13
Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A Replication and Extension Author(s): Beatriz Ilari Source: Journal of Research in Music Education , January 2015, Vol. 62, No. 4 (January 2015), pp. 332-343 Published by: Sage Publications, Inc. on behalf of MENC: The National Association for Music Education Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/43900263 JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at https://about.jstor.org/terms Sage Publications, Inc. and are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of Research in Music Education This content downloaded from 86.59.13.237 on Thu, 08 Jul 2021 11:29:36 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    4

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A

Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A Replication and Extension

Author(s): Beatriz Ilari

Source: Journal of Research in Music Education , January 2015, Vol. 62, No. 4 (January 2015), pp. 332-343

Published by: Sage Publications, Inc. on behalf of MENC: The National Association for Music Education

Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/43900263

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at https://about.jstor.org/terms

Sage Publications, Inc. and are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of Research in Music Education

This content downloaded from �������������86.59.13.237 on Thu, 08 Jul 2021 11:29:36 UTC��������������

All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

Page 2: Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A

Short Form

Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A Replication and Extension

Beatriz Ilari1

Journal of Research in Music Education 2015, Vol. 62(4) 332-343

© National Association for

Music Education 2014

Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

DOI: 10.1177/0022429414555984

jrme.sagepub.com

(DSAGE

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to replicate and extend previous findings on spontaneous movement and rhythmic engagement with music in infancy. Using the identical stimuli and procedures from the original study, I investigated spontaneous rhythmic movements in response to music, infant-directed speech, and contrasting rhythmic patterns in 30 Brazilian infants (ages 5, I I , and 1 9 months). Findings were consistent with the original study in that more spontaneous rhythmic movements were found in response to music and metrically regular stimuli than to speech. Brazilian babies, however, showed higher means for spontaneous rhythmic movement to music than those reported in the original study. Consistent with the developmental systems approach, these results suggest that culture plays a larger role in spontaneous rhythmic engagement to music and rhythmic entrapment than previously suggested.

Keywords early childhood, spontaneous movement, development, rhythmic entrainment, culture

Music and movement are inextricably linked (Phillips-Silver, 2009). Human beings synchronize their body movements to the beat of music in a process that often has been described as coordinated rhythmic movement (Phillips-Silver, Aktipis & Bryant, 2010), or rhythmic entrainment (Merker, Madison, & Eckerdal, 2009). Broadly speaking, entrainment is "the process in which the rhythms displayed by two or more phenomena become synchronized, with one of the rhythms often being more powerful or dominant

'University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA

Corresponding Author: Beatriz Ilari, Department of Music Teaching and Learning, Thornton School of Music, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089, USA. Email: [email protected]

UIŁI National Association räTLT*! /or Music Education

This content downloaded from �������������86.59.13.237 on Thu, 08 Jul 2021 11:29:36 UTC��������������

All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

Page 3: Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A

Ilari 333

and capturing the rhythm of the other" (Clayton, Sager & Will, 2004, p. 10). That is, rhythmic entrainment relates to the coordination of temporally structured events through interaction (Clayton et al., 2004). This occurs, for example, when a person sings and dances while listening to a favorite song or when two or more people play chamber music or sing together in a choir. In all cases, voices and bodies are coordi- nated and synchronized to a common rhythmic pulse (Phillips-Silver et al., 2010). Rhythmic entrainment is at the core of collective music making (Clayton et al., 2004; Cross, 2005; Kirschner & Tomasello, 2009; Lucas, Clayton, & Leante, 2011). Cultural groups the world over engage in collective music practices that often involve dancing or movement. Although these practices range enormously in terms of who can access them, repertoire, uses of instruments, and associated meanings (e.g., Lucas et al., 2011; Turino, 2006), a common element to them is, as noted, the synchronization of voices and body movements to the pulse of a common, repetitive acoustic and/or audiovisual pattern (Clayton et al., 2004). Interestingly, recent studies suggest that there may be some associations between rhythmic entrainment, human cooperation, and social cohesion (Anshel & Kipper, 1988; Clayton, 2009; Cross, 2005; Hove & Risen, 2009; Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009). This aligns with the idea of music, and par- ticularly rhythm, as a "tie that binds."

While rhythm is often credited as the source of body movement and rhythmic entrain-

ment, it is the existence of metrical structures that actually compels humans to move in

synchrony to the musical beat (Zentner & Eerola, 2010). Frequently described as a uni- versal property of music, meter depicts an abstract hierarchical structure of music, in which there is an alternation of strong and weak beats. The processing of meter is highly complex and multilayered (see Philips-Silver et al., 2010). Through a process called beat induction, listeners extract metrical structures (e.g., the underlying beat) from the rhyth-

mic patterns that they perceive, even when the pulse is not explicit (see Honing, 2011; Patel, 2008). Beat induction is necessary for rhythmic entrainment to occur. A network of sensory modalities, including the auditory, the visual, and the vestibular, is also impli- cated in rhythmic entrainment, involving a combination of (a) perception, (b) produc- tion, (c) integration, and (d) adjustment of body movements in response to perceived rhythmic and metric structures (Phillips-Silver et al., 2010; Phillips-Silver & Trainor, 2005). These "building blocks" play a central role in the way humans - children and adults alike - entrain to the musical beat (Phillips-Silver et al., 2010). Early in life, babies can perceive violations to rhythmic and metric structures (Bergeson & Trehub, 2006; Thorpe & Trehub, 1989; Trehub & Thorpe, 1989; Winkler, Háden, Ladinig, Sziller, & Honing, 2009), tell contrasting meters apart (Hannon & Trehub, 2005), and transfer information from "body movements to the auditory encod- ing of a musical beat" (Phillips-Silver et al., 2010, p. 5), as when they show attentional biases for a meter to which they were bounced a few minutes prior to testing (Philips- Silver & Trainor, 2005). Babies, however, are not able to synchronize fully their bod- ies to the musical pulse. Bodily responses to music are said to be involuntary in the beginning of life and become gradually more robust, voluntarily controlled, and syn- chronized as children develop and grow. That is, the ability to entrain to the musical beat does not appear to emerge until children are well into the preschool years

This content downloaded from �������������86.59.13.237 on Thu, 08 Jul 2021 11:29:36 UTC��������������

All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

Page 4: Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A

334 Journal of Research in Music Education 62(4)

(Kirschner & Tomasello, 2009; Provasi & Bobin-Bègue, 2003). While age has been identified as an important factor in young children's responses to the musical beat (Drake, Jones, & Baruch, 2000; Rainbow, 1981), it should not be viewed as com- pletely deterministic, as marked individual differences also have been found in several studies (e.g., Kirschner & Tomasello, 2009). These findings suggest that musical entrainment takes time to fully mature and may be affected by several other factors, including types of tasks and specific demands (i.e., tapping, vocalizing, marching), stimuli tempi (Rose, Fisher, & Shewmaker, 2012), music training (Drake et al., 2000), culture (Kirschner & Ilari, 2014), and social context (Eerola, Luck, & Toiviainen, 2006; Kirschner & Tomasello, 2009; Rainbow, 1981; Schieuter & Schieuter, 1985), to name a few.

While scholars seem to agree on the importance of rhythmic entrainment in music learning and collective music making, its origins are still contested. There is much debate as to whether the ability to rhythmically entrain to music is a learned behavior or a consequence of human predispositions to respond to music and sounds with a regu- lar pulse (see Clayton, 2009; Drake et al., 2000; Honing, 2011). Along with examina- tions of electrophysiological brain activity (Winkler et al., 2009) and measurements of attentional biases in young children (e.g., Trehub & Hannon, 2006; Phillips-Silver & Trainor, 2005), studies on spontaneous responses to musical and nonmusical sounds, with regular and irregular pulse, also may provide some insight into this question. In a recent study, 5- to 24-month-olds displayed more spontaneous rhythmic movements to recorded music with regular pulse and metrically regular stimuli than to speech sounds (Zentner & Eerola, 2010). The authors interpreted these findings as human predisposi- tions for metrically regular stimuli. Yet, their sample was composed of Swiss and Finish children, who were likely to be exposed primarily to Western musical structures, in which regular meters are most common. The study raises the question as to whether culture may play a role in children's spontaneous movement responses to rhythmic regularities in musical and speech sounds. Would similar results hold if children from a very different cultural background underwent the same testing procedures?

The purpose of the present study was to replicate and extend Zentner and Eerola's (2010) study in a non-European sample. Using the identical procedures and stimuli from the original study, I investigated spontaneous rhythmic movements in response to varied auditory stimuli, namely, music, infant-directed speech, and rhythmic pat- terns, in Brazilian babies. Findings from this study are relevant for music education, as they shed some light on important issues, such as the origins of meter perception and metrical understanding in music (see Bamberger, 1991) and their relationship to cul- ture (for a discussion, see Morrison & Demorest, 2009).

Method

Sample

Forty-three healthy Brazilian infants ages 5, 11, and 19 months took part in the study. They were recruited by means of flyers placed on the murals of two universities and

This content downloaded from �������������86.59.13.237 on Thu, 08 Jul 2021 11:29:36 UTC��������������

All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

Page 5: Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A

Ilari 335

several early childhood enrichment programs in a large city in southern Brazil. All children came from middle-class families as locally articulated, and 40% were attend- ing different early childhood music programs at the time of data collection. Data from 13 infants were excluded from the final analysis due to restlessness {n = 3), equipment failure (n = 2), parental interference or excessive amount of engagement with child during the experiment (n = 4), and posterior report of hearing impairment, cold, or ear infection at the time of testing (n = 4). The final sample was composed of 30 infants, with an equal number of children and an equivalent number of boys and girls distrib- uted in each of the three age groups. Demographic information, parental music back- ground, and uses of music in the home were investigated by means of a semistructured interview (see Ilari, Moura, & Bourscheidt, 201 1), which was administered prior to the listening test and right after consent forms were read and signed.

Procedures

As in the original study (Zentner & Eerola, 2010), infants were seated comfortably on their caregiver's lap facing a 17-inch Dell computer screen, which was placed at eye level and approximately 5 feet 10 inches away from the infant-parent dyad. Two loud- speakers were mounted to each side of the computer screen, playing six different stim- uli at comfortable levels, in six different orders. Still images of colored shapes were presented on the computer screen and changed every 10 s to keep children on task. A high-definition Sony video camera was placed on top of the computer monitor and captured the full body image of the child for subsequent analyses. Parents and caregiv- ers were instructed to sit as still as possible while holding the child in their lap and directing their attention to the screen. Parents also wore tight-fitting earphones with masking stories and music to prevent any interference with children's responses. The entire test lasted approximately 12 min. Children received a small gift and an "Infant Scientist" certificate as a token of appreciation for their participation.

Mus/c Excerpts

The exact same stimuli from Zentner and Eerola (2010) were used in the present study. In total, six different stimuli from the original study were presented to infants, includ-

ing (Stimulus 1) a fast paced, Finnish children's song; (Stimulus 2) a sample of moth- erese (infant-directed speech) in English; (Stimulus 3) an isochronous, regular beat rhythmic pattern in 4/4 (428-ms inter-onset interval), played by a drum set and with a timbre change about halfway into the excerpt; and (Stimuli 4 to 6) three different ver- sions taken from the beginning of the finale from Saint-Säens's "Carnaval des ani- maux" orchestral suite. These included the original orchestral version of the Saint-Säens finale (Stimulus 4); a version of the same excerpt with added ritardandos and accele- randosi here called "Saint-Säens fluctuating" (Stimulus 5); and a rhythmic version displaying only the figurai and metric structures of the music excerpt, here called "Saint-Säens skeleton" (Stimulus 6). To prevent order effects, stimuli were presented in six different orders. Each stimulus was 1 min 42 s in duration. Ten seconds of

This content downloaded from �������������86.59.13.237 on Thu, 08 Jul 2021 11:29:36 UTC��������������

All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

Page 6: Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A

336 Journal of Research in Music Education 62(4)

silence followed the presentation of each stimulus (for more information and to listen to the stimuli, please refer to the appendix found in Zentner & Eerola, 2010).

Results

Following the original study (Zentner & Eerola, 2010), body movements were coded manually and subsequently were analyzed. Three independent observers, who were completely blind to the aims and hypotheses of the study, coded infant spontaneous movements by watching all videos without their corresponding soundtracks. Movements of limbs, heads, and full body were taken into account if they were "repeated in the same form at least three times at regular short intervals" (Thelen, 1979, cited in Zentner & Eerola, 2010, p. 1). Interobserver reliability was computed at .92.

A mixed-design ANOVA with duration of movement in response to six different stimuli as the within-subject factor and age as the between-subject factor was com- puted. Mauchly's test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated, X2(14) = 59.57 p < .001 , therefore degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse- Geisser estimates of sphericity, 8 = 0.48. The analysis revealed main effects of stimu- lus, F(2.4, 65.8) = 9.064, p < .001, r|2 = .25 1, and no interaction effects, F(4.88, 65.8) = .976, p = .438, Tļ2 = .006 ( ns ). Additionally, the two stimuli that generated the largest amount of periodic movements were the Saint-Säens original and the Finnish tune, yet there were no statistically significant differences between them, ¿(29) =1.185,/? = .246 (ns). Following the original study, these two scores were collapsed and averaged into a music composite score, which was compared with scores for rhythmic pattern and motherese speech. Post hoc t tests with the Bonferroni adjustment suggested that there were statistically significant differences for the following pairs: (a) music composite and motherese speech, ¿(29) = 4.832,/? < .001, and (b) rhythmic pattern and motherese speech, ¿(29) = 4.333,/? < .001. Figure 1 depicts mean rhythmic duration movement (in seconds) for each type of musical stimulus and music composite.

Discussion

On the basis of an earlier work by Zentner and Eerola (2010), I examined spontaneous movements in response to music, infant-directed speech, and rhythmic patterns in Brazilian babies. Findings from the current study were in many ways similar to those from the original one (Zentner & Eerola, 2010). First, it was noteworthy that no sig- nificant age differences or trends in spontaneous responses to auditory stimuli were found for this particular group of babies. This is somewhat in accordance with Zentner and Eerola (2010), who found only marginal effects of age for one of their samples, and in full agreement with previous studies that reported individual differences in children's rhythmic synchronization skills and accuracy (Eerola et al., 2006; Kirschner & Tomasello, 2009). While dissimilarities in the development of motor coordination and control may explain these individual differences, one should not rule out the influ- ence of enculturation on children's spontaneous movements to auditory stimuli (see

This content downloaded from �������������86.59.13.237 on Thu, 08 Jul 2021 11:29:36 UTC��������������

All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

Page 7: Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A

Ilari

25

14.883 16 158 20 -r-

13.607 -r- T

Ih. Li MUSIC SSOriginal SSFIuctuating SSSkeleton Finnish Song Motherese Metronome

COMPOSITE

Figure I . Mean rhythmic movement duration (in seconds), per excerpt (error bars show SD).

Hannon & Trainor, 2007; Phillips-Silver & Trainor, 2005). Music engagement in fami- lies with babies and young children varies considerably (Young & Ilari, 2012), and affordances in the home environment are known to impact motor development in both infancy and toddlerhood (Miquelote, Santos, Caçola, Montebelo, & Gabbard, 2012). Therefore, it is possible that some participating babies were encouraged comparatively more to move to music than were others. Because music engagement in participating families was described in more qualitative than quantitative terms in the semistruc- tured interviews (Ilari et al., 2011), this question cannot be answered by this study alone.

Of particular interest to this study was a comparison between spontaneous move- ments to music and motherese speech. Participating infants showed more spontaneous periodic movements in response to music than to motherese speech. This finding not only is consistent with the original study (Zentner & Eerola, 2010) but also aligns with previous studies suggesting differential responses to music and speech in babyhood (e.g., Reigado, Rocha, & Rodrigues, 2011). Apart from possible overlaps between music and speech in early life, like analogous, communicative functions of motherese speech and infant-directed singing (Brandt, Gebrian, & Sieve, 2012; McMullen & Saffran, 2003; Trainor, 1996; Trehub & Trainor, 1993), these two forms of human communication through sounds are also acoustically different. One important distinc- tion refers to the fact that music "relies on analysis over longer time windows" (Brandt et al., 2012, p. 4). The existence of a clear, repetitive, and regular pulse underlying the acoustic signal is arguably more prominent in music that commonly is presented to infants (e.g., lullabies, children's songs, folk tunes) than in motherese speech. In other words, it is likely that stimulus properties played a role in children's rhythmic attend- ing and motor responses (see Drake et al., 2000). Alternatively, it is also possible that even the youngest children in the sample (i.e., 5-month-olds) already had learned com-

This content downloaded from �������������86.59.13.237 on Thu, 08 Jul 2021 11:29:36 UTC��������������

All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

Page 8: Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A

338 Journal of Research in Music Education 62(4)

municative and social functions associated with both music and speech, thus respond- ing differently to each of them. This is a question that merits further study.

It was also interesting that the Saint-Säens fluctuating version generated some spontaneous movement in participating infants, as occurred among European infants. In their study, Zentner and Eerola (2010) explained this unexpected finding as the experimenters' "failure to produce a truly irregular stimulus" (p. 4). They also added that slowing down and speeding up is a characteristic of folk music that also has been shown to produce entrainment (Zentner & Eerola, 2010). Given that the repertoire of early childhood music in Brazil is driven largely by folk music (Ilari, 2009), it is pos- sible that children simply were displaying behaviors that were part of their upbringing and culture as they moved in response to the Saint-Säens fluctuating version. This obviously ties in with the idea of enculturation processes, as described earlier.

Even if the current study did not examine rhythmic coordination with music directly (see Zentner & Eerola, 2010, Experiment 2), the quantification of spontaneous, peri- odic movements to music and other auditory stimuli in these 30 babies generated some interesting findings. On the one hand, the fact that this study replicated the original one, or that Brazilian babies moved in similar ways as their European peers, somewhat reinforces the notion that humans may be predisposed to metrically regular auditory stimuli. On the other hand, it was interesting to note that Brazilian babies showed higher means for spontaneous rhythmic movements to music (music composite = approximately 14 s) than their European counterparts (music composite = approxi- mately 7 s), which suggests some cultural variation (see Phillips-Silver & Trainor, 2005). These findings are not in complete opposition to one another. Rather, their integration is consistent with the developmental systems approach (Gottlieb, 2007). According to this view, there is a false dichotomy between culture and biology, as the development of any organism is the result of interactions between four complex levels, namely (a) genetic activity, (b) neural activity, (c) behavior, and (d) environment (i.e., physical, social, and cultural aspects; Gottlieb, 1991, 2007). Thus, it is possible that humans come into the world predisposed and equipped, to some extent, to engage with metrically organized auditory stimuli (Zentner & Eerola, 2010) and that, as they develop and grow, the interactions between these predispositions, culture, age, and experience result in different levels of rhythmic entrainment. This aligns with works conducted with school-age children (e.g., Rohwer, 1998) and comparative studies (Hattori, Tomonaga, & Matsuzawa, 2013).

Finally, there is reason to believe that early childhood music programs may impact rhythmic enculturation in infants (Gerry, Faux, & Trainor, 2010; Trainor, Marie, Gerry, Whiskin, & Unrau, 2012), including where spontaneous movements are concerned. However, Zentner and Eerola (2010) found no significant correlations between the two. Given the sample size and the large variance in terms of attendance and time spent in music classes, it was not possible to determine the association between spon- taneous movements and participation in early childhood music education programs in the current study. Yet, this finding is worthy of further investigation as it has clear implications for practice. Authors of future research should attempt to control for both quantity and quality of time spent in early childhood music programs, as participation

This content downloaded from �������������86.59.13.237 on Thu, 08 Jul 2021 11:29:36 UTC��������������

All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

Page 9: Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A

Ilari

is often of a transient nature, and curricula and teaching approaches vary considerably. Additionally, the current study was carried out in a laboratory setting, which leaves one to wonder about the nature of young children's spontaneous movements in natu- ralistic settings (e.g., home) and less constrained conditions. Using longitudinal designs, therefore, researchers could examine correlations between the quantification of spontaneous movements and the degree of rhythmic synchronization in children from different age groups, cultures, and early childhood music programs, in and out of research laboratories.

Conclusion

Movement is an integral part of early childhood music education. Early in life, young children shake, rock, bounce, and move in response to the music that they hear. Unsurprisingly, the building blocks that later will enable children to synchronize to the beat of music appear to be present very early in life. The rudiments of beat induction already are seen in newborns (Winkler et al., 2009), and babies show responses to the musical beat by moving their bodies and limbs, often in quasiperiodic ways (Zentner & Eerola, 2010), even in an artificial laboratory setting like the one created for the purpose of the present study. Outside of research laboratories, teachers and parents have long witnessed the richness of young children's spontaneous movements, includ- ing "unintentional" attempts to synchronize to the beat from very early on. In spite of many important discussions concerning the development of rhythmic, pulse, and met- ric understanding, the field of early childhood music education has not embraced fully the concept of rhythmic entrainment. Yet there are many reasons as to why it is rele- vant for both researchers and practitioners working with young children.

The study of rhythmic entrainment and its developmental course has much to con- tribute to music education in that it offers new ways to conceptualize music percep- tion, production, and learning. Through the lens of entrainment, these processes can be understood as integrated, embodied, and interactive (Clayton et al., 2004). After all, "sound is perceived not only in terms of the auditory signal, but also in terms of the intentional, hierarchically organized sequences of expressive motor acts behind the signal" (Overy & Molnar-Szakacs, 2009, p. 492).

Unsurprisingly, interaction is a key feature of rhythmic entrainment, especially when it involves more than one individual. According to Cross, Laurence, and Rabinowitch (2012), when two or more people entrain to the same musical beat, their breathing rates, heartbeat, brain waves, attention, and movements become coordi- nated, which, in turn, may result in a sense of "togetherness" and may promote the emergence of empathy, human cooperation, and social cohesion (see also Kirschner & Tomasello, 2010). Perhaps it does not come as a surprise that children as young as 4 show higher synchronization accuracy when invited to drum with a partner as opposed to with a machine playing isochronous sounds (Kirschner & Tomasello, 2009). This finding not only is consistent with practices in early childhood music education, which tend to be both collective and participatory, but also raises questions about the ratio- nale for and ways of teaching steady beat to young children (see Bennett, 2012).

This content downloaded from �������������86.59.13.237 on Thu, 08 Jul 2021 11:29:36 UTC��������������

All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

Page 10: Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A

340 Journal of Research in Music Education 62(4)

Arguably, studies on the development of rhythmic entrainment may provide some clues into these long-standing questions, as they extend beyond rhythmic education or steady-beat perception and production. By understanding how humans engage with musical sounds from very early on, we also are granted opportunities to understand why, as a species, we are compelled to do so.

Acknowledgments The author would like to thank all participating families, Dr. Marcel Zentner (The University of York, UK), and Dr. Tuomas Eerola (University of Jyväskylä, Finland) for sharing their stimuli and expertise.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The author received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

References

Anshel, A., & Kipper, D. A. (1988). The influence of group singing on trust and cooperation. Journal of Music Therapy , 3 , 145-155.

Bamberger, J. (1991). The mind behind the musical ear . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Bennett, P. (2012). Why do we do that? Questioning five common practices in elementary music. In Proceedings of ISME's Early Childhood Music Commission (pp. 57-58). Victoria, Australia: International Society for Music Education. Retrieved from http://www.isme.org

Bergeson, T., & Trehub, S. E. (2006). Infants' perception of rhythmic patterns. Music Perception , 25(4), 345-360. doi: 10. 1525/mp.2006.23 .4.345

Brandt, A., Gebrian, M., & Sieve, R. L. (2012). Music and early language acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1-17. DOI: 1 0.33 89/fpsyg.20 12.00327

Clayton, M. (2009). The social and personal functions of music in cross-cultural perspective. In S. Hallam, I. Cross, & M. Thaut (Eds.), Oxford handbook of music psychology (pp. 35-44). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Clayton, M., Sager, R., & Will, U. (2004). In time with the music: The concept of entrainment and its significance for ethnomusicology. ESEM Counterpoint, 1 , 1-84.

Cross, I. (2005). Music and meaning, ambiguity and evolution. In D. Mieli, R. Macdonald, & D. Hargreaves (Eds.), Musical communication (pp. 27-43). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Cross, I., Laurence, F., & Rabinowitch, T. (2012). Empathy and creativity in group musical practices: Towards a concept of empathie creativity. In G. E. McPherson & G. Welch (Eds.). The Oxford handbook of music education (pp. 337-353). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Drake, C., Jones, M. R., & Baruch, C. (2000). The development of rhythmic attending in auditory sequences: Attunement, referent period, focal attending. Cognition , 77, 251. doi: 10.1016/ S00 1 0-0277(00)00 1 06-2

This content downloaded from �������������86.59.13.237 on Thu, 08 Jul 2021 11:29:36 UTC��������������

All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

Page 11: Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A

Ilari 34 1

Eerola, T., Luck, G., & Toiviainen, P. (2006). An investigation of preschoolers' corporeal syn- chronization with music. In M. Baroni, A. R. Addessi, R. Caterina, & M. Costa (Eds.), Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (pp. 472-476). Bologna, Italy: ICMPC-ESCOM. Gerry, D., Faux, A., & Trainor, L. J. (2010). Effects of Kindermusik training on infants' rhythmic enculturation. Developmental Science , 13, 545-551. doi: 10. Ill 1/j. 1467- 7687.2009.00912.x

Gottlieb, G. ( 1 99 1 ). Experiential canalization of behavioral development: Theory. Developmental Psychology , 27, 4-1 3. doi: 1 0. 1 . 1 .2 1 1 .34 1 0

Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science , 10, 1-11. doi: 10.1 111/ j. 1467-7687.2007.00556.X

Hannon, E. E., & Trainor, L. J. (2007). Music acquisition: Effects of enculturation and for- mal training on development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 466-472. doi: 10. 1016/j .tics.2007.08.008

Hannon, E. E., & Trehub, S. E. (2005). Metrical categories in infancy and adulthood. Psychological Science , 16, 48-55. doi: 10. 1 1 1 l/j.0956-7976.2005.00779.x

Hattori, Y., Tomonaga, M., & Matsuzawa, T. (2013). Spontaneous synchronized tapping to an auditory rhythm in a chimpanzee. Scientific Reports, 3. doi:10.1038/srep01566

Honing, H. (2011). Musical cognition : A science of listening. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Transaction.

Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It's all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affili- ation. Social Cognition, 27, 949-961. doi: 10.1 52 l/soco.2009.27. 6.949

Ilari, B. (2009). Música na infancia e na adolescencia: Um livro para pais, professores e aficio- nados [Music in childhood and adolescence: A book for parents, teachers and aficionados]. Curitiba, Brazil: IBPEX.

Ilari, B., Moura, A., & Bourscheidt, L. (201 1). Between interactions and commodities: Musical parenting of infants and toddlers in Brazil. Music Education Research, 13(1), 51-67. doi: 10.1080/14613808.2011.553277

Kirschner, S., & Ilari, B. (2014). Joint drumming in Brazilian and German preschool children: Cultural differences in rhythmic entrainment, but no prosocial effects. Journal of Cross- Cultural Psychology, 45( 1 ), 1 37-1 66. doi: 10.11 77/0022022 1 1 3493 139

Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Joint drumming: Social context facilitates synchroni- zation in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 299-314. doi: 1 0. 1 0 1 6/j .jecp.2008. 07.005

Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (20 1 0). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-

old children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 354-364. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav. 2010.04.004

Lucas, G., Clayton, M., & Leante, L. (201 1). Intergroup entrainment in Afro-Brazilian Congado ritual. Empirical Musicology Review, 6(2), 75-102. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.20 12.00026

McMullen, E., & Saffran, J. E. (2003). Music and language: A developmental comparison. Music Perception, 21, 289-31 1. doi:10.1525/mp.2004.21.3.289

Merker, B. H., Madison, G. S., & Eckerdal, P. (2009). On the role and origin of isochrony in human rhythmic entrainment. Cortex, 45, 4-17. doi: 10. 1016/j. cortex.2008.06.01 1

Miquelote, A. F., Santos, D. C. C., Caçola, P. M., Montebelo, M. I., & Gabbard, C. (2012). Effects of the home environment on motor and cognitive behavior in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(3), 329-334. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.02.002

This content downloaded from �������������86.59.13.237 on Thu, 08 Jul 2021 11:29:36 UTC��������������

All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

Page 12: Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A

342 Journal of Research in Music Education 62(4)

Morrison, S., & Demorest, S. (2009). Cultural constraints on music perception and cognition. In J. Y. Chião (Ed.), Progress in brain research: Vol. 1 78. Cultural neuroscience: Cultural influences on brain function (pp. 6-77). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier.

Overy, K., & Molnar-Szakacs, I. (2009). Being together in time: Musical experience and the mirror neuron system. Music Perception , 26, 489-504. doi:10.1525/MP.2009.26.5.489

Patel, A. D. (2008). Music, language, and the brain . New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Phillips-Silver, J. (2009). On the meaning of movement in music, development and brain.

Contemporary Music Review, 28(3), 293-314. doi: 10.1525/mp.2003. 20.4.431 Phillips-Silver, J., Aktipis, C. A., & Bryant, G. A. (2010). The ecology of entrainment:

Foundations of coordinated rhythmic movement. Music Perception, 28, 3-14. doi: 10. 1 525/ mp.2010.28.1.3.

Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2005). Feeling the beat: Movement influences infant rhythm perception. Science, 308(5121), 1430. doi: 10.1 126/science.l 1 10922

Provasi, J., & Bobin-Bègue, A. (2003). Spontaneous motor tempo and rhythmical synchronisa- tion in 2/4- and 4-year-old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 220-23 1 . doi: 1 0. 1 080/0 1 650250244000290

Rainbow, E. (1981). A final report on a three-year investigation of rhythmic abilities of pre- school aged children. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 66/67 , 69-73.

Reigado, J., Rocha, A., & Rodrigues, H. (201 1). Vocalizations oí îniants (9-1 1 months old) in response to musical and linguistic stimuli. International Journal of Music Education, 29(3), 241-256. doi: 10.11 77/025576 1411 408507

Rose, P., Fisher, R., & Shewmaker, S. (2012). Effects of manual/pedal movement, tempo and gender on accuracy of steady-beat in kindergarten students. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 192, 39-48.

Rohwer, D. (1998). Effect of movement on steady-beat perception, synchronization and per- formance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 46(3), 414-424. doi: 10.2307/3345553

Schieuter, S. L., & Schieuter, L. J. (1985). The relationship of grade level and sex differences to certain rhythmic responses of primary grade children. Journal of Research in Music Education, 32, 23-29. doi: 10.2307/3344755

Thelen, E. (1979). Rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants. Animal Behaviour, 27, 699-7 1 5 . doi: 1 0. 1 0 1 6/0003-3472(79)90006-X

Thorpe, L. A., & Trehub, S. E. (1989). Duration illusion and auditory grouping in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 25, 122-127.

Trainor, L. J. (1996). Infant preferences for infant-directed versus noninfant-directed play- songs and lullabies. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 83-92. DOI: 10.101 6/SO 1 63- 6383(96)90046-6

Trainor, L. J., Marie, C., Gerry, D., Whiskin, E., & Unrau, A. (2012). Becoming musically enculturated: Effects of music classes for infants on brain and behavior. Annals of the New

York Academy of Sciences, 1252, 129-138. doi:l 0.1 1 1 1/j.l 749-6632.20 12.06462.x Trehub, S. E., & Hannon, E. E. (2006). Infant music perception: Domain-general or domain-

specific mechanisms? Cognition, 100, 13-99. doi: 10. 101 6/j.cognition.2005.1 1.006. Trehub, S. E., & Thorpe, L. A. (1989). Infants' perception of rhythm: Categorization of auditory

sequences by temporal structure. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43, 217-229. Trehub, S. E., & Trainor, L. J. (1993). Listening strategies in infancy: The roots of language

and musical development. In S. McAdams & E. Bigand (Eds.), Cognitive aspects of human audition (pp. 278-327). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

This content downloaded from �������������86.59.13.237 on Thu, 08 Jul 2021 11:29:36 UTC��������������

All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

Page 13: Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A

Ilari 343

Turino, T. (2006). Music as social life. Urbana-Champaign: University of Illinois Press. Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science , 20,

1-5. doi: 10.1 1 1 1/U467-9280.2008.02253.X

Winkler, I., Háden, G. P., Ladinig, O., Sziller, I., & Honing, H. (2009). Newborn infants detect the beat in music. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106 , 2468-2471. doi: 1 0. 1 073/pnas.080903 5 1 06

Young, S., & Ilari, B. (2012). Musical participation from zero to three: Towards a global perspective. In G. McPherson & G. Welch (Eds.), Oxford handbook of music education (pp. 279-295). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Zentner, M., & Eerola, T. (2010). Rhythmic engagement with music in infancy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 5568-5573. doi: 10. 1073/pnas. 1000121 10

Author Biography Beatriz Ilari is assistant professor of music education at the University of Southern California. Her research interests include music in childhood, musical development, culture, and the social psychology of music.

Submitted April 1, 2013; accepted January 31, 2014.

This content downloaded from �������������86.59.13.237 on Thu, 08 Jul 2021 11:29:36 UTC��������������

All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms