Role of Health Locus of Control Beliefs in Cancer Screening of Elderly Hispanic Women

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/11/2019 Role of Health Locus of Control Beliefs in Cancer Screening of Elderly Hispanic Women

    1/7

    HealthPsychology

    1993, Vol.12,

    No .

    3,193-199

    Copyright1993by theAmerican Psychological Association, Inc.,and

    the Division of Health

    Psychology/0278-6133/93/$3.00

    RoleofHealth LocusofControl

    Beliefs

    inCancer ScreeningofElderly

    Hispanic

    Women

    Nancy

    I. Bund ek,GaryMarks, andJeanL.Richardson

    This

    study

    examined the

    health locus

    of control

    beliefs

    of

    elderly

    Hispanic women and relation

    between

    frequency

    of

    breast

    self-examination

    (BSE),attention

    to

    health-relatedinformation,

    and

    recency

    of Pap

    smear

    and physician breast examination. As

    hypothesized,

    holding a belief that

    health outcomes are controlled by oneself (internal control) was positively related to screening

    behaviors overwhich

    one has a

    high

    degree of

    personal control, such

    as

    frequency

    of BSE and

    attention to health-related

    information.

    Belief

    that

    medical professionals

    control

    healthoutcomes

    waspositively related

    to physician-dependent screening

    activities, such

    as

    recency

    of Pap smear

    and

    physician breast

    exam. The findings confirm the

    specificity

    of association between

    health

    control beliefs and preventive behaviors and

    demonstrate

    the importance of these

    beliefs

    in

    medical

    screening

    by

    Hispanic

    women.

    Understanding

    people's

    beliefs about

    the

    factors that con-

    trol health outcomes

    may be

    critical

    to

    understanding

    people's

    health-related behaviors. Early work by B. S. Wallston,

    Wall-

    ston, Kaplan, and Maides

    (1976)

    focused on internal and

    external dimensions of health locus of control, an outgrowth of

    Rotter's(1954,1966)distinction between internal

    and

    external

    expectancies

    of

    reinforcement.

    The

    early two-dimensional

    scale

    was

    later expanded

    to the

    Multidimensional Health

    Locus

    of

    Control (MHLOC) scale

    (K. A.

    Wallston, Wallston,

    &

    DeVellis,

    1978).

    Internal control refers to the belief that

    health outcomes

    are

    determined

    byone's own

    actions

    and

    decisions. Control by powerful others refers to the belief that

    the actions ofdoctors and

    other

    health professionals deter-

    mine

    health outcomes through the instructions, recommenda-

    tions, and medications they provide. Chance control refers to

    the belief that health and illness are largely a matter of chance

    or fate.

    These

    beliefs generally

    form

    earlyin life as aresultofearly

    childhood experiences wi th illness inone's family, and may

    remain

    relatively stable across time (Lau, 1982). The three

    health control dimensions are relatively independent of one

    another. The strongest correlation has been

    found

    between

    chance control

    and

    control

    bypowerful

    others(rs

    = .20 to

    .35;

    Marks, Richardson, Graham, & Levine, 1986; K. A. Wallston

    et al.,

    1978),

    presumably reflecting a common component of

    external

    control.

    Initial

    research focused

    on the

    relationship between locus

    of

    control

    and people's

    interest

    in

    health-related information

    Nancy

    I.

    Bundek, Gary

    Marks, and

    Jean

    L. Richardson, Institute for

    Health Promotion and Disease Prevention Research,D epartment of

    Preventive

    Medicine, University ofSouthernCalifornia.

    This research w assupported by National Cancer

    Institute

    Grant

    CA3566.W egratefully acknowledgeJohnC .Hisserich,JuliaM. Solis,

    Lourdes Birba,

    Fernando

    Torres-Gil, and Linda Collins for their

    contributions tothis project.

    Correspondence concerning

    this article

    should be

    addressed

    to

    Nancy I.

    Bundek, University

    of

    Southern

    California,

    Institute

    for

    Prevention

    Research, 1000

    South Fremont, Suite

    641,

    Alhambra,

    California91803-1358.

    (see

    K. A.

    Wallston

    &

    Wallston, 1982). Using

    the

    early

    two-dimensional scale, K. A. Wallston, Maides, and Wallston

    (1976)

    found

    that college students

    who

    valued health highly

    and had an internal orientation requested more pamphlets on

    hypertension than

    did

    those

    who

    were internal

    w i thlow

    health

    values

    or

    those

    who

    were external. Among older non-Hispanic

    men and women (mean age = 57 years), Toner and Manuck

    (1979)

    found

    that internals requested more information on

    heart disease than did externals. No

    effects,

    however, were

    found

    for younger participants (mean age = 25 years).These

    results are consistentwi ththose of K. A. Wallston et al.(1976)

    ifone

    assumes that

    the

    elderly place greater value

    on

    health

    than

    do

    younger people

    (K. A.

    Wallston

    &

    Wallston,

    1982).

    A few studies have examined the extent to which health

    control beliefs are associated

    wi th

    medical screening practices.

    Redeker

    (1989)

    administered

    the

    MHLOC scale

    to a

    sample

    of

    non-Hispanic women and

    found

    that those who had never

    practiced breast self-examination (BSE) tended

    to

    have lower

    internal controlscoresthan did those who had performed BSE

    three or more times a year. Beliefs about

    powerful

    others were

    not examined. Hallal

    (1982)

    sampled English-speaking women

    and

    found

    a

    significant

    negative correlation between control by

    powerfulothers and ever practicing BSE and a nonsignificant

    positive relationship between internal control and ever perform-

    ingBSE.

    Methodological limitations, however, raise concern about

    HallaPs

    (1982) findings. First,

    frequency

    of BSE was measured

    dichotomously: those who ever practiced BSE (80%) versus

    those who never practiced BSE (20%). Such unbalanced

    groups

    may

    produce

    highly

    unstable results. Moreover,

    the

    former

    group included women

    who

    performed

    BSE

    monthly

    as

    well

    as those who performed it less than once a year. Thus,

    Hallal's results may not

    reflect

    the precise manner in which

    frequency

    of BSE relates to a particular health control

    dimension.

    One purpose of the present study was to test the idea that a

    specificMHLOC belief promotes a specific health practice. In

    other words,

    we

    tested

    the specificity of

    association between

    beliefs and

    behaviors (Fishbein

    & Ajzen,

    1975).

    We

    were

    193

  • 8/11/2019 Role of Health Locus of Control Beliefs in Cancer Screening of Elderly Hispanic Women

    2/7

    194

    N . B U N D E K , G . MA R K S , A N D J . R IC H A R D S O N

    especially

    interested

    in the

    manner

    in

    which beliefs

    relate to

    frequencyof performingBSE, recencyof gynecologicalscreen-

    ing, and attentiveness tohealth-related

    information.

    We

    wouldexpect

    peoplehighininternalcontrolto perform

    screeningbehaviors tha t havea strong personal control compo-

    nent, such

    as BSE and payingattention to

    information

    about

    health

    issues

    (Redeker, 1989;

    Toner

    &

    Manuck,

    1979; K. A.

    Wallston

    et al., 1976). Although gynecological screening in-

    volves some

    degree

    of

    personal control

    in setting up an

    appointment,thistype of

    screening

    behavior

    stronglyempha-

    sizes the role of adoctoras thecontrollerof health

    outcomes.

    Thus,it should berelated

    more

    strongly to

    belief

    incontrolby

    powerful

    others

    than to

    belief

    ininternal

    control.

    Accordingly,

    wetested

    the

    following

    three

    hypotheses:

    (1) The

    frequency

    of

    performing

    BSEcorrelatesdirectly with internal control be-

    liefs.

    (2) Attentiveness to health-related information

    corre-

    lates directly with internal control beliefs. (3) Recency of

    gynecological

    screeningcorrelatesdirectly with belief

    in con-

    trol

    by

    powerful

    others.

    A

    second purpose

    of the

    study

    was to

    examine these

    hypotheseswi th

    a

    sample

    of

    Hispanic

    women.

    Previous studies

    of

    health

    control beliefs

    have

    been performed

    almost

    exclu-

    sivelyw i t h non-Hispanicsamples.Infact,K. A.

    Wallston

    et al.

    (1978)identifiedand validated the multidimensionalnatureof

    these beliefs

    with

    a predominantly

    White

    sample.

    Thus,

    it is

    reasonable to ask whether

    MHLOC

    beliefs are

    relevant

    to

    medicalscreening behaviorsinother

    cultural

    groups.

    Hispanic

    women

    in the

    United States

    represent one such

    important

    group.

    They

    are

    diagnosedwithbreast

    and

    cervical

    cancers at a

    more

    advanced stage ofdisease than are non-

    HispanicWhite

    women

    (Richardsonet al.,

    1992;

    Samet,Hunt,

    Lerchen, &Goodwin, 1988;

    Westbrook,

    Brown, & McBride,

    1975). This has prompted concern about the cancer screening

    practices

    of Hispanic women. Indeed, compared

    with

    other

    groups, they

    are screened

    irregularly, thus contributing

    to

    later-stagediagnosis andreducedchances ofsurvival(Ander-

    son,

    Lewis,

    Giachello,

    Aday, &

    Chiu, 1981).

    Most of the

    past

    research

    on screening

    practices

    of Hispanic

    women

    (and men)

    has focused on the rolesof acculturation (Chavez,Cornelius,

    &Jones, 1985;

    Chesney,

    Chavira, Hall, & Gary, 1982; Deyo,

    Diehl, Hazuda,

    &

    Stern, 1985; Marks

    et

    al., 1987; Wells,

    Hough,Golding,Burnam, & Karno, 1987) and accessto care

    (Anderson et al., 1981; Richardson et al., 1987; Solis, Marks,

    Garcia,

    &

    Shelton,

    1990). Our

    focus

    was on the role

    that

    psychological variablesplay

    in

    preventivehealthbehaviors.

    Method

    Sample

    O ur data were part of Proyecto a Su Salud (Project to Your

    Health),

    a

    longitudinal study

    of

    cancer symptom knowledge

    an d

    screening practices of elderly Hispanic wo men. Pa rticipants lived in 17

    publicly

    subsidized housing projects

    in Los

    Angeles.

    These

    projects

    we re selected because they had a very high percentage of Hispanic

    residents.

    A ll

    w o m en

    55

    years

    of age or

    older received

    a

    mailed

    solicitationto participate a nd then werecontactedi n

    person

    by a study

    representative. Hispanic ethnicity (i.e., family origin from a Latin

    Am erican country, including Cuba and Puerto Rico) was confirmed by

    the

    interviewer. Of the 890 Hispanic womencontacted, 603(67.8%)

    agreed

    to be in the

    study.

    T he

    study involved

    an

    intake assessment

    of

    he alth knowledge

    an d

    screening behaviors. Following

    th e

    intake, housing projects w ere

    randomly

    assigned

    to one of threeconditions (comprehensive health

    education program,

    min ima l - in forma t ion

    control group,

    or no-

    information

    control group).

    The

    comprehensive program consisted

    of

    four 2-hr group educational programs pertaining to breast, cervical,

    colorectal,

    an d

    oral cancers.

    The minimal-information

    control pro-

    gram

    consisted

    o f a

    45-min presentation about

    warning

    signs

    of

    cancer,

    risk

    factors, screening recommendations,

    and the

    importance

    of

    early

    detection.

    A

    follow-upquestionnaire

    was

    administered

    to all

    subjects

    approximately 1year

    after

    the intervention.

    The p resent d ata involve wom en in eithe r of the two control

    conditions ( = 429).These groups were combined because the two

    conditions

    did not

    produce

    an y m a i n or

    interaction effects

    on any

    outcome measure. W e omitted women who received the comprehen-

    sive

    education program

    (n =

    174) because

    it was

    designed

    to

    improve

    medical

    screening practices.

    A ll

    variablesusedin the

    analysis,

    except measures of demographics

    an d acculturation, were taken from

    th e

    follow-up questionnaire.

    O f

    the 429 women in the two control conditions, 270 provided complete

    data on the

    study

    variables and constituted our analytic sample. To

    check for possible attrition bias, we compared women who dropped

    out of the study (n = 159) with our analytic sample on several variables

    measured at intake: demographics, dateof last physical examination,

    frequency

    w ith wh ich they did BSE, nervousness a bout BSE, physician

    breast exam and Pap smear, and recency of these screening proce-

    dures. Dropouts

    differed from

    participants p < .05) only in the

    frequency

    with which they ha d performed BSE. T he primary

    differ-

    ence was tha t 27.4% of the participants reported that they never

    practiced BSE, compared with41.2% ofthosewh o dropped out. Thus,

    generalizations

    from ou r

    study

    m ay be

    more applicable

    to

    w o m en

    w ho

    practice

    BS E with at least some regularity.

    Questionnaire Design an d

    Administration

    The questionnaire was written in English, translated into Spanish,

    and then back-translated to

    identify ambiguity

    of meaning. Both

    versions were pilot-tested

    w i t ha

    sm all sample

    o f

    Hispanic women

    from

    the housing projects. In the main study, the questionnaire was

    administered

    at

    each participant's home

    by

    Spanish-speaking women

    trained

    in

    interviewing techniques.

    T he

    interview

    w as

    conducted

    in the

    languageof the participant's choice(75.9%chose Spanish).

    Independent

    Variables

    Demographics and acculturation. W e measured several demo-

    graphic factors, including age, education, marital status, monthly

    income,

    an d

    health insurance coverage. Acculturation

    w as

    measured

    with

    18 items from the Acculturation Rating Scale fo r Mexican

    Americans (Cuellar, Harris,

    & Jasso,

    1980), focusing primarily

    on

    language preference andusage,country of birth, and years residing in

    the United

    States.

    In tern al reliability was very high

    (Cronbach's

    alpha = .95). Responses were standardized to a mean ofzero and a

    variance

    of one and

    then averaged

    to form on e

    overall acculturation

    scorefo r

    each subject. Standardizing

    the rawscores

    gave equal weig ht

    to

    each item

    in thescale.

    Health

    locus

    of

    control. Health locus

    of

    control

    w as

    m easured

    with

    nineitems from

    th e

    MHLOCscale

    (K. A.

    Wallston

    et

    al.,1978).

    T he

    original scale used

    six

    items

    to

    measure each dimension

    of

    control.

    To

    keep our questionnaire at a manageable length, we used the three

    items that most strongly defined each subdimension in terms of the

    highest

    item-subscale correlations (Marks et

    al.,

    1986; C. H.

    Wolk,

    personalcomm unication, 1982).Theseitems

    and the

    responseformat

    ar e

    presented

    in the

    Append ix.

  • 8/11/2019 Role of Health Locus of Control Beliefs in Cancer Screening of Elderly Hispanic Women

    3/7

    HISPANIC CANCER SCREENING

    195

    The internal reliability for each dim ension was quite high.

    Cron-

    bach's alpha

    w as .82 for the

    three internal control items,

    .82 for the

    powerful

    others

    items, and .72 for the

    three chance control

    items.

    Responses

    to the

    items

    within

    each dimension were standardized

    and

    averaged

    to

    form three subscalescores

    for

    each participant.

    D ep enden t

    Variables

    Frequency

    of

    BSE. It isgenerally accepted that once pe r m o n this

    the optimal frequency for perform ing BSE (A lagna,

    Morokoff,

    Bevett,

    &

    Reddy,

    1987).

    Thisfrequency

    enables

    womento develop the most

    sensitivity

    to

    detect breast lumps

    and

    changes

    in

    their breasts.

    PerformingBSE

    more frequently

    may

    diminish

    one's

    ability

    to

    detect

    subtle changes. This recommendation governed our decision about

    how

    wecodedour BSEfrequency

    data.

    Each woman was asked whether she had performed BSE and how

    often

    she did it. Seventeen p ercent rep orted doing BSE on a m onthly

    basis; for purposes of analysis, they were assigned a code of 4.

    Thirty-seven

    percent

    reported that they performed BSE more fre-

    quentlythanoncea mo nth; they were assigned a code of 3. Those w ho

    reportedperforming

    BSE

    less th anonce

    a

    month (21,0%) were coded

    2,

    and

    those

    who

    reported never doing

    BSE

    (25.0%) were coded

    1.

    Empirical

    support

    fo r

    this coding scheme

    is

    provided

    by the

    women's demonstration of their ability to detect lumps in a

    foam

    breast model. During q uestionnaire adm inistration, they were pre-

    sentedwitha life-size foam m odel of a breast t hat co ntained five lum ps

    of

    varyingsizes. They were asked to ex am ine it as they would their own

    breastsand toreportthe number of lumps found.For the 72% who

    agreed to examine the model

    (n =

    195), women who performed B SE

    monthly

    found

    significantlym ore lumps than didthosewho performed

    BSE

    more than once

    a

    month

    (Ms =

    2.57

    vs.

    1.48,p

  • 8/11/2019 Role of Health Locus of Control Beliefs in Cancer Screening of Elderly Hispanic Women

    4/7

    196

    N . B U N D E K ,

    G.

    M A R K S ,

    AND J.

    R IC H A R D S O N

    Table

    2

    Means an d Standard

    Deviations

    of

    Study

    Variables

    Variable

    Belief

    i n

    intern al control

    Belief

    in

    control

    by

    powerful others

    Belief inchance control

    Frequency

    of

    breast self-examination

    Recency of gynecological screening

    Attention

    to health-related information

    M

    3.44

    3.27

    2.08

    2.45

    3.90

    3.37

    SD

    0.57

    0.59

    0.87

    1.04

    1.27

    0.79

    Note.

    Entries

    a re

    based

    on raw

    scoremeans.

    control, followedbypow erful others. Paired

    t

    tests indicated

    that the means fo r internal control an d control bypowerful

    others differed significantly,

    t(269)

    = 4.41, p < .01. Both

    differed greatly

    from

    chance control: powerful others versus

    chance, t(269) = 27.0,p < .001, and intern al versus chance,

    t(269) =28.55,p < .001. In sum, our subjects had a stronger

    tendency

    to attribute health outcomes to their own actions or

    to the efforts oftheir doctors th antochance.

    Correlations

    Among

    Variables

    Table 3 presents the Pearson correlations among the study

    variables. Internal control

    w as

    related directly

    to BSE

    fre-

    quency

    and topaying attention tohealth-related inform ation,

    supportingHypotheses 1 and 2. Attestof differences between

    dependent

    correlations (Cohen & Cohen, 1975, p. 53)indi-

    cated that frequency

    of BSE

    correlated with internal control

    more strongly than

    it did

    with control

    by

    powerful others,

    f(267)

    = 2 .11, p < .01. Sim ilarly, attention to h ealth-related

    information correlated with

    internal

    control more strongly

    thanit didwith

    control

    by

    powerful others,

    t(267) =

    4.43,

    p