14
Research Report On beyond mirror neurons: Internal representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humans Laurel J. Buxbaum a,b, * , Kathleen M. Kyle a , Rukmini Menon a a Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Korman 213, 1200 W. Tabor Road, Philadelphia, PA 19141, USA b Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA 19107, USA Accepted 27 May 2005 Available online 5 July 2005 Abstract A considerable recent literature argues that the same representations, encoded by inferior prefrontal and parietal cells known as ‘‘mirror neurons’’, may be activated in both production and recognition of object-related actions. Here, we test several predictions derived from the contemporary literature on the parity between production and recognition and the putative emergence of the mirror neuron system from a system coding hand – object interactions. Forty-four patients with left-hemisphere stroke, 21 of whom exhibited ideomotor apraxia, performed a number of pantomime imitation and recognition tasks, and performance was scored with respect to hand posture, arm posture, amplitude, and timing. Consistent with predictions, there were strong relationships between object-related pantomime imitation and object-related pantomime recognition, and between imitation and recognition of the hand posture component of object-related actions. Skilled object-related gesture representations are likely to be closely tied to evolutionarily more primitive systems controlling object grasping, to emerge from a mapping between object and action information coded by ventral and dorsal streams, and to be lateralized to the left hemisphere in humans. D 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Theme: Neural basis of behavior Topic: Cognition Keywords: Apraxia; Action; Gesture; Imitation; Recognition 1. Introduction According to the ‘‘direct matching hypothesis,’’ actions performed by others are recognized by activating the same spatiomotor representations used for performing the action oneself. Numerous recent investigations in infant develop- ment (e.g., [37]) and adult cognitive psychology (e.g., [4,47]) suggest that there is a common coding between perception and action. A possible physiological foundation for at least some aspects of this common coding is provided by the recent discovery of so-called ‘‘mirror neurons’’ in the inferior prefrontal cortex (area F5, the putative homologue of human Broca’s area) and inferior parietal lobule (area PF) in the monkey. These cell units discharge both when the monkey produces an object-related action and when a comparable action is performed by an experimenter. The neurons respond best to specific types of prehensile actions upon objects (e.g., grasping), and are silent when a hand alone or object alone is viewed [20,49]. Functional neuroimaging evidence suggests that inferior prefrontal cortex may be involved in both action observation and production in humans as well as in monkeys. Obser- vation and execution of grasping movements or simple finger movements has been shown to activate Brodmann area (BA) 44 or 45 in a number of PET, fMRI, and MEG studies [3,22,28,30,40]. These findings are consistent with the possibility that human prefrontal cortex contains a 0926-6410/$ - see front matter D 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.05.014 * Corresponding author. Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Korman 213, 1200 W. Tabor Road, Philadelphia, PA 19141, USA. Fax: +1 215 456 5926. E-mail address: [email protected] (L.J. Buxbaum). Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2005) 226 – 239 www.elsevier.com/locate/cogbrainres

On Beyong Mirror Neurons

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

mirror neurons

Citation preview

  • ch R

    l re

    jec

    een

    13, 12

    ty, Ph

    27 M

    line 5

    finger movements has been shown to activate Brodmann

    T, fMRI, and MEG

    Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2* Corresponding author. Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Korman1. Introduction

    According to the direct matching hypothesis, actions

    performed by others are recognized by activating the same

    spatiomotor representations used for performing the action

    oneself. Numerous recent investigations in infant develop-

    ment (e.g., [37]) and adult cognitive psychology (e.g.,

    [4,47]) suggest that there is a common coding between

    perception and action. A possible physiological foundation

    for at least some aspects of this common coding is provided

    by the recent discovery of so-called mirror neurons in the

    inferior prefrontal cortex (area F5, the putative homologue

    of human Brocas area) and inferior parietal lobule (area PF)

    in the monkey. These cell units discharge both when the

    monkey produces an object-related action and when a

    comparable action is performed by an experimenter. The

    neurons respond best to specific types of prehensile actions

    upon objects (e.g., grasping), and are silent when a hand

    alone or object alone is viewed [20,49].

    Functional neuroimaging evidence suggests that inferior

    prefrontal cortex may be involved in both action observation

    and production in humans as well as in monkeys. Obser-

    vation and execution of grasping movements or simplecoding handobject interactions. Forty-four patients with left-hemisphere stroke, 21 of whom exhibited ideomotor apraxia, performed a number

    of pantomime imitation and recognition tasks, and performance was scored with respect to hand posture, arm posture, amplitude, and timing.

    Consistent with predictions, there were strong relationships between object-related pantomime imitation and object-related pantomime

    recognition, and between imitation and recognition of the hand posture component of object-related actions. Skilled object-related gesture

    representations are likely to be closely tied to evolutionarily more primitive systems controlling object grasping, to emerge from a mapping

    between object and action information coded by ventral and dorsal streams, and to be lateralized to the left hemisphere in humans.

    D 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

    Theme: Neural basis of behavior

    Topic: Cognition

    Keywords: Apraxia; Action; Gesture; Imitation; RecognitionAbstract

    A considerable recent literature argues that the same representations, encoded by inferior prefrontal and parietal cells known as mirror

    neurons, may be activated in both production and recognition of object-related actions. Here, we test several predictions derived from the

    contemporary literature on the parity between production and recognition and the putative emergence of the mirror neuron system from a systemResear

    On beyond mirror neurons: Interna

    and recognition of skilled ob

    Laurel J. Buxbauma,b,*, Kathl

    aMoss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Korman 2bThomas Jefferson Universi

    Accepted

    Available on0926-6410/$ - see front matter D 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

    doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.05.014

    213, 1200 W. Ta

    5926.

    E-mail address: [email protected] (L.J. Buxbaum).eport

    presentations subserving imitation

    t-related actions in humans

    M. Kylea, Rukmini Menona

    00 W. Tabor Road, Philadelphia, PA 19141, USA

    iladelphia, PA 19107, USA

    ay 2005

    July 2005

    005) 226 239

    www.elsevier.com/locate/cogbrainresarea (BA) 44 or 45 in a number of PEbor Road, Philadelphia, PA 19141, USA. Fax: +1 215 456studies [3,22,28,30,40]. These findings are consistent with

    the possibility that human prefrontal cortex contains a

  • metric three-dimensional shapes [8]. Patients exhibiting this

    pattern often have lesions involving the left inferior parietal

    lobe (IPL) and intraparietal sulcus (IPS). Critically, as will

    be discussed further below, there is also evidence that the

    Brain Research 25 (2005) 226239 227mirror neuron system similar to that described in the

    monkey [20].

    In addition to simple object prehension and simple finger

    movements, humans produce and recognize a considerable

    repertoire of skilled object-related (so-called transitive)

    movements and pantomimes. Unlike simple grasping based

    on object structure, recognition and production of learned

    gestures and pantomimes entail a declarative semantic

    component [32]. Skilled gestures also have strong require-

    ments for decoding the particular spatial configuration of the

    hand and fingers that distinguishes one skilled gesture from

    another. Thus, the evidence that production and observation

    of simple grasping and finger movements involve the same

    or overlapping neural structures does not compel the

    conclusion that imitation and recognition of complex,

    skilled, meaningful behavior share the same underlying

    cognitive representations. Additional support for the latter

    hypothesis must be gleaned from studying the relationship

    of imitation and recognition of complex skilled actions. An

    important source for such data may be found in patients with

    ideomotor apraxia (IM).

    Individuals with IM are deficient in producing transitive

    (familiar object-related) gesture in gesture pantomime (to

    command or sight of object) and gesture imitation tasks.

    Errors may be postural and/or may involved deficits in

    amplitude and timing [4446]. Deficits persist with actual

    object use but are more subtle [44], presumably in part

    because of the feedback from object structure helps to

    constrain degrees of freedom of the movement (see [8]). IM

    occurs in nearly 60% of left-hemisphere cerebral vascular

    accident patients (LCVA) [1], and involves the non-paretic

    left hand of approximately 50% of these patients [34]. In

    contrast to their deficits in pantomiming or imitating familiar

    object-related actions, IM patients are frequently relatively

    unimpaired in the production or imitation of intransitive (non-

    object related, symbolic) gestures such as waving goodbye,

    signaling stop, or beckoning come here [38].

    Evidence from several laboratories, including our own,

    indicates that patients with IM may have particular

    difficulties producing and recognizing the hand postures

    appropriate for skilled object-related actions. In fact, there is

    growing evidence that representations for skilled, object-

    related hand posture may be particularly vulnerable to loss

    in IM, and encoded distinctly from on line programming

    of hand posture for prehensile manipulations of objects.

    Several reports indicate that apraxics impaired hand posture

    may be in contrast to their substantially better arm posture

    and trajectory for skilled actions [55], particularly in

    pantomime tasks [44]. Hand posture errors are the most

    frequent error type of IM patients on skilled sequencing

    tasks [56] and pantomimed prehension tasks [33]. IM

    patients are deficient in recognizing the appropriate hand

    posture for interacting with familiar objects, but perform

    normally in selecting hand postures for prehensile inter-

    L.J. Buxbaum et al. / Cognitiveactions with novel objects in response to their structure [7]

    and relatively normally in reaching to and grasping geo-hand posture component of skilled-object related panto-

    mime is not deficient in these patients simply because it is

    more difficult than other gesture components [9], or more

    difficult than on-line reaching and grasping. For example,

    the observed pattern of deficient skilled object-related

    pantomime in the face of preserved object grasping doubly

    dissociates from the pattern observed in patients with optic

    ataxia due to superior parietal damage, who frequently

    exhibit deficient grasping of visually-presented objects with

    spared pantomime [43], and, in at least some cases, spared

    ability to shape the hand when required to grasp familiar,

    meaningful objects (cf. patient AT, [29]).

    IM has been characterized variously as a disorder of

    learned skilled movements [26], a difficulty making volun-

    tary gestures [35], an impairment in gesturing to command

    [58], and a deficit in the imitation of meaningless movements

    [12]. We have suggested that these characterizations may not

    capture some of the key features of the disorder [5]. The

    marked disparity between performance of transitive and

    intransitive actions across a range of production (e.g., to

    command, to sight of object) and imitation tasks, and the

    relative preservation of on line aspects of motor control

    responsive to the structural features of objects, has influenced

    our characterization of IM as a deficit in stored representa-

    tions of the position and movements of the limb (and

    particularly, the hand) subserving skilled object-related

    actions. Further, these representations are likely to be

    mediated by the left inferior parietal lobe [42].

    The hypothesis that the inferior parietal lobe contains

    gesture representations critical both for production and

    recognition of actions with objects predicts that the two

    should be impaired in parallel in patients with IM due to

    parietal damage. Consistent with this [27], left parietal lesions

    have been associated with both production and recognition

    deficits, and frontal lesions only with production impair-

    ments. Note that this is potentially inconsistent with the

    notion of a mirror neuron system in the frontal lobe that is

    critical for gesture recognition. Several more recent studies

    have suggested more generically that IM patients are

    impaired in the recognition of gesture (e.g., [14,61]). On

    the other hand, other recent studies have failed to find an

    association between gesture production and recognition

    [15,24,31]. It should be noted that these studies have used

    small samples ranging from 1 to 14 patients, and it remains

    possible that there was insufficient power to detect an

    association.1

    1 One exception is a study by Bell [2] with 38 subjects; however, the

    pantomime recognition task used in that study required subjects to

    understand the association between an observed gesture and an associatedobject, and it is possible that this requires more extensive semantic

    knowledge than that tapped by gesture recognition alone.

  • Forty-four left-hemisphere stroke patients participated in

    the study. All patients had suffered a single left-hemisphere

    basis of the average of scores on (1) novel gesture imitation

    and (2) gesture to sight of objects.2 Appendix A provides

    details of the administration and scoring of the meaningless

    gesture imitation and gesture to command tasks, as well as

    transitive and intransitive gesture imitation tasks to be

    described below.

    Table 1 shows subject demographics as well as scores on

    the IM apraxia composite score. Scores on the two tests

    comprising the composite score (gesture to sight of object

    and meaningless gesture imitation) were highly correlated

    (r = 0.74, P < 0.0001). Subjects were characterized as

    Brain Research 25 (2005) 226239cerebral vascular accident; one subject had an additional

    small asymptomatic right occipital infarct pre-dating the

    left-hemisphere stroke. All subjects gave informed consent

    to participate in accordance with guidelines of Albert

    Einstein Healthcare Network and were paid for their

    participation. Subjects were referred to the study from a

    large database of potential research subjects in the Phila-

    delphia area maintained by Moss Rehabilitation Research

    Institute. Subjects were excluded if database records

    indicated language comprehension deficits of sufficient

    severity to preclude comprehension of task instructions.

    Subjects over the age of 80 and/or with histories of co-

    morbid neurologic disorders, alcohol or drug abuse, or

    psychosis were also excluded. All subjects gave informed

    consent to participate in accordance with the guidelines of

    the IRB of Albert Einstein Healthcare Network, and were

    paid for their participation.

    We pursued two complimentary strategies in analyzing

    the data. The first strategy was to classify participants as

    apraxic or not, and assess whether performance of the two

    groups differed on measures of interest. The second strategy

    was to treat data as continuous variables to assess the

    strength of relationships of scores on the measures of

    interest.We derive a number of predictions from the hypothesis

    that IM reflects deficient inferior parietal representations

    subserving the production, imitation, and recognition of

    skilled object-related gesture pantomime and skilled hand

    object interactions, six of which we test here. The first

    prediction is that patients with IM should be disproportion-

    ately impaired in the imitation of transitive as compared to

    intransitive gestures. The second is that they should be

    particularly impaired in the hand posture component of

    gesture imitation. The third is that we should observe an

    association between performance of transitive imitation and

    transitive recognition tasks. The fourth is that there should

    be a similar and more specific association between

    imitation and recognition of the hand posture component

    of transitive actions. The fifth is that the association

    between transitive recognition and intransitive gesture

    imitation should be considerably weaker. The sixth is that

    patients with deficient hand posture production and

    recognition should have lesions that include the left inferior

    parietal lobe. In the study that follows, we test these

    predictions with a group of 44 left-hemisphere stroke

    patients. We then discuss the implications of the data for

    informing the likely characteristics of skilled object-related

    action representations.

    2. Subjects

    L.J. Buxbaum et al. / Cognitive228Subjects were characterized as exhibiting ideomotor

    apraxia (hereafter, IM) or not (hereafter, LCVA) on the4. Lesion analysis

    Clinical T-1 or T-2-weighted MRI scans were available

    for 36 of the 44 subjects (18 LCVA and 18 IM). Lesions

    were segmented and interpreted by an experienced neuro-

    logist. Subtractions of the lesioned regions of the IM versus

    LCVA groups were performed by one of the authors using

    the MRIcro image analysis program developed by Dr. Chris

    Rorden (see http://www.psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/

    cr1/mricro.html). Fig. 1 shows the result of this subtraction

    2 The use of a composite of novel gesture imitation and gesture to sight of

    objects to define apraxia was based on several considerations. First, the

    study reported here is one of several studies ongoing in our laboratory

    assessing various aspects of IM, and the designation of IM in these related

    studies is inclusive, as it is designed to detect IM due either to loss of

    knowledge of the gestures associated with objects and/or deficits in praxis

    production. Second, although these aspects of praxis can dissociate, in the

    great majority of patients they tend to co-occur (see text for evidence of

    strong correlation in the present study). Third, as described earlier, there is

    considerable disagreement in the literature regarding the most appropriate3. Language comprehension

    Participants performed the Comprehension subtest of the

    Western Aphasia Battery (Kertesz Ref). Maximum possible

    score was 200 points. Apraxics exhibited somewhat greater

    deficits in comprehension (mean = 166, range = 117196)

    than did LCVA (mean = 187, range = 111200), t(42) = 3.1,

    P < 0.01.exhibiting IM if they obtained a composite score more than

    2 standard deviations below the mean of age matched right-

    handed control subjects who performed the tasks with their

    left hands (control n = 10; 5 females; mean age 64.7, range

    4377; mean education 14 years, range 1018 years; mean

    score = 92.5, SD = 4.5, range = 8598). Subjects

    characterized as IM (n = 21) and LCVA (n = 23) by this

    criterion did not differ in age (t = 0.50, P = 0.70), education

    (t = 0.65, P = 0.63), or months elapsed between lesion and

    test (t = 0.82, P = 0.25).test for IM. The combined score captures patients who would be defined as

    having IM by many published criteria.

  • ure co

    A7 IM 38 50 44

    A8 IM 73 58 65

    BrainA9 IM 63 58 60

    A10 IM 65 75 70

    A11 IM 63 63 63Table 1

    Subject demographics and scores on gesture production and imitation

    Subject Group Gesture to sight of object Meaningless imitation Gest

    A1 IM 64 60 62

    A2 IM 75 58 66

    A3 IM 60 40 50

    A4 IM 45 73 59

    A5 IM 75 75 75

    A6 IM 55 25 40

    L.J. Buxbaum et al. / Cognitiveanalysis. The lesion loci in the IM patients is consistent with

    previous reports (e.g., [23]).

    5. Experimental tasks

    5.1. Study 1: imitation of transitive and intransitive gesture

    In the first study, we sought to replicate previously

    reported findings that patients with IM are more likely to

    have difficulties with imitation of transitive as compared to

    intransitive gesture pantomimes. We also assessed the

    prediction that IM patients would have disproportionate

    difficulty in producing the hand posture component of

    A12 IM 75 58 66

    A13 IM 70 68 69

    A14 IM 50 78 64

    A15 IM 65 58 61

    A16 IM 60 50 55

    A17 IM 55 90 73

    A18 IM 75 58 66

    A19 IM 50 53 51

    A20 IM 65 50 58

    A21 IM 58 65 61

    L1 LCVA 90 90 90

    L2 LCVA 85 93 89

    L3 LCVA 88 90 89

    L4 LCVA 88 93 90

    L5 LCVA 90 93 91

    L6 LCVA 90 93 91

    L7 LCVA 97 85 91

    L8 LCVA 85 90 88

    L9 LCVA 93 93 93

    L10 LCVA 88 93 90

    L11 LCVA 88 95 91

    L12 LCVA 90 93 91

    L13 LCVA 90 95 93

    L14 LCVA 88 100 94

    L15 LCVA 95 95 95

    L16 LCVA 100 93 96

    L17 LCVA 88 93 90

    L18 LCVA 93 93 93

    L19 LCVA 83 90 86

    L20 LCVA 93 88 90

    L21 LCVA 78 95 86

    L22 LCVA 85 85 85

    L23 LCVA 90 98 94mposite Age Education Gender Handedness Lesion volume (cm3)

    55 18 F R 208.4

    49 16 F R 110.5

    79 12 F R 58.2

    79 11 F R 8.5

    50 12 M R 64.3

    59 16 M R 68.7

    63 12 F R 253.5

    67 12 F R 7.2

    56 14 M R 44.9

    79 16 F R NA

    64 12 M R NA

    Research 25 (2005) 226239 229transitive gestures. In addition to the IM and LCVA patients,

    also tested were the same 10 age-matched healthy control

    subjects described earlier.

    5.1.1. Methods

    Participants watched videotapes of an examiner perform-

    ing 10 transitive and 5 intransitive pantomimes with the

    right hand, and were asked to imitate the gesture as precisely

    as possible with the unimpaired left hand. Transitive

    gestures were hammering, cutting with scissors, sawing,

    using a screwdriver, writing with a pencil, using a comb,

    winding a watch, brushing teeth, flipping a coin, and eating

    with a fork. Intransitive gestures were saluting, waving

    goodbye, hitch-hiking, signaling stop, and beckoning

    50 10 F R 151.9

    49 8 M R 49.0

    42 16 F R 143.6

    60 12 M R 161.5

    78 11 M R 51.1

    42 16 F R 131.3

    67 14 M R 119.7

    39 10 M R 180.8

    58 10 F R 45.3

    41 10 F R 46.1

    35 12 F R 41.8

    56 20 M R 96.8

    51 14 F R 56.7

    64 12 M R 77.4

    77 12 M R NA

    42 15 F R 16.8

    55 12 M R 69.3

    51 12 F R 41.4

    58 12 M R 58.6

    58 3 M R 0.5

    50 12 M R NA

    65 19 M R 61.6

    77 16 F R 13.6

    50 18 F R 31.2

    51 8 M L 28.0

    80 12 F R 0.4

    55 16 F R 18.4

    56 12 M R 22.4

    54 12 F R NA

    42 8 M R 178.0

    54 18 M L 95.1

    40 16 F R 142.2

    64 16 M R 25.6

  • Four KruskalWallis ANOVAs were performed, one with

    each gesture component; therefore, a Bonferroni-corrected

    P value of .0125 was required for significance. Again, we

    tested for between-group differences in transitive versus

    e indicate the difference in the proportion of patients in the two groups having

    ate relatively more lesion in IM group in increments of 20%. Left and right are

    er, there were several regions in which lesions were more likely. These include

    39 and 40 (inferior parietal).

    L.J. Buxbaum et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2005) 226239230come here. Participants were permitted to begin imitation

    while watching the videos. Gestures were scored according

    to the detailed error taxonomy described in Buxbaum,

    Giovannetti, and Libon [6] (and see [8,9]) and detailed in

    Appendix A.3

    5.1.2. Results

    Scores are reported here in terms of percent correct: IM

    transitive mean = 63%, intransitive mean = 89%, mean

    transitive intransitive difference = 25.6%; LCVA tran-

    sitive mean = 93%, intransitive mean = 99%, mean

    difference = 4.7%; CTL transitive mean = 93%, intransitive

    mean = 100%, mean difference = 5.8%. In this and all

    subsequent analyses, the difference between transitive and

    intransitive performance was calculated for each subject,

    Fig. 1. Subtractions of lesioned regions of IM versus LCVA groups. Thes

    involvement in a given region. Colors further to right on the color bar indic

    reversed. There were no regions uniquely damaged in the IM group; howev

    Brodmann areas 6 and 44 (dorsolateral frontal), 22 and 37 (temporal), andand between-group comparisons of the difference scores

    were performed with KruskalWallis nonparametric one-

    way ANOVAs. Post hoc testing was performed with Mann

    Whitney tests. There was an effect of group, H = 28.36, P

    0.36). These data replicate previous reports that object-

    related gestures are more difficult for IM patients than are

    symbolic gestures.

    In the next analysis, we assessed whether IM patients

    were equally impaired in all components of transitive

    gesture. Fig. 2 shows the data entered into these analyses.

    3 To assess scoring reliability, gestures for 6 of the participants were

    scored by 2 independent coders. Percent agreement between the coders

    ranged from 78% to 100% across the 6 subjects (mean 88% agreement;

    Cohens kappa = 0.60).Fig. 2. Performance of IM, LCVA, and CTL groups in imitation of

    intransitive and transitive gesture pantomimes, scored for hand posture

    (HP), arm posture (AP), amplitude (AMP), and timing (TIM) components.

  • Brainintransitive performance. There were significant between-

    group differences in hand posture, H = 35.2, P < 0.0001;

    arm posture, H = 10.5, P = 0.003; and amplitude, H = 12.3,

    P = 0.002. Post hoc testing of the 3 significant ANOVAs

    with MannWhitney tests using a Bonferroni-corrected P

    value of .0055 (i.e., 0.05/9) indicated that there were

    significant differences between the IM patients and the

    other two groups for hand posture and amplitude (P 0.27). Thus, the

    disproportionate disparity between transitive and intransi-

    tive gesture in the IM group was reliably observed in several

    gesture components.

    In a final analysis, we addressed the concern that the

    disproportionate impairment in transitive as compared to

    intransitive hand postures in the IM group may be related

    to the greater complexity of the former. We examined the

    data from the IM and LCVA groups for a subset of 5

    transitive gestures having a simple, stable hand posture

    denoting grasp of a tool (hammering, sawing, combing

    hair, brushing teeth, and eating with a fork) and all 5 of the

    intransitive gestures examined previously (waving good-

    bye, beckoning come here, hitch-hiking, signaling stop,

    and saluting). Within-group comparisons were performed

    with Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Tests (with a Bonferroni-

    corrected P value of .008, i.e., .05/6, required for signi-

    ficance). For transitive movements, IM patients hand

    posture (mean 50.4% correct) tended to be more deficient

    than arm posture (66.7%; P = 0.02), amplitude (69%; P =

    0.01), or timing (82%; P = 0.009). For intransitive

    imitation, IM patients hand posture (mean 91% correct)

    tended to be better than arm posture (84%; P = 0.01), and

    was equal to amplitude (91%) and timing (89.5%). In

    between-group comparisons, MannWhitney U tests (with

    a Bonferroni-corrected P value of 0.016 required for

    significance) confirmed that the disparity between transitive

    and intransitive hand postures was more pronounced for the

    IM patients than the other two groups (Ps < 0.008), who

    did not differ from one another (P = 0.5).

    5.1.3. Discussion

    In this study, we replicated previous findings indicating

    that transitive gesture imitation is more impaired in patients

    with IM than is intransitive imitation. Moreover, consistent

    with the possibility that the system that is damaged in IM

    patients is specialized for handobject interactions, the hand

    posture component of gesture imitation for transitive

    (object-related) gestures proved to be the most impaired

    aspect of IM patients performance.

    One possible objection to the proposed interpretation is

    that transitive hand postures may simply be more difficult

    than other components of transitive gesture, and more

    difficult than intransitive hand postures, and thus more

    L.J. Buxbaum et al. / Cognitivesensitive to any type of impairment. There are two lines of

    evidence against this interpretation. First, when we com-pared IM and LCVA patients performance in imitation of

    intransitive hand postures and transitive postures having a

    simple, stable grasp configuration, the intransitive hand

    postures were still strikingly superior. Another line of

    evidence comes from data we have reported from 4 patients

    with corticobasal degeneration (CBD), a degenerative

    disorder affecting primarily the superior parietal lobes (areas

    5 and 7 bilaterally) in early stages of disease progression [9].

    The disorder has been known as primary progressive

    apraxia because of its devastating effects on action

    production. Although the CBD patients were more impaired

    than the IM patients overall, they were less impaired in the

    hand posture component of transitive gesture imitation

    (mean 88% correct) than arm posture (46%), amplitude

    (54%), or timing (58%). The data are consistent across

    subjects: hand posture was the least impaired gesture

    component in all 4 CBD subjects. Additionally, unlike the

    patients with IM due to stroke reported here, the transitive

    hand postures of the CBD patients (mean 88% correct) were

    slightly better than their intransitive hand postures (mean

    80% correct). These data strongly suggest that transitive

    hand posture is not simply more sensitive to brain damage

    than other aspects of gesture imitation. The system damaged

    in the IM patients appears to be particularly strongly

    involved in coding information about the position of the

    hand for object-related gestures.

    It is also of note that Mozaz et al. [38] recently argued

    that the frequently observed difference between transitive

    and intransitive gestures cannot be reduced to differences in

    movement complexity. Healthy subjects were asked to

    produce both gesture types as well as to discriminate static

    photographs of transitive and intransitive gestures. They

    performed more poorly with transitive gestures, both on the

    production and picture discrimination tasks. The investi-

    gators argued that the difficulty with the latter is not likely

    attributable to the differential complexity of transitive versus

    intransitive movements, but instead reflects differences in

    the underlying representations of the two gesture types.

    One additional point of interest in the present study is that

    the deficit in transitive gestures, and in transitive hand posture

    in particular, was observed on an imitation task. According to

    2-route models of gesture production, gesture imitation can

    be accomplished via a direct route that bypasses repre-

    sentational knowledge, but permits calculation of the current

    position of the actors body parts in space, and transformation

    of these coordinates into a body-centered system of coor-

    dinates appropriate for the observers action [5,21]. Presum-

    ably, such a route would be engaged regardless of whether a

    gesture was meaningful or not, and transitive or not. The use

    of such a direct route would not explain the difference

    between transitive and intransitive gestures, unless again the

    former were simply harder in terms of spatiomotor trans-

    coding, and as we have discussed, the data from the CBD

    patients speak against this possibility. Instead, these data

    Research 25 (2005) 226239 231suggest that a representational (or indirect) system that is

    sensitive to gesture transitivity is recruited for imitation

  • Data from the Spatial condition of the gesture recognition

    task are shown in Fig. 3. It can be seen that IM patients are

    more impaired in recognition of the hand posture compo-

    nent of gestures than in the other components.

    The data from the Spatial recognition condition were

    subjected to a repeated measures ANOVA with group as a

    between-subjects factor and gesture component (hand

    posture, arm posture, amplitude/timing) as a within-subjects

    factor. There was a significant main effect of group,

    F(2,51) = 28.1, P < 0.0001, and of gesture component,

    F(2,102) = 18.1, P < 0.0001, and a significant interaction

    of group gesture component, F(4,102) = 6.6, P 0.1]. Thus, taking into account

    the poorer comprehension of the IM group does not explain

    their particularly strong predilection toward errors in hand

    posture recognition.

    5.2.3. Discussion

    Although previous literature led us to expect that IM

    patients would be impaired in both semantic and spatial

    aspects of gesture recognition ([19,27,59], the findings with

    regard to semantic gesture recognition were not strongly

    consistent with this expectation. Although IM patients were

    indeed more impaired than non-IM patients in matching

    auditory and spoken words to gestures presented with

    semantic foils, that difference disappeared when we

    controlled for comprehension severity. This may be an

    artifact of the recognition test requirements, which had a

    strong language comprehension component. Another possi-

    bility is that the same underlying semantic processes are

    required both for the semantic aspects of gesture recognition

    and language comprehension. We are currently constructing

    a pre-test to assess comprehension of the test items that will

    help to distinguish these possibilities.

    The results from the spatial version of the gesture

    recognition task indicate that not all aspects of IM patients

    recognition problems are confounded with language impair-

    ment. IM patients were significantly worse than non-apraxics

    in distinguishing correct gestures from spatially-similar foils,

    and the difference between groups in recognition of the hand

    posture component of the gesture persisted even when

    comprehension was controlled. In the remaining analyses,

    therefore, we focus on the spatial aspects of gesture re-

    cognition and in particular the hand posture component, and

    the relationship of spatial gesture recognition to gesture

    production.

    5.3. Additional analyses of the relationship of production

    and spatial gesture recognition

    On the hypothesis that the same representations subserve

    production and recognition of transitive (but not intransi-

    L.J. Buxbaum et al. / Cognitivetive) gesture, we expect the former relationship to be

    stronger than the latter.To assess the prediction that components of transitive

    gesture recognition (recognition of hand posture, arm

    posture, amplitude/timing) should correlate more strongly

    with production of these same gesture components in

    transitive than in intransitive gesture, we performed non-

    parametric (Spearman) correlational analyses of the compo-

    nents of gesture recognition (hand posture, arm posture and

    amplitude/timing) and these same components in the

    transitive and intransitive imitation tasks. As 18 correlations

    were performed, a Bonferroni-corrected P value of

  • scores for the Low Recognition and High Recognition

    Groups using a MannWhitney Test. As shown in Fig. 4,

    there was greater relative impairment of the Low Recog-

    nition group in transitive as compared to intransitive

    gestures (U = 62.0, P < 0.0001).

    5.3.1. Discussion

    The data from three analyses of the relationship between

    gesture production and recognition tell a consistent story.

    The first analysis showed that transitive gesture imitation is

    a strong and unique predictor of transitive gesture recog-

    nition. The second analysis showed that individual compo-

    nents of transitive gesture recognition (hand posture, arm

    posture, and amplitude/timing) are more strongly related to

    these same components instantiated in transitive as com-

    pared to intransitive gesture imitation. The third analysis

    L.J. Buxbaum et al. / Cognitive Brain234showed that patients who fare poorly in transitive gesture

    recognition are disproportionately impaired in transitive (as

    compared to intransitive) imitation, whereas patients who

    are better at transitive gesture recognition show less

    disparity between the two types of imitation. Together,

    these data argue that the same representations subserve the

    recognition and imitation of transitive gesture. Furthermore,

    they suggest that these representations may be componen-

    tial, with hand posture representations for transitive actions

    particularly vulnerable to disruption in left-hemisphere

    stroke (but not in CBD, as noted earlier). In the General

    discussion section, we will present a model that addresses

    what may be special about these representations for

    transitive gesture and how they may differ from intransitive

    gesture representations.

    5.4. Neuroanatomy

    A final question regards the neuroanatomic basis of

    transitive gesture representations, and particularly the

    component of those representations that is specialized for

    handobject interactions. As noted, mirror neurons active

    Fig. 4. Transitive and intransitive gesture imitation performance of all

    patients, divided into High Gesture Recognition and Low GestureRecognition groups on the basis of their performance of the Spatial

    Gesture Recognition task.during observation of grasping actions have been identified

    in monkey F5, the putative homologue of BA 45 in humans,

    as well as in the inferior parietal lobe. In the present study,

    we approached lesion analyses in two ways to be described

    below.

    5.4.1. Methods

    Patients lesions were segmented by a neurologist (H. B.

    Coslett) and drawn into MRIcro software by an experienced

    physician (R. Menon). Brodmann areas were identified by

    the first author and Dr. Coslett, who were blinded to

    subjects identities, using templates from Damasio and

    Damasio [11] and Mai and Assheuer [36].

    Patients were ranked in terms of their performance on

    the Spatial gesture recognition task, the hand posture score

    from the gesture recognition task, the total score from the

    transitive gesture imitation task, and the hand posture score

    from the transitive gesture imitation task (4 separate

    rankings). For each measure, we divided the patient group

    into thirds (insofar as permitted due to ties) and discarded

    the patients whose performance was in the central third of

    the distribution. The lesion data from the high and low

    performing groups on the recognition task are shown in

    Fig. 5.

    We used MRIcro software to identify whether a lesion

    was present or absent in 5 Brodmann areas of interest: areas

    44 and 45 (inferior prefrontal) and areas 39, 40, and 7

    (posterior parietal lobe and intraparietal sulcus) in the high

    versus low performing group. Lesions in BA 39 (angular

    gyrus) and the inferior portion of area 7, including the

    superior bank of the intraparietal sulcus, were more

    frequently associated with low Spatial gesture recognition

    scores than high Spatial recognition scores v2 > 4.6, P 4.3,P < 0.02 for both), and with low total gesture imitation

    scores than high total gesture imitation scores (v2 > 5.0, P 0.2).

    5.4.2. Discussion

    Lesion analyses suggest that in stroke patients, the

    lesion(s) significantly associated with deficits in the

    recognition of transitive gesture, and the hand posture

    component of transitive gesture, are located in the inferior

    parietal lobe and intraparietal sulcus. For prefrontal cortex,

    in contrast, the association with deficits in gesture recog-

    nition or imitation did not even approach significance. This

    is at least partially consistent with earlier findings of Varney

    and Damasio [60] indicating that patients with deficits in

    Research 25 (2005) 226239matching pantomimes to associated objects were likely to

    have lesions in area 40 (supramarginal gyrus), areas 22 and

  • BrainL.J. Buxbaum et al. / Cognitive37 (posterior superior temporal lobe), and the basal ganglia,

    but not the prefrontal cortex. They are also consistent with

    data from Ferro, Martins, Mariano, and Caldas [18],

    indicating that parietal lobe involvement was frequent in

    post-acute (>3 months post-stroke) and chronic patients

    with gesture recognition impairments. Together with the

    present data, this suggests that in humans, Brocas area may

    play a smaller role than the inferior parietal lobe in

    recognizing complex familiar actions and hand postures.

    6. General discussion

    The hypothesis that IM reflects a deficit in the

    representations underlying skilled object-related gestures,

    with particular degradation of the hand posture component

    of skilled object-related gestures, enabled us to generate a

    number of predictions that were tested in the present study.

    We demonstrated that patients with IM were disproportion-

    ately impaired in the imitation of transitive as compared to

    intransitive gestures, and were particularly impaired in the

    Fig. 5. Subtracted lesioned regions in high versus low performing groups on the

    Spatial Recognition task (bottom left and right). See Fig. 1 for additional explanaResearch 25 (2005) 226239 235hand posture component of transitive gesture imitation. This

    pattern persisted even when we examined only the transitive

    gestures having the simplest hand postures. We also

    demonstrated a strong association between performance of

    transitive imitation and recognition tasks (compared to a

    much weaker association between transitive recognition and

    intransitive gesture imitation), as well as a particular

    association between imitation and recognition of transitive

    hand postures. Importantly, the deficits in object-related

    hand posture recognition were not associated with aphasia

    severity. Finally, we showed that deficiencies in the spatial

    aspects of object-related gesture recognition and hand

    posture recognition were associated with lesions to the left

    inferior parietal lobe and intraparietal sulcus.

    These data suggest that the same representations sub-

    serve the imitation and recognition of complex, skilled,

    object-related movements, and thus provide support for the

    likelihood that the direct matching hypothesis applies to

    these complex, meaningful movements as well as to the

    simple grasping and finger movements previously examined

    in monkey and fMRI studies. In addition, the present data

    Spatial Recognition task (top left and right) and Hand Posture score of the

    tion.

  • Brainsuggest that the representations for skilled object-related

    arm and hand actions may differ in important ways from the

    representations subserving symbolic, non-object-related

    gesture. In the following section, we discuss possible

    reasons for this disparity.

    One possible basis for the distinction between transitive

    and intransitive gesture representations is that the former is

    more closely related to an evolutionarily pre-existing system

    specialized for manual grasping of objects. Fagg and Arbib

    [17] take into account previous findings by Sakata and

    colleagues (e.g., [52]) and Rizzolatti et al. (e.g., [50]) to

    develop a model of the interactions between the anterior

    intraparietal sulcus (AIP) and prefrontal F5 in programming

    grasping movements. They suggested that AIP represents

    the grasps afforded by objects, whereas F5 selects and

    drives grasp execution. Thus, F5 aids in selecting from

    among the multiple affordances that may be present in an

    object based on the relevance of these affordances given

    task constraints. AIP is hypothesized to provide an active

    memory of the affordance selected by F5, and to

    participate in updating this memory to reflect the grasp that

    is actually executed. This knowledge can then be used to

    inform subsequent interactions with objects (see also

    [41,51,63,64]). A related conceptualization is offered by

    Schettino, Adamovich, and Poizner [53], who suggest that

    hand preshaping in early phases of reaching movements

    reflects selection of a family of grasps based on the basic

    geometry of the target object as well as developmental

    experiences with handobject interactions. They contrast

    this with adjustments occurring later in the reach that

    modulate the basic grasp based on current information about

    the object and hand.

    Oztop and Arbib [41] develop this area of work further to

    address the relationship between action execution and action

    recognition. They offer a model that incorporates another

    subclass of neurons in F5, called canonical neurons [39],

    that discharge when a suitably graspable object is viewed. In

    brief, the model illustrates how the mirror neurons may have

    evolved to augment canonical neurons by providing visual

    feedback on hand-state, i.e., the relationship of the shape

    of the hand to the shape of an object. The hand-state

    representation forms the basis for the ability to generalize

    from ones hand to anothers hand, which in turn is

    hypothesized to undergird the understanding of others

    actions.

    A number of investigators, then, hypothesize that the brain

    computes and stores representations of hand posture based on

    previous experiences with prehensile handobject interac-

    tions. These representations of hand posture are postulated to

    inform (1) subsequent interactions with objects, and (2)

    recognition of object-related prehensile actions, and (3)

    arguably, to be present in both monkeys and humans. How

    does the mirror system that stores memories of prehensile

    actions differ from the skilled transitive gesture system of

    L.J. Buxbaum et al. / Cognitive236humans? The extensive range of complex, skilled postures

    and movements exhibited by humans (consider typing,fingering a stringed instrument, grasping and re-grasping a

    screwdriver as it is turned) requires knowledge regarding

    functional uses of objects, the functional portions of the

    object that are to be grasped, and the specific positions of the

    fingers, hand, and arm for particular objects. In some

    instances, the functional hand posture may be at odds with,

    or an elaboration of, the prehensile hand posture called for by

    object structure. The fact that the object-specific representa-

    tions can be evoked even in pantomime tasks, without an

    object present, suggests that they differ from the representa-

    tions encoded by mirror neurons.

    In humans, such an indirect route is likely to have

    evolved to map between stored representations of objects in

    the ventral stream, and representations of the body and

    objects in space mediated by the dorsal stream. Even in the

    monkey, the IPL is richly interconnected with both dorsal

    and ventral stream structures. Regions within monkey IPL

    project to distinct subdivisions of the dorsolateral frontal

    cortex [10]. Reciprocal connections also exist between the

    parietal lobe (area LIP) and the inferior temporal cortex

    (areas TE and TEO), an area known to be involved in both

    humans and monkeys in object recognition [13,62]. But

    when overall brain volume is controlled, human IPL is

    significantly larger than that of the rhesus monkey or

    chimpanzee [16]. It has been suggested that human left IPL

    has undergone evolutionary expansion paralleling the

    development of language [16].

    We suggest that left IPL/IPS system in humans mediates

    between representations of object identity in the ventral

    pathway and object/action structure in the dorsal pathway.

    By conveying information about object identity to the dorsal

    stream, we propose that the left IPL allows movements to be

    selected that are appropriate to an objects category

    membership, rather than only to its structural attributes.

    Furthermore, we conjecture that the representations are

    shaped through learning to map between identity and

    structure information, taking the form of abstract movement

    and posture representations that capture only those aspects

    of the target action that are invariant with respect to the

    initial body posture and the shape and location of any target

    object. We can speculate that these transitive skilled action

    representations are uniquely human, and are an elaboration

    of the representations of handobject interactions encoded

    by mirror neurons, both in terms of their complexity and

    their requirements for long-term memory capacity. Their

    abstract nature i.e., their independence from the details of

    current constraints of objects and the environment renders

    them potentially useful for recognizing transitive actions of

    others even when the actions are pantomimes performed

    without objects.

    Unlike object-related actions, intransitive, symbolic

    actions are not likely to retain such close ties to evolutio-

    narily more primitive systems for object grasping, and are

    not likely to be as strongly lateralized to the left hemisphere.

    Research 25 (2005) 226239Rapcsak et al. [48] reported a right-handed man who,

    consequent to a stroke resulting in virtually complete

  • same representations underlie perception and action for

    abstracted (pantomimed) versions of complex functional

    ment of Health. The Department of Health specifically

    disclaims responsibility for any analyses, interpretations, or

    Brainconclusions. We are grateful to Branch Coslett for perform-

    ing lesion analyses and Michael Arbib for his thoughtful

    comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript.

    Appendix A. Details of praxis screening tasks and

    scoring

    A.1. Meaningless movement imitation

    Participants imitated 10 meaningless movements that

    were spatial and temporal analogues of transitive gestures in

    terms of plane of gesture (horizontal or vertical) and the

    joints around which the movement occurred. The hand

    posture was also modified to be unlike the meaningful

    gestures (see Buxbaum, Giovannetti, and Libon [6] for

    details). For example, the meaningless movement analogous

    to hammering was a vertical movement performed by the

    side of the body, with movements of the elbow and shoulder

    joints, and the hand in a claw shape. Participants were

    permitted to begin imitation (with the unimpaired left hand)

    while still observing the target gesture. Performance was

    videotaped and later scored by an experienced coder inactions, just as for prehensile interactions with physical

    objects, suggests that the mirror property may be a basic

    organizing principle of the brain.

    Acknowledgments

    Supported by NIH RO1-NS36387 and NIDRR H133G0

    30169 to the first author, and by the Pennsylvania Depart-destruction of the left hemisphere, was severely impaired in

    transitive pantomime but relatively unimpaired with intran-

    sitive actions. Consistent with these data, left and right stroke

    patients demonstrate equivalent impairments in intransitive

    gesture pantomime and imitation [25]. Finally, as noted,

    normal subjects perform more accurately in discriminating

    intransitive gestures than transitive gestures, suggesting that

    the former representations may be more widely distributed

    and/or more readily activated than the latter [38].

    In summary, we have presented evidence that the same

    representations, mediated by the left inferior parietal lobe

    and intraparietal sulcus, are likely to be evoked in pro-

    duction and recognition of pantomimed object-related

    actions. The skilled gesture representations are active even

    without the physical presence of objects, and unlike mirror

    neurons, they appear to encode body and hand postures that

    are specific to the functional use of particular objects.

    Despite their differences, however, the evidence that the

    L.J. Buxbaum et al. / Cognitiveaccordance with guidelines published in Buxbaum, Giova-

    netti, and Libon [6] with respect to the components handposture, arm posture, amplitude, and timing, for a maximum

    score per gesture of 4 points. Normative data for the

    meaningless imitation test are presented in Buxbaum,

    Johnson-Frey, and Bartlett-Williams [8].

    A.2. Gesture to sight of objects

    Participants were instructed to show how they would

    hold and use a common object (e.g., hammer, scissors,

    screwdriver, saw, toothbrush) displayed on the tabletop,

    pretending they had it in their hand. They were not

    permitted to touch the object. The first instance of body

    part as object error (e.g., for toothbrushing, forefinger

    extended and moved over teeth) was corrected with a

    repetition of the task instructions and an additional

    instruction to show me how you would hold the object

    as if it were in your hand. The second instance of such

    errors was not corrected. There were 10 trials.

    A.3. Error scoring

    Both the apraxia screening tests and the gesture imitation

    tests were scored according to the taxonomy reported in

    Buxbaum, Giovannetti, and Libon [6] and reproduced

    below.

    1. Hand Posture

    Score as 0 if hand posture/grasp is unrecognizable,

    flagrantly incorrect, or only transiently correct (small

    fragment of total gesture with correct posture or

    grasp). Score 0 for Fbody part as object_ (BPO)errors.

    Score as 1 if posture is correct or subtly incorrect

    (e.g., hand aperture slightly too big or small; wrist

    angle slightly incorrect).

    2. Arm Posture/Trajectory

    Score as 0 if arm posture and/or trajectory (e.g.,

    joint angles, plane of movement relative to body/

    environment (e.g., side to side instead of back and

    forth), shape of movement (e.g., circular instead of

    linear) are flagrantly incorrect or only transiently

    correct (small fragment of total gesture with correct

    posture).

    Score as 1 if both arm posture and trajectory are

    correct; or if arm posture and/or trajectory are subtly

    incorrect (e.g., elbow slightly too bent, trajectory at

    slight angle relative to what is appropriate, shape of

    movement slightly distorted).

    3. Amplitude

    Score as 0 if size of movement is clearly too large or

    too small (e.g., sawing with small scratching

    movement), or if size is only transiently correct (small

    fragment of total gesture with correct amplitude).

    Score as 1 if size is correct or subtly too large or too

    Research 25 (2005) 226239 237small (e.g., slight Fovershoot_ or Fundershoot_ inmovement amplitude).

  • Brain4. Timing/Frequency

    Score as 0 if speed of movement is flagrantly too

    fast or slow; and/or if number of cycles of movement

    is flagrantly too few or many (e.g., Fflipping_ coin 4times in succession; Fscissoring_ only once).

    Score as 1 if speed of movement is subtly too fast or

    slow; and/or if frequency is subtly inappropriate (e.g.,

    flipping coin twice; scissoring only twice).

    References

    [1] C. Barbieri, E. DeRenzi, The executive and ideational components of

    apraxia, Cortex 24 (4) (1988) 535543.

    [2] B. Bell, Pantomime recognition impairment in aphasia: an analysis of

    error types, Brain Lang. 47 (1994) 269278.

    [3] G. Buccino, F. Binkofski, G.R. Fink, L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, V. Gallese,

    R.J. Seitz, K. Zilles, G. Rizzolatti, H.J. Freund, Action observation

    activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an

    fMRI study, Eur. J. Neurosci. 13 (2001) 400404.

    [4] G. Butterworth, On reconceptualizing sensori-motor coordination in

    dynamic system terms, in: H.B.B.I. Bertenthal (Ed.), Sensory Motor

    Organization and Development in Infancy and Early Childhood,

    Kluwer Academic Press, The Netherlands, 1990, pp. 5773.

    [5] L.J. Buxbaum, Ideomotor apraxia: a call to action, Neurocase 7 (2001)

    445458.

    [6] L.J. Buxbaum, T. Giovannetti, D. Libon, The role of the dynamic body

    schema in praxis: evidence from primary progressive apraxia, Brain

    Cogn. 44 (2000) 166191.

    [7] L.J. Buxbaum, A. Sirigu, M.F. Schwartz, R.L. Klatzky, Cognitive

    representations of hand posture in ideomotor apraxia, Neuropsycho-

    logia 41 (2003) 10911113.

    [8] L.J. Buxbaum, S.H. Johnson-Frey, M. Bartlett-Williams, Deficient

    internal models for planning handobject interactions in apraxia,

    Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 917929.

    [9] L.J. Buxbaum, K. Kyle, M. Grossman, H.B. Coslett, Left inferior

    parietal representations for skilled handobject interactions: evidence

    from stroke and corticobasal degeneration, Cortex (in press).

    [10] C. Cavada, The visual parietal areas in the macaque monkey: current

    structural knowledge and ignorance, NeuroImage 14 (2001) S21S26.

    [11] H. Damasio, A.R. Damasio, Lesion Analysis in Neuropsychology,

    Oxford Univ. Press, New York, NY, 1989.

    [12] E. DeRenzi, Methods of limb apraxia examination and their bearing

    on the interpretation of the disorder, in: E.A. Roy (Ed.), Advances in

    Psychology: Neuropsychological Studies of Apraxia and Related

    Disorders, vol. 23, North Holland, Amsterdam, 1985.

    [13] C. Distler, D. Boussaoud, R. Desimone, L.G. Ungerleider, Corical

    connections of inferior temporal area TEO in macaque monkeys,

    J. Comp. Neurol. 334 (1993) 125150.

    [14] J.R. Duffy, R.J. Duffy, Three studies of deficits in pantomimic

    expression and pantomimic recognition in aphasia, J. Speech Hear.

    Res. (1981) 7084.

    [15] C. Dumont, B. Ska, Pantomime recognition impairment in Alzheimers

    disease, Brain Cogn. 43 (2000) 177181.

    [16] D. Eidelburg, A.M. Galaburda, Inferior parietal lobule: divergent

    architectonic asymmetries in the human brain, Arch. Neurol. 41

    (1984) 843852.

    [17] A. Fagg, M.A. Arbib, Modeling parietalpremotor interactions in

    primate control of grasping, Neural Netw. 11 (1998) 12771303.

    [18] J.M. Ferro, I.P. Martins, G. Mariano, A.C. Caldas, CT scan correlates

    of gesture recognition, J. Neurol., Neurosurg. Psychiatry 46 (1983)

    943952.

    L.J. Buxbaum et al. / Cognitive238[19] G. Gainotti, M. Lemmo, Comprehension of symbolic gestures in

    aphasia, Brain Lang. 3 (1976).[20] V. Gallese, L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, G. Rizzolatti, Action recognition in

    the premotor cortex, Brain 119 (1996) 593609.

    [21] L.J. Gonzalez Rothi, C. Ochipa, K.M. Heilman, A cognitive neuro-

    psychological model of limb praxis, Cogn. Neuropsychol. 8 (1991)

    443458.

    [22] S. Grafton, M.A. Arbib, L. Fadiga, G. Rizzolatti, Localization of grasp

    representations in humans by positron emission tomography: 2.

    Observation compared with imagination, Exp. Brain Res. 112 (1996)

    103111.

    [23] K.Y. Haaland, D.L. Harrington, R.T. Knight, Neural representations of

    skilled movement, Brain 123 (2000) 23062313.

    [24] U. Halsband, J. Schmitt, M. Weyers, F. Binkofski, G. Grutzner, H.J.

    Freund, Recognition and imitation of pantomimed motor acts after

    unilateral parietal and premotor lesions: a perspective on apraxia,

    Neuropsychologia 39 (2001) 200216.

    [25] M. Heath, E.A. Roy, S.E. Black, D.A. Westwood, Intransitive limb

    gestures and apraxia following unilateral stroke, J. Clin. Exp. Neuro-

    psychol. 23 (2001) 628642.

    [26] K.M. Heilman, L.J. Gonzalez Rothi, Apraxia, in: K.M. Heilman, E.

    Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical Neuropsychology, Oxford Univ. Press,

    New York, 1993, pp. 141150.

    [27] K.M. Heilman, L.J. Rothi, E. Valenstein, Two forms of ideomotor

    apraxia, Neurology 32 (1982) 342346.

    [28] M. Iacoboni, R.P. Woods, M. Brass, H. Bekkering, J.C. Mazziotta, G.

    Rizzolatti, Cortical mechanisms of human imitation, Science 286

    (1999) 25262528.

    [29] M. Jeannerod, J. Decety, F. Michel, Impairment of grasping move-

    ments following a bilateral posterior parietal lesion, Neuropsychologia

    32 (1994) 369380.

    [30] S.H. Johnson-Frey, F.R. Maloof, R. Newman-Norland, C. Farrer,

    Actions or handobject interactions? Human inferior frontal cortex

    and action observation, Neuron 39 (2003) 10531058.

    [31] D. Kareken, F. Unverzagt, K. Caldemeyer, M. Farlow, G.

    Hutchins, Functional brain imaging in apraxia, Arch. Neurol. 55

    (1998) 107113.

    [32] R. Klatzky, J. Pellegrino, B. McCloskey, S. Lederman, Cognitive

    representations of functional interactions with objects, Mem. Cogn. 21

    (1993) 294303.

    [33] K. Laimgruber, G. Goldenberg, J. Hermsdorfer, Manual and hemi-

    spheric asymmetries in the execution of actual and pantomimed

    prehension, Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 682692.

    [34] H. Liepmann, The Left Hemisphere and Action, University of Western

    Ontario, London, 1905.

    [35] F. Lucchelli, O.L. Lopez, P. Faglioni, F. Boller, Ideomotor and

    ideational apraxia in Alzheimers disease, Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 8

    (1993) 413417.

    [36] J. Mai, J. Assheuer, Atlas of the Human Brain, 2nd edR, Academic

    Press, 2003.

    [37] A.N. Meltzoff, M.K. Moore, Imitation of facial and manual gestures

    by human neonates, Science 198 (1977) 7578.

    [38] M. Mozaz, L. Rothi, J. Anderson, G.P. Crucian, K. Heilman, Postural

    knowledge of transitive pantomimes and intransitive gestures, J. Int.

    Neuropsychol. Soc. 8 (2002) 958962.

    [39] A. Murata, L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, V. Gallese, V. Raos, G. Rizzolatti,

    Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the

    monkey, J. Neurophysiol. 78 (1997) 22262230.

    [40] N. Nishitani, R. Hari, Temporal dynamics of cortical representation for

    action, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 97 (2000) 913918.

    [41] E. Oztop, M.A. Arbib, Schema design and implementation of

    the grasp-related mirror neuron system, Biol. Cybern. 87 (2002)

    116140.

    [42] B. Pelgrims, M. Andres, X. Seron, J.-R. Duhamel, A. Sirigu, E.

    Olivier, Role of the Left Supramarginalis Gyrus in Coding Hand

    Gestures: Implication for Ideomotor Apraxia, Cognitive Neuroscience

    Society, New York, 2005.

    Research 25 (2005) 226239[43] M.T. Perenin, A. Vighetto, Optic ataxia: a specific disruption in

    visuomotor mechanisms, Brain 111 (1988) 643674.

  • [44] H. Poizner, L. Mack, M. Verfaellie, L.J.G. Rothi, K.M.

    Heilman, Three-dimensional computergraphic analysis of apraxia:

    neural representations of learned movement, Brain 113 (1990)

    85101.

    [45] H. Poizner, M. Clark, A. Merians, B. Macauley, L. Gonzalez Rothi, K.

    Heilman, Joint coordination deficits in limb apraxia, Brain 118 (1995)

    227242.

    [46] H. Poizner, A.S. Merians, M.A. Clark, B. Macauley, L.J. Rothi, K.M.

    Heilman, Left hemispheric specialization for learned skilled, and

    purposeful action, Neuropsychology 12 (1998) 163182.

    [47] W. Prinz, Perception and action planning, Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 9

    (1997) 129154.

    [48] S.Z. Rapcsak, C. Ochipa, P.M. Beeson, A.B. Rubens, Praxis and the

    right hemisphere, Brain Cogn. 23 (1993) 181202.

    [49] G. Rizzolatti, G. Luppino, The cortical motor system, Neuron 31

    (2001) 889901.

    [50] G. Rizzolatti, L. Fadiga, M. Matelli, V. Bettinardi, E. Paulesu, D.

    Perani, F. Fazio, Localization of grasp representations in human by

    PET: 1. Observation vs. execution, Exp. Brain Res. 111 (1996)

    246252.

    [51] D. Rosenbaum, S. Engelbrecht, M. Bushe, L. Loukopoulos, Knowl-

    edge model for selecting and producing reaching movements, J. Mot.

    Behav. 25 (1993) 217227.

    [52] H. Sakata, M. Taira, A. Murata, S. Mine, Neural mechanisms of visual

    guidance of hand action in the parietal cortex of the monkey, Cereb.

    Cortex 5 (1995) 429438.

    [53] L. Schettino, S. Adamovich, H. Poizner, Effects of object shape and

    visual feedback on hand configuration during grasping, Exp. Brain

    Res. 151 (2003) 158166.

    [54] J. Schwoebel, L. Buxbaum, H.B. Coslett, Representations of the

    human body in the production and imitation of complex movements,

    Cogn. Neuropsychol. 21 (2004) 285299.

    [55] A. Sirigu, L. Cohen, J.R. Duhamel, B. Pillon, B. Dubois, Y. Agid, A

    selective impairment of hand posture for object utilization in apraxia,

    Cortex 31 (1995) 4155.

    [56] A. Sunderland, S.M. Sluman, Ideomotor apraxia, visuomotor control

    and the explicit representation of posture, Neuropsychologia 38 (2000)

    923934.

    [57] A. Tessari, R.I. Rumiati, The strategic control of multiple routes in

    imitation of actions, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 30 (6)

    (2004) 11071116.

    [58] A. Travinicek-Marterer, W. Danielczyk, M. Simanyi, P. Fischer,

    Ideomotor apraxia in Alzheimers disease, Acta Neurol. Scand. 88

    (1993) 14.

    [59] N.R. Varney, A.L. Benton, Qualitative aspects of pantomime recog-

    nition defect in aphasia, Brain Cogn. 1 (1982) 132139.

    [60] N.R. Varney, H. Damasio, Locus of lesion in impaired pantomime

    recognition, Cortex 23 (1987) 699703.

    [61] L. Wang, H. Goodglass, Pantomime, praxis, and aphasia, Brain Lang.

    42 (1992) 402418.

    [62] M. Webster, J. Bachevalier, L.G. Ungerleider, Connection of inferior

    temporal areas TEO and TE with parietal and frontal cortex in

    macaque monkeys, Cereb. Cortex 4 (1994) 470483.

    [63] A. Witney, P. Vetter, D.M. Wolpert, The influence of previous

    experience on predictive motor control, NeuroReport 12 (2001)

    649653.

    [64] D.M. Wolpert, Z. Ghahramani, J. Flanagan, Perspectives and problems

    in motor learning, Trends Cogn. Sci. 5 (2001) 487494.

    L.J. Buxbaum et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2005) 226239 239

    On beyond mirror neurons: Internal representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humansIntroductionSubjectsLanguage comprehensionLesion analysisExperimental tasksStudy 1: imitation of transitive and intransitive gestureMethodsResultsDiscussion

    Study 2: recognition of transitive gestureMethodsResultsDiscussion

    Additional analyses of the relationship of production and spatial gesture recognitionDiscussion

    NeuroanatomyMethodsDiscussion

    General discussionAcknowledgmentsDetails of praxis screening tasks and scoringMeaningless movement imitationGesture to sight of objectsError scoring

    References