performance. Along the samelines, people who have experienced a variety of psi experiencesmay find the narrowness of laboratory tasks an insult to theirabilities and so not perform (Swann, 1987). Further, theostensible psi experiences self-reported with marijuanaintoxication may be genuine but state-specific (Rossi, 1987;Tart, 1972a). The particular constellations of abilities helpfulto psi functioning operating in the d-ASC of marijuana

intoxication may not transfer well to the ordinary state ofconsciousness. Finally, it must be noted that the 1975 sampleactually showed statistically significant (albeit small) ESPfunctioning in the laboratory, whereas the 1977 sample did not.The 1977 sample may have been more deluded about the actualparanormality of their ostensible psi experiences than the 1975sample. These alternative interpretations are subject toempirical test in principle, although the practical difficultiesof obtaining the necessary legal approvals and support forlaboratory research with marijuana are unlikely to be overcome atthis time.

Conclusions

The empirical data reported here are, of course, quite preliminary, much of the data being abstracted from a larger,general study of marijuana intoxication per se. The findingsshould be taken only as suggestive until more extensive empiricalstudies validate or disprove them. Given the unlikelihood ofsuch studies with our current political and social climate, Ihave presented the material, in spite of its preliminary nature,for its inherent interest: To my knowledge, it is all we have.

Page 16: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 16/21

In looking at the development of ordinary, "normal"consciousness from a transpersonal perspective (Pearce, 1973,1974; Tart, 1983a, 1986b), a main function of ordinary *consensus*consciousness is to selectively shape perception, thinking,feeling, and behavior to a narrow, integrated range that embodiesthe myths, hopes, fears, wisdom, and survival strategies of theparticular culture a person is born in. This specialization hassurvival value in many ways, but can ultimately be detrimental toindividuals and societies because the automatization of mentalprocesses involved and emotional attachment to and defense ofthese automated patterns of perception and reaction inhibit thecapacity to deal with a changing world in a flexible, adaptivemanner. In the more educated portion of our Western culture,this shaping and conditioning frequently includes a rejection ofclassical religious beliefs and experiences and a rejection ofpsi phenomena.

Altered states appeal to people for many reasons, but theone of most importance to us here is that they provide glimpsesor moments of escape from the constrictive forces of consensusconsciousness, pointing out possibilities of growth, maturation,adventure, and pleasure for individuals and society. Contactwith a Supreme Being or similar experiences can take a personfrom a confused, depressed state of ordinary existence and make

his or her world full of hope and meaning, for example. An OBE,as a second example, especially if it is a component of anear-death experience, usually convinces the experiencer that heor she need not fear death, that death is not an end but anopening into a new world of joyful possibilities.

But our common cultural conditioning is to dismiss suchexperiences as imaginary at best, as avoidance of the hard factsof material reality, and, at worst, as signs of craziness. Thisis why ostensible psi experiences, whether genuine or not, arepsychologically important. Something *impossible* happens,"impossible" given the belief structure of a purely materialworld that excludes psi. The conceptually impossible event can

be interpreted to mean that the restrictions of possibilities inthe consensus consciousness belief structure may be a common*belief*, but not an absolute limit. We may indeed havepossibilities more in line with a spiritual view of humankindthan with a strictly materialistic view. Thus ostensible psiexperiences are important to many in contemporary culture becausethey provide validation for a more spiritual view.

I have qualified psi experiences with the word ostensible inthe above paragraph to focus on the effects of such experienceson psychological attitudes and beliefs per se. Some ostensiblepsi experiences are, of course, genuine psi experiences, meetingscientific criteria of being inexplicable by any reasonable

extension of normal scientific, materialistic theories. Theoccurrence of highly evidential psi events of that sort makes theabove argument more powerful.

Living in difficult, dis-spirited times, then, many peoplewill deliberately seek spiritual and psychic experiences.Marijuana use will continue to be an important vehicle in thissearch, even though many other methods, drug and non-drug, willbe employed. Further understanding of the effects of marijuanaon consciousness will be of value, then, in promoting the

Page 17: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 17/21

manifestation of psi in the laboratory, in aiding clients tounderstand and integrate ordinary life and marijuana-inducedostensible psi and spiritual experiences, and in understandingthe nature of the human mind.

References

Astin, A., Green, K., & Korn, W. (1987). "The American Freshman:Twenty-Year Trends, 1966-1985". Los Angeles: HigherEducation Research Institute, UCLA.

Becker, H. S. (1967). History, culture and subjectiveexperience: An exploration of the social bases ofdrug-induced experiences. "Journal of Health and SocialBehavior, 8", (3), 163-176.

Blewett, D. (1963). Psychedelic drugs in parapsychologicalresearch. "International Journal of Parapsychology, 5"(1),43-74.

Cavanna, R., & Servadio, E. (1964). "ESP Experiences with LSD25and Psilocybin: A Methodological Approach"(Parapsychological Monographs No. 5). New York:Parapsychology Foundation.

Cavanna, R., & Ullman, M. "(Eds.)" (1968). "Psi and Altered Statesof Consciousness: Proceedings of an International Conference

on Hypnosis, Drugs, Dreams, and Psi." New York:Parapsychology Foundation.Clark, W. H. (1967). Religious aspects of the psychedelicsubstances and the law . "International Journal ofParapsychology,, 9"(1), 32-36.

Dobkin de Rios, M. (1984a). "Hallucinogens: Cross-CulturalPerspectives". Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press,1984.

Dobkin de Rios, M. (1984b). The vidente phenomenon in thirdworld traditional healing: An Amazonian example. "MedicalAnthropology", (Win), 60-70.

Drury, N. (1984). "Vision-Quest: A Personal Journey Through Magicand Shamanism". Chalmington, England: Prism, 1984.

Fair, J. D. (1975). "An Investigation of the Relationship BetweenParanormal Phenomena and Altered States of Consciousness".Unpublished doctoral dissertation, United StatesInternational University, 1975.

Gabbard, G. O. & Twemlow, S. W. (1984). "With the Eyes of theMind: An Empirical Analysis of Out-of-Body States." NewYork: Praeger.

Garrett, Eileen J. (1961). Patterns of clairvoyance."Proceedings of Two Conferences on Parapsychology andPharmacology", pp. 14-16. New York: ParapsychologyFoundation.

Greeley, A. M. (1975). "The Sociology of the Paranormal". BeverlyHills: Sage.

Grof, S., & Grof, C. (Eds.). (1989). "Spiritual Emergency: WhenPersonal Transformation Becomes a Crisis". Los Angeles: J.P. Tarcher.

Hochhauser, M. (1977). Alcohol and marijuana consumption amongundergraduate polydrug users. "American Journal of Drug andAlcohol Abuse, 4"(1), 65-76.

Honorton, C. (1974). Psi-conducive states of awareness. In E.D. Mitchell & J. White (Eds.), "Psychic Exploration: AChallenge for Science" (pp. 616-639). New York: Putnam's.

Honorton, C. (1975). ESP and altered states of consciousness.

Page 18: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 18/21

In J. Beloff (Ed.), "New Directions in Parapsychology" (pp.38-59). Metuchen: Scarecrow Press.

Honorton, C., & Krippner, S. (1969). Hypnosis and ESPperformance: A review of the experimental literature."Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research, 63",214-252.

Johnston, L., O'Malley, P., & Bachman, J. (1986). "Drug Use AmongAmerican High School Students, College Students, and OtherYoung Adults: National Trends Through 1985." Rockville, MD:National Institute of Drug Abuse.

Kelly, E. F. & Locke, R. G. (1981). "Altered States ofConsciousness and Psi: An Historical Survey and ResearchProspectus." (Parapsychological Monograph No. 18.) NewYork: Parapsychology Foundation.

Kern, M. D. (1964). The University of California ExtensionDivision Liberal Arts Conference: Seminar on psychicalresearch and the psychedelic drugs. "Journal of the AmericanSociety for Psychical Research, 58", 75-76.

Krippner, S. (1964). The hypnotic trance, the psychedelicexperience, and the creative act . "American Journal ofClinical Hypnosis, 7", 140-147.

Krippner, S. (1975). "The Song of the Siren: A ParapsychologicalOdyssey". New York: Harper & Row.

Krippner, S., & Davidson, R. (1974). Paranormal events occurring

during chemically-induced psychedelic experience and theirimplications for religion. "Journal of Altered States ofConsciousness, 1," 175-184.

Krippner, S., & Davidson, R. (1970). Religious implicationsof paranormal events occurring during chemically-induced"psychedelic" experience. "Pastoral Psychology, (Sep), 21",27-34.

LeShan, L. (1974). Psychic phenomena and mystical experience.In E. Mitchell & J. White (Eds.), "Psychic Exploration: AChallenge for Science" (pp. 571-576). New York: Putnam's.

Long, J. K. (1976). Shamanism, trance, hallucinogens, andpsychical events: Concepts, methods, and techniques forfieldwork among primitives. In A. Bharati (Ed.), "Realm of

the Extra-Human: Agents and Audiences" (pp. 300-313). TheHague: Mouton.Masters, R. E. L. (1974). Consciousness and extraordinaryphenomena. In E. Mitchell & J. White (Eds.), "PsychicExploration: A Challenge for Science" (pp. 598-615). NewYork: Putnam's.

Masters, R., & Houston, J. (1966). "The Varieties of PsychedelicExperience". New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Mishlove, J. (1983). "Psi Development Systems". Jefferson:McFarland.

Moody, R. (1975). "Life After Life?". Atlanta, GA: MockingbirdBooks.

Moody, R. (1977). "Reflections on Life After Life". Atlanta, GA:

Mockingbird Books.National Institute of Drug Abuse. (1986). "NIDA Capsules".Nicol, J. F., & Nicol, B. H. (1961). Experimental uses ofchemical compounds. In "Proceedings of Two Conferences onParapsychology and Pharmacology" (pp. 27-29). New York:Parapsychology Foundation.

Osis, K. (1961). A pharmacological approach to parapsychologicalexperimentation. In "Proceedings of Two Conferences onParapsychology and Pharmacology" (pp. 74-75). New York:Parapsychology Foundation.

Page 19: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 19/21

Osmond, H. (1961). Variables in the LSD setting. "Proceedings ofTwo Conferences on Parapsychology and Pharmacology" (pp.33-35). New York: Parapsychology Foundation.

Pahnke, W. (1966). Drugs and mysticism. "International Journalof Parapsychology, 8", 295-320.

Pahnke, W. N., & Richards, W. A. (1969). Implications of LSD andexperimental mysticism. In C. T. Tart (Ed.), "Altered Statesof Consciousness: A Book of Readings" (pp. 399-428). NewYork: Wiley.

Palmer, J. (1979). A community mail survey of psychicexperiences. "Journal of the American Society for PsychicalResearch, 73", 221-251.

Palmer, J., Tart, C. T., & Redington, D. (1976). A large-sampleclassroom ESP card-guessing experiment. "European Journal ofParapsychology, 1", (3), 40-56.

Parker, A. (1975). "States of Mind: ESP and Altered States ofConsciousness." New York: Taplinger.

Paul, M. A. (1966). Two cases of altered consciousness withamnesia apparently telepathically induced. "PsychedelicReview, 8", 4-8.

Pearce, J. C. (1971). "The Crack in the Cosmic Egg." New York:Julian.

Pearce, J. C. (1974). "Exploring the Crack in the Cosmic Egg."New York: Julian.

Pekala, R. (1991). "Quantifying Consciousness: An EmpiricalApproach". New York: Plenum Press.Polls Indicate (1987). Polls indicate paranormal experienceswell on their way to becoming normal. "Brain/Mind Bulletin,12", (7), 1, 5.

Puharich, A. (1962). "Beyond Telepathy". Garden City: Doubleday.Ring, K. (1980). "Life at Death: A Scientific Investigation ofthe Near-Death Experience". New York: Coward, McCann &Geoghegan.

Ring, K. (1984). "Heading Toward Omega: In Search of the Meaningof the Near-Death Experience". New York: Wm. Morrow.

Rossi, E. 1987). From mind to molecule: A state-dependentmemory, learning and behavior theory of mind-body healing.

"Advances, 4", No. 2, 46-60.Sabom, M. B. (1982). "Recollections of Death: A MedicalInvestigation". New York: Harper & Row.

Schmeidler, G. R. (1982). Psi and states of consciousness."Theta, 10", 6-9.

Schmeidler, G. R. (1988). "Parapsychology and Psychology: Matchesand Mismatches". Jefferson, NC: McFarland.

Smythies, J.R. (1960). New research frontiers inparapsychology and pharmacology. "International Journal ofParapsychology, 2"(2), 28-38.

Smythies, J.R. (1983). The impact of psychedelic drugs onphilosophy and psychical research. "Journal of the Societyfor Psychical Research, 52", 194-200.

Smythies, J. (1987). Psychometry and mescaline. "Journal of theSociety for Psychical Research, 54", 266-268.

Sommer, R. (1988). Two decades of marijuana attitudes: The moreit changes, the more it is the same. "Journal ofPsychoactive Drugs, 20", No. 1, 67-70.

Swann, I. (1987). "Natural ESP: The ESP Core and Its RawCharacteristics". New York: Bantam.

Targ, R., & Hurt, D. (1972). Use of an automatic stimulusgenerator to teach extrasensory perception. "ProceedingsIEEE International Symposium of Information Theory".

Page 20: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 20/21

Tart, C. T. (1966). Card guessing tests: Learning paradigm orextinction paradigm? "Journal of the American Society forPsychical Research, 60", 46-55.

Tart, C. T. (1967). Psychedelic experiences associated with anovel hypnotic procedure, mutual hypnosis. "American Journalof Clinical Hypnosis, 10", 65-78.

Tart, C. T. (1968). Hypnosis, psychedelics and psi: Conceptualmodels. In R. Cavanna & M. Ullman (Eds.), "Psi and AlteredStates of Consciousness" (pp. 24-41). New York:Parapsychology Foundation.

Tart, C. T. (1970a). Did I really fly? Some methodologicalnotes on the investigation of altered states ofconsciousness and psi phenomena. In R. Cavanna (Ed.), "PsiFavorable States of Consciousness: Proceedings of anInternational Conference on Methodology in Psi Research" (pp.3-10). New York: Parapsychology Foundation.

Tart, C. T. (1970b). Marijuana intoxication: Common experiences."Nature, 226", 701-704.

Tart, C. T. (1971a). "On Being Stoned: A Psychological Study ofMarijuana Intoxication". Palo Alto, CA: Science and BehaviorBooks.

Tart, C. T. (1971b). ESP and pot. "Psychic, 3", (2), 26-30.Tart, C. T. (1972a). States of consciousness and state-specificsciences. "Science, 176", 1203-1210.

Tart, C. T. (1972b). Concerning the scientific study of thehuman aura. "Journal of the Society for Psychical Research,46", 1-21.

Tart, C. T. (1974). On the nature of altered states ofconsciousness, with special reference to parapsychologicalphenomena. In W. G. Roll, R. L. Morris & J. D. Morris(Eds.), "Research in Parapsychology 1973" (pp. 163-218).Metuchen: Scarecrow Press, 1974.

Tart, C. T. (1975). "The Application of Learning Theory to ESPPerformance." (Parapsychological Monographs No. 15). NewYork: Parapsychology Foundation.

Tart, C. T. (1976). "Learning to Use Extrasensory Perception".Chicago IL: University of Chicago Press.

Tart, C. T. (1977). Drug-induced states of consciousness. In B.Wolman (Ed.), "Handbook of Parapsychology" (pp. 500-525). NewYork: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

Tart, C. T. (1978). Psi functioning and altered states ofconsciousness: A perspective. In B. Shapin & L. Coly(Eds.), "Psi and States of Awareness" (pp. 180-210). NewYork: Parapsychology Foundation.

Tart, C. T. (1980). Using altered states of consciousness tofacilitate or study psi: Some methodological suggestions(Summary). In W. G. Roll (Ed.), "Research in Parapsychology1979" (pp. 11-12). Metuchen, N.J.: Scarecrow Press.

Tart, C. T. (1982). The controversy about psi: Twopsychological theories. "Journal of Parapsychology, 46",

313-320.Tart, C. T. (1983a). "States of Consciousness". El Cerrito, CA:Psychological Processes. (Original work published 1975.)

Tart, C. T. (1983b). Information acquisition rates inforced-choice ESP experiments: Precognition does not work aswell as present-time ESP. "Journal of the American Societyfor Psychical Research, 77", 293-310.

Tart, C. T. (1983c). Laboratory PK: Frequency of manifestationand resemblance to precognition (Summary). In W. G. Roll,J. Beloff & R. A. White (Eds.), "Research in Parapsychology

Page 21: Marijuana and Psi

7/31/2019 Marijuana and Psi

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/marijuana-and-psi 21/21

1982" (pp. 101-102). Metuchen: Scarecrow Press.Tart, C. T. (1984). Acknowledging and dealing with the fear ofpsi. "Journal of the American Society for PsychicalResearch, 78", 133-143.

Tart, C. T. (1986a). Psychics' fears of psychic powers. "Journalof the American Society for Psychical Research, 80", 279-292.

Tart, C. T. (1986b). "Waking Up: Overcoming the Obstacles toHuman Potential". Boston: New Science Library.

Tart, C. T., & Kvetensky, I. (1973). Marijuana intoxication:Feasibility of experiential scaling of level. "Journal ofAltered States of Consciousness, 1", 15-21.

Tart, C. T., & LaBore, C. (1986). Attitudes toward stronglyfunctioning psi: A preliminary survey. "Journal of theAmerican Society for Psychical Research, 80", 163-173.

Tart, C. T., Palmer, J., & Redington, D. J. (1979). Effects ofimmediate feedback on ESP performance: A second study."Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research, 73",151-165.

Timm, U. (1973). The measurement of psi. "Journal of theAmerican Society for Psychical Research, 67", 282-294.

Ullman, M., & Krippner, S. (1970). "Dream Studies and Telepathy"(Parapsychological Monographs No. 11). New York:Parapsychology Foundation.

van Asperen de Boer, S. R., Barkema, P. R., & Kappers, J. (1966).

Is it possible to induce ESP with psilocybin? Anexploratory investigation. "International Journal ofNeuropsychiatry, 2", 447-473.

Vasiliev, L. L. (1965). "Mysterious Phenomena of the Human Psyche"(Sonia Volochorg, Trans.). New Hyde Park: University Books.

Wasson, R. G. (1962). Hallucinogenic fungi of Mexico."International Journal of Parapsychology, 4"(4), 41-58.

Weil, A. (1972). "The Natural Mind: A New Way of Looking atDrugs and the Higher Consciousness". Boston: HoughtonMifflin.

Whittlesey, J. R. B. (1960). Some curious ESP results in termsof variance. "Journal of Parapsychology, 24", 220-222.

Wilson, A. J. C. (1949). Ayahuasca, peyotl, yag . "Proceedings

of the Society for Psychical Research", "48", 353-363.Wookey, E. E. (1982). Hypnosis. In I. Gratton-Guinness (Ed.),"Psychical Research: A Guide to its History, Principles andPractices" (pp. 229-237). Wellingborough, Northamptonshire,England: Aquarian Press.

Footnotes