13
Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics K. Emrith, 1, * M. J. Chantler, 1 P. R. Green, 2 L. T. Maloney, 3 and A. D. F. Clarke 1 1 School of Mathematical and Computer Sciences, Heriot-Watt University, Edinburgh, UK 2 School of Life Sciences, Heriot-Watt University, Edinburgh, UK 3 Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York 10003, USA * Corresponding author: [email protected] Received August 10, 2009; revised January 7, 2010; accepted March 8, 2010; posted March 24, 2010 (Doc. ID 115546); published April 30, 2010 We investigate the ability of humans to perceive changes in the appearance of images of surface texture caused by the variation of their higher order statistics. We incrementally randomize their phase spectra while holding their first and second order statistics constant in order to ensure that the change in the appearance is due solely to changes in third and other higher order statistics. Stimuli comprise both natural and synthetically generated naturalistic images, with the latter being used to prevent observers from making pixel-wise com- parisons. A difference scaling method is used to derive the perceptual scales for each observer, which show a sigmoidal relationship with the degree of randomization. Observers were maximally sensitive to changes within the 20%–60% randomization range. In order to account for this behavior we propose a biologically plau- sible model that computes the variance of local measurements of phase congruency. © 2010 Optical Society of America OCIS codes: 330.5000, 330.5020, 330.5510. 1. INTRODUCTION The frequency channel model has provided many valuable insights into human spatial vision [1,2], and it has also provided a wealth of features for computer classification of the image texture [3,4]. Much of this research has been carried out on the first and second order statistics of im- ages and it is now well understood as to how these statis- tics influence perception, and how models—typified by the filter-rectify-filter (FRF) structure—can be used to ac- count for these effects [58]. Many studies have also ex- ploited the frequency channel model to investigate “nth order statistics” particularly with respect to the preatten- tive segregation of patterned image regions or abutted images [919]. Although these studies provided strong psychophysical evidence of the ability of humans to per- form discrimination and segregation tasks, they were, however, based on highly stylized binary textures con- structed using geometric elements for which the control and computation of third and higher order statistics are reasonably straightforward. Unfortunately standard methods for modeling higher order statistics per se in natural (gray-level) images are often extremely complex [2022] and it is difficult to ob- tain an intuitive understanding of what the many param- eters represent. However, many researchers have pointed out that most of the visually pertinent information in an image is encoded in its phase spectrum and there are nu- merous examples that show that phase randomizing an image (also referred to as “scrambling”) leaves it largely unrecognizable [2328]. Furthermore, it is well known that there is an intimate relationship between the higher order statistics and the phase spectra, and studies have demonstrated that visually salient features in images (such as edges and bars) correspond to the points where the various harmonics representing the image (as a two- dimensional signal) have the same phase alignment (known as phase congruency) [29,30]. While phase randomization almost certainly changes the higher order statistics of images, it is also likely to af- fect the first order statistics since phase randomizing a wide bandwidth image will (due to the central limit theo- rem) result in a normal distribution. Some studies have investigated the first order natural image statistics that could potentially account for the visual effects of phase perturbing natural images [8,31,32]. Additionally, it has also been reported that, while the first and second order statistics are independent in natural images, a correla- tion between them appears when the images are phase scrambled [8,33,34]. Researchers have recently exploited techniques for gradual phase randomization of natural images in order to investigate the amount that is required before recogni- tion or segregation becomes impossible [32,3540]. Of these studies only two [32,38] have related the observer’s performance to metrics directly derived from the image data. Thomson et al. [32] measured higher order moments (skewness and kurtosis) derived from first order statistics of the image histogram rather than higher order statis- tics. Hansen and Hess [38] used phase scrambling over different frequency bands to investigate the degree of cross-scale phase alignment required for the identifica- tion of natural or naturalistic scenes. They proposed a “structural sparseness metric” (SSM) in order to aid the interpretation of their results and concluded that observ- ers are less tolerant to phase scrambling when identifying more structured scenes (for example, a scene representing 1232 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/Vol. 27, No. 5/May 2010 Emrith et al. 1084-7529/10/051232-13/$15.00 © 2010 Optical Society of America

Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to ...Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. K. Emrith,1,*M. J. Chantler,1P

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    7

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to ...Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. K. Emrith,1,*M. J. Chantler,1P

1Tipocatficpotipfhsar

ooteoimiutod

1232 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/Vol. 27, No. 5 /May 2010 Emrith et al.

Measuring perceived differences in surface texturedue to changes in higher order statistics

K. Emrith,1,* M. J. Chantler,1 P. R. Green,2 L. T. Maloney,3 and A. D. F. Clarke1

1School of Mathematical and Computer Sciences, Heriot-Watt University, Edinburgh, UK2School of Life Sciences, Heriot-Watt University, Edinburgh, UK

3Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York 10003, USA*Corresponding author: [email protected]

Received August 10, 2009; revised January 7, 2010; accepted March 8, 2010;posted March 24, 2010 (Doc. ID 115546); published April 30, 2010

We investigate the ability of humans to perceive changes in the appearance of images of surface texture causedby the variation of their higher order statistics. We incrementally randomize their phase spectra while holdingtheir first and second order statistics constant in order to ensure that the change in the appearance is duesolely to changes in third and other higher order statistics. Stimuli comprise both natural and syntheticallygenerated naturalistic images, with the latter being used to prevent observers from making pixel-wise com-parisons. A difference scaling method is used to derive the perceptual scales for each observer, which show asigmoidal relationship with the degree of randomization. Observers were maximally sensitive to changeswithin the 20%–60% randomization range. In order to account for this behavior we propose a biologically plau-sible model that computes the variance of local measurements of phase congruency. © 2010 Optical Society ofAmerica

OCIS codes: 330.5000, 330.5020, 330.5510.

(td(

tfwricpasts

gtttpd(otdct“iem

. INTRODUCTIONhe frequency channel model has provided many valuable

nsights into human spatial vision [1,2], and it has alsorovided a wealth of features for computer classificationf the image texture [3,4]. Much of this research has beenarried out on the first and second order statistics of im-ges and it is now well understood as to how these statis-ics influence perception, and how models—typified by thelter-rectify-filter (FRF) structure—can be used to ac-ount for these effects [5–8]. Many studies have also ex-loited the frequency channel model to investigate “nthrder statistics” particularly with respect to the preatten-ive segregation of patterned image regions or abuttedmages [9–19]. Although these studies provided strongsychophysical evidence of the ability of humans to per-orm discrimination and segregation tasks, they were,owever, based on highly stylized binary textures con-tructed using geometric elements for which the controlnd computation of third and higher order statistics areeasonably straightforward.

Unfortunately standard methods for modeling higherrder statistics per se in natural (gray-level) images areften extremely complex [20–22] and it is difficult to ob-ain an intuitive understanding of what the many param-ters represent. However, many researchers have pointedut that most of the visually pertinent information in anmage is encoded in its phase spectrum and there are nu-

erous examples that show that phase randomizing anmage (also referred to as “scrambling”) leaves it largelynrecognizable [23–28]. Furthermore, it is well knownhat there is an intimate relationship between the higherrder statistics and the phase spectra, and studies haveemonstrated that visually salient features in images

1084-7529/10/051232-13/$15.00 © 2

such as edges and bars) correspond to the points wherehe various harmonics representing the image (as a two-imensional signal) have the same phase alignmentknown as phase congruency) [29,30].

While phase randomization almost certainly changeshe higher order statistics of images, it is also likely to af-ect the first order statistics since phase randomizing aide bandwidth image will (due to the central limit theo-

em) result in a normal distribution. Some studies havenvestigated the first order natural image statistics thatould potentially account for the visual effects of phaseerturbing natural images [8,31,32]. Additionally, it haslso been reported that, while the first and second ordertatistics are independent in natural images, a correla-ion between them appears when the images are phasecrambled [8,33,34].

Researchers have recently exploited techniques forradual phase randomization of natural images in ordero investigate the amount that is required before recogni-ion or segregation becomes impossible [32,35–40]. Ofhese studies only two [32,38] have related the observer’serformance to metrics directly derived from the imageata. Thomson et al. [32] measured higher order momentsskewness and kurtosis) derived from first order statisticsf the image histogram rather than higher order statis-ics. Hansen and Hess [38] used phase scrambling overifferent frequency bands to investigate the degree ofross-scale phase alignment required for the identifica-ion of natural or naturalistic scenes. They proposed astructural sparseness metric” (SSM) in order to aid thenterpretation of their results and concluded that observ-rs are less tolerant to phase scrambling when identifyingore structured scenes (for example, a scene representing

010 Optical Society of America

Page 2: Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to ...Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. K. Emrith,1,*M. J. Chantler,1P

ai

emsldicodipt

AGtsdct(fdit

itogpursia

2Atoadtaatwhmtuqsoufr

oolq3odv

ATsstt

BActwwcmmropeetsoasiino

iWmmdscvtdesfictridisvf

Emrith et al. Vol. 27, No. 5 /May 2010/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1233

dense forest will contain significantly more structuralnformation than one depicting the sea or a blue sky).

To our knowledge, all of the above mentioned papersmployed phase spectra scrambling while seeking toaintain constant power spectra. However, while several

tudies do normalize the mean and variance of the imageuminance they do not explicitly control other higher or-er moments (skewness and kurtosis) derived from themage histogram. Thus it is unclear as to whether the per-eived image variations are due to changes in the higherrder moments of first order statistics or due to higher or-er statistics. Furthermore, none of these studies directlynvestigate the correlation of an image metric with theerceived changes caused by the gradual perturbation ofhe phase spectrum.

. Current Studyiven that the contribution of first and second order sta-

istics for segregation or discrimination tasks is well re-earched, it is intriguing to investigate how well we canetect changes in natural images that are due solely tohanges in higher order statistics (that is statistics higherhan second order). The goals of this paper therefore are1) to investigate the ability of observers to perceive dif-erences in images caused solely by changes in higher or-er statistics and (2) to propose a biologically plausiblemage processing model that accounts for these percep-ions.

We present two experiments in which we use naturalmages and synthesized naturalistic images to investigatehe ability of humans to detect small changes in higherrder statistics. We do this by keeping the image histo-ram and the power spectrum constant while graduallyhase randomizing the image. In the first experiment wese a large number of phase randomization levels to de-ive perceptual scales for each observer, whereas in theecond experiment we use a smaller number of random-zation levels with, however, a more extensive set of im-ges.

. METHODS2-alternative forced choice (2AFC) procedure was used

o capture human judgments. This method was preferredver some other popular methods such as the method ofdjustment [41] or ratio scaling since it reduces the bur-en of having to arbitrarily assign values to randomizedextures being discriminated, is easy to implement, andlso requires a few trials to fit the observers’ judgments toperceptual scale. The estimation of a perceptual scale

hat corresponds to the amount of phase randomizationas performed using the technique of maximum likeli-ood difference scaling (MLDS) [42]. The MLDS is aethod that has been used for estimating supra-

hreshold differences across a range of images that havendergone some physical changes, for example, in theuantification of color differences [42], the direct mea-urement of human perception of image compression [43],r the estimation of gloss scales [44]. The MLDS works bysing a set of four stimuli (a quadruple) chosen randomlyrom a full set of textures with different degrees ofandomization. The MLDS requires the use of non-

verlapping quadruples for each reference texture. A setf N randomized images (for each reference texture) al-ows the generation of N ! / �4! �N−4�!� non-overlappinguadruples. For 11 degrees of randomization we obtain30 non-overlapping quadruples [e.g., (2,3) and (2,8) is anverlapping quadruple and is not counted in the 330 qua-ruples]. A more detailed explanation of the MLDS is pro-ided in Appendix A.

. Observershe observers asked to perform the experiments pre-ented in this study had normal or corrected-to-normal vi-ion. All of the observers were naïve to (1) perceptual tex-ure characteristics and the nature of the stimuli, and (2)he purpose of the experiments.

. Stimuliset of 12 stimuli comprising six natural textures and six

omputer synthesized naturalistic textures were used inhe psychophysical experiment. The natural texturesere captured under unknown illumination conditions,hereas the naturalistic textures were synthesized under

ontrolled conditions. In the second case, a Lambertianodel of reflectance was used to render surface heightaps that were generated using a random or semi-

andom placement of texture elements. Where primitivesverlapped we took the maximum of the height of anyrimitive at that position. Unlike the natural textures,ach synthetic naturalistic one could be generated repeat-dly using different seeds controlling the placement of theexture elements. A total of ten seeds was used for eachynthetic texture. This procedure ensured that the pairsf synthetic textures could not be compared pixel-wise,nd so the synthetic textures provide a control for the pos-ibility that observers compared the natural texture pairsn this way. Figure 1 shows all the reference textures usedn the experiment. The first two rows in Fig. 1 show theatural textures, and rows 3 and 4 show the naturalisticnes.

All the reference textures were forced to follow normalntensity distributions before being phase randomized.

hile the naturalistic textures were generated with nor-al intensity distributions, the natural textures wereapped to normal distributions using the mean and stan-

ard deviation of their original distributions. Figure 2hows the pea images before (left column) and after (rightolumn) the mapping process. To obtain test stimuli witharying amounts of higher order statistics, the referenceextures were subjected to different degrees of phase ran-omization. To ensure that the randomized images forach reference texture varied only in their higher ordertatistics, the textures were normalized to have the samerst and second order statistics. The normalization pro-ess was performed at each randomization stage so thathe resulting image was constructed using the partiallyandomized phase spectrum of each reference texture andts original power spectrum. This allowed the second or-er statistics to be kept constant for all partially random-zed images. Additionally, each randomized image wasubjected to a D’Agostino–Pearson normality test toerify whether its intensity distribution had deviatedrom the original normal distribution. If the null hypoth-

Page 3: Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to ...Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. K. Emrith,1,*M. J. Chantler,1P

e0tort

iofp=tm

rpta(f1apirau

CALwsgMmtfit

3ToaaWdittone(r

ASe�

Frt

Fis mapped to a normal one.

1234 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/Vol. 27, No. 5 /May 2010 Emrith et al.

sis of a non-normal distribution was not rejected at the.05 level of significance, the processed image was forcedo the reference texture’s normal distribution and the sec-nd order statistics were again adjusted. This process isepeated until both first and second order statistics werehe same as the original image.

Gradual phase randomization was performed by add-ng a random variable to the principal phase values of theriginal texture images. The random variable was drawnrom the uniform distribution �0,��. Eleven degrees ofhase randomization were used with degree 0 being �0 and degree 10 being �=2� for 100% phase randomiza-

ion and with the other degrees representing linear incre-ents in �.Complex conjugate symmetry was maintained in the

andomized phase spectra in order to provide a zeroower imaginary spatial domain image and to ensure thathe second order statistics in the real spatial domain im-ge remained constant. Figure 3 shows one syntheticblood) texture and one natural (seeds) texture at four dif-erent levels of phase randomization (0%, 30%, 60%, and00%). All the reference textures were non-periodic (andlso tileable in the case of synthetic textures), and fullyhase randomizing them results in visually continuousmages. On the other hand, phase randomizing highly pe-iodic textures does not lead to visually continuous im-ges (see Fig. 4) and therefore such textures were notsed in this study.

. Experimental Setup20 in. TFT (thin-film transistors) monitor (NEC

CD2090UXi) with a pixel pitch of 0.255 mm (100 dpi)as used to display a 2�2 array (quadruple) of images of

ize 512�512 pixels for each trial. The calibration of theamma responses ��=2.2� was performed using a Garethacbeth Eye One Pro spectrometer. The luminance of theonitor was fixed at 120 cd/m2 with the color tempera-

ure set at 6500 K for a frame rate of 60 Hz. Observersxed the screen from a distance of 70 cm, where it sub-ended a visual angle of 11°.

. EXPERIMENT 1he objective of experiment 1 was to investigate how wellbservers could discriminate between pairs of texture im-ges that differ in their higher order statistics, expresseds the 11 degrees of randomization described in Section 2.hile a larger number of trials (with more than 11 ran-

omization levels) would provide higher confidence levelsn estimating the perceptual scales using the MLDS, 330rials per reference image is more realistic for the percep-ual task considered. Experiment 1, however, considerednly a subset of natural and synthetic images since it isot practical for observers to judge trials from all 12 ref-rence textures (i.e., 3960 trials) at one go. Two naturalgravel and seeds) and two synthetic (blood and RanFrac)eference textures were used in this experiment.

. Procedureix observers participated in this experiment. The observ-rs were presented with two pairs (a quadruple) of stimuli

ig. 1. Images used in the psychophysical experiments. Top twoows show the six natural images and the bottom two rows showhe six computer synthesized textures.

ig. 2. Example of a natural image whose intensity distribution

a ,b� and �c ,d� displayed one above the other and were
Page 4: Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to ...Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. K. Emrith,1,*M. J. Chantler,1P

adqblhonp

ptwtaesssoevlfipau

BPa

(avwstosstrtec0

sTiidttt

rr6sp

stinis

htbrp

Fh

Emrith et al. Vol. 27, No. 5 /May 2010/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1235

sked to identify the pair that had the larger perceptualifference. The trials for each texture were presented se-uentially with the observer having the option to take areak in between each set of trials presented. No timeimit was imposed on observers in making their choice;owever, the interface presented to them required thatne of the two pairs was selected (forced choice mecha-ism). Observers did not have the option to return to arevious trial.The MLDS technique requires that each trial is com-

osed of textures having an ordered degree of randomiza-ion; however, there was no restriction in the way inhich the images were presented to the observers. Thus

he position (top or bottom) of each pair was randomizedt each trial and also the position (left or right) withinach pair was also randomized. Additionally, since thetimuli belonging to each reference texture were pre-ented sequentially, the order in which the sets were pre-ented to each observer was alternated. This was done inrder to balance any effect of fatigue. The result Ri forach trial i was saved in a binary form �Ri=0/1� with aalue of zero corresponding to the upper pair having thearger perceptual difference or 1 for the lower pair. Thenal results for each test texture were fed to the MLDSrogram to estimate the perceptual scales. A MLDS pack-ge implemented using the R programming language wassed for the estimation process [45].

. Resultslots of the estimated perceptual scales for six observersre displayed in Figs. 5 and 6 for the chosen naturalistic

Fig. 3. Reference textures at different level

ig. 4. Effect of full phase randomization on the appearance of aighly periodic texture (left, original; right, randomized).

blood and RanFrac) and natural (gravel and seeds) im-ges. Each plot shows how the difference scale valuesary for the selected textures when their phase spectraere gradually randomized. The bootstrap procedure de-

cribed by Maloney and Yang [42] was used to estimatehe confidence intervals (�1 SD) shown in the plots. Webserve that all the plots in both Figs. 5 and 6 exhibit aigmoidal behavior, monotonically increasing from 0 andaturating at 1. For both natural and synthesized tex-ures, the plots clearly show that for an amount of phaseandomization varying from 0% to 20%, the changes inhe difference scale values are low for most of the observ-rs. This indicates that observers encountered appre-iable difficulty in discriminating texture pairs within the%–20% range.The plots for the synthetic textures (Fig. 5) show sharp

lopes for the range 20%–60% of phase randomization.his shows a greater ability of observers in discriminat-

ng between the synthetic texture pairs presented, thusndicating that observers were more sensitive to smallerifferences in randomization within this range. Beyondhe 60% mark, all observers perceived a little change inhe appearance in the partially randomized synthetic tex-ures.

Inspection of Fig. 3 suggests the basis for the sigmoidalelationship; at 30% randomization, the texture elementsemain as visible as in the original image, whereas from0% onward they are not. It should be noted that the ob-ervers were not explicitly asked to judge the textureairs based on the visibility of texture elements.While the plots for natural textures show similar

hapes (see Fig. 6), we observe that the steep slopes ex-end to 70%–80% phase randomization. The greater abil-ty of humans to judge perceptual difference betweenatural textures within a range up to 80% phase random-

zation may be due to the pixel-wise comparisons that ob-ervers were able to make for natural textures.

Additionally, the plots in Fig. 5 show very similar be-avior for the two synthesized textures, while this is nothe case for the two natural textures. We observe that theehavior for the seed texture is more linear within theange 20%–60% than for the gravel texture. A possible ex-lanation may be that while the synthetic textures are

domization: Blood (top) and seeds (bottom).

s of ran
Page 5: Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to ...Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. K. Emrith,1,*M. J. Chantler,1P

mft

4Eb

tIpudps

Ft

Fs

1236 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/Vol. 27, No. 5 /May 2010 Emrith et al.

ade up of a single texture element, the texture elementsrom the natural textures vary in size, shape, and con-rast.

. EXPERIMENT 2xperiment 2 was carried out to investigate whether theehavior of the perceptual scales for the natural and syn-

ig. 5. Plots showing the behavior of individual observers’ differextures blood and RanFrac.

ig. 6. Six plots showing the behavior of difference scales with ch

eeds.

hetic textures is maintained for a larger set of textures.n this experiment a set of eight reference textures, com-rising four synthetic and four natural textures, wassed. To allow a larger set to be tested, the number of ran-omization levels was decreased leading to a fewer trialser reference image. Figures 5 and 6 from experiment 1howed that beyond 80% randomization, observers were

ales with changing amount of phase randomization for synthetic

g amount of phase randomization for natural textures gravel and

ence sc

angin

Page 6: Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to ...Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. K. Emrith,1,*M. J. Chantler,1P

ur8r

ATtF

wewt

BFs(

Fl

Emrith et al. Vol. 27, No. 5 /May 2010/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1237

nable to perceive any changes in the appearance of theandomized textures. Thus, only the first nine (i.e., 0%–0%) degrees of randomization were presented in the cur-ent experiment.

. Procedurehe same procedure as that for experiment 1 was used. Aotal of four observers participated in this experiment.or each reference texture a set of 126 trials (quadruples)

ig. 7. Plots showing the behavior of difference scales for a set ofevels (0%–80%). Column 1 shows the plots for all textures and c

as presented to observers (i.e., a total of 1008 trials foright textures). While the trials for each reference textureere presented in sequence, the presentation order for

he natural and synthetic textures was randomized.

. Resultsigure 7 shows the plots (left column) for the natural andynthesized textures tested and also the mean behaviorright column) for four observers who participated in this

nthetic and four natural textures using only nine randomization2 shows the average behavior for four subjects.

four syolumn

Page 7: Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to ...Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. K. Emrith,1,*M. J. Chantler,1P

efiopaod(pmtmslpfl

5DIccssd

toprrc[cprttpwftts

t[sslebstsecsafi

dMgtBbii

tacauiotirttms

ARmvteiautmstlscvwt2t

dwisr1tsaFa0sata

1238 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/Vol. 27, No. 5 /May 2010 Emrith et al.

xperiment. The plots for the eight different textures con-rm the general behavior (i.e., monotonic and sigmoidal)f the perceptual scales derived in experiment 1, and alsorovide additional evidence that observers had a greaterbility to discriminate smaller changes in the appearancef synthetic textures within the 20%–60% range of ran-omization while being more sensitive to a greater rangeup to 70%–80%) for natural textures. Additionally, theerceptual scales for the natural textures also appearore linear, with a rather constant slope, as compared to

he scales for the synthetic textures. These observationsay be due to the fact that observers may have used

trong localized information as characterized by high-ights and shadow information to make pixel-wise com-arison for natural textures, although most structural in-ormation was destroyed beyond the 60% randomizationevel as illustrated in Fig. 3.

. MODELING OF PERCEPTUALIFFERENCES

n this section we seek a biologically plausible model thatan generate a perceptual measure with the followingharacteristics: (1) it has a monotonic sigmoidal relation-hip with increasing phase randomization and (2) ithows a steep change in the range 20%–60% of phase ran-omization.It is well known that higher order statistics (i.e., higher

han second order) are affected by the phase relationshipf patterns [26]; however, a simple model to representhase information is difficult to achieve due to its complexepresentation (mainly due to phase wrapping in theange �−� ,+��). Since it is known that natural imagesontain structure that is aligned locally in phase space29,30], we have investigated and applied Kovesi’s phaseongruency model [30] to represent the change in the ap-earance of textured surfaces at different levels of phaseandomization. We observed in Fig. 3 that randomizinghe phase spectra of the texture images destroys the spa-ial arrangements of local features and changes the ap-earance of the texture surfaces. In this study, therefore,e provide what we believe to be a novel feature derived

rom Kovesi’s phase congruency model [30], which charac-erizes the change in the appearance of the surface tex-ures, and we show that this feature meets the criteria de-cribed above.

Kovesi’s phase congruency model [30] was inspired byhe local energy model presented by Morrone and Burr29], which models the way in which the human visualystem uses odd and even symmetric receptors in the vi-ual cortex to decode local features such as edges andines. Morrone and Burr’s model [29] uses phase congru-nce information to detect these local features and it haseen applied successfully both to the segmentation of vi-ual scenes and to predict the perceptual appearance ofhese scenes [30,46,47]. Their model consists of twotages. In the first stage quadrature filter pairs (odd andven symmetric) are applied at different spatial frequen-ies and orientations. A function based on the sum ofquares of the responses from these filters is computed,nd peaks corresponding to salient features are identi-ed. The second stage classifies these peaks in terms of

ifferent perceptual features (such as edges or bars).orrone and Burr’s model [29], however, does not provide

ood localization of local features due to its dependence onhe local contrast. Kovesi [30] improved Morrone andurr’s model [29] to provide better localization of featuresy computing the phase congruence information that isnvariant to changes in the image contrast and also bydentifying and compensating for noise.

The appearance of surface texture is primarily charac-erized by the presence of local perceptual features suchs edges, lines, or corners. Thus, any change in those per-eptual features would contribute to changing the appear-nce of surfaces. Kovesi’s model [30] has been successfullytilized for the detection of edges and localized features in

mages and has also been shown to perform better thanther detectors such as Canny or Prewitt [30,48]. We haveherefore employed it in order to investigate how the edgenformation changes with the varying amount of phaseandomization. Figure 8 shows phase congruency maps ofhe blood texture at different levels of phase randomiza-ion. These maps were generated using a MATLAB imple-entation of phase congruency available at [49] and de-

cribed in [30].

. Single Feature Representationather than a phase congruency map, we require a singleeasure per image as the basis of the perceptual scale. A

isual inspection of the maps shown in Fig. 8 suggestshat as the level of phase randomization is increased, thedge information is gradually degraded, leading to a noisemage when the texture is fully randomized. This appearss a change in the distribution of the edge intensity val-es as shown in the bottom row in Fig. 8, which suggestshat the histogram statistics of the phase congruencyaps may provide useful perceptual scales. Figure 9

hows how the mean, variance, skewness, and kurtosis ofhe phase congruency histograms vary with increasingevels of phase randomization for the blood, RanFrac, andeed textures. While both skewness and variance changeonsiderably with the change in randomization level, theariance is the only feature that behaves in a monotonicay for the three textures investigated. We observe that

he behavior of skewness is not monotonic within the 0%–0% range for two of the textures considered (it is mono-onic only for the RanFrac texture).

The phase congruency variance showed no significantifference in the behavior across the randomization levelshen extracted from naturalistic textures generated us-

ng different placement seeds [see Fig. 10(a)]. This mea-ure also converges at 100% randomization for both natu-al and synthetic textures as shown in Figs. 10(b) and0(c). Additionally, changes in the variance are greatest inhe range 20%–60% of phase randomization. All these ob-ervations make the phase congruency variance a suit-ble measure to represent the psychophysical data. Inigs. 10(b) and 10(c) we observe that although the vari-nce converges for the selected textures, it is different at% randomization. This suggests that the current mea-ure of higher order statistics may also account for themount of structural information across different tex-ures in addition to existing mechanisms that employ firstnd second order statistics [1,8,11,12,17,50] to do so.

Page 8: Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to ...Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. K. Emrith,1,*M. J. Chantler,1P

BAvse

aoFt

Fs

Fos

Emrith et al. Vol. 27, No. 5 /May 2010/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1239

. Models a model for the computation of the phase congruencyariance, we propose a two stage process, with the firsttage corresponding to the computation of phase congru-

ig. 8. Phase congruency maps (middle row) for different levelsbtained after applying Kovesi’s phase congruency model [30]. Bohape when the image is randomized.

ncy, while in the second stage point-wise nonlinearity

nd pooling operations are used to estimate the variancef the phase congruency map for each randomized image.igure 11 illustrates the different steps involved in this

wo stage model.

0%, 60%, and 100%) of phase randomized blood images (top row)ow shows how the edge intensity histogram of each map changes

The first stage is specified in [30]. It uses a bank of

ig. 9. Behavior of the mean, variance, skewness, and kurtosis of the phase congruency distribution for textures blood, RanFrac, andeeds across the different levels of randomization.

(0%, 3ttom r

Page 9: Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to ...Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. K. Emrith,1,*M. J. Chantler,1P

lffadesficndte

wstq

EtEmtpvtPgoa

cssibaifs

Frn

1240 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/Vol. 27, No. 5 /May 2010 Emrith et al.

ogarithmic-Gabor quadrature filters tuned to differentrequencies and orientations so as to capture localizedeature information in the texture images. The filters arepplied in the Fourier domain and the resulting spatialomain outputs are used to generate the phase congru-ncy maps. The channels for encoding the edges corre-pond to a sequence of FRFs in the form of filter-rectify-lter-rectify-filter. With FRF layers able to detect onlyhanges in second order statistics, at least one additionalonlinear layer is needed to capture changes in higher or-er statistics. The first FRF layer allows the generation ofhe phase congruency map PC, where the phase congru-ncy at each orientation, PCo, is computed as follows:

PCo�x,y� =Wo�x��Eo�x,y� − T�

�n

Ano�x,y� + �, �1�

here Wo�x� is a weighting function for the frequencypread at a given orientation o and Ano�x ,y� is the ampli-ude information derived using the response of the

ig. 10. Variation in phase congruency variance with changingandom placement of textons at different seeds, (b) six differentatural textures.

uadrature filter at each scale n and orientation o. c

o�x ,y� is the energy accumulated by the quadrature fil-ers at N scales for a given orientation o and is given byo�x ,y�=�nAno��no�x ,y�, where �no�x ,y� is the weightedean phase angle computed at each scale and orienta-

ion. T is used for noise compensation and � is a smallositive constant that is used when the sum of responseectors is very small leading to an ill-conditioned compu-ation of phase congruency. The phase congruency mapC is obtained by summing the noise compensated ener-ies at all orientations and then normalizing by the sumf amplitudes of the individual quadrature pairs appliedt all the scales and orientations.In the second stage, the phase congruency variance is

omputed as follows: = 1D��PC−�2. It represents the

econd nonlinearity (rectify-filter) layer that the visualystem uses to perceive differences in the appearance. s the mean phase congruency and D represents the num-er of pixels in the image. Note that although the vari-nce is computed over the whole phase congruency map,t could also be computed over a local window. However,or the purpose of this paper we only require a single mea-ure per image. Fitting the psychophysical data with the

of phase randomization for (a) a texture image generated usingtic textures generated using the same seed, and (c) six different

omputed measure leads to a linear relationship in the

Fig. 11. Model: A two stage process for computing the higher order statistics measure to account for change in appearance.

levelssynthe

Page 10: Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to ...Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. K. Emrith,1,*M. J. Chantler,1P

lhn

6WtiitTntio

pcfwt[tits

ssrrdtnewtn

6tnop(cnTpt

ditbHre

rrtgafctvtrhiwd

fHb

Fb

Emrith et al. Vol. 27, No. 5 /May 2010/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1241

og-log space. Figure 12 illustrates this relationship. Theigh correlation �R2� values indicate excellent fits for bothatural [Fig. 12(a)] and synthetic [Fig. 12(b)] textures.

. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONhile it is well documented in the literature that most of

he structural information within an image is character-zed by its higher order statistics, no studies have so farnvestigated how well humans perceive small changes inhe appearance as a result of changing such statistics.he current paper has addressed this issue by using bothaturalistic and natural textures. We randomly per-urbed their phase spectra by differing degrees while forc-ng all randomized images to have identical first and sec-nd order statistics as the originals.

Although several studies have investigated the effect ofartially randomizing phase spectra on perception, the fo-us of those studies was on the ability of observers to per-orm recognition tasks. No quantitative measurementsere made of the perceptual differences in the images

hat resulted. Thomson et al. [32] and Hansen and Hess38] are the only authors to have proposed image metricshat change with the varying amount of phase random-zation in their respective studies. However, neither ofhese studies controlled the first order statistics of theirtimuli during the partial phase randomization process.

The experiment presented in this study captured ob-ervers’ perceptions of changes in the appearance using aet of natural textures and synthetic textures (with natu-alistic appearance). A perceptual scale, derived from theesulting psychophysical data, was shown to have sigmoi-al monotonically increasing behavior for all imagesested. We observed, from the perceptual scales for bothatural and synthetic textures, that observers had consid-rable difficulty in perceiving differences in texture pairshich were phase randomized by less than 20%. For syn-

hetic images, observers had a greater ability to discrimi-ate small changes in the appearance within the 20%–

ig. 12. Linear relationship between perceptual difference and plood and RanFrac, and (b) gravel and seeds.

0% range and encountered appreciable difficulty beyondhe 60% mark. However, while the perceptual scales foratural images had the same shape, they indicated thatbservers had the ability to perceive changes in the ap-earance over a wider range of phase randomization20%–70%). This may be due to the fact that observersould directly compare the gray levels in one region of aatural image with the same region in its paired image.he use of a randomized placement of texture elementsrevented the observers from using the same strategy forhe synthetic images.

Although the conditions in which the images were ran-omized suggest that a change in the appearance of themages may correspond to a change in the visibility of theexture elements, we cannot assume that the observersased their judgments on the perception of the structure.owever, we showed (see Fig. 3) that the behavior in the

ange 20%–60% of randomization corresponds to consid-rable change in the visibility of the image structure.

We have also proposed an image-based metric that cor-elates well with the perceived changes. Kovesi’s algo-ithm [30] was used to generate phase congruency mapshat reflect the degree of phase congruence in local re-ions of the image. The algorithm is based on Morronend Burr’s model [29] for the detection of visually salienteatures in images, which is motivated both by the psy-hophysical data and by the properties of single cells inhe visual cortex. In addition to being biologically moti-ated, the proposed metric satisfies two additional condi-ions necessary to account for the perceptual scale de-ived: (1) it has monotonic sigmoidal behavior and (2) itas the greatest change in gradient when extracted from

mages that are randomized in the range 20%–60%. Itas also shown to correlate linearly with the perceptualifference measurement.The measure proposed shares a common concept, the

requency channel model, with the SSM proposed byansen and Hess [38]. While the SSM exploits the distri-ution of a set of bandpass filter outputs, the measure we

ongruency variance in a log-log space for (a) synthesized textures

hase c
Page 11: Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to ...Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. K. Emrith,1,*M. J. Chantler,1P

upthf[b[

fctpftdatct

ADTtps�

p�It�mpntae

aatplLtewt

Itt

ws

Itkfo=o

wt

fet spot

vrce

tbatisibatc

btcfuMsSretdv

1242 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/Vol. 27, No. 5 /May 2010 Emrith et al.

se employs a set of quadrature bandpass filters to com-ute the phase congruency in the image. We have chosenhe latter because spatial phase congruence informationas been widely used in the extraction of visually salient

eatures (e.g., edges, bars, and lines) in images29,30,48,51,52]. The SSM measure, in contrast, has onlyeen applied in the study performed by Hansen and Hess38].

While previous studies in texture discrimination haveocused on the ability of humans to discriminate differentategories of textures, the current study has investigatedhe ability of humans to perceive small changes in the ap-earance of the same texture. By using textures that dif-er solely in higher order statistics we have demonstratedhat humans are very sensitive to the third and higher or-er statistics that contribute to the change in the appear-nce. Additionally, we provide a measure to characterizehe change in the appearance and show that this measureorrelates well with the perceived perceptual difference inextures.

PPENDIX A: MAXIMUM LIKELIHOODIFFERENCE SCALINGhe MLDS is based on a model of the observer’s percep-ion of differences in psychophysical stimuli ordered on ahysical scale. Initially, the experimenter selects a set of ptimuli, �I1 ,I2 , . . . ,Ip�, with corresponding values ��1�2� ¯ ��p� on the physical scale. On each trial the ex-

erimenter presents an observer with quadruplesIa ,Ib ;Ic ,Id� and asks him to judge which pair, Ia ,Ib orc ,Id, exhibits the larger perceptual difference. We replacehe notation �Ia ,Ib ;Ic ,Id� with the simpler notationa ,b ;c ,d� for convenience. Over the course of the experi-ent, the observer sees many different quadruples. In

ast work, experiments have used the set of all possibleon-overlapping quadruples a�b�c�d for p stimuli andhe resulting scales have proven to be readily interpret-ble. Moreover, Maloney and Yang [42] reported extensivevaluations of this subset of all possible quadruples.

The data consist of a list of all quadruples presentednd the observer’s judgments. The goal of the MLDS is tossign values to � 1� 2� ¯ � p� that best account forhe observer’s judgments. Maloney and Yang [42] pro-osed a stochastic model of difference judgment that al-ows the observer to exhibit some variation in judgment.et Lab= � b− a� denote the unsigned perceived length ofhe interval Ia ,Ib. The proposed decision model is anqual-variance Gaussian signal detection model [53]here the signal is the difference in the lengths of the in-

ervals,

��a,b;c,d� = � d − c� − � b − a�. �A1�

f � is positive, the observer should judge the second in-erval larger; when negative, the first. We assume thathe decision variable employed by the observer is

��a,b;c,d� = ��a,b;c,d� + � = Lcd − Lab + �, �A2�

here �N�0,�2�: given the quadruple, �a ,b ;c ,d�, the ob-erver selects the pair Ic ,Id if and only if

��a,b;c,d� � 0. �A3�

n each experimental condition the observer completes nrials, each based on a quadruple qk= �ak ,bk ;ck ,dk�, with=1,n. The observer’s response is coded as Rk=0 (the dif-

erence of the first pair is judged larger) or Rk=1 (the sec-nd pair is judged larger). We fit the parameters �� 1 , 2 , . . . , p� and � by maximizing the likelihood of thebserver’s responses,

L��,�� = k=1

n

����qk�

��1−Rk�1 − ����qk�

���Rk

, �A4�

here ��x� denotes the cumulative standard normal dis-ribution and ��qk�=��ak ,bk ;ck ,dk� as defined in Eq. (A2).

At first glance, it would appear that the stochastic dif-erence scaling model just presented has p+1 free param-ters, 1 , . . . , p, together with the standard deviation ofhe error term, �. However, any linear transformation of1 , . . . , p together with a corresponding scaling by �−1 re-ults in a set of parameters that predict exactly the sameerformance as the original parameters. Without any lossf generality, we can set 1=0 and p=1, leaving us withhe p−1 free parameters, 2 , . . . , p−1, and �.

Equation (A4) is the likelihood for a Bernoulli randomariable. Taking the negative logarithm allows the pa-ameters to be estimated simply with a minimization pro-edure. We used the package MLDS described in [45] tostimate difference scales.

The fitted values 1 , . . . , p form the difference scale in-ended to capture human performance. These values cane plotted against the physical values ��1��2� ¯ ��p�s a convenient summary of performance. We note thathe choice of physical scale is arbitrary and any increas-ng transformation of the physical scale is a valid physicalcale. The difference scale, however, is not arbitrary oncets limits are fixed to be 0 and 1. This opens up the possi-ility of redefining physical scales of roughness or otherttributes so that physical spacing better approximateshe perceived difference as has been done for loudness byoding physical scale units in decibels.

Maloney and Yang [42] evaluated the distributional ro-ustness of the MLDS. They varied the distributions ofhe error term � while continuing to fit the data with theonstant variance Gaussian error assumption. Theyound that the MLDS was remarkably resistant to fail-res of the distributional assumptions. Knoblauch andaloney [45] also considered the possibility that the ob-

erver cannot judge differences in any consistent manner.uch a failure would likely result in a large value of �elative to the scale limits of 0,1. They also proposed sev-ral diagnostic procedures intended to detect failures ofhe judgment model underlying the MLDS. Such proce-ures are analogous to testing for the pattern of residualalues in the linear regression.

Page 12: Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to ...Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. K. Emrith,1,*M. J. Chantler,1P

R

1

1

1

1

1

1

1

1

1

1

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

3

3

3

3

3

3

3

3

3

3

4

4

4

4

4

4

4

4

4

Emrith et al. Vol. 27, No. 5 /May 2010/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1243

EFERENCES1. M. S. Landy and N. Graham, “Visual perception of texture,”

in The Visual Neurosciences, L. M. Chalupa and J. S.Werner, eds. (MIT, 2004), pp. 1106–1118.

2. J. Malik and P. Perona, “Preattentive texture discrimina-tion with early vision mechanisms,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 7,923–932 (1990).

3. T. Randen and J. H. Husøy, “Filtering for texture classifica-tion: a comparative study,” IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal.Mach. Intell. 21, 291–310 (1999).

4. M. J. Chantler, M. Petrou, A. Penirschke, M. Schmidt, andG. McGunnigle, “Classifying surface texture while simulta-neously estimating illumination,” Int. J. Comput. Vis. 62,83–96 (2005).

5. P. Perona and J. Malik, “Detecting and localizing edgescomposed of steps, peaks and roofs,” in Proceedings of theICCV (1990), pp. 52–57.

6. I. Mareschal and C. L. Baker, “A cortical locus for the pro-cessing of contrast-defined contours,” Nat. Neurosci. 1,150–154 (1998).

7. I. Mareschal and C. L. Baker, “Cortical processing ofsecond-order motion,” Visual Neurosci. 16, 527–540 (1999).

8. A. P. Johnson and C. L. Baker, Jr., “First- and second-orderinformation in natural images: a filter-based approach toimage statistics,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 21, 913–925 (2004).

9. B. Julesz, “Visual discrimination of textures with identicalthird-order statistics,” Biol. Cybern. 31, 137–140 (1978).

0. T. Caelli and B. Julesz, “Psychophysical evidence for globalfeature processing in visual texture discrimination,” J. Opt.Soc. Am. 69, 675–678 (1979).

1. B. Julesz, “Spatial nonlinearities in the instantaneous per-ception of textures with identical power spectra,” Philos.Trans. R. Soc. London, Ser. B 290, 83–94 (1980).

2. B. Julesz, “A theory of preattentive texture discriminationbased on first-order statistics of textons,” Biol. Cybern. 41,131–138 (1981).

3. J. I. Yellott, Jr., “Implications of triple correlation unique-ness for texture statistics and the Julesz conjecture,” J.Opt. Soc. Am. A 10, 777–793 (1993).

4. C. Chubb, J. Econopouly, and M. S. Landy, “Histogram con-trast analysis and the visual segregation of IID textures,” J.Opt. Soc. Am. A 11, 2350–2374 (1994).

5. J. D. Victor, “Images, statistics and textures: implications oftriple correlation uniqueness for texture statistics and theJulesz conjecture: comment,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 11, 1680–1684 (1994).

6. J. D. Victor and M. M. Conte, “The role of high-order phasecorrelations in texture processing,” Vision Res. 36, 1615–1631 (1996).

7. C. Chubb and J. I. Yellott, “Every discrete, finite image isuniquely determined by its dipole histogram,” Vision Res.40, 485–492 (2000).

8. C. W. Tyler, “Theory of texture discrimination of based onhigher-order perturbations in individual texture samples,”Vision Res. 44, 2179–2186 (2004).

9. C. W. Tyler, “Beyond fourth-order texture discrimination:generation of extreme-order and statistically-balanced tex-tures,” Vision Res. 44, 2187–2199 (2004).

0. M. O. Franz and B. Scholkopf, “Implicit Wiener series forhigher-order image analysis,” in Advances in Neural Infor-mation Processing Systems, L. K. Saul, Y. Weiss, and L. Bot-tou, eds. (MIT, 2005), Vol. 17, pp. 465–472.

1. T. E. Hall and G. B. Giannakis, “Bispectral analysis andmodel validation of texture images,” IEEE Trans. ImageProcess. 4, 996–1009 (1995).

2. M. K. Tsatsanis and G. B. Giannakis, “Object and textureclassification using higher order statistics,” IEEE Trans.Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 14, 733–750 (1992).

3. A. V. Oppenheim and J. Lim, “The importance of phase insignals,” Proc. IEEE 69, 529–541(1981).

4. L. N. Piotrowski and F. Campbell, “A demonstration of thevisual importance and flexibility of spatial-frequency ampli-

tude and phase,” Perception 11, 337–346 (1982).

5. D. J. Field, “Relations between the statistics of natural im-ages and the response properties of cortical cells,” J. Opt.Soc. Am. A 4, 2379–2394 (1987).

6. D. J. Field, “What the statistics of natural images tell usabout visual coding,” in Human Vision, Visual Processingand Digital Display, Proc. SPIE 1077, 269–276 (1989).

7. D. C. Knill, D. Field, and D. Kersten, “Human discrimina-tion of fractal images,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 7, 1113–1123(1990).

8. Y. Tadmor and D. J. Tolhurst, “Both the phase and ampli-tude spectrum may determine the appearance of naturalimages,” Vision Res. 33, 141–145 (1993).

9. M. C. Morrone and D. C. Burr, “Feature detection in humanvision: a phase-dependent energy model,” Proc. R. Soc. Lon-don, Ser. B 235, 221–245 (1988).

0. P. Kovesi, “Phase congruency: a low-level image invariant,”Psychol. Res. 64, 136–148 (2000).

1. M. A. Thomson and D. H. Foster, “Role of second- and third-order statistics in the discriminability of natural images,” J.Opt. Soc. Am. A 14, 2081–2090 (1997).

2. M. A. Thomson, D. H. Foster, and R. J. Summers, “Humansensitivity to phase perturbations in natural images: a sta-tistical framework,” Perception 29, 1057–1069 (2000).

3. V. Mante, R. A. Frazor, V. Bonin, W. S. Geisler, and M. Car-andini, “Independence of luminance and contrast in naturalscenes and in the early visual system,” Nat. Neurosci. 8,1690–1697 (2005).

4. J. T. Lindgren, J. Hurri, and A. Hyvärinen, “Spatial depen-dencies between local luminance and contrast in naturalimages,” J. Vision 8, 1–13 (2008).

5. C. Baker, A. Yoonessi, and E. O. Arsenault, “Texture seg-mentation in natural images: contribution of higher-orderimage statistics to psychophysical performance,” J. Vision8, 350 (2008).

6. W. Einhäuser, U. Rutishauser, E. P. Frady, S. Nadler, P.Konig, and C. Koch, “The relation of phase noise and lumi-nance contrast to overt attention in complex visual stimuli,”J. Vision 6, 1148–1158 (2006).

7. F. A. Wichmann, D. I. Braun, and K. R. Gegenfurtner,“Phase noise and the classification of natural images,” Vi-sion Res. 46, 1520–1529 (2006).

8. B. C. Hansen and R. F. Hess, “Structural sparseness andspatial phase alignment in natural scenes,” J. Opt. Soc. Am.A 24, 1873–1885 (2007).

9. A. Yoonessi and F. A. A. Kingdom, “Comparison of sensitiv-ity to color changes in natural and phase-scrambledscenes,” Vision Res. 25, 676–684 (2008).

0. O. R. Joubert, G. A. Rousselet, M. Fabre-Thorpe, and D.Fize, “Rapid visual categorization of natural scene contextswith equalized amplitude spectrum and increasing phasenoise,” J. Vision 9, 1–16 (2009).

1. S. Padilla, O. Drbohlav, P. R. Green, A. D. Spence, and M. J.Chantler, “Perceived roughness of 1/ fbeta noise surfaces,”Vision Res. 48, 1791–1797 (2008).

2. L. T. Maloney and J. N. Yang, “Maximum likelihood differ-ence scaling,” J. Vision 3, 573–585 (2003).

3. C. Charrier, L. Maloney, H. Cherifi, and K. Knoblauch,“Maximum likelihood difference scaling of image quality incompression-degraded images,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 24,3418–3426 (2007).

4. G. Obein, K. Knoblauch, and F. Viénot, “Difference scalingof gloss: nonlinearity, binocularity, and constancy,” J. Vision4, 711–720 (2004).

5. K. Knoblauch and L. T. Maloney, “MLDS: maximum likeli-hood difference scaling in R ,” J. Stat. Software 25, 1–26(2008).

6. M. C. Morrone and D. C. Burr, “Capture and transparencyin coarse quantized images,” Vision Res. 37, 2609–2629(1997).

7. A. Perna and M. Morrone, “The lowest spatial frequencychannel determines brightness perception,” Vision Res. 47,1282–1291 (2007).

8. P. Kovesi, “Edges are not just steps,” in Proceedings of the

ACCV2002 (2002), pp. 822–827.
Page 13: Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to ...Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. K. Emrith,1,*M. J. Chantler,1P

45

5

5

5

1244 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/Vol. 27, No. 5 /May 2010 Emrith et al.

9. http://www.csse.uwa.edu.au/~pk/Research/MatlabFns/.0. C. Chubb, M. S. Landy, and J. Econopouly, “A visual

mechanism tuned to black,” Vision Res. 44, 3223–3232(2004).

1. G. S. Hesse and M. A. Georgeson, “Edges and bars: wheredo people see features in 1-D images?” Vision Res. 45, 507–

525 (2005).

2. E. J. Kelman, R. J. Baddeley, A. J. Shohet, and D. Osorio,“Perception of visual texture and the expression of disrup-tive camouflage by the cuttlefish, sepia officinalis,” Proc. R.Soc. London, Ser. B 274, 1369–1375 (2007).

3. D. M. Green and J. A. Swets, Signal Detection Theory andPsychophysics (Wiley, 1974).