18
Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt University Ph.D. Final Oral Defense March 12, 2003

Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics,

and Children Who Are Late to Talk

Celeste Domsch

Department of Hearing

& Speech Sciences

Vanderbilt University

Ph.D. Final Oral Defense

March 12, 2003

Page 2: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Purpose

To examine

parental education and language

in relationship to

language development

in children

who are late to talk

Page 3: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Identifying Children Who Are Late to Talk(CWLT)

• Normal hearing• Normal nonverbal intelligence• Do not have autism or other neurological

disorders• Are not bilingual• Score in bottom 10th percentile for expressive

vocabulary

Page 4: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Hypotheses tested:

1. That parental educational level was positively associated with parental language measures;

2. That parental educational level was positively associated with child language measures;

3. That parental language use was positively associated with child language development

Page 5: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Method

• 20 participants (20 CWLT and their parents)– CWLT

• 16 male (two of 16 were twins), 4 female• Mean age = 29.9 months, SD = 4.1• Mean nonverbal IQ = 103.6, SD = 9.3• Mean vocabulary size = 70.3 words, SD = 52.8• 11 CWLT in speech/language treatment

Page 6: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Method

• Parents (19 sets; one family had twins)– Mean maternal education = 14.42 years, SD = 2.36– Range = 12 to 19 years– Mean paternal education = 14.32 years, SD = 2.47– Range = 12 to 19 years– 15 families intact, 3 divorced and remarried, 1 never

married– Mother was primary caretaker for 18 CWLT; father

was primary caretaker for remaining 2 CWLT

Page 7: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Method

• Procedures– Each family received 5-7 home visits over

8-months• Parent completed vocabulary checklist

• Experimenter collected language sample– Parent playing with CWLT for 15 min.

– Videotaped

• Experimenter tested receptive and expressive language on final visit

Page 8: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Method

• Main Dependent Measures– Parents

• Years of education from questionnaire• Mean length of utterance (MLU) from language sample • Total number of words (TNW) from language sample• Number of different words (NDW) from language sample

– CWLT• Vocabulary size in words from parent checklist• MLU from language sample• TNW from language sample• NDW from language sample• Receptive vocabulary test score• Expressive vocabulary test score

• Data Analyses– Pearson product-moment correlations– Hierarchical Linear Modeling

Page 9: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Results

• Testing H1: that parents with more education talked more to their CWLT

• Are parental educational levels positively associated with parent MLU, TNW, or NDW?

• Parental education not correlated with parent MLU, TNW or NDW; however, parent MLU, TNW and NDW all correlated with one another

(p < .05)

Page 10: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Results

• Testing H2: that more educated parents had CWLT who were more verbal

• Is parental educational level positively associated with child MLU, TNW, or NDW?

• Neither child TNW nor NDW was correlated with parental educational level; however, child MLU was correlated with parental educational level (r = .445, p < .05)

Page 11: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Results

• Testing H2: that more educated parents had CWLT who were more verbal

• Is parental educational level positively associated with child language test scores?

• Parental educational level was not correlated with either child receptive or expressive vocabulary scores

• Interestingly, 9 CWLT scored in the normal range for both receptive and expressive vocabulary 8-months after intake; 5 remained delayed in both

Page 12: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Results

• Testing H3: that parents who talked more had CWLT who were more verbal themselves

• Are parental language measures (e.g., MLU, TNW, NDW) positively associated with child language test scores?

• Neither Parent MLU nor TNW was correlated with Child receptive or expressive scores

• Parent NDW was correlated with receptive language (r = .55, p < .05), but not with expressive language

Page 13: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Hierarchical Linear Modeling (HLM)

• Used to analyze changes in children’s language over time (sampled 5-7 times over 8-months)

• Level-1 model tested whether individual CWLT differed from one another in their initial status and rate of growth for various language measures

• Level-2 model tested whether selected parent factors were significant predictors of growth

Page 14: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

HLM Questions

• Is parental educational level positively associated with child vocabulary growth?

• Is initial parental MLU positively associated with growth in child MLU?

• Is initial parental TNW positively associated with growth in child TNW?

• Is initial parental NDW positively associated with growth in child NDW?

Page 15: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

HLM Results

• CWLT varied significantly in their initial status and rate of growth for all measures (vocabulary size, MLU, TNW, NDW)

• But selected parent factors (parental education, MLU, TNW, NDW respectively) did not predict growth

• That is, parents with more education, or who used more total words, a greater variety of words, or longer sentences, did not have late talkers who showed faster language growth

Page 16: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Discussion• Parental education was positively correlated with

child MLU

• Parental NDW was positively correlated with child receptive language test scores

• CWLT varied significantly in initial status and rate of growth for vocabulary size, MLU, TNW, and NDW

• But these variations were not apparently related to parental education or parental MLU, TNW, or NDW respectively

Page 17: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Conclusions

• CWLT appear to pay attention to, and benefit from, hearing a variety of words, even if they do not immediately produce them

• Parental educational level was not a reliable predictor of rich language input to CWLT

• Parental educational level also had no apparent effect on vocabulary growth or measures of child language (except MLU)

• Thus, lower levels of parental education did not constitute an additional risk factor for slowed language development in CWLT

Page 18: Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt

Future Directions

• Include an even more diverse group of parents (i.e., non-high-school graduates)

• Compare results from optimal vs. typical language sampling techniques

• Group CWLT by severity of expressive delay (bottom 5th percentile vs. bottom 10th)