2
Metacognition and Cognitive- Behaviour Therapy: A Special Issue Adrian Wells 1 * and Christine Purdon 2 1 Manchester University, Manchester, UK 2 University of Waterloo, Canada Cognitive-behavioural approaches explaining the development and persistence of psychological disorder have resulted in the development of some of the most effective treatment interventions to date. However, there is still much to accomplish in understanding and treating psychological dis- order. Moreover, a number of recent theorists have emphasized the limitations of general cognitive theory such as Schema theory, and have suggested revised frameworks for conceptualizing cognition in emotional dysfunction. Cognitive-behavioural approaches have tended to focus on a limited range of cognition in explaining psychological disorder. In particular, the focus has been dominated by a consideration of the content of thoughts and beliefs rather than cognitive processes such as attention. It is also unclear how key aspects of cognition such as beliefs and their effects on information processing should be represented in information processing terms. Beliefs about the social and physical self figure predominantly in cognitive models of anxiety and depression, and treatment focuses on modifying the content of these anxiogenic or depressogenic thoughts and beliefs. However, there are many other categories of belief and self-knowledge, such as the knowledge that individuals have about their own thinking. Such knowledge undoubtedly imparts an effect on appraisals and behaviour, two key variables implicated in the maintenance of psychological disorder. It is our view that a consideration of the area of metacognition will allow us to proceed beyond these limitations. Flavell (1979) introduced the term ‘meta-cognition’ in the context of developmental psychology to describe the cognitive processes and structures that monitor and control aspects of cognition. Metacognition is the aspect of the information processing system that monitors, inter- prets, evaluates, and regulates the contents and processes of its own organization. According to Flavell (1979) and Moses and Baird (1998), there are two general components of metacognition, that they referred to as ‘meta-cognitive knowledge’ and ‘metacognitive regulation’. Metacognitive knowl- edge consists primarily of knowledge or beliefs about the factors that affect the course and outcome of cognitive enterprises. This knowledge may be accurate or inaccurate and, like other types of information stored in memory, can be triggered unintentionally by retrieval cues. Once activated, metacognitive knowledge is likely to influence the course of thought processes. Metacognitive regula- tion refers to a number of executive functions, such as planning, resource allocation (i.e. selective atten- tion), monitoring, checking, and error detection and correction, which in turn reflect either monitoring or control processes. The monitoring process keeps track of ongoing cognition, whereas the control process modifies ongoing cognitive (e.g. by shifting attentional focus). The control and monitoring processes interact with metacognitive knowledge. Recent models of psychopathology, particularly obsessive-compulsive disorder, have begun to emphasize the role of beliefs about one’s thoughts and appraisal of thoughts themselves in the development and persistence of the disorder, and this emphasis has proven valuable in advancing our understanding and treatment of emotional prob- lems. However, such models have not focused on the metacognitive elements of these beliefs/apprai- sals explicitly. In independent writings we have argued for a specific consideration of metacognition in theoretical and treatment approaches to psycho- logical disorder. There are several potential advant- ages to explicitly examining metacognition in this context. In a broader context metacognition has been conceptualized as general purpose plans that CCC 1063–3995/99/020071–02$17.50 Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 6, 71–72 (1999) *Correspondence to: Dr A Wells, University of Manchester, Dept. of Clinical Psychology, Rawnsley Building, Manchester Royal Infirmary, Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9WL, UK.

Metacognition and cognitive-behaviour therapy: a special issue

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Metacognition and Cognitive-Behaviour Therapy: A Special Issue

Adrian Wells1* and Christine Purdon2

1Manchester University, Manchester, UK2University of Waterloo, Canada

Cognitive-behavioural approaches explaining thedevelopment and persistence of psychologicaldisorder have resulted in the development ofsome of the most effective treatment interventionsto date. However, there is still much to accomplishin understanding and treating psychological dis-order. Moreover, a number of recent theorists haveemphasized the limitations of general cognitivetheory such as Schema theory, and have suggestedrevised frameworks for conceptualizing cognitionin emotional dysfunction.

Cognitive-behavioural approaches have tended tofocus on a limited range of cognition in explainingpsychological disorder. In particular, the focus hasbeen dominated by a consideration of the content ofthoughts and beliefs rather than cognitive processessuch as attention. It is also unclear how key aspectsof cognition such as beliefs and their effects oninformation processing should be represented ininformation processing terms. Beliefs about thesocial and physical self figure predominantly incognitive models of anxiety and depression, andtreatment focuses on modifying the content of theseanxiogenic or depressogenic thoughts and beliefs.However, there are many other categories ofbelief and self-knowledge, such as the knowledgethat individuals have about their own thinking.Such knowledge undoubtedly imparts an effecton appraisals and behaviour, two key variablesimplicated in the maintenance of psychologicaldisorder.

It is our view that a consideration of the area ofmetacognition will allow us to proceed beyondthese limitations. Flavell (1979) introduced the term`meta-cognition' in the context of developmentalpsychology to describe the cognitive processesand structures that monitor and control aspects of

cognition. Metacognition is the aspect of theinformation processing system that monitors, inter-prets, evaluates, and regulates the contents andprocesses of its own organization. According toFlavell (1979) and Moses and Baird (1998), there aretwo general components of metacognition, that theyreferred to as `meta-cognitive knowledge' and`metacognitive regulation'. Metacognitive knowl-edge consists primarily of knowledge or beliefsabout the factors that affect the course and outcomeof cognitive enterprises. This knowledge may beaccurate or inaccurate and, like other types ofinformation stored in memory, can be triggeredunintentionally by retrieval cues. Once activated,metacognitive knowledge is likely to influence thecourse of thought processes. Metacognitive regula-tion refers to a number of executive functions, suchas planning, resource allocation (i.e. selective atten-tion), monitoring, checking, and error detection andcorrection, which in turn reflect either monitoringor control processes. The monitoring process keepstrack of ongoing cognition, whereas the controlprocess modifies ongoing cognitive (e.g. by shiftingattentional focus). The control and monitoringprocesses interact with metacognitive knowledge.

Recent models of psychopathology, particularlyobsessive-compulsive disorder, have begun toemphasize the role of beliefs about one's thoughtsand appraisal of thoughts themselves in thedevelopment and persistence of the disorder, andthis emphasis has proven valuable in advancing ourunderstanding and treatment of emotional prob-lems. However, such models have not focused onthe metacognitive elements of these beliefs/apprai-sals explicitly. In independent writings we haveargued for a specific consideration of metacognitionin theoretical and treatment approaches to psycho-logical disorder. There are several potential advant-ages to explicitly examining metacognition in thiscontext. In a broader context metacognition hasbeen conceptualized as general purpose plans that

CCC 1063±3995/99/020071±02$17.50Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Clinical Psychology and PsychotherapyClin. Psychol. Psychother. 6, 71±72 (1999)

*Correspondence to: Dr A Wells, University of Manchester,Dept. of Clinical Psychology, Rawnsley Building, ManchesterRoyal Infirmary, Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9WL, UK.

guide information processing, and maintain mal-adaptive processing configurations responsible foremotional vulnerability, such as those typified byrumination and threat monitoring (Wells andMatthews, 1994, 1996). It follows from this thatthe effectiveness of cognitive therapy may beenhanced by strategies that retrain metacognitiveregulation and monitoring processes. Furthermore,general beliefs about the functioning of mind couldplay a considerable role in how specific thoughtevents are interpreted and responded to. Suchbeliefs would have obvious relevance to emotionaldisorder that is characterized by the persistentrecurrence of unwanted thoughts.

Through this Special Issue we aim to increase theprofile of metacognition as an area of investigationin Cognitive Therapy. To this end we have invitedcontributions from eminent researchers who havebeen working in areas that we believe are directly orindirectly related to metacognition and can help inachieving this aim. In this Special Issue, Nelson andStuart present an extension of theory on metacogni-tion in emotional regulation. Rachman and Shafrandelineate the role of Thought Action Fusion, a typeof cognitive distortion, in obsessions. Wells presentshis metacognitive model and treatment of general-ized anxiety disorder, and reviews evidence for themodel. Bouman and Meijer present a study ofmetacognition and worry specificity in hypo-chondriasis. Purdon and Clark examine the role ofmetacognition in obsessive-compulsive disorderand consider the roles of ego-dystonicity and exces-sive thought control efforts. Matthews, Hillyard andCampbell report a study of metacognition andmaladaptive coping in which both are componentsof Test Anxiety. Borkovec, Hazlett-Stevens, andDiaz discuss the role of positive beliefs about worryin generalized anxiety disorder and in treatment.Emmelkamp and Aardema present an empiricalstudy testing specific relationships between meta-cognitive beliefs and obsessive-compulsive symp-toms. Teasdale examines metacognition andmindfulness and their relevance to preventingdepressive relapse. Finally, Papageorgiou andWells present a study comparing the processand metacognitive dimensions of depressive and

anxious thoughts and their associations withemotional intensity.

REFERENCES

Borkovec, T. D., Hazlett-Stevens, H. and Diaz, M. L.(1999). The role of positive beliefs about worry ingeneralized anxiety disorder and its treatment. ClinicalPsychology and Psychotherapy, 6, 126±138.

Bouman, T. K. and Meijer, K. J. (1999). A preliminarystudy of worry and metacognitions in hypochondriasis.Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6, 96±101.

Emmelkamp, P. M. G. and Aardema, A. (1999). Metacog-nition, specific obsessive-compulsive beliefs andobsessive-compulsive behaviour. Clinical Psychologyand Psychotherapy, 6, 139±145.

Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitor-ing: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry.American Psychologist, 34, 906±911.

Matthews, G., Hillyard, E. J. and Campbell, S. E. (1999).Metacognition and maladaptive coping as componentsof test anxiety. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6,111±125.

Moses, L. J. and Biard, J. A. (1998). Metacognition. In: R. A.Wilson and F. C. Keil (Eds), The MIT Encyclopedia of theCognitive Sciences. Cambridge: MIT Press, in press.

Nelson, T. O., Stuart, R. B.., Howard, C. and Crawley,M. (1999). Metacognition and clinical psychology: Apreliminary framework for research and practice.Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6, 73±79.

Papageorgiou, C. and Wells, A. (1999). Process and meta-cognitive dimensions of depressive and anxiousthoughts and relationships with emotional intensity.Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6, 156±162.

Purdon, C. and Clark, D. A. (1999). Metacognition andobsessions. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6,102±110.

Rachman, S. and Shafran, R. (1999). Cognitive distortions:thought-action fusion. Clinical Psychology and Psy-chotherapy, 6, 80±85.

Teasdale, J. D. (1999). Metacognition, mindfulness and themodification of mood disorders. Clinical Psychology andPsychotherapy, 6, 146±155.

Wells, A. (1999). A metacognitive model and therapy forgeneralized anxiety disorder. Clinical Psychology andPsychotherapy, 6, 86±95.

Wells, A. and Matthews, G. (1994). Attention and Emotion:A Clinical Perspective. Hove, UK: Erlbaum.

Wells, A. and Matthews, G. (1996). Modelling cognition inemotional disorder. The S-REF model. BehaviourResearch and Therapy, 34, 881±888.

Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 6, 71±72 (1999)

72 A. Wells and C. Purdon