1
HIGHLIGHTS NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 5 | JANUARY 2004 | 7 If a magician gave you a list of words to memorize, and was then able to predict precisely which words you would be able to recall, you would no doubt be impressed. Reporting in the Journal of Neuroscience, Sederberg and colleagues show that this feat can be achieved without resorting to magic or ‘mind-control’ tricks. Using intracranial recordings from the cortex, they identified specific patterns of rhythmic neuronal activity that correlated with successful memory formation. Sederberg et al. used a group of 10 patients, aged from 8–17, each of whom had up to 128 electrodes implanted into their brains to monitor epilepsy. The patients were asked to memorize a list of 20 common nouns while the authors recorded their cortical and subcortical brain activity. The patients were then asked to recall as many words as possible, after being distracted briefly with an arithmetical task to reduce bias towards the most recently presented words. The authors observed increases in both low-frequency theta oscillations (4–8 Hz) and high-frequency gamma oscillations (28–64 Hz) during the presentation of words that were subsequently recalled successfully. The theta oscillations were detected largely in the right temporal and frontal cortex, whereas the gamma oscillations were more widespread. What is the importance of gamma and theta power for memory formation? Both types of activity have been implicated in processes that are crucial to memory encoding — gamma oscillations in attention, and theta oscillations in long-term potentiation. Approaches such as that of Sederberg et al. should help us to understand how different cortical regions are recruited to these processes as new memories are laid down, and to clarify the precise role of this rhythmic activity in memory formation. Heather Wood References and links ORIGINAL RESEARCH PAPER Sederberg, P. B. et al. Theta and gamma oscillations during encoding predict subsequent recall. J. Neurosci. 23, 10809–10814 (2003) FURTHER READING Paller, K. A. & Wagner, A. D. Observing the transformation of experience into memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 93–102 (2002) WEB SITE Encyclopedia of Life Sciences: http://www.els.net/ Learning and memory Antibiotic yields anxiolytic NEUROPHARMACOLOGY Benzodiazepines such as diazepam are commonly prescribed to control anxiety, and act by enhancing the action of the neurotransmitter GABA (γ-aminobutyric acid) at the brain GABA A receptor. But drugs of this class have sedative side effects, an undesirable feature that has spurred a search for alternatives by Kelvin Gee and colleagues. In a Nature Medicine advanced online publication, the team describe a first-in-class anxiolytic that is free of sedative side effects. The search strategy devised by Gee et al. was based on the hypothesis that mitigation of side effects might be achieved by targeting non-benzodiazepine-binding sites of GABA A receptors. Compounds related to the fluoroquinolone anti- biotic norfloxacin were chosen as screening candidates for two reasons: first, a small proportion of patients taking this antibiotic experience anxiety as a side effect; second, norfloxacin is known to antagonize GABA A receptors at an undefined site. So, chemical modification of norfloxacin could generate compounds with the opposite, GABA-enhancing anxiolytic effect. In a standard assay for GABA modulation, several norfloxacin- related chemotypes inhibited the binding of the convulsant TBPS to GABA A receptors. Analysis of the most active product, dubbed compound 4, showed that this compound does not act directly on the GABA A receptor-binding sites for TBPS, norfloxacin, benzodiazepines or GABA. To confirm the GABA-modulatory action of compound 4, electrophysio- logical studies were conducted on HEK293 cells expressing different human GABA A receptor subtypes. Compound 4 enhanced GABA- mediated currents in a dose- dependent manner, but only in those cells that expressed the α 2 subunit of the receptor. Failure of the benzo- diazepine binding-site antagonist flumazenil to block this potentiation confirmed that compound 4 binds to a non-benzodiazepine site. So struc- tural manipulation of norfloxacin did produce a compound that binds to an alternative GABA A receptor site, but did this modification result in anxi- olytic rather than anxiety-inducing effects? In two rodent models of anxiety, treatment with compound 4 relieved anxiety-related behaviour. Notably, the magnitude of this effect was compara- ble to that afforded by diazepam. But the real breakthrough came from assessments of motor performance using the rotarod test — diazepam caused motor impairment whereas compound 4 did not, even at the maximal soluble dose. Compound 4 might therefore be at the forefront of a new approach to the design of robust anxiolytics with improved side-effect profiles. Suzanne Farley References and links ORIGINAL RESEARCH PAPER Johnstone, T. B. C. et al. Modifying quinolone antibiotics yields new anxiolytics. Nature Med. 30 November 2003 (doi: 10.1038/nm967) FURTHER READING Christopoulos, A. Allosteric binding sites on cell-surface receptors: novel targets for drug discovery. Nature Rev. Drug Discov. 1, 198–210 (2002) Oscillations predict recall MEMORY COOH O N N F C 2 H 5 HN HN Cl OH O O N Compound 4 Norfloxacin ©2004 Nature Publishing Group

Memory: Oscillations predict recall

  • View
    212

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

H I G H L I G H T S

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 5 | JANUARY 2004 | 7

If a magician gave you a list of words to memorize, and was thenable to predict precisely which words you would be able to recall,you would no doubt be impressed. Reporting in the Journal ofNeuroscience, Sederberg and colleagues show that this feat can beachieved without resorting to magic or ‘mind-control’ tricks.Using intracranial recordings from the cortex, they identifiedspecific patterns of rhythmic neuronal activity that correlatedwith successful memory formation.

Sederberg et al. used a group of 10 patients, aged from 8–17,each of whom had up to 128 electrodes implanted into theirbrains to monitor epilepsy. The patients were asked to memorizea list of 20 common nouns while the authors recorded theircortical and subcortical brain activity. The patients were thenasked to recall as many words as possible, after being distractedbriefly with an arithmetical task to reduce bias towards the mostrecently presented words.

The authors observed increases in both low-frequency thetaoscillations (4–8 Hz) and high-frequency gamma oscillations(28–64 Hz) during the presentation of words that weresubsequently recalled successfully. The theta oscillations weredetected largely in the right temporal and frontal cortex, whereasthe gamma oscillations were more widespread.

What is the importance of gamma and theta power formemory formation? Both types of activity have been implicatedin processes that are crucial to memory encoding — gammaoscillations in attention, and theta oscillations in long-termpotentiation. Approaches such as that of Sederberg et al. shouldhelp us to understand how different cortical regions arerecruited to these processes as new memories are laid down, andto clarify the precise role of this rhythmic activity in memoryformation.

Heather WoodReferences and links

ORIGINAL RESEARCH PAPER Sederberg, P. B. et al. Theta and gamma oscillationsduring encoding predict subsequent recall. J. Neurosci. 23, 10809–10814 (2003)FURTHER READING Paller, K. A. & Wagner, A. D. Observing the transformation ofexperience into memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 93–102 (2002)WEB SITEEncyclopedia of Life Sciences: http://www.els.net/Learning and memory

Antibiotic yields anxiolytic

N E U R O P H A R M A C O LO G Y

Benzodiazepines such as diazepamare commonly prescribed to controlanxiety, and act by enhancing theaction of the neurotransmitterGABA (γ-aminobutyric acid) at thebrain GABA

Areceptor. But drugs of

this class have sedative side effects,an undesirable feature that hasspurred a search for alternatives byKelvin Gee and colleagues. In aNature Medicine advanced onlinepublication, the team describe afirst-in-class anxiolytic that is free ofsedative side effects.

The search strategy devised byGee et al. was based on the hypothesisthat mitigation of side effects might be achieved by targeting non-benzodiazepine-binding sites ofGABA

Areceptors. Compounds

related to the fluoroquinolone anti-biotic norfloxacin were chosen asscreening candidates for two reasons:first, a small proportion of patientstaking this antibiotic experience anxiety as a side effect; second,norfloxacin is known to antagonizeGABA

Areceptors at an undefined

site. So, chemical modification of norfloxacin could generate compounds with the opposite,GABA-enhancing anxiolytic effect.

In a standard assay for GABAmodulation, several norfloxacin-related chemotypes inhibited thebinding of the convulsant TBPS to GABA

Areceptors. Analysis of

the most active product, dubbedcompound 4, showed that this compound does not act directly onthe GABA

Areceptor-binding sites for

TBPS, norfloxacin, benzodiazepinesor GABA.

To confirm the GABA-modulatoryaction of compound 4, electrophysio-logical studies were conducted onHEK293 cells expressing differenthuman GABA

Areceptor subtypes.

Compound 4 enhanced GABA-mediated currents in a dose-dependent manner, but only in thosecells that expressed the α

2subunit

of the receptor. Failure of the benzo-diazepine binding-site antagonist flumazenil to block this potentiation confirmed that compound 4 binds to a non-benzodiazepine site. So struc-tural manipulation of norfloxacin didproduce a compound that binds to an alternative GABA

Areceptor site, but

did this modification result in anxi-olytic rather than anxiety-inducingeffects?

In two rodent models of anxiety,treatment with compound 4 relievedanxiety-related behaviour. Notably, themagnitude of this effect was compara-ble to that afforded by diazepam. Butthe real breakthrough came fromassessments of motor performanceusing the rotarod test — diazepamcaused motor impairment whereascompound 4 did not, even at the maximal soluble dose. Compound 4might therefore be at the forefront of anew approach to the design of robustanxiolytics with improved side-effectprofiles.

Suzanne FarleyReferences and links

ORIGINAL RESEARCH PAPER Johnstone, T. B. C.et al. Modifying quinolone antibiotics yields newanxiolytics. Nature Med. 30 November 2003 (doi:10.1038/nm967)FURTHER READING Christopoulos, A. Allostericbinding sites on cell-surface receptors: noveltargets for drug discovery. Nature Rev. DrugDiscov. 1, 198–210 (2002)

Oscillations predict recall

M E M O R Y

COOH

O

NN

F

C2H5

HN

HN

Cl

OH

O O

N

Compound 4

Norfloxacin

© 2004 Nature Publishing Group