52
14:ar30, 1 CBE—Life Sciences Education Redesigning a General Education Science Course to Promote Critical Thinking Matthew P. Rowe,* †‡ B. Marcus Gillespie, †§ Kevin R. Harris, || Steven D. Koether, Li-Jen Y. Shannon, # and Lori A. Rose* *Department of Biological Sciences, § Department of Geography & Geology, College of Sciences, and # Department of Computer Science, Sam Houston State University, Huntsville, TX 77340; || Center for Assessment & Improvement of Learning, Tennessee Tech University, Cookeville, TN 38505 Submitted February 20, 2015; Revised April 24, 2015; Accepted April 24, 2015 Monitoring Editor: Ross Nehm Recent studies question the effectiveness of a traditional university curriculum in helping students improve their critical thinking and scientific literacy. We developed an introductory, general educa- tion (gen ed) science course to overcome both deficiencies. The course, titled Foundations of Science, differs from most gen ed science offerings in that it is interdisciplinary; emphasizes the nature of science along with, rather than primarily, the findings of science; incorporates case studies, such as the vaccine-autism controversy; teaches the basics of argumentation and logical fallacies; contrasts science with pseudoscience; and addresses psychological factors that might otherwise lead students to reject scientific ideas they find uncomfortable. Using a pretest versus posttest design, we show that students who completed the experimental course significantly improved their critical-thinking skills and were more willing to engage scientific theories the general public finds controversial (e.g., evolution), while students who completed a traditional gen ed science course did not. Our results demonstrate that a gen ed science course emphasizing the process and application of science rather than just scientific facts can lead to improved critical thinking and scientific literacy. Article A primary goal of education in general, and higher educa- tion in particular, is to improve the critical-thinking skills of students (Facione et al., 1995; Van Gelder, 2005; Bok, 2006). Sadly, higher education appears insufficient to the task, with recent studies (Arum and Roksa, 2010; Arum et al., 2011; Pascarella et al., 2011) showing minimal gains in students’ critical-thinking and analytical skills during their under- graduate careers, reducing their employment potential upon Vol. 14, 1–12, Fall 2015 © 2015 M. P. Rowe, B. M. Gillespie, et al. CBE—Life Sciences Educa- tion © 2015 The American Society for Cell Biology. This article is distributed by The American Society for Cell Biology under license from the author(s). It is available to the public under an Attribu- tion–Noncommercial–Share Alike 3.0 Unported Creative Commons License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/3.0). “ASCB®”and “The American Society for Cell Biology®” are regis- tered trademarks of The American Society for Cell Biology. These authors contributed equally to this work. Present address: Department of Integrative Biology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824. Conflict of interest: Authors M.P.R., B.M.G., S.D.K., and L.A.R. were responsible for the development and evaluation of the instruction- al materials and assessments other than the Critical thinking As- sessment Test (CAT). K.R.H. is an employee in the Center for As- sessment & Improvement of Learning at Tennessee Technological University, a nonprofit entity that, with support from the National Science Foundation, developed, validated, and distributes the CAT on a fee-per-use basis. The authors will gladly provide any and all of the course materials, other than the CAT assessment tool, to in- structors interested in reviewing the materials for potential use in DOI:10.1187/cbe.15-02-0032 CBE Life Sci Educ September 2, 2015 14:ar30 their courses. For details regarding experimental analyses, results, and interpretations, contact M.P.R. For details regarding course development and structure, contact B.M.G. Address correspondence to: Matthew P. Rowe (rowemat1@msu .edu) or B. Marcus Gillespie ([email protected]). INTRODUCTION If we teach only the findings and products of science— no matter how useful and even inspiring they may be—without communicating its critical method, how can the average person possibly distinguish science from pseudoscience? Sagan, 1996, p. 21 by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from http://www.lifescied.org/content/suppl/2015/07/24/14.3.ar30.DC1.html Supplemental Material can be found at: by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015 http://www.lifescied.org/ Downloaded from

Redesigning a General Education Science Course to Promote Critical Thinking

  • Upload
    darosaf

  • View
    229

  • Download
    7

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Artigo

Citation preview

14:ar30, 1

CBE—Life Sciences Education

Redesigning a General Education Science Course to Promote Critical ThinkingMatthew P. Rowe,*†‡ B. Marcus Gillespie,†§ Kevin R. Harris,|| Steven D. Koether,¶ Li-Jen Y. Shannon,# and Lori A. Rose*

*Department of Biological Sciences, §Department of Geography & Geology, ¶College of Sciences, and #Department of Computer Science, Sam Houston State University, Huntsville, TX 77340; ||Center for Assessment & Improvement of Learning, Tennessee Tech University, Cookeville, TN 38505

Submitted February 20, 2015; Revised April 24, 2015; Accepted April 24, 2015Monitoring Editor: Ross Nehm

Recent studies question the effectiveness of a traditional university curriculum in helping students improve their critical thinking and scientific literacy. We developed an introductory, general educa-tion (gen ed) science course to overcome both deficiencies. The course, titled Foundations of Science, differs from most gen ed science offerings in that it is interdisciplinary; emphasizes the nature of science along with, rather than primarily, the findings of science; incorporates case studies, such as the vaccine-autism controversy; teaches the basics of argumentation and logical fallacies; contrasts science with pseudoscience; and addresses psychological factors that might otherwise lead students to reject scientific ideas they find uncomfortable. Using a pretest versus posttest design, we show that students who completed the experimental course significantly improved their critical-thinking skills and were more willing to engage scientific theories the general public finds controversial (e.g., evolution), while students who completed a traditional gen ed science course did not. Our results demonstrate that a gen ed science course emphasizing the process and application of science rather than just scientific facts can lead to improved critical thinking and scientific literacy.

Article

A primary goal of education in general, and higher educa-tion in particular, is to improve the critical-thinking skills of students (Facione et al., 1995; Van Gelder, 2005; Bok, 2006). Sadly, higher education appears insufficient to the task, with recent studies (Arum and Roksa, 2010; Arum et  al., 2011; Pascarella et  al., 2011) showing minimal gains in students’ critical-thinking and analytical skills during their under-graduate careers, reducing their employment potential upon

Vol. 14, 1–12, Fall 2015

© 2015 M. P. Rowe, B. M. Gillespie, et al. CBE—Life Sciences Educa-tion © 2015 The American Society for Cell Biology. This article is distributed by The American Society for Cell Biology under license from the author(s). It is available to the public under an Attribu-tion–Noncommercial–Share Alike 3.0 Unported Creative Commons License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/3.0).“ASCB®”and “The American Society for Cell Biology®” are regis- tered trademarks of The American Society for Cell Biology.

†These authors contributed equally to this work.‡Present address: Department of Integrative Biology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824.Conflict of interest: Authors M.P.R., B.M.G., S.D.K., and L.A.R. were responsible for the development and evaluation of the instruction-al materials and assessments other than the Critical thinking As-sessment Test (CAT). K.R.H. is an employee in the Center for As-sessment & Improvement of Learning at Tennessee Technological University, a nonprofit entity that, with support from the National Science Foundation, developed, validated, and distributes the CAT on a fee-per-use basis. The authors will gladly provide any and all of the course materials, other than the CAT assessment tool, to in-structors interested in reviewing the materials for potential use in

DOI:10.1187/cbe.15-02-0032CBE Life Sci Educ September 2, 2015 14:ar30

their courses. For details regarding experimental analyses, results, and interpretations, contact M.P.R. For details regarding course development and structure, contact B.M.G.Address correspondence to: Matthew P. Rowe (rowemat1@msu .edu) or B. Marcus Gillespie ([email protected]).

INTRODUCTION

If we teach only the findings and products of science—no matter how useful and even inspiring they may be—without communicating its critical method, how can the average person possibly distinguish science from pseudoscience?

Sagan, 1996, p. 21

by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from

http://www.lifescied.org/content/suppl/2015/07/24/14.3.ar30.DC1.htmlSupplemental Material can be found at:

by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from

M. P. Rowe, B. M. Gillespie, et al.

14:ar30, 2 CBE—Life Sciences Education

graduation (Arum and Roksa, 2014). Science courses, with their focus on evidence and logic, should provide exemplary exposure to and training in critical thinking. Here, too, we appear to be failing, both at the level of individual science classes and programmatically in the science core, given the ineffectiveness of these courses to either improve students’ scientific knowledge or mitigate their acceptance of pseudo-scientific claims (Walker et al., 2002; Johnson and Pigliucci, 2004; Impey et al., 2011; Carmel and Yezierski, 2013).

The inadequacy of standard approaches to teaching sci-ence is demonstrated by the fact that 93% of American adults and 78% of those with college degrees are scientifically illit-erate (Hazen, 2002); that is, they do not understand science as an empirically based method of inquiry, they lack knowl-edge of fundamental scientific facts, and they are unable to understand the science-related material published in a news-paper such as the Washington Post (Miller, 1998, 2012). Such deficiencies extend to science majors as well. For example, a study of 170 undergraduates at the University of Tennes-see found that, while science majors knew more science facts than non–science majors, there were no differences between the two groups in their conceptual understanding of sci-ence or their belief in pseudoscience (Johnson and Pigliucci, 2004). This poor understanding of science adversely affects the ability of individuals to make informed decisions about science-related issues, including well-established theories like the big bang, which is rejected by nearly two-thirds of Americans (National Science Foundation, 2014). The woe-ful lack of scientific literacy similarly provides insight into the public (though not scientific) controversies surrounding such issues as evolution (Miller et al., 2006), global climate change (Morrison, 2011; Reardon, 2011), and the safety of childhood immunizations (Mnookin, 2011; Offit, 2011). In short, there appears to be a gap between a fundamental goal of science education, to produce scientifically literate citi-zens, and the results of the pedagogical approaches intended to meet this goal. Particularly troublesome is the ripple effect of inadequate science education at the university level, lead-ing to poor teacher preparation and threatening the quality of science instruction in our public schools (Eve and Dunn, 1990; Rutledge and Warden, 2000).

Commonly identified causes of the impotency of science courses, especially the introductory courses taken by the majority of college students, are their tendency to focus on scientific “facts” rather than on the nature of science (John-son and Pigliucci, 2004; Alberts, 2005), often reinforced by exams that reward memorization over higher-order thinking (Alberts, 2009; Momsen et al., 2010); the reluctance to directly engage students’ misconceptions (Alters and Nelson, 2002; Nelson, 2008; Alberts, 2005; Verhey, 2005); the failure to con-nect “science as a way of knowing” with decisions faced by students in their daily lives (Kuhn, 1993; Walker et al., 2002); and the resistance of faculty trained in more innovative ped-agogical approaches to actually employ them (Ebert-May et  al., 2011). The traditional approach to science education not only fosters scientific illiteracy, but also alienates many students from science (Seymour and Hewitt, 1997; Ede, 2000; Johnson, 2007) and, ultimately, jeopardizes America’s global competitiveness (National Academy of Sciences, National Academy of Engineering, and Institute of Medicine, 2010). While methods emphasizing active learning demonstrate significant pedagogical improvements for students majoring

in the sciences (Freeman et al., 2014), ∼85% of the 1.8 million students graduating from college annually in the United States are not science majors (Snyder and Dillow, 2013). Our goal, therefore, was to develop and test an intervention targeting this larger, frequently overlooked, yet extremely important audience. But what would scientific literacy comprise for students completing only one or two science courses during their college careers? What tools could we use to measure said literacy? And how might we best, in a single course or two, help our students achieve it?

Our answer to these questions was an integrative, general education (gen ed) science course titled Foundations of Sci-ence (FoS), selected as the centerpiece of the Quality Enhance-ment Plan for reaffirmation at Sam Houston State University (SHSU; Sam Houston State University, 2009). Per Sagan’s (1996) admonition, the FoS course focuses as much on the nature of science as on its facts. We intentionally sought to demystify the process of science by selecting examples, such as the vaccine-autism controversy, that not only held the stu-dents’ attention but also, and as importantly, helped demon-strate the utility of “evidentiary thinking” in their daily lives. A brief list of the central tenets of the course is provided be-low; more detail is available in the “Expanded Course Ratio-nale and Structure” in our Supplemental Material.

Critical ThinkingOur central hypothesis was that critical thinking—defined as the ability to draw reasonable conclusions based on evi-dence, logic, and intellectual honesty—is inherent to scientif-ic reasoning (Facione, 1990, 2015; American Association for the Advancement of Science [AAAS], 1993; Bernstein et al., 2006) and is therefore an essential aspect of scientific literacy. Scientific literacy, then, can best be achieved by offering an alternative type of integrated science course that focuses on these foundations rather than on the traditional “memorize the facts” approach to science education. A simple, operation-al approach to critical thinking is provided by Bernstein et al. (2006) via a set of questions one should ask when presented with a claim (e.g., vaccines cause autism, global warming is a hoax, there are no transitional fossils). 1) What am I be-ing asked to accept? 2) What evidence supports the claim? 3) Are there alternative explanations/hypotheses? And, fi-nally, 4) what evidence supports the alternatives? The most likely explanation is the one that is best supported. Evidence matters, but only when all of the evidence for and against each of the competing hypotheses has been examined—fully, thoughtfully, and honestly. Sounds like science, doesn’t it? But how can we get science-phobic college students to use it? Perhaps by focusing on topics the non–science student finds interesting, including astrology, homeopathy, Bigfoot, and even intelligent design. But aren’t these ideas just pseudosci-entific nonsense? Of course, but students need to understand why they are pseudo rather than real science, and critical thinking/scientific literacy is the key. This is the approach adopted by Theodore Schick and Lewis Vaughn (2014) in How to Think about Weird Things: Critical Thinking for a New Age, one of the two main texts we adopt in the course.

This text and the course also help students identify and analyze the validity and soundness of arguments. We in-clude a discussion of common heuristics and several logical fallacies, some examples being correlation proves causation,

by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from

Critical Thinking and Gen Ed Science

Vol. 14, Fall 2015 14:ar30, 3

appeal to the masses, and ad hominem attacks. An under-standing and awareness of strong versus weak arguments, and the informal fallacies used to surreptitiously circumvent the former, are essential to critical thinking and to the evalu-ation of claims—whether scientific or pseudoscientific.

Integrating Content with ProcessWhile there has been a clarion call for teachers to focus more on scientific process and less on scientific facts (Rutherford and Ahlgren, 1990; AAAS, 1993, 2010), content still matters. Therefore, in addition to the critical-thinking text by Schick and Vaughn, we also use an integrated science textbook (e.g., Hewitt et al., 2013; Trefil and Hazen, 2013) as our second text, typically a custom printing that includes only those chapters whose content we cover in the course. We are fortunate that our course includes both “lecture” and “lab” components, providing multiple, weekly opportunities for active learning. We employ, as a cornerstone of our approach, case studies we have built specifically for the FoS course. Cases, we have found, permit us to teach content and process at the same time, in a manner that engages the non–science student. One of our cases, for example, examines the purported connec-tion between vaccines and autism (Rowe, 2010). Working in small groups, students examine the data from Andrew Wakefield et al.’s (1998) paper, the proverbial match that lit the current firestorm of antivaccine hysteria (Mnookin, 2011; Offit, 2011). After dissecting Wakefield’s data and his conclu-sions, students are tasked with designing a better study. In so doing, they learn a great deal about sample size, replication, double-blind studies, and scientific honesty, that is, the pro-cedural underpinnings of good science. But the students also learn about antibodies, antigens, herd immunity, and autism spectrum disorders, that is, the findings of science. Similar-ly, in a case in which students use the science of ecology to go “hunting” for the Loch Ness monster (Rowe, 2015), they must learn and then apply scientific “findings” ranging from the second law of thermodynamics to minimum viable popu-lation sizes to postglacial rebound. A large part of the success we witness in our experimental course is due, we believe, to this integration of scientific facts with scientific process.

Addressing Cognitive BarriersAn emphasis on evidentiary thinking combined with an in-tegration of content and process will achieve little if students are unable or unwilling to objectively evaluate a claim, hy-pothesis, or theory. Cognitive barriers can stand in the way of rational decision making (Posner et al., 1982; Sinatra et al., 2008). We designed the FoS course to overcome two such bar-riers. One hurdle is peoples’ personal experiences, which, for many, trump critical thinking (Chabris and Simons, 2010). If something feels real, looks real, tastes real, if we saw it, ex-perienced it, then it must be true. Zinc is not effective against the common cold? Why, then, did my headache disappear when I used zinc-infused cough drops? Vaccines do not cause autism? What else could explain why my son stopped walking two days after his MMR shot? To help students un-derstand the limitations of anecdotal evidence, including their own personal experiences, we guide them through an exploration of the science of perception and memory. We use illusions to show how our brain unconsciously takes short-cuts that can lead to misperceptions. And we employ simple

exercises to demonstrate the malleability and fallibility of memories. Critical thinking requires we recognize that our perceptions and our memories may be flawed.

The second barrier starts once perceptions and memories have solidified into an opinion. Opinions, once formed, resist change; the more important the belief, the more stubbornly we hang onto it, even in the light of contradictory evidence (Tavris and Aronson, 2007). An honest evaluation of compet-ing explanations requires that students understand cognitive dissonance and its servant twins, expectation bias and con-firmation bias. Facts do not matter to someone who does not want to hear them, and evidence is easily discounted when examined with prejudice. Indeed, simply throwing facts at biased conclusions may cause further retrenchment as, for example, was demonstrated in a recent study (Nyhan et al., 2014) of the rebellion against childhood immunizations. Re-sults of the study, which surveyed 1759 parents, are discour-aging, in that an intervention presenting the overwhelming evidence that vaccines do not cause autism made parents less likely to vaccinate, not more (Nyhan et al., 2014).

Social judgment theory (SJT) offers an explanation of Nyhan et al.’s (2014) counterintuitive results. SJT postulates there is a range, a latitude, of ideas similar to a person’s cur-rent position he or she might be willing to consider as being true if presented with information that supports the idea. However, if the idea is too different from the person’s initial belief, if it lies outside his or her latitude of acceptance, it will be rejected (Erwin, 2014). Furthermore, the more involved a person is with a view, the wider the latitudes of rejection and the narrower the latitudes of acceptance (Benoit, n.d.). If we want students to understand and accept the big bang theory and the theory of evolution, ideas many find uncomfortable, we cannot simply present the overwhelming evidence in favor of these ideas, we must also accommodate and over-come the dissonance these explanations engender. SJT was, therefore, a central, guiding tenet in the topical organization of the course, briefly outlined below. Topics in the first third of the course are, we believe, the most unusual, so we focus on those here. Additional details of the topics included in the course, the reasons we included them, and the materials we used to teach them can be found in the “Expanded Course Rationale and Structure” in our Supplemental Material, along with a copy of an example course syllabus.

Topical OrganizationWe begin the course by discussing the witch hunts of the 14th through 18th centuries. By some accounts, more than half a million innocent victims were horribly tortured and then killed under the mistaken belief they were the cause of miscarriages, crop failures, and storms, that is, calamities and misfortunes we now know have underlying natural, not supernatural, causes (Sagan, 1996; Cawthorne, 2004). A com-mon question we frequently pose to the students is “What is the harm in believing in something that is not true?” The stu-dents, having no personal stake in the fates of these historical victims, easily grasp the importance of evidence, skepticism, and the need for multiple working hypotheses when seeking causal explanations.

Lest the students think witch hunts are a thing of the past, we segue to a discussion of modern witch hunts, with a fo-cus on the satanic ritual abuse mass hysteria of the 1980s

by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from

M. P. Rowe, B. M. Gillespie, et al.

14:ar30, 4 CBE—Life Sciences Education

as international. The average age of the institution’s under-graduates is 22 yr. Approximately half of the students are first-generation college students. Because the FoS course is an open-enrollment, gen ed core science course with no prerequisites, the demographic makeup of the course likely represents that of the university. We compared the effective-ness of the FoS course with several traditional introducto-ry science courses for nonmajors taught at the university, courses which, as gen ed survey courses, should also reflect the demographics of the university as a whole.

Experimental ApproachWe used a pretest versus posttest design to assess the effective-ness of the FoS. Our treatment group consisted of several sec-tions of the experimental course taught over multiple semes-ters (Table 1). Our comparison group was composed of several different, traditional gen ed science courses, also sampled over multiple semesters, offered by the departments of chemistry, physics, biology, and geography/geology (Table 1). During the study period of Fall semester 2008 through Fall semester 2012, the average class size in each section of our experimental FoS course was 51.75 (±1.17 SE) students; the lab/discussion sections that accompanied the FoS course were capped at 30 students/section. Over the same period, average class size in the traditional courses that formed our comparison group was 51.00 (± 6.07 SE) students. All of the comparison courses also included a lab, similarly capped at 30 students.

Assessment ToolsTo examine changes in student analytical skills, we used the Critical thinking Assessment Test (CAT) developed by the Center for Assessment & Improvement of Learning at Ten-nessee Tech University (TTU; Stein and Haynes, 2011; Stein et al., 2007). The CAT exam assesses several aspects of criti-cal thinking, including the evaluation and interpretation of information, problem solving, creative thinking, and com-munication. Student skills encompassed by the CAT include their ability to interpret graphs and equations, solve basic math problems, identify logical fallacies, recognize when ad-ditional information might be needed to evaluate a claim, understand the limitations of correlational data, and devel-op alternative explanations for a claim. These aspects of the CAT exam conform to accepted constructs that characterize critical thinking (Facione, 1990, 2015), and align well with those taught in the FOS course, which specifically empha-sizes the ability to draw appropriate conclusions based on multiple working hypotheses, evidence, and reason. The CAT instrument consists of 15 questions, most of which are short-answer responses. More than 200 institutions of higher education are now using the CAT for assessing programmat-ic changes designed to improve critical thinking among col-lege students, permitting us to compare our results not only with traditional gen ed science courses being taught at our own institution but also with national norms.

To examine changes in the attitudes of students about science in general, and controversial scientific theories in particular, we used the Measure of Acceptance of the The-ory of Evolution (MATE), a 20-question, Likert-scale survey (Rutledge and Warden, 1999; Rutledge and Sadler, 2007) that has been widely used for assessing the acceptance of evolutionary theory among high school teachers and college

and 1990s (Nathan and Snedeker, 2001). As with the earlier hunts, hundreds of people were accused, convicted, and sent to jail, even though there was little or no empirical evidence to support the allegations (Lanning, 1992). Here, too, the stu-dents, with little emotional investment and, thus, little disso-nance, draw the reasonable conclusion that scientific literacy, evidence, and critical thinking are good things, because they prevent harm.

We then discuss the nature of science as a systematic, objective, and reliable means of evaluating testable claims. Mindful of SJT, we do not dismiss other ways of knowing (e.g., intuition, spirituality) but highlight the strengths and successes of the scientific approach, including its unique reliance on evidence, skepticism, logic, multiple working hypotheses, and Occam’s razor, that is, the foundations of science. We stress the importance of self-correction, a char-acteristic unique to science yet frequently misunderstood by students as a weakness. And, using examples, we introduce students to the pernicious effects of dissonance, dishonesty, and bias as impediments to understanding.

The next section of the course deals with the limits to per-ception and memory mentioned earlier, topics critical for un-derstanding why anecdotal evidence, eyewitness accounts, and even personal experiences are insufficient for accepting a claim. By this point in the course, students are beginning to understand Richard Feynman’s famous quote “The first principle is that you must not fool yourself and you are the easiest person to fool” (Feynman and Leighton, 1985, p. 343). If their own perceptions and memories can be faulty, might not some of their opinions be too?

The remainder of the course covers content more typical of an integrative science course, including but not limited to cosmology, geology, cell biology, and ecology, with some-what atypical side trips to explore the paranormal and in-vestigate alternative medical therapies. But even here, we attempt to capture the nonmajors’ attention by having them analyze claims they find engaging; they learn a lot about plate tectonics, for example, by investigating the claim that a continent, Atlantis in this case, can disappear.

The theory of evolution is, by design, reserved for the last week of the course. By then, most students recognize the importance of evidence and logic and critical thinking. They have sharpened the tools in their “baloney detection kit” (Sagan, 1996) and understand that it is not just snake-oil salesmen who market baloney but that we are pretty good at selling it to ourselves. With latitudes of acceptance broad-ened, they are ready to tackle the scientific theory many find the most discomforting of all.

METHODS

Institutional SettingOur experiment was conducted at SHSU, a public, doctoral research university located in Huntsville, Texas. Founded in 1879, it offers 138 bachelor’s, master’s, and doctoral degrees. With the exception of an underrepresentation of Asians, the ethnic composition of SHSU broadly matches that of the United States, with 57% of its 19,000-plus students self-reporting as Caucasian/white, 18% as Hispanic, 17% as African American/black, 1% as Asian, and 4% as either multiracial or other ethnicities. Two percent are classified

by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from

Critical Thinking and Gen Ed Science

Vol. 14, Fall 2015 14:ar30, 5

responses on the MATE, and they were informed that their answers would not be graded. However, students were still able to earn rewards equivalent to those of students taking the CAT based on their performance on the locally devel-oped assessment tool.

Assessment Reliability and ValidityArguments regarding the effectiveness of the FoS course de-mand both reliability and validity. While these concepts are frequently ignored (Campbell and Nehm, 2013), researchers who address the issues of reliability and validity often mis-take them as required properties of one’s assessment tools rather than, correctly, as characteristics of the interpretations we make from the tools’ results (Cronbach and Meehl, 1955; Messick, 1995; Brown, 2005; Campbell and Nehm, 2013). The reliability and validity of interpretations based on the CAT have strong evidentiary support (Tennessee Technological University, 2010; Stein and Haynes, 2011; Stein et  al., 2007, 2010).

Interpretations based on the MATE also have demon-strated reliability and validity, at least for certain popula-tions (Rutledge and Warden, 1999; Rutledge and Sadler, 2007). A recent study (Wagler and Wagler, 2013), however, found the MATE lacked construct validity for Hispanic ele-mentary education majors and questioned the utility of the tool for assessing student acceptance of evolutionary theory. Our results do not support this criticism, an argument we present more fully in our Discussion.

students (Moore and Cotner, 2009; Nadelson and Souther-land, 2010; Peker et al., 2010; Kim and Nehm, 2011; Abraham et al., 2012).

Beginning in the Fall of 2010, approximately half the stu-dents in each of the experimental and comparison courses were assessed pre- and postcourse using the CAT, the other half with the MATE. The pretests were administered during the second week of the term, while the posttests were given in the penultimate week of classes. Instructors teaching both the FoS and the traditional courses agreed on identical in-centives each semester, with the exception of Fall 2010: as no credit (baseline data before creation of the FoS) or as extra credit/part of the course grade thereafter (Table 1). Details regarding how the incentive was applied are provided in the example course syllabus in our Supplemental Materials.

All CAT exams were graded using a modified rubric that enabled the exams to be graded quickly. These scores were used to assign performance points to the students. A subset of all the CAT exams from each course was randomly se-lected for formal grading using the rubric developed by the Center for Assessment & Improvement of Learning at TTU. Based on the grading procedures established by the center, graders were blind to the identity of the student, whether an exam was a pretest or posttest, and the treatment group. Results of the formal grading are reported herein.

The MATE was coupled with a locally developed assess-ment not presented in this publication. Because the responses on the MATE assessment represent personal opinions and attitudes, no incentives were provided to students for their

Table 1. CAT scores in traditional versus experimental gen ed science courses, by semester

Course Treatmenta Term N Designb Incentivec

CAT pre score

CAT post score tactual (df)

Pre–post p value

Effect size

1 Introductory geographyd T Fall 2008 36 Post only None 15.002 Introductory geologye T Fall 2008 40 Post only None 15.053 Introductory biologyf T Spring 2009 37 Post only None 14.664 Introductory geographyd T Spring 2009 39 Post only None 14.915 Introductory environmental

studiesgT Fall 2010 10 Pre and post EC 17.07 16.90 t(9) = 0.232 ns

6 Introductory physicsh T Fall 2011 16 Pre and post EC 13.94 14.63 t(15) = −0.696 ns7 Introductory chemistryi T Fall 2011 25 Pre and post EC 13.16 13.68 t(24) = −0.586 ns8 FoSj E Fall 2009 53 Pre and post PoC 16.03 19.77 t(52) = −5.385 <0.001 +0.719 FoSj E Spring 2010 53 Pre and post PoC 17.95 22.43 t(52) = −5.872 <0.001 +0.7610 FoSj E Fall 2010 47 Pre and post PoC 15.52 19.98 t(46) = −4.848 <0.001 +0.3611 FoSj E Spring 2011 69 Pre and post PoC 14.95 19.60 t(68) = −8.999 <0.001 +0.8412 FoSj E Fall 2011 25 Pre and post EC 13.41 17.75 t(24) = −3.984 <0.001 +0.8513 FoSj E Fall 2012 25 Pre and post EC 12.25 16.16 t(24) = −3.310 <0.01 +0.83

aT = traditional (i.e., comparison) gen ed science course for nonmajors; E = experimental FoS course.bBefore the introduction of the FoS course in the Fall of 2009, the CAT assessment was conducted only once, at the end of the semester.cEC = extra credit; PoC = part of the course grade.dGEOG 1301: Weather and Climate.eGEOL 1304: Historical Geology.fBIOL 1308: Contemporary Biology.gBIOL 1301: Environmental Science.hPHYS 1305: Fundamentals of Physics.iCHEM 1306: Inorganic and Environmental Chemistry.jCross-listed as both BIOL 1436 and GEOG 1436: Foundations of Science.

by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from

M. P. Rowe, B. M. Gillespie, et al.

14:ar30, 6 CBE—Life Sciences Education

FoS course. Similarly, we have posttest MATE scores from 1250 undergraduates, with 417 representing the three tra-ditional courses and 833 from the five semesters of the FoS course.

RESULTS

Critical ThinkingFoS Experiment versus Traditional Gen Ed Science Courses. Our results are robust and consistent; quite simply, students who complete the experimental FoS course show significant improvement in their critical-thinking skills, as measured by the CAT, while students who complete a tra-ditional gen ed science course do not. In no semester, for example, did students completing a traditional course show improvement in their critical-thinking scores (all p values > 0.49; Table 1), while students completing the experimental course showed highly significant improvement each semes-ter (all p values < 0.01, Cohen’s d typically > 0.70; Table 1). An analysis of pooled end-of-course (posttest only) CAT scores for all six semesters of the FoS course (Table 1, rows 8–13) versus the pooled posttest CAT scores for all six tradition-al gen ed science courses (Table 1, rows 1–7) reinforce this finding; students completing the FoS course scored signifi-cantly higher (19.76 ± 0.35) than did students completing a traditional (14.83 ± 0.37) introductory science course for non-majors (t(473) = 4.93, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.89; Figure 1A). A comparison of our pooled pre- versus posttest CAT scores for all six semesters of the FoS course (Table 1, rows 8–13) versus the pooled CAT scores for the three different gen ed science courses (introductory environmental studies, intro-ductory physics, and introductory chemistry) for which we had pre- and postcourse CAT test scores (Table 1, rows 5–7) show similar results. Students who completed the FoS course showed highly significant improvement in critical thinking (pretest = 15.45 ± 0.34, posttest = 19.76 ± 35; t(271) = 13.43, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.76), while there was no change in the critical thinking scores for students completing a traditional course (pretest = 14.17 ± 0.64, posttest = 14.61 ± 0.72; t(50) = 0.80, p = 0.43; Figure 1B).

The slightly higher pretest CAT scores for students in the experimental course relative to students taking a traditional course (15.45 vs. 14.61, respectively, Figure 1B) might suggest the significant pre versus post improvement in the former represents a cohort rather than a treatment effect; that is, stu-dents selecting an experimental course like FoS may possess better critical-thinking skills to begin with, generating more improvement over the course of a semester regardless of the science course. To assess this, we ran an ANCOVA on the postcourse CAT scores using each student’s precourse CAT score as a covariate. Results adjusting for each student’s en-try-level critical-thinking ability still showed a highly signif-icant effect of our experimental treatment (Figure 1C). That is, students who complete the FoS course show significantly better postcourse CAT scores than their peers who complete a traditional course, even when differences in students’ pre-course critical-thinking abilities are taken into account (mean adjusted postcourse critical-thinking score in the FoS course experimental course = 19.64 ± 0.65, mean adjusted postcourse critical-thinking score in traditional courses = 15.26 ± 0.28; F(1, 320) = 38.29, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.339).

Statistical AnalysesPretest versus posttest changes in student scores on the CAT were analyzed using a matched-pairs t test. Personal identi-fiers were not available in our MATE assessments, prevent-ing the use of a matched-pairs t test; we therefore used a less powerful independent-samples t test when analyzing the MATE results. Assessments of end-of-semester scores in our experimental course (the FoS) versus those in comparison courses (traditional gen ed science courses) were also made using t tests for independent samples, as were analyses of our FoS results versus the national norms available from the Center for Assessment & Improvement of Learning at TTU. The sample data in all tests were examined for violations of the parametric assumptions of normality and variance equality. Where needed, t tests assuming unequal sample variances were applied, while data violating the assumption of normality were log-transformed. In the few cases in which transformations failed to generate a normal distribution, we reduced our α value from 0.05 to 0.025 (Keppel, 1982). An analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) compared the postcourse CAT score for the FoS course with traditional courses while accounting for a student’s entering ability by using his or her precourse CAT score as the covariate. The ANCOVA assumptions of regression-slope homogeneity and treat-ment-covariate independence were met. As a further aid to understanding the strength of our results (Maher et al., 2013), we also report our effect sizes (Cohen’s d). Results presented in the text are mean ± 1 SE.

Sample SizesCAT. We have CAT results for eight semesters (Table 1), be-ginning in the Fall of 2008 and ending in the Fall of 2012 (the CAT assessment tool was not used in the Spring of 2012). A total of 475 SHSU undergraduate students have been assessed via the CAT; 203 students representing our com-parison group from six different traditional gen ed science courses (with one course, introductory geography, being as-sessed twice); and 272 students representing our experimen-tal treatment consisting of six different semesters of our FoS course. During the first two semesters of this experiment, we administered the CAT once at the end of the semester, and only in our traditional gen ed science courses, restricting us to a “postcourse” comparison on the full data set. Begin-ning with the first offering of our experimental course in the Fall of 2009, we administered the CAT both at the beginning and again at the end of the semester to three different tradi-tional gen ed science courses and six semesters of the FoS course, permitting us to use a more powerful “pre- versus postcourse” evaluation comparing the effectiveness of our experimental FoS with traditional gen ed science courses. We also compared the CAT performance of both treatment groups with the national norms for students attending 4-yr colleges and universities, a database of nearly 39,300 stu-dents available from the Center for Assessment & Improve-ment of Learning at TTU.

MATE. We have MATE results for five semesters, beginning in the Fall of 2010 and ending in the Fall of 2012. We have pretest MATE scores from 1443 undergraduate students; 561 from three different traditional gen ed science courses and 882 representing five different semesters of our experimental

by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from

Critical Thinking and Gen Ed Science

Vol. 14, Fall 2015 14:ar30, 7

they were freshmen or sophomores (i.e., lower-division students), and for 106 students who completed the course when they were juniors or seniors (i.e., upper-division stu-dents). Lower-division students enrolling in the FoS course have significantly higher pretest CAT scores (14.80 ± 0.40) than do lower-division students nationally (13.66 ± 0.05, t(165) = 2.827, p < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.22) and highly sig-nificantly better CAT scores (19.54 ± 0.41, t(165) = 14.305, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.13) in their posttest CAT at the end of the semester. Indeed, the average posttest CAT score for lower-division FoS students is comparable to the national mean (19.04 ± 0.05) for upper-division (junior/senior) stu-dents (t165 = 1.063, p = 0.289; Figure 2A).

The results for our upper-division students are quite different. Pretest and posttest CAT scores of upper-di-vision FoS students (again pooled over all six semesters, rows 8–13 in Table 1) compared with national norms show that upper-division FoS students have pretest CAT scores (16.48 ± 0.60) significantly below the national aver-age (19.04 ± 0.05) for juniors and seniors (t(105) = −4.287, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = −0.42); this deficit is erased, however,

Lower- versus Upper-Division Students and Comparison with National Norms. Analyzing our results by class stand-ing not only presents a more detailed picture of where our intervention might be most effective but also permits a com-parison with national norms. We have pre- and posttest CAT scores for 166 students who completed the FoS course when

Figure 1. Students who complete the experimental FoS course show significant improvement in their critical-thinking scores, as mea-sured by the CAT, while students who complete a traditional gen ed science course do not. Histograms show means + 1 SE. (A) Pooled end-of-course (posttest) CAT scores for all six semesters of the FoS course (Table 1, rows 8–13) vs. the pooled posttest CAT scores for all six traditional gen ed science courses (Table 1, rows 1–7). (B) Pooled pre- vs. posttest CAT scores for all six semesters of the FoS course (Table 1, rows 8–13) vs. the pooled CAT scores for the three different gen ed science courses (introductory environmental studies, intro-ductory physics, and introductory chemistry) for which we had pre- and postcourse CAT test scores (Table 1, rows 5–7). (C) Posttest CAT scores adjusted by pretest CAT scores for the same data set used in B.

Figure 2. Non–science students selecting to enroll in one of their gen ed science courses as entry-level freshmen or sophomores may represent a different subset of students than those who delay tak-ing such core courses until they are juniors or seniors, but both co-horts show highly significant improvement in their critical-thinking ability after completing the FoS course. Histograms show means + 1 SE. (A) Pretest and posttest CAT scores of lower-division (LD; i.e., freshman/sophomore) FoS students (pooled over all six semesters, rows 8–13 in Table 1) compared with national norms. (B) Pretest and posttest CAT scores of upper-division (UD; i.e., junior/senior) FoS students (again pooled over all six semesters, rows 8–13 in Table 1) compared with national norms.

by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from

M. P. Rowe, B. M. Gillespie, et al.

14:ar30, 8 CBE—Life Sciences Education

of evolution (pretest = 66.17 ± 0.45, posttest = 75.45 ± 0.49; t(1686.15) = 13.93, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.67), while there was no change in the acceptance of evolution for students com-pleting a traditional course (pretest = 65.27 ± 0.56, posttest = 64.91 ± 0.71; t(976) = 0.40, p = 0.69; Figure 3).

DISCUSSION

Critical ThinkingOur results demonstrate that an introductory, gen ed science course for nonmajors, a course focusing on the nature of science rather than just its facts, can lead to highly signifi-cant improvements, with large effect sizes, in the ability of college students to think critically. Most college courses do not significantly improve CAT performance in a pre/post design; substantive gains are typically observed only at the program/institutional level (Center for Assessment & Im-provement of Learning, TTU, unpublished data). Moreover, results from more than 200 institutions using the CAT show the average improvement in critical thinking observed over 4 yr of a typical undergraduate curriculum is 26% (Harris et  al., 2014); students who successfully completed the FoS course improved their CAT scores by almost 28% (15.45 vs. 19.76; Figure 1B). In short, students who complete a single-semester FoS course demonstrate levels of improve-ment in their critical-thinking skills typically requiring mul-tiple years of college experience, demonstrating that it is possible to teach higher-order thinking skills to nonmajors in a single science course they are required to take, many begrudgingly.

A finer-grained analysis of our results further illustrates the need to rethink how we are teaching our gen ed science courses. The pretest CAT score for our lower-division stu-dents, pooled over all six semesters, was significantly higher than the national average for this age group (Figure 2A). By the end of the semester, our lower-division students’ critical-thinking scores moved well beyond the national norm for freshmen/sophomores and were comparable to

Figure 3. Students who complete the experimental FoS course show a significant increase in their acceptance of evolution, as mea-sured by the MATE, while students who complete a traditional gen ed science course do not. Pooled pre- vs. posttest MATE scores for five semesters of the FoS course (Table 2, rows 4–8) vs. the pooled MATE scores for the three different gen ed science courses (intro-ductory environmental studies, introductory physics, and introduc-tory chemistry) for which we had pre- and postcourse MATE scores (Table 2, rows 1–3). Histograms show means + 1 SE.

Table 2. MATE scores in traditional versus experimental gen ed science courses, by semester

Course Treatmenta Term

Pre Post

tactual (df)Pre–post p value

Effect sizeN MATE score N MATE score

1 Introductory environmental studiesb

T Fall 2010 33 70.64 28 68.00 t(59) = 0.579 ns

2 Introductory physicsc T Fall 2011 129 67.25 92 66.48 t(219) = 0.423 ns3 Introductory chemistryd T Fall 2011 399 64.18 297 64.13 t(694) = 0.047 ns4 FoSe E Fall 2010 136 64.57 137 74.15 t(265) = −5.940f <0.001 +0.725 FoSe E Spring 2011 143 68.39 136 76.21 t(277) = −4.792 <0.001 +0.576 FoSe E Fall 2011 233 67.31 216 78.46 t(447) = −8.678 <0.001 +0.827 FoSe E Spring 2012 239 66.30 226 76.16 t(463) = −7.914 <0.001 +0.738 FoSe E Fall 2012 131 63.17 118 69.25 t(247) = −3.396 = 0.001 +0.43

aT = traditional (i.e., comparison) gen ed science course for nonmajors; E = experimental FoS course.bBIOL 1301: Environmental Science.cPHYS 1305: Fundamentals of Physics.dCHEM 1306: Inorganic and Environmental Chemistry.eCross-listed as both BIOL 1436 and GEOG 1436: Foundations of Science.fThis comparison required a t test for unequal sample variances: the adjusted df = 264.94.

after one semester in our experimental course (posttest FoS CAT = 20.12 ± 0.63; t(105) = 1.717, p = 0.090; Figure 2B).

Student Acceptance of the Theory of EvolutionResults on the MATE parallel those from the CAT; in no se-mester did students completing a traditional course show improvement in their acceptance of evolutionary theory (all p values > 0.27; Table 2), while students completing the exper-imental course showed highly significant improvement each semester (all p values ≤ 0.001, all Cohen’s d > 0.43; Table 2). A pooled analysis comparing students across all semesters in the experimental course with students from the three differ-ent traditional courses further highlights the success of the ex-perimental approach; students who completed the FoS course showed highly significant improvement in their acceptance

by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from

Critical Thinking and Gen Ed Science

Vol. 14, Fall 2015 14:ar30, 9

comparable studies suggest we have much to learn about the factors influencing student acceptance of evolutionary theory. To contribute, we plan additional analyses, mining our database to examine the effects of gender, ethnicity, high school grade point average, and student attitudes on the MATE and on the CAT.

Instructors (who are also colleagues and friends) in the traditional gen ed science courses that served as our com-parison group were disappointed their students showed no improvement in critical thinking after a semester of science. But, they argued reasonably, why should we expect student acceptance of evolutionary theory to improve in introduc-tory gen ed chemistry or physics classes, given that biolog-ical evolution is not discussed in such courses? Four points are relevant, the last being most important. First, we suggest that all college graduates, science majors or not, should ap-preciate how the term “theory,” used scientifically, differs from its conversational definition. Second, evolutionary theory was covered in the environmental studies course (Table 2) in which we used the MATE, yet students still failed to demonstrate improvement in their acceptance of the the-ory in this traditionally taught gen ed science course. Third, even though evolution is a topic we address explicitly in the FoS course, it is covered during the last week of the semester, the week following the posttest administration of the MATE.

The most important issue, however, relates to what the MATE may be measuring. Several authors have argued that the MATE more likely measures an individual’s knowledge about evolution rather than his or her acceptance of the theory (Smith, 2010a; Wagler and Wagler, 2013). And while it is generally presumed that some content knowledge is required for a student to accept evolution as the best expla-nation of biological diversity, evidence also suggests that dispositional change may be required before a student is willing to entertain the theory (Sinatra et al., 2003; Smith, 2010a,b). Whether the MATE measures an individual’s content knowledge about evolution or his or her disposi-tion toward the theory is beyond the scope of this analysis. Our results, however, are robust; a course focusing on the nature of science and applying SJT leads to significantly improved engagement of the non–science college student with evolution (see also Pigliucci, 2007; Lombrozo et  al., 2008).

Assessment Validity, RevisitedWagler and Wagler (2013) criticized the construct validity and, thus, the generalizability of the MATE for popula-tions other than the high school teachers used to originally test the tool’s validity (Rutledge and Warden, 1999). The Waglers found, for example, that the MATE lacked con-struct validity for their sample of Hispanic college students majoring in elementary education. Construct validity is the degree to which a test actually measures the mental attri-bute it claims to measure (Brown, 2000); for the MATE, the attribute is thought to be an individual’s acceptance of the theory of evolution (Rutledge and Warden, 1999). One tech-nique for assessing construct validity uses factor analyses with structural equation modeling to identify the number of dimensions of the construct; if a significant unifying di-mension or dimensions cannot be identified, the tool may be suspect; this was the approach used to demonstrate that

the CAT scores achieved by juniors and seniors nationwide (Figure 2A). This is the good news.

The pattern for our upper-division students, however, is more worrisome, as their pretest CAT average is signifi-cantly lower than the national mean for juniors and seniors (Figure 2B). Given that our lower-division students start with significantly better CAT scores than their peers nationally, results showing that our juniors and seniors are significantly worse (before taking the FoS course) than their countrywide counterparts might suggest our institutional curriculum degrades rather than improves a student’s critical-thinking skills. An alternative interpretation is that the non–science students who choose, as freshmen or sophomores, to take one of their science requirements, especially an experimental course like the FoS course, represent a cohort different from the students who delay taking their core science courses until near the end of their undergraduate careers. The for-mer may be less science-phobic than the latter and, thus, more practiced at and receptive to evidentiary thinking. If this interpretation is correct, as science educators, we need to embrace pedagogies that connect with our more anxious students, lest their experiences further alienate them from science as a way of knowing. The approaches adopted in the FoS course may be part of the solution, as the significant deficit in critical thinking we observe in upper-division stu-dents, compared with national norms, is gone by the end of the semester (Figure 2B).

Student Acceptance of Evolutionary TheoryResults also demonstrate that our experimental course led to significant improvements, again with large effect sizes, in the willingness of students to engage with the theory of evolu-tion. But to what degree? Rutledge and Sadler (2007), authors of the MATE, have identified five levels of acceptance associ-ated with their instrument: very high (89–100), high (76–88), moderate (65–75), low (53–64), and very low (<52). At the beginning of the semester, students in the FoS course exhib-ited, on average, borderline low to moderate (66.17 ± 0.45) scores on the MATE, improving to the boundary between moderate and high acceptance by the end of the course (75.45 ± 0.49). While we hoped for greater improvement, the end-of-course MATE scores for FoS students are comparable with those of both high school biology teachers in Indiana (77.59 ± 0.84; Rutledge and Warden, 2000) and preservice high school science teachers in Korea (73.79 ± 1.00; Kim and Nehm, 2011). A study of introductory biology students (both majors and nonmajors) attending a public university in Wisconsin who completed a special module exploring mac-roevolution and its misconceptions (Abraham et  al., 2012), also employing a pretest versus posttest design, deserves special mention given the similarities to our experiment. The average postintervention MATE score for the Wisconsin stu-dents (75.0 ± 0.52) was similar to the average post-FoS MATE score for students in this study (75.45 ± 0.49). The preinter-vention scores for students in the two studies, however, were dramatically different (70.8 ± 1.14 for nonmajors, 73.0 ± 0.58 for majors in the Wisconsin study; 66.17 ± 0.45 for the non-majors in this study), as were the effect sizes of the two inter-ventions (Cohen’s d for Wisconsin = 0.19; Cohen’s d for this study = 0.67). The similarities in postintervention scores giv-en the dissimilarities in preintervention scores of these two

by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from

M. P. Rowe, B. M. Gillespie, et al.

14:ar30, 10 CBE—Life Sciences Education

the MATE lacked construct validity for preservice teachers (Wagler and Wagler, 2013). We applied the same technique to our MATE results and similarly found that no model, either uni- or multidimensional, could be fitted to the data (unpublished data). But researchers should never rely on a single method for assessing the validity of their interpre-tations (Cronbach and Meehl, 1955; Messick, 1995; Brown, 2000, 2005; Campbell and Nehm, 2013). Two related exper-imental approaches for assessing the construct validity of a test are intervention studies and differential-groups studies (Cronbach and Meehl, 1955; Messick, 1995; Brown, 2000, 2005). In the former, a group is tested before and following their exposure to the construct; significant improvement demonstrates the construct validity of the intervention. Dif-ferential-groups studies employ two groups, one presented with the construct, the other not; significantly better scores by the informed group similarly demonstrate the validity of the training. We used both approaches in this study; the “construct” was a novel gen ed science course (the FoS) fo-cusing on the nature of science rather than just its facts (for more details please see “Expanded Course Rationale and Structure” in our Supplemental Materials). Students who completed the training demonstrated, over multiple sec-tions of the course spanning multiple years, highly sig-nificant improvement both in their critical-thinking skills (as measured by the CAT; Table 1 and associated figures) and in their willingness to engage the theory of evolution (assessed with the MATE; Table 2 and associated figures). Students who did not receive this training, those who instead completed a traditional gen ed science course, showed no improvement on either metric. While validity is never absolute (Messick, 1995; Brown, 2005; Campbell and Nehm, 2013), we argue that the power and consistency of our results are strong validation of the success of the inter-vention.

CONCLUSIONS

Students completing the FoS course significantly improve their critical-thinking skills. Given the ineffectiveness of gen ed sciences courses in particular (Impey et  al., 2011, 2012) and the college curriculum more broadly (Arum and Roksa, 2010, 2014) to produce such change, we are proud to share our successes. But we recognize the improvements we demonstrate, in both critical thinking and in the will-ingness of students to engage with scientific ideas they often reject, are a snapshot in time, an improvement over a single semester. Our hope, of course, is that students completing an experimental course like the FoS would, upon graduation, be more scientifically literate as adults, that they would understand and value science as a way of knowing, and that they could digest a science-related story in the Washington Post (Miller, 1998). As a single litmus test, would it not be wonderful if all college graduates, not just our science, technology, engineering, and mathematics stu-dents, had the confidence and the ability to make intelligent decisions about whether or not to vaccinate their children? We all depend on an educated citizenry with the skills to make, quite literally, just such life-and-death decisions. We must design and teach our nonmajors science courses to-ward this end.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This project was supported through the Quality Enhancement Plan at SHSU. We thank Brent Rahlwes, Cheramie Trahan, Samantha Martin, and Kelsey Pearman, outstanding teaching assistants who not only led the case studies during the lab sections associated with the course but also enthusiastically helped improve the material; Joe Hill, for contributing course materials and teaching a section of the class; Tim Tripp, for having taught several sections of the course; Rita Caso, for her sound advice and unwavering support in her ca-pacity as director of the Office of Institutional Research & Assess-ment at SHSU; and Cory Kohn, Louise Mead, Ross Nehm, and two anonymous reviewers for providing valuable suggestions on an earlier version of this article. All electronic data files (i.e., xlsx and sav) and reports (i.e., pdf) associated with the CAT are stored on a limited-access, secure FTP server administered by the Center for Assessment & Improvement of Learning at TTU. The physical copy of each CAT test has been digitized (pdf) and stored on a limited-ac-cess, secure hard drive; all physical tests were then destroyed. The MATE data are stored on a limited-access, secure server adminis-tered by the Office of Institutional Effectiveness at SHSU. Approval for this study was granted by the Internal Review Board Committee (#2013-04-7942) of SHSU.

REFERENCES

Abraham JK, Perez KE, Downey N, Herron JC, Meir E (2012). Short lesson plan associated with increased acceptance of evolutionary theory and potential change in three alternate conceptions of macro-evolution in undergraduate students. CBE Life Sci Educ 11, 152–164.

Alberts B (2005). A wakeup call for science faculty. Cell 123, 739–741.

Alberts B (2009). Redefining science education. Science 323, 437.

Alters BJ, Nelson CE (2002). Perspective: teaching evolution in high-er education. Evolution 56, 1891–1901.

American Association for the Advancement of Science (AAAS) (1993). Project 2061–Benchmarks for Science Literacy: A Tool for Curriculum Reform, Washington, DC.

AAAS (2010). Vision and Change: A Call to Action, Washington, DC.

Arum R, Roksa J (2010). Academically Adrift: Limited Learning on College Campuses, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Arum R, Roksa J (2014). Aspiring Adults Adrift: Tentative Transi-tions of College Graduates, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Arum R, Roksa J, Cho E (2011). Improving Undergraduate Learning: Findings and Policy Recommendations from the SSRC-CLA Longi-tudinal Project, Brooklyn, NY: Social Science Research Council.

Benoit WL Persuasion, Communication Institute for Online Scholar-ship. www.cios.org/encyclopedia/persuasion/Esocial_judgment _1theory.htm (accessed 6 February 2015).

Bernstein DA, Penner LA, Clarke-Stewart A, Roy EJ (2006). Psychol-ogy, Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Bok D (2006). Our Underachieving Colleges: A Candid Look at How Much Students Learn and Why They Should be Learning More, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Brown JD (2000). What is construct validity? Shiken: JALT Test Eval SIG Newsl 4, 8–12.

Brown JD (2005). Testing in Language Programs: A Comprehensive Guide to English Language Assessment, New York: McGraw-Hill.

Campbell CE, Nehm RH (2013). A critical analysis of assessment quality in genomics and bioinformatics education research. CBE Life Sci Educ 12, 530–541.

Carmel JH, Yezierski EJ (2013). Are we keeping the promise? Inves-tigation of students’ critical thinking growth. J Coll Sci Teach 42, 71–81.

by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from

Critical Thinking and Gen Ed Science

Vol. 14, Fall 2015 14:ar30, 11

Lombrozo T, Thanukos A, Weisberg M (2008). The importance of un-derstanding the nature of science for accepting evolution. Evol Educ Outreach 1, 290–298.

Maher JM, Markey JC, Ebert-May D (2013). The other half of the story: effect size analysis in quantitative research. CBE Life Sci Educ 12, 345–351.

Messick S (1995). Validity of psychological assessment: validation of inferences from persons’ responses and performances as scientific inquiry into score meaning. Am Psychol 50, 741–749.

Miller JD (1998). The measurement of civic scientific literacy. Public Underst Sci 7, 203–223.

Miller JD (2012). What colleges and universities need to do to ad-vance civic scientific literacy and preserve American democra-cy. Liberal Education 98. www.aacu.org/publications-research/ periodicals/what-colleges-and-universities-need-do-advance -civic-scientific (accessed 7 February 2015).

Miller JD, Scott EC, Okamoto S (2006). Public acceptance of evolu-tion. Science 313, 765–766.

Mnookin S (2011). The Panic Virus: A True Story of Medicine, Science, and Fear, New York: Simon & Schuster.

Momsen JL, Long TM, Wyse SA, Ebert-May D (2010). Just the facts? Introductory undergraduate biology courses focus on low-level cog-nitive skills. CBE Life Sci Educ 9, 435–440.

Moore R, Cotner S (2009). Educational malpractice: the impact of including creationism in high school biology courses. Evol Educ Outreach 2, 95–100.

Morrison D (2011). Science denialism: evolution and climate change. Reports Natl Center Sci Educ 31, 1–10.

Nadelson LS, Southerland SA (2010). Examining the interaction of acceptance and understanding: how does the relationship change with a focus on macroevolution? Evol Educ Outreach 3, 82–88.

Nathan D, Snedeker M (2001). Satan’s Silence: Ritual Abuse and the Making of a Modern American Witch Hunt, Lincoln, NE: Author’s Choice Press.

National Academy of Sciences, National Academy of Engineer-ing, and Institute of Medicine (2010). Rising above the Gathering Storm, Revisited: Rapidly Approaching Category 5, Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

National Science Foundation (2014). Science and Engineering Indi-cators 2014, Arlington, VA: National Science Board.

Nelson CE (2008). Teaching evolution (and all of biology) more effectively: strategies for engagement, critical reasoning, and con-fronting misconceptions. Integr Comp Biol 48, 213–225.

Nyhan B, Reifler J, Richey S, Freed GL (2014). Effective messages in vaccine promotion: a randomized trial. Pediatrics 133, E835–E842.

Offit PA (2011). Deadly Choices: How the Anti-Vaccine Movement Threatens Us All, New York: Basic.

Pascarella ET, Blaich C, Martin GL, Hanson JM (2011). How robust are the findings of academically adrift? Change 43, 20–24.

Peker D, Comert G, Kence A (2010). Three decades of anti-evolu-tion campaign and its results: Turkish undergraduates’ acceptance and understanding of the biological evolution theory. Sci Educ 19, 739–755.

Pigliucci M (2007). The evolution-creation wars: why teaching more science just is not enough. McGill J Educ 42, 285–306.

Posner GJ, Strike KA, Hewson PW, Gertzog WA (1982). Accom-modation of a scientific conception: toward a theory of conceptual change. Sci Educ 66, 211–227.

Reardon S (2011). Climate change sparks battles in classroom. Science 333, 688–689.

Rowe MP (2010). Tragic choices: autism, measles, and the MMR vaccine. In: Science Stories: Using Case Studies to Teach Critical

Cawthorne N (2004). Witch Hunt: History of a Persecution, London: Chartwell.

Chabris C, Simons D (2010). The Invisible Gorilla, New York: Crown.

Cronbach LJ, Meehl PE (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psych Bull 52, 281–302.

Ebert-May D, Derting TL, Hodder J, Momsen JL, Long TM, Jardeleza SE (2011). What we say is not what we do: effective evaluation of faculty professional development programs. BioScience 61, 550–558.

Ede A (2000). Has science education become an enemy of scientific rationality? Skeptical Inquirer 24, 48–51.

Erwin P (2014). Attitudes and Persuasion, New York: Psychology Press.

Eve RA, Dunn D (1990). Psychic powers, astrology and creation-ism in the classroom? Evidence of pseudoscientific beliefs among high school biology and life science teachers. Am Biol Teach 52, 10–21.

Facione PA (1990). Critical Thinking: A Statement of Expert Con-sensus for Purposes of Educational Assessment and Instruction, Millbrae, CA: California Academic Press.

Facione PA (2015). Critical Thinking: What It Is and Why It Counts, San Jose, CA: California Academic Press. www.insightassessment .com/Resources/Tools-For-Teaching-For-and-About-Thinking/ Critical-Thinking-What-It-Is-and-Why-It-Counts/Critical-Thinking -What-It-Is-and-Why-It-Counts-PDF (accessed 21 April 2015).

Facione PA, Sánchez CA, Facione NC, Gainen J (1995). The disposi-tion toward critical thinking. J Gen Educ 44, 1–25.

Feynman RP, Leighton R (1985). “Surely You’re Joking, Mr. Feynman!”: Adventures of a Curious Character, New York: W.W. Norton.

Freeman S, Eddy SL, McDonough M, Smith MK, Okoroafor N, Jordt H, Wenderoth MP (2014). Active learning increases student perfor-mance in science, engineering, and mathematics. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111, 8410–8415.

Harris K, Stein B, Haynes A, Lisic E, Leming K (2014). Identifying courses that improve students’ critical thinking skills using the CAT instrument: a case study. Proceedings of the 10th Annual Interna-tional Joint Conferences on Computer, Information, System Scienc-es, and Engineering 10, 1–4.

Hazen RM (2002). Why should you be scientifically literate? ActionBioscience. www.actionbioscience.org/education/hazen.html (accessed 7 February 2015).

Hewitt PG, Lyons SA, Suchocki JA, Yeh J (2013). Conceptual Inte-grated Science, 2nd ed., Boston: Addison-Wesley.

Impey C, Buxner S, Antonellis J (2012). Non-scientific beliefs among undergraduate students. Astron Educ Rev 11, 1–12.

Impey C, Buxner S, Antonellis J, Johnson E, King C (2011). A twen-ty-year survey of science literacy among college undergraduates. J Coll Sci Teach 40, 31–37.

Johnson AC (2007). Unintended consequences: how science profes-sors discourage women of color. Sci Educ 91, 805–821.

Johnson M, Pigliucci M (2004). Is knowledge of science associated with higher skepticism of pseudoscientific claims? Am Biol Teach 66, 536–548.

Keppel G (1982). Design and Analysis: A Researcher’s Handbook, 2nd ed., Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Kim SY, Nehm RH (2011). A cross-cultural comparison of Korean and American science teachers’ views of evolution and the nature of science. Int J Sci Educ 33, 197–227.

Kuhn D (1993). Science as argument: implications for teaching and learning scientific thinking. Sci Educ 77, 319–337.

Lanning KV (1992). Investigator’s Guide to Allegations of “Ritual” Child Abuse, Quantico, VA: Federal Bureau of Investigation.

by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from

M. P. Rowe, B. M. Gillespie, et al.

14:ar30, 12 CBE—Life Sciences Education

Thinking, ed. CF Herreid, NA Schiller, and KF Herreid, Arlington, VA: NSTA Press. http://sciencecases.lib.buffalo.edu/cs/collection/ detail.asp?case_id = 576&id = 576 (accessed 7 February 2015).

Rowe MP (2015). Crazy about cryptids! An ecological hunt for Nessie and other legendary creatures. National Center for Case Study Teaching in Science. http://sciencecases.lib.buffalo.edu/cs/ collection/detail.asp?case_id=779&id=779 (accessed 26 June 2015).

Rutherford FJ, Ahlgren A (1990). Science for All Americans, Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Rutledge ML, Sadler KC (2007). Reliability of the measure of accep-tance of the theory of evolution (MATE) instrument with university students. Am Biol Teach 69, 332–335.

Rutledge ML, Warden MA (1999). The development and validation of the measure of acceptance of the theory of evolution instrument. Sch Sci Math 99, 13–18.

Rutledge ML, Warden MA (2000). Evolutionary theory, the nature of science and high school biology teachers: critical relationships. Am Biol Teach 62, 23–31.

Sagan C (1996). The Demon-haunted World: Science as a Candle in the Dark, New York: Ballantine.

Sam Houston State University (2009). QEP: SHSU: Foun-dations of Science. www.shsu.edu/qep/documents/ QualityEnhancementPlanCombined.pdf (accessed 7 February 2015).

Schick T, Vaughn L (2014). How to Think about Weird Things: Criti-cal Thinking for a New Age, New York: McGraw-Hill.

Seymour E, Hewitt NM (1997). Talking about Leaving: Why Under-graduates Leave the Sciences, Boulder, CO: Westview.

Sinatra G, Brem S, Evans EM (2008). Changing minds? Implications of conceptual change for teaching and learning about biological evo-lution. Evol Educ Outreach 1, 189–195.

Sinatra GM, Southerland SA, McConaughy F, Demastes JW (2003). Intentions and beliefs in students’ understanding and acceptance of biological evolution. J Res Sci Teach 40, 510–528.

Smith MU (2010a). Current status of research in teaching and learning evolution: I. Philosophical/epistemological issues. Sci Educ 19, 523–538.

Smith MU (2010b). Current status of research in teaching and learn-ing evolution: II. Pedagogical issues. Sci Educ 19, 539–571.

Snyder TD, Dillow SA (2013). Digest of Education Statistics, 2012, Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics.

Stein B, Haynes A (2011). Engaging faculty in the assessment and improvement of students’ critical thinking using the critical think-ing assessment test. Change 43, 44–49.

Stein B, Haynes A, Redding M, Ennis T, Cecil M (2007). Assessing critical thinking in STEM and beyond. In: Innovations in E-Learning, Instruction Technology, Assessment, and Engineering Education, ed. M Iskander, Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.

Stein B, Haynes A, Redding M, Harris K, Tylka M, Lisic E (2010). Faculty driven assessment of critical thinking: national dissemi-nation of the CAT instrument. In: Technological Developments in Networking, Education and Automation, ed. K Elleithy, T Sobh, M Iskander, V Kapila, MA Karim, and A Mahmood, Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.

Tavris C, Aronson E (2007). Mistakes Were Made (but Not by Me), Orlando, FL: Harcourt.

Tennessee Technological University (2010). CAT Instrument Technical Information. www.tntech.edu/files/cat/reports/CAT _Technical_Information_V7.pdf (accessed 7 February 2015).

Trefil J, Hazen RM (2013). The Sciences: An Integrated Approach, 7th ed., Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Van Gelder T (2005). Teaching critical thinking. Coll Teach 45, 41–46.

Verhey SD (2005). The effect of engaging prior learning on student attitudes toward creationism and evolution. BioScience 55, 996–1003.

Wagler A, Wagler R (2013). Addressing the lack of measurement in-variance for the measure of acceptance of the theory of evolution. Int J Sci Educ 35, 2278–2298.

Wakefield A, Murch SH, Anthony A, Linnell J, Casson DM (1998). Ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-specific colitis, and perva-sive developmental disorder in children. Lancet 351, 637–641.

Walker WR, Hoekstra SJ, Vogl RJ (2002). Science education is no guarantee of skepticism. Skeptic 9, 24–27.

by guest on August 13, 2015http://www.lifescied.org/Downloaded from

Supplemental Material CBE—Life Sciences Education

et al.S SS SSSSS Rowe

  1  

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1   2  1. Expanded Course Rationale and Structure 3   4  Jon D. Miller, the Director of the International Center for the Advancement of Scientific 5  Literacy, wrote, “the healthy functioning of democracy depends crucially upon the 6  existence of a literate public; and in modern industrial societies, true democracy must 7  embrace scientific literacy (Miller, 1998). We share this concern, as the lack of scientific 8  literacy adversely affects our ability, as a society, to make informed decisions about 9  science-related issues (Impey et al., 2012) such as global climate change, loss of 10  biodiversity, resource use, and the efficacy of vaccines. And, as discussed in the main 11  article, this situation also makes it difficult for people to judge the merits of well-12  established scientific theories, including the Theory of Evolution and the Big Bang 13  Theory. 14   15  In order to address these issues, the Foundations of Science (FoS) course was designed to 16  focus on the development of critical thinking skills and basic scientific literacy defined – 17  consistent with Miller’s definition – as understanding key scientific terminology, 18  concepts, and theories and, most importantly, understanding science as a reliable way of 19  knowing about the natural world based on its use of critical thinking and logical 20  arguments, empirical evidence, skepticism, the scientific method, objectivity/intellectual 21  honesty, and peer review. This approach, as well as other aspects of the course design 22  discussed below, is also consistent with the recommendations put forward by the AAAS 23  in its Project 2061 (American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1993; 24  Rutherford and Ahlgren, 1990), which was developed to establish recommendations for 25  science literacy in the U.S. 26   27   28  Nature of Science 29   30  Part of the rationale for developing an integrated science course, and one which addresses 31  science as a way of knowing, was the recognition that, if students take only two required 32  science courses in college, they will necessarily graduate with gaping holes in their 33  knowledge of science because they will have learned little or nothing of the key ideas in 34  the science disciplines that were not included in their coursework. 35   36  In addition, because undergraduate science courses typically do not address the nature of 37  science and scientific reasoning, students will not develop an understanding of the 38  rationale for the scientific method as a means of reducing or eliminating error, nor will 39  they develop their scientific reasoning skills and their ability to apply them to real-world 40  situations. Nor will they will be able to distinguish good science from ‘bad’ science, or 41  real science from pseudoscience. 42   43  The fact that most Gen-Ed science courses do not address these concepts leaves graduates 44  unprepared to make informed decisions regarding science-related issues, and it makes it 45  far more likely they will reject well-supported theories in science. As evidenced by the 46  

  2  

rise of an anti-vaccine ‘movement’ in the U.S. and elsewhere, they may also reject the 47  well-established efficacy of vaccines and the health benefits afforded by them. Lacking 48  an understanding of the scientific methods by which medicines are tested for safety and 49  efficacy, and placing higher confidence in anecdotal accounts, many people abandon 50  thoroughly researched medical practices and treatments in favor of untested and even 51  harmful ‘alternative medicines’ and practices. In short, there are numerous indicators 52  that people are implicitly rejecting the validity of the scientific method itself. This anti-53  science attitude has profoundly negative implications and consequences for society as 54  shown by the outbreak of whooping cough in the U.S. in 2012. More than 11,000 cases 55  were reported to the CDC, at least 12 of which were fatal. This was the largest number of 56  cases reported in the last 50 years (Rosenau, 2012). This outbreak of an entirely 57  preventable disease occurred because parents rejected the scientific basis of vaccines and 58  chose not to have their children vaccinated. 59   60  The critical thinking/scientific-reasoning framework of the course is encapsulated, as we 61  discussed in the paper, in a set of questions Bernstein et al. (2006) suggest be asked when 62  evaluating a claim. James Lett (1990) expanded those questions into a more formalized 63  set of rules known by the acronym FiLCHeRS, a framework we find particularly useful 64  for helping students distinguish science from pseudoscience. The easily assimilated 65  acronym (once students understand what it means to be filched) stands for Falsifiability, 66  Logic, Comprehensiveness of evidence, Honesty (as in ‘intellectual honesty’), 67  Replication of research, and Sufficiency of evidence, respectively. Every claim and 68  theory in the course, both scientific and pseudoscientific, is evaluated using Lett’s rules. 69  Our repeated use of the framework provides students with a systematic, coherent method 70  for analyzing claims. They quickly learn to spot pseudoscientific claims because such 71  claims violate many of the rules, while good scientific claims do not. In short, Lett’s 72  framework provides a compelling demonstration of the reliability of the scientific 73  method. Step-by-incremental step, we try to expand the students’ latitudes of acceptance 74  (see sections on SJT in the main manuscript and also below) by helping them understand 75  the power of science as a way of knowing. 76  

77   78  Critical Thinking 79   80  In order to think critically, the student must first know what critical thinking is and value 81  it as an essential component of informed decision-making. They also must understand at 82  least some of the ways in which critical thinking can be subverted based on biases, 83  misperceptions, faulty memory, and cognitive dissonance so that they can avoid these 84  sources of error. For this reason, the lecture and lab include discussions and activities 85  that directly demonstrate limits in the accuracy of our students’ own perceptions and 86  memories. These activities are not only engaging, but they vividly demonstrate the 87  unreliability of anecdotal testimony and, by inference, the need for the scientific method. 88   89  Ultimately, any hypothesis or theory in science is based on an argument; therefore, it is 90  essential for students to understand the structure of an argument (premises and 91  conclusion), the characteristics of a valid and sound deductive argument, and the 92  

  3  

characteristics of a reasonable, or sound, inductive argument. Therefore, as discussed in 93  the main article, we do something that is unique in a science course, but which is 94  fundamentally important to the success of the course; namely, we include a discussion of 95  arguments, as well as common heuristics and logical fallacies. Very few college students 96  are aware of what an argument is, or of heuristics and fallacies, prior to taking the course, 97  and few students would be exposed to them outside of a philosophy course; yet, an 98  understanding and awareness of them is essential to critical thinking and to the evaluation 99  of claims. Because the reliability of science as a way of knowing rests to a large degree 100  on the arguments it makes, students must understand what constitutes a good argument; 101  i.e., they must understand the necessity of having good reasons for either accepting or 102  rejecting a claim, which is based on an argument. 103   104  An example of this is provided by the argument we use to establish the multi-billion year 105  age of the universe. This argument is discussed in the section of the course dealing with 106  astronomy and the Big Bang Theory. 107   108  

1) Premise 1: We can see galaxies located more than 13 billion light years away. 109  2) Premise 2: By definition, it takes light one year to travel a distance of one-light 110  

year. 111  3) Conclusion: The universe must be at least 13 billion years old in order for light to 112  

have reached us from galaxies located 13 billion lights years away. 113   114  

Our students, having learned about arguments, can now recognize this is a valid and 115  sound argument, and so it is reasonable for them to conclude that the universe really is 116  billions of years old. Had they not learned about arguments, this conclusion might 117  otherwise carry little weight and its conclusion be dismissed as mere opinion. 118   119   120  Pseudoscience 121   122  One of the most important aspects of the course is that students are asked to use what 123  they have learned in the course, both scientific facts and critical thinking concepts, to 124  evaluate a variety of unsupported and/or pseudoscientific claims. The need to include 125  such extraordinary claims derives from the readily observable fact that the media 126  bombards people with such claims on a daily basis – yet most people, including college 127  graduates, lack the scientific knowledge and critical thinking skills to evaluate them 128  (Johnson and Pigliucci, 2004). Consequently, many people uncritically accept them, as 129  happened recently when the Animal Planet channel ran two pseudo-documentaries on the 130  alleged existence of mermaids. The second documentary was the most watched show in 131  the history of the channel, with about 3.6 million viewers (Day, 2013). By incorporating 132  pseudoscientific and extraordinary claims into the course, and using scientific facts, laws 133  of nature, and critical thinking skills taught in the course, students gain real-world 134  experience in using this information to rationally evaluate claims – and they learn science 135  in the process. This is consistent with an approach advocated by Martin (1994) who 136  argued that science teachers should include pseudoscience in their courses, not for 137  purposes of teaching it, but to help students learn to distinguish science from 138  

  4  

pseudoscience and to critically evaluate claims. Were students taught science in this 139  manner, and armed with critical thinking skills as part of their science education, few 140  would fall for the sorts of claims presented in the mermaid pseudo-documentary – or for 141  a host of other pseudoscientific claims that permeate our culture. 142   143  The evaluation of the pseudoscientific claim regarding the Loch Ness monster (Rowe, 144  2015) was mentioned in the main article. Another example concerns the evaluation of 145  claims pertaining to astrology which, according to SEI 2014 (National Science 146  Foundation, 2014), 42% of Americans believe is either “sort of scientific” (32%) or “very 147  scientific (10%). Prior to the discussion of this topic, students first learn relevant 148  astronomy pertaining to stars, galaxies, and interstellar distances. They also learn about 149  the four fundamental forces of nature. Using this information, students can then begin to 150  evaluate the claim that a mere 200 stars, out of about 200 billion in our galaxy, somehow 151  exert – through means that defy scientific understanding of the universe – an effect on the 152  personalities of people and their lives based on the position of these stars (and planets) at 153  the time of their birth. By learning about the fundamental forces of nature (of which 154  there are only four and no more), students realize that the strong and weak nuclear forces 155  could not exert such an effect because their range is limited to the nucleus of an atom, 156  and that both the gravitational and electromagnetic force are too weak – at those distances 157  – to produce a biological effect. Indeed, they calculate that the doctor standing next to a 158  mother at the time of her child’s birth exerts a gravitational force millions of times 159  greater than does the nearest star. And, even if these two forces do have the ability to 160  ‘reach’ a person, students are asked to consider the related question of how, exactly, it 161  would affect personality and/or control a person’s future? What is the proposed 162  mechanism? As students learn in the course, pseudoscience never offers a plausible, 163  observable, or testable mechanism to explain how alleged extraordinary or paranormal 164  phenomena are supposed to occur. In contrast, scientific explanations require that such a 165  mechanism be offered. 166   167   168  Critical Thinking and Psychological Factors Affecting the Acceptance of Ideas 169   170  Students also learn relevant psychology pertaining to the reasons why people may believe 171  astrology and other extraordinary claims are true even when there is no objective 172  evidence for them. In the case of astrology, the specific psychological factor that is 173  addressed is the Forer Effect (also called the Barnum Effect); i.e., the tendency of people 174  to think a general statement which applies to most anyone (such as that in a horoscope) 175  appears to apply exclusively to them. This combination of knowledge and analysis 176  enables students to understand that astrology is pseudoscience, but in the process of 177  discussing astrology, they learn about astronomy and physics, as well as relevant 178  psychological factors which influence our perceptions. In a similar manner, students use 179  the laws of nature and relevant psychology to evaluate claims about UFOs, alien 180  abductions, ghosts, and paranormal phenomena. In the process, they learn how easy it is 181  to misperceive events, or fall victim to critical-thinking pitfalls such as selective recall 182  and confirmation bias, and thereby incorrectly conclude that something is true when it is 183  not. 184  

  5  

185  It is important to emphasize that, as instructors, we do not – acting as ‘experts’ – tell our 186  students that the Loch Ness monster, or astrology, or any other claim in the course is 187  wrong; nor do we tell them that a scientific theory is ‘correct’ and that they should simply 188  accept it because “we say so.” We let them judge the claims and theories for themselves 189  based on the evidence, relevant science, and critical thinking skills they have learned. 190  This ‘compare and contrast’ approach reduces the likelihood of an automatic rejection of 191  a scientific explanation by eliminating the human tendency to reject an idea because it 192  was presented in such a way as to imply that the students’ freedom of choice is being 193  taken away from them; as in, “Don’t try to tell me what to think” (Erwin, 2014). 194  Furthermore, evidence regarding some extraordinary claims is ambiguous and may yet 195  prove to be true. In such cases, this is acknowledged and it reinforces the fact that not 196  everything is clear-cut. We stress that science is a process of acquiring knowledge, and 197  much has yet to be learned. Just as importantly, we stress throughout the course that the 198  truth of an idea is not determined by whether we like it, or how many people believe it, 199  but by the quality of evidence and logic used to support it (the S and L in the FiLCHeRS 200  acronym). Our results show that this approach is successful in teaching students the 201  effectiveness of the scientific method and critical thinking in evaluating claims, and in 202  distinguishing science from pseudoscience. 203   204  In developing the course, we recognized that an appreciation of critical thinking and 205  acceptance of science and scientific theories involves far more than a student’s exposure 206  to scientific facts and the scientific method. Perhaps more importantly, it also involves 207  their worldview; i.e., their beliefs and attitudes toward science. These affect their 208  psychological readiness to examine various scientific theories and to engage in critical 209  thinking (Alters and Nelson, 2002). Especially as regards the Big Bang Theory and the 210  Theory of Evolution, students’ prior beliefs may prevent them from even considering the 211  possibility that these theories might be correct because the ideas are perceived as a threat 212  to their religious worldview. Failure to address this fundamentally important 213  psychological component of students’ thinking will almost inevitably result in a failure to 214  convince students that these ideas are not mere opinions but, rather, well-supported 215  theories based on empirical evidence. In short, if a scientific theory (or its implications) 216  is too far removed from a students’ current worldview, and if it is considered threatening, 217  it will almost certainly be summarily rejected; in fact, a student may become even more 218  convinced that it is wrong if simply presented with “the scientific facts.” 219   220  Accordingly, great care must be taken when discussing controversial topics so as not to 221  threaten the students’ religious worldview (see NOMA below) or reinforce these negative 222  perceptions. Furthermore, simply presenting the facts regarding these theories is unlikely 223  to produce a shift in their perceptions of these theories. However, Social Justice Theory 224  (SJT), which pertains to attitude change, provides a means of addressing this 225  psychological factor. According to this well-established theory, which is based on what 226  are termed latitudes of acceptance and rejection, a position that is substantially different 227  from a person’s initial position can eventually be accepted if that person’s latitude of 228  acceptance and rejection is incrementally shifted toward the new position. Small, gradual 229  shifts of opinion can lead to a willingness to consider once incongruent ideas (Benoit, no 230  

  6  

date). In contrast, a single, big shift leading to the acceptance of a highly incongruent 231  idea does not usually occur. This is why a science course dealing with these topics 232  should include an in-depth discussion of the nature of science, critical thinking, and the 233  benefits of science, as well as information which will be relevant to the discussion of 234  evolution (e.g., the age of the earth and universe). This information should be discussed 235  before evolution is discussed because it leads to gradual shifts in students’ latitudes of 236  acceptance. Attempting to discuss evolution first, without having carefully laid a 237  foundation for it, will be unlikely to produce attitude change because it lies too far 238  outside a student’s latitude of acceptance. 239   240  Given that the Internet and airwaves are full of unfounded and pseudoscientific claims 241  made by people lacking relevant expertise in science, our discussion of critical thinking 242  also includes an evaluation of what constitutes an expert, especially a scientific expert 243  (e.g., relevant degree, publication record, etc.). This is critically important because, if 244  students cannot distinguish a reliable source of information from one that is not, they will 245  be more likely to accept pseudoscience – and to reject real science. For example, if 246  parents think a celebrity is a better source of information regarding the efficacy of 247  vaccines, they might choose to not have their child vaccinated because they consider the 248  celebrity to be more knowledgeable than a doctor or the medical establishment. 249   250  Our working assumption for teaching both scientific facts and critical thinking concepts 251  in the same course is rather obvious; namely, if students have facts, but cannot think 252  logically, they will reach the wrong conclusion; and, if they can think critically, but lack 253  scientific knowledge, they will also reach the wrong conclusion. Both are necessary, and 254  by relating the critical thinking concepts to specific scientific and pseudoscientific claims, 255  students learn the concepts better. 256   257   258  Additional Information about Topics and Topical Organization 259   260  The course is organized into a sequence of topics specifically arranged so as to shift 261  students’ latitudes of acceptance and rejection in favor of science. In the main article, 262  reference was made to the discussion, at the beginning of the course, of witch hunts and 263  the Satanic Ritual Abuse cases as a means of demonstrating the necessity of both 264  scientific literacy and critical thinking in order to avoid harm. In other words, the point 265  of this discussion is to show that not only are science and critical thinking not bad things 266  to be feared or rejected, they are actually good things to be embraced because they 267  provide so many benefits to humanity and prevent so much harm. 268   269  One source of confusion we also address early in the course are the distinctions between 270  a fact, hypothesis, law, and theory. Understanding the distinctions between these terms is 271  essential because most students are confused by them (Alters and Nelson, 2002), and this 272  confusion serves, to some degree, as the basis for rejecting anthropogenic climate change, 273  the efficacy of vaccines, the Theory of Evolution, and the Big Bang Theory (as in, “they 274  are only theories”). This confusion also leads students to embrace many pseudoscientific 275  concepts because, in the minds of many, “theories are just opinions” and their own 276  

  7  

preferred opinion is just as good as the ‘misguided’ opinions of scientists. The discussion 277  of the Big Bang theory is specifically intended to illustrate these distinctions and to 278  emphasize the nature of a scientific theory; i.e., that a theory is a well-established 279  explanation of what is observed. Once the students understand the observations upon 280  which the Big Bang theory is based, and the ‘logic’ of the theory, they are much more 281  likely to accept it, and the multi-billion year age for the universe. The same applies to the 282  Theory of Plate tectonics and the Theory of Evolution. We also stress that no theory is 283  complete; i.e., that it is an approximation of reality and is subject to change based on new 284  evidence. This idea, which is the idea that science, unlike other ways of 285  knowing/thinking, is self-correcting, is stressed throughout the course. 286   287  Beginning with the discussion on astronomy, we make a concerted effort to inculcate a 288  sense of awe in our students – a sense of the grandeur and wonder of the universe as 289  revealed by science. This, we hope, further helps students embrace science as a way of 290  knowing. In short, if science is seen as a source of beauty and wonder, rather than as a 291  boring, cold, heartless endeavor that diminishes a sense of wonder, students are more 292  likely to engage science. Many students, for example, develop an appreciation for 293  cosmology once they understand that they really, truly are made of stardust. How 294  wonderful. 295   296  In the second part of the course, we introduce students to the experimental method. This 297  includes such concepts as independent and dependent variables, confounding variables, 298  placebo effects, control groups, experimental groups, double-blind studies, experimenter 299  bias, and sample size. The emphasis, as always, is on sources of potential error and how 300  the scientific method attempts to control for them. We emphasize, either implicitly or 301  explicitly, that the procedures embedded in the scientific method for reducing and 302  eliminating error are what makes science “a good thing” – something to be appreciated 303  rather than rejected. 304   305  These concepts are reinforced through a discussion of the FDA approval process, 306  followed by discussions and analyses of various complementary and alternative 307  medicines, such as homeopathy and therapeutic touch. As regards these two “therapies,” 308  students have learned enough at this point in the course to realize they clearly violate the 309  laws of nature, and any anecdotal evidence of their efficacy is due to a placebo effect, 310  spontaneous remission, misdiagnosis, etc. Knowing this enables students to draw the 311  conclusion that homeopathy and therapeutic touch are examples of pseudoscience. Just 312  as importantly, they learn that CAM claims in general are virtually never tested or 313  evaluated. Understanding this reinforces the efficacy of science and leads to healthy 314  skepticism of CAM claims in general. 315   316  Psychic research is covered following the discussion of CAM – again with an emphasis 317  on the ways in which these phenomena, even if real, appear to violate our current 318  understanding of the laws of nature. The lack of successful replication of seemingly 319  positive results (the R in the FiLCHeRS rules) is also discussed as part of the analysis of 320  these claims. While not definitively ‘debunking’ psychic phenomena, students come to 321  understand that the scientific community has not accepted the existence of paranormal 322  

  8  

phenomena – not because scientists are ‘biased’, but because the evidence is insufficient 323  to conclude that paranormal phenomena are real (the S in the FiLCHeRS rules). 324  Scientists also do not accept paranormal phenomena because they contradict a well-325  established body of knowledge in science. This principle of non-contradiction as a 326  criterion of truth (i.e., the necessity of logical consistency between a claim and well-327  established knowledge) is stressed throughout the course. 328   329  Following the foray into CAM and paranormal phenomena, we discuss principles of 330  geology and plate tectonics, the formation and age of the earth, rock types, relative and 331  absolute dating techniques, uniformitarianism, and finally genetics and evolution. By 332  providing evidence of the ancient age of the universe and earth through the discussion of 333  astronomy and geology, students more readily accept the scientifically established age of 334  the earth. For students who might otherwise be swayed by advocates of Young Earth 335  Creationism, this is critical for their potential acceptance of evolution because it shows 336  them that the earth is, in fact, ancient in age, having existed for the vast amount of time 337  required for biological evolution to have occurred (Smith, 2010a, b). 338   339  Furthermore, by continually demonstrating the reliability of science as a way of knowing 340  throughout the course, and specifically contrasting Creationism/Intelligent 341  Design/Irreducible Complexity with the evidence for evolutionary theory, we are able to 342  gradually shift the students’ latitude of acceptance toward a willingness to accept 343  evolution because they can see the evidence for themselves – and the logic upon which it 344  is based. 345   346  The principle of logical consistency as a criterion for truth is particularly important in the 347  discussion of young-earth creationism because its acceptance requires the negation of 348  findings from astronomy, geology, paleontology, genetics, physics, and chemistry. In 349  short, it is inconsistent with these other fields; consequently, one would have to discard 350  virtually all of science, and the scientific method itself, in order to accept it. However, 351  only by establishing the validity of astronomy, geology, the scientific method, etc., earlier 352  in the course does this argument carry weight. This is why the topics are discussed in a 353  specific sequence – and why evolution is covered last in the course. Without having laid 354  an appropriate foundation, the discussion of evolution earlier in the course would almost 355  certainly result in fewer students’ willingness to consider it and, in accordance with 356  Social Justice Theory, might actually lead them to reject not only the idea of evolution, 357  but other scientific conclusions discussed in the course as well. 358   359  As with the “Age of the Universe argument,” we also use an “Evolution argument” 360  which, after having covered genetics, mutations, and the evidence of evolution, is very 361  compelling to students. One version of the argument for evolution we use is: 362   363  

1) Premise 1: Genetic change occurs; i.e., mutations occur producing new genes, new 364  alleles, and new genotypes. (*We also stress that, contrary to popular belief, not 365  all mutations are bad. This mistaken belief must be addressed because it 366  necessarily precludes an acceptance of evolution.) 367  

  9  

2) Premise 2: Genetic changes can be passed from parent to offspring (i.e., the 368  changes are inherited from parents) 369  

3) Premise 3: Natural selection occurs due to competition, with those 370  genotypes/phenotypes best suited to their current environment surviving and 371  producing more offspring 372  

4) Conclusion: Changes in the allelic frequencies of a population necessarily occur; 373  therefore evolution occurs. 374  

375  Students accept each of the premises and, having done so, they are more likely to accept 376  the conclusion that evolution occurs. Students recognize this as a valid and sound 377  argument. Indeed, at this point, we flip the question from “Does evolution occur?” to, 378  “How could it not occur?” Again, without understanding an argument, or what makes an 379  argument valid and sound, this approach would probably not be effective; hence the need 380  to include these concepts in a science course. Critical thinking/logic is essential to an 381  acceptance of evolution, as was shown by a study of non-major biology students. Those 382  who were less skilled in critical thinking were also more likely to hold nonscientific 383  beliefs and their nonscientific views were not easily changed (Lawson and Weser, 1990). 384   385   386  Limitations of Science 387   388  There is one additional aspect to this process that we think is of critical importance to the 389  observed shift in students’ willingness to accept, or consider, the Big Bang Theory and 390  the Theory of Evolution. In order to address concerns students may have that these ideas 391  threaten their religious beliefs, which constitute a key factor leading to the rejection of 392  scientific theories and, by implication, science itself, we adopted Stephen Jay Gould’s 393  concept of Non-Overlapping Magisteria (NOMA) (Gould, 1999). This concept is based 394  on the idea that there are different domains of human experience and that science deals 395  exclusively with the domain that can be empirically investigated. Science does not, by its 396  nature, address questions of morality, or ultimate meaning or purpose, as do religion, 397  ethics, and philosophy. Accordingly, we stress the strength of science throughout the 398  course in terms of its ability to advance knowledge of empirical matters, but we also 399  acknowledge its limits regarding other aspects of the human experience pertaining to 400  meaning, purpose, and values. We also point out that the assertion one can either “be 401  religious” or accept evolution, but not both, is an example of one of the fallacies they 402  have learned; namely, the False-Dichotomy fallacy. As Joshua Rosenau of the National 403  Center for Science Education said, “Recognizing and defusing the social pressures 404  underlying science denial are key in convincing people that it is even worth considering 405  scientific ideas that seem contrary to those of their social identity” (Rosenau, 2012). 406   407  Having established the distinctions between science and non-science early in the 408  semester, and putting forward the NOMA principle, our students are more receptive to 409  scientific theories because they do not feel threatened by them. We recognize that not all 410  scientists share this view about NOMA and might be philosophically opposed to this 411  approach; however, the effectiveness of our approach, which addresses the psychology of 412  

  10  

belief and is respectful of the worldviews of our students, has been shown to be quite 413  effective based on our results involving the MATE assessment. 414   415  Our approach is also consistent with that recommended by Smith (2010a, b) in a 416  comprehensive review of the literature concerning philosophical and pedagogical issues 417  regarding the teaching of evolution. He specifically addresses the need to acknowledge 418  both students’ worldview (and how it affects their willingness to consider evolution), and 419  their misconceptions regarding science and evolution. Accordingly, he emphasizes the 420  necessity of using pedagogical approaches based on recognition of cognitive factors 421  affecting belief and conceptual change, as well as the need to teach the nature of science 422  – not just the facts of science. He also argues that a NOMA-based approach is more 423  successful in facilitating change in attitudes regarding evolution and, we would argue, 424  would apply to the Big Bang theory and other scientific ideas students find 425  discomforting. 426   427   428  Use of Case Studies and Assignments 429   430  There is one additional aspect of the course design we believe has been critical in our 431  success; i.e., an active and cooperative learning approach built around case studies. 432  Smith et al. (2005) reported that, between 1924 and 1997, more than 168 studies were 433  conducted in an attempt to assess the relative effectiveness of various methods of 434  learning, namely, cooperative, competitive, and individualistic pedagogies. A meta-435  analyses of these studies showed that the cooperative learning approach resulted in 436  significant and substantial increases in learning; i.e., higher achievement, relative to 437  either the individualistic or competitive approaches. The measures used to gauge the 438  amount of learning included information learned, accuracy of knowledge, critical 439  thinking/reasoning, and ability to creatively solve problems. In short, research strongly 440  supports the conclusion that cooperative learning in its various forms is superior to 441  lecture alone. 442   443  This same pattern was found by Springer et al.’s (1999) meta-analysis of the 444  effectiveness of small group interactive engagement on learning by undergraduates in 445  STEM courses. Results showed that students who worked in small groups performed at 446  higher levels, had better attitudes, and were more likely to remain STEM majors than 447  students in a traditional lecture class. In a similar review of the literature, Johnson, 448  Johnson, and Smith (2007) reported that cooperative learning (which is incorporated into 449  the case study approach used in the FoS course) tends to have several positive results 450  which included, but were not limited to, improved learning and retention of the material, 451  the more frequent use of critical thinking and metacognition, and improved problem 452  solving. Based on the results of almost a century of research comparing cooperative 453  learning with individual and competitive learning, the FoS course was designed to require 454  active engagement and critical thinking on the part of students as they work together as 455  members of a group to evaluate claims in lecture, in homework assignments, and in lab. 456   457   458  

  11  

Peer Evaluation System 459   460  To ensure student engagement in group work, and a more accurate accounting of a 461  student’s participation within the group, we implemented a peer evaluation system 462  modeled after Larry Michaelsen’s Team-Based Learning approach (Parmelee et al., 463  2012); the system requires students to evaluate the contribution of their team members, 464  for group-related tasks only, for purposes of determining each student’s group score. 465  The use of this type of system, which was highly recommended by faculty affiliated with 466  the Case Study program at SUNY-Buffalo, helps alleviate students’ concern that some 467  members of the group will do all the work, but everyone – including slackers – will get 468  the same grade. 469   470   471  Conclusion 472   473  Based on the assessments used to evaluate our course, the number of students assessed 474  (475 for CAT test; 1443 for the pre-MATE assessment and 1251 for the post-MATE 475  assessment), as well as the length of the assessment period (4-5 years), the results are 476  robust and demonstrate the greater efficacy of the FoS course for teaching scientific 477  reasoning/critical thinking and science literacy relative to traditional approaches to 478  teaching General-Education science courses for non-majors. The success of the FoS 479  course is based on the inclusion of principles of critical thinking and logical fallacies, 480  information pertaining to the limits of perception and memory, the nature of science, and 481  details regarding the scientific method. In addition, it specifically contrasts science with 482  pseudoscience, and uses a case study approach requiring students to use their scientific 483  knowledge to evaluate claims. This encourages higher order thinking and the perceived 484  relevance of the material. Because the course deals with ideas that could potentially 485  create dissonance in many students, it directly addresses this concept through 486  consideration of relevant psychological factors affecting belief. Finally, the use of group 487  work enhances learning, and the use of a peer evaluation system encourages participation 488  of all students when doing group work. 489   490  It is this combination of approaches, which is not part of traditional science courses, that 491  we believe has made the course successful in promoting the development of critical 492  thinking and the acceptance of discomforting scientific theories. It has helped our 493  students better evaluate the innumerable examples of pseudoscientific claims that 494  permeate our society. By adopting this approach to science education, our students have 495  a greater understanding and appreciation of science and why it works, and they are more-496  informed and better-prepared to make decisions than are students who complete more 497  traditional general-education science courses. 498   499   500   501  

  12  

REFERENCES 502   503  American Association for the Advancement of Science. (1993). Project 2061 - 504  Benchmarks for Science Literacy: A Tool for Curriculum Reform, Washington, DC. 505   506  American Association for the Advancement of Science. (2010). Vision and Change: A 507  Call to Action, Washington, DC. 508   509  Alters, B.J., and Nelson, C.E. (2002). Perspective: teaching evolution in higher education. 510  Evolution 56, 1891-1901. 511   512  Benoit, W.L. Persuasion (Communication Institute for Online Scholarship). 513  http://www.cios.org/encyclopedia/persuasion/Esocial_judgment_1theory.htm (accessed 6 514  February 2015). 515   516  Bernstein, D.A., Penner, L.A., Clarke-Stewart, A., and Roy, E.J. (2006). Psychology, 517  Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 518   519  Day, P.K. (2013). Animal Planet's mermaid hoax special draws record ratings. Los 520  Angeles Times. http://articles.latimes.com/2013/may/30/entertainment/la-et-st-animal-521  planet-mermaid-hoax-special-record-ratings-20130530 (accessed 6 February 2015). 522   523  Erwin, P. (2014). Attitudes and Persuasion, New York: Psychology Press. 524   525  Gould, S.J. (1999). Rocks of Ages: Science and Religion in the Fullness of Life, New 526  York: Ballantine. 527   528  Impey, C., Buxner, S., and Antonellis, J. (2012). Non-scientific beliefs among 529  undergraduate students. Astron Educ Rev 11, 1-12. 530   531  Johnson, D.W., Johnson, R.T., and Smith, K. (2007). The state of cooperative learning in 532  postsecondary and professional settings. Educ Psychol Rev 19, 15-29. 533   534  Johnson, M., and Pigliucci, M. (2004). Is KNOWLEDGE of SCIENCE Associated with 535  Higher Skepticism of Pseudoscientific Claims? Am Biol Teach 66, 536-548. 536   537  Lawson, A.E., and Weser, J. (1990). The rejection of nonscientific beliefs about life: 538  effects of instruction and reasoning skills. J Res Sci Teach 27, 589-606. 539   540  Lett, J. (1990). A field guide to critical thinking. Skeptical Inquirer 14, 153-160. 541   542  Martin, M. (1994). Pseudoscience, the paranormal, and science education. Sci & Educ 3, 543  357-371. 544   545  Miller, J.D. (1998). The measurement of civic scientific literacy. Public Underst Sci 7, 546  203-223. 547  

  13  

548  National Science Foundation. (2014). Science and Engineering Indicators 2014, 549  Arlingtong, VA: National Science Board. 550   551  Parmelee, D., Michaelsen, L.K., Cook, S., and Hudes, P.D. (2012). Team-based learning: 552  A practical guide: AMEE Guide No. 65. Med Teach 34, E275-E287. 553   554  Rosenau, J. (2012). Science denial: a guide for scientists. Trends Microbiol 20, 567-569. 555   556  Rowe, M.P. (2015). Crazy about cryptids! An ecological hunt for Nessie and other 557  legendary creatures. National Center for Case Study Teaching in Science. 558  http://sciencecases.lib.buffalo.edu/cs/collection/detail.asp?case_id=779&id=779 559  (accessed 27 June 2015). 560   561  Rutherford, F.J., and Ahlgren, A. (1990). Science for All Americans, Oxford: Oxford 562  University Press. 563   564  Smith, K.A., Sheppard, S.D., Johnson, D.W., and Johnson, R.T. (2005). Pedagogies of 565  engagement: Classroom-based practices. J Eng Educ 94, 87-101. 566   567  Smith, M.U. (2010a). Current Status of Research in Teaching and Learning Evolution: I. 568  Philosophical/Epistemological Issues. Sci & Educ 19, 523-538. 569   570  Smith, M.U. (2010b). Current Status of Research in Teaching and Learning Evolution: II. 571  Pedagogical Issues. Sci & Educ 19, 539-571. 572   573  Springer, L., Stanne, M.E., and Donovan, S.S. (1999). Effects of small-group learning on 574  undergraduates in science, mathematics, engineering, and technology: A meta-analysis. 575  Rev Educ Res 69, 21-51. 576   577   578   579  

  14  

580  2. Example Course Syllabus 581  

 582    583  

Foundations of Science

 584    585  

586  

  15  

Foundations  of  Science  587  BIOL  1436-­‐04;  CRN:  23793  588  Spring  Semester  2015  589  

 590  Course  Number  and  Title:  BIOL  1436-­‐04:    Foundations  of  Science  (4  credits)  591  Class  Time:  Tuesday  and  Thursday  (9:30-­‐10:50)  592  Class  Meeting  Room:  Lee  Drain  Building  (LDB)  207  593  Name:  Dr.  Marcus  Gillespie:   Office  Number:  LDB  200  (Dean’s  Office  area)  594     Office  Hours:  MWF  9:00-­‐11:00  in  LDB  200  (Dean’s  595  

Office)  596     Phone:  294-­‐1945      597     E-­‐mail:  [email protected]  598  

*   I   always   try   to   have   an   “open-­‐door”   policy   as  599  regards  office  hours,  so  please   feel   free  to  call  600  or  come  by  any  time  that  you  have  a  question.    601  

 602  Catalog  Description:    The  course  focuses  on  the  nature  of  science  as  a  reliable  method  of  603  acquiring   knowledge   about   the   natural   world.     Students   will   learn   how   to   apply   key  604  scientific   facts,   concepts,   laws   and   theories   to   distinguish   science   from   non-­‐science,   bad  605  science,   and   psedudoscience   by   analyzing   a   variety   of   claims   and   case   studies.     By  606  employing   an   innovative,   interdisciplinary   approach   to   science   education,   this   course   is  607  designed   to   increase   science   literacy   and   critical   thinking   skills   for   introductory-­‐level  608  students   who   are   not   science   majors.   Students   MUST   enroll   concurrently   in   the  609  corresponding  lab  for  this  course.    Credit:  4  610    611  Course  Description/Rationale:    The  rationale  for  this  course  is  to  enhance  your  scientific  612  literacy   by   making   science   both   interesting   and   relevant.   This   will   be   accomplished   by  613  helping  you  understand  how  science  works  and  how  you  can  apply  science  in  your  daily  life,  614  especially   when   evaluating   extraordinary/unusual   claims   in   which   almost   everyone   is  615  interested  –  including  UFOs,  ESP,  and  mysterious  creatures  like  Big  Foot.      616  

 617  Accordingly,  the  overarching  objectives  of  this  course  are  to  enhance  your  scientific  literacy  618  and  critical  thinking  skills  using  an  integrated,  multidisciplinary  approach  that  draws  upon  619  key  concepts  from  the  natural  sciences,  psychology,  and  critical  thinking.    The  three  broad  620  goals  of  this  integrated  course  are:    621  

 622  1)  to  enhance  your  understanding  and  appreciation  of  science  as  a  proven  and  reliable  623  

method  of  comprehending  the  natural  world,  and  to  help  you  distinguish  scientific  624  from  non-­‐scientific  and  pseudoscientific  ways  of  thinking  about  the  world;    625  

 626    2)  to  provide  you  with  a  more  well-­‐rounded  understanding  of  science  by  teaching  you  627  

the   basic   principles,   facts,   laws,   and   theories   from   the  natural   sciences   and,  when  628  relevant,  from  psychology;    629  

 630  

  16  

3)   to   teach   you   specific   rules   of   critical   thinking   so   that   you   can   use   them,   and   your  631  knowledge  of  science  and  the  scientific  method,   to  make  more   informed  decisions.    632  All  three  goals  are  inseparable  and  are  interwoven  throughout  the  course.  633  

 634  These  three  goals  will  be  accomplished  by  using  information  from  the  natural  sciences,  the  635  scientific   method,   and   rules   of   critical   thinking   to   examine   a   range   of   claims   that   are  636  common  in  our  society.    These  claims  include,  but  are  not   limited  to,  extraordinary  claims  637  and   pseudoscientific   claims   such   as   those   pertaining   to   astrology,   UFOs,   legendary  638  creatures,  the  lost  continent  of  Atlantis,  alternative  medicines,  paranormal  phenomena,  and  639  others.    Through  an  examination  of  these  and  other  topics,  as  well  as  the  evidence  for  key  640  scientific  theories,  you  will  learn  more  about  the  nature  of  science  and  the  scientific  method,  641  how  to  more  reliably  evaluate   the  veracity  of  claims,  and  how  to  avoid  common  errors   in  642  reasoning   that   lead   to  erroneous  conclusions.    This  knowledge  will  help  protect  you   from  643  fraudulent   and  misleading   claims   and   will   enable   you   to   make  more   informed   decisions  644  regarding   issues   of   significance   to   our   society.     Finally,   it   is  my  hope   that   you  will   gain   a  645  greater  appreciation  of  the  beauty  and  wonder  of  the  natural  world  as  revealed  by  science.      646    647  

Upon  successful  completion  of  the  course,  you  will  be  able  to:  648    649  1.   Understand   and   apply   scientific   terminology   pertaining   to   the   nature   and   conduct   of  650  

science,   such   as   hypothesis,   law,   theory,   control   group,   placebo   group,   confirmation  651  bias,  and  double-­‐blind  study;  652  

 653  2.   Apply  methods  of  reasoning  used  by  scientists:   i.e.,   the  scientific  method  based  on  the  654  

requirements   of   falsifiability/testability,   logical   consistency,   comprehensiveness   of  655  evidence,   intellectual   honesty   (objectivity),   replication   of   results,   and   sufficiency   of  656  evidence;  657  

 658  3.   Analyze  and  evaluate  common  logical  fallacies  and  perceptual  biases  that  interfere  with  659  

the   ability   to   draw   reasonable   and/or   correct   conclusions,   as   well   as   the   difference  660  between  facts,  informed  opinions,  and  uninformed  opinions;  661  

 662  4.   Learn   key   concepts   and   theories   from   a   variety   of   scientific   disciplines,   especially  663  

physics,  biology,  and  geology;  664    665  5.   Demonstrate  how  to  distinguish  science  from  pseudoscience  by  scientifically  evaluating  666  

a  wide  variety  of  extraordinary  claims  that  are  common  in  our  culture  today.  667    668  Just  as   importantly,  upon  completion  of   this   course,  we  hope   that  you  will  have  a  greater  669  appreciation  of  the  role  of  science  in  all  of  our  lives  and  the  need  for  scientific  literacy  and  670  critical  thinking  to  help  make  informed  decisions  about  issues  currently  facing  our  society.  671    672  Methods   of   Instruction:   This   course   is   based   on   a   combination   of   traditional   lecture  673  format,  coupled  with  the  use  of  “case  studies”  which  involve  classroom-­‐based  group  work,  674  class  discussions,  homework  assignments,  and  readings.    The  use  of  case  studies  (which  are  675  stories   with   an   educational   purpose)   has   been   shown   to:   significantly   increase   student  676  interest,  enjoyment,  and  involvement  with  a  course;  improve  grades;  and  enhance  students’  677  critical  thinking  ability.    678    679  

  17  

Students   are   required   to   take   the   lab   concurrently  because  the  lecture  680  and  lab  constitute  a  single  course.    The  lab  is  also  based  on  the  use  of  case  studies.  681    682  Course  Materials:  There  are  two  textbooks  for  the  course  and  a  lab  manual.    The  first  book  683  listed   below   (Foundations   of   Science)   is   an   integrated   science   text   that   provides   the  684  scientific  knowledge   for   the   course.    The   second   text   (How   to  Think  about  Weird  Things)  685  provides  an  understanding  of  how  to  use  both  critical   thinking  and  scientific  reasoning   to  686  evaluate  extraordinary/weird  claims.  687    688  

1)  Foundations  of  Science  -­‐  Custom  (This  is  a  custom  edition  of  Conceptual  689  Integrated  Science),  by  Hewitt,  Lyons,  Suchocki,  and  Yeh,  2012,  690  Pearson/Addison-­‐Wesley,  San  Francisco.   ISBN  97812696855350  691  

 692  2)  How  to  Think  About  Weird  Things:  Critical  Thinking  for  a  New  Age  –  693  

7e,  2013,  by  Theodore  Schick  and  Lewis  Vaughn,  McGraw-­‐Hill.   ISBN  694  9780078038365  (paperback).  695  

 696  3)   Lab   manual:   Foundations   of   Science   Lab   Manual   ISBN  697  9780738068237  698  

 699  Scantrons:   You   will   need   approximately   5   of   the   "long"   Scantron   test   forms   (the   100-­‐700  question  version;  50  on  front  and  50  on  back  [form  #882-­‐E])  and  10  of  the  "short"  Scantron  701  test  forms  (15  question  "Quizzstrip";  form  #815-­‐E);  you  might  also  need  a  calculator  for  lab.  702    703  Supplementary   Readings:   If   used,   these  will   be   distributed   either   in   class   or   placed   on  704  BlackBoard.  705    706  

Grading  Criteria  707    708  Because   the   lecture  and   lab  portions  of   the   course  are   considered   to  be  part  of   the  709  same  course,  the  final  course  grade  is  based  on  a  combination  of  lecture  tests,  lecture  710  coursework,   and   lab   work.     In   other   words,   there   is   no   separate   lab   grade.    711  Specifically,   the   lecture   tests   constitute   48%   of   the   grade,   the   lecture   assignments  712  constitute   24.6%,   attendance   constitutes   3%,   and   the   lab   assignments   constitute  713  24.4%.    Because  of  this,  students  must  remain  enrolled  in  both  the  lecture  and  lab  for  714  the   entire   semester;   they   cannot   drop   either   the   lecture   or   the   lab   and   receive   a  715  grade  for  the  course.    The  4  in  the  1436  designation  for  the  course  indicates  that  this  716  is  a  4-­‐credit  course  that  has  a  lab  component.  717    718  Grading   will   be   based   on   3   lecture   exams   (including   the   final),   eight   (8)   sets   of   reading  719  questions,   3   group   case   study   activities,   3   group   homework   assignments,   individual   and  720  group  lab  quiz  grades,  peer  evaluations  by  your  fellow  group  members  in  both  lecture  and  721  lab   (see  details   below),   a   syllabus  quiz,   and   attendance.     You  will   also  be   given   a  critical  722  thinking   assessment   at   the   beginning   and   end   of   the   semester   that   serves   as   extra  723  credit.    This  extra  credit  can  be  very   important   to  your  overall  grade,  so  PLEASE  do  your  724  best  on  both  exams!      725    726  

  18  

Please   note   that   the   number   of   assignments   may   be   changed   slightly   (e.g.,   add   or   drop   a  727  homework  assignment)  if  circumstances  warrant  such  a  change.    If  this  happens,  it  will  have  a  728  slight  effect  on  the  percentage  points  associated  with  each  aspect  of  the  course.  729  

 730  In   an   integrated   course   such   as   this,   each   topic   serves   as   the   foundation   for   subsequent  731  material;   consequently,   students   should   remember   and   understand   all   of   the   basic  732  principles   covered   previously   in   the   course   in  order   to  apply   them   in   the  case  studies  733  and  labs,  and  to  do  well  on  exams.      734    735  

Tests:  There  are  3  major  exams  and  each  will  consist  of  multiple-­‐choice  and  matching-­‐736  type  questions,   and  will   be  worth  750  points.   (Don't  panic!    There  won't  be  375   two-­‐737  point   questions   –   just   a   standard   number   of   questions).   Tests   total   2250   points   and  738  constitute  48%  of  the  total  course  grade.  739  

 740  Reading  Quizzes:  Each  week,  you  will  be  assigned  readings  from  the  books  listed  above  741  and,  in  some  cases,  from  PowerPoint  lectures  that  are  posted  on  BlackBoard  but  which  742  are   not   discussed   in   class.     To   ensure   that   students   read   these   assignments,  a   set   of  743  reading   questions   will   normally   be   given   every   two   weeks   over   the   reading  744  material.     These   assignments   will   be   completed   outside   of   class   online,   in  745  BlackBoard.     You   are   asked   to   use   both   your   books   and   notes   to   complete   the  746  assignments.     Once   available,   you   may   re-­‐take   the   reading   quiz   assignment   as   many  747  times   as   you  wish  before   the  due  date   for   the   reading   assignment.     If   you   experience  748  computer   problems,   please   contact   the   online   helpdesk   (936-­‐294-­‐2780)   before   the  749  assignment   is   due.     The   reading   quizzes  will   be   available   for   a  week,   or  more,   before  750  they  are  due.    They  can  be  retaken  as  many  times  as  you  want  before  the  due  date  751  and  it  is  the  highest  score  that  is  accepted.    The  quizzes  are  randomly  created  from  a  752  pool   of   questions.     The   pool   typically   consists   of   60   to   90   questions.     Because   the  753  computer  randomly  selects  questions  from  the  question  pool  when  it  generates  a  quiz,  754  each   version   of   the   quiz  will   be   different   and  may   consist   of   some  questions   that   are  755  repeated,  as  well  as  new  questions.  The  more  times  you  take  it,  the  more  questions  you  756  will  see  before  the  test.    We  suggest  you  complete  the  reading  quizzes  early  in  case  you  757  have  questions  or  computer  problems.    Because  the  reading  quizzes  are  available  for  an  758  extended  period  of  time  and  can  be  re-­‐taken  before  the  due  date,  late  reading  quizzes  759  will  not  be  accepted.    Again,  we  do  not  recommend  waiting  to  the  last  available  day  to  760  complete  the  reading  quizzes,  as  you  may  experience  computer  and/or  technical  issues.    761  By  attempting  the  quizzes  earlier  in  the  week,  you  will  ensure  you  earn  a  higher  grade  762  and  submit  the  assignment  on  time.  763    764  As  regards  the  reading  assignments,  I  strongly  recommend  that  you  thoroughly  read  765  the  material  –  don’t  just  skim  it.    If  you  try  to  avoid  actually  reading  the  material  766  and,  instead,  merely  skim  the  chapter  until  you  find  something  that  ‘looks  right,’  767  you  will  not   learn   the  material.     This   technique   really  doesn’t  work  because,   as  768  emphasized  throughout  the  course,  facts  presented  in  isolation  from  one  another  769  don’t  make   sense.     You  have   to   see   the   connections  among   the   facts   in  order   to  770  make   sense   of   them   –   and   to   remember   them!   This   is   why   reading   all   of   the  771  material  for  comprehension  does  work!        772  

 773  Pacing  your  work  is  the  key  to  not  being  overwhelmed.  774    775  

  19  

Once   the   quizzes   have   been   submitted,   the   answers   will   be   posted   on   BB.     A  776  screen  will  show  you  which  questions  you  earned  credit  on  and  which  you  missed.    777  In  many  cases,  explanations  are  provided  for  the  answers  as  well.    Many  students  778  print  off  their  completion  reports  for  study  guides.    Please  remember  that  this  course  is  779  about  understanding  and  reasoning  –  not  memorization.    So,  you  should  always  look  780  over   the   completion   reports   to  ensure   that  you  understand   the  concepts.    In   other  781  words,   the   quizzes   and   completion   reports   serve   as   a   study  782  guide  for  the  readings.  783    784  There  are   a   total   of  570   points  possible   for   these  quizzes,   including  20  points   for   the  785  syllabus  quiz.    Together,  these  are  worth  12.2%  of  the  course  grade.      786    787  

Case  Studies  and  Peer  Evaluation  788    789  In   this   class,   students  will   be   divided   into   groups   by   the   instructor.     Each   group  will  790  consist   of   about   5   students  who  will  work   together   throughout   the   semester   on   both  791  case  studies  and  the  three  group  homework  assignments  that  will  be  completed  outside  792  of  class.      As  you  will  see,  group  scores  are  usually  better  than  individual  scores,  and  793  so  this  process  normally   improves  an   individual’s  grade.    In  addition,  group  effort  794  helps  everyone   learn   the  material  better  because  everyone   is   involved   in   teaching  795  one  another.    So,  individuals  normally  do  better  on  tests  as  a  result  of  this  prior  group  796  preparation  process  –  assuming  they  put  in  the  effort.    Group  work  in  lecture  constitutes  797  12.4%  of  the  total  course  grade.    Groups  also  will  be  formed  in  lab,  and  group  work  in  798  lab   constitutes   9.6%   of   the   total   course   grade.     So,   in   total,  group   scores   comprise  799  22%  of  one’s  grade  in  the  course.  800    801  Many  students  are  initially  uneasy  about  the  idea  of  working  in  groups  because  it  802  is  often  the  case  that,  in  previous  classes,  some  members  of  their  group  did  all  or  most  803  of   the  work,  while   others   did   little   or   nothing   –   but   everyone   received   an   equivalent  804  grade.    This  should  not  be  a  problem  in  this  course  because  of  the  importance  of  group  805  peer  evaluation  procedures   to  a  student’s  grade.    The  procedures   for  performing  peer  806  evaluations  are  described  below.  807  

 808  Peer  Evaluation  Process  809  

 810  If  you  are  in  a  group  consisting  of  5  members  (including  yourself),  you  will  be  allotted  811  40   points   to   distribute   among   the   members   of   your   group   following   each   group  812  assignment.    You  do  not  give  points  to  yourself.    (If  you  are  in  a  group  of  6  members,  you  813  will  be  given  50  points,  etc…)     If  you  believe   that  everyone  contributed  equally   to   the  814  group  work,   then  you  would  pay/give  everyone  10  on   the  assignment.     If  everyone   in  815  the   group   feels   the   same  way,   then   everyone   receives   a   total   of   40   points   from   their  816  peers,  which  results   in  an  average  score  of  10  (40/4  =  10).    Please  note   that  10   is   the  817  maximum  number  of  points  that  may  be  awarded.  818    819  You  must  be  fair  in  your  assessments,  but  if  someone  in  your  group  did  not  contribute  820  adequately,  then  you  should  give  them  fewer  points.    If   they  were  not  present  or  did  821  not  contribute  to  an  assignment,  they  should  receive  no  points.  822    823  

  20  

It   is   imperative   that   you   assign   these   scores  PRIVATELY   (NOT   in   front   of   your   team  824  members)   AND   that   you   do   this   on   the   day   the   case   study   was   conducted   or   the  825  assignment  turned  in!    It   is  also  critically  important  that  you  do  not   ‘agree’  to  give  826  each   other   good   scores.     This   is   guaranteed   to   undermine   the   integrity   of   the  827  process  and  will  inevitably  result  in  bad  feelings  if  someone  in  the  group  doesn’t  828  do  his  or  her  fair  share  of  the  work  because  he  or  she  thinks  they’re  going  to  get  a  829  good  score  no  matter  what  they  do.  830    831  Also,  in  order  to  be  fair  and  accurate,  DO  NOT  wait  until  the  midterm  or  the  end  of  the  832  semester   to   assign   these  participation   scores   (for   reasons   that  will   be  apparent  when  833  we   discuss   the   limits   of   peoples'  memories);   rather,  assign   the   scores   immediately  834  after  the  assignment  is  completed.  835    836  At  the  end  of  the  semester,  your  peer  evaluation  score  is  equal  to  the  average  of  837  the   amount   of   peer   evaluation   points   you   received   from   the   members   of   your  838  group  -­‐  converted  to  a  percent.  Accordingly,  an  average  of  10  points  equals  100%;  839  an  average  of  90  equals  90%,  and  so  on.    This  score  is  then  used  to  determine  the  840  number   of  group   points   that   you  will   receive   at   the   end   of   the   semester.     If  you  841  receive  an  average  of  10,  you  will  receive  100%  of  the  points  earned  by  your  group  on  842  the  group  assignments.     If  you  receive  an  average  of  9.2,   then  you  will   receive  92%  of  843  the  group  points,  and  so  on.      844    845  If  you  have  an  average  of  less  than  7,  you  will  not  receive  ANY  of  the  group  points.  846  

 847  Use  the  following  additional  criteria  when  assigning  points:  848  

 849  1) Be  fair!    If  a  person  made  a  genuine  effort  to  contribute,  then  award  10.    Do  not  give  850  

points  to  a  student  for  an  assignment  if  that  student  was  absent  the  day  a  group  851  assignment   was   done   in   class.     And,   do   not   give   any   points   on   a   group   homework  852  assignment  if  the  person  did  not  contribute.      853    854  

2) You  cannot  give  anyone  in  your  group  more  than  10  points.    (This  prevents  people  855  from   giving   their   friends   an   unfairly   large   amount   of   points,   which   would  856  necessarily  hurt  other  members  of  the  group  because  there  would  be  less  points  to  857  distribute  to  other  group  members).      858    859  

3) You  do  not  have   to  distribute  all  of   the  points.    If  someone  does  not  contribute  860  appropriately,  then  give  him  or  her  less  than  10  points.    And,  as  stated  previously,  if  861  someone  is  absent  in  your  group  on  the  day  of  the  assignment,  then  give  him/her  no  862  points;  i.e.,  a  zero.      863    864  

 865  4)  As  stated  above,  anyone  receiving  an  average  of  less  than  7.0  on  his  or  her  peer  866  

evaluation  at  the  end  of  the  semester  will  automatically  lose  his  or  her  group-­‐867  based  points.    So,   for  example,   if  a  student  receives  an  average  of   less  than  7.0   in  868  lecture,   the   student  will   lose   all   of   the  group-­‐based  points   earned  by   the  group   in  869  lecture.    This  amounts  to  a  maximum  580  points  out  of  4690  possible  in  the  course  870  and  constitutes  12.4%  of   the   total  course  grade;   i.e.,   just  over  one   letter  grade.     In  871  the   same  way,   if   a   student   receives   an  average  of   less   than  7.0  on  his  or  her  peer  872  evaluation  in  lab,  the  student  will  lose  all  of  the  group-­‐based  points  in  lab,  which  is  a  873  

  21  

maximum   of   450   points.     This   equals   9.6%   of   the   total   course   grade.     And,   if   a  874  student  received  less  than  7.0  in  both  lecture  and  lab,  they  would  lose  up  to  22%  of  875  the  total  course  points;  i.e.,  more  than  2  letter  grades.    The  point  is,  “Do  your  best  to  876  contribute  to  the  groupJ !”  877  

 878  It   is   the   last   rule   that   normally   ensures   everyone   will   contribute   to   the   group’s  879  efforts.    Also,  the  fact  that  the  score  is  an  average  prevents  anyone  who  might  be  unfair  in  880  the   awarding   of   points   from   single-­‐handedly   undermining   the   final   grade   of   a   group  881  member.    And,   if  one  student  gives  a   score   that   is  much   less   than   those  of  other   students  882  (which   implies   that   it   is   unfair),   I   have   the   option   of   ignoring   that   score.     In   fact,   I   can  883  override  a  low  average  score  if  there  is  evidence  that  the  grade  was  unfairly  assigned  by  the  884  group.    This  serves  as  a  safety  net  for  each  student.      885    886    887  This   type   of   peer-­‐evaluation  method   has   been   used   in  many   universities   and  works   very  888  well.     Students   like   it   because   it   encourages   everyone   to   pull   their   own   weight   and  889  contribute  to  the  group.  890    891  Example:    Imagine  that  a  student  named  Linda  received  peer  evaluation  amounts  in  lecture  892  of  8,  10,  9,  10,  and  10,  for  a  total  of  47,  which  is  an  average  of  9.4,  or  94%.    John  received  all  893  10s   and   so   received   all   of   the   group   points.     Billy,   who   skipped   class,   didn’t   sit   with   his  894  group,  and  contributed  very  little  to  the  group,  received  scores  of  2,  0,  3,  0,  and  2  for  a  total  895  of  7  points  and  an  average  of  1.4,  or  14%.     So,  Linda   received  94%  of   the  group’s  overall  896  grade  for  the  semester.    With  an  average  of  14%,  poor  Billy  lost  580  points,  which  means  his  897  overall   course   grade   dropped   by   1.2   letter   grades.     And,   because   his   average   was   71%  898  before  the  deduction,  Billy  failed  the  course  (71%  -­‐  12.2%  =  58.8%).  This  is  not  the  happy  899  ending  any  of  us  wants  to  see!  900    901  

How  to  Earn  a  Good  Peer  Evaluation  Score  902    903  

1)  Sit  with  your  group  every  day  and  learn  everyone’s  names.    Get  to  know  them.  904    905  2)  Come  prepared  to  contribute  to  the  case  studies  and  quizzes  by  attending  all  classes  (so  906  

you   know  what’s   going   on),   and   reading   the   assigned  material.   In   other  words,  make  907  sure  you  can  and  do  contribute  constructively  to  the  discussions.  908  

 909  3)  Be  positive  and  friendly  and  treat  the  other  members  of  the  group  the  way  you  want  to  910  

be  treated.    In  other  words,  be  courteous  and  respectful  of  others’  comments  and  ideas  -­‐  911  even  if  you  don’t  agree  with  them.    Be  willing  to  accept  that  your  initial  thoughts  might  912  be  incorrect,  but  also  don’t  be  afraid  to  courteously  express  your  views  even  if  they  are  913  different  from  those  of  others  in  the  group.  914  

 915  4)  Contribute  significantly  to  the  group  homework  assignments.    Do  your  part  and  do  it  on  916  

time  –  not  at  the  last  minute.    *  You  should  keep  a  copy  of  what  you  have  written  in  case  917  there   is  a  dispute  regarding  your  contribution.    Remember,   I   can  override   the  group’s  918  evaluation   in   the  unlikely   event   that   it  was  unfair.    However,   this  normally   requires  919  that  you  be  able  to  document  what  you  contributed  so  that  I  can  base  my  decision  920  on  evidence  rather  than  hearsay.  921  

 922  

  22  

5)  Come  to  any  and  all  group  meetings  and,  if  you  absolutely  cannot  be  at  a  meeting  because  923  of  work   or   other   legitimate   schedule   conflicts,  make   sure   you   keep   in   touch  with   the  924  group  via  e-­‐mail,  Facebook,  or  phone  and   let   them  know  ahead  of  time   that  you  can’t  925  come.    Most  people  will  understand  if  they  know  someone  has  legitimate  reasons  for  not  926  attending  a  meeting.    But,  you  need  to  contribute  ideas,  written  material,  etc.,  even  if  you  927  can’t  join  the  group  in  person.  928  

 929  An   initial,   trial   peer   evaluation  will   be   done   approximately   half   way   through   the  930  semester.    This  evaluation  will  NOT  count  as  part  of  the  grade  and  will  serve  only  to  give  931  each  person  feedback  from  the  members  of  his  or  her  group  so  that  he  or  she  can  correct  932  any  problems  that  might  exist.  933    934  Very  important  note:  Although  points  are  not  given  for  completing  peer  evaluations,  935  points  will  be  deducted  if  the  rules  described  above  were  not  followed  and/or  if  you  936  do  not  submit  a  peer  evaluation  for  your  group  members.    Specifically,  40  points  will  937  be  deducted  for  not  submitting  a  peer  evaluation  when  it  is  requested.    So,  please  do  938  the  evaluation!  939  

 940  Homework  Assignments  941  

 942  There  will   be   three  group   homework   assignments  worth   a   total   of   400   points   (8.5%   of  943  course   grade).   These   assignments   entail   analyses   of   actual   arguments   and   claims.   For  944  example,   the   first   assignment   involves   evaluating   a   series   of   arguments.     The   second  945  assignment  entails  an  analysis  of  a  product  that  is  available  to  “help  maintain  your  health”.    946  The  question  your  group  will   try   to  answer   is,   “Does   it  work?”     “Is   it  based  on   science  or  947  pseudoscience?”    Doing   these   assignments  will   help   you   evaluate   the   innumerable   claims  948  you  will  encounter  in  your  life.  949    950  The   third   assignment   is   known   as   FiLCHeRS   and   is   worth   220   points.   This   assignment  951  involves  the  application  of  the  FiLCHeRS  rules  (which  are  discussed  in  class)  to  an  analysis  952  of  an  extraordinary  claim  you  will  be  assigned  to  evaluate.    The  assignment  consists  of  both  953  multiple  choice  and  short  answer  questions  and  is  a  capstone  assignment  in  that  it  entails  954  using  information  learned  throughout  the  course  to  evaluate  the  claim.  955  

Attendance  and  Make-­‐up  Policies  956    957  This   course   abides   by   University   Policy   and   Regulations   concerning   attendance   (See   the  958  Undergraduate   Catalog).     Accordingly,   “regular   and   punctual   attendance"   is   expected   of  959  each  student  at  Sam  Houston  State  University:  960    961  In   a   course   such   as   this,   in  which   group   effort   is   a   significant   part   of   the   grade,   students  962  genuinely  need  to  come  to  class  so  that  they  can  contribute  to  their  group’s  success.    Those  963  who   are   prepared   and   contribute   positively   will   be   highly   valued   by   their   group!     This  964  course  also  moves  quickly  and  many   ideas  build  on  one  another  and  are  used  throughout  965  the  course.    So,  if  a  student  misses  class,  he  or  she  will  almost  certainly  be  hurt  academically.    966  In  short,  attendance  matters  and  is  required.      967    968  Because  attendance  is  so  important,  I  give  each  student  150  points  at  the  beginning  of  the  969  semester.    Although  this  is  part  of  the  total  points  possible  for  the  course,  it  is  non-­‐academic  970  (i.e.,   not   dependent   on   tests   and   assignments)   and   so   serves   as   a  grade  cushion.     All   you  971  

  23  

have  to  do  to  keep  these  points  is  to  come  to  class.    How  much  easier  can  it  get!    However,  972  because   attendance   is   so   important,   students   will   lose   30   points   for   each   unexcused  973  absence   after   the   first   absence.     (In   order   for   an   absence   to   be   excused,   some   form   of  974  documentation   must   be   provided   to   show   that   it   was   legitimate;   this   can   include   a  975  physician's   note,   a   funeral   announcement,   legal   notice,   etc.     The   documentation  must   be  976  provided  within  one  week  of  returning  to  class.)    Also,  tardies  can  be  counted  as  absences.    977  So,  if  a  student  misses  6  times,  is  tardy  6  times,  or  has  some  combination  thereof  (e.g.,  three  978  unexcused  absences  and   three   tardies),  he  or   she  will   lose   their   150  points,  which  equals  979  3.1%  of  his  or  her  total  grade.    980    981  If  someone  misses  more  than  6  times,  that  student  automatically  FAILS  THE  COURSE  982    983  

So,  please  come  to  class!  984  Examples  985  

  0-­‐1  absence/tardies  –  no  point  deduction  986     2  absences/tardies  –  30  points    987     3  absences/tardies  –  60  points    988     4  absences/tardies  –  90  points    989     5  absences/tardies  –  120  points    990     6  absences/tardies  –  150  points  (3.1%)  991     >  6  absences/tardies  =  F  992    993  Please  understand  that   these  policies  are   intended  to  prevent  students   from  994  failing  the  class  because  of  skipping  so  many  classes  that  they  can’t  learn  the  995  material.     In   effect,   these   attendance   rules  keep  most   students  on   track  and  996  reduces  the  number  of  students  that  might  otherwise  fail  the  course.  997    998  

1.  In  addition  to  the  required  attendance/tardy  policy,  it   is  important  that  you  stay  for  999  the  entire  class  -­‐-­‐  please  do  not   leave   the   class   room  early  unless  you  are  sick  or  1000  have  cleared  it  with  me  before  class  begins.    Students  can  be  counted  absent  if  they  1001  leave  the  class  early  without  permission.  1002  

 1003  2.  If  you  know  you  will  miss  a  class  (because  of  an  excusable  event,  such  as  an  "away"  1004  baseball  game  and  you  are  a  member  of  SHSU's  baseball  team),  let  me  know  ahead  of  1005  time  and  we  can  make  arrangements  for  a  make-­‐up  exam.  1006  

       1007        If,   for  whatever  reason,  you  miss   an   exam,  please  contact  me  as  soon  as  possible  to  1008  determine  if  and  when  the  exam  may  be  made-­‐up.    Make-­‐up  exercises  and  exams  are  1009  only   allowed   based   on   my   approval,   and   only   if   you   have   contacted   me   within   a  1010  reasonable  amount  of  time  (one  day!)  following  the  absence.      1011  

 1012  3.  Late  Work  Policy:  The  three  group  homework  assignments  are  to  be  turned  in  1013  at   the  beginning  of  class  (on  the  day  they  are  due).    These  assignments  can  be  1014  handed   in  a  maximum  of  one   class  period  after   the  due  date;  however,  points  1015  will  be  deducted  depending  upon  how  late   it   is  submitted.     If,   for  example,   the  1016  homework   assignment   is   due   on   Tuesday   at   9:30   AM,   but   is   handed   in   on  1017  Tuesday  at  1  PM,  5%  will  be  deducted.    If  the  paper  is  turned  in  on  Wednesday,  1018  10%  of  the  value  of  the  assignment  will  be  subtracted.    And  if  it  is  submitted  at  1019  

  24  

the  beginning  of   the  class  on  the  Thursday   immediately   following  the  Tuesday  1020  due   date/time,   20%  will   be   deducted.     It  must   be   emphasized   that,   after   that  1021  date,  the  assignment  cannot  be  turned  in  and  no  grade  will  be  received  for  the  1022  assignment.  1023  

 1024  *   This   late   policy   does   not   apply   to   the   reading   quizzes.     These   cannot   be  1025  completed  after  the  due  date  and  time.  1026  

 1027  Please   check   BlackBoard   as   soon   as   the   grades   are   posted.     Students   have   a  1028  maximum  of   two  weeks  to  contest  a  grade.    For  example,  if  the  grade  is  incorrect,  1029  or  if   it  was  not  posted,  you  need  to  notify  me  within  two  weeks  of  my  posting  of  the  1030  grade.    After  two  weeks,  if  no  errors  have  been  reported  to  me,  the  grade  stands  as  is.  1031  

 1032  What  happens  if  you  miss  a  Case  Study?  If  you  miss  a  case  study  in  lecture  because  of  1033  an  excused  absence,  you  can  partially  make   it  up  by  completing   it  on  your  own.    This  will  1034  entail   writing   an   essay   response   to   any   questions   that   may   have   been   asked   in   class  1035  regarding   the  case,   as  well   as   taking   the  quiz  over   the  case   study.     The  maximum   score  1036  that   a   student   can   achieve   is   the   score   earned   on   the   assignment,   OR   the   group’s  1037  score  –  whichever  is  lower.    This  policy  ensures  that  your  grade  is  tied  to  the  group  grade,  1038  but   it  also  provides  some  grade   ‘cushion’   for   those   that  may  be  sick  or  unable   to  come   to  1039  class  on   the  day  of   the   case   study,  while  also  discouraging   students   from  simply   skipping  1040  the  day  of  a  case  study.    Please  remember  that  your  group  must  (based  on  the  rules  for  1041  peer  evaluations)  give  you  a  zero   for  group  participation  on  the  case  study   if  you  are  1042  absent.  1043    1044  

Lab  Grades  1045    1046  

The  lab  grade  will  consist  of  both  individual  scores  and  group-­‐derived  scores.    Most  of  1047  the  labs  will  be  based  on  case  studies  that  will   involve  instructor-­‐led  discussions  in  which  1048  members   of   groups   work   together   to   develop   responses,   propose   hypotheses   and  1049  experimental   designs,   or   offer   explanations   for   what   has   been   reported   or   observed.     In  1050  short,  labs  involve  a  lot  of  discussion  –  both  within  each  group  and  among  groups.    The  lab  1051  instructor   will   facilitate   these   discussions.     The   discussions   make   the   labs   fun   and  1052  interesting   because   they   are   not   based   simply   on   rote  memorization   and   fill-­‐in-­‐the-­‐blank  1053  activities;  rather,  they  involve  group  discussion  and  exploration  of  topics.  1054    1055  At  the  beginning  of  the  lab,  each  student  will  be  given  a  short,  Individual  Lab  1056  Quiz  (ILQ)  over  the  information  provided  in  the  lab  readings  and  relevant  readings  1057  assigned  in  lecture.    This  is  intended  to  ensure  that  everyone  reads  the  lab  exercise  1058  and  textbook  background  readings  (listed  on  the  lecture  syllabus)  before  coming  1059  to  class  so  they  will  be  prepared  for  the  lab  discussion.    The  quiz  will  include  some  1060  vocabulary  terms  listed  at  the  end  of  the  lab  exercise  and  related  lecture  notes  and  1061  readings.  Questions  will  be  multiple-­‐choice  and/or  short  answer  essay.  1062    1063  At  the  end  of  the  lab,  each  group  will  be  given  a  Group  Lab  Quiz  (GLQ)  regarding  1064  the  information  covered  in  lab.    The  group  will  work  together  as  a  team  to  complete  1065  it.     Groups  will   be   created   by   the   lab   instructor   at   the   beginning   of   the   year.   The  1066  purpose  for  the  group  work  is  to  enhance  understanding  of  the  material  by  having  1067  

  25  

group  members  help  teach  each  other  the  material  and  reinforce  the  key  concepts  1068  covered  in  the  lab.    The  group  scores  obtained  over  the  semester  will  be  adjusted  by  1069  the   peer   evaluation   score   the   student   receives   from   his/her   peers   using   the  1070  procedures  outlined  on  the  peer  evaluation  form.      1071    1072  A   total  of  10   lab  case  studies  will  be  completed  and  students  will   be   allowed   to  1073  drop   both   their   lowest   individual   and   their   lowest   group   lab   grade.    1074  Accordingly,   the   lab   quizzes,   both   individual   and   group,   total   1140   points.     These  1075  points  will  account  for  approximately  24.4%  of  your  overall  course  grade.  1076    1077  In  summary,  students  can  earn  690  individual  points  and  450  group  points.  Please  1078  remember   that,   in   this   course,   the   lecture   and   lab   grades   are   combined   to  1079  determine   your   overall   course   grade.     Thus,   there   is   a   total   of   1140   lab   points  1080  possible  in  lab.  1081    1082  In   total,   the   lab   portion   of   the   course   grade   constitutes   24.4%   of   your   grade   –   which   is  1083  almost   identical   to   the   amount   that  would   be   earned   relative   to   a   standard   lecture   +   lab  1084  class.  In  other  words,  if  you  took  a  science  class  in  which  the  lecture  and  lab  were  separate,  1085  and   earned   4   hours   of   credit   for   this   combination,   the   lab   would   constitute   1   of   4   total  1086  hours,  or  25%  of  the  grade  component  for  the  science  class.    However,  please  remember  1087  that,  in  this  course,  the  lecture  and  lab  grades  are  combined  to  determine  your  overall  1088  course  grade.      1089    1090  

Extra  Credit  1091    1092  At  both  the  beginning  and  end  of  the  semester,  you  will  be  given  the  opportunity  to  1093  earn  extra   credit  worth  up   to  9%  of   the   total   course  grade!      That’s  422  points!    This  1094  opportunity   to   significantly   improve   your   grade  will   be   in   the   form   of   a   critical   thinking  1095  assessment   –   either   the   CAT   assessment   or   the   FSE   assessment.     This   assessment,  which  1096  will   be   given   in   lab,   is   required   by   the   University’s   reaccreditation   requirements.     It   is  1097  extremely  important  that  you  do  your  best  on  both  exams  because  your  scores  reflect  upon  1098  the  university  and   indicates  how  well  our  students  are  doing  relative   to  students  at  other  1099  universities  in  the  United  States.    It’s  your  chance  to  not  only  earn  a  lot  of  bonus  points,  but  1100  also  to  make  SHSU  look  good!    So,  please  do  your  best.      1101    1102  Each  assessment  is  worth  120  points.    The  grading  procedure  for  this  assessment  consists  1103  of  simply  adding   the   two  scores   together.    However,   if   the  sum  of   the   two  scores   is  above  1104  144  points,  a  multiplier  is  used  to  further  increase  the  number  of  points  you  can  earn.  (It’s  a  1105  bit  like  the  multiplier  used  on  some  lotteries.)  This  means  that  the  procedure  for  awarding  1106  bonus  points  is  very  generous.      1107    1108  For   example,   if   you   made   a   combined   score   of   110   points   on   the   assessments,   the   110  1109  points  will  be  added   to  your  grade.    And,   if   your   total   on   both   assessments   is   greater  1110  than   144   points,   you   will   receive   even   more   extra   credit   points!     The   amount   you  1111  would  receive  for  scoring  above  144  points  is  equal  to  the  number  of  points  you  earn  above  1112  144,  multiplied  by  2   –  with   a  maximum   of   190   extra   points   possible.     (190   points   is  1113  equal  to  4%  of  the  course  grade.)    So,  if  you  received  the  maximum  number  points  on  1114  these  exams,  you  would  receive  a  grand  total  of  430  bonus  points  (9%  of  the  course  1115  

  26  

grade),  which  is  almost  an  entire  letter  grade!    This  is  why  it’s  important  to  do  your  best  1116  on  both  assessments.  1117    1118  For  example,   if  you  made  a  70  the  first  time  you  took  the  assessment  and  a  95  the  second  1119  time,  you  would  have  earned  a  total  of  165  points.    Because  the  combined  score  for  the  two  1120  assessments  is  21  points  more  than  144  points,  the  multiplier  is  used  and  you  would  earn  1121  42  more  bonus  points  in  addition  to  the  165  you’d  already  earned:  165  -­‐  144  =  21;  21  x  2  =  1122  42;  165  +  42  =  207  total  bonus  points).    *Because  these  are  bonus  points,  they  would  NOT  1123  be  adjusted  by  a  peer  score.    They’re  all  yours!  1124    1125  Because  you  are  being  asked  to  take  this  critical  thinking  assessment  at  the  beginning  of  the  1126  semester   (the  pre-­‐test)  before  you  have  been   taught   the   course  material,  we  do  not  want  1127  you  to  be  discouraged  if  you  do  not  do  as  well  as  you  might  have  expected  on  the  pre-­‐test.    1128  That   is  why  we  give  additional  bonus  points   if  you  achieve  a  combined  score  above  144  –  1129  which  is  a  mere  60%  of  the  possible  points  on  the  assessments!    Because  the  score  on  the  1130  second  assessment   (the  post-­‐test)  SHOULD   improve   if   you   learn   from   the   course   and  1131  you  do  your  best  on  the  assessment,  you  can  easily  make  a  good  overall  score  and  earn  a  1132  significant  number  of  bonus  points.    You  should  know  that  a  few  students  have  actually  1133  earned  the  maximum  number  of  bonus  points  possible!  1134    1135  Please  note  that  these  assessments  are  the  only  possible  sources  of  extra  credit   in  the  1136  course.    In  other  words,  no  individual  extra  credit  is  given  and,  with  the  exception  of  one  1137  individual  and  one  group  lab  grade,  no  other  grades  are  dropped.  1138  

 1139  Grade  Determination  1140  

 1141  Your   grade   is   based   on   the   percentage   of   points   earned   relative   to   the  1142  maximum  number  possible  for  the  course  (4,690).  The  percentage  of  the  total  1143  possible  points  determined  by  individual  effort  is  78%  (3658  out  of  4690  possible),  1144  and   that   determined   by   group   effort   is   22%   (1143   out   of   4690   possible).   So,  1145  although  group  effort  is  fundamentally  important  to  the  design  of  the  course  and  to  1146  the   way   in   which   labs   and   case   studies   are   run,   your   grade   is   determined  1147  primarily  by  your  individual  scores;  i.e.,  by  your  individual  effort.   In  short,  you  1148  are   ultimately   responsible   for   the   majority   of   the   grade   points   you   earn   in   the  1149  course.   The  group  work   should  help   you  do  better  by  helping  you   learn   the  1150  material  more  thoroughly.  1151    1152  Please  note  that  The  State  of  Texas  REQUIRES  that  universities  have  students  engage  1153  in   group   activities   because   it   is   crucial   to   their   career   preparation.     This   is   an  1154  additional   reason   why   group   work   is   required   and   why   you   will   evaluate   one  1155  another’s  contributions  to  the  group.  1156    1157  All  of  the  tests  and  assignments  for  the  course,   including  lab  assignments,  are  listed  in  the  1158  Grade   Form   on   page   13.   To   keep   track   of   your   grades,  you   need   to   record   each   and  1159  every  grade  you  receive  on  this  form.    (Please  note  that  Black  Board  will  not  calculate  1160  your  grade;  it’s  simply  a  place  to  store  the  grades  for  individual  assignments.)      1161    1162  Using   the   form   below,   you   can   estimate   your   grade   at   any   point   in   the   semester   by  1163  comparing  the  total  number  of  points  you  have  earned  to-­‐date  to  the  total  number  of  points  1164  

  27  

possible  at  that  point  in  the  course.    You  can  only  estimate  the  grade  because,  prior  to  the  1165  end   of   the   semester,   your   score   on   group  work  will   not   be   adjusted   based   on   your   peer  1166  evaluations.    However,  you  should  have  a  very  good  sense  of  how  you  are  doing  based  on  1167  the   original,   unadjusted   group   scores   coupled   with   your   awareness   of   your  1168  participation  in  the  group.      1169  

Abbreviations  used  in  grade  form  1170    1171  Lecture  component         Lab  Component  1172  CS  =  Case  Study       ILQ  =  Individual  Lab  1173  Quiz  1174  RQ  =  Reading  Quiz         GLQ  =  Group  Lab  Quiz  1175  SQ  =  Syllabus  Quiz    1176  HW  =  Homework    1177  

  CT  =  Critical  Thinking  Test  1178  1179  

  28  

Grade  Record  Form  1180  I.  Lecture  Grades  (75.7%  of  total)   II.  Lab  Scores  (24.4%  of  total)  1181    1182  Individual  grades   1.  Individual  Lab  Scores  (14.8%)  1183  1.  Test  grades  (48%)   ILQ  1  _____  (74)    1184  Test  1  ______  (750)   ILQ  2  _____  (77)    1185  Test  2  ______  (750)   ILQ  3  _____  (77)    1186  Test  3  ______  (750)   ILQ  4  _____  (77)    1187  

A.  Total  test  =  _______   ILQ  5  _____  (77)    1188     ILQ  6  _____  (77)    1189  

2.  Reading  Quizzes  (12.2%)   ILQ  7  _____  (77)    1190  SQ  1  ______  (20)   ILQ  8  _____  (77)    1191  RQ  1  ______  (60)   ILQ  9  _____  (77)    1192  RQ  2  ______  (40)   ILQ  10  ____  (77)    1193  RQ  3  ______  (60)                                                                      1194  RQ  4  ______  (60)                                                                      F.Total  Individual  ____  (Drop  lowest  ILQ)  1195  RQ  5  ______  (60)                  1196  RQ  6  ______  (40)    1197  EnvHW_____(50)  1198  RQ  7  ______  (60)  1199  RQ  8  ______  (60)   2.  Group  lab  Scores  (9.6%)  1200  RQ  9  ______  (60)   GLQ  1  _____  (50)  1201  B.  Total  Quiz  =  ______   GLQ  2  _____  (50)  1202  

  GLQ  3  _____  (50)  1203  3.  Group  Grades  in  Lecture  (12.4%)   GLQ  4  _____  (50)  1204  

               NASA  CS              _____  (60)         GLQ  5  _____  (50)  1205                  Xango  CS              _____  (60)   GLQ  6  _____  (50)  1206                  Argument  HW_____  (90)         GLQ  7  _____  (50)  1207                    Water  HW          _____  (90)   GLQ  8  _____  (50)  1208  Vacc/Autism  CS        _____  (60)   GLQ  9  _____  (50)  1209    FiLCHeRS  HW          _____  (220)   GLQ  10  ____  (50)  1210    1211  Total  group  lecture  =  _______                      Total  group  ______  (Drop  lowest  GLQ)  1212    1213  

C.  Total  group  x  peer  score  =  ______              G.  Total  group  x  peer  score  _____  1214        1215  

4.  Attendance  (150  pts.)  (3.1%)  1216     -­‐30  for  each  unexcused  absence  or  tardy  -­‐  after  the  first  absence  or  tardy  1217  

D.  Total  Attendance  ____  (150  max.  if  no  absences)  1218    1219  5.     Extra  Credit:  Critical  Thinking  Assessment  Scores  (worth  up  to  9%)    1220  

      CT  pre-­‐test  _____  (120  max)    1221       CT  post-­‐test  ____  (120  max)    1222  

E.  Total  CT  points  _____  1223    1224  

  29  

To  obtain  your  final  grade  (percent),  add  the  totals  labeled  A,  B,  C,  1225  D,  E,  F  and  G,  divide  by  4690,  and  multiply  by  100.    1226  

Point  range  for  final  course  grade  1227    1228  

    A  =  4221-­‐4690   D  =  2814-­‐3282  1229       B  =  3752-­‐4220   F  =  less  than  2814  1230       C  =  3283-­‐3751  1231  

 1232  Academic   Honesty:     All   students   are   expected   to   engage   in   all   academic   pursuits   in   a  1233  manner   that   is  above  reproach.    Students  are  expected   to  maintain   complete  honesty  and  1234  integrity   in   academic   experiences   both   in   and   out   of   the   classroom.     Any   student   found  1235  guilty  of  dishonesty  in  any  phase  of  academic  work  will  be  subject  to  disciplinary  action  that  1236  is  consistent  with  university  policies.    Please  read  the  following:  1237    1238  

1) Students  are  encouraged  to  study  in  groups  to  prepare  for  tests.    However,  “group  1239  effort”   is   definitely   not   permitted   when   taking   exams!     This   will   result   in   an  1240  automatic  zero  on  a  test.    Two  such  occurrences  will  result  in  an  F  in  the  course.  1241  

 1242  Proper   Course   Behavior:   All   of   these   rules   are   standard   and   are   based   on   common  1243  courtesy,   respect,   and   honesty   –   all   of  which   are   necessary   to   ensure   a   positive   learning  1244  environment.  1245    1246  

1) Students   will   refrain   from   behavior   in   the   classroom   that   intentionally   or  1247  unintentionally  disrupts  the  learning  process  and,  thus,  impedes  the  mission  of  the  1248  university.     Cellular   telephones,   pagers   and   ALL   other   electronic   communication  1249  devices  must  be  turned  off  before  class  begins.      1250    1251  Students   are   prohibited   from   eating   or   drinking   in   class,   using   tobacco   products,  1252  making   offensive   remarks,   reading   newspapers,   sleeping,   talking   at   inappropriate  1253  times,  wearing  inappropriate  clothing,  or  engaging  in  any  other  form  of  distraction.    1254  Inappropriate   behavior   in   the   classroom   will   result   in   a   directive   to   leave   class.    1255  Students  who  are  especially  disruptive  also  may  be  reported  to  the  Dean  of  Students  1256  for  disciplinary  action  in  accordance  with  university  policy.  1257  

 1258  2) Please  do  not  use  cell  phones  or  I-­‐pods  in  class  at  any  time,  unless  instructed  to  1259  

do  so,  because  it  distracts  not  only  you,  but  the  instructor  and  other  students.    If  1260  you  use  a  laptop  computer  or  I-­‐Pad,  please  use  it  only  to  access  the  lectures.  1261    1262  If   you   have   an   emergency-­‐type   situation   that   requires   that   you   be   in   cell  1263  phone  contact  with  someone  (e.g.,  relative  in  hospital;  spouse  overseas  in  the  1264  military),   then   please   tell   me   before   class   begins   and   put   the   phone   in   the  1265  vibrate  mode.  1266  

     1267  3) Please  come  to  class  on  time—there  is  no  reason  to  be  late  to  class  on  a  frequent  1268  

basis.      1269    1270  

4) Please   remain   in   class   until   it   is   finished   because   leaving   early   disrupts   the  1271  class   and   will   count   as   an   absence   unless   you   have   cleared   it   with   me,   or  1272  

  30  

unless  it  is  an  emergency.    If  you  have  a  job  that  overlaps  with  class  time,  then  you  1273  need  to  drop  the  course  or  change  your  work  schedule.    1274  

 1275  5) Please  remove  hats  during  exams.  1276    1277  6) For  obvious  reasons,  students  CANNOT  LEAVE  THE  ROOM  DURNING  AN  1278  

EXAM  and  then  return.    If  this  happens,  the  test  will  be  taken  up  and  your  grade  1279  will  be  based  on  the  portion  of  the  test  that  you  completed.    If  you  have  a  cold  or  1280  allergy,   please   bring   tissues   to   class   so   that   you   won’t   want   to   leave   to   get  1281  tissues  during  the  test.  1282  

 1283  Study  Tips:  Please  read  and  follow  these  tips  to  enhance  your  grade  in  the  course.    I  want  1284  you  to  do  well!  1285    1286  1.  This  course  deals  with  arguments  and  evidence  for  or  against  certain  claims.    So,  1287  in  order  to  study,  you  should  imagine  that  you  have  been  asked  to  write  an  essay  1288  in  which  you  must  present  evidence  and  arguments  to  either  support  or  refute  a  1289  claim.    This  helps  you   learn  and  retain   the  material  –  and   it  makes   the   learning  1290  process  more  fun  and  interesting.    This  approach  amounts  to  pretending  that  you  1291  are  teaching  the  material  to  someone  else.    You  cannot  simply  memorize  your  notes  1292  and   definitions   and   expect   to   do   well   on   the   tests.     You   must   truly   understand   the  1293  material  in  order  to  obtain  a  good  grade.  1294  

 1295  2.  Take  notes.  Although  significant  amount  of  the  information  covered  in  class  is  presented  1296  in  abbreviated  form  on  the  Power  Point  lectures,  you  will  almost  certainly  need  to  write  1297  additional   notes   in   order   to   recall,   integrate,   and   understand   the   information.     In  1298  addition,  note  taking  requires  active  listening;  i.e.,  a  conscious  attempt  to  determine  what  1299  is  important  and  to  look  for  connections  between  ideas.    Lectures  aren’t  simply  a  bunch  1300  of   facts   and   definitions   thrown   together.     In   the   class,   the   lectures   are   arguments  1301  either  for  or  against  certain  claims  and  you’ll  need  to  understand  the  arguments.      1302  

 1303  3.  Review  your  notes  before   the  next  class.    Constant  reviewing  will  help  you  learn  the  1304  material  in  smaller  ‘bites’  of  information  –  which  makes  it  much  easier  to  learn.    Just  as  1305  importantly,   reviewing   your   notes   before   the   next   lecture   will   help   you   see   how   the  1306  previous  material  connects  with  the  material  to  be  covered  in  the  upcoming  class.  1307  

 1308  4.  This   course   requires   that   students   learn  a   significant  amount  of  material  on   their  own,  1309  independent   from   the   lecture  material.   Furthermore,   the   reading  quizzes   are  based  on  1310  the   reading  material!    So,  reading   the   textbooks   and   reader   for   this   course   really,  1311  truly   is   a   necessity.    The  ability  to   learn  on  your  own  is  one  of   the  most   important  1312  skills   you   will   learn   in   college,   and   it   is   one   of   the   most   important   skills   that  1313  employers  look  for  in  job  candidates.  1314  

 1315  5.  When  it  comes  time  to  review  for  an  exam,  first  read  the  highlighted  portions  of  the  text,  1316  then  concentrate  on  your  notes.    You  might  also  want  to  follow  the  procedures  below:  1317  

 1318  a.   As   you   review   your   notes,   first   concentrate   on   absorbing   the   key   ideas   and  1319  

understanding  the  organization  of  the  material  -­‐  why  certain  ideas  followed  others  in  1320  the  class  and  how  they  are  related.  1321  

 1322  

  31  

b.  Once  this  is  done,  begin  to  focus  on  the  details  -­‐  the  “whys.”    As  stated  above,  tests   in  1323  this  course  are  absolutely  not  based  on  the  mere  memorization  of  definitions,  or  1324  on  the  recognition  of  verbatim  statements  from  lecture;  rather,  the  test  questions  1325  assume   you   already   know   the   definitions   and   that   you   understand   the   concepts  1326  discussed   in   lecture.     So,   you   will   not   be   asked   definitions;   rather   you   will   be  1327  asked  to  apply  facts  and  principles,  i.e.,  to  think  with  the  information  you  have  1328  learned.     Of   course,   you   have   to   know   the   definitions   to   begin   the   process   of  1329  answering  questions;  so,  by  all  means,  learn  the  definitions  as  the  first  step  in  learning  1330  the  materialJ  1331  

 1332  Visitors  in  the  Classroom:  Unannounced  visitors  to  the  classroom  must  present  a  current,  1333  official  SHSU  identification  card  to  be  permitted  in  the  classroom.    They  must  not  present  a  1334  disruption  to  the  class  by  their  attendance.    If  the  visitor  is  not  a  registered  student,  it  is  at  1335  the   instructor's   discretion   whether   or   not   the   visitor   will   be   allowed   to   remain   in   the  1336  classroom.     This   policy   is   not   intended   to   discourage   occasional   visiting   of   classes   by  1337  responsible  persons.    1338    1339  Americans  with  Disabilities  Act:    Any  student  seeking  accommodations  should  go  to  the  1340  Counseling  Center  and  Services  for  Students  with  Disabilities  at  the  very  beginning  of  the  1341  semester   and   complete   a   form   that   will   grant   permission   to   receive   special  1342  accommodations.     Please   do   not   wait   until   test   day   to   do   this   –   the   request   for  1343  accommodations  must   be   done   at   the   beginning   of   the   semester   and   students   that  1344  have   permission   to   use   the   services   at   the   Counseling   Center   must   make  1345  appointments  several  days  ahead  of  scheduled  tests.    Walk-­‐ins  aren’t  permitted.    Also,  1346  please  be  sure  to  send  me  an  e-­‐mail  two  days  before  an  exam  to  remind  me  to  take  the  test  1347  to  the  Counseling  Center.  1348    1349  Religious  Holy  Days:  If  a  student  desires  to  be  excused  from  class,  assignment,  or  a  test  on  1350  a  religious  holy  day,  then  the  student  must  notify  the  instructor  of  each  scheduled  class  that  1351  he/she  will  be  absent   for  religious  reasons.     In  such  cases,   the  student  will  be  required  to  1352  take  the  test  or  submit  the  assignment  early—unless  there  are  good  reasons  for  not  being  1353  able  to  do  so  and  the  instructor  has  agreed  to  those  reasons.  1354    1355  Special   Circumstances:     If   unusual   circumstances   arise   during   the   semester,   such   as   a  1356  medical  problem,  death  in  the  family,  etc.,  which  adversely  affects  your  attendance  PLEASE  1357  discuss   this  with  me   immediately   and  provide  documentation.    Don’t  wait   until   the  1358  end   of   the   semester   to   discuss   the   problem  with  me.     If  you  keep  me   informed,   I  will  1359  gladly   do   my   best   to   accommodate   your   situation.     However,   please   understand   that,  1360  because  of  the  nature  of  the  course,  there  are  limits  as  to  how  much  can  be  excused  and  so,  1361  at  some  point,  it  may  be  necessary  for  you  to  drop  the  course.    Also,  if  you  wait  until  after-­‐1362  the-­‐fact,   at   the   end   of   the   semester,   to   let   me   know   that   you   were   experiencing   these  1363  adverse  circumstances,  there  is  nothing  I  can  do  about  it  at  that  time.    I  cannot  retroactively  1364  make   accommodations   and   I   do   not   give   extra   credit   assignments   to   make   up   for   grade  1365  deficiencies.  1366    1367  SCHEDULE:    *This  schedule  is  subject  to  change  at  any  time  based  on  class  progress.  1368  Major  lecture  topics  are  listed  in  bold-­‐face,  black  font.      1369    1370  

Reading  assignments  are  in  green  font  and  include    1371  

  32  

all  material  covered  since  the  preceding  quiz  1372  Case  studies  are  in  blue  font.  1373  Tests  are  in  orange  font.  1374  

Reading  Quizzes  are  in  red  font.  1375  Homework  assignments  are  in  purple  font.  1376  

 1377  Please   note   that   some   of   the   readings   include   only   sections   of   a   chapter  1378  (indicated   by   the   word   “part”),   whereas   others   include   the   entire   chapter,  1379  indicated   by   the  word   “all”.     Please   don’t  wait   until   the   last  minute   to   do   the  1380  readings!    1381    1382  

• FOS  =   Foundations   of   Science   (custom  edition   of   the  Conceptual   Integrated  1383  Science  textbook  by  Hewitt  et  al.    1384  

• Schick  =  How  to  Think  about  Weird  Things  by  Schick  and  Vaughn  1385    1386    1387    1388    1389    1390  

  Lectures   Labs  1391    1392  1st   1/15   Introduction  to  course:  Weird  Things  People  Believe  and              No  1393  lab  1394       “Witch  Trials  of  the  Past  and  Present:  Why  Evidence    1395                  and  Reason  Matter”    1396     Read  FOS  -­‐  Chapter  1  all:  “About  Science”  pp.  1-­‐14    1397     Read  Schick  –  Chapter  1  all:  “Close  Encounters    1398  

  With  the  Strange”  pp.  1-­‐13  1399  -­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐  1400  2nd   1/20   Complete  Witch  Trials  and  begin  Nature  of  Science   No  lab  1401       Read  Schick  Chapter  6:  “Science  and  Its  Pretenders”  1402     part  158-­‐181  (nature  of  science  &  scientific  reasoning)  1403       Read  Schick  –  Chapter  3:  “Arguments  Good,  Bad    1404                and  Weird”  parts    33-­‐39  and  49-­‐57.  (Pay  particular  1405                attention  to  pages  49-­‐55  dealing  with  informal  1406                fallacies.    You  will  reference  these  throughout  the  course)  1407    1408     1/22   Continue  Nature  of  Science  lecture                        1409       Collect  student  information  for  creating  groups  1410       Syllabus  Quiz  due    1411       Reading  Quiz  1  due      1412  -­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐  1413  3rd   1/27   NASA  Activity               Labs  Begin  &  1414  

Read  Schick  Chapter  4:                                           Extra  Credit  -­‐  1415  Pre  1416  

  33  

“Knowledge,  Belief  and  Evidence”  parts  62-­‐84  and    1417  summary  on  page  90  (opinion  vs.  knowledge  and  expertise)  1418  Argument  Homework  assigned:  due  2/19  1419    1420  

1/29   Nature  of  Science  lecture        1421     “Why  Things  Aren’t  Always  What  They  Seem  to  Be”  1422  

Reading  Quiz  2  due  1423  -­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐  1424  4th   2/3   Begin  lecture  on  the  Limits  to  Perception  and  Memory        Checks    Lab  1425       Read  Schick  Chapter  5:  “Looking  for  Truth  in          1426  

Personal  Experience”  part  96-­‐143  (perception  and  memory  1427  problems)  1428  

 1429     2/5     Continue  Limits  to  Perception  and  Memory      1430  

Reading  Quiz  3  due  1431  -­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐    1432  5th       2/10   Xango  Case  Study                                               Salem  Lab  1433  

Read  FOS  Chapter  2  all:  “The  Universe”  pp.  15-­‐34    1434    1435  2/12     Continue  Limits  to  Perception  and  Memory  1436  

  Read  FOS  Chapter  3:  “The  Atom”35-­‐56  1437     Read  FOS  Chapter  4  “Energy  and  Momentum”  57-­‐76  1438  -­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐  1439    1440  6th     2/17   Begin  Astronomy  1  Lecture:          1441     Perception  lab  1442  

“What  are  those  Lights  in  the  Sky?    Stars,  Planets,  Galaxies”  and    1443  “The  Size  of  the  Universe”  1444  Read  Schick  Chapter  7:  "Case  Studies  in  the      1445     Extraordinary”  part  234-­‐248  (UFO  abductions)  1446  

 1447     2/19   Continue  Astronomy  1  lecture    1448       Argument  Homework  due  1449  -­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐  1450  7th     2/24   Test  1  (NOS  &  LPM)           Astrology  Lab  1451  

Read  Schick  Chapter  4:  "Knowledge,  Belief  and      1452     Evidence”  part  84-­‐90  (astrology  section)  1453  Read  FOS  Chapter  5  “Heat”  77-­‐98  1454  

 1455     2/26   Continue  Astronomy  1  lecture  1456  

Reading  Quiz  4  due      1457  Read  the  Laws  and  Relativity  lecture  posted  on  BB.  1458     This  information  is  critical  to  doing  the  Star    1459       Trek  lab  –  especially  the  section  on  relativity.    1460  

-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐  1461  8th   3/3            Begin  Astronomy  2  Lecture:  “The  Big  Bang  and       Star  Trek  Lab    1462  

  34  

the  Nature  of  the  Universe  –  or  is  it  a  Multiverse?”      1463     Read  FOS  Chapter  6:  "Describing  Motion"  99-­‐116  1464     Read  FOS  Chapter  7:  "Newton’s  Laws  of  Motion"    1465       117-­‐138  1466    1467  

3/5   Complete  Astronomy  2  lecture              1468       “Ghost  Busting  with  Newton’s  Laws”  1469       Read  Power  Point  lecture  on  Black  Board  titled  “The        1470  

  Paranormal  –  Part  1:  History  of  Ghosts,  Psychic  1471     Energy,  Psychic  Powers,  Psychic  Detectives,  1472  Psychic     Healers  and  Mediums.”  1473  

  ****Mid-­‐term  peer  evaluation  due****  1474  -­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐  1475  3/10     Spring  Break  1476  3/12     Spring  Break  1477  -­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐  1478  9th   3/17   Begin  Paranormal  Phenomena  –  Part  2  lecture      Haunting  Lab  1479  

AAW  Water  Homework  assigned;    1480  Part  1  (individual  component)  due  4/7    1481  Part  2  (group  component)  due  4/14  1482  Read  Schick  Chapter  2  all:  “The  Possibility  of  the                                                  1483              Impossible”  pp.  14-­‐29  (the  possibility  of  ESP  and  1484     precognition)  1485  Read  Schick  Chapter  6:  “Science  and  Its  Pretenders”    1486  

part  197-­‐213  (parapsychology)  1487  Read  Schick  Chapter  7:  “Case  Studies  in  the  1488  Extraordinary”  parts  220-­‐227  and  248-­‐276  1489  (talking  to  the  dead,  near-­‐death  experiences,  and  1490  ghosts)  1491  

  3/19   Continue  Paranormal  Phenomena  –  Part  2  lecture                    1492  Reading  Quiz  5  due                            1493  

-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐  1494  10th     3/24   Begin  CAM  1  lecture  on  “Complimentary,  Alternative,   CAM  Lab  1495       and  Quack  Medicines  and  Diets:  take  two  ginkgo  tablets    1496       and  some  homeopathic  elixir  and  you'll  be  fine!”  1497       Read  Schick  Chapter  7  (homeopathy)  part  227-­‐231  1498       Read  Schick  Chapter  7  (climate  change)  part  283-­‐288  1499  

 1500    10th   3/26   Test  2  (Astronomy,  Laws,  and  Paranormal)  1501       Read  Schick  Chapter  5  “Looking  for  Truth  in    1502     Personal  Experience”  part  141-­‐150  (anecdotal    1503     evidence,  placebo  effects  and  controlled  studies)  1504  -­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐  1505  11th     3/31   Continue  CAM  1  lecture           Geology  lab            1506  

Read  FOS  Chapter  8:  "Human  Biology  –  Care  and                                      1507  Maintenance"  139-­‐160    1508  

  35  

 1509  4/2   Begin  CAM  2  lecture    1510  

Reading  Quiz  6  due                            1511  Read  FOS  Chapter  9:  “Rocks  and  Minerals  parts  161-­‐184                              1512  

-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐  1513  12th     4/7   Continue  CAM  2  lecture                                                                                                                Natural  Selection  1514  Lab  1515  

Read  FOS  Chapter  10:  “Plate  Tectonics”                    1516  pp.  185-­‐210            1517  

Read  FOS  Chapter  11  all:  “The  Solar  System”    1518     pp.  211-­‐232  1519  Water  Homework  part  1  due  1520  

   1521     4/9   Vaccine-­‐Autism  Case  Study          1522  

Reading  Quiz  7  due  1523  -­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐  1524  13th   4/14   Atlantis  and  Crystal  Power;  What  Rocks              Extra  Credit  -­‐  1525  Post          1526       and  Minerals  Can  and  Can’t  Tell  Us”                1527       Begin  lecture  on  The  Origin  of  Planet  Earth  (Geology)    1528  

Water  Homework  part  2  due  1529       FiLCHeRS  assigned;  due  4/30  (no  late  work  accepted)  1530      1531     4/16   Finish  Geology  lecture  and    1532       begin  Cryptids  lecture  –    “Legendary  Creatures  and  a  Discouraging    1533       Lack  of  Evidence:  Nessie,  Big  Foot,  and  the  Chupacabra!”                1534  

Read  FOS  Chapter  12:  "The  Basic  Unit  of  Life  –  the    1535              Cell"  pp.  233-­‐260  1536  Read  FOS  Chapter  13  all:  "Genetics"  –  pp.  261-­‐286                      1537  Read  Schick  Chapter  8  all:  "Relativism,  Truth  and  Reality”      1538  -­‐-­‐  pp.  295-­‐315  1539  

-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐  1540  14th   4/21   Finish  Cryptid  lecture  and                 Whale  Lab  1541           Begin  lecture  on  genetics  -­‐“Can  Vulcans  and  Humans    1542       Make  Babies?    The  Genetic  Code  of  Life”    1543  

Reading  Quiz  8  due  1544        1545  

Read  FOS  Chapter  14  all:  "Evolution”  -­‐-­‐  pp.  287-­‐316        1546       Read  Schick  Chapter  6:  “Science  and  Its  Pretenders”    1547     part  181-­‐197  (creationism)  1548    1549  

4/23   Finish  Genetics  and  begin  Evolution  1550    1551  15th     4/28   Continue  Evolution  lecture            1552  

Reading  Quiz  9  due  1553    1554  

  36  

  4/30   “There  is  Grandeur  in  this  View:  Evolutionary        1555       Theory  as  the  Foundation  of  Biology:  Scientific        1556       Synthesis  and  Consistency”  1557  

Homework  FiLCHeRS  due  1558  Peer  evaluations  due  1559  

-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐  1560    1561  Final  –  Covers  material  on  Alternative  Medicines  and  Diets,  Geology,  Cryptids  and  1562  Principle  of  Ecology  Power  Point,  Genetics,  and  Evolution  and  RQ  6  (dealing  with  1563  Schick  Chapter  5),  RQ  7,  and  RQ  8,  as  well  as  the  related  lab  material.    It  is  not  a  1564  comprehensive  final  exam,  but  you  do  need  to  know  the  logical  fallacies  and  critical  1565  thinking  tools  used  throughout  the  course.  1566    1567    1568  

Final  Exam  Time:  Tuesday,  May  5th  from  8:00-­‐10:00  AM  1569    1570  

 1571  • A  summary  list  of  all  of  the  READING  QUIZZES,  their  due  dates,  and  the  1572  

chapters  they  cover  is  provided  on  the  next  2  pages.  1573    1574  A  summary  list  of  the  group  homework  assignments  is  provided  on  the  1575  

last  page  of  this  document.1576  

  37  

Reading  Quizzes  for  Spring  Semester  2015  1577    1578  

*  All  quizzes  are  due  by  5:00pm  on  their  respective  dates  below.    If  you  1579  experience  computer  or  submission  trouble,  please  contact  the  helpdesk  (936-­‐1580  294-­‐2780).    Once  the  assignment  has  opened,  you  may  take  the  quiz  as  many  1581  times  as  you  wish,  until  the  time  it  is  due.      1582  

 1583  • Schick  =  How  to  Think  about  Weird  Things  by  Schick  and  Vaughn  1584  • FOS  =  Foundations  of  Science  text.    *  In  addition  to  the  new,  custom  1585  

Foundations  of  Science  (FOS)  page  numbers,  the  earlier  Conceptual  Integrated  1586  Science  (CIS)  page  numbers  are  also  listed  after  the  FOS  page  numbers  in  1587  case  you  have  the  earlier  edition.    They  are  shown  in  green,  italicized  font.  1588  

• PowerPoint  lectures  on  BB  are  in  purple  1589    1590    1591  

Quiz  1:  Thursday  1/22      1592  1)  Schick  –  Chapter  1  all:  “Close  Encounters  with  the  Strange”  pp.  1-­‐13  1593  2)  Read  FOS  -­‐  Chapter  1:  “About  Science”  pp.  1-­‐14    1594     (CIS  –  Chapter  1  all:  “About  Science”  pp.  1-­‐12)  1595  3)  Read  Schick  Chapter  6:  “Science  and  Its  Pretenders”  part  158-­‐181  (nature  of  1596  

science  and  scientific  reasoning)  1597  4)  Read  Schick  –  Chapter  3:  “Arguments  Good,  Bad  and  Weird”  parts  33-­‐39  and  49-­‐1598  

57.  (Pay  particular  attention  to  pages  49-­‐57  dealing  with  informal  fallacies)  1599    1600  

Quiz  2:  Thursday  1/29  1601  1)  Read  Schick  Chapter  4:  “Knowledge,  Belief  and  Evidence”  parts  62-­‐84  and  1602  

summary  on  90  (opinion  vs.  knowledge  and  expertise)  1603    1604  

Quiz  3:  Thursday  2/5    1605  1)  Read  Schick  Chapter  5:  “Looking  for  Truth  in  Personal  Experience”  part  96-­‐143  1606  (perception  and  memory  problems)  1607    1608  

Quiz  4:  Thursday  2/26    1609  1)  Read  FOS  Chapter  2:  “The  Universe”  pp.  15-­‐34    1610     (CIS  Chapter  28:  all  “The  Universe”  pp.  649-­‐666)  1611  3)  Read  FOS  Chapter  3:  “The  Atom”  pp.  35-­‐56  1612     (CIS  Chapter  9:  “The  Atom”  part  167-­‐179)  1613  4)  Read  FOS  Chapter  4:  “Energy  and  Momentum”  pp.  57-­‐76  1614     (CIS  Chapter  4  on  Energy  part  63  -­‐74)  1615  5)  Read  Schick  Chapter  4  Knowledge,  Belief  and  Evidence”  part  84-­‐90  (astrology  1616  

section)  1617  6)  Read  Schick  Chapter  7:  Case  Studies  in  the  Extraordinary”  part  234-­‐248  (UFO  1618  

abductions)  1619  7)  Read  FOS  Chapter  5:  “Heat”  pp.  77-­‐98  1620     (CIS  Chapter  6  “Heat”  part  98-­‐104)  1621  

  38  

 1622    1623    1624    1625  

Quiz  5:  Thursday  3/19  1626  ~1)  Laws  and  Relativity  lecture  posted  on  BB.    This  information  is  critical  to  the  Star  1627  

Trek  lab    1628  2)  Read  FOS  Chapter  6  “Describing  Motion”  pp.  99-­‐116  1629     (CIS  Chapter  2  all:  "Describing  Motion"  pp.  17-­‐30)  1630  3)  Read  FOS  Chapter  7:  "Newton’s  Laws  of  Motion"  pp.  117-­‐138  1631     (CIS  Chapter  3:  "Newton’s  Laws  of  Motion"  part  36-­‐49)  1632  ~4)  The  Paranormal  lecture  posted  on  BB–  Part  1:  History  of  Ghosts,  Psychic  1633  

Energy,  Psychic  Powers,  Psychic  Detectives,  Psychic  Healers  and  Mediums.”  1634  5)  Read  Schick  Chapter  2  all:  “The  Possibility  of  the  Impossible”  pp.    14-­‐29  (the  1635  

possibility  of  ESP  and  precognition)  1636  6)  Read  Schick  Chapter  6:  “Science  and  Its  Pretenders”  part  197-­‐213  1637  

(parapsychology)  1638  7)  Read  Schick  Chapter  7:  “Case  Studies  in  the  Extraordinary”  parts  220-­‐227  and  1639  

248-­‐276  (talking  to  the  dead,  near-­‐death  experiences,  and  ghosts)  1640    1641  

Quiz  6:  Thursday  4/2  1642  1)  Read  Schick  Chapter  7  (homeopathy)  part  227-­‐231  1643  2)  Read  Schick  Chapter  5  “Looking  for  Truth  in  Personal  Experience”  part  141-­‐150  1644  

(anecdotal  evidence,  placebo  effects  and  controlled  studies)  1645  3)  Read  FOS  Chapter  8:    Human  Biology  –  Care  and  Maintenance”  pp.  139-­‐160  1646  

(CIS  Chapter  20:  "Human  Biology  II  –  Care  and  Maintenance"  part  461-­‐463  and  1647  page  70  on  the  "Placebo  Effect"  1648  

ENV  Homework:  4/2  1649  1)  Read  Schick  Chapter  7  (climate  change)  part  283-­‐288  1650  *  This  assignment  includes  an  analysis  of  claims  regarding  global  climate  change.    1651  

The  information  for  this  will  be  included  as  part  of  the  assignment.  1652    1653  

Quiz  7:  Thursday  4/9  1654  1)  Read  FOS  Chapter  9:  “Rocks  and  Minerals”  pp.  161-­‐184  1655  

(CIS  Chapter  23:  “Rocks  and  Minerals"  parts  531-­‐537  and  541-­‐552    1656  2)  Read  FOS  Chapter  10:  “Plate  Tectonics”  pp.  185-­‐210  1657  

(CIS  Chapter  22  all:  “Plate  Tectonics”  pp.  505-­‐526  1658  3)  Read  FOS  Chapter  11:  “The  Solar  System”  pp.  211-­‐232  1659  

(CIS  Chapter  27  all:  “The  Solar  System”  pp.  320-­‐338)  1660    1661  

Quiz  8:  Tuesday  4/21  1662  1)  Read  FOS  Chapter  12:  "The  Basic  Unit  of  Life  –  the  Cell"  –  pp.  233-­‐260  1663     (CIS  Chapter  15:  "The  basic  Unit  of  Life  –  the  Cell"  -­‐  parts  319-­‐328  and  334-­‐336  1664  

(cell  reproduction)  1665  2)  Read  FOS  Chapter  13  all:  "Genetics"  –  pp.  261-­‐286  1666     (CIS  Chapter  16  all:  "Genetics"  –  pp.  348-­‐368)  1667  

  39  

3)  Read  Schick  Chapter  8  all:  "Relativism,  Truth  and  Reality”  pp.  295-­‐315    1668    1669  

Quiz  9:  Tuesday  4/28  1670  4)  Read  FOS  Chapter  14  all:  "Evolution”  pp.  287-­‐316  1671  

(CIS  Chapter  17  all:  "Evolution”  pp.  372-­‐396)        1672  5)  Read  Schick  Chapter  6:  “Science  and  Its  Pretenders”  part  181-­‐197  (creationism)  1673    1674  

 1675    1676    1677    1678  

Group  Homework  Assignments  1679    1680  *  These  are  group  assignments  and  descriptions  of  them  will  be  provided  at  the  time  1681  they  are  assigned.      1682  

 1683  The  argument  assignment  and  the  AAW  Water  Homework  assignments  require  1684  both  individual  and  group  effort.  So,  time  must  be  allotted  to  coordinate  work  with  1685  the  group  members.    1686    1687  The  FiLCHeRS  homework  assignment  consists  of  both  short  answer  and  multiple  1688  choice  questions.  Some  questions  will  require  that  you  look  up  information  on  the  1689  Internet.    For  these  questions,  you  will  be  asked  to  cite  the  web  addresses  of  the  1690  sites  you  consulted  to  obtain  the  information.  1691  

 1692  1.  Argument  HW  –  assigned  1/27  1693                    -­‐  due  2/19  1694    1695  2.  Water  HW  –  assigned  3/17  1696       -­‐  Part  1  due  4/7  1697       -­‐  Part  2  due  4/14  1698    1699  4.  FiLCHeRS    HW–  assigned  4/14  1700                        -­‐  due  4/30    1701  

 1702   1703