165
God Setting in the Acquisition of a Motor Skill with Children who Have Learning Disabilities Nicole A. Savoie A niesis Submitted to the Fzulty of Graduate Studies In Partial Fulfillment of The Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts (Educaiion) Deparûnent of Physical Educaiion Division of Graduate Studies and Research Fwulty of Educaiion McGi 11 University Montreal, Quebec, Canada

Canada · Questionnaire Conceming experimed (D O-your-b est) Appendix D- 1 Questionnaire Concerning Experiment @a-your-best) (French Version). 146 Appendix E Original Cue Cards for

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • God Setting in the Acquisition

    of a Motor Skill with Children who Have Learning Disabilities

    Nicole A. Savoie

    A niesis Submitted to the Fzulty of Graduate Studies In Partial Fulfillment of The Requirements for the Degree o f Master of Arts (Educaiion)

    Deparûnent of Physical Educaiion

    Division of Graduate Studies and Research Fwulty of Educaiion McGi 11 University

    Montreal, Quebec, Canada

  • National Library u*1 of Canada Bibliothèque nationale du Canada Acquisitions and Acquisitions et Bibliographic Services services bibliographiques

    395 Wellington Street 395. nie Wellington Ottawa ON K IA O N 4 Ottawa ON K1A ON4 Canada Canada

    The author has granted a non- exclusive licence dowing the National Library of Canada to reproduce, loan, distribute or sell copies of this thesis in microform, paper or electronic formats.

    The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantial extracts £kom it may be printed or otherwise reproduced without the author's permission.

    Your fi& Votre reforence

    Our iYe Norre 18:drence

    L'auteur a accordé une licence non exclusive permettant a la Bibliothèque nationale du Canada de reproduire, prêter, distribuer ou vendre des copies de cette thèse sous la forme de microfiche/f%q de reproduction sur papier ou sur format électronique.

    L'auteur conserve la propriété du droit d'auteur qui protège cette thèse. Ni la thèse ni des extraits substantiels de celle-ci ne doivent être imprimés - ou autrement reproduits sans son autorisation.

  • ii

    ABSTRACT

    According to h c k e ;nidL&am (1990), goal-setting is a powerfùl motivational

    tool which captivates the individual's attention and sense of effort in pooling d l

    a d a b l e raourca to accomplish a task with accrucd determination. Goal-setting has

    been used successfùlly in sports and physical activities (Kyllo & Landers, 1995).

    Positive results have also beai realized with low achievers in academic tasks (Bandura

    & Schunk, 1981).

    The purpose of this investigation was to test the effects of goal-setting on

    basketball fiee throwing with nomally achieving (NA) boys and girls, and with

    children having leamiog disabilities (LD), ages 9- 13 years. This shidy aiso tested the

    relationship between Perceived Physica! SeKCornpetence (Harter, 1978b) and

    performance.

    Thirty-fhree diildren with LD and 39 NA children practiced during 10 meen-

    minute sessions, over a pariod of four weeks. Prior to the start of the experiment,

    subjects were randomly assigned to a goal condition, either goal-setting or do - your-

    best (control). Subsequently, they were tested and grouped by ski11 level, high or

    low, accordingto their object control scores on the Test of Gross Motor Developrnent

    (üirich, 1985). A baseline score for the basketball f k e throw was achieved in three

    sep- îriafs of IO shots atthe basket The average score was considered the baseline

    level. A pre- and post-qualitative assessrnent of their bal1 throwing technique

  • iii

    according to ten criteria (indicated in Chapter 3) was dso done. The Perceived

    Physical Self-Competence Scale was administered prior to and at the end of the

    training sessions. The do-your-best groups were surveyed at the end o f the training

    sessions to find out whether they were setting gods or not.

    Results indicated that the children with LD in the goal-setting group

    outp erformed the control group. However, goal-setting failed to dserentiate the

    performances o f NA children No correlation was established between Perceived

    Pfysical SeK-co~lfpetence and the pdormance of these children. Methodological key

    points arc dis& and suggestions are given for fuiwe research with goal-setîing and

    children.

  • iv

    RésumC

    Selon Locke et Laîham (1990), l'utilisation d'objectifs précis et spécifiques

    améliore laperf~nnance en saturant l Y ~ e n t i o n de l'individu et son sens de l'effort afin

    de recruter toutes les ressources disponibles pour réaliser une tâche mec une plus

    graide efficacité. L'utilisation d'objectifs s'est prouvée efficace dans les sports et les

    activités physiques (Kyllo & Landers, 1995). Du côté des enfants en difficulté

    d'apprentissage, l'utilisation d'objectifs a eu des résultats positifk dans le secteur

    académique pandura & Schunk, 1981).

    Le but de cette recherche était de déterminer les effets de L'utilisation des

    objectif5 dans laperforniance du lancer au panier avec des garçons et filles, âgés entre

    9 et 13 ans, du secteiir régulier(SR) et du secteur en difficultés d'apprentissage (DA).

    Cetk étude a m i évalué la relation entre les perceptions d' auto-compétence physique

    (Perceived Physical Self-Competence (Harter 1978b)) et la performance pour

    identifier s'il y avait une relation linéaire entre les deux facteurs.

    Trente-trois élèves en difficulté d'apprentissage (DA) et 39 élèves SR

    participèrent à 10 séances de 15 minutes chacune, de pratique en lancer au panier,

    pour une durée de 4 semaines. Avant le début de cet entraînement, les sujets furent

    assig~és rnilrasmddrais le groupe avec objectifs ou le groupe T a i s de ton mieux". On

    procéda à une évaluation de leurs habiletés motrices concernant la manipulation

    d'objets par le test de Ulrich (19851, le Test of Gross Motor Development. Cela

  • v

    ~ à c l a s s e r les élèves dans un niveau d'habileté élevé ou fkble. Une évaluation

    de base sur les lancers au panier se fit par trois tests séparés de dix lancers. La

    moyenne des trois tests kt retenue comme éaluation de base. Chaque sujet a

    égaiement subi une pré- et pst-évaluation qualitative du lancer au panier selon dix

    &&es spécaiés au chapitre 3. De plus, chaque sujet a complété le test de Perception

    de l'arao-coaipétence physique (Perceived Physical Self-Cornpetence Scaie), avant et

    après l'entraînement. Les sujets dans les groupes 'Tais de ton mieux" eurent à

    compléter un questionnaire leur demandant s'ils avaient utilisé des objectifs lors des

    évaluations apres chaque séance d'entraînement..

    Les résultats de cette recherche indiquèrent que Les objectifs eurent un effet

    bénéfique avec les éIèves D A L'utilisation d'objectifs n'a pas eu d'effet significatif

    avec les élèves SR Soixante-dix-sept pourced des élèves du groupe "Fais de ton

    mieux" se fixaient des objectifs, ce qui a pu entraver I'observation de l'usage des

    objectif5 vasus T a i s de ton mieuf'. Aucune corrélation n'a pu être remarquée entre

    la perception d'auto-compétence physique et la performance.

  • Iwould like to thank Dr. Greg Reid for his constant encouragement, patience

    and expertise throughout my studies. Dr. Reid has been a precious and patient guide,

    who, in his o w n wisdom, succeeded in rnotivating me towards the cornpletion of this

    thesis.

    1 would like to express rny gratitude to my husband Pierre and my two

    dmghters, Sarah and Cynthia for having been so patient and supportive of my work.

    Without their understanding, these studies would have been extremely difficult if not

    impossible to complete.

    1 would also like to th& France Richer and Éric Brisebois, physical

    educaîion specialists, who allowed me to use their class tirne for training and testing

    o f the children with LD. A speciaf thank you to a11 the regular stream md special

    educaîion teachers and the principal, Mrs. Yvette Campeau, at Saint-Jean-Baptiste

    elementary school for their cooperaîion in this study.

  • vii

    Table of Contents

    Abstract Résumé Acknowledgments Table of Contents List of Appendices List of Tab les List of Figures

    Chapter

    1 NIXODUCTION 1.1 Statement of the Purpose 1.2 Hypotheses 1.3 Definitions 1.4 Delimitations 1.5 Limitation

    2 REVIEW OF THE LlTERATO-RE .. 10 2.1 Learning Disabilities ..Il

    2.1.1 ûverview of Definitions, Incidence and Etiology ..Il 2.1.2 Historical Perspective ..13 2.1.3 Educational Characteristics .. 16 2.1.4 Motor Characteristics ..19 2.1.5 Motivational Characteristics . .32

    2.2 Goal-Setting Theory ..38 2.2.1 Introduction ..38 2.2.2 Factors thaî Mediate Goal-Setting ..40 2.2.3 The Application of Goal-Setting in Sports and Exercise ..41 2.2.4 Cbildren and Goal-Setting . .47 2.2.5 Gd-Setting in the Academic Realm ..52

    2.3 Summary ..53

    3 METHODOLOGY 3.1 3ubjects 3.2 Assessment and Questionnaires

    3.2.1 Object Control Assessment

  • 3.2.2 Harter's Perceived Physical Self-Cornpetence Scale 3.2.3 Questionnaire Concerning Experiment

    Experimental Conditions 3.3.1 TheTask 3.3.2 Trials 3.3.3 Goal Conditions

    Procedures 3.4.1 Practice Sessions 3.4.2 Practice Scheduling 3.4.3 Apparatus

    Design and Analysis of Data

    4 RESULTS 4.1 Drop-outs 4.2 Results for Children with LD 4.3 Results for Nomally Achieving Children 4.4 Cornparison of Results Between Children with LD and

    NA Children 4.5 Leming Effects and Retention 4.6 Qualitative Assessrnent of Basketball Free Throw 4.7 Results Related to Perceived Physical Self-Competence 4.8 ResultsofUseofGoalsbyDo-Your-BestSubjects 4.9 Surnmary of Rewlts

    5 DISCUSSION 5.1 Analysis of Drop-outs 5.2 Learning Effects and Retention 5.3 Goal-Setting Effects 5.4 The Influence of Skill Level 5.5 Perceptions of Physical Self-Competence 5.6 Summary

    6 SmiMARY AND CONCLUSIONS 6.1 Summary of the Methodology 6.2 Summary of the Findings 6.3 Conclusions 6.4 hplications/Applications of this Research

    viii

    ..58 3 9

    ..60

    ..60

    . .63

    . .63

    . .65

    ..66

    ..07

    . .68

    . -68

    . .70

    ..71

    ..71

    . .74

    . .80

    .. 92

    ..92

    ..93

    .. 94

    . .95

    ..97

    ..97

    ..98

    ..99 .102 ,105 ,107

    ,108 .108 .111 .112 ,112

  • 6.5 Recommendations for Further Studies .113

    List of Appendices

    Appendix A - Znforrned Consent Form

    Appendix A-1 uifonned Consent Fonn (French Version)

    Appendix B Perceived Physical Self-Cornpetence Scale

    Appendix B-1 Perceived Pliysical Self-Competence Scaie (French Version) -142

    Appendix C Questiomake Conceming Experiment (Goal-setting)

    Appendix C-l Questionnaire Conceming Experiment (Goal-setting) (French Version) .144

    Appendix D Questionnaire Conceming experimed (D O-your-b est)

    Appendix D- 1 Questionnaire Concerning Experiment @a-your-best) (French Version). 146

    Appendix E Original Cue Cards for Free Throw ( El, E2, E3, E4 and ES) 147-15 1

    Appendix F Qualitative Assessment o f Basketbal1 Free lhrow

  • List of Tabies

    Table

    1.

    2.

    3.

    4.

    5 .

    6.

    7.

    8.

    9.

    10.

    Il.

    12.

    Distribution of Studmts According to Goal Condition

    Distribution of Students According to Skill Level

    Goal Incrernents for Setting Goals

    Distribution of Drop-out Shidents with LD

    ANOVA Table for Children with LD Across 12 Trials

    Descriptive Statistics for Children with LI3 Across 12 Trials

    ANOVA Table for Normally Achieving Children Across 12 Trials

    Descriptive Statistics for Nonnally Achieving Children

    Across 12 Triais

    Mean Scores for AI1 Children Across 4 Trials

    ANûVA Table for Combined Groups Across 4 Trials

    Mean Scores for Trials Across Al1 Subjects

    Perceived Physical Self-Competeoce Scores, Means and

    Standard Deviations

  • List of Figures

    Figure

    Pedomance According to SkiIl, Children with LD . .75

    Performance According to Treatment, C h i l h n with LD . .76

    Perfommce According to Trials, Children with LI3 . .77

    Performance According to Skill, Normally Achieving Children ..81

    Performance According to Trials, Nomally Achieving Children .. 82

    Performance According to Skill, Al1 Children . .85

    Performance of Al1 Chilken, According to Treatment,

    Across 4 Trials

    Comparative Performance Between LD and NA Children,

    According to Treatment

    Performance of Each Group with Goal-Setiing

    Performance of Each Group in the Do-Your-Best Condition

  • CHAPTER 1

    INTRODUCTION

    Motivating children with leaming disabilities (LD) has proven to be a

    diffiicult task for teachers. These children are characterized by academic

    achievement that is well below their learning potediai (Kavale & Reese, 1992).

    Similar to nomally achieving children, they have average and above average IQs

    (85 and over), no apparent physical trauma, normal vision and hearing. fIowever,

    experiencing acadernic fàîlure may lead to feelings of incompetence (Deci, Hodges,

    Pierson & Tomassone, 1992; Licht & Kistner, 1986). In hun, causal attributions

    for incompetence are ofien linked to leanied helpiessness (Canino, 1981; Dweck &

    Repucci, 1973).

    Children who are persistent at a task in the fàîe of failure take responsibiliîy

    for outcome and attribute failure to lack of effort. On the uther hand, chilctren who

    give up easily are disthguished, not by their ability level, but by their attribution of

    failure to factors beyond their control such as luck, task dficulty or perceptions of

    low abiliîy @weck, 1986).

    When lack of ability is believed to be the cause of failure, a sense of

    powerlessness in behavior outcornes may develop (Weiner, 1974). Hence, leamed

  • 2

    helplessness emerges because these beliefk towards failure are perceived as

    unalterable (Elliott & Pweck, 1988; M h , 1982). However, when attributions

    are linked to exiemal factors such as effort or task difficulty, change or possibility

    of hprovement are more readily conceivabIe @weck, 1986).

    Children with leaming disabilities tend to view thek successes or failures

    more gequently resulting nom good or bad luck (Pearl, 1982). They rarely regard

    failure as resulting fiom lack of effort. These children do not see the point in

    investing more eEort, or taking responsibility for whaî they are do ing because îhey

    feel that this will not influence or improve their academic standing. Learned

    helpless children exhibit much less persistence at a task and have dificulty finding

    alternative solutions to a problem when compared to mastery oriented children

    (Diener & h e c k , 1978). niey spend more time looking for the blame than

    searching for a solution (Diener & Dweck, 1978).

    Essentially, learned helplessness is a sense of powerlessness to control

    outcorne of events and affects motivation and persistence in any given task

    Aîîernpts at remediating learned helplessness have focussed on prornoting effort

    attributions and having children recognize the gains they are making in acquiring

    new knowledge and skills (Dweck, 1986). This has been shown to be more

    effective than providing these children with success only experiences (Dweck,

    1986). Research conceming children with learning disabilities has not clearly

  • 3

    demonstrated that they are Iearned helpl ess, but has demonstrated that they

    sipificantly minimize the importance of effort as a source of failure or success in

    the academic and non-academic situation (Canino, 1981). Hence this tendency

    rnay lead to learned helplessness.

    In the motor domain, learned helplessness has been observed by the

    avoidance of activity (Martinek & Griffith, 1994). Children with LD are at a

    greater risk of experiencing motor daculties than normal& achieving children

    (Shemill, 1993). As agroup, these children have motor developmental delays

    (Bruininks 62 Bruininks, 1977) and may exhibit inefficient niovement (Brunt &

    Distefàno, 1982; Brunt, Magill, & Eason, 1983; Bryan & Smiley, 1983; Gniber,

    1969; Haubenstricker, 1982; Kelly, 1990; Kerr & Hughes, 1987; Lazarus, 1990;

    Reid, 1982). Furthemare, physicaf awkwardness is more prevalent in this group

    (Taylor, 1982) than in the nonnally aFhieving population. However, some children

    with I D display motor proficiency levels quite similar to those of normally

    achieving children (Miyahara, 1994). Consequently, these children may not al1

    share patterns of learned helplessness in the motor domain.

    The process of goal-setting with children has been the subject of very few

    empirîcal studies in physical activity. Henderson, May and Umney (1989)

    investigated chilken with movement difficulties and nonnally achieving children

  • 4

    with regard to how they spontaneousiy set goals. They reported that children with

    rnovement difficulties set goals regardless of feedback They would set either lower

    than already achieved goals or unrealistically high goals. Furthemore, this goal-

    setting pattern was generalized across dEerent tasks (beanbag throw, maze pursuit

    and letter cancellation). This pattern of thinking has been related to leamed

    helplessness (Henderson et al., 1989). Failing to achieve umealistic goals does not

    threaten self-esteem and aiming for already achieved levels of knowledge or ability

    carries fimited risks of failm.

    Goal-setting is a self-regulating mechanism for action vandura, 1986;

    Locke 62 Latham, 1990). It is a powefil motivaiion tool which captivaies the

    individuai's attention and sense of effort in pooling dl available resources to

    accomplish a task with accmed determination (Locke 62 Latham, 1985). Locke,

    Shaw, Saari and Latham (1981) confirmed in the industrial seüing (99 of 110

    snidies) thaî specific, ditjcult goals, i f accepted, ~ s u l t in higher perfomaace than

    easy goais, vague goals or no goal. Locke and Latham (1990) also reviewed

    nurnerous dudies that demonstrated how long-termplus short-tenn goals were

    more effective than distal goals only.

    Short-tem (proximal) goal-setting has been found to be useful Ui enhancing

    persistence and motivation with low achieving sîudents (Bandura & Schunk, 1981)

    and leming disabled students (Kline et al., 1990; Sawyer et al., 1992; Schunk &

  • 5

    Swartz, 1991). Bandura and Schunk (1981) demonstrafed the effectiveness of

    goal-setting in improving mathematical performance with low achieving children.

    Proximal goal-sening fostered accurate assessrnent of progress and appropriate

    estimation of success on upcoming mathematical tasks. Sirnilarly, children with LD

    have diaculty appraising what they are capable of dohg (Canino, 1981) and

    therefore, the process of goal-sethg which involves self-evvatuation and self-

    prediction of progress might be a usefùl tooI for enhancing ieaming.

    The goal-setting constnict is iduenced by perceived self-efficacy and level

    of cornmitment to the achievement of the task at hand (Locke gt Latham, 1990).

    In this study, however, perceived physical self-cornpetence (Harter, 1982) has been

    used in the place of the evduation of seKeEcacy and refers to an equivaferrt

    concept for chi ldren.

    Goal-Setîina in the Sport Dornain

    In the sport domain, results in testing goal- setting assurnptions have not

    been as consistent as in the industrial setting. Significant efEects of god-setting

    were reported with children (Baniett & Stanicek, 1979; Burton, 1984, 1993;

    Erbaugh & Bmeü, 1986; Weinberg, Bniya, Longino &Jackson, 19881, with

    adolescents (Bar-Eli et al., 1994) and with adults (ErSmeyer, 1987; Hall & Byrne,

    1988; Hall et al., 1987; Tenenbaum, Pinchas, Elbaz, Bar-Eli & Weinberg, 1991).

    However, other studies have indicaîed neutrai or partial results regarding the

  • 6

    effectiveness of goal-sen@ (Anshel, Weinberg & Jackson, 1992; Barnetf 1977;

    Hollingsworth, 1975; Weinberg, Fowler, Jakson, Bagnall & Bniya, 199 1). Locke

    (1991) commented ihat these neutral results may have been due to the fàct ?hat do-

    your-best and cmîrol groups were setting goals. This would have confomded the

    goal-setting effect More recently, however, a meta-analysis of 3 6 different *dies

    in goal-setting revealed an eEect s i x of 0.34 (SD= 0.026) indicating that goal-

    setting does enhance perfiomance in the sport and exercise domain (Ky110 &

    Landen, 1995).

    While goal-setting is generally effective with high achievers (Locke &

    Latham, 1990) there is little data conceming goal-setting in physical activity and

    special populations. A recent study yielded significant irnprovements in a maximal

    sit-up task with adolescents who had behavioral disorders when a short-term plus

    long-term was used(Bar-Eli et al., 1994).

    In surnmq, goal-seaing has proven to be a robusr concept in the induskial

    realm (Locke t Laîham, 1990). Superior improvements with goal-setting have

    also been found in the sport domain (Bar-Eli et al., 1994; Barnen & Stanicek,

    1979; Burton, 1984,1993; Erbaugh & Barnett, 1986; Erffmeyer, 1987; Hall &

    Byrne, 1988; Hall, et al., 1987; Tenenbaum et al., 1991; Weinberg et al., 1988).

    Furthemore, low achieving pupils and children with LD have perforxed

    significantly better with goal-setîing techniques in the academic realm (Bandura &

  • Schunk, 1981; Kline et ai., 1990; Sawyer et al.; Schunk & Swartr, 1991).

    Thmefore, demonstrating the eEectiveness of goal-setting in the sport domain with

    children having leaming disabilities may prove goal-setting to be aviable tool for

    optimizing leanaing in this domain.

    1.1 Staternent of the Purpose

    nie main purpose ofthis study was to investigate the effects of goal-setting

    and the influence of ski11 level in the mastery of a motor ski11 with children who

    have leaming disabilities. A sub-purpose of ttiis study was to detexmine whether

    skill level was arnore relevant predictor of success than the learning attributes of

    these children.

    1.2 Hypotheses

    1. Skill improvement for the goaf-setiing groups will be significantly

    greater than for the control groups.

    2. Goal setting p u p s (LD and normaily achieving) in the low rnotor

    skill level will yield the greatest improvements.

    3. There will be no significant diffemces across trials between LD

    and nonnally achieving children of similar rnotor ski11 level in sirnilar

    goal-setiing groups.

  • 8

    4. Perceptions of physical self-cornpetence will be positively related

    with performance.

    1.3 Definitions

    Leamina disabilities: Classification of children who are more than two years

    behind in language andor mathematical skills mavale & Reese, 1932)

    Learned heldessness: Sense of powerlessness in behavior outcornes based on

    attributions of lack of ability or extemal locus of conîrol (Dweck, 1986)

    Proximal eoal-setting: Regulating one's behavior by attempting to reach a realistic

    standard within a short period of time (Bandura & Schunk, 1981)

    perceived self-cornoetence: Perceptions of one's ability in a particular domain

    (social, physicai or intellectual) marter, 1982)

    Motor devcloprncntal de1 -8: Motor patterns that are not yet integrated at the

    expected age level (Lazarus, 1990)

    Ph~sical awlnvardness: Syndrome of "chilcken without known neuromuscular

    pioblems who fail to perform culturaily-normative motor skills with acceptable

    proficiency" (Wall, 1982)

    1.4 Delimitations

    Children with LD in this study are caucasian, French-speaking and live in a

    small suburb of Montreal. Their ages Vary f?om 9 to 13 years.

  • 1.5 Limitation

    The children with LD have not been randomly smpled but they should not

    be logically differcnt e o m other LD children because they fa11 into the traditionai

    definition. The object control skifls tests used for the older children, those between

    11 and 13 years is not standardized for this age group. However, it was assumed

    th& the Test of Gross Motor Development (Ulrich, 1985) which describes maturt

    ski11 patterns would be sensitive enough to classify pupils into high and low cate-

    gories of object contml proficiency.

  • CHAPTERTWO

    RF,VIEW OF LITERATURE

    The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of goal-setting in the

    acquisition of a motor skill with children liaving leaming disabilities (LD). The

    diildm wae gouped into a hi& or low category of ski11 Ievel and compared to non-

    LD p u p s (hi& and lm skill level) of chilQea It was assumed aiat dl subjects who

    received specinc goal-setting instruction would outperform subjects in a do-your-b est

    go al condition, the goal-setting acting as a ~e~regulatory rnechanism for pursuhg

    achievement.

    This chapter has been divided into two major sections: 2.1 Learning

    Disabilities and 2 2 Goal-Setting Theory. Subsections for Learning Disabilities will

    covwthe followhg topics: 2 1.1 ûverview of Definition, Incidence and Etiology, 2.1.2

    Historical Perspective+ 2 2 3 Educational Characteristics, 2.1.4 Motor Characteristics,

    and 2 1.5 Motivational Characteristics. The goal-sethg theory will be covered by :

    22 1 htroduction, 2.2.2 Factors thai Mediate Goal-Setting, 2.2.3 The Application of

    Goal-Settitlg in Sports and Exercise, 2.2.4 Childien and Goal-Setting and 2.2.5 Goal-

    Seîting in the Academic Realm.

  • 2.1 Leamiug Disabilities

    2.1.1 Oveuview of Definiho . . n. Incidence and E t i o l w

    Definition o f a Leatlrirap DisabW

    'Ibis sedion is devoted to outlining characteristics of children with a learning

    didility. A disaepancy between one's potential for leaming and achievement defmes

    aleanhg disability. Learning disabilities have sparked the interest of professionals

    fi-om many disciplines such as neurology, psychology, education, and occupational,

    speech, d 1- therapy. However, inconsistencies and debate regarding critical

    issues such as definition, characteristics, a d etiology have e s e n (Epstein, Cullinan,

    Lessen 6t Lloyd, 1980; Moats & Lyon, 1993). Despite these chailenges, a present

    workiog dennition for leaming disabilities is a lag of two years below expected grade

    level for a child having an IQ within the normal range (85-115) (Kavale â Reese,

    1992).

    Incidence

    As of 1989, almosî 50% cf al1 students receiving special educatioii services in

    elementary and szcotidary schools in the U.S. were students with LD (Kavale &

    Reese, 1992; Lerner, 1993). These students represented 5% of al1 school aged

    children. Comparaîively, in the Provincc of Qucbcc, for the school year ending in

    1994-1995,68% of children rece iving sp ecific services were identified as having rnild

  • 12

    and sevae learning disabilities, excluding mental retardation (MHQ., 1995). These

    children represented 8% of al1 school aged children Males ouînumber females at a

    3:l ratio according to U.S. statistics (Feagans, 1987).

    uww Four major hypotheses have emerged as to the cause of LD. These are:

    neiirological orpsychophysiologicd disrurbances (Flowers, 1993; Opp, 1994; Shalev

    & Gross-Tsur, 1993; Snow, 1992), infBrmation processing deficits (Ayres, 1979;

    Cmickshi-i& 1977; Frostig, 19721, ineEcient teaching methods (Epstein et al., 1980)

    and inactive learning (Bender, 1987; Poplin, 1988). The psychophysiological

    proponetits posit th& certain a!teratiotis in neural physio l o g may have detrimental

    efteds cm leamhg Advocates of information processing deficits believe that leaming

    disabilities originate in the distuhance of one or more salient psychologicai abilities

    such as motor development, visuai, auditory or kinesthetic perception (Epstein et al.,

    1980). In the aiird viewpoint, proponents of dyspedagogia daim thai academic

    difficulties are caused by inadequaie instnrction (Epstein et al., 1980). Finally, the

    inactive leamer hypothesis has emanated f?om the observation that these children

    f k p d y fail to use leaniing sMegies thai facilitate leaming and that when they are

    taughttiiese strategies, they are able to perfom almost as well as normally achieving

    children (Bender, 1987; Poplin, 1988; Sawyer et al., 1992).

  • 2.1.2 ffistorical Y emective

    The foilowirlphistoricai p- will trace the origins of the designation and

    developrnent of the field of Iearning disabilities. As early as the nineteenth century,

    teadiers indphysiciais described cliildren who were unable to Leam to read and write

    despite nomal intellechial abilities (Opp, 1994). These children could not be

    chsif ied as having mental retardaiion but their identification and recognition for

    specific &livay services was not forthcoming in the United States until the mid 1960 S.

    SMar iy to otherhumansciences, the field of leming disabilities has evolved

    through four historical phases: a foundation phase (1800-1930), a transition phase

    (1930-1967), an integration phase (1960-1980) and a contemporary phase (1980-

    present) (Lemer, 1993). In the foundation phase (1800-1930), German

    neuroscientists such as Wemicke, Litchtein, Berlin, Goldsheider and Liepmann

    investigaîed aphasia, speech, reading and writing disorders linked to brain damage

    (Opp, 1994). A leading theory emerged: an impairment in the ability to comunicate

    be it verbal or written resulted fiom the dismption of neural pahways (ûpp, 1994).

    Brain research was pursued into the 20th century by Kurt Goldstein who

    sûdiedlnain-injd soldiers . He reported certain behavioral characteristics of these

    so ldiers which were also present in children with learning dificulties studied by

    StI;niss and W a n a (cited in Feagans, 1987): forced responsiveness to stimuli, figure-

    ground coafusion, perseveration, hyperactivity, meticulousness and catastrophic

  • 14

    d m ï h e characteristics are still used in present day diaguostic interviews and

    tests with these children (Feagans, 1987).

    The transition phase (1930-1960) was highlighted by the work of two

    indivimials who itxvdgated the neurological and educational deficits of children with

    leaming dkbilities; Samuel Ortm and AEed Stnuss. Samuel Orton, a neurologist,

    hypotfit=sizi=d that dyslexia originated in a lack of cerebral dominuice which impeded

    the development of language (cited in Lazanis, 1990). Strauss, on the other hand,

    establisfied a distindion between two types of mental retardation: endogenous (inbom)

    and exoepmus ( d l t i n g fiom brain insult). He later collaborated with L m Lehtinen

    to dwise special leamhg ewironments for these "brain-injured" or exogenous children

    (cited in Feagans, 1987). The fo llowing principl es were recomrnended: a) optimal

    structure, b) reduction of space, c) reduction of ail irreievant stimuli and d) use of

    bright and attractive instructional material.

    As the LD field progressed towards the integrarion phase (19604980). a

    prominait educator Daned CNickshank studied intellectually nomal children who also

    demonstrated soine of the behavioral characteristics manifested by "brain-injured"

    chi ldren (Feagans, 1987). He adopted Strauss' stark leaming environmznts in the

    hope of enbanchg the leamhg of these highly distractible youth. IR 1963, Dr.

    Samuel Kirk officially labeled the field of "learning disabilities" in support of conver-

    ging information Corn such disciplines as neurolog, psychology and education, and

  • 15

    pressme fiwipae&al associations (Moats & Lyon, 1993). Subsequently, the passing

    ofthe AmencaiPublic Law, PL 91-230, 1969, which asserted the rights of individuais

    wi8iID to speciflc hûuctioaal intervention, provided M e r support to the establish-

    meai of this field During this phase, school programs for children with LD began to

    flourish ( h e r , 1993).

    Entering the contemporary phase (1 980- present) student-type distinctions

    were refined and remedial services transformed (Lemer, 1993). Mild and severe

    lemhg disabilities were differentiated Attention deficit disorders with and without

    hyperactivity have been recognized as specific conditions independent of a leaming

    disability, although occurting more fi-equently in this group of childrea (Fea-s,

    1987; Mo& &Lyon, 1993). Furthemore, educators presently agee th& culhrral and

    linguistic diversity requires specific intervention for adeqyaîe leaming. It has been

    demonstrated thaî there is an overrepresentation of himigrant ciiildren in the LD

    group (Lemer, 1993). Chmghg administrative amangemenu have influenced

    placement into special classes or mainstreaming and service delivery such as free-flow

    teaching and collaboraiive consultation for these children (Lenier, 1993). To better

    understand the link between thz motor behavior of these children and tlieir leaming

    patterns, the followiqg section describes their educaîional and motor chai-acteristics.

  • 2.1.3 Educationd Characteristics -

    L e a r e cbabiiities emanpass a wide raage of d=culties which underscores

    the fktthat each one of these children is unique. Yet, some general leanring patterns

    have been observed The following are some ofthe major findings in the cognitive

    doniain: Mure to use cognitive strategies (Ellis, Deshler, Lenz, Schurnaker & Clark,

    199 1; Giordano, 1982; Sawyer et al., 1992; Torgesen, 1980; Torgesen & Goldmaa,

    lm, short-term verbd rnemory deficits (Snow, 1992; Torgesen & Goldman, 1977) and c@hctional mental flexibility and planning skills (Snow, 1992). These children

    also tend to be field dependent in their cognitive approach to Itaming (Lazanis, 1990)

    meaning thai tliey fail to sampie fully fi-om the available cue set. They approach

    teaming as spectators, resorting to trial and error strategies instead of focussing on

    pmcesses and relationships. Subtyping children with LD for educational purposes

    ac'mowledges the diversity found within m.

    Educational practice has divided LD into tfiree groiips: 1) reading disability

    or dyslexia, 2) arithmetic disability or dyscalculia and 3) reading and arithmetic

    disability (Shafrir & Siegel, 1994; Shalev & Gross-Tsur, 2993). For example, in

    Shafrir and Siegel (19941, both the reading disability and the reading and arithmetic

    disability groups displayxi defcits in plionologiçal processing, vo cabulary spelling and

    short t e m memory. However, tliese deficits were not present in the arithmetic

    disability group. Finally, a spatial visual deficit was detected in the reading and

  • 17

    anthmetic disability and the arithmetic disability groups only. This resaarch concludes

    tfiatcaDun leaming oritcBmes are associaicd with specific types of learning disabilities.

    The sane is &O ûue for motor behavior. Certain subgroups of children witli LD have

    no motor difliculties whatsoever while others have a developmental coordination

    disorder (Haubenstricker, 1982).

    Motor Training Used to Imnrove Academic Standing

    Attempts to improve academic performance through motor training became

    popular in the 1960s. Following in aie footsteps ofliaget, Delacato and Kephart,

    Marianne Frostig creaîed a Percephial-Motor Training Program (Frostig & Home,

    1964) based onLuna!s concept of pairing a weak psychological finction with a strong

    me so that new connections would be created in the brain (Frostig, 1972). Children

    with LD had evidenced disturbances in visual perceptuaf abilities, figure-ground

    perception and iriltegr;bive abilities. However, the û;mstèr of perceptual-motor training

    to academic skills was not dernonstrated (C- & Martin, 1969; Kavale & Maitson,

    1983; Reid, 1982) ad even the percephiabmator skills of these children did not show

    significant improvernent with these programs (Kavale & Matison, 1983).

    Similady, A.J. Ayres (1979), considered a leader in occupational therapy,

    bro@ forth a remedial program called S ensory Integration nierapy (SI) which was

    f& aimed at children with mental retardaiion but was eventually used with children

    haviogLD. This pro- was conceived to provide vestibular stimulation, t h i c h in

  • 18

    hm would rzrnediate the perceptual and integrative disturbances of childRn with LD

    and allow hem to leam more efficiently (Ayres, 1972b as quoted in Hoehn &

    Baumeister, 19N). Critical analysis of studies o f Seasoiy Integration therapy reveded

    no unique banefits werz provided to these çhildrcn and that this remedial therapy

    was not effective (Hoehn & Baumcistcr, 1994). Thercfore, the association bebveen

    femedidng motor &lqs with thc view o f irnproving cognitive pro cesses has not been

    established.

    Children with LD became a concem for physical educators because of their

    djfkent lemirtg styles and behavioral liabits in the g t t ~ Professi onals in the adapted

    physical education field now focus their attention on the identification and solution of

    the psychomotor problems of these children (Sherrill, 1993).

    Admted Ph~sical Education for Chil- With LI3

    Indie field of physical education, Shen41 (1993) was the first author to write

    achapter on LD in aphysical education textbook (Sherrill, 1472). She relied on the

    works of Cruickstiank (managing learning environments) and Kephart who stressed

    balance ;adivities and imitation of rnovement games, to create a pedagogical approach

    fur childrerx with LD.

    Anothaprominent physical educator, Bryant J. Craüy (in the early 70s), at the

    Unbmily ofcalifornia at Los Angeles, contributed to the percephial-motor pedagogy

    by recommending highly structured rnovement experiences to rernediate clurnsiness

  • 19

    ad gbmes ushg Ietters and numbers to supplement classroom insûuction. He rejected

    the notion thaî perceptual-rnotor training led to academic improvement (Cratly &

    Mariin , 1969).

    More recently, authors in the adapted physical education domain have also

    devoted chapters o f their workbooks to learning disabilities ( C m 1990; Horvaî,

    1990). B luechardt and Shephard (1995) have reported the usefulness o f motor and

    social skills training pmgrarns for children with LD. These authors addressed the need

    fot improvement ofseIf-woftfi and social skills with these children because poor motor

    s k i k have led to peer rejection m d limited self-worth (self-esteem).

    2.1.4 Motor Characteristics of Clhildren

    With Leamine Disabilities

    Along with deficits in cognitive processes, these children are also at risk of

    demonstrating motor impairments &or lowr motor proficiency ( L a a m s , 1990)

    which in tum may interfere with their social integration (Mender, Kerr & ûrlick,

    1982). Inefficient movernent for a child means having poor play skills, difticulty

    handling changes in the environment and relating with same age peers (Bouffârd,

    Thompson & Watkinson, 1992; Gniber, f 969; Reid, 1982) In turn, avoidance or

    Limitedparticipation in physical activity may lead to t-educzd fimess (Sherrill, 1993;

    Bryan & Smilzy, 1983) and passive Iifistyls (Margalit, 1983).

    For the pst tu;* yeam, fitaaîure regarding leaming disabilities has indicaîed

  • 20

    the pmsnce ofmotor dificulties ranging fiam subtle disiurbaiices to a developmental

    coordination disocder (DCD) or physical awkwardness within this group (Ayres, 1 979;

    Bmininks & Bniininks, 1977; Cartwright, Cartwright & Ward, 1985; C-, 1980;

    Cratty & Martin, 1969; Freides, Barbati, van Kampen-HorowiG Sprehne, Iversen,

    Silver &Woodward, 1980; Frostig, 1972; Golick, 1978; Gruber, 1969; Henderson &

    Sugden, 1992; Jacklin, 1987; Kelly, 1990; Lasse, Henderson, Elliman, Hall, Knight

    & Jongmans, 1991; Mender et al., 1982; Reid, 1981, 1982; Sherrill& mer , 1985;

    Smith, 1985; Tansley, 1986; Wall, 1982; Wornack & Womack, 1982).

    Certain niotor variables have corne under close scmtiny. The variables are:

    body awareness, spatial orientation, bilateral coordination, laîerality, balance,

    mvenmüoverflow, visual rnotor difficulties, dissociation and temporal organization

    and auditory stimulation.

    Bodv awareness, matid orientation and biIateral coordination

    Cfiildren with LD fiequently demonstrate delay in body awareness (Craîty &

    Martin, 1969; Fro& 1972; Footlik, 1970; Kelly, 1990) which is reiaied to difficulties

    ofperception ofposition in space and of directiouality (Frostig, 1972; Golick, 1979;

    L w , 1990; Shmill, 1993). This diEcul~y was partially blamed an lack of latzral

    dominance wliich was held to b e a crucial part of cognitive dwelopmed (Lazanis,

    1990). Childm widi LD were thought to be incapable of achieving laterd dominance

  • 21

    at an eariy age and neurologists hypothesized that cousistent sidedness in leaming

    would create irnproved lateralization in the child (Lazams, 1990). However, no

    fornial demonstration has emerged indicatiiig thai the degree of handedness is relaied

    to the degrec of lateralization (Lazanis. 1990). Yct, consistency of handedness and

    footedness appear to be relaied to bilaterd coordination (Bruininks & Bruininks,

    1977). Bilateral coordination, which impl ies the simultaneous or altemathg use of

    upper and l m limbs is also delayed in these children relaiively to same aged peers

    (Bruininks & Bruininks, 1977; Shemill& m e r , 1985).

    Ballance

    These cliildren experience difficulties with balance when tested oii staîic

    balance tests (Bniininks & Bruininks, 1977; Sherrill & Mer, 1985; Mender et a!.,

    1982; CraUy&MartUi, 1969; Freides et al, 1980). However, Miyahara's (1994) study

    indicaied thnt about 7% of the sample had exiremeij pour balance along with 25.5%

    who did poorly on al1 goss rnotor subtests including balance. The remainder of the

    LD children did not have rnotor difiiculties-

    Movement overflow

    Moment ovedow refers to extraneous movement irrelevant to the intended

    motor function. These extramous movements usually disappear with the maturation

    of the central nervous system, altiiough they are observable in young chikiren.

    Howsicr, in children witii LD they art: apparent for a significanily longer period (Foo-

  • 22

    tlik, 1970; Lazanis, 1990, 1994; Tansley, 1986). The persistence of movement

    ovedow could indicate developmenral dclay of the central nervous system (Cohen,

    Ta& Mahadeviah & Birch, 1967; Wo18; Gunnoe & Cohen, 1983). Furthemore,

    rnovcment overflow is associatcd with lack o f inliibitùq control (Lazavs, 1990,

    1994). ïhis conirol rncchanism allows the child to gaugc rhc required amount of force

    or spced in a rnovcment (Lannis, 1994). To execute a skilled action, movement

    constmcy and consistency are required (Wall, McClements, Bouffird, Findlay &

    Taylor, 1985). Acquiring this constancy and consistency has proven to be more

    challenging to children with LD as reported iii the variability of their performance

    results wtten compared ta same age peers (Bnint & Distefano, 1982; Bnmt et al,

    1983).

    Visual-ma tor difficulties

    A[ttiw& sonie have assumed that faulty visuai perception is the cause o f poor

    motor functioning, research lias aîtempted to determine whether these ctiildren's

    diEculties are visuai, percepnid or rnotor in name (Bmt & Distefano, 1982;

    O'Brien, Cermak & Murray, 1988; Pumztt & Steinhaiier, 1984). Examinhg the

    visuai-pacephial andvisual-niotor scores of ccclumsy" and ccnon-clumsy"children with

    LD and Nonnally Achievhg children, differences in bolh areas ware observed

    between NA and "clumsy" children but none between "clumsy" and ccnm-clumsy"

    children with LD (O'Brien et al., 1988). It was reported in another study, that the

  • 23

    dktukmce iay in both the motor-coordinative component and the integration between

    the visual-perceptual and motor-coordinative components (Maitison, McIntyre, Brown

    & Murray, 1986). Responding to a visual stimulus for clianging ninni~z direction

    proved to be a challenge tu these childreo as they increased their reaction time &

    movement time (Bnint St Distefano, 1982). The contrcl subjects demonstrated an

    increase in reaction time but very littlc variation in running speed (movement time).

    This would indicate thaî children with LD have more dficiilty organizing a motor

    response to unexpected change.

    Eyesight (Kerr & Hughes, 1987; Mon-Williams, Pascal â WW, 1994) and

    v i sual perception (Rosblad fZ von Hofsten, 1994) of chi ldren with developrnental

    coordination disorder hm been reported to be sirnilar to that of controls. Ophtairnic

    difficulty did not explain problems in rnovernent control. Yet, a shidy concerning

    viszral memory did identie significant difEèrences with cliildren having DCD wlien a

    15 s d e l a y w Snposed before being allowed to reproduce agraphical task (Dwyer &

    McKenzio, 1994). This shidy showed differences in visuai rehearsal strategies. It

    would appear that the integrative difEiculties between intended aid produced action

    obsavd by Mjlimison et al. (1986) may lie in visual memory deficiis raîher than visual

    or visual-perceptual dtficits. To coricludt this szi;tiùri, studies concerning visuai,

    visual motor and visuai perceptual deficits have indicated that the visual and visual

    perceptual fùnctioning of children wiih LD were intact (Brunt & Distefano, 1982;

  • 24

    Mm-WfIIians et al., 1994; O'Brien et al., l9S8; Purutett & Steinhaiier, 1984; Rosblad

    & von Hofiten, 1994). nie deficit appears to lie in the organization of the motor

    response (Brunt & Distefano, 1982; O'Brien et al., 1988) and in visual memory

    deficits (hlaüison et ai., 1986).

    Jlissociation

    Dissociation is the ability to gnsp the relationship between parts that make up

    wholes and is usually not fully developed before the age of 9 (Sherrill, 1993).

    However, delays in this ability are more apparent in children with LD (Shemll, 1993).

    This characteristic is associaîed with tliair daficits in figure-gound perception

    (F 'eqy, 1987;Fmstig 1772; Sherrill, 1993) which nfer to difficulties in picking out

    an object from a complex background or contision with the tems near, far, hi& and

    low.

    Temporal owanization and auditory stimulation

    Body movements require exact timing and proper sequencing of components

    especiailywienaccuracy, speed, power, timing and force are involved Wealmess in

    timing has been recognized through tests of synchronized rhythmical tapping

    (Buininks & B ~ u i n i ~ , 1977), foot patting (Keudrick & Hauten, 1979) and teaching

    ofdance or rhythmicai aciivities with thesa childrzn (Slizrrill, 1993). Children with

    developrriental coordiliatiori disorder have also exhibitad more variation in perfor-

  • 25

    mance thau controls in riiaintaining a set rate of tapping and accurately judging thne

    intends (Williams, Woollacott & Ivry, 1992). Observations of difficulties in recall

    arid in proper sequetiçing of movements were nutüd by prztitioners and found to be

    d e v i a d by the use of verbal reli~arsal (Kowalski & Sherrill, 1992; Ta7;uus, 1990;

    Sherrill, 1993).

    Chi ldren with LD were asked to perfonn a reciprocai tapping task using a

    sîyfus to touch 16 different taret combinations ( K m & Hughes, 1987). Their initial,

    progressive and fuial results were lower than aiat of normally achieving childrea

    Howeva, heir progress at handling the increased diEculty of the task was similar to

    that of normaily achieving children. The resuks lead Kert and Hughes (1987) to

    conch.de that children with LD had difficulties handling information at the onset of the

    task. Upon examining the motor profile of children with LD, the vdability of their

    perfmnmce even with extensive practice highiights the necessity of investigating their

    particularrieeds While some of thest: children have poor body awareness, others may

    have balance or coordination deficits and others are as successfùl as normally

    achieving children in physical activitieï (Miyahara, 1994). Recognizing thai a

    subgroup of children with LD are more at risk of displaying motor difticulties,

    athnpts have been made to fmd a relationship between certain cognitive disabilitics

    and motor disturbances.

  • subnrou~ine Based an Motor Charactefistics

    When subgrouping these children for motor purposes, researchers have

    grouped them either by specific cognitive disabilities such as dyslexia (reading

    disabled) or dyscdculia aud pmvided a motor profile by specific leaming disability

    (Golick, 1978; L m , 1990), sr by consiclzrhg al1 LD and tzsting individuals on

    spççific motor skik such as balance, bilateral coordination and eye-hand coordination,

    in order to subgroup them according to motor strengths and weaknesscs (Bruininks &

    Bniininks, 1977; Haubenstricker, 1982; Miyahara, 1994; Sherrill & Mer, 1985).

    Golick (1978), fiom clinical eqerience, identified physical awkwardness in the

    g o y of children havirig dificulties with space, tinie and numbers but reported good

    body management within the group having language disorders and specific dyslexia

    lazans (1990) noted three subgoups: a reading disable&'dyslexic group displaying

    subtle motor de fi ci^? a cluster of children who exhibit visual-spatial-motor dificuities

    and a group displaying no motor inconipetence wltose leaniing disabilities were

    associaied with afkèctive façtors.

    Bruininks and Bruininks (1977) were thz fvst authors to ç o h the motor

    dysfuuctions of children witli LD as a group. Frzidzs et al. (1980)also reported

    sigufhnldeficits on measures of postural and equilibrium reflzxzs as wcll as skills.

    Pursnuog this research, Sherrill and Pqfir (1 985) reported k e e subgroups within their

    -le of children hwing LD: 13% were severely delayed in thek motor fùnitions,

  • 27

    75% s c d m m g e on some tests but below average on other tests and the remaining

    12% demonsîraîed no motor detays. A recent study, using balance as the primary

    distinctive trait for subgrouping LD helped establish four distinct profiles based oo

    gross motor h t i o n s Pfiyahara, 19 94). In a samplz of 147 students with LD, 43 -6%

    of the children wcre frcc fiom severe motor problems, 25.5% were poorly

    coordinated @oor performance in ail gross motor subtests) and 23.6% had good

    balance but deficits on other motor tests. The rernaining 7.3% had extrernely poor

    balance skills but parfomcd well in mength and bail skills and displayed average

    results in rurming speed and bilateral ceordination. Taylor (1982) also fond that

    within the LD group of children 27.7% of them had significant rnotor dificulties.

    Furtherrnore, their ditficulties range fi-om mild to severe and c m be found in one

    specific area such as balance or in several, such as g o s s rnotor, fine motor and

    balance.

    Fitness and Leisure

    One of the comequences of early movement dificulties and limited positive

    social interactions in play situations, is the risk of dzveloping a passive lifestyle

    (Bodkd et al., 19%; Margaiit, 1984; Sheierrill, 1993). Margalit (1984) observed that

    thesa childrzn speni morc time in passive and soliiüry xtivitizs and were more

    dependent on tlieir parents for leisure time activities. During fiee play at school,

    Boufkd et al. (1992) noticed tfiat children with rnotor dficulties were less fiequently

  • 28

    inîeracting with peers, were much less active in fiee play and tended to avoid using

    large playground apparatus. Their fihiess levels were reparted to be lower than

    norrnally achieving children as well (Bryan & Smiley, 1983; Shemili, 1993).

    h conclusion, it has been shown that children with I D are at risk of demons-

    û-aîing ineficient movement, and motor learning is more challenging to them than to

    nonnally achieving children. Their dficulties range h m mild to severe and cm be

    found in one specific area such as balance, or in several areas, such as gross motor,

    fine motor and balance. The exact causes are still unEcnown. However, research

    poirds to weak W a l and visual memory skills (Kowalski & Shedll, 1992; Maüison

    et al., 1986; Snow, 1992; Torgesen, 1980; Torgesen & Goldman, 1977), lack of

    effective practice and exposure to physicai activities (Bouffxd et al., 1996). This

    situation, in hm, rnay hinder their atfective, social and physical well-being.

    Furthemore, a particular subgroup of these children, up to 27.7% of them (Taylor,

    2982) rnay be physically awkward which, if left umemediated, rnay persist into

    adulthood (Losse et al., 1991).

    -E Disabilities and Deveio~mental Coordination Disorder

    Physical awkwaccfness or developmental coordination disorder@CD) is a more

    fiequently occuring syndrome in children with LD. Henderson and Sugden(1992)

    estimated that 15% of the nomally achieving child population have moderaie to severe

    rnovanad difficulties. In groups of chilken with LD, over 25% of them display this

  • 29

    syndrome (Mihayara, 1994; Taylor, 1982).

    'Ihe developmd C O O T & ~ disorder designates the condition of physically

    awkward children who have no neurornuscular problems but "fail to perform

    Cuitrpaily-normative motor skills with acceptable proficiency." (Wall, 1982, p. 257).

    Origindly, Gubbay (1978) titled clumsiness of limbs or gait or awkwardness as

    developmeniai apiaxia The term ccclurnsy" has been fi-equently used Ui past literaîure

    to designate subjects having these motor dysfunctions (Freides et al., 1980; Maeland,

    1992; O'Brien et al., 1988; Shaw, Levine & Belfer, 1982; Williams et al., 1992).

    Children with DCD exhibit deficits in rnotor planning which refers to the ability tu

    initiate, correctly sequence, and terminate a chah of movements (Gubbay, 1978;

    Haubenstricker, 1982; Keogh, Sugden, Reynard & Calkins, 1979; Wall, Reid &

    Paon, 1990). Hence, they require more tirne to l e m a new ski11 whzn compared to

    age matched peers and are persistently slower in reaction and movement time than

    their peers (Missiuna, 1994; van Dellen & Geuze, 1987). Henderson and Sugden

    (1992) posit that these children are lagging in movement skills necessary for school.

    achievement and thai the degree of severity and diversity of irnpairments will Vary

    from one child to the next. However, they contend th& children with severe

    coordination deficits will commonly display disturbances in almost any motor task

    presmtd to them. Motor skills appear to lie on a continuum fkom gross to fine niotor

    andmrmytada involve the interplay of both types of movement, for example, catching

  • 30

    abdL Mermore , the observation of gross motor coordination only (of€en the case

    Uiphysid echzc;Pim settings) cannot appraise accurately a child's motor skills. Thus,

    they have created a comprehensive screening test, the Movement ABC (Henderson

    & Sugc)en, 19%) which is the most recent adaptation of the TOM1 test (Stott, Moyes,

    &He&mm, 1972) and covers marnai dextenty (fine motor tasks), bail skills, static

    and dynarnic balance items.

    The Iink between a subgrmrp of children with LD and children wi?h

    DCD is that they both dernonstrate difficuIties in reaching muvernent consistency

    (Brunt & Distefano, 1982). Movement consistency, which is the prime objective of

    motor l eming is more of a challenge to children with DCD (Marchiori, Wall &

    Bedingfïeld, 1987 Missiuna, 1994; Wall et ai., 1990) as well as for chi ldm with LD

    (Brunt & Distefano, 1982). Investigation into the causes of DCD has brought forth

    diffiàeothypottr-: limited knowledge base of motor skills (Wall, 1982), the iack of

    kkestktic sensitivity hypotfiesis (Bairstow & M o , 1981), and disturbances of the

    centrai timekeeping mechanisms (Williams et al., 1992). Wall's (1982) knowledge-

    base d approach contended th& these children lacked the practice, declarative

    Imowle* and dkctive attitude required to promote appropriate motor development

    Kkstfietic sensitivity was demonstrated to be less well processed by clumsy

    (physically awkward) children by BaVstow and Lasdo's kinaesthetic sensitivity test

    (Lado & Bairstow, 1985a). According to these authors, such a deficiency prevents

  • 3 1

    tfianfhn ade

  • 32

    emotiondly to testhg may also partiaily explain their variance of performance

    @enderson & Sugden, 1992).

    It has been shown thai children with DCD are afnicted with low self-esteem

    (Stott, Henderson & Moyes, 1986) and even more so ifthey have both LD and DCD

    (Shaw et al., 1982). Children with LD and DCD were more unhappy and had fewer

    Eiends of the same sex than the other children with LD (Shaw et al., 1982).

    The aît of Iearning is constantly influenced by many fwtors. When Iooking

    atmskq o r i d o n , Schunk (1989) indicaied that the most salient predictors of what

    and how rapidly students l e m are : 1) cognitive abilities, 2) outcome expectancies

    md beliefk, and 3) value piaced on these outcornes. Licht and Kist.net (1986) fùrther

    reported that teacher feedback and class goal stmcture are also critical features

    iduencing educational progress for children with LD. The following section will

    explainmotivational tendencies ofthesi children and how they compare with children

    who are rnastery oriented

    2- 1-5 Motivationai Characteristics

    While childreu with LD may do poorly in acadernic subjects and consider

    themselves as weak students, they are able to daerentiate their abilities in other

    domains such as games and sport (GriftTtlis, 1975). Low ~ e ~ c o n c e p t s in the

    academic domain do not necessarily mean low self-concepts in the social and physical

  • 33

    domaios, as children are able to assess themselves differently fiom one domain to

    aiotha(Ektu, 1980,1982; Montgomery, 1994). Regrettably, however, low persis-

    tence at atask (Bender, 1987; Yong & Mchtyre, 19%) and disregard for effort in the

    açademic context (Licht & Kistner, 1986; Pearl, 1982) seems to prevail in physical

    eadeavours (Kelly, 1990). An important question is how to motivate these children

    to put in the effort required for improvement and sriccess?

    Accordhg to Ncholls' developmentai theory o f children's motivation, concepts

    ofabilityandeffort evolve as afùnction ofthe child's age (Nicfiolls, 1984a). Ayoung

    child believes thaî intelligence and ab ility (in a particular domain) can change and be

    improved upon. As children get older (10-11) they begin to construe intelligence

    andor ability as fixed amibutes, therefore, recognizing their own limitations. They

    also beginto mgkz that effort may not compensate for lack of ability in developing

    greater levels of cornpetence. Finally, around the age of 12-13 the child becomes

    convinced of these limiting fztors, and that effort and ability are completely

    digerenti ate d

    Childrai wibiID who acperience early and repeated failures in school respond

    d i f 5 d y Eom nomially achieving chi f dren in their aitributions for success and failure

    (Deci &-der, 1986; Deci et al., 1992; Fuhler, 1991; GrifEths, 1975; Montgome-

    ry, 1994; Pead, 1982; Yong & McIntyre, 1992). These children are low in motivation

    for school work Ficht L Kistner, 1986; Yong & Mchtyre, 1992), have trouble

  • 34

    feeling competent (Deci et al., 1992), lack persistence at a task (Bender, 1987;

    hndall , Kaîkovsky at Crandall, 1965) have low self-esteem ki the academic arena

    ( G B h s , 1975), are inactive in their l e m h g (Bender, 1987; Poplin, 1988; Torgesen,

    l977), have d i i l l t y fèeling responsib le for the ir suc cesses or failures (Crandall et al.,

    1%5;Fuhler, 1991; Pearl, 1982) and aitribute success and failure to luck or task ease

    Pearl, 1982). Deci et al. (1992) found ihat children with LD particularly needed to

    focus on comp etence and imo lvernent variab les, thai is, situations they can control in

    order to regain positive self-perceptions. Ofparticular interest for the present shidy

    is their attributionai styles for success and fàilure.

    Mastery Orientation Versus ZRarned Heblessness

    Attribution theory (Weiner,1974) explains human behavior by the causal

    perceptions responsible for what happens to oneself in the day-to-day environment

    When ccniparing rnastery oriented children and learned helpless children, the former

    fée1 respomiblefor successes and usually attribute failtues to lack of effort (Diener &

    k e c k 1978; Dweck, 1986; Dweck & Repucci, 1973; Elliott gi Dweck, 1988). The

    latter tend to give up in the face of dficulty and failure and look for causes of their

    failure outside of their own control raiher than solutions to the problem at hand.

    Children with LD have been reported to attribute success to luck or task ease and to

    biarne failure on others such as the teacher (Diener & Dweck, 1978; Fuhler, 1991;

  • 35

    Licht & Kisber, 1986; Pearl, 1982; Pintrich, Andeman 64. Klobucar, 1994).

    Fmûmtely, attributions can be redirected with learned helpless children so thai they

    will consider effort as essential to their success (Dimer & Dweck, 1978).

    eaniine Goals and Outcome Goals

    Goal structure in class may also innuence the learning behavior of children

    withLD Wcht & Kistner, 1986). In physical education classes and in elite sport , an

    indivichial's goal on will m o w the way a task will be addressed (Papaioannou,

    1995). This will depend on the individual's spontaneous tendency to be learning

    oriented (tasic iwohed) or outcorne oriented (ego-involved) (Burton, 1993; Weinberg,

    19%) and also on how the environment is managed by the person in authority, either

    prornoting a learrting or an outcome orientation (Papaioannou, 1995). Researchers

    have ofien distinguished between learning and outcome goals. Learning goals are

    goals set for an individual to improve at a ski11 or task. Learning goals enhance experi-

    mentation and exploration of problem solving and allow for trial and mor .

    ûukome goals are fixed goals thai are nonnative, therefore, set in relation to

    others, highlighting social comparison For example, children need to demonstrate

    competençe sirnilar to same aged peers in a physical activity in order to be motivaied

    to pursue such activity, otherwise, intrinsic motivation for the activity will decrease

    flailmd, Gawm & Hailiwell, 1986). In one experiment, Elliott and Dweck (1988)

  • 36

    noted negbf/e a f k t in performance goals and complete elimlliation of these reactions

    inaie learning goals. In physical education, a combination of high leamhg and low

    outcome goal structure, (thus placing emphasis on mastering a task), sustaiaed high

    lwels ofdvat ion in children of al1 levels of perceived ability (Papaioannou, 1995).

    mdren's S m e

    In children's sport, the perception of dernonsirated competence appears to be

    the key variable related to perceptions of success and fâilure (Duda, 1987). It has

    b m shownht children are able to differentiaie their perceptions of self-cornpetence

    in di&mt domains (Harter, 1978b). In sports and gmes, children cm have feelings

    ofcornpetnice without the feedback of adults; they appraise aieir ability accordhg to

    the performance of same age peers (Harter, 1978b). Continued participation in the

    sporting domain is ftequently linked to feelings of competence in the particularly

    activity @uda, 1987) although social affiliation is also a major contributhg factor in

    chilch's sport participation (Klint & Weiss, 1987). Feelings of competence can be

    generated by the achievement of task onented goals or outcome goals (Duda, 1987).

    Wishing to defeat the opponent or surpassing an existing standard implies ego-

    LNolvenrad or an outcome goal orientation. According to Burton (1 994), Danish and

    Hale (1983) and Gould (19861, doing so proves to be too dificult and demotivating

    for the participant. The participant should concentrate on improving technique or

    suapassirigpastperf~nnaflces which implies task orientation or leaming goals. A task-

  • 37

    involved goal is one based on past pet.formance and remains within one's control.

    Leaming goals or task involvement have received fùrther support in Duda's (1 987)

    rwiew of children's sport literaaire and in Papaioamiou's (1995) study on favorable

    leaming conditions in the physical educaîion class. However, as far as optimal

    performance increments are concemed, Kyllo and Landers' (1 995) meta-analy sis of

    goal-setîing indicated tfiat participants who seek outcome goals in absolute terms

    outperform participants who are working on rel aiive goals.

    Setthggoals can have a m i v e or beneficial effects depending on how goals

    are set and how well suited they are for the individual (Burton, 1993; Weinberg,

    1994). Perceptions of one's ability may d a n c e or undermine one's goal acceptance

    (Nicholls, 1984a). High perceived ability individuas will prefer tasks at or above

    moderaie diEculty levels because they do not expect to decline. Conversely, low

    perceived ability individuals rnay handle atask with one of three tendencies: (1) they

    are still cornmitted to demonstraîing high ability despite failures, (2) some are more

    cntdintfratoieir ability level is low and lack comrnitment to demonstrate high ability,

    and (3) some are coltvhced thaîiheir ability level is low and will only accept easy tasks

    (Nicholls, 1984a).

    IndMMs and coaches-teachers should adjust goals, recognize whaî barriers

    are obstructing success @anish & Hale, 1983) and how the goal-setter react. to

    success-failure, that is, performance-orientation, success-orientation or failure-

  • 38

    orientation (Burton, 1993). Striving towards goal achievement, chi ldren with

    rnoverned diniculties, when lefl unguided, tended to set either very easy, already

    achieved goals, o r unnanainable goals which alleviated responsibility for failure or

    success (lienderson et al., 1989). Nonetheless, Vallerand and Reid (1990) have

    recornrnended the use of goal-setting as a self-regulaîor of action wiih special

    populatias-motor behavior. To conclude, effective goal-setting requires the

    implication of the goal-setter and recognition of how thai individual reacts to goal-

    setting in failure and in success siniaiions in order to create the most favorable

    conditions for performance improvement (Burton, 1993; Locke & Latham, 1990;

    Weinberg, 1994).

    2.2 Goal-Settuig Theury

    2.2.1 Introduction

    God-seühg is an endorsed method for rnotivating higher achievernent in sport

    (Burton, 1984; 1993; Danish & Kale, 1983; Gould, 1986; Kylfo & Landers, 1995;

    Pemberton L McSwegin, 1989; Weinberg, 1994). Yet, until recently, limited

    empincal research on goal-setting in sports has failed to prove overail effectiveness.

    However, am&-madysis of existing research produced by Kyllo and Landers (1995)

    indicaiedoÿPgoa.1-setting in sports and exercise produced significant effects over non

    goal-setting conditions.

    The foundations of goal-setting theory lie in the work of Locke and Latham

  • 39

    (1990), ia the business and industrial setting Over 500 studies, in management and

    psydiologyime established the effectiveness of goal-setting (Locke & Latham, 1990).

    Accordingto Locke and Latham (1985), goal-setting aEects performance by directing

    activity, mobilViog &kt, increas iBg persistence and rnotivathg search for appropriate

    strategies. Bandura (1986) also recognizes goal-sethg as a self-regulator of action.

    Reaching goals enhances one's self-efficaçyl and level of aspiration, which

    s u b s e e erdice the h d ~ d u a l to set new goals (Schunk, 1989). This psychological

    medranism is consistent witfi contemporary motivation theory whereby motivation is

    dependent on developing and maintainhg high perceived ab ility ttirough consistent

    goal aitainment (El 1 iott & Dweck, 1988; Nicholls, 1984a).

    Bandura (1986) identified certain events that are likely to occur upoa setting

    a goaL Once agoal is internalized, a need has been creaîed to reduce the discrepancy

    between the pmmt simion and the desired one. The effectiveness in achieving one's

    goals will depend upon interna1 and extemal mediaiors (Locke & Latham, 1990).

    1

    self-eficacy is the judgement one makes about how weii one will succeed at a new task based on past performance

  • 40

    2.2.2 J!'actors That Mediate GoaI-Settiqg

    Memal mediators include ability and the knowledge base of the task at hand

    (Burton, 19931, the perceived value of outcornes and cornmitment to them (Burton,

    1993;Locke & Laiham, 1985), perceptions of one's sense of control or responsibility

    (Bandura, 1986; Henderson et al., 1989), ~e~appraisa l skills (Bandura, 19861, the

    level of self-efiicaq and asp iraiion of the individual (Henderson et al., 1989) and goal

    orientation of the individual (Burton, 1994; Gould, 1986; Nicholls, 1984a). In this

    study, and in Locke and Latham's theory of goal-setting (1990), manipulation of the

    odaiial mediadois is aprime concern. Two idenid mediators were observed, thaî is,

    goal cornmitment and self-efficacy through ttie assessrnent of the perceived physical

    self-cornpetence scale.

    Extenid Mediators

    Exremal mediators are the factors which can be rnanipulated by individuals in

    authority such as teachers, parents, coaches, and bosses. These mediators include

    feedbisck ofresults, task difficulty,task complexity, self-aeterminaîion of goals, group

    versus individuai goals, and the use of leaming or outcome goals. A goal-setting

    theory has emerged after more than 25 years of experimentaîion with extemal

    moderaiors. The main finding are thai specific, difficult goals produce higher levels

    of performance than easy goals, no goal or do-your-best (Locke & Latham, 1990).

  • 41

    2-23 The Armlication of Goal-settinn in S ~ o r t s and Exercise

    In the sport and exercise realm, tasks used for goal-setting research have been

    quite varieci, ranging fi-om simple tasks such as sit-ups (Bar-Eli et al., 1994; Hall &

    Byme, 1988; Tenenbaum et al., 1991; Weinberg et al., 1991) to complex tasks such

    as juggling (Anshel et al., 1992; Barneît, 1977; Hollingsworth, 1975). Within a more

    complex motivaiional effects becorne obvi ous in latw stages because it takes the

    individual more t h e to master new task strategies (Locke & Latham, 1990).

    Therefore, short-tem experiments may not indicate the efEects of goal-setting on a

    complex task. One of the underlying dficulties encountered in this type of research

    is c m h g astudy in whicti control subjects are not setting goals (Locke, 1991; Locke,

    19%; Wemkg & Weigand, 1993, 1996). Feedback or knowledge of results is o h

    a m o ~ f à c t o r f o r spart participants and it is dificult to withkold this information,

    thus preveding participants to set goals in do-your-best conditions (Weinberg &

    Weigand, 1993, 1996).

    Sane araiors hime explored several mediaiors of goal-setting ai the same tirne

    such as effort and ability attributions (Wraith & Biddle, 1989) and effects of goal

    di ff~culty on intrinsic motivation (Anshei et al., 1992; Bar-Eli, Levy-Kolker,

    Tenenbaum & Weinberg, 1992; Weinberg et al., 1991) while others have simply

    e b n e d the main effect of setting specific goals (Barnen & Stanicek, 1979; Boyce,

    1990; Edwards, 1988; Hoilingsworth, 1975).

  • 42

    Goal S~ecificity

    C M specjficity refers to clear attainable standards thai designate the type and

    mount of effort recpired to attain them. These standards produce higher levels of

    performance than general intentions to do one's best (Locke & Latham, 1990).

    Specific performance goals serve to motivate the unrnotivated and to foster positive

    adtihuies towards specific tasks or activities (Bandura, 1986). Burton (1993) m e r

    points out that specificity will tend to reduce the variance in performance. However,

    to observe the difikences between goal-setters and non goal-setters is somewhai more

    dÏfkuit in the sport domain due to spontaneous goal-setting which tends to confound

    resuits (Locke & Latham, 1985).

    Of16 shWies deahgwith varied skilts and done with a variety of subjects, ten

    demonstrated the effectiveness of goal-setting compared to a control grooup (Bar-Eli

    et al., 1994; Bamett L Staniceck, 1979; Boyce, 1990; Edwards, 1988, Erbaugh &

    Bamett, 1986; Erffmeyer, 1987; Hall & Bym, 1988; Hall et ai., 1987; Tenenbaum

    et al., 1991; Weinberg, Bruya, Longino & Jackson, 1988). Three shidies involving

    juggling (Anshel et al., 1992; Bmeît, 1977; Hollingsworth, 1975) failed to show

    difkmces behveen goal-sethg groups and do-your-best groups. Since juggling may

    be considered a complex task, more time may have been required for goal-setting

    svbjects to outperform the do-your-best subjects at this task. It may also be assumed

    that, in such a ta&, dl participants were setting goals in view of mastering the task.

  • 43

    Fipthennore, the goal effectiveness cuve flattens as individuals approach the

    limits of their ability (Locke & Latham, 1990). This rnay explain the non-significant

    ces~lltr; of aie fo 11 owing studies. No dzerences were found between mil itary trainees

    in a two-month summer training camp who were tested on physical tasks with and

    without goal-setting (Bar-Eli et al., 1992). One might also suggest thai the do-your-

    besZg~oup of trainees rnay be setting goals for thernselves as social comparison was

    not avoided Weinberg et aI.(lW 1) conducted two experiments which did not support

    the e e c t s of goal-setting. The first experiment involved elementary school children

    in an 8 week sit-up program. The second exp eriment tested univers ity undergraduates

    on a3mimte maximal basketbal1 set shot test, consisting of 5 consecutive trials with

    rest in between. In the first experiment, children were given dieir goals in private

    derences andtoldnot to reved them. However, al1 goal groups practised and were

    tested in the same gymnasium at the sarne time. This means that the do-your-best and

    easy-goals condition were pmtising and beiog tested together. Thmefore, social

    comparison or cornpetition may have been present which may have acted as a

    powerful motivaior. Thus, al1 groups rnay have been setting goals. In the second

    experiment, Weinberg et a1.(1991) supplied a questiomaire to find out wheîfier

    subjects in do-your-best condition were setthg goals, and it was found thai 88% of

    these subjets were setting specific goals d e r Trial 1.

  • 44

    Goal Difficdty

    For research purposes, Locke (1991) emphasized that specifk goals must be

    diffiailt (only 10% of the subjects can reach them) because easy goais tend to lead to

    lowerpafbîmmceaiando-your-best and moderaie goals lead to the same level as do-

    your-best goal. This postulate has beea found to be tnie in induspial and

    psychological research (Locke & Latham, 1990). In the sport realm, this hypothesis

    has not been supported; only moderate goals (10 to 50% chance of success) resulted

    in a mean effect size significant nom zero (Kyllo & Landers, 1995). Burton (1993),

    Gould (1986) and Danish and Hale (1983) have contended that moderately difficult

    goals provide the best grounds for improvement in sport perforn~ance. Weinberg and

    Weigand (1996) still dispute that this not yet clear.

    Positive effects of difficuit specific goals were found in three maximal sit-up

    experirnerits with children (Weinberg et al., 1988), youth (Bar-Eli et al., 1994) and

    aMtF; (Tenenbaum et ai., 1991). Weinberg et al., (1988) found that the specific goal

    groups which were set to increase by 4% in the short-tenn and by 20% in the long-

    term outperformed the do-your-best group. Bar-Eli et a1.(1994) reported that the

    W-km (8% increase) plus long-tem (40% increase) goal group outperformed the

    long-fam (4Bh) goal group. Tenenbaum et al. (199 1) obtained similar results to Bar-

    Eli et ai. (1994) whereas the combined goal group (short-tem plus long-terni goal

    group) outperformed the short-term and long-tem only groups. The goal increases

  • 45

    were 8% for short term and 40% for long-tem. The results indicaîed thaî the short-

    term plus long-tem group increased by 29.43% ttnough 10 weeks, the short-terrn

    &coup irnprovedby 2 0 . m and the long-term group improved by 10.5% The do-your-

    best and control groups did not progress significantly.

    Two otbshidies involved instructional class-like situations and proved goal-

    setting eEectiveness according to goal dif£ïculty. Boyce (1990) exarriined riflery

    insûudon withuniversity -dents. A perfect triai test would result in 50 points. Each

    shooter aümpted 5 shots at a target containing six concentris rings; the outemost ring

    scored 5 points, the next ring 6 points and so forth to the imermost ring which was

    worth 10 points. High performance goals were established according to previous

    riflery classes, Nationai Rifle Association and research advocating high performance

    goals: 25 p o h or better on Trial 1, 30 points or better on Trial 3,40 points or better

    onTrial4 aid 40 points or better on Trial 5. The goal-setting group outperfonned the

    control group on Trials 2, 3, 4, 5 and retention Barnen and Stanicek (1979)

    mcceddly examined goal-setting within a 10-week archery class whereby the goal-

    setting group outperformed the no goal group. Goal dficulty was not specified as

    paticipanîs were askedto setthernselves numerical goals zcompanied with individual

    verbal goals. h o t h e r study examining hand-grip strength by Hall et al. (1987)

    reveaied a significant difference between a do-your-best condition and the two goal-

    setting conditions. However, subjects in the moderaiely dificult goal condition

  • 46

    (irq>rave by 40 s) and in the most difficuit goal condition (improve by 70 s) performed

    similarly. This demonstrated thaî it is still difficult to determine optimal levels of

    difficulty to stimulate physical performance.

    Locke and Latham (1985) have added the caveat thaî exceedingly difficult

    goals would lead to discouragement and poor performance and that unaitainable goals

    are abandoned when failure to meet them brings aversive consequemes. Burton

    (1984) has agreed with this statement when using goal-setting with collegiate

    cornpetitive swimers . However, four studies examining extrernely difficult goals in

    the sport realrn did not find aiiy dxerences arnong goal-setting groups nor

    d e t e r i d o n ofhtrinsic motivation within the extremely difi cult goals group (Anshel

    et al., 1992; Bar-Eli et ai., 1992; Weinberg, et al., 1988; Weinberg et ai., 1991). It

    must be noted that these experiments were short-tem and failure of attaining goals

    may not have produced aversive consequences. Furthemore, upon examining each

    individual subject, al1 four studies reported significant improvement with certain

    subjects, nul1 increases and decrsases in performance with others suggesting that,

    wimm agroup, there may be differences between individual reactions and one may not

    conclude that extremely difficult goals are performance enhancing for everyone. In

    long-term research with swimmers, Burton (1 984) demonstrated that goals must be

    challenging yet must be attainable, to promote self-efficacy and persistence. This

    indicates tbat individual Mèrences such as skill level should be of concern when using

  • goal-setting (Burton, 1993; Kyllo & Landers, 1995; Weinberg, 1994).

    22-4 Chiidren and GoaLS*

    Gd-seüingresearch has s h o w that children are quite recqtive to the use of

    goal-setiing. A group of very young children (5 to 7 years) mastered ajumping task

    over a -y rotating bar with the assistance of goal-setîing (Erbaugh & Barnett,

    1986). Erbaugh and Bametf (1986) also obsetved significant behaviour differences

    between the goal-setîing group and the non goal-setting groups. The children in the

    goal-setting condition appeared much more aware of task demands and displayed

    niiocimal effort and task persistence. They also used a self-evaluative counting strategy

    which was unnoticed within the do-your-best group. A group of ofder children

    between the ages o f 9 to 11 years dernonstrated significant improvement in their

    performances of a hockey flip shot task with the use of goal-setting (Edwards, 1988).

    Weinberg et aL (1988) aiso indicated significant performance differences for children

    seiîing goals in a maximal sit-up task.

    Goal dïfiicuity is directly iduenced by goal proximity. Proximal (short-tm)

    subgoals should be challenging but clearly attainable îhrough extra effort in the pursuit

    of a far more demanding long-tenn goai (Bandura & Schunk, 198 1; Burton, 1993;

    Danish & Hale, 1983; Gould, 1986; Locke & Latham, 1990; Weinberg, 1994). It is

    expected that perceived difficulty of the long-term goal will change as subgoals are

    reached (Bandura, 1986). Danish and Haie (1983) and Gould (1986) illustrate this

  • 48

    concept as clirnbing up a staircase to the door of success. Reaching each stair is like

    accornplishing a subgoal. As each subgoal is reached, self-efficacy is enhanced and

    the attainment of the ultimate final goal appears more and more accessible.

    Goal Pro-ty

    nie effectiveness of goal-setting depends on how far into the fitrire the goals

    are projected. Reaching proximal goals enhances self-efEcacy and prornotes

    persistence at a task (Bandura & Schnk, 198 1). When long-term goal-setting was

    used to produce self-directed change in r e k t o r y behaviour such as alcoholism and

    uleigtit rcduction, failure was more fi-equent (Bandura, 1986). Daily goals produced

    significantly better results (Bandura, 1986).

    Children(7to 10 y m of age) with very poor math skills were assigned to one

    of four conditions, in a shidy to promote the leming of rnathernaîics: (1) proximal

    goals, (2) distal goals, (3) no goal and (4) no treatment (Bandur& Schunk, 198 1).

    nie shildren in the proximal goal condition progressed more rapidly in self-directed

    leaming, achieved higher mastery of basic mathematical operations and developed

    geaimperceived self-efficacy and intrinsic interest in mathematics than their peers in

    the other conditions.

    In the sport and exercise setting, three of four studies revealed that the group

    invoIved in short-term plus long-terni goals achieved the greatest performance

  • 49

    improvements (Bar-Eli et al., 1994; Hall & Byrne, 1988; Tenenbaum et al., 1991).

    Bar-Eli a al. (1 994) estab lished the benefit of the combination of proximal and distal

    goal-setîing condition over distal only goal-seiting condition with behaviorally

    disordered youth in a one-minute maximal sit-up task over a 10 week period. In

    anottier experhenî, 54 co1Iege students sp ent three weeks in weight training and it was

    demonstrated that the short-term plus long-tenn goal-setiing groups achieved the best

    resulîs ( M l & Byrne, 1988). The final study with high school students in a 3-minute

    maximal sit-up task fiuther supported the assumption that the short-term plus long-

    term goal group wculd outperform the other goal groups such as short-term or long-

    term only goal groups (Tenenbaum et al., 1991). Furthemore, results ofKy110 and

    Landen' (199